Correspondence should be addressed to Denise Davidson;

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Correspondence should be addressed to Denise Davidson;"

Transcription

1 Autism Research and Treatment, Article ID , 11 pages Research Article Recognition of Emotion from Facial Expressions with Direct or Averted Eye Gaze and Varying Expression Intensities in Children with Autism Disorder and Typically Developing Children Dina Tell, 1 Denise Davidson, 2 and Linda A. Camras 3 1 Department of Health Promotion, Loyola University Chicago, Marcella Niehoff School of Nursing, 2160 S. First Avenue, Maywood, IL 60153, USA 2 DepartmentofPsychology,LoyolaUniversityChicago,1032W.SheridanRoad,Chicago,IL60660,USA 3 Department of Psychology, DePaul University, 2219 N. Kenmore Avenue, Chicago, IL 60614, USA Correspondence should be addressed to Denise Davidson; ddavids@luc.edu Received 8 November 2013; Revised 7 February 2014; Accepted 12 February 2014; Published 3 April 2014 Academic Editor: Geraldine Dawson Copyright 2014 Dina Tell et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Eye gaze direction and expression intensity effects on emotion recognition in children with autism disorder and typically developing children were investigated. Children with autism disorder and typically developing children identified happy and angry expressions equally well. Children with autism disorder, however, were less accurate in identifying fear expressions across intensities and eye gaze directions. Children with autism disorder rated expressions with direct eyes, and 50% expressions, as more intense than typically developing children. A trend was also found for sad expressions, as children with autism disorder were less accurate in recognizing sadness at 100% intensity with direct eyes than typically developing children. Although the present research showed that children with autism disorder are sensitive to eye gaze direction, impairments in the recognition of fear, and possibly sadness, exist. Furthermore, children with autism disorder and typically developing children perceive the intensity of emotional expressions differently. 1. Introduction A number of studies have shown deficits in face, eye gaze, and emotion processing in individuals with autism disorder (see [1 5], for reviews). It has been suggested that the deficits found for face and eye processing may underlie impairments seen in the recognition of emotion by such individuals and subsequently may contribute to their social impairments [5]. Although some studies have shown that children and adults with autism disorder show reflexive orienting to eye gaze cues (see [4],forareview),otherstudieshaveshownthat these individuals look less at the eye region of the face (e.g., [6, 7]),engageinlessmutualeyegazebehavior(e.g.,[8]), and show more deficits in gaze following behaviors (e.g., [9])than neurotypical individuals. A number of critical issues remain concerning the facilitativeeffectsofeyegazecuesonemotionrecognitionin children with autism disorder as well as typically developing children. According to the shared signal hypothesis [10, 11], when eye gaze direction is combined with the intent communicated by an emotional expression, it should enhance the perception of that emotion. That is, direct eye gaze should facilitate the processing of facially communicated approachoriented emotions (e.g., anger and joy), whereas averted eye gazeshouldfacilitatetheprocessingoffaciallycommunicated avoidance-oriented emotions (e.g., fear and sadness). Insupportofthesharedsignalhypothesis,Adamsand Kleck [10] found that happy and angry expressions were more quickly identified with direct than averted eye gaze, whereas fear and sadness were more quickly identified with averted than direct eye gaze, at least in neurotypical adults. Additional research with neurotypical adults has shown that angry faces are judged more angry with direct eye gaze than averted eye gaze (e.g., [12, 13]). Consistent with these findings, in a developmental study, Akechi and colleagues found that typically developing children were faster at detecting a facial

2 2 Autism Research and Treatment expression accompanying a gaze direction with a congruent emotional expression: anger with a direct gaze and fear with an averted gaze [14]. In contrast, Akechi et al. [14] found that children with autism disorder did not show this pattern of responding: eye gaze direction did not affect their reaction times in detecting angry or fearful faces, although they were just as accurate as typically developing children. According to Akechi et al. [14], these results suggest that children with autism disorder are less likely than their typically developing peers to integrate communicative signals present in the eyes with their emotional quality. Akechi et al. [14] postulated that children with autism disorder may not spontaneously integrate eye gaze direction and its communicative intent, especially in connection to emotional expressions or within social contexts. Still, although a number of studies have examined sensitivity to eye gaze cues in adolescents and adults with autism disorder (see [3, 4], for reviews) and emotion recognition in such individuals (see [1, 2, 5], for reviews), far fewer studies have examined the effects of eye gaze direction on emotion recognition in children with autism disorder. In the Akechi et al.[14] study, only two emotional expressions, anger and fear, were presented to children. A primary goal of the present research was to explore the effects of eye gaze direction on emotion recognition in children with autism disorder and typically developing children across a greater range of emotional expressions and expression intensities. In particular, children with autism disorder and typically developing children were shown happy, angry, sad, fearful, and neutral faces with direct or averted eye gaze directions at two expression intensities (50% and 100%). One reason for presenting this range of emotions was because past studies have found that while emotion recognition in individuals with autism disorder may be on par with neurotypical individuals for happy expressions (e.g., [15 17]), several studies have demonstrated that such individuals may show impairment in the recognition of negative emotions, including anger (e.g., [18]), sadness (e.g., [16]), and fear (e.g., [17, 19]). These studies, however, were conducted primarily with adolescents and/or adults, and not children with autism disorder. Moreover, it is possible that the developmental trajectory for emotion recognition may differ between children with autism disorder and typically developing children. While emotion recognition appears to be correlated with age in typically developing children (e.g., [20 23]); not all studies have found similar developmental improvements in children with autism disorder (e.g., [24]). Equally important, differences between children with autism disorder and typically developing children in recognizing emotions may be less prevalent when emotional expressions are depicted at stronger or greater intensities than when less intense expressions are presented. However, the intensity of emotional expressions has only occasionally been studied as a factor affecting children s recognition of emotion. When photographs of emotional expressions of varying intensity levels were shown to a sample of 4- to 15- year-old typically developing children, these children were more accurate in matching emotions when photographs were presented at 50% expression intensity than when they were presented at 25% expression intensity, at least for fear, sadness, anger, and happy expressions [25]. In a study of both typically developing children and children with autism disorder, Mazefsky and Oswald [26] foundthat highfunctioning children with autism disorder were less accurate than both children with Asperger s syndrome and typically developing children in perceiving emotion from low intensity voice cues, but not with high intensity cues. In a similar vein, Law Smith et al. [27] found that high functioning adolescents with autism disorder were significantly worse than typically developing adolescents at detecting surprise, anger, and disgust using dynamic facial stimuli at lower, but not higher, intensities. Finally, Wallace and his colleagues showed that adolescents with autism disorder required more intense facial expressions for accurate emotion recognition than typically developing adolescents [28]. These are important differences given that in everyday settings emotional expressions are often subtle. Only by examining a range of different emotions using both highly intense and less intense facial displays can a more accurate picture of emotion recognition abilities in children with autism disorder and typically developing children be generated. 2. Overview of Present Research In light of past findings, the present research examined the effects of eye gaze direction and expression intensity on emotion recognition in children with autism disorder and typically developing children between eight and twelve years of age. Using a photograph task, children s accuracy in recognizing facial expressions of happy, angry, sad, and fear emotional expressions, along with neutral expressions, was investigated. All emotional expressions were presented with computer-generated direct or averted eye gaze at 50% or 100% expression strength. While other intensities can be presented (e.g., 25% or 75%), these intensities were selected because fewer differences were expected between more closely matched intensity rates (e.g., 0% versus 25%; 75% versus 100%) than less closely matched intensity rates (50% versus 100%). Specific predictions for each emotional expression were as follows. (1) Based on past studies that have shown that emotion recognition in individuals with autism disorder may be on par with neurotypical individuals for happy expressions (e.g., [15, 16]), it was predicted that children with autism disorder would be as accurate as typically developing children in recognizing happy expressions in all conditions (i.e., with direct and averted eye gaze and at 50% and 100% expression intensities). (2) It was predicted that children with autism disorder would recognize angry expressions at full (100%) strength as well as typically developing children. At 50% expression strength, children with autism disorder should perform more poorly than typically developing children. These findings were expected

