Personality and Coping: A Study of Twins Reared Apart and Twins Reared Together

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Personality and Coping: A Study of Twins Reared Apart and Twins Reared Together"

Transcription

1 Behavior Genetics, Vol. 35, No. 2, March 2005 (Ó 2005) Personality and Coping: A Study of Twins Reared Apart and Twins Reared Together Kenji Kato 1,*, and Nancy L. Pedersen 1,2 Received 20 Nov Final 30 July 2004 The relative importance of genetic and environmental factors for stress coping styles, age and gender differences, and the relationship between coping styles and personality traits were assessed in middle-aged and older adult twins reared apart and reared together, as part of the ongoing Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging (SATSA). The Billings and Moos Coping Measure was administered to 1339 individual twins (in 446 intact pairs). The mean age was 58.0 ± Moderate genetic influences and significant gender differences in variance estimates were found for the three coping scales (Problem Solving, Turning to Others, and Avoidance). Turning to Others and Avoidance in women also showed shared rearing environmental influences. In contrast, no age differences in variance estimates were found in this sample. Multivariate model fitting indicated that genetic influences on adults coping differentially reflect genetic factors in common with personality traits. The sources of covariation also showed significant gender differences. KEY WORDS: Coping; gender difference; personality; twins. 1 Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden. 2 Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California, USA. * To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, P.O. Box 281, SE , Stockholm, Sweden. Tel: Fax: Kenji.Kato@meb.ki.se 147 INTRODUCTION Coping strategies are cognitive, behavioral or physiological processes aimed at diminishing or terminating stress. While adaptive coping can ameliorate the consequences of stress, maladaptive coping strategies can lead to an increased risk for chronic diseases such as high blood pressure, diabetes, and heart disease (Maes et al., 1996). Furthermore, extremes in coping styles can influence susceptibility to psychiatric disorders, particularly depression and certain anxiety disorders. Understanding individual differences in coping therefore may have implications for prevention or intervention. Despite the considerable attention to coping in the psychological literature in the last two decades, very few studies have been conducted thus far that have explicitly focused on genetic and environmental sources of individual differences in coping. Coping is a multidimensional construct that is not adequately represented by a single measure, and numerous inventories to assess coping have been proposed (Schwarzer and Schwarzer, 1996). Although little agreement exists regarding the optimal conceptualization of coping, a problem-focused and emotion-focused terminology has been frequently used among researchers (Lazarus, 1993). In addition, factor-analytic studies of coping have repeatedly identified three factor solutions such as problem solving, seeking support, and avoidance (Amirkhan, 1990), task-oriented, emotion-oriented, and avoidance-oriented (Endler and Parker, 1990), or cognitive self-control, solace seeking, and ineffective escapism (Rohde et al., 1990). There is a growing amount of evidence that indicates the importance of personality dispositions in predicting and explaining variance in coping style /05/ /0 Ó 2005 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.

2 148 Kato and Pedersen and its consequences (Costa et al., 1996). Some consensus has been reached supporting the use of the five-factor model as a framework for research on the relationship between personality and coping (e.g., McWilliams et al., 2003; O Brien and DeLongis, 1996). In particular, Neuroticism and Extraversion have been prominent in the literature, owing largely to Hans Eysenck s influential work (Costa et al., 1996). For example, Neuroticism is associated with self-blaming, wishful thinking, becoming passive and withdrawn, whereas Extraversion is correlated with talking, positive thinking, and restraint. Although less prominent, there is also some evidence that Openness to Experience is related to specific coping styles (Costa et al., 1996). People with high Openness to Experience rethink the problem and seek for new information. It is reasonable to speculate that individual differences in coping styles reflect individuals different personality dispositions. Gender may be a potential source of individual differences in coping styles. Gender differences in mean levels have been broadly reported in the stress and coping literature (Beasley et al., 2003). For example, in their study of depressed patients, Billings and Moos (1984) found that women used emotional discharge more frequently than men. The greater depressiveness of women might be due partly to their less effective coping (Hanninen and Aro, 1996). In addition, age could also be an origin of individual differences in coping. Findings about age differences in mean levels are, however, complex and puzzling. Some studies have suggested that differences in coping exist among young, middle-aged, and elderly persons (e.g., Aldwin, 1991), however, the strength and direction of the age-coping association was not clear (Strack and Feifel, 1996). A longitudinal study revealed no consistent age-related differences in the use of coping (McCrae, 1989). In any case, finding differences in means does not necessarily imply differences in variation. To scrutinize the origins of individual differences, one must employ study designs with genetically informative samples, such as twin, adoption, or family studies. There has been a surprisingly limited number of behavioral genetic studies on stress coping. Kendler et al. (1991) reported the first results of behavioral genetic analysis on coping among young adult female twins (mean age 29 ±8 years). Mellins et al. (1996) also investigated coping among schoolaged twins. In addition, Busjahn et al. (1999) reported results among young adult twins (mean age 34 ± 14 years). These three studies all show evidence of genetic influences on most coping scales. To date, however, no study has been reported concerning genetic and environmental influences on coping in middle age or an older adult sample. Considering the putative effects of adaptive and maladaptive coping on health status or the severity of physical and psychiatric diseases, it would be worthwhile to broaden our knowledge of individual differences in coping with an older and larger sample with both genders. To explore the sources of variance in adults coping, the present study employed an adoptive twin study design. This study is part of the ongoing Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging (SATSA, Pedersen et al., 1991), a substudy of the Swedish Twin Registry (Lichtenstein et al., 2002; Pedersen et al., 2002). The uniqueness of SATSA is that it combines the twin and adoption study designs, by including twin pairs who were separated in their early childhood (twins reared apart) as well as twin pairs who were reared in the same family (twins reared together). Inclusion of twins reared apart permits differentiation between influences of the environment shared during the rearing period (shared rearing environment) and those contributing to similarity even after separation (correlated environment). Considering that coping styles have proved to be heritable in children and younger adults, we hypothesized that coping styles would be heritable in middle-aged and older adults. The literature also led us to postulate that shared environmental influences on coping would be limited, and the majority of the variance would be of the non-shared variety. Because so little is known about gender differences in genetic and environmental factors for coping, it is difficult to propose hypotheses concerning gender differences in the present study. However, it is not unlikely that there are gender differences in these factors, in that such differences have been reported in other SATSA studies of health and stress-related phenotypes (Finkel et al., 2003; Gatz et al., 1992; Lichtenstein and Pedersen, 1995; Pedersen et al., 1989; Saudino et al., 1997) as well as by others (e.g., Bierut et al., 1999; Kendler et al., 2001). As mentioned above, personality dispositions are likely to play an important role in predicting variance in coping. It is also true that numerous studies have demonstrated genetic influences on personality traits throughout the lifespan (Eaves et al., 1989; Loehlin, 1992). Further, there is some evidence that genetic influences on personality are

