Maladaptive conflict monitoring as evidence for executive dysfunction in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Maladaptive conflict monitoring as evidence for executive dysfunction in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome"

Transcription

1 Developmental Science 8:1 (2005), pp REPORT Blackwell Publishing, Ltd. Maladaptive conflict monitoring as evidence for executive dysfunction in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome Joel P. Bish, 1 Samantha M. Ferrante, 1 Donna McDonald-McGinn, 1 Elaine Zackai 1 and Tony J. Simon 1, 2 1. Children s Hospital of Philadelphia, USA 2. University of Pennsylvania, USA Abstract Using an adaptation of the Attentional Networks Test, we investigated aspects of executive control in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (DS22q11.2), a common but not well understood disorder that produces non-verbal cognitive deficits and a marked incidence of psychopathology. The data revealed that children with DS22q11.2 demonstrated greater difficulty than controls in locating and processing target items in the presence of distracters. Importantly, children with DS22q11.2 showed a deficit in the ability to monitor and adapt to stimulus conflict. These data provide evidence of inadequate conflict adaptation in children with DS22q11.2, a problem that is also present in schizophrenia. The findings of specific executive dysfunction in this group may provide a linkage between particular genetic abnormalities and the development of psychopathology. Chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (DS22q11.2) is a recently defined syndrome (Dunham et al., 1999) that encompasses DiGeorge (DiGeorge, 1965) and Velocardiofacial syndromes (Shprintzen et al., 1978). It is a congenital condition resulting from a 3 Mb deletion of the long (q) arm of chromosome 22 and has an estimated prevalence of at least 1 in 4000 live births (Burn & Goodship, 1996). The commonly observed manifestations of DS22q11.2 include cleft palate, heart defects, T-cell abnormalities, neonatal hypocalcemia, facial dysmorphisms, in addition to mild to moderate cognitive deficits (Bingham et al., 1997). The cognitive deficits demonstrated in DS22q11.2 include an overall delay in cognitive, psychomotor and language development and an overall IQ in the range of (Gerdes et al., 1999; Moss et al., 1999; Swillen, Vogels, Devriendt & Fryns, 2000; Wang, Woodin, Kreps-Falk & Moss, 2000). In addition to the cognitive deficits, children with DS22q11.2 show an extremely high incidence of psychopathology, especially schizophrenia, as they reach adulthood. It is estimated that around 30% of all children with DS22q11.2 will develop a schizophrenia spectrum disorder (Feinstein, Eliez, Blasey & Reiss, 2002). Deficits in the areas of visuospatial and numerical performance have also been recently reported (Bearden et al., 2001; Simon, Bearden, McDonald-McGinn & Zackai, 2005). Using common neuropsychological tests, Bearden et al. (2001) showed selective deficits in visualspatial memory, with relative preservation of verbal functioning and object memory. Additionally, recent reports of executive dysfunction in this syndrome (Woodin et al., 2001; Sobin et al., 2004) may provide a link between behavioral performance on neuropsychological and cognitive tests and the development of psychopathology. Relationships between the catechol o-methyltransferase (COMT) genotype, a gene directly affected in DS22q11.2 by virtue of the deletion of one copy, and executive function have been reported (Bearden et al., in press) and may help to determine the course of the development of disinhibitory pathologies (schizophrenia, ADHD) in certain subsets of this population. Interestingly, the COMT genotype has also been directly associated with executive functions as implemented in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of schizophrenics (Egan et al., 2001). Our current research is aimed at extending the domain specific findings of visuo-spatial and executive dysfunction Address for correspondence: Dr Joel P. Bish, Children s Hospital of Philadelphia, 3535 Market Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA; bish@ .chop.edu, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA.

2 Attentional dysfunction and 22q in this population. Our method is to use cognitive processing tasks designed to directly interrogate the neural structures underlying the functional deficits shown in this group. We recently reported deficits in spatial orienting, enumeration and magnitude comparison in children with DS22q11.2 (Simon et al., in press). These tasks were selected because each would be predicted to systematically involve a characteristic pattern of performance that is dependent on the posterior parietal lobe (PPL) to a greater or lesser extent. The results would then enable us to evaluate our hypothesis that PPL dysfunction is a major cause of the visuospatial and numerical cognitive deficits seen in DS22q11.2. The children with DS22q11.2 showed systematic difficulties in all three tasks, demonstrating a graded performance based on the amount of involvement of the posterior parietal lobe. The purpose of the present study is to further investigate the nature of executive control dysfunction manifested by children with DS22q11.2 with hopes of providing potential behavioral markers of the development of psychopathology. For example, affected children with increased executive control dysfunctions in addition to other factors may be those that have an increased risk for psychopathology later in life. Posner and Petersen (1990) originally proposed that attentional function could be segregated into the sub-functions of alerting, orienting and executive control. Alerting is characterized as maintaining an alert state over time and has been associated with the norepinephrine system of the right frontal and parietal lobes (Coull, Frith, Frackowiak & Grasby, 1996). Orienting refers to the ability to use spatial cues to selectively attend to a particular location in the visual field and has been associated with activity in the superior parietal lobule, the temporal parietal junction (e.g. Corbetta, Kincade, Ollinger, McAvoy & Shulman, 2000), frontal eye fields (e.g. Hopfinger, Buonocore & Mangun, 2000), superior colliculus (e.g. Mesulam, 1981), and the pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus (e.g. Petersen, Robinson & Morris, 1987). Executive control includes the ability to monitor and adapt to conflict within the stimulus response environment through inhibition and involves functional activation of the anterior cingulate and the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex (Bush, Luu & Posner, 2000; MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger & Carter, 2000; Botvinick, Nystrom, Fissell, Carter & Cohen, 1999). Operation of this system has been associated with dopaminergic genes such as COMT (Fosella et al., 2002), one copy of which is deleted in individuals with DS22q11.2. In order to investigate the executive control network and its interaction with the attentional orienting system in children with DS22q11.2, we used an adaptation of the Attentional Network Test (ANT) that had previously been altered for use with children (Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz & Posner, 2002). The ANT is an elegantly designed, simple task that independently tests the three previously mentioned attentional functions. The ANT is a combination of a spatial cueing response time task (Posner, 1980) and a flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974) and requires participants to identify the directional heading (left or right) of a specific target. In our version of the task, participants are presented with four spatial cues types (none, valid, neutral and invalid) that may or may not draw attention to the spatial location of a subsequently appearing target. The comparison of trials with neutral and no cues provides an index of the alerting network. This is because trials including a neutral cue should provide an alerting benefit compared to trials without a cue in which the target itself produces an alerting response. Comparison of trials with valid spatial cues and those with neutral cues provides an index of the ability to use spatial cues to orient attention to the appropriate location in space prior to the onset of the target. The orienting network can also be evaluated through the comparison of invalid versus neutral or valid cues. The addition of the invalid cue condition in our version of the task was intended to enable further investigation of our previous findings. Those data demonstrated selective deficits in children with DS22q11.2 in disengaging attentional resources from an inappropriately cued location (Simon et al., 2005). The ANT also includes three flanker conditions that evaluate the executive network previously described. The flanker conditions refer to the presence and type of irrelevant flankers on both sides of the target stimulus. The target can be accompanied by no flankers (i.e. appear by itself ), by congruent flankers (i.e. ones pointing in the same direction as the target), or by incongruent flankers (i.e. ones pointing in the opposite direction to the target). Incongruent flankers require greater inhibition of irrelevant information than do congruent flankers. It has been shown that individuals with executive control deficits experience increased difficulty in processing incongruent flankers compared to congruent flankers (Crone, Jennings & van der Molen, 2003). Based on previous findings of executive function deficits (Woodin et al., 2001; Bearden et al., in press; Sobin et al., 2004), we hypothesized that children with DS22q11.2 would demonstrate dysfunctional executive control. By comparing conditions with congruent versus incongruent flankers, we could evaluate a child s ability to focus attention on a specific target location without suffering the adverse effects of processing the conflicting flankers. We could also measure dynamic conflict monitoring and adaptation by examining the Gratton effect (Gratton, Coles & Donchin, 1992). The Gratton effect describes a pattern of performance in which processing

