NEXT GENERATION ANTIDEPRESSANTS

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "NEXT GENERATION ANTIDEPRESSANTS"

Transcription

1 NEXT GENERATION ANTIDEPRESSANTS MOVING BEYOND MONOAMINES TO DISCOVER NOVEL AND DIFFERENTIATED TREATMENT STRATEGIES FOR MOOD DISORDERS Berghorst, L., Pizzagalli, D.A. (2010). Defining depression endophenotypes. In C.E. Beyer and S.A. Stahl (Eds.), Next Generation Antidepressants. Moving Beyond Monoamines To Discover Novel And Differentiated Treatment Strategies For Mood Disorders. Cambridge University Press, pp CONTENTS

2 Contributors 5 Preface Chad E. Beyer 7 Chapter 1 Current Depression Landscape: A State of the Field Today 8 Laurence Mignon and Stephen M. Stahl Chapter 2 Novel Therapeutic Targets for Treating Affective Disorders 27 Eliyahu Dremencov and Thomas Cremers Chapter 3 Developing Novel Animal Models of Depression 56 Lotte de Groote, Malgorzata Filip and Andrew C. McCreary Chapter 4 Translational Research in Mood Disorders: Using Imaging 91 Technologies in Biomarker Research Jul Lea Shamy, Adam M. Brickman, Chris D. Griesemer, Anna Parachikova and Mark Day Chapter 5 Defining Depression Endophenotypes 126 Lisa H. Berghorst and Diego A. Pizzagalli Chapter 6 Genetic and Genomic Studies of Major Depressive Disorder 160 Roy H. Perlis Chapter 7 Medicinal Chemistry Challenges in the Design of Next Generation Antidepressants 182 David P. Rotella Chapter 8 Application of Pharmacogenomics and Personalized Medicine for the Care of Depression 212 Keh-Ming Lin, Chun-Yu Chen and Yu-Jui Yvonne Wan

3 CHAPTER 5 Defining Depression Endophenotypes Lisa H. Berghorst Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA Diego A. Pizzagalli, PhD Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) is a highly prevalent and recurrent disorder and a leading cause of disease burden across the world [1]. In the United States alone, for example, the lifetime prevalence rate has been estimated to be 16.6%, affecting over 30 million people, with more than 80% of these individuals experiencing recurrent episodes [2,3]. At both the individual and population levels, depression engenders severe impairment in functioning across social, cognitive, and occupational domains [4,5]. Given the pervasive and detrimental effects of depression, it is disconcerting that in the largest prospective treatment study to date (the Sequenced Treatment Alternative to Relieve Depression (STAR*D) study), only about one-third of participants remitted after treatment with a standard, first-line antidepressant (the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram), and the probability of remission generally decreased over subsequent treatment levels [6]. Unfortunately, efforts to design more effective treatment strategies for depression are limited by the fact that the etiological pathways underlying this disorder remain complex and elusive. In recent years, concerns have been raised that the ongoing quest to understand the etiology and pathophysiology of MDD might be hindered in part by difficulties in defining and characterizing psychiatric phenotypes (e.g., [7,8,9]). With respect to MDD, it has been suggested that the current classification criteria encompass a heterogeneous mix of illnesses that share similar final pathways likely reached via multiple pathophysiological processes [7]. One way to address this heterogeneity is to take an endophenotypic approach and focus on intermediate phenotypes that are more narrowly defined and quantified than DSM-IV diagnoses [10]. Our goal in this chapter is to summarize literature on the potential utility of applying an endophenotypic approach to depression research, with a focus on one of the most promising depressive endophenotypes anhedonia, defined as loss of pleasure or lack of reactivity to pleasurable stimuli [11]. We begin with an overview of the endophenotype concept and its central criteria, which include the following [7,10]: 1) biological and clinical plausibility, 2) specificity, 3) state-independence, 4) familial association, 5) cosegregation, and 6) heritability. In depression research, various endophenotypes have been proposed and assessed with respect to these criteria, including impaired reward function (anhedonia), impaired learning and memory, increased stress sensitivity, REM sleep abnormalities, and tryptophan depletion, among others (see [7] for review). Our focus in this chapter will be on anhedonia, as it has received some of the strongest empirical evidence [7,12,13]. To this end, we incorporate epidemiological, behavioral, neuroimaging, and genetic studies to examine the aforementioned endophenotypic criteria for anhedonia. Thereafter, we discuss an objective way to measure anhedonia in a laboratory setting and summarize recent findings using this paradigm. The chapter concludes with a discussion of important directions for future research efforts using an endophenotypic approach.