3 Autism Research and Treatment 3 regardless of eye gaze direction and were based in part on the findings of Law Smith et al. [27], who showed that recognition of anger in adolescents with autism disorder was intact at 100% expression strength, but not at lower levels of intensity. (3) It was predicted that children with autism disorder would recognize fear expressions less well than typically developing children, regardless of eye gaze direction or expression intensity, based on past findings showing a fear recognition impairment in individuals with autism disorder (e.g., [17, 19]). Averted eye gaze was expected to facilitate recognition of fear in typically developing children, especially at 50% expression intensity. (4) Sad emotional expressions at 100% intensity and averted eyes were expected to be recognized equally well by children with autism disorder and typically developing children, based on the findings of Akechi et al. [14]. However, given that studies have shown that adults with autism disorder do not recognize sadness expressions as well as their neurotypical peers (e.g., [16]), children with autism disorder may not recognize expressions of sadness as well as typically developing children, especially at 50% intensity and regardless of eye gaze directions. (5) Children with autism disorder would be more likely to label neutral (no emotion) expressions with an emotion label than typically developing children. This prediction was based on the results of Kuusikko et al. [29],whoshowedthatchildrenwithautismdisorder perceived ambiguous emotional stimuli as negative emotions, whereas typically developing children did not perceive them as emotional. (6)Childrenwithautismdisorderwouldrateemotions with direct gaze as more intense and were expected to rate 50% emotional displays as more intense, than typically developing children. These predictions were based on the results of past studies that have shown that direct eye gaze can elicit arousing physiological effects in children with autism disorder and may even be too overstimulating for such children (e.g., [30]). 3. Method 3.1. Participants Children with Autism Disorder. Twenty-eight children with autism disorder were identified and recruited through specialized schools and clinical centers in Chicago, IL. One child relocated before the investigators could complete the procedures, and five children were unable to participate due to various scheduling difficulties, resulting in a final sample of 22 children with autism disorder (mean age = 10.31; age range = 8 12). Seventeen were male and 5 were female. All children with autism disorder who participated in this study were recruited from organizations that serve exclusively students with a formal diagnosis of autism disorder. Clinical diagnosis for all children was established by medical evaluation with a developmental pediatrician and/or by a licensed clinical psychologist in accordance with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV [31] and the criteria for autism as outlined by IDEA ( Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004 ). Almost all of the children (n = 20) were administered the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-G; [32]) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview Revised (ADI- R; [33]) as part of their evaluation by the organization that was providing their educational/therapeutic services. The records of two children noted that they had been administeredthechildhoodautismratingscale(cars;[34]) and thevinelandadaptivebehaviorscales(vinelandii;[35]), although their scores from these measures were not included in their records. Moreover, we were not able to locate these children s ADOS-G or ADI-R scores or determine if they were available. However, because these two children were attending the same program as the other children with autism disorder and because these two children did not perform differently on any measures than the other children with autism disorder, the decision was made to include their data in our sample. Children with autism disorder were eligible to participate if they had (1) a formal and clinical diagnosis of autism disorder without comorbid conditions including ADHD, ODD, bipolar disorder, and anxiety disorder; (2) a verbal mental age (VMA) at or above 5 years; (3) English as their primary language; and (4) an IQ performance score (PIQ) greater than 75. For the latter, all children with autism disorder had been administered the WISC-III by a licensed professional within a year or less of the present study. In our sample, children with autism disorder had PIQ scores in the average range (M =102.68,SD=6.64).Children with lower PIQ scores (and VM scores below 5 years) were not recruited because of the nature of our experimental task. According to children s records, children met the cutoff for autism disorder in the social domain, ADOS (social + communication): M = 11.00, SD = 4.66; ADI-R (social interaction): M = 20.00, SD = 6.11; in the communication and language domain (ADI-R: M =12.33,SD=4.67);andinthe repetitive/stereotyped behavior domain ADOS (stereotyped behavior): M = 1.44, SD = 1.68; ADI-R (restricted/repetitive behaviors): M = 6.1, SD = As an additional measure of severity of symptoms, parents of children with autism disorder were asked to complete the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; [36]). The SRS is a 65-item questionnaire designed to assess social awareness, social information processing, capacity for reciprocal social communication, social anxiety/avoidance, and autistic preoccupations and traits. It has shown good reliability (median alpha =.85) and is conceptually appropriate as it measures severity of symptoms rather than the presence or absence of them, thus capturing the spectrum-like condition of the diagnosis [36]. On average, children with autism disorder were evaluated to have moderate to severe levels of social impairment, M Tscore = ,SD = 8.49,and range = T-scores above 76 arestronglyassociatedwithaclinicaldiagnosisofautism

4 4 Autism Research and Treatment 0% 50% 100% (a) 0% 50% 100% (b) Figure 1: Examples of angry and happy expressions. Each emotional expression is presented as neutral, at 50% and 100% emotion intensity (from left to right) and with direct eye gaze (top) and averted eye gaze (bottom). disorder and suggest severe interference in everyday social functioning Typically Developing Children. Typically developing children (TD; mean age = 9.8 years; age range = 8 12 years) were recruited through schools in the Chicago area in order to match children with autism disorder and typically developing children on sex, chronological age, and verbal mental age. Only typically developing children who were enrolled in normal, age-appropriate classrooms were considered. Of the typically developing children recruited, 22 children with autism disorder were matched to 22 typically developing children on sex (17 males, 5 females) and chronological age (±2 months). Group differences in verbal mental age could not be completely eliminated (AD: VMA = 9.32, SD = 1.69; TD: VMA = 10.64, SD = 1.67) and were statistically controlled for the analysis of covariance. Verbal mental age was obtained from age equivalent scores from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III; [37]). from the MacBrain Face Stimulus Set [38]. These highintensity expressions were considered to be at 100% strength. To create intermediate (i.e., 50% in-tensity) expressions, a procedure described in Calder et al. [39] was used. This process consisted of positioning 160 points manually on the anatomical landmarks in each neutral and intense emotional photograph of the same person and combining these images using Morph X software ( This resulted in the morphed intermediate expression that represented a 50% deviationinthepatternofrelevantmusclemovementsaway from the neutral expression [39]. Each emotional expression was presented at 0% (or neutral), 50%, and 100% expression strength (see Figure 1). Utilizing Adobe Photoshop software, face stimuli with direct eye gaze were computer-altered to display averted eye gaze. Averted gaze shift (right or left) was randomly assigned for each averted gaze face. Photographs were printed in black and white scale approximately 20 cm 24 cm (see Figure 1) Measures and Procedure. Each participating child was tested individually in a quiet area outside of the classroom at a time specified by the school. A school professional (e.g., teacher, nurse, and teacher s aide) was present at each session for all children. The experimental procedure was divided into two 20-minute sessions to accommodate children s school schedules. The first session consisted of the Emotion Recognition Task and the Emotion Expression Intensity Rating Task, and the second session consisted of the Emotion Situation Task which provided data for another study not included here andtheadministrationoftheppvt.theprocedureforeach experimental task is outlined below Emotion Recognition Task. Facial expressions of happy, angry, sad, fear, and neutral emotions of twelve adult Caucasian actors (6 males and 6 females) were selected Emotion Photographs Validation Check. In order to presentemotionalstimuliwithdirectoravertedeyegaze at varying expression intensities in the present study, we had to construct the stimuli ourselves, using the procedures outlined above, because none currently existed. To validate the photographic stimuli, a sample (N =40) of undergraduates students at our university was asked to identify each emotional expression prior to the beginning of the study. Usingfrequencyanalysis,itwasfoundthat98%ofcollege students identified happy, anger, sad, and fear correctly at 100% expression strength with direct or averted eye gaze. At 50% expression strength, there was more variation: for sad and fear expressions, averted eye gaze expressions were identified correctly more often (M = 81%) than expressions with direct eye gaze (M =70%).Incontrast,happyandangry expressions were identified correctly 89% 98% of the time, respectively, regardless of eye gaze direction. Details about

5 Autism Research and Treatment 5 the validation procedure itself are available from the first author Emotion Recognition Task Procedure. During the emotion recognition task, each child was presented with a set of 60 photographs of facial expressions. Forty-eight photographs included happy, angry, sad, and fearful expressions at 50% and 100% expression strengths, each with direct or averted eye gaze, and 12 photographs showed neutral faces with direct or averted eye gaze. There were a total of 12 facial expressions per emotion. To control for possible order effects, four different sets were created. Within each set, the direction of the eye gaze and expression strength were counterbalanced such that photographs with direct and averted eye gaze and 50% and 100% expression strength did not appear back to back. Potential priming effect was controlled by organizing facial expressions such that a 50% photograph never followed a 100% photograph for the same emotional expression. Each set was rotated among the participating children as enrollment progressed. A categorization procedure in which children indicated how the person in each picture felt was used. The task started with two practice items in order to assure that children understood the nature of the task. After successful practice trials, each child was presented with the rest of the set, one photograph at a time. Photographs remained visible for as long as the child needed to identify the emotion displayed. After presenting participants with a photograph, the researcher asked How do you think this person feels? followed by presentation of a response panel of schematic faces that included verbal labels that the experimenter pointed to while saying Does she/he feel happy, sad, angry, scared or just ok? The schematic faces were black and white line drawings (4 cm 4cm)adaptedfrompastresearch[40, 41]. The display of schematic faces was counterbalanced in order to control for biases due to a preference for a particular position. The child was allowed to either point to a schematic face or verbally state the answer. All children, however, verbally stated their answers Emotion Intensity Rating Task and Procedure. Asubset of 18 pictures was selected from the emotion recognition task. Two expressions (randomly selected) were used as practice items. Sixteen experimental stimuli included four emotional expressions (happy, angry, sad, and fear) at 50% and 100% of their expression strength, with direct and averted eye gaze. Using a 4-point scale, each participant was asked to judge the intensity of the emotion presented on the facial stimuli. Similar to the Hoffner and Badzinski [42] rating scale procedure, four circles increasing in size were labeled: a little bit (happy, angry, sad, and scared), pretty (happy, angry, sad, and scared), very (happy, angry, sad, and scared), and very, very (happy,angry,sad,and scared). Each picture was presented one at a time. Participants were given as much time as they needed to accomplish this task. 4. Results 4.1. Preliminary Analyses and Data Screening. Prior research suggests that the recognition of emotions might be related to overall development of verbal ability, as several studies haveshownthatwhenchildrenwithautismdisorderand typically developing children were matched on verbal mental age (VMA) no differences on facial emotion recognition tasks were found [43, 44]. In the present research, VMA was obtained from age equivalent scores from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III). An independent-samples t-test revealed that the difference in VMA between children with autism disorder and typically developing children was significant, t(42) = 2.69, P <.05. Therefore, in order to account for the effects of verbal ability, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used with VMA as a covariate in all remaining analyses. An examination of the homogeneity of regression assumption necessary for the ANCOVA showed no violation, as evident by the nonsignificant diagnostic group (AD, TD) VMA interactions (Fs(1, 40) = , P.41) Children s Ability to Recognize Facially Expressed Emotions: The Effects of Expression Intensity and Eye Gaze Direction. Children s mean percent accuracy scores for each emotion, across expression intensity and eye gaze directions, are shown in Table 1. To establish that children selected emotion labels for each facial expression at or above chance, goodness of fit χ 2 tests were utilized. Results indicated that children were not choosing emotions based on chance alone, χ 2 (4, N=44) = , P <.001.Amixed4(emotion type:happy,angry,sad,fear) 2 (expression intensity: 50%, 100%) 2 (eye gaze direction: direct, averted) 2(diagnostic group: AD, TD) ANCOVA was performed on the accuracy scores. The Bonferroni approach was used with all post hoc pairwise comparisons in order to control for familywise error rate Recognition of Emotional Expressions: Between Group Effects at 100% and 50% Intensities. A significant diagnostic group emotion type expression strength eye gaze direction four-way interaction, Λ =.80, F(3, 39) = 3.34, P <.03, andpartialη 2 =.21 was found. Follow-up analyses revealed several between-group findings, as shown in Table 1. Children with autism disorder and typically developing children did not differ in their ability to correctly identify happy and angry emotional expressions at 100% and 50% strength with direct and averted eye gaze, Fs(1, 41) = , P >.05; see Table 1. In contrast, children with autism disorder were significantly less accurate than typically developing children at identifying fear across both levels of emotion intensity (i.e., 100% and 50%), and with direct and averted eye gaze, Fs(1, 41) = , P <.05; see Table 1. A trend toward significance (F(1, 41) = 3.65, P <.06) was also observed for sad emotional expressions at 100% strength with direct eye gaze, such that typically developing children were more accurate at identifying sadness than children with autism disorder.