3 Personality and Coping 149 important for other purported measures related to environmental responses (Saudino et al., 1997). Thus, given these considerations, it is possible that at least some of the genetic influence for coping, if such variance exists in middle-aged and older adults, are attributable to genetic factors for personality. METHODS Participants This study is part of the ongoing SATSA (Pedersen et al., 1991), which is a longitudinal program of gerontological genetics. The SATSA sample is a subset of the population-based Swedish Twin Registry (Lichtenstein et al., 2002; Pedersen et al., 2002). SATSA consists of same-sex twins who were separated before age 11 and reared apart and a sample of twins who were reared together matched on gender, age, and county of birth. Participants in SATSA received mail-out questionnaires in 1984, 1987, 1990, and Details on SATSA were described elsewhere (Pedersen et al., 1991). The present data were gathered during the third wave of questionnaires sent in October This was the only occasion of SATSA that included a coping measure. Of 1339 individual twins who responded to the questionnaire, both members of 58 pairs of MZ (monozygotic) twins reared apart, 101 pairs of MZ twins reared together, 147 pairs of DZ (dizygotic) twins reared apart, and 140 pairs of DZ twins reared together responded to the questions on stress coping. The sample consisted of 795 women (59.4%) and 544 men (40.6%). The mean age of the twins was 58.0 ± 12.8 as of January 1, 1990, and 71.9% of the pairs were over 50 years of age. The mean ages of female and male twins were 58.6 ± 13.4 and 57.1 ± 12.0, respectively. In the analysis of age differences in variance estimates, the twins were classified into two subgroups cut at the median age of the pair respondents (58.19 years old) so that the pair respondents can be equally distributed in these two subgroups. The mean ages of younger subgroup and older subgroup were 45.8 ± 8.2 and 67.5 ± 5.8, respectively. Zygosity was determined on the basis of a series of questions regarding physical similarity, such as During childhood, were you and your twin partner as alike as two peas in a pod? and How often did strangers have difficulty in distinguishing between you and your twin partner when you were children? Measures Stress coping Stress coping was measured with a 33-item questionnaire based on Billings and Moos (1984). All the items were in Swedish. Table I shows the back translation of the questionnaire. Responses were recorded on a four-point Likert-type frequency scale. The options were:never, Seldom, Now and then, andoften. Confirmatory factor analyses were performed first by using PROC CALIS procedure in SAS version 8 (SAS Institute, 2002) for the purpose of testing the Billings and Moos structure of coping scales. Exploratory factor analysis was then carried out by the method of principal components with promax rotation using the PROC FACTOR procedure in SAS. Single respondents and one randomly selected twin of each pair were used in the exploratory factor analysis. Components that had eigenvalues greater than 1.0 were selected. In subsequent analyses, all available data (both single respondents and pair respondents) were used. Additional confirmatory factor analyses were performed by using Mx (Neale et al., 1999) in order to check whether non-independence of twin pair observations distorted the factor solution that was obtained above. Unit weight scale scores were formed by assigning each item to the component on which the item loaded most heavily (Table I). Items with factor loading under 0.30 were omitted in the summation of scale scores, resulting in 27 items in total. For individuals who missed responding to one item for a particular scale, the missing value was substituted by the mean of the rest of the individual s responses for the particular scale. Individuals having more than one missing item for a particular scale were excluded from the calculation of scale scores. Personality Extraversion and Neuroticism traits were measured by a short form of the Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI, Eysenck and Eysenck, 1975), which has been widely used in Scandinavian twin studies (Floderus-Myrhed et al., 1980; Pedersen et al., 1988; Viken et al., 1994). Each scale score was based on the sum of 0 and 1 (No Yes) responses to nine

4 150 Kato and Pedersen Table I. Items of Stress Coping Questionnaire and Factor Loadings Item Factor loading Problem Solving 1. I try to find out more about the situation I try to see it from the positive side I make a plan of action and follow it I consider alternative solutions for handling the problem I rely on my past experience since I was in a similar situation before I take things one at a time I try to step back from the situation and be more objective I think through the situation in order to understand it better I try not to act hastily or follow my first hunch I know what must be done and I make an effort to make things work I compromise to get something positive from the situation Turning to Others 2. I talk with my spouse or other relative about the problem I talk with a friend about the problem I seek help, for example, from a doctor, lawyer or clergyman I discuss my feelings with others I take it out on others when I get angry or depressed I let my feelings out in one way or another I seek help from others with similar experiences Avoidance 6. I prepare for the worst I tell myself things that help me feel better I get away from things for a while I avoid being with people on the whole I promise myself that things will be different next time I refuse to believe that it happened I accept that what is is; nothing can be done I try to reduce tension by eating more I try to reduce tension by taking more tranquilizing drugs Note: The instrument starts with the statement When I have a problem/feel stressed... items. Openness to Experience was measured by a shortened version of the NEO-Personality Inventory (NEO-PI, Bergeman et al., 1993; Costa and McCrae, 1985). The six items of this scale were scored on a five-point Likert-type frequency scale ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree), and then summed for a total score. which parameters should be included in subsequent structural equation modeling. In the calculation of intraclass correlations, only the pair respondents were included and a double-entry technique was used in order to avoid possible bias introduced by non-random assignment of twins as twin A or twin B. Statistical Analyses Equality of variances was tested between MZ and DZ twins or twins reared apart and reared together by using PROC TTEST procedure in SAS. Intraclass correlations The intraclass correlations for MZ and DZ pairs were calculated to provide an indication of Univariate model fitting Maximum likelihood model fitting analyses were performed to estimate genetic and environmental factors influencing the coping scales. In the present study, the models were based on the following structural equations for the four rearing-by-zygosity groups, namely, MZ twins reared apart (MZA) and reared together (MZT), and DZ twins reared apart (DZA) and reared together (DZT):

5 Personality and Coping 151 COV MZA ¼ A þ C; COV MZT ¼ A þ S þ C; COV DZA ¼ 1=2A þ C; COV DZT ¼ 1=2A þ S þ C; where COV is the covariance between twin pairs, A is additive genetic variances, S is shared rearing environmental variances, and C is correlated environmental variances. Because our sample has both reared apart and reared together twins, we can differentiate between shared rearing environmental influences and correlated environmental influences. The latter refers to all twin similarity which cannot be explained by genetic influences or shared rearing environmental influences. Examples of correlated environmental influences would be similarities in rearing environments despite being in different homes (selective placement), similarities in adult life experiences, and post-separation contact. We performed model fitting by using raw data of the coping scales separately for men and women with age as a moderator (definition) variable, and for younger and older subgroups with gender as a moderator variable. All twins were included in the analyses, regardless of the response status of the cotwin. Maximum-likelihood estimates of the parameters and model-fit statistics such as v 2 value and Akaike s Information Criterion (AIC, Akaike, 1987) were calculated by using Mx. Age and gender differences in genetic and environmental influences were tested as follows:in the first series of analyses, we allowed all the parameters to be estimated separately by gender or by age-group. In the second series of analyses, we constrained these parameters to be equal across men and women or across younger and older subgroups, and compared the unconstrained full model and the nested constrained model. A significant difference in v 2 goodness-of-fit indicates that the constrained (nested) model fits the data worse than the full model. In the third series of analyses, specific parameters were dropped. Among the nested models which fit the data, the best-fit model was chosen by using AIC, which reflects both the goodness-of-fit and parsimony of the model. Multivariate Model Fitting Multivariate model fitting analyses were carried out to explore the extent to which genetic and environmental factors that influence personality traits also are important for coping styles. In each model, we included one of the three coping scales and the personality scales that were significantly (p < 0.05) correlated with that particular coping scale for both men and women. Cholesky decomposition models consisting of these variables were fitted to the raw data by using Mx. Age was included in the models as a definition variable. The same multivariate models were run for men and women. First, we allowed all the parameters to be estimated separately by gender. We then constrained these parameters to be equal across men and women, and compared the unconstrained full model and the nested constrained model. The importance of genetic factors in common to coping and the personality measures was tested by dropping the oblique paths. The importance of environmental covariance was tested in a similar fashion. Best-fit models were selected using AIC criteria. RESULTS Factor Analysis The v 2 fit of confirmatory factor analysis for the Billings and Moos structure was (df = 339). In the exploratory factor analysis, three factors were identified by the traditional eigenvalue criteria. Each item of the questionnaire was assigned to the factor-derived scale on which the item loaded most heavily (Table I). Because the difference in fit functions of confirmatory factor analyses using SAS suggested that our model fits better than the Billings and Moos structure (Dv 2 = 203.7, Ddf = 18), we disregarded their classification and employed our own terminology. Additional confirmatory factor analyses using Mx did not indicate distorted factor solution due to non-independence of twin pair observations. The first factor, labeled Problem Solving, reflected a positive, practical coping. The second factor, labeled Turning to Others, reflected coping by seeking support from others. The third factor, labeled Avoidance, reflected a negative, escapist coping. Table I shows the assignment of items to factors. Chronbach s alphas for Problem Solving, Turning to Others, and Avoidance were 0.82, 0.73, and 0.68, respectively. The promax-derived interfactor correlations were 0.40 between Problem Solving and Turning to Others, 0.16 between Problem Solving and Avoidance, and 0.07 between Turning to Others and Avoidance. Scale scores of these factors were mod-