4 THE ENDOPHENOTYPE CONCEPT IN RELATION TO PSYCHIATRY Definition and Value of Endophenotypes The identification of etiological and pathophysiological processes underlying mental disorders has proven to be an exceptionally difficult mission and these processes remain largely unknown despite vigorous research efforts over several decades. As previously noted, a substantial contributing factor to this difficulty may be the structure of current classification systems, including the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; [11]), in which the diagnosis of mental disorders revolves around symptom clusters and clinical course [7,9]. This descriptive, categorical approach is implemented in an effort to maximize diagnostic reliability, however, it brings the issue of validity into question for various mental disorders, including MDD [14]. As a result of the organizational framework of the DSM-IV, these categories of disorders may encompass a heterogeneous mix of illnesses [7,15]. The heterogeneity of clinical phenotypes is postulated to reflect the involvement of a multitude of genes none of which is likely necessary or sufficient for triggering a given disorder as well as complex interactions between genes and environmental factors [10,16,17]. In response to this issue of heterogeneity, one approach is to focus research efforts on endophenotypes, or intermediate phenotypes that are hypothesized to lie within the etiological link between genes and clinical disease [8,10]. Accordingly, the endophenotypic approach enables the identification of the upstream consequences of genes as well as the downstream traits or facets of clinical phenotypes [p. 637; 10]. One assumption underlying this conceptualization is that endophenotypes involve comparatively fewer genes and enable a more direct measurement of the biological and environmental factors that contribute to a disorder than afforded by the broader perspective of the clinical phenotype [10]. This assumption was recently challenged by Flint and Mufano [18], who contended that some endophenotypes may not be considerably less genetically complex than clinical disease phenotypes, raising potential concerns about the incremental validity of the endophenotypic approach with respect to genetic architecture. While this concern is legitimate, it is important to recognize that the conclusions drawn by Flint and Mufano were predominantly based on results restricted to the effects of COMT genotype on endophenotypes, which may limit the generalizability of their conclusion. Moreover, Flint and Mufano acknowledge that an endophenotypic approach may still be beneficial to genetic research and contribute to higher reliability across data by enabling objective quantitative measures to be obtained from the large number of individuals necessary for genetic analyses. Nevertheless, additional research is clearly needed to evaluate the incremental validity of the endophenotypic approach, particularly with respect to etiological pathways underlying mental illnesses. Ultimately, these endeavors could have vast implications for improving the validity of our classification system and the effectiveness of treatment and prevention methods. Criteria and Validation of Endophenotypes Putative endophenotypes can be examined using psychological, physiological, neuroimaging, and biochemical methods [7]. In order for a psychological or biological variable to be classified as an endophenotype, it should meet the following criteria [7,10,19,20]: 1) Clinical and biological plausibility: Conceptual relationships exist between the endophenotype and the disease of interest. 2) Specificity: The endophenotype is linked more strongly to the psychiatric disorder of interest than to other psychiatric disorders. 1 3) State-independence: The endophenotype is stable over time and not dependent on illness status or treatment. In other words, the endophenotype should be apparent in an individual regardless of whether or not s/he is actively experiencing symptoms of the illness. 2 1 Hasler and colleagues [15] recently suggested that the specificity criterion might not be necessary because the biological validity of current definitions of mental disorders remains under question. 2 In an attempt to take into account developmental factors and symptom provocation methods in phenotypic expression, Hasler and colleagues [15] subsequently modified this criterion to state that it may be age-normed and may require an environmental challenge to be apparent.

