Age Differences in Genetic and Environmental Variations in Stress-Coping During Adulthood: A Study of Female Twins

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Age Differences in Genetic and Environmental Variations in Stress-Coping During Adulthood: A Study of Female Twins"

Transcription

1 Behav Genet (2012) 42: DOI /s ORIGINAL RESEARCH Age Differences in Genetic and Environmental Variations in Stress-Coping During Adulthood: A Study of Female Twins Yoon-Mi Hur Alexander J. MacGregor Lynn Cherkas Frances M. K. Williams Tim D. Spector Received: 21 December 2011 / Accepted: 19 April 2012 / Published online: 5 May 2012 Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012 Abstract The way people cope with stressors of day to day living has an important influence on health. The aim of the present study was to explore whether genetic and environmental variations in stress-coping differ over time during adulthood. The brief COPE was mailed to a large sample of the UK female twins (N = 4,736) having a wide range of age (20 87 years). Factor analyses of the items of the brief COPE yielded three coping scales: Problem-Solving, Support Seeking, and Avoidance. Monozygotic and dizygotic twin correlations tended to become lower with age for all three scales, suggesting that unique environmental factors may become more important with age during adulthood. Model-fitting results showed that relative influences of unique environmental factors increased from 60 % at age 20 years to 74% at age 87 years for Problem-Solving and 56 % at age 20 years to 76% at age 87 years for Avoidance. During the same age period, genetic factors decreased from 40 to 26 % for Problem-Solving and from 44 to 24 % for Avoidance. For Seeking Support, the magnitude of Edited by Deborah Finkel. Y.-M. Hur Mokpo National University, Jeonnam, South Korea Y.-M. Hur (&) Industry Academic Cooperation, 61 Dorim-ri, Cheonggye-myeon, Muan-gun, Jeonnam, South Korea ymhur@mokpo.ac.kr A. J. MacGregor L. Cherkas F. M. K. Williams T. D. Spector Department of Twin Research, Kings College London, London, UK A. J. MacGregor School of Medicine, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK genetic and unique environmental factors was not significantly different across the adulthood. For all three scales, shared environmental effects were negligible. Overall, our findings implicate that the effects of environment that stem from idiosyncratic experience of stressful life events accumulate and become increasingly important in adulthood. Keywords Stress-coping Twin Genetics Aging Epigenetics Introduction Coping defined as cognitive and behavioral efforts to manage psychological stress is an important trait related to human adaptation and health (Lazarus 1993). It has been demonstrated that coping is associated with diabetes and cardiovascular disease (Maes Leventhal and Ridder 1996; Snieder et al. 2002) various forms of psychopathology (Sawyer et al. 2009; Vinberg et al. 2010) social functioning (Lazarus 1993) and personality (Watson and Hubbard 1996; Kato and Pedersen 2005; Kendler et al. 1991). For example patients with depression and chronic fatigue syndrome have been shown to use more avoidance coping relative to problem-focused coping strategies than normal controls (Afari et al. 2000; Kendler et al Although it has been argued that coping is a product of social learning and experience (Heszen-Niejodek 1997), several twin studies have shown the importance of genetic influences on variation in coping. Recently, Jang et al. (2007) found in an adult volunteer twin sample that % of the variance for Emotion-Oriented Coping, Task-Oriented Coping, and Social Diversion were due to genetic factors. The results of Jang et al. s study, however, are limited by the small sample size (N = 171 pairs).

2 542 Behav Genet (2012) 42: Somewhat higher estimates of genetic influences were documented by other studies that employed relatively large twin samples. In 446 pairs of reared-together and rearedapart middle- and old-aged twins, Kato and Pedersen (2005) found genetic influences to range from 15 to 51 % on Problem-Solving, Turning to Others, and Avoidance. With the same measure of coping used in the Jang et al. (2007) study, Kozak et al. (2005) found in 612 pairs of adult Polish twins that the contribution of genetic factors ranged from 33 to 39 %. These estimates were similar to those reported by Kendler et al. (1991) who found genetic factors accounted for approximately 30 % of the variation in Problem-Solving and Turning to Others in 827 pairs of young adult female twins in the USA. In summary, previous twin studies of coping indicated that, depending on the measure and sample, genetic factors explained approximately % of individual differences in coping in adulthood. Twin studies have documented evidence of environmental factors in coping as well. However, environmental factors important for stresscoping were those unique to individuals in a family, which explained about % of the variance of stress-coping. The magnitude of shared family environmental influences was zero to very small (Kendler et al. 1991; Kato and Pedersen 2005). While estimates of genetic and environmental contributions to individual differences in coping during adulthood have been relatively well established, very little attention has been given to the issue of whether genetic and environmental variations in coping change over time in adult years. Coping is a dynamic process that depends on situational as well as dispositional factors (Lazarus 1993). A review of 185 gerontological studies has shown that older people are more variable than younger people on a variety of psychological characteristics (Nelson and Dannefer 1992). Research examining coping during adulthood has documented significant age differences in the mean level of coping strategy as well. In general, the younger people tend to use more active, interpersonal, problem-focused forms of coping (e.g., seeking of social support, problem solving) than do the older people, and the older people use more passive, emotion-focused coping styles (e.g., distancing) (Folkman et al. 1987; Lazarus and DeLongis 1983). Across the life span, individuals undergo role changes and consequently, experience different types of environmental stressors. Although genes may predispose individuals to develop certain consistent coping styles to handle various stressors, as a result of accumulation of life experiences including participation in various therapies and intervention programs, individual s coping responses can be altered, which can lead the influence of unique environmental effects in coping responses to increase in late adulthood. Using a cross-sectional twin design, the present study aimed to investigate whether the magnitude of genetic and environmental influences on coping varies during adulthood. It is important to understand the etiological basis of age differences in coping during adulthood because this knowledge will enable us to implement effective prevention and intervention strategies to promote mental health as well as successful adaptation to aging. As far as we understand, to date, only one twin study has explored age differences in genetic and environmental variation in coping. In Kato and Pedersen s (2005) adult twin study, the authors divided the total sample into two age groups cut at the median age (58.19 years) of their sample, but failed to find any significant differences in the magnitude of genetic and environmental variation between the two age groups. However, their results should be interpreted with a caution at least for two reasons. First, the sample size in the study was not sufficiently large (in total, 446 pairs and 447 individual twins) to detect age effects. Unfortunately, it is difficult to figure out the magnitude of the difference in genetic and environmental influences between the two age groups as Kato and Pedersen (2005) did not report the effect size of the difference. Secondly, because they arbitrarily classified their total sample into two subgroups and compared them, the authors may have failed to capture the effects of age which may be manifested across adulthood continuously rather than discretely. In the present study, we treated age as a continuous moderator that can influence genetic and environmental variances in coping during adulthood. As the age range of our sample was broad (20 87 years), this study provided a sensitive test for age differences in heritability and the strength of environmental influences on coping across adulthood. The present study also had a sample larger than any other twin studies of coping published so far, which offered us an increased statistical power to detect effects of age in coping. Methods Sample Subjects, drawn from the TwinsUK register ( ac.uk) (Spector and Williams 2006), consisted of female twins of European ancestry who returned a mail-out survey of self-report questionnaires on a range of topics, mostly unrelated to coping. Recruitment to the TwinsUK register began in 1994 through a national media campaign. Participants were all females because the TwinsUK register was originally developed mainly for studies of adult female bone mineral density. However, twin subjects were not selected for any specific behavioral trait or health status,