6 152 Kato and Pedersen erately correlated: 0.40 between Problem Solving and Turning to Others, 0.24 between Problem Solving and Avoidance, and 0.20 between Turning to Others and Avoidance. All the correlation coefficients were significantly greater than zero at p < Total variances did not significantly differ for MZ and DZ twins and for twins reared apart and reared together in both men and women, except Turning to Others in men, indicating the fulfillment of a basic assumption for the twin method. Variances for Turning to Others for DZ and for twins reared apart in men were significantly greater than those for MZ and for twins reared together, respectively. The difference will reduce the fit of structural models in the case of this scale. Intraclass Correlations The intraclass correlations for each of the four rearing-by-zygosity groups by gender are shown in Table II. The correlations for MZ twins were consistently greater than those for DZ twins, suggesting moderate genetic influences for all the three scales. The intraclass correlations for MZ twins reared together were less than twice those for DZ twins reared together in women, suggesting correlated environmental contributions to twin similarity. of total variation for additive genetic (A), shared rearing environmental (S), correlated environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) components of variance. Change in goodness-of-fit statistics from the full model is also shown in the right-hand columns of the table. When the model was constrained to be equal across men and women, the goodness-offit was worsened significantly for all the three scales, indicating significant gender differences in the relative importance of genetic and environmental parameters. For Avoidance, the estimated additive genetic variation for women was substantially larger than that for men. Correlated environments were estimated as zero for all scales. Shared rearing environments were significant for Turning to Others and Avoidance in women. When the model was constrained to be equal across younger and older subgroups, none of the goodness-of-fit statistics was worsened significantly (Dv 2 = 5.02, Ddf = 4, p = 0.29, AIC = 2.98 for Problem Solving; Dv 2 = 6.52, Ddf = 4, p = 0.16, AIC = 1.48 for Turning to Others; and Dv 2 = 6.15, Ddf = 4, p = 0.19, AIC = 1.85 for Avoidance). These results suggest no age differences in parameter estimates for the three coping scales. We therefore focused on gender differences in subsequent multivariate analyses. Univariate Model Fitting Results of model fitting for the three coping scales by gender are summarized in Table III, with goodnessof-fit statistics, parameter estimates, and percentages Table II. Intraclass Correlations for Coping Scales by Gender, Zygosity, and Rearing Status Men Women MZA MZT DZA DZT MZA MZT DZA DZT Problem Solving n Turning to Others n Avoidance n Note: n = number of pairs; MZA = monozygotic twins reared apart; MZT = monozygotic twins reared together; DZA = dizygotic twins reared apart; DZT = dizygotic twins reared together. *p < 0.05; **p < Multivariate Model Fitting Table IV shows the phenotypic correlations between the coping scales and the personality scales (Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness to Experience) by gender. In general, high scores in Problem Solving and Turning to Others were positively correlated with high scores in Openness to Experience and Extraversion. High scores in Avoidance were correlated with high scores in Neuroticism and with low scores in Extraversion. The choice of scales to be included in the multivariate models was based on these results. Genetic and environmental factors to be included in each model were based on the results of the preceding univariate analyses. That is, AE model was tested for all scales in men and Problem Solving in women, whereas ASE model was tested for Turning to Others and Avoidance in women. Figures 1, 2, and 3 illustrate the best-fit models for the association between coping scales and their correlated personality scales by gender. For Problem Solving (Figs. 1a and 1b), genetic

7 Personality and Coping 153 Table III. Estimates of Percentages of Total Variation (95% CI) and Goodness-of-fit Statistics for Coping Scales by Gender Men Women Changes from full model Factors A S C E A S C E )2ln(L) Dv 2 Ddf p AIC Problem Solving 1. Full model 0.30 ( ) 0.00 ( ) 0.00 ( ) 0.70 ( ) 0.29 ( ) 0.07 ( )0.14 ( ) 0.50 ( ) Men and women 0.38 ( ) 0.01 ( ) 0.04 ( ) 0.57 ( ) equated 3. Best-fit model 0.30 ( ) 0.70 ( ) 0.51 ( ) 0.49 ( ) )4.08 Turning to Others 1. Full model 0.21 ( ) 0.00 ( ) 0.00 ( ) 0.79 ( ) 0.40 ( ) 0.16 ( )0.00 ( ) 0.44 ( ) Men and women 0.37 ( ) 0.06 ( ) 0.00 ( ) 0.56 ( ) equated 3. Best-fit model 0.21 ( ) 0.79 ( ) 0.40 ( )0.16 ( ) 0.44 ( ) )6.00 Avoidance 1. Full model 0.15 ( ) 0.00 ( ) 0.00 ( ) 0.85 ( ) 0.48 ( ) 0.14 ( )0.00 ( ) 0.38 ( ) Men and women 0.34 ( ) 0.06 ( ) 0.02 ( ) 0.58 ( ) equated 3. Best-fit model 0.15 ( ) 0.85 ( ) 0.48 ( ) 0.14 ( ) 0.38 ( ) )6.00 Note: A = additive genetic effect, S = shared rearing environmental effect; C = correlated environmental effect; E = non-shared environmental effect; 2 ln(l) ¼ 2 times the natural log likelihood; df = degrees of freedom; AIC = Akaike s information criterion. Table IV. Phenotypic Correlations between Coping Scales and Personality Scales Personality scales Coping scales Neuroticism Extraversion Problem Solving Turning to Others Openness to Experience Men Women Men Women Avoidance Men Women Note: *p < 0.05; **p < variation was entirely explained by one genetic factor in common to Openness to Experience in the best-fit model in men (Dv 2 = 2.05, Ddf = 4, p = 0.73, AIC = 5.95), whereas the full model fit best in women. For Turning to Others (Figs. 2a and 2b), the best-fit model had no genetic factor in common across the three scales in men (Dv 2 = 1.93, Ddf = 3, p = 0.59, AIC = 4.07), whereas a common genetic factor and a unique factor were included in the best-fit model in women (Dv 2 = 1.76, Ddf = 4, p = 0.78, AIC = 6.24). For Avoidance (Figs. 3a and b), genetic variation was entirely explained by two genetic factors in common to Neuroticism and Extraversion in men (Dv 2 = 0.59, Ddf = 1, p = 0.44, AIC = 1.41), whereas a common genetic factor as well as a unique factor remained in the best-fit model in women (Dv 2 = 1.65, Ddf = 6, p = 0.95, AIC = 10.36). When all the paths were constrained to be equal across gender, the constrained (nested) models fit significantly worse than the unconstrained full models (Dv 2 = 51.20, Ddf = 11, p < for Problem Solving; Dv 2 = 70.88, Ddf= 17, p < for Turning to Others; and Dv 2 = 73.79, Ddf = 18, p < for Avoidance), suggesting gender differences in patterns of covariation between personality and coping. Of the total genetic variation of each coping scale, the genetic factor unique to coping accounted for 0% for Problem Solving and Avoidance in men, 68% for Avoidance in women, 83% for Problem Solving in women, 90% for Turning to Others in women, and 100% for Turning to Others in men.

8 154 Kato and Pedersen Fig. 1. The best-fit model for the association between personality scales and Problem Solving in (a) men and (b) women. A = additive genetic factor; E = non-shared environmental factor. Figure 4 presents estimates of genetic and environmental contributions to the phenotypic correlations between each coping scale and personality scale. Genetic contributions were calculated by multiplying path coefficients of the genetic factors common to both the personality scale and the coping scale and summing them. Environmental contributions to the phenotypic correlation were calculated in a similar fashion. The consistency of the estimated with the observed phenotypic correlation suggests the adequacy of our models for capturing the associations between personality and coping. DISCUSSION The present study represents one of the first attempts to examine genetic and environmental influences on coping in middle-aged and older adult sample, gender differences in those influences, and multivariate analyses of the relationship between coping styles and personality traits. There were significant gender differences in the relative importance of genetic and environmental influences on coping scales. Variances for the coping scales in women were more attributable to genetic influences than in men. Relatively high correlations were found between Problem Solving, Turning to Others, and Openness to Experience, as well as between Avoidance, Neuroticism, and Extraversion. These associations were moderately or substantially due to genetic influences in both men and women, except Turning to Others in men. It is noteworthy that similar coping styles were found in our study and two of the other twin studies (Kendler et al., 1991; Mellins et al., 1996), despite different coping questionnaires and different types of samples. In these three twin studies, the coping scales were derived from factor analyses. The three coping strategies we obtained were similar to the scales that Kendler et al. found in young women. Similarly, our coping scales were analogous to what Mellins et al. found regarding children s coping:our Turning to Others seems to be equivalent to their Use of Parents and Use of Peers, while our Avoidance is similar to their Distraction. In terms of problem-focused and emotion-focused coping, the former seems to be close to our Problem Solving, and the latter seems to be close to our Avoidance. As noted by Kendler et al. and Mellins et al., Turning to Others could be interpreted as both problem-focused (seeking for informational support) and emotion-focused (seeking for emotional support) coping strategy. Genetic and Environmental Influences on Coping As we hypothesized, significant genetic influences were found for all three coping strategies except Avoidance in men. Furthermore, the shared rearing environment showed only limited influence, which was found in Turning to Others and Avoidance in women, whereas correlated environmental influences (which reflect selective placement or postseparation contact) were not observed in our results. In general, our results are consistent with the previous twin studies on coping, with moderate heritability, limited influences of shared environment, and substantial influences of non-shared environment. We were the first to assess and find significant gender differences in the relative importance of genetic and environmental influences on coping.