5 4) Heritability: A proportion of variance in the endophenotype is attributable to genetic variance. 5) Familial association: The endophenotype occurs more frequently in unaffected relatives of ill individuals than in the general population. 6) Cosegregation: Within families of ill individuals, the endophenotype occurs more frequently in family members who are affected with the illness than in family members who are unaffected by the illness. In addition to the aforementioned criteria, it is important to take into account the degree to which a putative endophenotype can be feasibly and reliably measured [15]. Along these lines, in the section Current Directions in Objective Laboratory Measurements of Anhedonia, we will describe our experience utilizing a laboratory-based measure to objectively assess an important component of anhedonia (reward responsiveness). ANHEDONIA AS A POTENTIAL DEPRESSIVE ENDOPHENOTYPE The notion that anhedonia may be a trait marker of vulnerability to depression has been under consideration for many years [21,22,23]. Within the past decade in particular, anhedonia has received considerable support as a promising depressive endophenotype [7,12,13]. As summarized in the following sections, empirical evidence supports the endophenotypic criteria of clinical and biological plausibility, familial association, and heritability, mixed or limited findings exist for state-independence and specificity, and few studies have addressed cosegregation. Throughout these sections, the emphasis will be on studies that include laboratory-based tasks in addition to self-report measures, rather than studies focusing exclusively on the latter. Clinical and Biological Plausibility of Anhedonia as a Depressive Endophenotype Among the DSM-IV criteria for a major depressive episode, anhedonia is a cardinal symptom with comparable status as depressed mood, given that one of these two features are required for clinical diagnosis [11]. In order to more explicitly understand the role of anhedonia in depression, researchers have examined a variety of domains, including affective and behavioral responses to positive stimuli, perceptual and attentional processing of positive cues, and ability to learn from reinforcement history. Investigations have also been conducted to examine whether anhedonic symptoms have predictive validity with regard to clinical outcome. Moreover, neuroimaging studies have been utilized to explore relationships between abnormal functioning within particular reward-related brain regions and depression. In the ensuing sections, findings stemming from these lines of inquiry will be considered in order to evaluate the clinical and biological plausibility of anhedonia as a depressive endophenotype. In an early study probing the encoding of positive stimuli, Berenbaum and Oltmanns [24] found that depressed individuals, relative to healthy controls, reported less positive affect and displayed fewer positive facial expressions when presented with positive film clips. Along similar lines, Sloan and colleagues [25] reported that depressed participants rated pleasant picture stimuli less positively, and displayed reduced frequency and intensity of pleasant facial expressions, compared to controls. Importantly, in both studies, findings were selective to positive stimuli and did not extend to negative stimuli. Evidence of reduced facial reactivity, (e.g., [26,27]) and affective responses (e.g., [28,29,30,31]) to positive stimuli has emerged from additional studies, although null findings have also been described (e.g., [32,33]). If present, blunted affective and behavioral reactivity to positive stimuli could arguably influence subsequent retrieval of such stimuli. However, before discussing studies on retrieval, it is worth considering perceptual and attentional processing of positive cues, as these processes could arguably also influence retrieval. In the realm of perceptual processing, findings are inconsistent regarding whether depressed individuals are impaired in their ability to recognize positive stimuli, such as happy facial expressions. Various researchers report no impairment on face recognition tasks in depression (e.g., [34,35,36]). Meanwhile, others have found that depressed individuals, relative to controls, are less accurate in recognizing happy facial expressions (e.g., [37,38]), and require more time (e.g., [39]) and greater intensity of emotional expression (e.g., [40]) in order to label faces as happy. Of note, recognition impairment was specific to positive facial stimuli only in the latter two studies [39,40]. Discrepancies