3 Behav Genet (2012) 42: nor did they have prior knowledge of the precise nature of the study. Overall, the distribution of the social class of the twins was very similar to that of the general population in the UK (Spector and Williams 2006). Zygosity was established through a standardized questionnaire and by DNA confirmation in doubtful cases. The study received approvals from King s College London Ethics Committee. In total, 4,736 individual twins consisting of 1,019 pairs of monozygotic (MZ), 981 pairs of dizygotic (DZ), and 736 individual twins were included in data analyses. The mean age of the MZ twins was 52.6 (±13.2, range = 20 87) years and of the DZ twins was 53.5 (±11.9, range = 22 85) years. Measure To measure coping, we carried out a confirmatory factor analysis using 28 items that came from the Brief COPE (Carver 1997) and two additional items tapping behavioral aspects of coping: I eat more than usual to make myself feel better and I do exercise to clear my mind, such as running, walking, swimming, yoga, or going to the gym. The confirmatory factor analysis was performed in a large sample of twins who responded to our 2001 survey (N = 1,720), which is a subsample of the present study. The confirmatory factory analysis yielded three interpretable factors: Problem-Solving (11 items), Seeking Support (7 items) and Avoidance (12 items). Absolute loadings on these three factors ranged from 0.23 to For each of the 30 items, twins were asked to respond on a 4-point Likert-type response format ranging from I usually don t do this at all to I usually do this a lot when experiencing certain stressful events. Responses were summed up to compute the score for each of the three scales. Sample sizes varied across the three coping scales due to incomplete reporting by some subjects. For all three scales, higher scores indicate greater use of the particular coping strategy. Cronbach s alpha coefficients were 0.78 for Problem-Solving, 0.71 for Avoidance, and 0.75 for Seeking Support. The scores of the three scales conformed to a normal distribution (skewness =-0.12 to 0.57; kurtosis =-0.05 to 0.39). The three coping scales in our study are similar to those commonly identified by previous coping research: Problem-Solving refers to responses that seek to eliminate the source of stress by actively dealing with the reality of the situation and is considered positive, practical coping style. Seeking Support represents the coping responses to reduce the stress by seeking informational and emotional supports from others. Avoidance describes a negative, passive coping strategy to engage in another task rather than the task at hand to escape from stressful situations. Problem-Solving was moderately correlated with Seeking Support (r = 0.32), but was independent from Avoidance (r = 0.07). There was a modest correlation between Seeking Support and Avoidance (r = 0.17) in our sample. Overall, this pattern of interscale correlations was consistent with those found in the literature of coping (e.g., Endler and Parker 1990). Statistical analyses There were two steps in data analyses. In the first step, we roughly divided the total sample into two age groups and compared means, variances, twin correlations, and the estimates of genetic and environmental influences for each coping scale across the two age groups. The two age groups were the young/middle-aged adult group (20 60 years) and the old-aged adult group (61 87 years). We combined young adults with middle-aged adults and made the young/middleaged adult group as the number of young adult twins in our sample was very small. This first step served as a preliminary analysis to confirm the results of analyses in the second step where we performed model-fitting analyses incorporating age as a continuous moderator (Purcell 2002). In the age-moderation model, the standard paths a, d, and e, indicating factors attributable to additive genetic (A) and nonadditive genetic (D), and unique environmental (E) influences, respectively, are allowed to vary as a function of a moderator variable (M, age in our analyses). Additive genetic factors (A), i.e., the sum of the average effect of all genes that influence a trait, were set to be correlated at 1.0 and 0.5 for MZ and DZ twins, respectively. Nonadditive genetic factors (D), i.e., those interactive effects of genes were set to be correlated at 1.0 and 0.25 for MZ and DZ twins, respectively. Finally, (E), environmental factors unique to each member of a twin pair and measurement error represented the remaining variance not explained by additive or nonadditve genetic factors. We did not include shared environmental factors in our model because our twin correlations showed no evidence of shared environmental influences (see Table 1). According to our age-moderation model, the phenotypic variance (V p ) can be expressed as: V p =(a+b a M) 2 +(d+b d M) 2 +(e+b e M) 2 In this equation, b a, b d, and b e represent the magnitude of the moderating effects associated with A, D, and E, respectively. If b is significantly different from zero, there is evidence for a moderating effect of age on genetic and environmental influences on coping. That is, influences of additive and nonadditive genetic factors and unique environmental factors vary as a function of age. In addition to the three variance components, we allowed a pathway l? b m M to represent the main effects of age on coping.

4 544 Behav Genet (2012) 42: Table 1 Means, variances, twin correlations and genetic and environmental variance components for three coping scales by age Problem-Solving Seeking Support Avoidance Young/middle Old Total Young/middle Old Total Young/middle Old Total Mean Variance r MZ 0.35(0.30, 0.43) 0.26 (0.15, 0.36) 0.33(0.27, 0.38) 0.40(0.34, 0.46) 0.35(0.24, 0.44) 0.38(0.33, 0.43) 0.37(0.30, 0.43) 0.33(0.23, 0.43) 0.36(0.30, 0.41) r DZ 0.07 (-0.01, 0.14) 0.11(-0.01, 0.22) 0.09(0.02, 0.15) 0.16(0.08, 0.23) 0.14(0.02, 0.25) 0.15(0.09, 0.21) 0.15(0.08, 0.22) 0.10(0.01, 0.22) 0.14(0.08, 0.20) A 0(0, 5.8) 3.8(0, 11.0) 0(0, 6.8) 3.8(0, 7.6) 2.9(0, 7.8) 3.6(0, 7.2) 5.7(0, 10.7) 2.7(0, 9.0) 5.0(0, 9.7) D 9.3(3.5, 11.0) 4.6(0, 11.8) 9.2(2.2, 10.8) 3.1(0, 7.8) 3.6(0, 8.3) 3.3(0, 7.4) 3.8(0, 10.9) 4.7(0, 9.6) 3.9(0, 9.9) E 17.3(15.8, 19.1) 22.7(19.5, 26.4) 18.8(17.4, 20.5) 10.1(9.1, 11.2) 11.3(9.6, 13.2) 10.4(9.6, 11.4) 16.0(14.5, 17.7) 15.4(13.3, 17.8) 15.8(14.6, 17.1) A (%) 0(0, 22) 12(0, 34) 0(0, 24) 22(0, 44) 16(0, 43) 21(0, 41) 22(0, 41) 12(0, 39) 20(0, 39) D (%) 35(11, 40) 15(0, 37) 33(7, 38) 18(0, 45) 20(0, 45) 19(0, 43) 15(0, 42) 21(0, 41) 16(0, 40) E (%) 65(60, 72) 73(63, 84) 67(62, 73) 60(54, 66) 64(54, 75) 60(56, 66) 63(57, 69) 67(59, 78) 64(59, 69) 95 % CIs are in parenthesis r MZ = Twin correlation for monozygotic twins; r DZ = Twin correlation for dizygotic twins Young/middle young/middle-aged adults (age range = years), old old-aged adults (age range = years), A additive genetic variance, D nonadditive genetic variance, E unique environmental variance The maximum likelihood raw data option in Mx (Neale et al. 2003) was used in our model-fitting analyses. Mx calculates twice the negative log-likelihood (-2LL) of the data. As the difference in -2LL is Chi-square distributed, when models were nested to each other, the likelihood ratio test (LRT) was used to evaluate alternative models. When models were not nested to each other, Akaike s information criterion (AIC) was used to judge the model. AIC is defined as -2LL for the model minus twice the number of degrees of freedom (AIC =-2LL - 2df). Models having smaller AIC are considered more parsimonious, and are thus preferred (Akaike 1987). To determine the best-fitting, most parsimonious model, we reduced parameters and compared the goodnessof-fit of the reduced models with that of the full model. Results Preliminary analyses MZ twin correlations were consistently greater than DZ twin correlations across all three scales in all age groups, suggesting the importance of genetic influences on coping across adulthood. The presence of nonadditive genetic factors was also indicated as DZ twin correlations were generally lower than half the MZ correlations for all three scales. Most MZ twin correlations fell below 0.40, suggesting that unique environmental factors were substantial for coping. None of the three scales showed evidence for significant shared environmental factors, however. In the total sample, age was very modestly correlated with Problem-Solving (-0.05, p \ 0.01), Seeking Support (-0.04, p \ 0.01), and Avoidance (-0.09, p \ 0.01). These modest correlations suggested that the mean effects of age on coping were minimal in our sample. Table 1 shows that the mean levels for the three coping scales were not significantly different among the three age groups. Of particular interest in the present study is variance. Interestingly, phenotypic variances for Problem-Solving and Seeking Support increased with age, with this trend being more pronounced for Problem-Solving than for Seeking Support. These increases in the phenotypic variance with age were primarily due to the increases of unique environmental influences, which was indicated in the increased estimates of unstandardized variance of unique environmental factors in old-aged adults as compared to those in young/middle-aged adults for Problem-Solving (young/ middle-aged adults = 17.3 and old-aged adults = 22.7) and Seeking Support (young/middle-aged adults = 10.1 and old-aged adults = 11.3) in Table 1. Indeed, the estimates of unstandardized variance of total genetic factors (A? D) for both scales were slightly decreased with age (9.3? 8.4 for Problem-Solving and 6.9? 6.5 for