9 Personality and Coping 155 Fig. 2. The best-fit model for the association between personality scales and Turning to Others in (a) men and (b) women. A = additive genetic factor; S = shared rearing environmental factor; E = non-shared environmental factor. Fig. 3. The best-fit model for the association between personality scales and Avoidance in (a) men and (b) women. A = ;additive genetic factor; S = shared rearing environmental factor; E = non-shared environmental factor. All the three coping scales showed differences in the magnitude of genetic influences between men and women. In addition, moderate but significant shared rearing environmental influences were found for Turning to Others and Avoidance in women. As mentioned previously, gender differences in variance estimates are relatively common in behavior genetic studies, particularly for health-related indicators (Finkel et al., 2003; Lichtenstein and Pedersen, 1995). In our study of Type A-like measures (Pedersen et al., 1989), a scale Pressure showed gender differences, with shared rearing environmental effects only in women. Also, Saudino et al. (1997) reported that genetic influences on measures of stressful life events were found only in women. Further, Gatz et al. (1992) found that genetic effects for a scale Depressed Mood appeared to be stronger in women than in men. Given the considerable differences in prevalence of stress-related health problems such as depression, the results of the present study as well as our previous studies would suggest that these phenotypic differences reflects different mechanisms of stress perception and response across gender. Because our SATSA participants comprise same-sexed pairs, we cannot speculate whether the consistent findings of gender differences in variance estimates reflect differences in genes operating in the coping styles for men and women.

10 156 Kato and Pedersen Fig. 4. Genetic and environmental contributions to the phenotypic correlations between personality scales and coping scales by gender. Note that the phenotypic correlations between Extraversion and Avoidance were reversed in order to facilitate the interpretation of the figure. N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; PS = Problem Solving; TO = Turning to Others; AV = Avoidance. In contrast, we did not find age differences in genetic and environmental influences on coping in these middle-aged and older adults. Our results do, however, differ somewhat from Kendler et al. (1991) who observed shared environmental influences instead of genetic influences on their denial measure, whereas their turning to others did not show shared environmental influences. On the other hand, Mellins et al. (1996) found no genetic influences on their Problem Solve but found shared environmental influences. With cross-sectional data, it is impossible to disentangle whether these inconsistencies represent age-related changes or cohort or study differences. Longitudinal data are therefore warranted in order to distinguish cohort effects from aging effects in coping. Unfortunately, the coping measure was only assessed at one occasion in SATSA. Coping Styles and Personality Traits As we hypothesized, genetic influences on coping scales were partly attributable to genetic factors in common with one or two personality traits, except Turning to Others in men. The results suggest that individual s coping styles to some extent reflect his or her personality, in part due to the influences of the common genetic factors. Uehara et al. (1999) reported that extraversion and frustration tolerance were significantly related to task-oriented coping, and neuroticism was associated with emotion-oriented coping in depressive outpatients. While the participants of the present study were not depressive, our results go further and demonstrate that those associations are partly attributed to genes that personality and coping styles have in common. Busjahn et al. (2002) found common genetic variation between Emotional Coping and blood pressure and linkage between Active Coping and b-2 adrenergic receptor gene, suggesting a role of this gene in the relationship between psychological and physiological phenotypes. Although the present study is cross-sectional and therefore causality is not obvious, our results may provide some implication that the covariation between personality and coping is mediated by the pleiotropic function of genes. In multivariate analyses, we further found that the patterns of covariation between personality and coping scales were considerably different in men and women. As seen in Figure 4, the relative importance of genetic and environmental contributions to the phenotypic correlations also differed by gender. In general, best-fit models in women showed more complex patterns of association. On the other hand, genetic covariation for Problem Solving and for Avoidance in men was entirely accounted for by one or two genetic factors in common with personality. These findings suggest that the role of personality in determining coping strategies is gender-specific and strategy-specific, and that women s coping is likely to be influenced by more factors that are distinct from genetic effects on personality.

11 Personality and Coping 157 Thus, at least for women, coping and personality are distinct constructs but show systematic associations. This distinction is notable given that both coping and personality (Neuroticism in particular) are important predictors of health outcomes. Limitations Our method relied on self-report of the subjects, without specifying any situation. As Lazarus (1993) pointed out, this might mislead subjects to answer preferred responses instead of their actual coping styles. Self-report may also result in spurious associations among the measures due to shared method variance. However, the consistency of the present study and previous studies where situations were specified may assure the validity of our results. As often discussed in the literature on stressful life events (e.g., Kendler et al., 2001), gender differences might be caused by the differences in the recall and reporting of their behavior, not in their real action or responses. Replication may be required to establish the reliability of our results. Despite some limitations, we believe that our findings can be seen as a first attempt to explore individual differences in middle-aged and older adults coping. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging (SATSA) is supported by the National Institute on Aging grants AG and 10175, the Swedish Council for Social Research, and the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Aging. REFERENCES Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 52: Aldwin, C. M. (1991). Does age affect the stress and coping process? Implications of age differences in perceived control. J. Gerontol. 46 (4): Amirkhan, J. H. (1990). A factor analytically derived measure of coping the coping strategy indicator. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 59: Beasley, M., Thompson, T., and Davidson, J. (2003). Resilience in response to life stress:the effects of coping style and cognitive hardiness. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 34: Bergeman, C. S., Chipuer, H. M., Plomin, R., Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G. E., Nesselroade, J. R., Costa, P. T. Jr., and McCrae, R. R. (1993). Genetic and environmental effects on openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness:an adoption/twin study. J. Pers. 61 (2): Bierut, L. J., Heath, A. C., Bucholz, K. K., Dinwiddie, S. H., Madden, P. A., Statham, D. J., Dunne, M. P., and Martin, N. G. (1999). Major depressive disorder in a communitybased twin sample:are there different genetic and environmental contributions for men and women? Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 56(6): Billings, A. G. and Moos, R. H. (1984). Coping, stress, and social resources among adults with unipolar depression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46 (4): Busjahn, A., Faulhaber, H. D., Freier, K., and Luft, F. C. (1999). Genetic and environmental influences on coping styles:a twin study. Psychosom. Med. 61 (4): Busjahn, A., Freier, K., Faulhaber, H. D., Li, G. H., Rosenthal, M., Jordan, J., Hoehe, M. R., Timmermann, B., and Luft, F. C. (2002). Beta-2 adrenergic receptor gene variations and coping styles in twins. Biol. Psychol. 61 (1 2): Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO personality inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Costa, P. T., Somerfield, M. R., and McCrae, R. R. (1996). Personality and coping:a reconceptualization. In M. Zeidner and N. S. Endler (eds.), Handbook of coping: theory, research, applications New York: John Wiley & Sons. Eaves, L. J., Eysenck, H. J., and Martin, N. G. (1989). Genes, culture and personality: an empirical approach. London: Academic Press Ltd. Endler, N. S. and Parker, J. D. A. (1990). Multidimensional assessment of coping:a critical evaluation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58 (5): Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Inventory. San Diego, CA: Educational & Industrial Testing Service. Finkel, D., Pedersen, N. L., Reynolds, C. A., Berg, S., De Faire, U., and Svartengren, M. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on decline in biobehavioral markers of aging. Behav. Genet. 33(2): Floderus-Myrhed, B., Pedersen, N., and Rasmuson, I. (1980). Assessment of heritability for personality, based on a shortform of the Eysenck Personality Inventory: a study of 12,898 twin pairs. Behav. Genet. 10(2): Gatz, M., Pedersen, N. L., Plomin, R., Nesselroade, J. R., and McClearn, G. E. (1992). Importance of shared genes and shared environments for symptoms of depression in older adults. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 101 (4): Hanninen, V., and Aro, H. (1996). Sex differences in coping and depression among young adults. Soc. Sci. Med. 43 (10): Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., Neale, M. C., and Prescott, C. A. (2001). Genetic risk factors for major depression in men and women: similar or different heritabilities and same or partly distinct genes? Psychol. Med. 31 (4): Kendler, K. S., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., Neale, M. C., and Eaves, L. J. (1991). Coping:a genetic epidemiological investigation. Psychol. Med. 21 (2): Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., and Prescott, C. A. (2001). Gender differences in the rates of exposure to stressful life events and sensitivity to their depressogenic effects. Am. J. Psychiat. 158(4): Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Coping theory and research:past, present, and future. Psychosom. Med. 55(3): Lichtenstein, P., De Faire, U., Floderus, B., Svartengren, M., Svedberg, P., and Pedersen, N. L. (2002). The Swedish Twin Registry: a unique resource for clinical, epidemiological and genetic studies. J. Intern. Med. 252 (3): Lichtenstein, P., and Pedersen, N. L. (1995). Social relationships, stressful life events, and self-reported physical health: genetic and environmental influences. Psychol. Health. 10(4): Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Genes and environment in personality development. Newbury Park, CA:Sage. Maes, S., Leventhal, H., and Ridder, T. D. (1996). Coping with chronic diseases. In M. Zeidner and N. S. Endler (eds.),