6 6 Autism Research and Treatment Table 1: Mean percent correct values for happy, sad, angry, and fear emotional expressions as a function of expression strength and eye gaze direction. 100% emotion strength 50% emotion strength Direct Averted Direct Averted Typically developing children Happy 88.0 (3.6) 92.8 (4.4) 58.0 (5.0) 79.5 (6.0) Sad 72.0 (7.2) 78.7 (7.2) 20.9 (7.8) 47.1 (5.2) Angry 78.0 (4.8) 96.3 (4.3) 41.6 (4.9) 47.1 (4.6) Fear 88.1 (6.6) 74.7 (6.0) 57.4 (5.2) 71.7 (8.1) Children with autism disorder Happy 94.8 (3.6) 89.6 (4.4) 50.1 (5.1) 86.2 (6.0) Sad 54.1 (6.4) 68.5 (7.1) 30.2 (5.1) 33.6 (5.2) Angry 77.5 (4.2) 86.2 (4.7) 40.3 (5.4) 49.5 (4.6) Fear 54.9 (5.6) 54.5 (6.0) 37.8 (5.9) 58.5 (7.0) Adjusted group means as percent correct values are shown. Group means are adjusted for VMA. Standard deviations are in parentheses. Asterisks indicate significant between-group differences (typically developing versus with autism disorder), P.05. Boldedvalues indicate significant within-groupdifferences (direct versus averted eye gaze), P.05. Mean accuracy (%) Happy Sad Angry Fear Mean accuracy (%) Happy Sad Angry Fear Direct eye gaze (TD) Averted eye gaze (TD) Direct eye gaze (ASD) Averted eye gaze (ASD) Direct eye gaze (TD) Averted eye gaze (TD) Direct eye gaze (ASD) Averted eye gaze (ASD) (a) 100% strength level (b) 50% strength level Figure 2: Mean percent accuracy scores for the effects of eye gaze direction on recognition of facial expressions at 100% and 50% expression strengths for children with autism disorder and typically developing children Recognition of Emotional Expressions: Within-Group Effects at 100% Emotion Intensity. Pairwise post hoc comparisons were also used to examine the within-group effects of eye gaze direction on children s accuracy in recognizing each of the four emotional expressions at each intensity (Table 1 and Figure 2). In a number of instances, a different pattern of within group findings emerged for children with autism disorder and typically developing children. Although averted eye gaze enhanced recognition of fear expressions at 100% intensity in typically developing children, it did not enhance performance in children with autism disorder, F(1, 39) = 5.52, P >.05 (see Table 1 and Figure 2). Additionally, full expressions of anger were recognized significantly better when presented with averted versus direct eye gaze in typically developing children, F(1, 41) = 13.88, P <.01, although only a trend was observed for children with autism disorder, F(1, 41) = 3.55, P <.06; see Table 1. Finally, at 100% expression strength, eye gaze direction did not influence recognition of happy expressions, Fs(1, 41) = , P >.05, intypically developing children or children with autism disorder (see Figure 2) Recognition of Emotional Expressions: Within-Group Effects at 50% Emotion Intensity. When emotional expressions were presented at 50% of their strength, typically developing children were better at recognizing expressions with averted eye gaze direction than direct eye gaze for happy, F(1,41)=10.86,P <.001, sadf(1,41)=13.86,p <.001, and fear F(1, 41) = 5.32, P <.02 expressions. Children with autism disorder benefited from averted eye gaze in the case of happy expressions, F(1, 41) = 32.97, P < TheEffectsofEmotionType:Betweenandwithin-Group Effects. Finally, an interactive effect of diagnostic group emotion type, Λ =.79,F(3,39)=4.89,P <.04,partial η 2 =.18, and two-way interaction was found for emotion type. Between-group pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni

7 Autism Research and Treatment 7 Table 2: Mean percent correct values for happy, sad, angry, and fear emotional expressions for children with AD and typically developing children. Happy Sad Angry Fear Typically developing children 79.6 (4.7) 54.5 (6.9) 65.8 (4.7) 73.2 (6.4) a Children with autism disorder 80.2 (4.8) 45.0 (5.9) 63.3 (4.9) 49.3 (7.1) b Adjusted group means as percent correct values are shown. Group means are adjusted for VMA. Standard deviations are in parentheses. a, b Superscripts indicate significant between-group differences, P.05. correction revealed that children with autism disorder were less accurate in their recognition of fear, F(1, 39) = 7.20, P <.02, than their typically developing peers. No group (AD, TD) differences were observed for happy, sad, or angry emotions, Fs(1, 41) =.04.28, P >.05. Typically developing children identified happy expressions with significantly greater accuracy than sadness and anger, but not fear, F(3, 37) = 11.65, P <.001. Children with autism disorder were better at recognizing happy expressions than sad, angry, and fear expressions, F(3,37)= 25.04,P <.001;see Table Children s Attributions of an Emotional State to Neutral Facial Expressions. A mixed 2 (diagnostic group: AD, TD) 2 (eye gaze direction: direct, averted) ANCOVA was performed on children s accuracy in labeling neutral facial expressions as no emotion. An interactive effect of diagnostic goup eye gaze direction was found F(1,41)=20.71,P <.05, η 2 =.25. Children with autism disorder were less accurate at identifying neutral expressions as nonemotional when they included direct eye gaze direction (M = 71%, SD = 5.1), than TD children (M = 85%, SD = 5.2). Children with autism disorder were more likely to label a neutral facial photograph with an emotion than typically developing children (see error analysis below). No diagnostic group differences were observed for neutral expressions with averted eye gaze Error Analysis in Recognition of Facially Expressed Emotions. Toexaminethetypeoferrorschildrenmadewhen identifying faces, emotional expressions were analyzed in termsofthefrequencywithwhichthetargetemotionwas identified incorrectly using a 4 (error type: happy, angry, sad, fear) 2 (expression intensity: 100%, 50%) 2 (eye gaze direction: direct, averted) 2(diagnosticgroup:AD,TD) ANCOVAs, with diagnostic group as a between-subject factor and expression intensity, eye gaze direction, and error type as within-subject factors. The analysis of errors revealed a significant error type expression intensity eye gaze direction interactions for the happy, angry, and fear emotions, Λs =.70.79, Fs(3, 40) = , P <.01,andpartialη 2 =.28. Children with autism disorder and typically developing children were more likely tolabelhappy,angry,sad,andfearexpressionsasneutral when faces were presented at 50% expression strength with direct eye gaze than when presented with averted eye gaze (all Ps <.01). Children with autism disorder mislabeled sad expressions at 50% expression strength with averted eye gaze as anger more often than typically developing children. A significant main effect of error type, F(3, 117) = 3.98, P <.03, partial η 2 =.16,wasfoundwhenchildrenwereviewing neutral expressions. Children across both groups confused neutral expressions with fear more so than with any other emotion Children s Ratings of Emotional Intensity: The Effects of Expression Intensity and Eye Gaze Direction. It was predicted that averted eye gaze would increase emotional intensity ratings for expressions of sadness and fear, whereas direct eye gaze would increase intensity ratings for happy and angry expressions in at least typically developing children. Interactive effects of diagnostic group expression strength, Λ =.76,F(1, 41) = 14.82, P <.001, partialη 2 =.28,and diagnostic group eye gaze direction, Λ =.88, F(1, 41) = 5.23, P <.02,partialη 2 =.11, were found. Whilebothchildrenwithautismdisorderandtypically developing children rated 100% expressions as more emotionally intense than 50% expressions, F(1, 41) = 83.45, P<.001, F(1, 41) = 12.11, P <.01, respectively, children with autism disorder rated facial expressions presented at 50% as more emotionally intense than typically developing children, F(1, 39) = 8.35, P <.001. Children with autism disorder also gave higher emotion intensity ratings to expressions with direct eye gaze than typically developing children, F(1, 41) =10.68,P <.01. Within-group comparisons revealed that children with autism disorder perceived facial expressions with direct eye gaze as more emotionally intense than those with averted eye gaze, F(1,41)=22.39,P <.001, whereas no differences in intensity ratings between direct and averted eye gaze expressions were observed for typically developing children (see Table 3). Intensity ratings also varied across different emotional expressions, Λ =.69,F(3, 39) = 5.77, P <.01 partial η 2 =.31, for children with autism disorder and typically developing children. Children with autism disorder rated expressions of fear as more intense than typically developing children Relation between Recognition Accuracy and SRS Scores in Children with Autism Disorder. Linear regression analyses revealed a significant negative relation between the social communication subscale and recognition of fear, β =.64, P <.05 in children with autism disorder: children with a more severe social communication impairment were less accurate at recognizing fear than children with autism disorder who showed less impairment. A similar trend emerged for recognition of sad expressions, β =.58, P <.07, in children with autism disorder. No other relations between the remaining SRS subscales scores and recognition accuracy (or