5 Behav Genet (2012) 42: Table 2 Model-fitting results for the three coping scales Model Problem-Solving Seeking Support Avoidance -2LL AIC df DX 2 p -2LL AIC df DX 2 p -2LL AIC df DX 2 p 1 ADEade AEae AEe AEa AE ADEde ADEae ADEad ADEe ADEd ADEa ADE The best-fitting model is in bold type A additive genetic effects, D nonadditive genetic effects, E unique environmental effects including measurement error. a the age-moderator effects on additive genetic factors, d the age-moderator effects on nonadditive genetic factors, e the age-moderator effects on unique environmental factors, -2LL -2 log likelihood Seeking Support ), confirming that the increase of the phenotypic variance for these two scales occurred due to the increase of unique environmental factors rather than genetic factors. For Avoidance, the phenotypic variance diminished with age. Although unstandardized variances of both unique environmental factors and total genetic factors were lowered with age, the decrease was more pronounced in the total genetic variance (9.5? 7.4) than in the unique environmental variance (16.0? 15.4). We therefore concluded that the decrease in the phenotypic variance in Avoidance could be largely due to a decrease in genetic variance. We tested these observations by fitting the agemoderation model below. Model-fitting analyses incorporating moderating effects of age Table 2 summarizes the results of fitting models with agemoderators. Figures 1, 2, and 3 present graphical descriptions of standardized variance components in the best-fitting model for the three coping scales. It should be noted that we did not attempt to remove A from the full model as it has been argued that an extreme amount of nonadditive genetic effects with no effects of additive genes is unlikely to explain genetic variance for complex traits (Eaves 1988). However, as our sample did not have sufficient statistical power to distinguish between A and D, all genetic variance was absorbed into D for the Problem- Solving scale, resulting in the estimate of A to be zero. For this reason, we summed up A and D to represent broad heritability, i.e., total genetic variance in Figs. 1, 2, and 3. Among the models tested for Problem-Solving, Model 9 was the best by AIC and LRT. In Model 9, the parameters, A, D, E, and the age-moderator on E(e) alone were maintained. The unstandardized unique environmental variance for Problem-Solving increased from 13.4 (95 % CI: ) at age 20 years to 25.6 (95 % CI: ) at age 87 years, whereas the total unstandardized genetic variance was the same [9.0 (95 % CI: )] from age 20 to 87 years in Model 9. Consistent with observations Standardized variance component 100% 90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% A + D Age in years Fig. 1 Standardized variance components of total genetic (A? D) and unique environmental factors including measurement error (E) for Problem-Solving from age 20 to 87 years in the best-fitting model E

6 546 Behav Genet (2012) 42: Standardized variance component 100% 90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% A+D Age in years Fig. 2 Standardized variance components of total genetic (A? D) and unique environmental factors including measurement error (E) for Seeking Support from age 20 to 87 years in the best-fitting model Standardized variance component 100% 90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% made from Table 1, these results suggested that the increase of the phenotypic variance with age found for Problem-Solving was attributable to the increase of unique environmental variance. In terms of the standardized variance component (Fig. 1), total genetic factors decreased from 40 % (95 % CI: %) at age 20 years to 26 % (95 % CI: %) at age 87 years, whereas unique environmental factors increased from 60 % (95 % CI: %) at age 20 years to 74 % (95 % CI: %) at age 87 years in Model 9. For Seeking Support, Model 5 was the best one, where none of the age-moderators was significant, with only A and E parameters being statistically significant. These results suggested that total genetic and unique environmental variances were constant across adulthood in E A+D Age in years Fig. 3 Standardized variance components of total genetic (A? D) and unique environmental factors including measurement error (E) for Avoidance from age 20 to 87 years in the best-fitting model E Seeking Support. Although the pattern of the phenotypic variance in two age groups observed in Table 1 implied possible increase in unique environmental variance with age, the effects were not large enough to attain statistical significance. Total standardized genetic variance and unique environmental variance for Seeking Support in Model 5 were, respectively, 38 % (95 % CI: %) and 62 % (95 % CI: %) (Fig. 2). For Avoidance, Model 4 was the best by AIC and LRT. The parameters A, E, and the age-moderator on A (a) were maintained in Model 4. The unstandardized total genetic variance for Avoidance decreased from 12.9 (95 % CI: ) at age 20 years to 5.0 (95 % CI: ) at age 87 years, whereas the unstandardized unique environmental variance was stable [16.1 (95 %CI: )]. Therefore, the decrease in the phenotypic variance from age 20 to 87 years shown in Table 1 was attributed to the decrease in genetic variance during adulthood. In terms of the standardized variance component (Fig. 3), genetic factors decreased from 44 % (95 % CI: 30 55%) at age 20 years to 24 % (95 % CI: 0 50 %) at age 87 years, whereas unique environmental factors increased from 56 % (95 % CI: %) at age 20 years to 76 % (95 % CI: %) at age 87 years in Model 4. Discussion Our study suggests that phenotypic, genetic, and unique environmental variations in human coping responses tend to vary over time during adulthood. In terms of unstandardized variance, the phenotypic variance of Problem- Solving increased with age due to an increase of unique environmental variance with age. In contrast, the phenotypic variance of Avoidance decreased with age due to a decrease of genetic variance with age. For Seeking Support, the phenotypic, genetic, and unique environmental variations were constant during adulthood, although the parameter estimates in Table 1 showed a pattern of age difference similar to that found for Problem-Solving. It is interesting to note that unstandardized variance of genetic and unique environmental factors for Problem- Solving and Avoidance changed with age in different directions. Problem-Solving is considered adaptive coping behavior, whereas Avoidance, maladaptive coping style (Kendler et al. 1991). A recent study by Vinberg et al. (2010) demonstrated that among individuals at a high risk for depression, the use of Problem-Solving strategy in the face of stressful life events played a role of protective factors, whereas frequent use of Avoidance coping strategies exacerbated the health outcome. Our results showed that unstandardized genetic variance for Avoidance was significantly reduced later in life. Given the