3 38 Joel P. Bish et al. of congruent trials improves when preceded by congruent trials relative to when preceded by incongruent trials. Additionally, performance on incongruent trials is improved when preceded by an incongruent trial relative to when preceded by a congruent trial. These findings are interpreted to indicate that the previous trial has set up the conflict context for the following trial. Specifically, the anterior cingulate is postulated to continuously monitor for conflict in the environment and invoke the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex to inhibit non-target stimuli when conflict is present (Botvinick et al., 1999). Additionally, the posterior parietal lobe is presumed to participate in altering the focus of spatial attention (Casey et al., 2000). In addition to our predictions regarding executive function, we hypothesized that children with DS22q11.2 would demonstrate difficulties with the cueing portion of the task, specifically the invalid cue condition, because of its reliance on the posterior parietal lobe. Since we know of no relevant data, we made no specific predictions based on the alerting aspect of the ANT. Method Participants Thirty-four children completed the ANT. Of the 34, 16 were typically developing children and 18 were children diagnosed with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Diagnosis was confirmed through the use of fluorescence in situ hybridization (FISH) test. The typically developing children had a mean age of 9 years, 7 months (SD = 1.8 years, range = 7 14 years) and consisted of seven females and nine males. The children with DS22q11.2 had a mean age of 9 years, 2 months (SD = 1.7 years, range = 7 14 years) and consisted of 12 females and six males. Design and procedure The 34 participants in this study were administered an adaptation of the ANT as part of a large battery of cognitive and neuropsychological tests. In our version of the child-based ANT (see Fan et al., 2002, for original task description), the child was required to locate and identify the orientation (left or right) of a friendly alien spaceship. The target was a small alien pictured in a spaceship on the side of which an arrow pointed in the same direction as the spaceship. The children were instructed to focus only on the central spaceship, which appeared immediately above or immediately below the fixation cross. When the target appeared (with or without flankers) the child s task was use the button box provided and to push the left button if the spaceship pointed left and the right button if it pointed right. The targets were presented within the context of a total of 12 possible trial types integrating four cue types and three flanker types. The specific details of trial timing, order and stimulus size in our version of the ANT were similar to the previously published child version (Fan et al., 2002). On each trial, the participant was presented with a central fixation cross for between 400 and 1600 ms in steps of 200 ms. Following this duration, a blank screen was presented for 50 ms followed by one of four cue types (none, valid, neutral, or invalid). The cue remained on the screen for 400 ms followed by another blank screen for 100 ms. Following this, the target was presented for 3000 ms or until the participant responded. Targets were presented 1.06 degrees of visual angle above or below the central fixation point at equal probabilities. For the No Cue condition, the fixation, in the absence of a cue, remained on the screen until the presentation of the target. In the valid Cue condition, an asterisk, subtending approximately 1 degree of visual angle, was presented in the exact location of the subsequently appearing target, either above or below the fixation cross. In the Neutral Cue condition, an asterisk was presented in the central location of the fixation cross, thereby providing no useful spatial information regarding the future location of the target. In the Invalid Cue condition, which had been added in our version of the task, the asterisk was presented in the opposite target location to the following target (i.e. cue is above fixation and target appears below fixation). The Valid Cue condition occurred on 75% of the trials with a spatially useful cue (i.e. valid or invalid but not neutral). The targets were presented either without flankers (single) or with flankers (congruent or incongruent). The flankers were identical to the target stimulus and in the Congruent condition pointed in the same direction as the target and in the Incongruent condition pointed in the opposite direction of the target. The three flanker types were equally distributed across all cue types. Prior to the experimental trials, each participant was presented with 12 demonstration trials, followed by a 24-trial practice block. A total of 144 experimental trials were randomly presented and lasted no more than 10 minutes. The child was given a short rest midway through the task. Results Data analysis was focused on the overall pattern of results via evaluation of response time for each condi-