12 158 Kato and Pedersen Handbook of coping: theory, research, applications New York: John Wiley & Sons. McCrae, R. R. (1989). Age differences and changes in the use of coping mechanisms. J. Gerontol. 44: McWilliams, L. A., Cox, B. J., and Enns, M. W. (2003). Use of the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations in a clinically depressed sample:factor structure, personality correlates, and prediction of distress. J. Clin. Psychol. 59(4): Mellins, C. A., Gatz, M., and Baker, L. (1996). Children s methods of coping with stress:a twin study of genetic and environmental influences. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. Allied Disciplines. 37(6): Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., and Maes, H. H. (1999). Mx:statistical modeling. Richmond: Department of Psychiatry, University of Virginia. O Brien, T. B., and DeLongis, A. (1996). The interactional context of problem-, emotion-, and relationship-focused coping: the role of the big five personality factors. J. Pers. 64 (4): Pedersen, N. L., Lichtenstein, P., Plomin, R., DeFaire, U., McClearn, G. E., and Matthews, K. A. (1989). Genetic and environmental influences for type A-like measures and related traits:a study of twins reared apart and twins reared together. Psychosom. Med. 51 (4): Pedersen, N. L., Lichtenstein, P., and Svedberg, P. (2002). The Swedish Twin Registry in the third millennium. Twin Res. 5 (5): Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G. E., Plomin, R., Nesselroade, J. R., Berg, S., and DeFaire, U. (1991). The Swedish Adoption Twin Study of Aging:an update. Acta Genet. Med. Gemel. (Roma). 40 (1):7 20. Pedersen, N. L., Plomin, R., McClearn, G. E., and Friberg, L. (1988). Neuroticism, extraversion, and related traits in adult twins reared apart and reared together. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 55 (6): Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. M., Tilson, M., and Seeley, J. R. (1990). Dimensionality of coping and its relation to depression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58 (3): SAS Institute (2002). The SAS system for Windows version 8. Cary, NC. Saudino, K. J., Pedersen, N. L., Lichtenstein, P., McClearn, G. E., and Plomin, R. (1997). Can personality explain genetic influences on life events? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72(1): Schwarzer, R., and Schwarzer, C. (1996). A critical survey of coping instruments. In M. Zeidner and N. S. Endler (eds.), Handbook of coping: theory, research, applications. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Strack, S., and Feifel, H. (1996). Age differences, coping, and the adult life span. In M. Zeidner and N. S. Endler (eds.), Handbook of coping: theory, research, applications New York: John Wiley & Sons. Uehara, T., Sakado, K., Sakado, M., Sato, T., and Someya, T. (1999). Relationship between stress coping and personality in patients with major depressive disorder. Psychother. Psychosom. 68(1): Viken, R. J., Rose, R. J., Kaprio, J., and Koskenvuo, M. (1994). A developmental genetic analysis of adult personality: extraversion and neuroticism from 18 to 59 years of age. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66(4): Edited by Wendy Slutske

Can Personality Explain Genetic Influences on Life Events?

Can Personality Explain Genetic Influences on Life Events? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1997, Vol. 72, No, 1, 196-26 Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 22-3514/97/J3. Can Personality Explain Genetic Influences on Life

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Scienc es 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 V Congress of Russian Psychological Society Genetic and Environmental Influences on

More information

THE IMPACT of regular tobacco

THE IMPACT of regular tobacco ORIGINAL ARTICLE Tobacco Consumption in Swedish Twins Reared Apart and Reared Together Kenneth S. Kendler, MD; Laura M. Thornton, PhD; Nancy L. Pedersen, PhD Background: Prior studies of twins reared together

More information

Studies have shown that familial aggregation is of

Studies have shown that familial aggregation is of No Evidence of Sex Differences in Heritability of Irritable Bowel Syndrome in Swedish Twins Pia Svedberg, 1 Saga Johansson, 3,4 Mari-Ann Wallander, 3,6 and Nancy L. Pedersen 2,5 1 Section of Personal Injury

More information

Genetic Influences on Midlife Functioning

Genetic Influences on Midlife Functioning 03-Willis-4658.qxd 3/24/2005 2:12 PM Page 65 Three Genetic Influences on Midlife Functioning Nancy L. Pedersen, Erica Spotts, and Kenji Kato Regardless of the definition of midlife, most of the research

More information

Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease

Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease (2007) 15, 872 877 & 2007 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 1018-4813/07 $30.00 ARTICLE www.nature.com/ejhg Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease Slobodan

More information

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating Cynthia M. Bulik,* Patrick F. Sullivan, and Kenneth S. Kendler Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics of Virginia

More information

Variability and Stability in Cognitive Abilities Are Largely Genetic Later in Life

Variability and Stability in Cognitive Abilities Are Largely Genetic Later in Life Behavior Genetics, VoL 24, No. 3, 1994 Variability and Stability in Cognitive Abilities Are Largely Genetic Later in Life R. Plomin, a N. L. Pedersen, a,2 P. Lichtenstein, 2 and G. E. McClearn I Received

More information

Discussion. were best fit by models of determination that did not include genetic effects.

Discussion. were best fit by models of determination that did not include genetic effects. A Behavior Genetic Investigation of the Relationship Between Leadership and Personality Andrew M. Johnson 1, Philip A. Vernon 2, Julie Aitken Harris 3, and Kerry L. Jang 4 1 Faculty of Health Sciences,The

More information

Traits and Metatraits: Their Reliability, Stability, and Shared Genetic Influence

Traits and Metatraits: Their Reliability, Stability, and Shared Genetic Influence PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Traits and Metatraits: Their Reliability, Stability, and Shared Genetic Influence Scott L. Hershberger and Robert Plomin Pennsylvania State University Nancy

More information

The equal environments assumption, which holds

The equal environments assumption, which holds An Investigation of a Measure of Twins Equal Environments Karen S. Mitchell, 1,2 Suzanne E. Mazzeo, 1,3 Cynthia M. Bulik, 4 Steven H. Aggen, 2,5 Kenneth S. Kendler, 2,5 and Michael C. Neale 2,5,6 1 Department

More information

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81.

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81. MSP RESEARCH NOTE B5PQ Reliability and Validity This research note describes the reliability and validity of the B5PQ. Evidence for the reliability and validity of is presented against some of the key

More information

Genetic contribution to the relationship between personality and depressive symptoms among older women

Genetic contribution to the relationship between personality and depressive symptoms among older women Psychological Medicine (2010), 40, 1357 1366. f Cambridge University Press 2009 doi:10.1017/s0033291709991401 Genetic contribution to the relationship between personality and depressive symptoms among

More information

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study Diabetologia (2013) 56:1031 1035 DOI 10.1007/s00125-013-2859-3 SHORT COMMUNICATION Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study S. Carlsson & A. Ahlbom & P. Lichtenstein

More information

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder DOI 10.1007/s10519-014-9670-x ORIGINAL RESEARCH Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder J. L. Ebejer S. E. Medland J.