6 across the aforementioned results may be due in part to the heterogeneous nature of depression, or to the possibility that some results may be confounded by response biases, especially if only accuracy and reaction time were measured. There is also mixed evidence with respect to attentional biases in depression. In several studies using the deployment-of-attention (e.g., [41,42]) or dot-probe [43] paradigms, depressed patients failed to show the positivity bias seen in healthy controls, who directed their attention toward positive stimuli. While these studies lend credence towards a depression-related attentional bias away from positive stimuli, others report null findings (e.g., [44]). Nevertheless, a potentially blunted attentional positivity bias, along with possible impairments in recognizing positive stimuli, and reduced affective and behavioral reactivity to positive stimuli at encoding, may all impede the ability of depressed individuals to retrieve positive cues. Indeed, depressed individuals are more likely to underestimate the occurrence of positive reinforcements received in the past (e.g., [45,46]). It is important to note that this pattern of impairment does not extend to estimations of punishments, as depressed individuals are actually more likely to overestimate the occurrence of punishments received in the past [46]. Furthermore, a biased view of past positive experiences may contribute towards a biased calculation of future outcomes, since individuals with depression also report lower expectations of positive future experiences than control subjects (e.g., [47,48]). Findings of underestimation of past positive reinforcements received may be related to emerging data in which depressed subjects display a reduced ability to modify behavior as a function of positive reinforcements received, as evident from a monetarily reinforced verbal recognition task [49,50], a gambling task [51], and a probabilistic reward task [13; see below section: Current Directions in Objective Laboratory Measurements of Anhedonia]. In sum, the above studies provide evidence that depression is characterized by: reduced affective and behavioral reactivity to positive stimuli; underestimation of the occurrence of past, and the likelihood of future, positive reinforcements; and a reduced ability to use reinforcement history to modify behavior. Evidence of blunted perceptual and attentional processing biases is more tenuous (see also [52]). Of clinical relevance, anhedonic symptoms and behaviors have been found to have predictive value in determining depression onset, course and outcome, as discussed hereafter. For example, reduced frequency of choosing high-magnitude reward options in a decision-making task predicted depressive symptoms one year later in a pediatric sample [51]. With respect to predicting levels of concurrent depressive symptoms, lower levels of approach-related behavior but not higher levels of avoidance-related behavior have been associated with increased severity of depression in currently depressed individuals [53]. Along these lines, lower levels of approach-related behavior [53,54], lower behavioral and heart rate reactivity to amusing films [55], and reduced recall of positive words [56] have been found to predict poorer longitudinal outcome and/or longer time to recovery in depressed individuals. Complementing such studies, Lethbridge & Allen [57] conducted a prospective study in a community sample of individuals with past depression and reported that larger levels of reduced positive affect following a sad mood induction correlated with a greater probability of MDD recurrence a year later; notably, relapse was not predicted by changes in negative affect or dysfunctional thinking, indicating specificity. Finally, additional support for anhedonia as a potential vulnerability factor for the development of depression comes from the fact that anhedonic symptoms are reported in unaffected individuals with increased genetic risk for depression (see below section: Familial Association of Anhedonia as a Depressive Endophenotype), in combination with evidence that anhedonia may be a trait-like characteristic (see below section: State-independence of Anhedonia as a Depressive Endophenotype). Collectively, these studies suggest that anhedonic symptoms may have predictive validity with regard to onset and severity of depression, poorer outcome, longer time to recovery, and higher likelihood of relapse. The biological plausibility of anhedonia as a depressive endophenotype is supported by its association with dysfunctions of the brain reward system in neuroimaging studies [58,59,60]. Important brain areas involved in incentive processing include the dorsal striatum (e.g., caudate, putamen), the ventral striatum (e.g., nucleus accumbens), the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) [59,61,62]. As will be discussed below, neuroimaging studies promise to contribute to a