8 8 Autism Research and Treatment Table 3: Mean intensity ratings for facial expressions by emotion strength and eye gaze direction in children with AD and typically developing children. Mean intensity ratings for facial expressions by emotion strength 100% emotion strength 50% emotion strength Typically developing children 1.82 (.90) a 2.73 (.88) Children with autism disorder 2.38 (.80) b 2.73 (.89) Mean intensity ratings of facial expressions by eye gaze direction Direct eye gaze Averted eye gaze Typically developing children 2.36 (.08) a 2.25 (.09) Children with autism disorder 2.72 (.08) b 2.33 (.09) Intensity ratings ranged from 1 (least intense) to 4 (most intense). Standard deviations are in parentheses. a,b Superscripts indicate significant between-group differences (typically developing versus with autism disorder), P.05. Asterisks indicate significant within-group differences, P.05. other DVs) were found, βs =.04.13, Ps >.05. PIQ scores, VMA scores and diagnostic scores were not significantly related to children s responses (rs =.07.23, Ps >.05). 5. Discussion In the present research, a number of significant findings regarding recognition of emotional expressions in children with autism disorder and typically developing children were revealed. As predicted, children with autism disorder and typically developing children did not differ in their ability to correctly identify happy and angry emotional expressions at 50% or 100% intensity with direct or averted eye gaze. However, the ability to identify fear at 50% or 100% expression intensity with direct or averted eyes was significantly less accurate in children with autism disorder than typically developing children. A trend was also found for sadness expressions: children with autism disorder were less accurate in recognizing sadness at 100% intensity and direct eye gaze than typically developing children. The lack of difference between children with autism disorder and typically developing children in recognizing happy expressions regardless of eye gaze direction and expression intensity is consistent with the assertion that recognition of positive emotion is relatively intact in children and adults with autism disorder (e.g., [19, 45, 46]). Happy expressions are characterized by a unique mouth pattern that alone could be sufficient to discriminate them from other emotions [47]. According to Gross [46], it is possible that a visual preference for the lower part of the face accounts for the superior performance found in their recognition of happiness in individuals with autism disorder, although this cannot be determined from our data. Also consistent with our prediction, the present study demonstrated that children with autism disorder are able to recognize expressions of anger as well as their typically developing peers. However, the expected facilitative effects of direct eye gaze on the recognition of anger were not found in either group. In fact, in typically developing children, averted eye gaze enhanced recognition of anger expressions. However,whiledirecteyegazemaynothaveimprovedchildren s recognition of anger, all children perceived expressions with direct eye gaze as more intense than expressions with avertedeyegaze.thisisconsistentwithpreviousresearchthat demonstrated greater ratings of intensity for angry faces that were coupled with direct eye gaze than averted eye gaze in neurotypical adults [48]. It is not clear why averted eyes resulted in better recognition of anger expressions, as this finding is inconsistent with thesharedsignalhypothesis.onepossibilityisthatchildren pay more attention to emotional expressions such as anger when they include averted eye gaze because they find that the same expressions with direct eyes too stimulating. This hypothesis is consistent with our findings that children rated faces with direct eye gaze, including angry expressions, as more intense than faces with averted eye gaze. It may also be that while the shared signal hypothesis holds true for adults, there may be a developmental trajectory to this effect, such that younger children do not demonstrate it. Additional research is needed to assess this assertion. The present findings also showed that children with autism disorder were not as accurate as typically developing children in recognizing fear, regardless of expression intensity or eye gaze direction. Our results add to the growingbodyofliteraturethathasreportedsimilar fear recognition impairments in adults with autism disorder (e.g., [45, 49]). A number of explanations have been given to explain this impairment in recognizing fear expressions, including atypical neurological functioning in individuals with autism disorder. For example, Ashwin et al. [15, 45] showed a differential pattern of neural activity in various social brain areas in adults with autism disorder, compared with typical adults, during the perception of fearful facial expressions. These differences included less activation in the leftamygdalaandleftorbitofrontalcortexbraininadultswith autism disorder than in neurotypical adults. Deficits in the recognition of fear may also be due to the fact that fear is communicated primarily by the eye region of the face and that individuals with autism disorder may pay less attention to the eyes. Although all children in this study showed sensitivity to eye gaze direction (e.g., both rated expressions with direct eyes as more intense than expressions with averted eyes for fear), averted eye gaze facilitated recognition in typically developing but not

9 Autism Research and Treatment 9 children with autism disorder. As Wallace et al. [19] note, eyes may appear differently for individual emotions, and for fear, the eyes themselves may be negatively arousing and maybeevenbeoverarousing,fortheindividualwithautism disorder. In expressing fear, there is a large amount of sclera visible that may lead to fearful eyes being perceived as threatening to the individual with autism. Thus, less attention maybepaidtothem[19]. Finally, our trend finding (P <.06) for less accurate recognition of sadness in children with autism disorder is consistent with the results of Wallace et al. [28], whose research revealed a particularly diminished sensitivity to sad expressions in adolescents with autism disorder. It is also consistent with Boraston et al. s [16] finding of less accurate recognition of sadness in adults with autism disorder. The present results provide evidence that difficulties in recognizing fear and sad expressions may begin early in individuals with autism disorder and thus warrant further investigation. Interestingly, negative relations between the social communication subscale of the SRS and recognition of fear and sadness were found in the present study, with children with more severe social communication impairment being less accurate at recognizing these emotions than children who showed less impairment. These results are consistent with Bal et al. [50],whoshowedthatmoresevereautism disorder symptoms identified through the SRS, especially the social communication subscale, were related to less accurate recognition of emotion from dynamic (video) stimuli. Nevertheless, it is unclear whether social communicative difficulties result in less accurate recognition of negative emotions such as fear and sadness or vice versa. It has been suggested that an understanding of certain emotions such as fear requires joint communication between parents (or other adults) and children [51]. That is, the capacity to communicate, coordinate, and share attention with a social partner regarding characteristics that underlie certain emotion situations, particularly those eliciting fear or sadness, are needed for adequate perception of those emotions. For example, learning through joint attention and communication with an adult or another individual may be required in order to develop an understanding that an expression of fear is a response to a potentially threatening object in the environment. Finally, in the present research it was found that intensity ratings for emotional expressions at 100% strength were similar for children with autism disorder and typically developing children. Moreover, both groups rated expressions presented at 100% strength as more intense than expressions presented at 50% strength. Notably, children with autism disorder rated expressions with direct eye gaze as more intense than expressions with averted eye gaze. Children with autism disorder also rated and fear emotional expressions as more intense than their typically developing peers. Children with autism disorder were also more likely than typically developing children to label neutral (no emotion) photographs with direct eyes as representing an emotion. Consistent with Kuusikko et al. [29], when children with autism disorder and typically developing children labeled neutral facial expressions with an emotion, they almost always labeled it with a negative emotion. Although we feel these are compelling findings, limitations of this study should be noted. Firstly, we were not able to match typically developing and children with autism disorder on both chronological age and verbal mental age (VMA). However, when used as a covariate, VMA did not reveal any additional findings. Secondly, while evidence was foundthatchildrenwithautismdisorderweresensitivetoeye gaze direction, given the extensive number of manipulations in the present study, we were unable to present more than three examples per emotion of averted and direct eye gaze directions. Thirdly, while we were able to recruit all of our children with autism disorder from classrooms or programs designed to serve children with autism disorder exclusively, and ADOS-G and ADI-R scores were included in most (over 90%) children s records, we did not administer these tests ourselves. We were also not able to include a range of children on the spectrum because of the makeup of the children in the classrooms. We acknowledge that this limits the generalizability of our findings. Finally, some have suggested that static photographical images are not ecologically valid, as emotion in everyday life is often expressed quickly and dynamically. Although static photographs may be ecologically limited, there are several reasons why the use of such stimuli in a study has value. Children s ability to recognize emotion from static images often eliminates the difficulty, and resultant floor effects, in their ability to recognize emotion from dynamic images. Additionally, emotion recognition training for children with autism disorder is often completed with static photographs of emotion. In fact, we purposely began this study before any emotion training began that year in the classroom, although we acknowledge that the children in our sample may have received emotion training in prior years. More careful inspection of the emotion training children received in a classroom or program prior to the start of research studies should be considered in future research. In spite of these limitations, an important implication of the present results is that photographs of varying expression intensities and eye gaze directions are used in emotion training, with an emphasis on presenting subtle intensities of expressions. This would provide more ecological validity with real-world emotions, to the extent that subtle displays of emotional expressions are an integral part of everyday life. 6. Conclusions To conclude by highlighting our key findings, the present study showed that eye gaze direction modulates emotion perception from facial expressions in both typically developing and children with autism disorder and that children with autism disorder are sensitive to gaze direction. However, children with autism disorder showed a more significant impairment in the recognition of fear expressions and a trend for sad expressions, rated happy and fear expressions as more intense, and rated most 50% strength expressions as more intense than typically developing children. These compelling findings merit further investigation. In future