7 Behav Genet (2012) 42: previous finding (e.g., Lieberman and Tobin 1983) indicating that the ways the elderly coped with stress made a significant difference in survival and well-being, we speculate that those who were predisposed to have extremely high level of Avoidance coping strategies were not present in our sample due to health and adaptation problems, leading to a smaller genetic variance at older ages. The lower mean level of Avoidance in the old-aged group as compared to the young/middle-aged group supported this speculation. In terms of standardized variance, whereas unique environmental factors increased, genetic factors decreased over time for both Problem-Solving and Avoidance. Charles and Almeide (2007) found unique environmental influences on the severity of perceived stress to increase with age during adulthood. Given that coping style is related to perception of stressful situations (Lazarus 1993), our results are consistent with those reported by Charles and Almeide (2007). Why do unique environmental influences on stress-coping increase during adulthood? Our findings suggest that the effects of idiosyncratic experiences of stressful events accumulate over the life course. Thus, unique environmental influences increasingly contribute to individual difference in coping patterns during adulthood. Folkman et al. (1987) found that sources of stress were different between younger and older adults: the younger adults reported finance, work, parenting, and family issues as salient life events, whereas the older adults reported retirement, death of a loved one, physical debilitation, and health issues to be important sources of stress. Stressful experiences are not limited to major life events, but also include the ongoing difficult conditions of daily life. As the number of stressful life events increases with age and as new sets of life events come into effects during adulthood, relatively small environmental influences on coping style in early adulthood can become increasingly important in late adulthood. There is a growing body of evidence that epigenetic factors can have a profound impact on behavioral phenotypes (Petronis 2006). Epigenetics refers to phenotypic changes caused by mechanisms that are unrelated to changes in the underlying DNA sequence, most notably driven by histone modifications and DNA methylation (Petronis 2006). From the epigenetic point of view, phenotypic differences in MZ twins could result, in part, from their epigenetic differences that change over the lifetime. Fraga et al. (2005) reported that DNA methylation differences between MZ twins increase with age, with divergence in time spent together, and with different lifestyles, implicating that the impact of genetic and unique environmental factors in psychological traits can change with age. In our study, MZ twin correlations were lower in the old-aged group than in the young/middle-aged group for all three coping scales (Table 1), suggesting a possibility that differences in epigenetic markings may have occurred in coping responses. Evidence for the role of epigenetic changes in determining individual differences in stresscoping comes from rodent studies. For example, Weaver et al. (2006) found that decreased licking, grooming, and nursing by rat mothers reduced DNA methylation and histone acetylation at a glucocorticoid receptor gene promoter in the hippocampus of the offspring, resulting in increased stress responses in later life, suggesting that environmental factors, by regulating gene expression, can lead to change in stress-coping responses later in life. Another example can be found in the Murgatroyd et al. (2009) study where the authors found that early life stress in mice can dynamically control DNA methylation in postmitotic neurons to generate stable changes in arginine vasopressin (AVP) gene expression that trigger alteration in stress-coping behavior. It would be of interest in future research to explore epigenetic mechanisms in human stress-coping behavior. The present study has several limitations that deserve attention. First, our results were on the basis of female twins, and therefore, cannot be generalized to adult males because sex differences in genetic and environmental variances in coping have been reported (Kato and Pedersen 2005). Secondly, although we attempted to use information on age in our sample maximally by treating age as a continuous variable, our data were cross-sectional in nature. People of different ages may vary in coping because they grew up under different historical conditions in which people perceive and deal with problems of living differently. Replication of our findings using longitudinal data will disentangle these cohort effects from the aging processes. Third, in the present paper we examined how age moderated genetic and environmental influences on coping. However, it is possible that other variables such as marital status and socioeconomic status may have an impact on genetic and environmental factors in coping during adulthood. Future studies should examine how these variables moderate genetic and environmental influences on coping. Finally, our twins are adult volunteers recruited through media campaigns. Although our sample has been shown to be representative of a singleton population ascertained from General Practice registers (Spector and Williams 2006), as with most adult volunteer samples, it is possible that those with better health and more adaptive twins have participated in our study. Acknowledgments The first author (Y.M. Hur) is supported by the Pioneer Fund, USA and Korea Research Foundation Grants. TwinsUK register is supported by the Wellcome Trust. We would like to thank twins who participated in our study and Irina Gillham-Nasenya for the data management.

8 548 Behav Genet (2012) 42: References Afari N, Karen B, Schmaling RH, Hartman S, Goldberg J, Buchwald DS (2000) Coping strategies in twins with chronic fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome. J Psychosom Res 48: Akaike H (1987) Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrica 52: Carver CS (1997) You want to measure coping but your protocol s too long: consider the brief COPE. Int J Behav Med 4: Charles ST, Almeide DM (2007) Genetic and environmental effects on daily life stressors: more evidence for greater variation in later life. Psychol Aging 22: Eaves LJ (1988) Dominance alone is not enough. Behav Genet 18(1):27 33 Endler NS, Parker JDA (1990) Multidimensional assessment of coping: a critical evaluation. J Pers Soc Psychol 58(5): Folkman S, Lazarus RS, Pimley S, Novacek J (1987) Age differences in stress and coping processes. Psychol Aging 2: Fraga MF, Ballestar E, Paz MF, Ropero S, Setien F, Ballestar ML, Heine-Suner D, Cigudosa JC, Urioste M, Benitez J, Boix- Chornet M, Sanchez-Aguilera A, Ling C, Carlsson E, Poulsen P, Vaag A, Stephen Z, Spector TD, Wu Y-Z, Plass C, Esteller M (2005) Epigenetic differences arise during the lifetime of monozygotic twins. PNAS 102: Heszen-Niejodek I (1997) Coping style and its role in coping with stressful encounters. Eur Psychol 2: Jang KL, Thordarson DS, Stein MB, Cohan SL, Taylor S (2007) Coping styles and personality: a biometric analysis. Anxiety Stress Coping 20:17 24 Kato K, Pedersen NL (2005) Personality and coping: a study of twins reared apart and twins reared together. Behav Genet 35: Kendler KS, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Neale MC, Eaves LJ (1991) Coping: a genetic epidemiological investigation. Psychol Med 21: Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Taylor A, Williams B, Newcombe R, Craig IW, Moffitt TE (2006) MAOA, maltreatment, and gene environment interaction predicting children s mental health: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 11(10): Kozak B, Strelau J, Miles JNV (2005) Genetic determinants of individual differences in coping styles. Anxiety Stress Coping 18:1 15 Lazarus RS (1993) Coping theory and research: past, present, and future. Psychosom Med 55(3): Lazarus RS, Delongis A (1983) Psychological stress and coping in aging. Am Psychol 38: Lieberman MA, Tobin SS (1983) The experience of old age: stress, coping, and survival. Basic Books, New York Maes S, Leventhal H, Ridder TD (1996) Coping with chronic diseases. In: Zeidner M, Endler NS (eds) Handbook of coping: theory, research, applications. Wiley, New York Murgatroyd C, Patchev AV, Wu Y, Micale V, Bockmühl Y, Fischer D, Holsboer F, Wotjak CT, Almeida OF, Spengler D (2009) Dynamic DNA methylation programs persistent adverse effects of early-life stress. Nat Neurosci 12: Neale M, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes H (2003). Mx: statistical modeling. Department of Psychiatry, VCU, Richmond Nelson EA, Dannefer D (1992) Aged heterogeneity: fact or fiction? The fate of diversity in gerontological research. Gerontologist 32(1):17 23 Petronis A (2006) Epigenetics and twins: three variations on the theme. Trends Genet 22(7): Purcell S (2002) Variance components models for gene environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research 5: Sawyer MG, Pfeiffer S, Spence SH (2009) Life events, coping and depressive symptoms among young adolescents. a one-year prospective study. J Affect Disord 117:48 54 Snieder H, Harshfield GA, Barbeau P, Pollock DM, Pollock JS, Treiber FA (2002) Dissecting the genetic architecture of the cardiovascular and renal stress response. Biol Psychol 61:73 95 Spector TD, Williams FM (2006) The UK adult Twin Registry (TwinsUK). Twin Res Hum Genetics 9: Vinberg M, Froekjaer VG, Kessing LV (2010) Coping styles in healthy individuals at risk of affective disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 198(39 44):2010 Watson D, Hubbard B (1996) Adaptational style and dispositional structure: coping in the context of the five-factor model. J Pers 64: Weaver IC, Cervoni N, Champagne FA, D Alessio AC, Sharma S, Seckl JR, Dymov S, Szyf M, Meaney MJ (2006) Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat Neurosci 7:

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating Cynthia M. Bulik,* Patrick F. Sullivan, and Kenneth S. Kendler Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics of Virginia

More information

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance Twin Research (1999) 2, 99 107 1999 Stockton Press All rights reserved 1369 0523/99 $12.00 http://www.stockton-press.co.uk/tr Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Scienc es 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 V Congress of Russian Psychological Society Genetic and Environmental Influences on

More information

Personality and Coping: A Study of Twins Reared Apart and Twins Reared Together

Personality and Coping: A Study of Twins Reared Apart and Twins Reared Together Behavior Genetics, Vol. 35, No. 2, March 2005 (Ó 2005) Personality and Coping: A Study of Twins Reared Apart and Twins Reared Together Kenji Kato 1,*, and Nancy L. Pedersen 1,2 Received 20 Nov. 2003 Final

More information

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability Eric Turkheimer and Erin E. Horn In 3, our lab published a paper demonstrating that the heritability

More information

American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 141B: (2006)

American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 141B: (2006) American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 141B:421 425 (2006) Brief Research Communication Evidence for Monozygotic Twin (MZ) Discordance in Methylation Level at Two CpG Sites

More information

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder DOI 10.1007/s10519-014-9670-x ORIGINAL RESEARCH Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder J. L. Ebejer S. E. Medland J.

More information

In Australian twins participating in three different

In Australian twins participating in three different Heritability and Stability of Resting Blood Pressure in Australian Twins Jouke-Jan Hottenga, 1 John B. Whitfield, 2 Eco J. C. de Geus, 1 Dorret I. Boomsma, 1 and Nicholas G. Martin 2 1 Department of Biological

More information

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins Behavior Genetics, Vol. 33, No. 5, September 2003 ( 2003) Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt,

More information

Studies have shown that familial aggregation is of

Studies have shown that familial aggregation is of No Evidence of Sex Differences in Heritability of Irritable Bowel Syndrome in Swedish Twins Pia Svedberg, 1 Saga Johansson, 3,4 Mari-Ann Wallander, 3,6 and Nancy L. Pedersen 2,5 1 Section of Personal Injury

More information

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H

More information

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Daily Life Stressors: More Evidence for Greater Variation in Later Life

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Daily Life Stressors: More Evidence for Greater Variation in Later Life Psychology and Aging Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 22, No. 2, 331 340 0882-7974/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.2.331 Genetic and Environmental Effects on Daily

More information

How Money Buys Happiness: Genetic and Environmental Processes Linking Finances and Life Satisfaction

How Money Buys Happiness: Genetic and Environmental Processes Linking Finances and Life Satisfaction Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 90, No. 4, 680 691 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.680 How Money Buys Happiness:

More information

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences University of Lethbridge Research Repository OPUS Faculty Research and Publications http://opus.uleth.ca Lalumière, Martin 2005 Behavioral genetics: The study of differences Lalumière, Martin L. Department

More information

Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder:

Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder: Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder: Same Genes, (Partly) Kenneth S. Kendler, M.D. Different Environments Revisited Abstract: In both clinical and epidemiological samples, major depression

More information

Heritability of Creative Achievement

Heritability of Creative Achievement This article was downloaded by: [213.157.19.77] On: 11 June 2014, At: 03:07 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H PSYC H O PAT H O LO G Y : PSYC H I AT R I C P R O B LEMS A N D T H E A S SO C I

More information

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study Diabetologia (2013) 56:1031 1035 DOI 10.1007/s00125-013-2859-3 SHORT COMMUNICATION Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study S. Carlsson & A. Ahlbom & P. Lichtenstein

More information

Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood

Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood CE ACTIVITY Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood Melissa A. Munn, MPE 1,2 * Michael C. Stallings, PhD 1,2 Soo Hyun Rhee, PhD 1,2 Laura E. Sobik, PhD

More information

Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration

Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration J Fam Viol (2007) 22:121 129 DOI 10.1007/s10896-006-9059-6 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration

More information

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION Chapter 8 GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION A Longitudinal Study in 3- to 12-Year-Old Children Dorret I. Boomsma, Ph.D. C.E.M. van Beijsterveldt, Ph.D. Meike Bartels, Ph.D. James

More information

The present study investigated the location of trait

The present study investigated the location of trait Phenotypic and Genetic Relations Between the HEXACO Dimensions and Trait Emotional Intelligence Livia Veselka, 1 K. V. Petrides, 2 Julie Aitken Schermer, 3 Lynn F. Cherkas, 4 Tim D. Spector 4 and Philip

More information

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children J. Intell. 2015, 3, 160-167; doi:10.3390/jintelligence3040160 Brief Report OPEN ACCESS Journal of Intelligence ISSN 2079-3200 www.mdpi.com/journal/jintelligence Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations

More information

Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis

Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis Roxann Roberson-Nay, PhD Virginia Commonwealth University Department of Psychiatry The SAD PD link Don Klein proposed

More information

Genetic contribution to the relationship between personality and depressive symptoms among older women

Genetic contribution to the relationship between personality and depressive symptoms among older women Psychological Medicine (2010), 40, 1357 1366. f Cambridge University Press 2009 doi:10.1017/s0033291709991401 Genetic contribution to the relationship between personality and depressive symptoms among

More information

Discussion. were best fit by models of determination that did not include genetic effects.

Discussion. were best fit by models of determination that did not include genetic effects. A Behavior Genetic Investigation of the Relationship Between Leadership and Personality Andrew M. Johnson 1, Philip A. Vernon 2, Julie Aitken Harris 3, and Kerry L. Jang 4 1 Faculty of Health Sciences,The

More information

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Subhrasikha Naik Senior Study and research chronic diseases Participate in gathering data for Project Cope, which is focused

More information

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail.

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. Author(s): Salmela-Aro, Katariina; Read, Sanna; Vuoksimaa, Eero; Korhonen,

More information

Discontinuous Traits. Chapter 22. Quantitative Traits. Types of Quantitative Traits. Few, distinct phenotypes. Also called discrete characters

Discontinuous Traits. Chapter 22. Quantitative Traits. Types of Quantitative Traits. Few, distinct phenotypes. Also called discrete characters Discontinuous Traits Few, distinct phenotypes Chapter 22 Also called discrete characters Quantitative Genetics Examples: Pea shape, eye color in Drosophila, Flower color Quantitative Traits Phenotype is

More information

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale Simon B. Sherry, 1 Paul L. Hewitt, 1 * Avi Besser, 2 Brandy J. McGee, 1 and Gordon L. Flett 3

More information

Preliminary Conclusion

Preliminary Conclusion 1 Exploring the Genetic Component of Political Participation Brad Verhulst Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics Virginia Commonwealth University Theories of political participation,

More information

UNDERSTANDING AND INFLUENCING PUPILS CHOICES AS THEY PREPARE TO LEAVE SCHOOL EXECUTIVE SUMMARY. Dr Kathryn Asbury. University of York

UNDERSTANDING AND INFLUENCING PUPILS CHOICES AS THEY PREPARE TO LEAVE SCHOOL EXECUTIVE SUMMARY. Dr Kathryn Asbury. University of York UNDERSTANDING AND INFLUENCING PUPILS CHOICES AS THEY PREPARE TO LEAVE SCHOOL EXECUTIVE SUMMARY Dr Kathryn Asbury University of York Professor Robert Plomin King s College London February 2017 Background