More information

In Australian twins participating in three different

In Australian twins participating in three different Heritability and Stability of Resting Blood Pressure in Australian Twins Jouke-Jan Hottenga, 1 John B. Whitfield, 2 Eco J. C. de Geus, 1 Dorret I. Boomsma, 1 and Nicholas G. Martin 2 1 Department of Biological

More information

Personality Stability in Late Adulthood: A Behavioral Genetic Analysis

Personality Stability in Late Adulthood: A Behavioral Genetic Analysis Personality Stability in Late Adulthood: A Behavioral Genetic Analysis Wendy Johnson, Matt McGue, and Robert F. Krueger University of Minnesota Twin Cities ABSTRACT A sample of 833 twins from the Minnesota

More information

LJEAVES II J EYSENCIZ 1\J G Nli\RTIN

LJEAVES II J EYSENCIZ 1\J G Nli\RTIN ';1 '\ 0 G 1 D\ 'C-l. ptrt':l'in'\lf"l ilea' I a p' nro~j'l'cn;l, J..:li.1,,,,,,,lUl.1 lull. 11.,.' I.' I. :u l, : Czt. \ ;i.:r! 11: LJEAVES II J EYSENCIZ 1\J G Nli\RTIN ACADEMIC PRESS LIMITED 24128 Oval

More information

Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder:

Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder: Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder: Same Genes, (Partly) Kenneth S. Kendler, M.D. Different Environments Revisited Abstract: In both clinical and epidemiological samples, major depression

More information

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H PSYC H O PAT H O LO G Y : PSYC H I AT R I C P R O B LEMS A N D T H E A S SO C I

More information

Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality: A Longitudinal Multiple-Rater Twin Study

Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality: A Longitudinal Multiple-Rater Twin Study Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 98, No. 6, 995 1008 0022-3514/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019558 Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality:

More information

Nonshared Environmental Influences and Personality Differences in Adult Twins

Nonshared Environmental Influences and Personality Differences in Adult Twins Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1990, Vol. 58, No. 1,103-110 Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/90/S00.75 Nonshared Environmental Influences and Personality

More information

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins Behavior Genetics, Vol. 33, No. 5, September 2003 ( 2003) Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt,

More information

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail.

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. Author(s): Salmela-Aro, Katariina; Read, Sanna; Vuoksimaa, Eero; Korhonen,

More information

THE CONSISTENCY OF RECALLED AGE AT FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE

THE CONSISTENCY OF RECALLED AGE AT FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE Revise 1st proof 6.11.96 J. biosoc. Sci. (1997) 29,1 7 1997 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom THE CONSISTENCY OF RECALLED AGE AT FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE MICHAEL P. DUNNE*, NICHOLAS

More information

Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis

Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis Roxann Roberson-Nay, PhD Virginia Commonwealth University Department of Psychiatry The SAD PD link Don Klein proposed

More information

Chapter X Personality

Chapter X Personality 2000, Gregory Carey (4/7/00) Personality - 1 Chapter X Personality Personality and its Measurement The word personality has various meanings that range from the qualities of an individual person to evaluative

More information

Introduction. TL Nelson 1 *, GP Vogler 1, NL Pedersen 2,3 and TP Miles 4

Introduction. TL Nelson 1 *, GP Vogler 1, NL Pedersen 2,3 and TP Miles 4 International Journal of Obesity (1999) 23, 449±455 ß 1999 Stockton Press All rights reserved 0307±0565/99 $12.00 http://www.stockton-press.co.uk/ijo Genetic and environmental in uences on waistto-hip

More information

Measurement Models for Sexual Orientation in a Community Twin Sample

Measurement Models for Sexual Orientation in a Community Twin Sample Behavior Genetics, Vol. 30, No. 4, 2000 Measurement Models for Sexual Orientation in a Community Twin Sample K. M. Kirk, 1, 4 J. M. Bailey, 2 M. P. Dunne, 1, 3 and N. G. Martin 1 Received 22 Feb. 2000

More information

Perceived Stress has Genetic Influences Distinct from Neuroticism and Depression

Perceived Stress has Genetic Influences Distinct from Neuroticism and Depression Behav Genet (2014) 44:639 645 DOI 10.1007/s10519-013-9636-4 ORIGINAL RESEARCH Perceived Stress has Genetic Influences Distinct from Neuroticism and Depression Liz Rietschel Gu Zhu Clemens Kirschbaum Jana

More information

Examining the Origins of Gender Differences in Marital Quality: A Behavior Genetic Analysis

Examining the Origins of Gender Differences in Marital Quality: A Behavior Genetic Analysis Journal of Family Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 20, No. 4, 605 613 0893-3200/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.4.605 Examining the Origins of Gender Differences

More information

The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for three measures of disordered eating

The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for three measures of disordered eating Psychological Medicine, 1999, 29, 925 934. 1999 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for three measures of disordered eating

More information

How Money Buys Happiness: Genetic and Environmental Processes Linking Finances and Life Satisfaction

How Money Buys Happiness: Genetic and Environmental Processes Linking Finances and Life Satisfaction Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 90, No. 4, 680 691 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.680 How Money Buys Happiness:

More information

Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood

Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood CE ACTIVITY Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood Melissa A. Munn, MPE 1,2 * Michael C. Stallings, PhD 1,2 Soo Hyun Rhee, PhD 1,2 Laura E. Sobik, PhD

More information

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children J. Intell. 2015, 3, 160-167; doi:10.3390/jintelligence3040160 Brief Report OPEN ACCESS Journal of Intelligence ISSN 2079-3200 www.mdpi.com/journal/jintelligence Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations

More information

Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions

Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions Psychological Medicine, 2000, 30, 1315 1325. 2000 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions

More information

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences University of Lethbridge Research Repository OPUS Faculty Research and Publications http://opus.uleth.ca Lalumière, Martin 2005 Behavioral genetics: The study of differences Lalumière, Martin L. Department

More information

Following in Your Father s Footsteps: A Note on the Intergenerational Transmission of Income between Twin Fathers and their Sons

Following in Your Father s Footsteps: A Note on the Intergenerational Transmission of Income between Twin Fathers and their Sons D I S C U S S I O N P A P E R S E R I E S IZA DP No. 5990 Following in Your Father s Footsteps: A Note on the Intergenerational Transmission of Income between Twin Fathers and their Sons Vikesh Amin Petter

More information

Growing Old but Not Growing Apart: Twin Similarity in the Latter Half of the Lifespan

Growing Old but Not Growing Apart: Twin Similarity in the Latter Half of the Lifespan Behav Genet (2013) 43:1 12 DOI 10.1007/s10519-012-9559-5 ORIGINAL RESEARCH Growing Old but Not Growing Apart: Twin Similarity in the Latter Half of the Lifespan Matt McGue Kaare Christensen Received: 12

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Religiousness: Findings for Retrospective and Current Religiousness Ratings

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Religiousness: Findings for Retrospective and Current Religiousness Ratings Genetic and Environmental Influences on Religiousness: Findings for Retrospective and Current Religiousness Ratings Laura B. Koenig, Matt McGue, Robert F. Krueger, and Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr. University

More information

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance Twin Research (1999) 2, 99 107 1999 Stockton Press All rights reserved 1369 0523/99 $12.00 http://www.stockton-press.co.uk/tr Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of

More information

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Daily Life Stressors: More Evidence for Greater Variation in Later Life

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Daily Life Stressors: More Evidence for Greater Variation in Later Life Psychology and Aging Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 22, No. 2, 331 340 0882-7974/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.2.331 Genetic and Environmental Effects on Daily

More information

The Relationship Between the Genetic and Environmental Influences on Common Internalizing Psychiatric Disorders and Mental Well-Being

The Relationship Between the Genetic and Environmental Influences on Common Internalizing Psychiatric Disorders and Mental Well-Being Behav Genet (2011) 41:641 650 DOI 10.1007/s10519-011-9466-1 ORIGINAL RESEARCH The Relationship Between the Genetic and Environmental Influences on Common Internalizing Psychiatric Disorders and Mental

More information

The present study investigated the location of trait

The present study investigated the location of trait Phenotypic and Genetic Relations Between the HEXACO Dimensions and Trait Emotional Intelligence Livia Veselka, 1 K. V. Petrides, 2 Julie Aitken Schermer, 3 Lynn F. Cherkas, 4 Tim D. Spector 4 and Philip

More information

Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality

Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality Graduate Faculty Psychology Bulletin Volume 3, No. 1, 2005 Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality Brian Norensberg, M.A. 1 & Peter Zachar Ph.D. 2 Abstract ~ This study investigates the

More information

Exploring the inter-relationship of smoking age-at-onset, cigarette consumption and smoking persistence: genes or environment?