10 10 Autism Research and Treatment research, electrophysiological and neural imaging methods withourstimuliwouldbevaluableinsheddinglightonthe origins of these and other differences between children with autism disorder and typically developing children in their processing of emotional expressions. Conflict of Interests The authors declare that there is no conflict of interests regarding the publication of this paper. Acknowledgments The authors thank the children who participated in this study and the staff and administrators at their schools or programs for their assistance throughout the study. Portions of this research were presented at the biennial meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development (2011) and the International Society for Research on Emotion (2013). References [1] S. Begeer, H. M. Koot, C. Rieffe, M. Meerum Terwogt, and H. Stegge, Emotional competence in children with autism: diagnostic criteria and empirical evidence, Developmental Review, vol. 28, no. 3, pp , [2] M. B. Harms, A. Martin, and G. L. Wallace, Facial emotion recognitioninautismspectrumdisorders:areviewofbehavioral and neuroimaging studies, Neuropsychology Review, vol. 20, no. 3, pp , [3] R. J. Itier and M. Batty, Neural bases of eye and gaze processing: the core of social cognition, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 33, no. 6, pp , [4] K. Nation and S. Penny, Sensitivity to eye gaze in autism: is it normal? Is it automatic? Is it social? Development and Psychopathology,vol.20,no.1,pp.79 97,2008. [5] M. Uljarevic and A. Hamilton, Recognition of emotions in autism: a formal meta-analysis, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,vol.43,no.7,pp ,2013. [6] K. M. Dalton, B. M. Nacewicz, T. Johnstone et al., Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism, Nature Neuroscience,vol.8,no.4,pp ,2005. [7] A. Klin, W. Jones, R. Schultz, F. Volkmar, and D. Cohen, Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism, Archives of General Psychiatry,vol.59,no.9,pp ,2002. [8]F.R.VolkmarandL.C.Mayes, Gazebehaviorinautism, Development and Psychopathology, vol. 2, pp , [9] S. R. Leekam, B. López, and C. Moore, Attention and joint attentioninpreschoolchildrenwithautism, Developmental Psychology,vol.36,no.2,pp ,2000. [10] R. B. Adams Jr. and R. E. Kleck, Perceived Gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion, Psychological Science,vol.14,no.6,pp ,2003. [11] R. B. Adams Jr. and R. E. Kleck, Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion, Emotion, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 3 11, [12] R. Graham and K. S. LaBar, Garner interference reveals dependencies between emotional expression and gaze in face perception, Emotion, vol. 7, no.2, pp , [13] D. Sander, D. Grandjean, S. Kaiser, T. Wehrle, and K. R. Scherer, Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: evidence for appraisal theories of emotion, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,vol.19,no.3,pp , [14]H.Akechi,A.Senju,Y.Kikuchi,Y.Tojo,H.Osanai,andT. Hasegawa, Does gaze direction modulate facial expression processing in children with autism spectrum disorder? Child Development,vol.80,no.4,pp ,2009. [15] C. Ashwin, E. Chapman, L. Colle, and S. Baron-Cohen, Impaired recognition of negative basic emotions in autism: a test of the amygdala theory, Social Neuroscience,vol.1,pp , [16] Z. Boraston, S.-J. Blakemore, R. Chilvers, and D. Skuse, Impaired sadness recognition is linked to social interaction deficit in autism, Neuropsychologia, vol. 45, no. 7, pp , [17] K. A. Pelphrey, N. J. Sasson, J. S. Reznick, G. Paul, B. D. Goldman, and J. Piven, Visual scanning of faces in autism, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol.32,no.4, pp , [18] J. L. Lindner and L. A. Rosén, Decoding of emotion through facial expression, prosody and verbal content in children and adolescents with Asperger s syndrome, JournalofAutismand Developmental Disorders, vol. 36, no. 6, pp , [19] S.Wallace,M.Coleman,andA.Bailey, Aninvestigationofbasic facial expression recognition in autism spectrum disorders, Cognition and Emotion,vol.22,no.7,pp ,2008. [20] L. A. Camras and K. Allison, Children s understanding of emotional facial expressions and verbal labels, Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,vol.9,no.2,pp.84 94,1985. [21] D. Davidson, The role of basic, self-conscious and selfconscious evaluative emotions in children s memory and understanding of emotion, Motivation and Emotion,vol.30,no.3,pp , [22] E. R. Tuminello and D. Davidson, What the face and body reveal: in-group emotion effects and stereotyping of emotion in African American and European American children, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,vol.110,no.2,pp ,2011. [23] S. C. Widen and J. A. Russell, A closer look at preschoolers freely produced labels for facial expressions, Developmental Psychology,vol.39,no.1,pp ,2003. [24] K. O Connor, J. P. Hamm, and I. J. Kirk, The neurophysiological correlates of face processing in adults and children with Asperger s syndrome, Brain and Cognition, vol. 59, pp , [25] C.M.Herba,S.Landau,T.Russell,C.Ecker,andM.L.Phillips, The development of emotion-processing in children: effects of age, emotion, and intensity, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 47, no. 11, pp , [26] C. A. Mazefsky and D. P. Oswald, Emotion perception in Asperger s syndrome and high-functioning autism: the importance of diagnostic criteria and cue intensity, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,vol.37,no.6,pp ,2007. [27] M. J. Law Smith, B. Montagne, D. I. Perrett, M. Gill, and L. Gallagher, Detecting subtle facial emotion recognition deficits in high-functioning Autism using dynamic stimuli of varying intensities, Neuropsychologia,vol.48,no.9,pp ,2010. [28] G. L. Wallace, L. K. Case, M. B. Harms, J. A. Silvers, L. Kenworthy, and A. Martin, Diminished sensitivity to sad facial

11 Autism Research and Treatment 11 expressions in high functioning autism spectrum disorders is associated with symptomatology and adaptive functioning, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 41, no. 11, pp ,2011. [29] S.Kuusikko,H.Haapsamo,E.Jansson-Verkasaloetal., Emotion recognition in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,vol.39,no.6,pp ,2009. [30] A. Senju, K. Yaguchi, Y. Tojo, and T. Hasegawa, Eye contact does not facilitate detection in children with autism, Cognition, vol.89,no.1,pp.b43 B51,2003. [31] American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Arthur, Washington, DC, USA, 4th edition, [32] C. Lord, S. Risi, L. Lambrecht et al., The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,vol.30, no. 3, pp , [33]C.Lord,M.Rutter,andA.L.Couteur, Autismdiagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders, JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders, vol. 24,no.5,pp ,1994. [34]E.Schopler,R.J.Reichier,andB.R.Renner,The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS), Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles, Calif, USA, [35] S. Sparrow, D. V. Cicchetti, and D. A. Balla, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, Minn, USA, 2nd edition, [36] J. N. Constantino and C. P. Gruber, Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) Manual, Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles, Calif, USA, [37] L. M. Dunn and L. M. Dunn, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test- Third Edition, American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, Minn, USA, [38] N. Tottenham, J. W. Tanaka, A. C. Leon et al., The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants, Psychiatry Research, vol.168,no.3,pp , [39] A.J.Calder,A.W.Young,D.I.Perrett,N.L.Etcoff,andD.Rowland, Categorical perception of morphed facial expressions, Visual Cognition,vol.3,no.2,pp ,1996. [40] G. Green, Early behavioral intervention for autism: what does research tell us? in Behavioral Interventions for Young Children with Autism, C. Maurice, G. Green, and S. Luce, Eds., pp , Pro Ed, Austin, Tex, USA, [41] S. Thayer and W. Schiff, Stimulus factors in observer judgment of social interaction: facial expression and motion pattern, The American Journal of Psychology,vol.82,no.1,pp.73 85,1969. [42] C. Hoffner and D. M. Badzinski, Children s integration of facial and situational cues to emotion, Child Development,vol.60,no. 2, pp , [43] F. Castelli, Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal development, Autism,vol.9, no. 4, pp , [44] K. Loveland and B. Tunali-Kotoski, The school-aged child with autism, in The Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders,F.R.Volkmar,A.Klin,andR.Paul,Eds.,pp , Wiley, New York, NY, USA, 3rd edition, [45] C. Ashwin, S. Baron-Cohen, S. Wheelwright, M. O Riordan, and E. T. Bullmore, Differential activation of the amygdala and the social brain during fearful face-processing in Asperger Syndrome, Neuropsychologia, vol. 45, no. 1, pp. 2 14, [46] T. F. Gross, The perception of four basic emotions in human and nonhuman faces by children with autism and other developmental disabilities, JournalofAbnormalChildPsychology, vol.32,no.5,pp ,2004. [47] P. Ekman, Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, vol. 99,no.3,pp ,1992. [48] W. Sato, T. Kochiyama, S. Yoshikawa, E. Naito, and M. Matsumura, Enhanced neural activity in response to dynamic facial expressions of emotion: an fmri study, Cognitive Brain Research,vol.20,no.1,pp.81 91,2004. [49] K. Humphreys, N. Minshew, G. L. Leonard, and M. Behrmann, A fine-grained analysis of facial expression processing in highfunctioning adults with autism, Neuropsychologia, vol. 45, no. 4, pp , [50]E.Bal,E.Harden,D.Lamb,A.V.VanHecke,J.W.Denver, and S. W. Porges, Emotion recognition in children with autism spectrum disorders: relations to eye gaze and autonomic state, JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders,vol.40,no.3, pp , [51] P. Mundy and C. Burnetter, Joint attention and neurodevelopment, in Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders,F.R.Volkmar,A.Klin,andR.Paul,Eds.,pp , John Wiley, Hoboken, NJ, USA, 3rd edition, 2005.