4 Attentional dysfunction and 22q tion, the original ANT network indices (e.g. Fan et al., 2002), and the specific evaluation of the Gratton effect in children with DS22q11.2. The mean and standard deviation response time (RT) and mean percentage errors were calculated for each condition and are presented in Table 1. Since we were interested in examining potential executive control differences in children with DS22q11.2, and since systematically elevated error rates are expected with executive dysfunction, we evaluated performance on this task with a combination of response time and error rates. We first applied accuracy exclusion criteria in which any subjects performing at or below chance (50%) on any of the trial conditions were excluded. These exclusion criteria forced the removal of one individual with DS22q11.2 from the analyses (52% inaccuracy for incongruent flankers trials). We then combined the RT and error rates using the formula RT/(1-%error). This adjustment is recommended for its consistencies with traditional views of RT (Townsend & Ashby, 1983), such as controlling for any speed/accuracy trade-offs and has been used to examine spatial cueing and executive control in children in the past (Akhtar & Enns, 1989). We felt it would be particularly appropriate to use when examining the performance of children with DS22q11.2 since they would be expected to make errors. Excluding all trials with errors would likely skew the performance of such children away from a representative characterization of their performance. Using this adjustment, RT remains unchanged with 100% accuracy and is increased in proportion with the number of errors. The adjusted RTs were then evaluated for equality of variance across groups using Levene s test for equality of error variances. Given the significant ( p =.05) increase in variability within the DS22q11.2 group across a number of conditions, we then transformed the adjusted RT s using the square root for all conditions and retested them with Levene s test of equality of variances. At that point the data met the assumption of equality of variances between groups for all conditions. The transformed adjusted RT s were then submitted to a repeated-measures ANOVA with cue type and flanker type as within-subjects factors and group as a between-subjects factor. The main effect of cue type was significant, as expected, F(3, 29) = , p <.001, power (.05) = 1.00, with the valid cue producing the most efficient performance across groups and flanker types (Figure 1). The main effect of flanker was also significant, F(2, 30) = , p <.001, power (.05) =.996, with the incongruent flankers producing slower performance for the combined groups and cue types (Figure 2). The effect of group was statistically significant, F(1, 31) = 4.972, p =.033, power (.05) =.579, indicating that children with DS22q11.2 were less efficient across all conditions. The flanker group interaction was significant, F(2, 30) = 3.508, p =.043, power (.05) =.610, and inspection of the simple effects revealed that the children with DS22q11.2 had more difficulty (p =.05) on trials with incongruent flankers relative to single and congruent trials when compared to typically developing children (Figure 2). Neither the cue type group nor the cue type flanker group interactions were significant at the.05 level. These results show a dysfunction in children with DS22q11.2 related specifically to conditions in which incongruent, distracting information is presented, and they replicate the findings of Sobin et al. (2004). The single indices for the various attentional domains (e.g. Fosella et al., 2002) were also evaluated, but only the executive index was near significance. The alerting index is computed by subtracting the RT of the neutral cue condition from the no cue condition and was not significantly different between groups t(31) =.224, p =.824. For children with DS22q11.2, the mean difference was with a standard deviation (SD) of and for the typically developing children, the mean difference was (SD = 54.66). The orienting index is computed by subtracting the RT of the valid cue condition Table 1 (a) Cueing effect mean adjusted response time (standard deviation) and mean error rates for controls and DS22q11.2 (b) Flanker effect mean adjusted response time (standard deviation) and mean error rates for controls and DS22q11.2 (a) Cue type RT (SD) % errors None Valid Neutral Invalid DS22q (377.05) 10.17% (357.04) 9.76% (323.64) 9.61% (412.63) 9.63% NC (170.81) 2.75% (127.83) 1.74% (144.88) 2.92% (145.65) 1.04% (b) Flanker RT (SD) % errors Single Congruent Incongruent DS22q (361.11) 7.97% (266.15) 6.33% (491.23) 15.09% NC (132.72) 2.35% (138.89) 1.87% (167.96) 2.44%

5 40 Joel P. Bish et al. Figure 1 Cueing effect mean adjusted response time (in ms). Adjustment = RT/(1-% error). Figure 2 Flanker effect mean adjusted response time (in ms). Adjustment = RT/(1-% error). Figure 3 Gratton effect mean unadjusted response time (in ms). CC = congruent preceded by congruent. IC = congruent preceded by incongruent. CI = incongruent preceded by congruent. II = incongruent preceded by incongruent. from the RT of the neutral cue condition and was not significant between groups t(31) =.398, p =.693. Children with DS22q11.2 had a mean difference of (SD = ) while typically developing children had a mean difference of (SD = 45.93). The executive index is calculated by subtracting the congruent flanker condition from the incongruent flanker condition and was almost significant between groups t(31) = 2.008, p =.053. The mean difference for the children with DS22q11.2 was (SD = ) while the mean difference for controls was (SD = 54.42). Again, these results support the findings of Sobin et al. (2004) in demonstrating marked deficits in the functioning of the executive control network in children with DS22q11.2. To investigate the hypothesis that children with DS22q11.2 have specific difficulties with conflict monitoring, we further evaluated the flanker condition means by separating them into more specific conditions. To investigate whether children with DS22q11.2 demonstrate a typical Gratton effect, we calculated group means for congruent trials preceded by congruent and incongruent trials. We also calculated group means for incongruent trials preceded by congruent and incongruent trials separately. We then subtracted the mean of congruent congruent (CC) trials from the mean of incongruent congruent (IC) trials as well as the mean of incongruent incongruent (II) trials from congruent incongruent (CI) trials. Finally, these difference scores were compared between groups. Analyses revealed that children with DS22q11.2 demonstrate a normal Gratton effect for congruent trials (IC CC), t(31) =.1.018, p =.318, but not for incongruent trials (CI II), t(31) = 2.094, p =.044 (Figure 3). That is, when an incongruent trial follows an incongruent trial, processing for the children with DS22q11.2 is handicapped even further and reaction time increases. This is the opposite pattern to that seen in typically developing children. The increase in response time to II trials relative to CI trials in children with DS22q11.2 thus reflects the inability to efficiently alter attentional resources based on previous environmental context. This specific inefficiency, as well as the more general differences revealed by the results of the ANOVA and the executive network index, provides direct evidence of executive dysfunction in children with DS22q11.2. Discussion This study demonstrates clear executive function deficits in children with DS22q11.2. Replicating and extending the findings of Sobin et al. (2004), the results indicate