Exploring the inter-relationship of smoking age-at-onset, cigarette consumption and smoking persistence: genes or environment? Psychological Medicine, 2007, 37, 1357 1367. f 2007 Cambridge University Press doi:10.1017/s0033291707000748 First published online 30 April 2007 Printed in the United Kingdom Exploring the inter-relationship

More information

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability Eric Turkheimer and Erin E. Horn In 3, our lab published a paper demonstrating that the heritability

More information

AN EXAMINATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP AMONG ACADEMIC STRESS, COPING, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE IN COLLEGE

AN EXAMINATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP AMONG ACADEMIC STRESS, COPING, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE IN COLLEGE Research in Higher Education, Vol. 41, No. 5, 2000 AN EXAMINATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP AMONG ACADEMIC STRESS, COPING, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE IN COLLEGE C. Ward Struthers, Raymond P. Perry, and Verena

More information

ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN SELF-RATED HEALTH AND PERSONALITY

ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN SELF-RATED HEALTH AND PERSONALITY ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN SELF-RATED HEALTH AND PERSONALITY Objective: The goal of our study was to examine how Big Five personality factors predict variability in self-rated health in a sample of older African

More information

Age Differences in Genetic and Environmental Variations in Stress-Coping During Adulthood: A Study of Female Twins

Age Differences in Genetic and Environmental Variations in Stress-Coping During Adulthood: A Study of Female Twins Behav Genet (2012) 42:541 548 DOI 10.1007/s10519-012-9541-2 ORIGINAL RESEARCH Age Differences in Genetic and Environmental Variations in Stress-Coping During Adulthood: A Study of Female Twins Yoon-Mi

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) PSYSOC 2013

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) PSYSOC 2013 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) 506 510 PSYSOC 2013 Personality Traits and Different Career Stages A Study on Indian School

More information

Optimism has a positive influence on mental and

Optimism has a positive influence on mental and Sex Differences in the Genetic Architecture of Optimism and Health and Their Interrelation: A Study of Australian and Swedish Twins Miriam A. Mosing, 1,2 Nancy L. Pedersen, 3 Nicholas G. Martin 1,2 and

More information

Preliminary Conclusion

Preliminary Conclusion 1 Exploring the Genetic Component of Political Participation Brad Verhulst Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics Virginia Commonwealth University Theories of political participation,

More information

On genetic variation in menarche and age at first sexual intercourse A critique of the Belsky Draper hypothesis

On genetic variation in menarche and age at first sexual intercourse A critique of the Belsky Draper hypothesis Evolution and Human Behavior 23 (2002) 365 372 On genetic variation in menarche and age at first sexual intercourse A critique of the Belsky Draper hypothesis David C. Rowe School of Family and Consumer

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Introduction

BRIEF REPORT. Introduction BRIEF REPORT Genetic and Environmental Associations between Body Dissatisfaction, Weight Preoccupation, and Binge Eating: Evidence for a Common Factor with Differential Loadings Across Symptom Type Shannon

More information

A Behavior Genetic Study of the Connection Between Social Values and Personality

A Behavior Genetic Study of the Connection Between Social Values and Personality ARTICLE AVAILABLE ONLINE Twin Research and Human Genetics Volume 14 Number 3 pp. 233 239 A Behavior Genetic Study of the Connection Between Social Values and Personality Julie Aitken Schermer, 1 Philip

More information

J. Sleep Res. (2012) Regular Research Paper

J. Sleep Res. (2012) Regular Research Paper J. Sleep Res. (2012) Regular Research Paper Modeling the direction of causation between cross-sectional measures of disrupted sleep, anxiety and depression in a sample of male and female Australian twins

More information

Genetic influences on disordered eating behaviour are largely independent of body mass index

Genetic influences on disordered eating behaviour are largely independent of body mass index Acta Psychiatr Scand 2008: 117: 348 356 All rights reserved DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01132.x Copyright Ó 2007 The Authors Journal Compilation Ó 2007 Blackwell Munksgaard ACTA PSYCHIATRICA SCANDINAVICA

More information

Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students

Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students Gary Felsten Stress Reactivity and Depressed Mood 1 Department of Psychology, Indiana University Purdue University Columbus 4601

More information

Review Article Sex Differences in Genetic and Environmental Influences on Adolescent Depressive Symptoms: A Meta-Analytic Review

Review Article Sex Differences in Genetic and Environmental Influences on Adolescent Depressive Symptoms: A Meta-Analytic Review Depression Research and Treatment Volume 2015, Article ID 476238, 7 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/476238 Review Article Sex Differences in Genetic and Environmental Influences on Adolescent Depressive

More information

Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration

Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration J Fam Viol (2007) 22:121 129 DOI 10.1007/s10896-006-9059-6 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration

More information

CHAPTER 6. Conclusions and Perspectives

CHAPTER 6. Conclusions and Perspectives CHAPTER 6 Conclusions and Perspectives In Chapter 2 of this thesis, similarities and differences among members of (mainly MZ) twin families in their blood plasma lipidomics profiles were investigated.

More information

Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries

Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries Donald Macnab Psychometrics Canada Copyright Psychometrics Canada 2011. All rights reserved. The Work Personality Index is a trademark of

More information

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION Chapter 8 GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION A Longitudinal Study in 3- to 12-Year-Old Children Dorret I. Boomsma, Ph.D. C.E.M. van Beijsterveldt, Ph.D. Meike Bartels, Ph.D. James

More information

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 1633 1639 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Short Communication The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic

More information

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES Correlational Research Correlational Designs Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are

More information

Control and Turnout. Christopher Dawes. University of California, San Diego. March 18, 2010

Control and Turnout. Christopher Dawes. University of California, San Diego. March 18, 2010 Control and Turnout Christopher Dawes University of California, San Diego March 18, 2010 Abstract Political scientists have recently explored the genetic basis of political participation. Fowler, Baker

More information

02a: Test-Retest and Parallel Forms Reliability

02a: Test-Retest and Parallel Forms Reliability 1 02a: Test-Retest and Parallel Forms Reliability Quantitative Variables 1. Classic Test Theory (CTT) 2. Correlation for Test-retest (or Parallel Forms): Stability and Equivalence for Quantitative Measures

More information

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement Psychological Methods 2007, Vol. 12, No. 4, 451 466 CORRECTED APRIL 10, 2008; SEE LAST PAGE Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 1082-989X/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.4.451

More information

Intelligence 31 (2003)

Intelligence 31 (2003) Intelligence 31 (2003) 589 605 A genetic two-factor model of the covariation among a subset of Multidimensional Aptitude Battery and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised subtests Michelle Luciano*,

More information

Bias in Correlations from Selected Samples of Relatives: The Effects of Soft Selection

Bias in Correlations from Selected Samples of Relatives: The Effects of Soft Selection Behavior Genetics, Vol. 19, No. 2, 1989 Bias in Correlations from Selected Samples of Relatives: The Effects of Soft Selection M. C. Neale, ~ L. J. Eaves, 1 K. S. Kendler, 2 and J. K. Hewitt 1 Received

More information

Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures

Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures Journal of Substance Abuse, 5, 227-246 (7993) Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures Andrew C. Heath Departments of Psychiatry, Psychology

More information

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas 1 Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas Reference: Barchard, K.A. (2002, May). Three subfactors of the empathic personality. Poster presented

More information

Paul Irwing, Manchester Business School

Paul Irwing, Manchester Business School Paul Irwing, Manchester Business School Factor analysis has been the prime statistical technique for the development of structural theories in social science, such as the hierarchical factor model of human

More information

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter Lawrence Feinstein Jyllian Call University of California, Los Angeles December, 1986 Further information on the instrument can be obtained

More information

Factor Structure of the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations (CISS) in Japanese Workers