12 MEDIATORS of INFLAMMATION The Scientific World Journal Gastroenterology Research and Practice Journal of Diabetes Research International Journal of Journal of Endocrinology Immunology Research Disease Markers Submit your manuscripts at BioMed Research International PPAR Research Journal of Obesity Journal of Ophthalmology Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine Stem Cells International Journal of Oncology Parkinson s Disease Computational and Mathematical Methods in Medicine AIDS Behavioural Neurology Research and Treatment Oxidative Medicine and Cellular Longevity

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Overview Diagnostic Criteria Current: Diagnostic & Statistical

More information

References 1. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2006). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th edition. Minnesota: American Guidance Service. 2. Wilkinson, K.

References 1. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2006). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th edition. Minnesota: American Guidance Service. 2. Wilkinson, K. Aim This presentation will present data from eye-tracking research technology concerning social attention patterns: gaze fixations on human figures either interacting or not interacting with each other

More information

Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal development

Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal development Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal development autism 2005 SAGE Publications and The National Autistic Society Vol 9(4) 428 449; 056082 1362-3613(200510)9:4

More information

An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions

An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions Leigh-Ann Heale Classic autism was first described by Kanner in 1943 (cited in Ellis,

More information

Grace Iarocci Ph.D., R. Psych., Associate Professor of Psychology Simon Fraser University

Grace Iarocci Ph.D., R. Psych., Associate Professor of Psychology Simon Fraser University Grace Iarocci Ph.D., R. Psych., Associate Professor of Psychology Simon Fraser University Theoretical perspective on ASD Where does attention and perception fit within the diagnostic triad of impairments?

More information

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Assessment and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder For this month s topic, I am excited to share my recent experience using the fifth edition of the Diagnostic

More information

PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions

PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions Session 6 The Concept of Emotion Lecturer: Dr. Annabella Osei-Tutu, Psychology Department Contact Information: aopare-henaku@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing

More information

Education Options for Children with Autism

Education Options for Children with Autism Empowering children with Autism and their families through knowledge and support Education Options for Children with Autism Starting school is a major milestone in a child s life, and a big step for all

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) 3313 3319 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Perception of emotions from facial expressions

More information

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D.

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. Autism Spectrum Disorder Neurodevelopmental disorder Reflects understanding of the etiology of disorder as related to alterations

More information

The emotional understanding of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Ronja Neumann. Tilburg University

The emotional understanding of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Ronja Neumann. Tilburg University 1 The emotional understanding of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Ronja Neumann Tilburg University ANR: 126287 Supervisor: Anke Scheeren, Dr. Bachelor thesis Psychology and Health Department Developmental

More information

Spatial cognition and emotion recognition as a function of ASD

Spatial cognition and emotion recognition as a function of ASD University of Richmond UR Scholarship Repository Honors Theses Student Research 2015 Spatial cognition and emotion recognition as a function of ASD Catalina Cumpanasoiu Follow this and additional works

More information

Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism

Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism By Lauren Lowry Hanen Certified SLP and Clinical Staff Writer Several years ago I worked on a diagnostic assessment team. I remember the first

More information

Emotion understanding and social competence in young children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders

Emotion understanding and social competence in young children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders Emotion understanding and social competence in young children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders Metaxia Patsiora Master Thesis Developmental and Educational Psychology Word Count : ca. 8,000 Student number

More information

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER What is the DSM-5? The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (the DSM) is developed by the

More information

Fact Sheet 8. DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder

Fact Sheet 8. DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder Fact Sheet 8 DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder A diagnosis of autism is made on the basis of observed behaviour. There are no blood tests, no single defining symptom and no physical characteristics that

More information

1/30/2018. Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Disclosures. Learning Objectives

1/30/2018. Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Disclosures. Learning Objectives Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Associate Professor Emory University School of Medicine Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition 1 Disclosures As

More information

Agenda. Making the Connection. Facts about ASD. Respite Presentation. Agenda. Facts about ASD. Triad of Impairments. 3 Diagnoses on spectrum

Agenda. Making the Connection. Facts about ASD. Respite Presentation. Agenda. Facts about ASD. Triad of Impairments. 3 Diagnoses on spectrum Making the Connection Respite Presentation Agenda Agenda Facts about ASD Triad of Impairments 3 Diagnoses on spectrum Characteristics of ASD Behaviour Facts about ASD It is the most common form of any

More information

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers!

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers! Marisa Filipe Dezembro de 2013 pdpsi10020@fpce.up.pt EXCL/MHC-LIN/0688/2012 Summary Introduction: Prosodic Abilities in Children and Young Adults with Typical & Non-Typical Development Prosodic abilities

More information

Understanding One s Own Emotions in Cognitively-Able Preadolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Understanding One s Own Emotions in Cognitively-Able Preadolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder DOI 10.1007/s10803-016-2769-6 ORIGINAL PAPER Understanding One s Own Emotions in Cognitively-Able Preadolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder Esther Ben-Itzchak 1,2 Shira Abutbul 1 Hadas Bela 1 Tom Shai

More information

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Parkland College A with Honors Projects Honors Program 2015 Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Hannah Li Parkland College Recommended Citation Li, Hannah, "Social and Pragmatic Language

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) Gantman, A., Kapp, S. K., Orenski, K., & Laugeson, E. A. (2012). Social skills training for young adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled

More information

Brain and Cognition, 48(2-3), (2002) Evaluation of nonverbal emotion in face and voice: some preliminary findings on a new battery of tests

Brain and Cognition, 48(2-3), (2002) Evaluation of nonverbal emotion in face and voice: some preliminary findings on a new battery of tests Brain and Cognition, 48(2-3), 499-514 (2002) Evaluation of nonverbal emotion in face and voice: some preliminary findings on a new battery of tests Marc David Pell McGill University, Montréal Abstract

More information

Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification. and. Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect. Nila K Leigh

Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification. and. Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect. Nila K Leigh 1 Who Needs Cheeks? Eyes and Mouths are Enough for Emotion Identification and Evidence for a Face Superiority Effect Nila K Leigh 131 Ave B (Apt. 1B) New York, NY 10009 Stuyvesant High School 345 Chambers

More information

Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Associate Professor Emory University School of Medicine Director of Research Operations Marcus Autism Center Vineland Adaptive

More information

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Marci Laurel, MA, CCC-SLP mlaurel@salud.unm.edu and Services (UCEDD) 1 Training Objectives Participants will: 1. Name at least three communication

More information

Spontaneous Attention to Faces in Asperger Sndrome using Ecologically Valid Static Stimuli

Spontaneous Attention to Faces in Asperger Sndrome using Ecologically Valid Static Stimuli Spontaneous Attention to Faces in Asperger Sndrome using Ecologically Valid Static Stimuli Hanley, M., McPhillips, M., Mulhern, G., & Riby, D. (2013). Spontaneous Attention to Faces in Asperger Sndrome

More information

Why do we look at people's eyes?

Why do we look at people's eyes? Journal of Eye Movement Research 1(1):1, 1-6 Why do we look at people's eyes? Elina Birmingham University of British Columbia Walter Bischof University of Alberta Alan Kingstone University of British Columbia

More information

Valence and Gender Effects on Emotion Recognition Following TBI. Cassie Brown Arizona State University

Valence and Gender Effects on Emotion Recognition Following TBI. Cassie Brown Arizona State University Valence and Gender Effects on Emotion Recognition Following TBI Cassie Brown Arizona State University Knox & Douglas (2009) Social Integration and Facial Expression Recognition Participants: Severe TBI

More information

What is Emotion? Emotion is a 4 part process consisting of: physiological arousal cognitive interpretation, subjective feelings behavioral expression.

What is Emotion? Emotion is a 4 part process consisting of: physiological arousal cognitive interpretation, subjective feelings behavioral expression. What is Emotion? Emotion is a 4 part process consisting of: physiological arousal cognitive interpretation, subjective feelings behavioral expression. While our emotions are very different, they all involve

More information

Originally appeared in Autism Spectrum News Exploring Educational Challenges Issue Vol. 5, No. 3 Winter Lynda Geller, Ph.D.