6 Attentional dysfunction and 22q that, in addition to the general spatial attention deficits seen in our previous studies (Bearden et al., 2001; Simon et al., 2005), children with DS22q11.2 have both generalized executive control dysfunction and increased difficulty in dynamically changing the focus of attention to inhibit the extraneous processing of irrelevant stimuli in their environment. Based on previous findings, (Botvinick et al., 1999) one interpretation of these results is that they arise from dysfunction of the executive control network, which depends upon the anterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. An intriguing alternative interpretation is that the dysfunction actually arises from an inability to fine-tune or engage attention to a specific target location in the presence of distracters (e.g. Casey et al., 2000). This competence depends upon the posterior parietal system, which has already been shown to be dysfunctional under other circumstances (Simon et al., 2005). The deficits in performance are demonstrated in a number of ways in this data. Most obviously, the inefficient processing (both slowed RT and increased error rates) for incongruent trials compared to both single and congruent trials indicates that children with DS22q11.2 suffer from distinct difficulty with conflicting information when visually presented close to the target stimulus. Additionally, a trend towards an improvement for congruent flankers in children with DS22q11.2 suggests that all stimuli (conflicting or not) appear to be encoded and may be influencing their efficiency at processing and responding to targets. Specifically, the trend toward a response time benefit shown for congruent stimuli compared to a single stimuli suggests that children with DS22q11.2 are directly influenced by all irrelevant stimuli in the immediate vicinity of the target, providing further evidence for the inability to dynamically alter their attentional focus. Finally, and perhaps most importantly, evaluation of the Gratton effect indicates that children with DS22q11.2 appear to have particular difficulties in the dynamic monitoring of conflict in their environment. Their performance suggests that children with DS22q11.2 are unable to use the ongoing context of conflicting information to modify their attentional focus. This was apparent in the case of repeated incongruent flanker trials. Control children were able to benefit from the identical attentional demands of the adjacent trials, while the reoccurrence of the incongruent flankers had the reverse effect of increasing the inhibitory load for children with DS22q11.2. This deficit may be a result of difficulties with stimulus response conflict monitoring which is a function of the anterior cingulate (Botvinick et al., 1999). It may also be a result of difficulties with adapting to the existing conflict through inhibitory processes, which is a function of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Finally, the problem could result from an interaction between the frontal executive network and the ability to dynamically shift the focus of attention in space, which results from posterior parietal functioning (Casey et al., 2000). Hypothetically, the conflict monitoring and inhibition process of the frontal executive network may be intact, but the mechanism to shift attention away from irrelevant stimuli may be dysfunctional. Interestingly, the results of this study did not replicate our previous findings of a relative inefficiency for target processing in invalidly cued locations compared to validly cued locations (Simon et al., 2005). One possible explanation of this failure to replicate the previous finding is the nature of the spatial cue. Our previous data used an endogenous, centrally located cue that requires that the individual participant interpret the symbolic meaning of the cue. This entails top-down evaluation prior to shifting the attentional focus. The current experiment used an exogenous cue, presented at the actual location of the target. Such cues automatically elicit the focus of attention to the cued location without the need for top-down interpretive processing (e.g. McCormick, 1997). This difference, along with the current demonstration of dysfunction of the executive network, may also indicate that children with DS22q11.2 have specific difficulties with the interaction of the top-down attentional networks with visuo-spatial processing, that is, orienting and executive control. Given the growing knowledge of the relationships between genetic disorders, executive dysfunction and the development of psychopathology, this investigation of the attentional networks in children with DS22q11.2 has potentially important clinical applications. As already mentioned, there is a strong link between DS22q11.2 and the development of psychopathology, especially schizophrenia. Individuals with schizophrenia commonly demonstrate executive control dysfunction of the sort reported here. Additionally, there is a strong relationship between executive dysfunction and COMT genotype in DS22q11.2. This study provides direct evidence of executive control dysfunction in children with DS22q11.2. In doing so, it notably replicates and extends earlier results (e.g. Sobin et al., 2004), but it may also indicate that executive control (i.e. functioning of the anterior executive control network), or the interaction between executive control and the spatial attention system (i.e. functioning of the posterior attention network) might act as potential biomarkers for the development of psychopathology in this group. For example, early risk factors for the development of psychopathology in this group may include the executive dysfunction demonstrated here. Based on the limited cross-sectional nature of this study, it is impossible to determine whether the demonstrated

7 42 Joel P. Bish et al. executive dysfunction continues into adulthood and contributes to psychopathology, or simply that children with DS22q11.2 exhibit a slower trajectory towards normally developed executive control. Indeed, in typically developing children it appears that prefrontal activation in response to cognitive control continues to develop into adolescence (Bunge, Dudukovic, Thomason, Vaidya & Gabrieli, 2002). However, our ongoing studies are attempting to address that issue. Finally, further studies, perhaps using functional neuroimaging, are necessary to determine which specific interpretation mentioned above is more accurate. Specifically, future investigations should be able to clarify whether the dysfunction demonstrated by children with DS22q11.2 is caused by disruption of the frontal, executive system, the parietal orienting system, or a combination of the two. It is clear that investigations of specific cognitive functions, like those shown here and in Sobin et al. (2004), will help to develop a characterization of the intricate deficits demonstrated in this population and facilitate progress towards the development of early screening and intervention techniques designed to improve long-term outcomes. References Akhtar, N., & Enns, J.T. (1989). Relations between covert orienting and filtering in the development of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, Bearden, C.E., Jawad, A.F., Lynch, D.R., Sokol, S., Kanes, S.J., McDonald-McGinn, D.M., Saitta, S.C., Harris, S.E., Moss, E., Wang, P.P., Zackai, E., Emanuel, B., & Simon, T.J. (in press). Effects of a functional polymorphism on prefrontal cognitive function in the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. American Journal of Psychiatry. Bearden, C.E., Woodin, M.F., Wang, P.P., Moss, E., McDonald- McGinn, D., Zackai, E., Emanuel, B., & Cannon, T.D. (2001). The neurocognitive phenotype of the DS22q deletion syndrome: selective deficits in visuo-spatial memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, Bingham, P., Zimmerman, R., McDonald-McGinn, D.M., Driscoll, D.A., Emanuel, B.S., & Zackai, E.H. (1997). Enlarged sylvian fissures in infants with interstitial deletion of chromosome 22q11.2. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 74, Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L.E., Fissell, K., Carter, C.S., & Cohen, J.D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selectionfor-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature, 402, Bunge, S.A., Dudukovic, N.M., Thomason, M.E., Vaidya, C.J., & Gabrieli, J.D. (2002). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: evidence from fmri. Neuron, 33, Burn, J., & Goodship, J. (1996). Developmental genetics of the heart. Current Opinion Genetic Development, 6, Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M.I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in the anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, Casey, B.J., Thomas, K.M., Welsh, T.F., Badgaiyan, R.D., Eccard, C.H., Jennings, J.R., & Crone, E.A. (2000). Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, Corbetta, M., Kincade, J.M., Ollinger, J.M., McAvoy, M.P., & Shulman, G. (2000). Voluntary control is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 3, Coull, J.T., Frith, C.D., Frackowiak, R.S.J., & Grasby, P.M. (1996). A fronto-parietal network of rapid visual information processing: a pet study of sustained attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia, 34, Crone, E.A., Jennings, J.R., & van der Molen, M.W. (2003). Sensitivity to interference and response contingencies in attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, DiGeorge, A. (1965). A new concept of the cellular basis of immunity. Journal of Pediatrics, 67, 907. Driscoll, D.A., Salvin, J., Sellinger, B., Budarf, M.L., McDonald- McGinn, D.M., Zackai, E.H., & Emanuel, B. (1993). Prevalence of 22q11 microdeletions in DiGeorge and velocardiofacial syndromes: implications for genetic counseling and prenatal diagnosis. Journal of Medical Genetics, 30, Dunham, I., Shimizu, N., Roe, A.M., & Chissoe, S., et al. (1999). The DNA sequence of human chromosome 22. Nature, 402, Egan, M.F., Goldberg, T.E., Kolachana, B.S., Callicott, J.H., Mazzanti, C.M., Straub, R.E., Goldman, D., & Weinberger, D.R. (2001). Effect of COMT Val 108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, Eliez, S., Schmitt, J.E., White, C.D., & Reiss, A.L. (2000). Children and adolescents with velocardiofacial syndrome: a volumetric MRI study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, Eriksen, B.A., & Eriksen, C.W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, Fan, J., McCandliss, B.D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M.I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, Feinstein, C., Eliez, S., Blasey, C., & Reiss, A.L. (2002). Psychiatric disorders and behavioral problems in children with velocardiofacial syndrome: usefulness as phenotypic indicators of schizophrenia risk. Biological Psychiatry, 15, Fosella, J., Sommer, T., Fan, J., Wu, Y., Swanson, J.M., Pfaff, D.W., & Posner, M.I. (2002). Assessing the molecular genetics of attention networks. BMC Neuroscience, 3, Gerdes, M., Solot, C.B., Wang, P.P., Moss, E.M., LaRossa, D., Randall, P., Goldmuntz, E., Clark, B.J., Driscoll, D.A.,