7 more explicit understanding of which aspects of reward processing (e.g., hedonic coding, ability to link actions to rewards) are likely dysfunctional in depression, because 1) particular brain regions have been linked to specific facets of incentive processing (e.g., [63,64,65,66,67,68]); and 2) abnormal activation of these neural areas is evident in studies that directly assess reward processing in depression [60,69,70,71,72,73,74]. To begin with, the dorsal striatum (i.e., caudate, putamen) is important for coding reward prediction errors [64,75] and linking actions to rewards [76], and is strongly activated in response to unpredictable rewards [65]. Forbes and colleagues [74] recently reported that depressed adolescents exhibited lower caudate activation than psychiatrically healthy adolescents during monetary reward anticipation and outcome in a card-guessing task. Moreover, among the depressed adolescents, reduced caudate activation was associated with decreased subjective ratings of positive affect in realworld environments. Importantly, findings of lower caudate activation during reward anticipation and consumption in depressed adolescents are in line with similar results emerging from adult depressed samples (e.g., [60]). Specifically, utilizing fmri and a monetary incentive delay task, we recently found that depressed adults displayed less bilateral caudate and left nucleus accumbens activation than controls during reward feedback and lower left putamen activation to reward-predicting cues [60]. The depressed participants also reported lower affective ratings during reward anticipation and consumption, and showed lower reward-related RT speeding. Accordingly, our results highlight depression-related impairment in functioning of both the dorsal and ventral striatum in response to a reward-processing task. Based on prior findings, we speculate that dysfunction in the dorsal and ventral striatum might index blunted action-reward reinforcement learning [76] and hedonic coding [67,77,78,79], respectively. Reduced ventral striatal activation (nucleus accumbens) also emerged from a recent study conducted by Steele and colleagues [71], who utilized fmri and a gambling task to examine postincentive behavioral adjustments and neural correlates in depression. Following positive feedback, participants with MDD failed to show the activation of ventral striatal regions and reduction in reaction time that was characteristic of controls, and the behavioral findings specifically correlated with anhedonic symptoms [71]. In a subsequent study, these investigators modeled reward prediction errors utilizing fmri and a Pavlovian reward learning paradigm that involved probabilistic associations between picture stimuli and water delivery to thirsty participants [73]. The authors found that treatment-resistant MDD participants, as compared to controls, had smaller reward-learning signals in the ventral striatum and dorsal ACC. Diminished ACC activation has also been observed in depressed children, relative to controls, during both the reward decision and reward outcome phases of a decision-making task [70]. Given that the ACC, especially its dorsal subdivision, has been implicated in linking outcome representations to actions and integrating reinforcement history to guide action [66], these findings suggest specific reward-related processes that may be impaired in depression. In addition to the ACC, depressed children in the pediatric sample investigated by Forbes et al. [70] also exhibited lower activation than controls in the caudate and right OFC when receiving lowmagnitude rewards. In light of prior findings, OFC dysfunction might index impairments in updating stimulus-reinforcement representations to guide behavior [63,80]. Taken as a whole, results from the aforementioned neuroimaging studies highlight certain aspects of reward processing that are likely to be dysfunctional in depression. For example, dysfunction in dorsal striatal regions, especially caudate hypoactivity, may reflect an impaired ability to learn connections between actions and rewards. Moreover, reduced functioning of the ventral striatal network (e.g., nucleus accumbens) in depression may be related to impaired hedonic coding and, along with dacc dysfunction, may underlie difficulty updating reward predictions. Of note, reduced ventral striatal activation in response to positive cues is largest in depressed individuals reporting elevated anhedonic symptoms [58,81], providing important convergent validity. Finally, OFC dysfunctions may be linked to impaired representation of the reward value of stimuli, as well as difficulty updating associations between stimuli and outcomes. Although the reviewed neuroimaging studies enhance our understanding of reward processing in depression, the aforementioned hypotheses need to be further examined using various paradigms in future studies before these theories can be definitively asserted. Specificity of Anhedonia as a Depressive Endophenotype