Originally appeared in Autism Spectrum News Exploring Educational Challenges Issue Vol. 5, No. 3 Winter Lynda Geller, Ph.D. Evidence-Based Interventions for Students on the Autism Spectrum Originally appeared in Autism Spectrum News Exploring Educational Challenges Issue Vol. 5, No. 3 Winter 2013 Lynda Geller, Ph.D. Spectrum

More information

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Edinburgh Research Explorer Edinburgh Research Explorer Emotional recognition in ASD from voices and faces Citation for published version: Stewart, ME, McAdam, C, Ota, M, Peppe, S & Cleland, J 2013, 'Emotional recognition in ASD

More information

Autism Research Update

Autism Research Update . Autism Research Update Issue 10: Challenging behaviours Autism Research Network Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth For more information contact: Dr Beatriz López, Department of Psychology

More information

Intensive Training. Early Childhood Intensive Training K-12 Intensive Training Building Your Future Intensive Training

Intensive Training. Early Childhood Intensive Training K-12 Intensive Training Building Your Future Intensive Training Intensive Training The Statewide Autism Resources and Training Project (START) Intensive Training focuses on evidencebased practices to increase knowledge and skills that enhance the educational programming

More information

SOCIAL JUDGMENTS ARE INFLUENCED BY BOTH FACIAL EXPRESSION AND DIRECTION OF EYE GAZE

SOCIAL JUDGMENTS ARE INFLUENCED BY BOTH FACIAL EXPRESSION AND DIRECTION OF EYE GAZE Social Cognition, Vol. 9, No. 4, 011, pp. 415 49 SOCIAL JUDGMENTS ARE INFLUENCED BY BOTH FACIAL EXPRESSION AND DIRECTION OF EYE GAZE Megan L. Willis Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia; University

More information

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: Can joint attention, imitation, and/or play skills predict future language abilities of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)? Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology)

More information

An investigation of basic facial expression recognition in autism spectrum disorders

An investigation of basic facial expression recognition in autism spectrum disorders Cognition and Emotion ISSN: 0269-9931 (Print) 1464-0600 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcem20 An investigation of basic facial expression recognition in autism spectrum disorders

More information

Study N Ages Study type Methodology Main findings m;f mean (sd) FSIQ mean (sd) Del;UPD Range Range

Study N Ages Study type Methodology Main findings m;f mean (sd) FSIQ mean (sd) Del;UPD Range Range Table 5 Social Cognition Study N Ages Study type Methodology Main findings m;f mean (sd) FSIQ mean (sd) Del;UPD Range Range Lo et al.(2013) 66 median 11 ToM 14 stories, 33 questions, 3 pretence Median

More information

Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome

Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome Jessica Klusek, MS CCC-SLP FPG Child Development Institute (FPG) University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (UNC) Molly Losh, PhD Northwestern

More information

Perspectives on Autism and Sexuality. University of British Columbia. Research Article Summaries. Tina Gunn

Perspectives on Autism and Sexuality. University of British Columbia. Research Article Summaries. Tina Gunn Perspectives on Autism and Sexuality University of British Columbia Research Article Summaries Tina Gunn PERSPECTIVES ON AUTISM AND SEXUALITY 2 Reason For Choosing Topic I became interested in autism and

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs DSM-IV to DSM-5: Primary Changes Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs Laurie Swineford, PhD CCC-SLP Washington State University DSM-IV Previously we used the

More information

DSM-IV Criteria. (1) qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following:

DSM-IV Criteria. (1) qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following: DSM-IV Criteria Autistic Disorder A. A total of six (or more) items from (1), (2), and (3), with at least two from (1), and one each from (2) and (3): (1) qualitative impairment in social interaction,

More information

Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal

Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal BBS-D-15-00893_ Mather_ Montagrin & Sander Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal Alison Montagrin 1,2,3 * & David Sander 1,2 1 Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, 2 Department of Psychology

More information

Overview. Need for screening. Screening for Autism Spectrum Disorders and Neurodevelopmental Disorders

Overview. Need for screening. Screening for Autism Spectrum Disorders and Neurodevelopmental Disorders Screening for Autism Spectrum Disorders and Neurodevelopmental Disorders Kathleen Lehman, Ph.D. CHDD February 2, 2009 Overview CAA and need for screening Overview of a number of screening measures Research

More information

Communication What does the research say?

Communication What does the research say? Communication Communication is often perceived as referring merely to the act of verbalising. However, verbalisations (and actions) can and do occur devoid of communicative intent. Communication is comprised

More information

Trait Perceptions of Dynamic and Static Faces as a Function of Facial. Maturity and Facial Expression

Trait Perceptions of Dynamic and Static Faces as a Function of Facial. Maturity and Facial Expression Trait Perceptions of Dynamic and Static Faces as a Function of Facial Maturity and Facial Expression Master s Thesis Presented to The Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences Brandeis University

More information

Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences

Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences DATE: October 13, 2018 PRESENTED BY: Jill Dolata, PhD, CCC-SLP & Cynthia Green, MS, CCC-SLP Goals Describe

More information

Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders

Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders Background Autistic is a broad term coined in the twentieth century by Bleuler that

More information

1 Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, UK 2 Department of Psychology, Harit-Watt University, Edinburgh, UK

1 Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, UK 2 Department of Psychology, Harit-Watt University, Edinburgh, UK The art of self-testing by attempting cryptic crosswords in later life: the effect of cryptic crosswords on memory self-efficacy, metacognition and memory functioning Nicholas M. Almond 1*, Catriona M.

More information

ASD Working Group Endpoints

ASD Working Group Endpoints Outcome Measures Lit Review Topics Current endpoints used in drug development, strengths and limitations Accounting for patients and carers perspective Suitability of outcomes for different age groups

More information

From: What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015

From:   What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015 Overcoming Intellectual Disability and Autism to Achieve Vocational & Academic Success Pathway to Empowerment Objectives 1 2 4 Learn to distinguish between intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorders.

More information

Eligibility Criteria for Children with ASD

Eligibility Criteria for Children with ASD AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER SERIES Eligibility Criteria for Children with ASD Review the Characteristics of Children with ASD* The following are the most common signs and symptoms of a child with ASD: The

More information

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc Evaluating Educational Services for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders Erik Mayville, Ph.D., BCBA-D The Institute for Educational Planning, LLC Connecticut Center for Child Development, Inc. & mayville@iepinc.org

More information

Autism. Laura Schreibman HDP1 11/29/07 MAIN DIAGNOSTIC FEATURES OF AUTISTIC DISORDER. Deficits in social attachment and behavior

Autism. Laura Schreibman HDP1 11/29/07 MAIN DIAGNOSTIC FEATURES OF AUTISTIC DISORDER. Deficits in social attachment and behavior Autism Laura Schreibman HDP1 11/29/07 MAIN DIAGNOSTIC FEATURES OF AUTISTIC DISORDER Deficits in social attachment and behavior Deficits in verbal and nonverbal communication Presence of perseverative,

More information

Running head: FACIAL EXPRESSION AND SKIN COLOR ON APPROACHABILITY 1. Influence of facial expression and skin color on approachability judgment

Running head: FACIAL EXPRESSION AND SKIN COLOR ON APPROACHABILITY 1. Influence of facial expression and skin color on approachability judgment Running head: FACIAL EXPRESSION AND SKIN COLOR ON APPROACHABILITY 1 Influence of facial expression and skin color on approachability judgment Federico Leguizamo Barroso California State University Northridge

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/1176170/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Mindblind Eyes: An Absence of Spontaneous Theory of Mind in Asperger Syndrome Atsushi Senju,* Victoria Southgate, Sarah White,

More information

Emotional Development

Emotional Development Emotional Development How Children Develop Chapter 10 Emotional Intelligence A set of abilities that contribute to competent social functioning: Being able to motivate oneself and persist in the face of

More information

Curriculum Vitae Madeline Harms, Ph.D.

Curriculum Vitae Madeline Harms, Ph.D. Curriculum Vitae Madeline Harms, Ph.D. Child Emotion Lab Waisman Center, rm 399 1500 Highland Ave Madison, WI 53705 Phone: 785-230-7332 (cell) e-mail: mharms3@wisc.edu Education Graduate: Institute of

More information

Purpose and Objectives of Study Objective 1 Objective 2 Objective 3 Participants and Settings Intervention Description Social peer networks

Purpose and Objectives of Study Objective 1 Objective 2 Objective 3 Participants and Settings Intervention Description Social peer networks 1 Title: Autism Peer Networks Project: Improving Social-Communication and Literacy for Young Children with ASD Funding: Institute of Education Sciences R324A090091 Session Presenters: Debra Kamps and Rose

More information

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi We ll cover Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) ASD in young adults Social Communication (definition, components, importance,

More information

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Alex Rice M.Cl.Sc SLP Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication

More information

Introductory Workshop. Research What is Autism? Core Deficits in Behaviour. National Autistic Society Study - UK (Barnard, et. al, 2001).

Introductory Workshop. Research What is Autism? Core Deficits in Behaviour. National Autistic Society Study - UK (Barnard, et. al, 2001). Schedule 1.! What is Autism and How is it treated? Intervention and Outcomes, New views on Research and Possibilities 2.! How do typical children develop Referencing, Regulation, Play and Peer Relationships?

More information

Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update. Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011

Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update. Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011 Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011 Overview Diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorders Screening/referral

More information

Paediatric Clinical Assessment for a possible Autism Spectrum Disorder

Paediatric Clinical Assessment for a possible Autism Spectrum Disorder Dr Rosie Richardson Plymouth Hospitals NHS Trust May November 2013 Paediatric Clinical Assessment for a possible Autism Spectrum Disorder Clearly there is a lot of variation in how we approach diagnosis

More information

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not.