8 Attentional dysfunction and 22q Jawad, A., Emanuel, B.S., McDonald-McGinn, D.M., Batshaw, M.L., & Zackai, E.H. (1999). Cognitive and behavior profile of preschool children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 85, Gratton, G., Coles, M.G.H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, Greenwood, P.M., & Parasuraman, R. (2003). Normal genetic variation, cognition, and aging. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 2, Hopfinger, J.B., Buonocore, M.H., & Mangun, G.R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3, Kates, W.R., Burnette, C.P., Jabs, E.W., Rutberg, J., Murphy, A.M., Grados, M., Geraghty, M., Kaufman, W.E., & Pearlson, G.D. (2001). Regional cortical white matter reductions in velocardiofacial syndrome: a volumetric MRI analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 49, MacDonald, A.W., Cohen, J.D., Stenger, V.A., & Carter, C.S. (2000). Dissociating the role of dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, McCormick, P.A. (1997). Orienting attention without awareness. Journal of Experimetnal Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 23, Mesulam, M.M. (1981). A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Annals of Neurology, 10, Moss, E.M., Batshaw, M.L., Solot, C.B., Gerdes, M., McDonald- McGinn, D.M., Driscoll, D.A., Emanuel, B.S., Zackai, E.H., & Wang, P.P. (1999). Psychoeducational profile of the 22q11.2 microdeletion: a complex pattern. The Journal of Pediatrics, 134, Petersen, S.E., Robinson, D.L., & Morris, J.D. (1987). Contributions of the pulvinar to visual spatial attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, Posner, M.I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, Posner, M.I., & Petersen, S.E. (1990). The attention systems of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, Shprintzen, R.J., Goldberg, R.B., Lewin, M.L., Sidotti, E.J., Berkman, M.D., Argamaso, R.V., & Young, D. (1978). A new syndrome involving cleft palate, cardiac anomalies, typical faces, and learning disabilities: velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Cleft Palate Journal, 15, Simon, T.J., Bearden, C.E., McDonald-McGinn, D.M., & Zackai, E.H. (2005). Visuospatial and numerical cognitive deficits in chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Cortex, 41, Sobin, C., Kiley-Brabeck, K., Daniels, S., Blundell, M., Anyane- Yeboa, K., & Karayiorgou, M. (2004). Networks of attention in children with the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, Swillen, A., Vogels, A., Devriendt, K., & Fryns, J.P. (2000). Chromosome 22q11 deletion syndrome: update and review of the clinical features, cognitive-behavioral spectrum, and psychiatric complications. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Semin Med Genet), 97, Townsend, J.T., & Ashby, F.G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. New York: Cambridge University Press. Wang, P.P., Woodin, M.F., Kreps-Falk, R., & Moss, E.M. (2000). Research on behavioral phenotypes: velocardiofacial syndrome (deletion 22q11.2). Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 42, Woodin, M., Wang, P.P., Aleman, D., McDonald-McGinn, D., Zackai, E., & Moss, E. (2001). Neuropsychological profile of children and adolescents with the 22q11.2 microdeletion. Genetics in Medicine, 3, Received: 1 April 2004 Accepted: 3 August 2004

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

Attentional Phenotypes for the Analysis of Higher Mental Function

Attentional Phenotypes for the Analysis of Higher Mental Function Short Communication TheScientificWorldJOURNAL (2002) 2, 217 223 ISSN 1537-744X; DOI 10.1100/tsw.2002.93 Attentional Phenotypes for the Analysis of Higher Mental Function John Fossella 1, Michael I. Posner,1,

More information

Selective impairment of attentional networks of orienting and executive control in schizophrenia

Selective impairment of attentional networks of orienting and executive control in schizophrenia Schizophrenia Research 78 (2005) 235 241 www.elsevier.com/locate/schres Selective impairment of attentional networks of orienting and executive control in schizophrenia Kai Wang a, Jin Fan b, T, Yi Dong

More information

Intellectual abilities in a large sample of children with Velo Cardio Facial Syndrome: an update

Intellectual abilities in a large sample of children with Velo Cardio Facial Syndrome: an update 666 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 51 part 9 pp 666 670 september 2007 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.00955.x Intellectual abilities in a large sample of children with Velo Cardio Facial

More information

Common and specific impairments in attention functioning in girls with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion, fragile X or Turner syndromes.

Common and specific impairments in attention functioning in girls with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion, fragile X or Turner syndromes. Thomas Jefferson University Jefferson Digital Commons Department of Pediatrics Faculty Papers Department of Pediatrics 3-14-2014 Common and specific impairments in attention functioning in girls with chromosome

More information

Atypical development of the executive attention network in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome

Atypical development of the executive attention network in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome J Neurodevelop Disord (2011) 3:76 85 DOI 10.1007/s11689-010-9070-3 Atypical development of the executive attention network in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome Joel Stoddard & Laurel Beckett

More information

Attentional Uncertainty in the Stroop Priming Task

Attentional Uncertainty in the Stroop Priming Task Western Kentucky University TopSCHOLAR Masters Theses & Specialist Projects Graduate School 5-2009 Attentional Uncertainty in the Stroop Priming Task Brandy Nicole Johnson Western Kentucky University,

More information

Parametric manipulation of conflict and response competition using rapid mixed-trial event-related fmri

Parametric manipulation of conflict and response competition using rapid mixed-trial event-related fmri NeuroImage 20 (2003) 2135 2141 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Parametric manipulation of conflict and response competition using rapid mixed-trial event-related fmri S. Durston, a,b, * M.C. Davidson, a

More information

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Hillyard(1973) Dichotic Listening Task N1 component enhanced for attended stimuli Supports early selection Effects of Voluntary

More information

The Multi-Source Interference Task: The Effect of Randomization

The Multi-Source Interference Task: The Effect of Randomization Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27:711 717, 2005 Copyright Taylor & Francis Ltd. ISSN: 1380-3395 DOI: 10.1080/13803390490918516 NCEN 1380-3395 Journal of Clinical and Experimental

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Overlapping numerical cognition impairments in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion or Turner syndromes

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Overlapping numerical cognition impairments in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion or Turner syndromes Neuropsychologia xxx (2007) xxx xxx Overlapping numerical cognition impairments in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion or Turner syndromes T.J. Simon a,, Y. Takarae a,1, T. DeBoer a,1, D.M. McDonald-McGinn

More information

Key questions about attention

Key questions about attention Key questions about attention How does attention affect behavioral performance? Can attention affect the appearance of things? How does spatial and feature-based attention affect neuronal responses in