More information

We expand our previous deterministic power

We expand our previous deterministic power Power of the Classical Twin Design Revisited: II Detection of Common Environmental Variance Peter M. Visscher, 1 Scott Gordon, 1 and Michael C. Neale 1 Genetic Epidemiology, Queensland Institute of Medical

More information

A Behavior Genetic Study of the Connection Between Social Values and Personality

A Behavior Genetic Study of the Connection Between Social Values and Personality ARTICLE AVAILABLE ONLINE Twin Research and Human Genetics Volume 14 Number 3 pp. 233 239 A Behavior Genetic Study of the Connection Between Social Values and Personality Julie Aitken Schermer, 1 Philip

More information

Genetic Factors in Temperamental Individuality

Genetic Factors in Temperamental Individuality Genetic Factors in Temperamental Individuality A Longitudinal Study of Same-Sexed Twins from Two Months to Six Years of Age Anne Mari Torgersen, Cando Psychol. Abstract. A previous publication reported

More information

How does adversity in childhood get under the skin

How does adversity in childhood get under the skin How does adversity in childhood get under the skin What can we learn from neuroscience and epigenetics? Eamon McCrory Professor of Developmental Neuroscience & Psychopathology, UCL Consultant Clinical

More information

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Risk factors for the development and outcome of childhood psychopathology SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Chapter 147 In this chapter I present a summary of the results of the studies described in this thesis followed

More information

BPD In Adolescence: Early Detection and Intervention

BPD In Adolescence: Early Detection and Intervention BPD In Adolescence: Early Detection and Intervention Blaise Aguirre, MD Medical Director 3East Residential Instructor in Psychiatry Harvard Medical School Quick Points The idea that we have to wait until

More information

Occupational Stress and Coping Strategies among Grade II Police Constables

Occupational Stress and Coping Strategies among Grade II Police Constables Occupational Stress and Coping Strategies among Grade II Police Constables M.Shanmuga Sundaram Assistant Professor, Department of Business Administration Kalasalingam University, Krishnan Kovil -626 126

More information

Growing Old but Not Growing Apart: Twin Similarity in the Latter Half of the Lifespan

Growing Old but Not Growing Apart: Twin Similarity in the Latter Half of the Lifespan Behav Genet (2013) 43:1 12 DOI 10.1007/s10519-012-9559-5 ORIGINAL RESEARCH Growing Old but Not Growing Apart: Twin Similarity in the Latter Half of the Lifespan Matt McGue Kaare Christensen Received: 12

More information

SEMs for Genetic Analysis

SEMs for Genetic Analysis SEMs for Genetic Analysis Session 8, Lecture 4 2/3/06 Basic Genetic Analysis of Twin Samples Using SEMs SEMs and variance explained Motivating example Simple correlation test The heritability concept Structural

More information

Effectiveness of Web-Based Multimedia Mental Health Educational Program on Anxiety, Depression, Self Concept and School Adjustment in Adolescents

Effectiveness of Web-Based Multimedia Mental Health Educational Program on Anxiety, Depression, Self Concept and School Adjustment in Adolescents IJCSNS International Journal of Computer Science and Network Security, VOL.10 No.5, May 2010 59 Effectiveness of Web-Based Multimedia Mental Health Educational Program on Anxiety, Depression, Self Concept

More information

Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease

Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease (2007) 15, 872 877 & 2007 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 1018-4813/07 $30.00 ARTICLE www.nature.com/ejhg Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease Slobodan

More information

Genetic and environmental influences on eating behaviors in 2.5- and 9-year-old children: a longitudinal twin study

Genetic and environmental influences on eating behaviors in 2.5- and 9-year-old children: a longitudinal twin study Dubois et al. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 2013, 10:134 RESEARCH Open Access Genetic and environmental influences on eating behaviors in 2.5- and 9-year-old children:

More information

The Relationship between the Attachment Patterns and the Coping Skills with Drug Abuse

The Relationship between the Attachment Patterns and the Coping Skills with Drug Abuse 2014, World of Researches Publication 2014, World of Researches Publication Ac. Ac. J. J. Psy. Psy. Stud. Stud. Vol. Vol. 3, 3, Issue Issue 1, 1, 92-96, 80-86, 2014 2014 Academic Journal of Academic Psychological

More information

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style

VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style VIEW: An Assessment of Problem Solving Style 2009 Technical Update Donald J. Treffinger Center for Creative Learning This update reports the results of additional data collection and analyses for the VIEW

More information

Genetic influences on disordered eating behaviour are largely independent of body mass index

Genetic influences on disordered eating behaviour are largely independent of body mass index Acta Psychiatr Scand 2008: 117: 348 356 All rights reserved DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01132.x Copyright Ó 2007 The Authors Journal Compilation Ó 2007 Blackwell Munksgaard ACTA PSYCHIATRICA SCANDINAVICA

More information

Measurement Models for Sexual Orientation in a Community Twin Sample

Measurement Models for Sexual Orientation in a Community Twin Sample Behavior Genetics, Vol. 30, No. 4, 2000 Measurement Models for Sexual Orientation in a Community Twin Sample K. M. Kirk, 1, 4 J. M. Bailey, 2 M. P. Dunne, 1, 3 and N. G. Martin 1 Received 22 Feb. 2000

More information

Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions

Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions Psychological Medicine, 2000, 30, 1315 1325. 2000 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions

More information

Individual Differences in Processing Speed and Working Memory Speed as Assessed with the Sternberg Memory Scanning Task

Individual Differences in Processing Speed and Working Memory Speed as Assessed with the Sternberg Memory Scanning Task Behav Genet (2010) 40:315 326 DOI 10.1007/s10519-009-9315-7 ORIGINAL RESEARCH Individual Differences in Processing Speed and Working Memory Speed as Assessed with the Sternberg Memory Scanning Task Anna

More information

Chapter 9. Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS)

Chapter 9. Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS) Chapter 9 Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS) Background Purpose The Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS) is a measure of perceived effectiveness of a specific counseling session. In general, measures

More information

Examining the Origins of Gender Differences in Marital Quality: A Behavior Genetic Analysis

Examining the Origins of Gender Differences in Marital Quality: A Behavior Genetic Analysis Journal of Family Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 20, No. 4, 605 613 0893-3200/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.4.605 Examining the Origins of Gender Differences

More information

The equal environments assumption, which holds

The equal environments assumption, which holds An Investigation of a Measure of Twins Equal Environments Karen S. Mitchell, 1,2 Suzanne E. Mazzeo, 1,3 Cynthia M. Bulik, 4 Steven H. Aggen, 2,5 Kenneth S. Kendler, 2,5 and Michael C. Neale 2,5,6 1 Department

More information

Economic Hardship and Elderly Suicidal Ideation: with a Focus on the Double Mediating Effects of Depression and Family Relationship

Economic Hardship and Elderly Suicidal Ideation: with a Focus on the Double Mediating Effects of Depression and Family Relationship , pp.64-68 http://dx.doi.org/10.14257/astl.2015.101.15 Economic Hardship and Elderly Suicidal Ideation: with a Focus on the Double Mediating Effects of Depression and Family Relationship Jung-Bin, Yang

More information

Health Behavioral Patterns Associated with Psychologic Distress Among Middle-Aged Korean Women

Health Behavioral Patterns Associated with Psychologic Distress Among Middle-Aged Korean Women ORIGINAL ARTICLE Health Behavioral Patterns Associated with Psychologic Distress Among Middle-Aged Korean Women Hye-Sook Shin 1, PhD, RN, Jia Lee 2 *, PhD, RN, Kyung-Hee Lee 3, PhD, RN, Young-A Song 4,

More information

Evidence for shared genetic influences on selfreported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian twins

Evidence for shared genetic influences on selfreported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian twins Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Open Access Publications 2008 Evidence for shared genetic influences on selfreported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian

More information

The Heritability of Introversion: Nature vs. Nurture. James T. Kashima DNA and Evolution 12:30 Section Spring 2014

The Heritability of Introversion: Nature vs. Nurture. James T. Kashima DNA and Evolution 12:30 Section Spring 2014 The Heritability of Introversion: Nature vs. Nurture James T. Kashima DNA and Evolution 12:30 Section Spring 2014 Abstract I tested whether the level of introversion was heritable by having 100 students

More information

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement Psychological Methods 2007, Vol. 12, No. 4, 451 466 CORRECTED APRIL 10, 2008; SEE LAST PAGE Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 1082-989X/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.4.451

More information

Gender Differences and Developmental Change in Externalizing Disorders From Late Adolescence to Early Adulthood: A Longitudinal Twin Study

Gender Differences and Developmental Change in Externalizing Disorders From Late Adolescence to Early Adulthood: A Longitudinal Twin Study Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 116, No. 3, 433 447 0021-843X/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.433 Gender Differences and Developmental

More information

J. Sleep Res. (2012) Regular Research Paper

J. Sleep Res. (2012) Regular Research Paper J. Sleep Res. (2012) Regular Research Paper Modeling the direction of causation between cross-sectional measures of disrupted sleep, anxiety and depression in a sample of male and female Australian twins

More information

ARTICLE. Genetic and Environmental Influences on Individual Differences in Sedentary Behavior During Adolescence

ARTICLE. Genetic and Environmental Influences on Individual Differences in Sedentary Behavior During Adolescence ONLINE FIRST ARTICLE Genetic and Environmental Influences on Individual Differences in Sedentary Behavior During Adolescence A Twin-Family Study Niels van der Aa, PhD; Meike Bartels, PhD; Saskia J. te

More information

Perceived Stress has Genetic Influences Distinct from Neuroticism and Depression

Perceived Stress has Genetic Influences Distinct from Neuroticism and Depression Behav Genet (2014) 44:639 645 DOI 10.1007/s10519-013-9636-4 ORIGINAL RESEARCH Perceived Stress has Genetic Influences Distinct from Neuroticism and Depression Liz Rietschel Gu Zhu Clemens Kirschbaum Jana

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Religiousness: Findings for Retrospective and Current Religiousness Ratings

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Religiousness: Findings for Retrospective and Current Religiousness Ratings Genetic and Environmental Influences on Religiousness: Findings for Retrospective and Current Religiousness Ratings Laura B. Koenig, Matt McGue, Robert F. Krueger, and Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr. University

More information

The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for three measures of disordered eating

The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for three measures of disordered eating Psychological Medicine, 1999, 29, 925 934. 1999 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for three measures of disordered eating

More information

Comparison between high school students in cognitive and affective coping Strategies

Comparison between high school students in cognitive and affective coping Strategies Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 46 ( 2012 ) 289 293 WCES 2012 Comparison between high school students in cognitive and affective coping Strategies Ezat

More information

Genetic Influences in Irritable Bowel Syndrome: ATwin Study

Genetic Influences in Irritable Bowel Syndrome: ATwin Study American Journal of Gastroenterology ISSN 0002-9270 C 2005 by Am. Coll. of Gastroenterology doi: 10.1111/j.1572-0241.2005.41700.x Published by Blackwell Publishing Genetic Influences in Irritable Bowel

More information

Twin singleton differences in intelligence?

Twin singleton differences in intelligence? Twin Research (2000) 3, 83 87 2000 Macmillan Publishers Ltd All rights reserved 1369 0523/00 $15.00 y www.nature.com/tr Twin singleton differences in intelligence? Daniëlle Posthuma 1, Eco JC De Geus 1,

More information

AN INTRODUCTION TO EPIGENETICS DR CHLOE WONG

AN INTRODUCTION TO EPIGENETICS DR CHLOE WONG AN INTRODUCTION TO EPIGENETICS DR CHLOE WONG MRC SGDP CENTRE, INSTITUTE OF PSYCHIATRY KING S COLLEGE LONDON Oct 2015 Lecture Overview WHY WHAT EPIGENETICS IN PSYCHIARTY Technology-driven genomics research

More information

Genetic Influences on Midlife Functioning

Genetic Influences on Midlife Functioning 03-Willis-4658.qxd 3/24/2005 2:12 PM Page 65 Three Genetic Influences on Midlife Functioning Nancy L. Pedersen, Erica Spotts, and Kenji Kato Regardless of the definition of midlife, most of the research

More information

Learning Abilities and Disabilities

Learning Abilities and Disabilities CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Learning Abilities and Disabilities Generalist Genes, Specialist Environments Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry,

More information

The Relationship Between the Genetic and Environmental Influences on Common Internalizing Psychiatric Disorders and Mental Well-Being

The Relationship Between the Genetic and Environmental Influences on Common Internalizing Psychiatric Disorders and Mental Well-Being Behav Genet (2011) 41:641 650 DOI 10.1007/s10519-011-9466-1 ORIGINAL RESEARCH The Relationship Between the Genetic and Environmental Influences on Common Internalizing Psychiatric Disorders and Mental

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG Stress Coping of Patients with Substance use Disorder in Latvia

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG Stress Coping of Patients with Substance use Disorder in Latvia Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 303 308 WCPCG 2014 Stress Coping of Patients with Substance use Disorder in Latvia Velga Sudraba

More information

Factor Structure of the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations (CISS) in Japanese Workers

Factor Structure of the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations (CISS) in Japanese Workers Psychology, 2014, 5, 1620-1628 Published Online September 2014 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.514172 Factor Structure of the Coping Inventory for Stressful

More information

Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures

Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures Journal of Substance Abuse, 5, 227-246 (7993) Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures Andrew C. Heath Departments of Psychiatry, Psychology

More information

STUDY ON THE CORRELATION BETWEEN SELF-ESTEEM, COPING AND CLINICAL SYMPTOMS IN A GROUP OF YOUNG ADULTS: A BRIEF REPORT

STUDY ON THE CORRELATION BETWEEN SELF-ESTEEM, COPING AND CLINICAL SYMPTOMS IN A GROUP OF YOUNG ADULTS: A BRIEF REPORT STUDY ON THE CORRELATION BETWEEN SELF-ESTEEM, COPING AND CLINICAL SYMPTOMS IN A GROUP OF YOUNG ADULTS: A BRIEF REPORT Giulia Savarese, PhD Luna Carpinelli, MA Oreste Fasano, PhD Monica Mollo, PhD Nadia

More information

Genetic and environmental risk factors for the weight and shape concerns characteristic of bulimia nervosa

Genetic and environmental risk factors for the weight and shape concerns characteristic of bulimia nervosa Psychological Medicine, 1998, 28, 761 771. 1998 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom Genetic and environmental risk factors for the weight and shape concerns characteristic of bulimia

More information

GENETICAL, ENVIRONMENTAL AND PERSONALITY FACTORS INFLUENCING THE AGE OF FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE IN TWINS

GENETICAL, ENVIRONMENTAL AND PERSONALITY FACTORS INFLUENCING THE AGE OF FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE IN TWINS J. biosoc. Sci. (1977) 9, 91-97 GENETICAL, ENVIRONMENTAL AND PERSONALITY FACTORS INFLUENCING THE AGE OF FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE IN TWINS N. G. MARTIN*, L. J. EAVES* AND H. J. EYSENCKt * Department of

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Literacy and Numeracy Performance in Australian School Children in Grades 3, 5, 7, and 9

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Literacy and Numeracy Performance in Australian School Children in Grades 3, 5, 7, and 9 Genetic and Environmental Influences on Literacy and Performance in Australian School Children in Grades 3, 5, 7, and 9 Katrina L. Grasby, William L. Coventry, Brian Byrne, Richard K. Olson & Sarah E.