Factor Structure of the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations (CISS) in Japanese Workers Psychology, 2014, 5, 1620-1628 Published Online September 2014 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.514172 Factor Structure of the Coping Inventory for Stressful

More information

Personal Goals of Older Female Twins

Personal Goals of Older Female Twins K. European Salmela-Aro Psychologist 2009 et al.: Hogrefe Genetic 2009; Vol. & Huber Effect 14(2):160 167 Publishers on Goals Personal Goals of Older Female Twins Genetic and Environmental Effects Katariina

More information

Parenting as Phenotype: A Behavioral Genetic Approach to Understanding Parenting

Parenting as Phenotype: A Behavioral Genetic Approach to Understanding Parenting The University of San Francisco USF Scholarship: a digital repository @ Gleeson Library Geschke Center Psychology College of Arts and Sciences 2012 Parenting as Phenotype: A Behavioral Genetic Approach

More information

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Subhrasikha Naik Senior Study and research chronic diseases Participate in gathering data for Project Cope, which is focused

More information

Chapter 9. Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS)

Chapter 9. Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS) Chapter 9 Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS) Background Purpose The Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS) is a measure of perceived effectiveness of a specific counseling session. In general, measures

More information

Factor Analytic Theories. Chapter 11

Factor Analytic Theories. Chapter 11 Factor Analytic Theories Chapter 11 Raymond Cattell 1905- Born Staffordshire, England Happy childhood Lived through WW I Admitted he was heavily influenced by the war Chemistry & Physics background before

More information

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION. Are Genetic Influences on Peptic Ulcer Dependent or Independent of Genetic Influences for Helicobacter pylori Infection?

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION. Are Genetic Influences on Peptic Ulcer Dependent or Independent of Genetic Influences for Helicobacter pylori Infection? ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Are Genetic Influences on Peptic Ulcer Dependent or Independent of Genetic Influences for Helicobacter pylori Infection? Hoda M. Malaty, MD, PhD; David Y. Graham, MD; Inger Isaksson,

More information

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Same-sex Sexual Behavior: A Population Study of Twins in Sweden

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Same-sex Sexual Behavior: A Population Study of Twins in Sweden Arch Sex Behav (2010) 39:75 80 DOI 10.1007/s10508-008-9386-1 ORIGINAL PAPER Genetic and Environmental Effects on Same-sex Sexual Behavior: A Population Study of Twins in Sweden Niklas Långström Æ Qazi

More information

Comparison between high school students in cognitive and affective coping Strategies

Comparison between high school students in cognitive and affective coping Strategies Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 46 ( 2012 ) 289 293 WCES 2012 Comparison between high school students in cognitive and affective coping Strategies Ezat

More information

Environmental Predictors of Personality Differences: A Twin and Sibling Study

Environmental Predictors of Personality Differences: A Twin and Sibling Study PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Environmental Predictors of Personality Differences: A Twin and Siling Study Philip A. Vernon University of Western Ontario Kerry L. Jang University of

More information

The Genetic and Environmental Sources of Resemblance Between Normative Personality and Personality Disorder Traits

The Genetic and Environmental Sources of Resemblance Between Normative Personality and Personality Disorder Traits Virginia Commonwealth University VCU Scholars Compass Psychiatry Publications Dept. of Psychiatry 2017 The Genetic and Environmental Sources of Resemblance Between Normative Personality and Personality

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Functional Abilities in Danish Twins Aged 75 Years and Older

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Functional Abilities in Danish Twins Aged 75 Years and Older Journal of Gerontology: MEDICAL SCIENCES 2000, Vol. 55A, No. 8, M446 M452 Copyright 2000 by The Gerontological Society of America Genetic and Environmental Influences on Functional Abilities in Danish

More information

Evidence for Genetic Influences on Personality From Self-Reports and Informant Ratings

Evidence for Genetic Influences on Personality From Self-Reports and Informant Ratings Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1992, Vol. 63. No. 1,85-96 Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. OO22-3514/92/S3.OO Evidence for Genetic Influences on Personality

More information

Genetic Factors in Temperamental Individuality

Genetic Factors in Temperamental Individuality Genetic Factors in Temperamental Individuality A Longitudinal Study of Same-Sexed Twins from Two Months to Six Years of Age Anne Mari Torgersen, Cando Psychol. Abstract. A previous publication reported

More information

Twin singleton differences in intelligence?

Twin singleton differences in intelligence? Twin Research (2000) 3, 83 87 2000 Macmillan Publishers Ltd All rights reserved 1369 0523/00 $15.00 y www.nature.com/tr Twin singleton differences in intelligence? Daniëlle Posthuma 1, Eco JC De Geus 1,

More information

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH The following document provides background information on the research and development of the Emergenetics Profile instrument. Emergenetics Defined 1. Emergenetics

More information

Population-Based Twin Registries: Illustrative Applications in Genetic Epidemiology and Behavioral Genetics from the Finnish Twin Cohort Study

Population-Based Twin Registries: Illustrative Applications in Genetic Epidemiology and Behavioral Genetics from the Finnish Twin Cohort Study Acta Genet Med Gemellol 39: 427-439 (1990) 1990 by The Mendel Institute, Rome Sixth International Congress on Twin Studies Population-Based Twin Registries: Illustrative Applications in Genetic Epidemiology

More information

Trait Negative Affect Relates to Prior-Week Symptoms, But Not to Reports of Illness Episodes, Illness Symptoms, and Care Seeking Among Older Persons

Trait Negative Affect Relates to Prior-Week Symptoms, But Not to Reports of Illness Episodes, Illness Symptoms, and Care Seeking Among Older Persons Trait Negative Affect Relates to Prior-Week Symptoms, But Not to Reports of Illness Episodes, Illness Symptoms, and Care Seeking Among Older Persons PABLO A. MORA, MS, CHANTAL ROBITAILLE, PHD, HOWARD LEVENTHAL,

More information

Factor Analytic Theories

Factor Analytic Theories Factor Analytic Theories Chapter 11 Chapter 11 Big Ideas Raymond Cattell Cattell s Definition of Personality Surface Traits versus Source Traits Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF) What is Factor Analysis?

More information

Sociodemographic Effects on the Test-Retest Reliability of the Big Five Inventory. Timo Gnambs. Osnabrück University. Author Note

Sociodemographic Effects on the Test-Retest Reliability of the Big Five Inventory. Timo Gnambs. Osnabrück University. Author Note Running head: TEST-RETEST RELIABILITY Sociodemographic Effects on the Test-Retest Reliability of the Big Five Inventory Timo Gnambs Osnabrück University Author Note Timo Gnambs, Institute of Psychology,

More information

Pamela A. F. Madden, Ph.D., Kathleen K. Bucholz, Ph.D., Nicholas G. Martin, Ph.D., and Andrew C. Heath, D.Phil.

Pamela A. F. Madden, Ph.D., Kathleen K. Bucholz, Ph.D., Nicholas G. Martin, Ph.D., and Andrew C. Heath, D.Phil. Smoking and the Genetic Contribution to Alcohol- Dependence Risk Pamela A. F. Madden, Ph.D., Kathleen K. Bucholz, Ph.D., Nicholas G. Martin, Ph.D., and Andrew C. Heath, D.Phil. Genes influence a person

More information

Australian children of alcoholic female twins

Australian children of alcoholic female twins Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Posters 2005: Alcoholism and Comorbidity 2005 Australian children of alcoholic female twins Wendy S. Slutske Follow this and additional works

More information

Personality measures under focus: The NEO-PI-R and the MBTI

Personality measures under focus: The NEO-PI-R and the MBTI : The NEO-PI-R and the MBTI Author Published 2009 Journal Title Griffith University Undergraduate Psychology Journal Downloaded from http://hdl.handle.net/10072/340329 Link to published version http://pandora.nla.gov.au/tep/145784

More information

Avoidant Coping Moderates the Association between Anxiety and Physical Functioning in Patients with Chronic Heart Failure

Avoidant Coping Moderates the Association between Anxiety and Physical Functioning in Patients with Chronic Heart Failure Avoidant Coping Moderates the Association between Anxiety and Physical Functioning in Patients with Chronic Heart Failure Eisenberg SA 1, Shen BJ 1, Singh K 1, Schwarz ER 2, Mallon SM 3 1 University of

More information