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch. The Frontal Lobes Readings: KW Ch. 16 Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions E.L. Highly organized college professor Became disorganized, showed little emotion, and began to miss deadlines Scores on intelligence

More information

Cognitive correlates of a functional COMT polymorphism in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome

Cognitive correlates of a functional COMT polymorphism in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome Cognitive correlates of a functional COMT polymorphism in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome By: V Shashi, MS Keshavan, TD Howard, MN Berry, MJ Basehore, E Lewandowski and TR Kwapil Shashi, V., Keshavan,

More information

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach Attentional Control 1 Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach Barry Giesbrecht & George R. Mangun Center for Mind & Brain University of California, Davis

More information

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso Control of visuo-spatial attention Emiliano Macaluso CB demo Attention Limited processing resources Overwhelming sensory input cannot be fully processed => SELECTIVE PROCESSING Selection via spatial orienting

More information

Social-Emotional Development in 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome: Psychiatric Risk Factors

Social-Emotional Development in 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome: Psychiatric Risk Factors Social-Emotional Development in 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome: Psychiatric Risk Factors Presentation Outline Behavioral, temperament and psychiatric features Autistic Spectrum Disorders in 22qDS? Cognitive

More information

Cognition. Post-error slowing: An orienting account. Wim Notebaert a, *, Femke Houtman a, Filip Van Opstal a, Wim Gevers b, Wim Fias a, Tom Verguts a

Cognition. Post-error slowing: An orienting account. Wim Notebaert a, *, Femke Houtman a, Filip Van Opstal a, Wim Gevers b, Wim Fias a, Tom Verguts a Cognition 111 (2009) 275 279 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Post-error slowing: An orienting account Wim Notebaert a,

More information

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) Introduction 17 subjects were tested to measure effects of a Brain.fm focus session on cognition. With 4 additional subjects, we recorded EEG data during baseline and while

More information

Developmental trajectories in cognitive development in 22q11 deletion syndrome

Developmental trajectories in cognitive development in 22q11 deletion syndrome Developmental trajectories in cognitive development in 22q11 deletion syndrome Ann Swillen, Ph.D. Center for Human Genetics UZ Leuven & KU Leuven, Belgium Cultural background Parenting Maturation School,

More information

Progress in Attention research

Progress in Attention research This is a chapter excerpt from Guilford Publications. Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, Second Edition. Edited by Michael I. Posner. Copyright 2012. Purchase this book now: www.guilford.com/p/posner

More information

Sensitivity of certain standardised tests to executive attention functioning in seven-year-old children

Sensitivity of certain standardised tests to executive attention functioning in seven-year-old children ELECTRONIC JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY No. 1 (2) 2003. ISSN: 1696-2095 Sensitivity of certain standardised tests to executive attention functioning in seven-year-old children Luis J.

More information

Measuring attention in the hemispheres: The lateralized attention network test (LANT)

Measuring attention in the hemispheres: The lateralized attention network test (LANT) Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Brain and Cognition 66 (2008) 21 31 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Measuring attention in the hemispheres: The lateralized attention network test (LANT) Deanna J.

More information

Computational Modelling of Deficits in Attentional Networks in mild Traumatic Brain Injury: An Application in Neuropsychology

Computational Modelling of Deficits in Attentional Networks in mild Traumatic Brain Injury: An Application in Neuropsychology Computational Modelling of Deficits in Attentional Networks in mild Traumatic Brain Injury: An Application in Neuropsychology Fehmida Hussain (f.hussain@sussex.ac.uk) Representation and Cognition Group,

More information

22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome: Are Motor Deficits More Than Expected for IQ Level?

22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome: Are Motor Deficits More Than Expected for IQ Level? Sacred Heart University DigitalCommons@SHU Speech-Language Pathology Faculty Publications Speech-Language Pathology 10-2010 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome: Are Motor Deficits More Than Expected for IQ Level?

More information

Language skills in children with velocardiofacial

Language skills in children with velocardiofacial Language skills in children with velocardiofacial syndrome (deletion 22q11.2) Bronwyn Glaser, BA, Donna L. Mumme, PhD, Christine Blasey, PhD, Michael A. Morris, PhD, Sophie P. Dahoun, MD, Stylianos E.

More information

Attention and Scene Perception

Attention and Scene Perception Theories of attention Techniques for studying scene perception Physiological basis of attention Attention and single cells Disorders of attention Scene recognition attention any of a large set of selection

More information

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation. REVIEW OF FRONTAL LOBE STRUCTURES Main organization of frontal cortex: 1. Motor area (precentral gyrus). 2. Premotor & supplementary motor areas (immediately anterior to motor area). Includes premotor,

More information

Altered time course of unconscious response priming in schizophrenia patients

Altered time course of unconscious response priming in schizophrenia patients Schizophrenia Research 150: 590-1 (2013) Altered time course of unconscious response priming in schizophrenia patients Markus Kiefer 1, Ph.D., Anna Morschett 1, M.D., Carlos Schönfeldt-Lecuona 1, M.D.,

More information

Motivation Sharpens Exogenous Spatial Attention

Motivation Sharpens Exogenous Spatial Attention CORRECTED AUGUST 3, 2007; SEE LAST PAGE Emotion Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 7, No. 3, 668 674 1528-3542/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.668 BRIEF REPORTS Motivation

More information

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism Justin H G Williams Morven McWhirr Gordon D Waiter Cambridge Sept 10 th 2010 Joint attention in autism Declarative and receptive aspects initiating

More information

Exploring the relationship between high level anomia, attention and cognitive processing deficits: a retrospective data analysis

Exploring the relationship between high level anomia, attention and cognitive processing deficits: a retrospective data analysis Exploring the relationship between high level anomia, attention and cognitive processing deficits: a retrospective data analysis INTRODUCTION Since stroke survivors with high level anomia often score within

More information

Biological Risk Factors

Biological Risk Factors Biological Risk Factors Ms Angelina Crea Provisional Psychologist Academic Child Psychiatry Unit Royal Children s Hospital Professor Alasdair Vance Head Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics

More information

Made you look! Consciously perceived, irrelevant instructional cues can hijack the. attentional network

Made you look! Consciously perceived, irrelevant instructional cues can hijack the. attentional network Made you look! Consciously perceived, irrelevant instructional cues can hijack the attentional network Katherine Sledge Moore, Clare B. Porter, and Daniel H. Weissman Department of Psychology, University

More information

2/27/2014. What is Attention? What is Attention? Space- and Object-Based Attention

2/27/2014. What is Attention? What is Attention? Space- and Object-Based Attention Space- and Object-Based Attention Attention 1 What is Attention? William James Principles of Psychology Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession by the mind in clear and vivid form,

More information

Temporal Attention Processing in Individuals with Chromosome 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome

Temporal Attention Processing in Individuals with Chromosome 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome Syracuse University SURFACE Theses - ALL May 2018 Temporal Attention Processing in Individuals with Chromosome 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome Justin Kopec Syracuse University Follow this and additional works

More information

The effects of unilateral pulvinar damage in humans on reflexive orienting and filtering of irrelevant information

The effects of unilateral pulvinar damage in humans on reflexive orienting and filtering of irrelevant information Behavioural Neurology 13 (2001/2002) 95 104 95 IOS Press The effects of unilateral pulvinar damage in humans on reflexive orienting and filtering of irrelevant information Shai Danziger a,, Robert Ward

More information

Development of attentional networks: An fmri study with children and adults

Development of attentional networks: An fmri study with children and adults www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 28 (2005) 429 439 Development of attentional networks: An fmri study with children and adults Kerstin Konrad, a, * Susanne Neufang, b,c Christiane M. Thiel, b,c

More information

Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship to trait anxiety

Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship to trait anxiety brain research 1481 (2012) 13 36 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship

More information

Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task

Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task 2007 volume 3 no 3 409-417 Advances in Cognitive Psychology Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task John F. Stins 1,2,

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective MILITARY PSYCHOLOGY, 21:(Suppl. 1)S108 S112, 2009 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0899-5605 print / 1532-7876 online DOI: 10.1080/08995600802554748 Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future:

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp 1288-1290 Improved executive functioning following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation [Brief Communications] Moser,

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

The Neurobiology of Attention

The Neurobiology of Attention The Neurobiology of Attention by Nadia Fike, MD/PhD Pediatric Neurology Center for Neurosciences Disclosures Nadia Fike, MD/PhD No relevant financial or nonfinancial relationships to disclose. Objectives

More information

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load Psychonomic Bulletin & Review In press Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load JOSHUA D. COSMAN AND SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Attentional capture by abrupt onsets can be

More information

Cognitive development in VCFS

Cognitive development in VCFS Progress in Pediatric Cardiology 15 (2002) 109 117 Cognitive development in VCFS a,b, b b b Tony J. Simon *, Carrie E. Bearden, Edward M. Moss, Donna McDonald-McGinn, a,b a,b Elaine Zackai, Paul P. Wang

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language Based on: Chapter 18 and 19, Breedlove, Watson, Rosenzweig, 6e/7e. Cerebral Cortex Brain s most complex area with billions of neurons and

More information

Birth Prevalence of Chromosome 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome: A Systematic Review of Population-Based Studies

Birth Prevalence of Chromosome 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome: A Systematic Review of Population-Based Studies Special Article Birth Prevalence of Chromosome 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome: A Systematic Review of Population-Based Studies Vipawee Panamonta MD*, Khunton Wichajarn MD**, Arnkisa Chaikitpinyo MD**, Manat

More information

Look Out It s Your Off-Peak Time of Day! Time of Day Matters More for Alerting than for Orienting or Executive Attention

Look Out It s Your Off-Peak Time of Day! Time of Day Matters More for Alerting than for Orienting or Executive Attention The University of San Francisco USF Scholarship: a digital repository @ Gleeson Library Geschke Center Psychology College of Arts and Sciences 2013 Look Out It s Your Off-Peak Time of Day! Time of Day

More information

Chapter 13. DiGeorge Syndrome

Chapter 13. DiGeorge Syndrome Chapter 13 DiGeorge Syndrome DiGeorge Syndrome is a primary immunodeficiency disease caused by abnormal migration and development of certain cells and tissues during fetal development. As part of the developmental

More information

The Impact of Cognitive Deficits on Conflict Monitoring Predictable Dissociations Between the Error-Related Negativity and N2

The Impact of Cognitive Deficits on Conflict Monitoring Predictable Dissociations Between the Error-Related Negativity and N2 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Impact of Cognitive Deficits on Conflict Monitoring Predictable Dissociations Between the Error-Related Negativity and N2 Nick Yeung 1 and Jonathan D. Cohen 2,3

More information

Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations

Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 742-748 Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations Çağlar Akçay and Eliot Hazeltine University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Recent

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Infant and Child Development Inf. Child Dev. 10: 39 45 (2001) DOI: 10.1002/icd.245 Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Olivier Pascalis a, *, Elisabeth

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Cardiac and electro-cortical responses to performance feedback reflect different aspects of feedback processing

Cardiac and electro-cortical responses to performance feedback reflect different aspects of feedback processing Cardiac and electro-cortical responses to performance feedback reflect different aspects of feedback processing Frederik M. van der Veen 1,2, Sander Nieuwenhuis 3, Eveline A.M Crone 2 & Maurits W. van

More information

Implicit Learning of Stimulus Regularities Increases Cognitive Control

Implicit Learning of Stimulus Regularities Increases Cognitive Control Implicit Learning of Stimulus Regularities Increases Cognitive Control Jiaying Zhao 1 *, Devin Karbowicz 2, Daniel Osherson 3 1 Department of Psychology and Institute for Resources, Environment and Sustainability,

More information

3. Title: Within Fluid Cognition: Fluid Processing and Fluid Storage?

3. Title: Within Fluid Cognition: Fluid Processing and Fluid Storage? Cowan commentary on Blair, Page 1 1. Commentary on Clancy Blair target article 2. Word counts: Abstract 62 Main text 1,066 References 487 (435 excluding 2 references already in the target article) Total

More information

Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction

Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction Revealing The Brain s Hidden Potential: Cognitive Training & Neurocognitive Plasticity. Introduction Global aging poses significant burdens as age-related impairments in cognitive function affect quality

More information

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Eiichi Hoshino 1,2, Ken Mogi 2, 1 Tokyo Institute of Technology, Department of Computational Intelligence and Systems Science. 4259

More information

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia In general, sensory processing as well as selective attention impairments are common amongst people with schizophrenia. It is important to note that experts have in

More information

Brain Imaging Data of ADHD

Brain Imaging Data of ADHD August 01, 2004 Brain Imaging Data of ADHD Amir Raz, Ph.D. The past two decades have ushered in a new era of methodological advances in tools for noninvasive imaging of the living brain. The information

More information

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M.