More information

Causes of individual differences in adolescent optimism: a study in Dutch twins and their siblings

Causes of individual differences in adolescent optimism: a study in Dutch twins and their siblings Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2015) 24:1381 1388 DOI 10.1007/s00787-015-0680-x ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Causes of individual differences in adolescent optimism: a study in Dutch twins and their siblings Rezan

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. The Heritability of Bipolar Affective Disorder and the Genetic Relationship to Unipolar Depression

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. The Heritability of Bipolar Affective Disorder and the Genetic Relationship to Unipolar Depression ORIGINAL ARTICLE The Heritability of Bipolar Affective Disorder and the Genetic Relationship to Unipolar Depression Peter McGuffin, MB, PhD, FRCP, FRCPsych; Fruhling Rijsdijk, PhD; Martin Andrew, MB, MRCPsych;

More information

Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality: A Longitudinal Multiple-Rater Twin Study

Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality: A Longitudinal Multiple-Rater Twin Study Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 98, No. 6, 995 1008 0022-3514/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019558 Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality:

More information

A twin study of the relationship between inhibitory control and behavior problems

A twin study of the relationship between inhibitory control and behavior problems Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 233 ( 2016 ) 165 169 Annual International Scientific Conference Early Childhood Care and Education, ECCE

More information

lescence and young adulthood. About 70% to almost 90% of the year-old and year-old males and between 52% and 71% of the year-old

lescence and young adulthood. About 70% to almost 90% of the year-old and year-old males and between 52% and 71% of the year-old The aim of the current thesis was to examine the etiology of individual differences in alcohol use in adolescents and young adults. In this chapter we discuss the main findings presented in this thesis.

More information

concerns in a non-clinical sample

concerns in a non-clinical sample Shame, depression and eating concerns 1 Gee, A. & Troop, N.A. (2003). Shame, depressive symptoms and eating, weight and shape concerns in a non-clinical sample. Eating and Weight Disorders, 8, 72-75. Shame,

More information

EFFECTS OF STRESS ACROSS GENERATIONS: WHY SEX MATTERS

EFFECTS OF STRESS ACROSS GENERATIONS: WHY SEX MATTERS Commentary submitted to Biological Psychiatry EFFECTS OF STRESS ACROSS GENERATIONS: WHY SEX MATTERS Invited commentary on: Saavedra-Rodriguez L, Feig LA (2012): Chronic Social Instability Induces Anxiety

More information

The role of conduct disorder in explaining the comorbidity between alcohol and illicit drug dependence in adolescence

The role of conduct disorder in explaining the comorbidity between alcohol and illicit drug dependence in adolescence Drug and Alcohol Dependence 87 (2007) 46 53 The role of conduct disorder in explaining the comorbidity between alcohol and illicit drug dependence in adolescence Tanya M.M. Button a,, Soo Hyun Rhee a,b,

More information

Identical Twins, Identical Fates? An Introduction to Epigenetics

Identical Twins, Identical Fates? An Introduction to Epigenetics Identical Twins, Identical Fates? An Introduction to Epigenetics by Sarah A. Wojiski School of Arts and Sciences Massachusetts College of Pharmacy and Health Sciences, Boston, MA Part I Coming Home Elise

More information

PERSONALITY OF POLISH SOLDIERS AND THEIR WAY OF STRESS COPING DURING BOSNIA PEACEKEEPING MISSION

PERSONALITY OF POLISH SOLDIERS AND THEIR WAY OF STRESS COPING DURING BOSNIA PEACEKEEPING MISSION PERSONALITY OF POLISH SOLDIERS AND THEIR WAY OF STRESS COPING DURING BOSNIA PEACEKEEPING MISSION Olaf Truszczynski, Jan Terelak, Mariusz Turek Polish Air Force Institute of Aviation Medicine, Department

More information

The Genetic Basis of Problem Behavior in 5-Year-Old Dutch Twin Pairs

The Genetic Basis of Problem Behavior in 5-Year-Old Dutch Twin Pairs Behavior Genetics, Vol. 34, No. 3, May 2004 ( 2004) The Genetic Basis of Problem Behavior in 5-Year-Old Dutch Twin Pairs C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt, 1,4 F. C. Verhulst, 2 P. C. M. Molenaar, 3 and D. I.

More information

What can we learn from twins about study and subject choice?

What can we learn from twins about study and subject choice? What can we learn from twins about study and subject choice? Kathryn Asbury Psychology in Education Research Centre, Department of Education, University of York How do we know about genetic influences

More information

Acknowledgements. Illness Behavior A cognitive and behavioral phenomenon. Seeking medical care. Communicating pain to others

Acknowledgements. Illness Behavior A cognitive and behavioral phenomenon. Seeking medical care. Communicating pain to others Acknowledgements Parent Training to Address Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: A Researcher s Perspective Dr. Kim Swanson National Institutes of Health Rona L. Levy, MSW, PhD, MPH Professor and Director

More information

Social Capital, Social Cohesion and Health.

Social Capital, Social Cohesion and Health. Social Capital, Social Cohesion and Health. Ichiro Kawachi, M.D., Ph.D. Professor of Social Epidemiology Chair, Dept of Society, Human Development & Health Harvard School of Public Health Conceptual approaches

More information

Resemblance between Relatives (Part 2) Resemblance Between Relatives (Part 2)

Resemblance between Relatives (Part 2) Resemblance Between Relatives (Part 2) Resemblance Between Relatives (Part 2) Resemblance of Full-Siblings Additive variance components can be estimated using the covariances of the trait values for relatives that do not have dominance effects.

More information

Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study

Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study JAMES J. HUDZIAK, M.D., WILLIAM COPELAND, B.A., LAWRENCE P. RUDIGER, PH.D., THOMAS M. ACHENBACH, PH.D., ANDREW C. HEATH, D.PHIL., AND RICHARD

More information

White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: Version: Accepted Version

White Rose Research Online URL for this paper:  Version: Accepted Version This is a repository copy of Perfectionism and maladaptive coping styles in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome, irritable bowel syndrome and fibromyalgia/arthritis and in healthy controls.. White Rose

More information

Predicting Offspring Conduct Disorder Using Parental Alcohol and Drug Dependence

Predicting Offspring Conduct Disorder Using Parental Alcohol and Drug Dependence Predicting Offspring Conduct Disorder Using Parental Alcohol and Drug Dependence Paul T. Korte, B.A. J. Randolph Haber, Ph.D. Abstract Introduction: Previous research has shown that the offspring of parents

More information

MENTAL TOUGHNESS: IS IT THE KEY TO ACADEMIC SUCCESS?

MENTAL TOUGHNESS: IS IT THE KEY TO ACADEMIC SUCCESS? Teaching & Learning Frances Hunt, Lee Usher, Liory Fern Pollak, Rosemary Stock, Siobhan Lynam, Moira Cachia University of West London, UK MENTAL TOUGHNESS: IS IT THE KEY TO ACADEMIC SUCCESS? This paper

More information

The genetic basis of the relation between speed-of-information-processing and IQ

The genetic basis of the relation between speed-of-information-processing and IQ Behavioural Brain Research 95 (1998) 77 84 The genetic basis of the relation between speed-of-information-processing and IQ F.V. Rijsdijk a, *, P.A. Vernon b, D.I. Boomsma a a Department of Psychonomics,

More information

PATIENTS PERCEPTIONS OF ILLNESS and TREATMENT. Applications to Cystinosis

PATIENTS PERCEPTIONS OF ILLNESS and TREATMENT. Applications to Cystinosis PATIENTS PERCEPTIONS OF ILLNESS and TREATMENT Applications to Cystinosis John Weinman Institute of Psychiatry & Institute of Pharmaceutical Sciences King s College London. OUTLINE INTRODUCTION TO ILLNESS

More information