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not. Autism Summary Autism What is Autism? The Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a developmental disability that can have significant implications on a child's ability to function and interface with the world

More information

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu Person Perception Forming Impressions of Others Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu Person Perception person perception: how we come to know about others temporary states, emotions, intentions and desires impression

More information

Culture and Emotion THE EVOLUTION OF HUMAN EMOTION. Outline

Culture and Emotion THE EVOLUTION OF HUMAN EMOTION. Outline Outline Culture and Emotion The Evolution of Human Emotion Universality in Emotion- The Basic Emotions Perspective Cultural Differences in Emotion Conclusion Chapter 8 THE EVOLUTION OF HUMAN EMOTION Emotion:

More information

Intensive Training. Early Childhood Intensive Training K-12 Intensive Training Building Your Future Intensive Training

Intensive Training. Early Childhood Intensive Training K-12 Intensive Training Building Your Future Intensive Training Intensive Training The Statewide Autism Resources and Training Project (START) Intensive Training focuses on evidencebased practices to increase knowledge and skills that enhance the educational programming

More information

Diagnostic tests for autism may miss many girls

Diagnostic tests for autism may miss many girls NEWS Diagnostic tests for autism may miss many girls BY DEBORAH RUDACILLE 27 JUNE 2011 1 / 8 2 / 8 3 / 8 4 / 8 Lonely child: Girls with Asperger syndrome show an interest in socializing, but have difficulty

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

Emotion recognition in children with Down syndrome: ability, influential factors and supporting development

Emotion recognition in children with Down syndrome: ability, influential factors and supporting development Emotion recognition in children with Down syndrome: ability, influential factors and supporting development Katie Cebula 1, Jennifer Wishart 1, Diane Willis 2, Tom Pitcairn 1 & Amanda Gillooly 1 1 University

More information

Children s Scales of Pleasure and Arousal. Sherri C. Widen and James A. Russell University of British Columbia

Children s Scales of Pleasure and Arousal. Sherri C. Widen and James A. Russell University of British Columbia Scales of Pleasure and Arousal 1 Children s Scales of Pleasure and Arousal Sherri C. Widen and James A. Russell University of British Columbia Poster presented at APS Conference, Toronto, ON July 16 th,

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Multidisciplinary Evaluation Team (MET) Report

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Multidisciplinary Evaluation Team (MET) Report Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Multidisciplinary Evaluation Team (MET) Report Date of Report: Student Name: Date of Birth: School: Evaluation Team: Psychologist: School Social Worker (SSW): Speech & Language

More information

AUDITORY ATTENTIONAL MECHANISMS IN SCHOOL- AGE CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

AUDITORY ATTENTIONAL MECHANISMS IN SCHOOL- AGE CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS AUDITORY ATTENTIONAL MECHANISMS IN SCHOOL- AGE CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS Sridhar Krishnamurti, Kristen Phillips, Meghan Phillips, Benjamin King Auburn University Relationship Disclosure Form

More information

Drive-reducing behaviors (eating, drinking) Drive (hunger, thirst) Need (food, water)

Drive-reducing behaviors (eating, drinking) Drive (hunger, thirst) Need (food, water) Instinct Theory: we are motivated by our inborn automated behaviors that generally lead to survival. But instincts only explain why we do a small fraction of our behaviors. Does this behavior adequately

More information

Peer Perception in Autism. Kathryn McVicar, MD Assistant Professor Clinical Pediatrics and Neurology Albert Einstien College of Medicine

Peer Perception in Autism. Kathryn McVicar, MD Assistant Professor Clinical Pediatrics and Neurology Albert Einstien College of Medicine Peer Perception in Autism Kathryn McVicar, MD Assistant Professor Clinical Pediatrics and Neurology Albert Einstien College of Medicine Peer Perception and Autism What is autism? A review of specific deficits

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

Cover Page. The handle   holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/19149 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Author: Maljaars, Janne Pieternella Wilhelmina Title: Communication problems in children

More information

What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA.

What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA. What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA. What is Autism? ) Autism is a complex developmental disability that has a neurological basis that causes impairments in social interactions, communication,

More information

8/23/2017. Chapter 21 Autism Spectrum Disorders. Introduction. Diagnostic Categories within the Autism Spectrum

8/23/2017. Chapter 21 Autism Spectrum Disorders. Introduction. Diagnostic Categories within the Autism Spectrum Chapter 21 Overview Core features of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) Studies seeking an etiology for ASDs Conditions associated with ASDs Interventions and outcomes Introduction ASDs Class of neurodevelopmental

More information

Rebecca Brewer, Federica Biotti, Caroline Catmur, Clare Press, Francesca Happe, Richard Cook, and Geoffrey Bird

Rebecca Brewer, Federica Biotti, Caroline Catmur, Clare Press, Francesca Happe, Richard Cook, and Geoffrey Bird RESEARCH ARTICLE Can Neurotypical Individuals Read Autistic Facial Expressions? Atypical Production of Emotional Facial Expressions in Autism Spectrum Disorders Rebecca Brewer, Federica Biotti, Caroline

More information

DSM 5 Criteria to Diagnose Autism

DSM 5 Criteria to Diagnose Autism DSM 5 Criteria to Diagnose Autism Patient Name Patient Date of Birth Patient Health Plan Provider Name and Credential Date of Exam Only a doctoral level clinician (MD, PhD, and/or PsyD) can complete this

More information

WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One

WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One Autism is a life-long developmental disability that prevents people from understanding what they see, hear, and otherwise sense. This results in severe problems with social

More information

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS MEDICAL POLICY PAGE: 1 OF: 7 If the member's subscriber contract excludes coverage for a specific service it is not covered under that contract. In such cases, medical policy criteria are not applied.

More information

4 Detailed Information Processing in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome: The Usefulness of Neuropsychological Tests and Self-reports

4 Detailed Information Processing in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome: The Usefulness of Neuropsychological Tests and Self-reports 4 Detailed Information Processing in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome: The Usefulness of Neuropsychological Tests and Self-reports Submitted Chapter 4 Abstract Detailed information processing in 42

More information

The Role of Emotion Suppression in Reaction Time and Threat Response During Highly Intense Stress. Alan Neider Briarcliff High School

The Role of Emotion Suppression in Reaction Time and Threat Response During Highly Intense Stress. Alan Neider Briarcliff High School The Role of Emotion Suppression in Reaction Time and Threat Response During Highly Intense Stress Alan Neider Briarcliff High School Purpose/Introduction This experiments goal was to determine the role

More information

Supplementary Online Content 2

Supplementary Online Content 2 Supplementary Online Content 2 Bieleninik Ł, Geretsegger M, Mössler K, et al; TIME-A Study Team. Effects of improvisational music therapy vs enhanced standard care on symptom severity among children with

More information

Emotion processing in high-functioning autistic children: a priming task

Emotion processing in high-functioning autistic children: a priming task Western Washington University Western CEDAR WWU Masters Thesis Collection WWU Graduate and Undergraduate Scholarship 2010 Emotion processing in high-functioning autistic children: a priming task Ashley

More information

T. Rene Jamison * and Jessica Oeth Schuttler

T. Rene Jamison * and Jessica Oeth Schuttler Jamison and Schuttler Molecular Autism (2015) 6:53 DOI 10.1186/s13229-015-0044-x RESEARCH Open Access Examining social competence, self-perception, quality of life, and internalizing and externalizing

More information

Visual face scanning and emotion perception analysis between Autistic and Typically Developing children

Visual face scanning and emotion perception analysis between Autistic and Typically Developing children Visual face scanning and emotion perception analysis between Autistic and Typically Developing children Uzma Haque Syeda Dhaka, Bangladesh uzma.haque@eee.du.ac.bd Ziaul Zafar Dhaka, Bangladesh zafarziaul@gmail.com

More information

2017 Gatlinburg Conference Symposium Submission SS-10

2017 Gatlinburg Conference Symposium Submission SS-10 Symposium Title: Anxiety in Autism and Fragile X Syndrome Chair: Jane E. Roberts 1 Discussant: Susan Hepburn 2 Overview: Anxiety disorders constitute the most common and debilitating mental health problem

More information

BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS Page: 1 of 7 MEDICAL POLICY MEDICAL POLICY DETAILS Medical Policy Title APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM Policy Number 3.01.11 Category Behavioral Health/ Government Mandate

More information

SURVEY OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER CONCERNS

SURVEY OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER CONCERNS Survey of Autism Spectrum Disorder Concerns Presented by Curtis L. Timmons, Ph.D., LSSP GOALS OF THE WORKSHOP 1. Understand why there were changes between the DSM-IV and the DSM-5 2. Understand the current

More information

Language is the behavior of the brain; communication is the behavior of the body and brain. Financial Disclosure

Language is the behavior of the brain; communication is the behavior of the body and brain. Financial Disclosure Financial Disclosure Asperger Syndrome & HFA Informal Dynamic Social Thinking Assessment Michelle Garcia Winner www.socialthinking.com I am affiliated with Think Social Publishing. I publish multiple books

More information

Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- NOS)

Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- NOS) Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- NOS) What is Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS)? (*Please note that the criteria according to the DSM-V changed

More information

Autism and Related Disorders:

Autism and Related Disorders: Autism and Related Disorders: CHLD 350a/PSYC350 Lecture II: Assessment Katherine D. Tsatsanis, Ph.D. Yale Child Study Center Clinical Director, Developmental Disabilities Clinic Pervasive Developmental

More information

Brief Report: Attention Effect on a Measure of Social Perception

Brief Report: Attention Effect on a Measure of Social Perception J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38:1797 1802 DOI 10.1007/s10803-008-0570-x BRIEF REPORT Brief Report: Attention Effect on a Measure of Social Perception Jodene Goldenring Fine Æ Margaret Semrud-Clikeman Æ Brianne

More information

The use of Autism Mental Status Exam in an Italian sample. A brief report

The use of Autism Mental Status Exam in an Italian sample. A brief report Life Span and Disability XX, 1 (2017), 93-103 The use of Autism Mental Status Exam in an Italian sample. A brief report Marinella Zingale 1, Simonetta Panerai 2, Serafino Buono 3, Grazia Trubia 4, Maurizio

More information