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M. Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making Johanna M. Jarcho 1,2 Elliot T. Berkman 3 Matthew D. Lieberman 3 1 Department of Psychiatry

More information

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set In press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set Timothy J. Wright 1, Walter

More information

Differences in visual orienting between persons with Down or fragile X syndrome

Differences in visual orienting between persons with Down or fragile X syndrome Brain and Cognition 65 (2007) 128 134 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Differences in visual orienting between persons with Down or fragile X syndrome Tara Flanagan a, James T. Enns b, Melissa M. Murphy c,

More information

Enhanced Visual Search for a Conjunctive Target in Autism: A Research Note

Enhanced Visual Search for a Conjunctive Target in Autism: A Research Note J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. Vol. 39, No. 5, pp. 777 783, 1998 Cambridge University Press 1998 Association for Child Psychology and Psychiatry Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved 0021 9630 98

More information

Elena Festa Martino IIRG Institution Grant # Grant Period 9/1/07 8/31/08 Reporting Period 9/1/07 8/31/08 Year 1 Report Due 9/1/08

Elena Festa Martino IIRG Institution Grant # Grant Period 9/1/07 8/31/08 Reporting Period 9/1/07 8/31/08 Year 1 Report Due 9/1/08 INTERIM Scientific Progress Report Institution Project Title Brown University Efficacy of qeeg Neurocognitive Training in Early-Stage Alzheimer s Disease Elena Festa Martino Principal Investigator Alz.

More information

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1 The attentional window configures to object boundaries University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 2 ABSTRACT When

More information

Catherine Fassbender, Ph.D.

Catherine Fassbender, Ph.D. Catherine Fassbender, Ph.D. Fassbender Catherine, Ph.D., Assistant Professional Researcher, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, School of Medicine Education B.A., Psychology, University College

More information

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Name: Student #: BEFORE YOU BEGIN!!! 1) Count the number of pages in your exam. The exam is 8 pages long; if you do not

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/24/2018: Lecture 05-2 Note: This Powerpoint presentation

More information

Nonverbal Cognitive Impairments in Fragile X Syndrome: A Neurocognitive Basis Shared With Other Developmental Disorders?

Nonverbal Cognitive Impairments in Fragile X Syndrome: A Neurocognitive Basis Shared With Other Developmental Disorders? Nonverbal Cognitive Impairments in Fragile X Syndrome: A Neurocognitive Basis Shared With Other Developmental Disorders? Tony J. Simon Ph.D. tjsimon@ucdavis.edu N TRI The M.I.N.D. Institute NeuroTherapeutics

More information

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate Supplementary Results Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate behavioral experiment was conducted (n = 16) to verify (a) that retrieval-induced forgetting is observed

More information

birds were used. Every two weeks during treatment, all thirty-two items were probed. A final probe was collected six to seven weeks post-treatment.

birds were used. Every two weeks during treatment, all thirty-two items were probed. A final probe was collected six to seven weeks post-treatment. The Complexity Account of Treatment Efficacy (CATE) predicts that selective training of complex targets will produce generalization to less complex targets that are operationally defined as possessing

More information

The Brain on ADHD. Ms. Komas. Introduction to Healthcare Careers

The Brain on ADHD. Ms. Komas. Introduction to Healthcare Careers The Brain on ADHD Ms. Komas Introduction to Healthcare Careers Ms. Komas Period 9/2/2016 Komas 1 HOOK: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) plagues between 5% and 7% of children and less than

More information

COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE HOW TO STUDY MORE EFFECTIVELY (P 187-189) Elaborate Think about the meaning of the information that you are learning Relate to what you already know Associate: link information together Generate and test

More information

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY Thomas Lachmann *, Gunjan Khera * and Cees van Leeuwen # * Psychology II, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany # Laboratory

More information

Importance of Deficits

Importance of Deficits Importance of Deficits In complex systems the parts are often so integrated that they cannot be detected in normal operation Need to break the system to discover the components not just physical components

More information

Prof. Greg Francis 7/8/08

Prof. Greg Francis 7/8/08 Attentional and motor development IIE 366: Developmental Psychology Chapter 5: Perceptual and Motor Development Module 5.2 Attentional Processes Module 5.3 Motor Development Greg Francis Lecture 13 Children

More information

Behavioral Task Performance

Behavioral Task Performance Zacks 1 Supplementary content for: Functional Reorganization of Spatial Transformations After a Parietal Lesion Jeffrey M. Zacks, PhD *, Pascale Michelon, PhD *, Jean M. Vettel, BA *, and Jeffrey G. Ojemann,

More information

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Nart Bedin Atalay (natalay@selcuk.edu.tr) Selcuk University, Konya, TURKEY Mine Misirlisoy

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Moriguchi and Hiraki 10.1073/pnas.0809747106 SI Text Differences in Brain Activation Between Preswitch and Postswitch Phases. The paired t test was used to compare the brain activation

More information

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection The New Yorker (2/7/5) Selective Attention Perception and awareness are necessarily selective (cell phone while driving): attention gates access to awareness Selective attention is deployed via two modes

More information

Preparatory allocation of attention and adjustments in conflict processing

Preparatory allocation of attention and adjustments in conflict processing www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 35 (2007) 949 958 Preparatory allocation of attention and adjustments in conflict processing Tracy L. Luks, a, Gregory V. Simpson, a Corby L. Dale, a and Morgan

More information

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting Ahnate Lim (ahnate@hawaii.edu) Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii at Manoa 2530 Dole Street, Honolulu, HI 96822 USA Scott Sinnett (ssinnett@hawaii.edu)

More information

Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2668 2682 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Common and unique components of inhibition and

More information

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT Why delaying the onset of alcohol and other drug use during adolescence is so important Aaron White, PhD Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research National Institute

More information

Acute aerobic exercise effects on event-related brain potentials

Acute aerobic exercise effects on event-related brain potentials 8 Acute aerobic exercise effects on event-related brain potentials Charles H. Hillman, Matthew Pontifex and Jason R. Themanson The study of acute exercise effects on neurocognitive function has grown in

More information

Nicole M. Dudukovic, Ph.D. Department of Psychology 1227 University of Oregon Eugene, OR Phone:

Nicole M. Dudukovic, Ph.D. Department of Psychology 1227 University of Oregon Eugene, OR Phone: Nicole M. Dudukovic, Ph.D. Department of Psychology 1227 University of Oregon Eugene, OR 97403 Email: ndudukov@uoregon.edu Phone: 541-346-7225 EDUCATION 2007 Stanford University, Ph.D., Psychology (Cognitive

More information

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL CHARACTERISTICS OF CHILDREN WITH THE 22q11 DELETION SYNDROME: A DESCRIPTIVE ANALYSIS

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL CHARACTERISTICS OF CHILDREN WITH THE 22q11 DELETION SYNDROME: A DESCRIPTIVE ANALYSIS Child Neuropsychology, 11: 39 53, 2005 Copyright Taylor & Francis Inc. ISSN: 0929-7049 print DOI: 10.1080/09297040590911167 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL CHARACTERISTICS OF CHILDREN WITH THE 22q11 DELETION SYNDROME:

More information

Immunologic Features 22q11.2DS. Chiraag S. Patel, MD

Immunologic Features 22q11.2DS. Chiraag S. Patel, MD Immunologic Features 22q11.2DS Chiraag S. Patel, MD Objectives Genetics and DiGeorge Syndrome Thymus Immune cells (T-, B-, and NK cells) 22q11.2DS-associated disorders autoimmune and allergic diseases

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information