8 There is limited support for the specificity of anhedonia as a depressive endophenotype because the presence of anhedonia has been demonstrated in other mental illnesses, especially schizophrenia and substance use disorders [82,83]. In a recent examination of anhedonia in patients with depression, psychosis, or substance abuse, all three patient groups demonstrated significantly higher scores on a self-report measure of anhedonia, the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure scale, relative to controls [84]; however, depressed patients also scored significantly higher than the two other patient groups. With regard to anhedonia in substance abusers, Martin-Soelch and colleagues [85] found that former opiate addicts, as compared to controls, showed reduced activation of neural reward circuits in response to nonmonetary positive reinforcement. Although this neural hypoactivation in former opiate addicts did not extend to monetary positive reinforcement, their subjective ratings of monetary value were significantly lower than controls. Along with characterizing previous drug abusers, anhedonia may play a key role in relapse into drug use, likely due to reduced dopamine (DA) release associated with withdrawal [86]. However, the specific ways in which anhedonia and substance abuse are related, including the directionality between these factors, are complicated by comorbid psychopathology and remain to be fully elucidated. In addition to its association with substance use disorders, anhedonia has also been closely linked to schizophrenia, and is indeed considered a prominent negative symptom of this disorder [87]. In a longitudinal study of schizophrenia and MDD, Blanchard and colleagues [88] found that both groups had higher scores than controls on a self-report measure of social anhedonia at baseline (inpatient hospitalization). Critically, whereas social anhedonic symptoms significantly declined in recovered depressed patients at a one-year follow-up, these symptoms remained stable in schizophrenia patients, raising the possibility that anhedonic symptoms may be trait-like in schizophrenia and more statedependent in MDD. In an attempt to clarify the relationships between anhedonia, depression, and schizophrenia, Romney and Candido [89] used factor analysis to examine the loading of anhedonia on three main factors depression, positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Using self-report measures from schizophrenic and depressed samples, they reported that anhedonia loaded significantly on the depression factor but not the negative symptoms factor, and concluded that anhedonia is predominantly a depressive symptom that should be differentiated from the general affective blunting characteristic of schizophrenia. Conversely, as highlighted by Loas [90], other researchers who conducted related factor analytic studies found anhedonic symptoms in schizophrenia to be independent of depressive symptoms (e.g., [91,92]). In another study relevant to this debate, Joiner and colleagues [93] found that MDD patients, as compared to schizophrenic patients, had significantly higher scores on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) anhedonic symptoms scale, while both groups had comparable non-anhedonic depressive symptoms scores and total BDI scores. These authors concluded that a much stronger relationship exists between anhedonic symptoms and depressive versus schizophrenic diagnostic status, lending support for anhedonia as a relatively specific marker for depression. While the contrasting results of the aforementioned studies remain to be clarified, the use of objective laboratory-based measures of anhedonia may help to resolve some of the discrepancies by identifying specific aspects of reward processing that might be differentially affected in various clinical syndromes. In this regard, findings from recent studies by our laboratory [13] and Gold s group [94] provide initial evidence that different disorders might affect distinct aspects of reward processing. Of note, both studies used the same probabilistic reward task to evaluate how participants modulated their behavior as a function of reinforcement history. We found that unmedicated MDD patients had a reduced response bias toward the more frequently rewarded stimulus in the absence of immediate reward, although they were responsive to single rewards [13]. In contrast, Heerey, Bell-Warren and Gold [94] reported that participants with schizophrenia showed a normative response bias and did not have impaired sensitivity to reward or impaired ability to modulate behavior based on prior rewards received. Of note, these researchers also administered a probabilistic decision-making task to the same participants with schizophrenia and controls, and the schizophrenia group exhibited a reduced ability to evaluate potential outcomes when given competing response options, likely stemming from working memory deficits. Overall, Heerey, Bell-Warren and Gold [94] postulated that the fundamental mechanisms underlying reward-based learning in schizophrenia may actually be unimpaired, but these individuals might lack the ability to integrate such affective cues with other cognitive information to

9 assess potential outcomes and guide behavior. 3 Accordingly, this initial evidence suggests that rewardrelated behavior in schizophrenia and MDD might be characterized by distinct dysfunctions. Although anhedonic symptoms may not be exclusive to depression, some specificity to depression in relation to other mental illnesses, such as anxiety, has been demonstrated. Given the high degree of clinical overlap between depression and anxiety [97], much time and effort has been expended towards parsing out which variables aid in differentiating between these illnesses. As suggested by a tripartite model of symptom clusters proposed by Watson and colleagues [98], although high negative affect characterizes both depression and anxiety, low positive affect is relatively specific to depression. Following from this model, various studies have demonstrated associations between depressive symptoms and reduced generation, recall, and anticipation of positive experiences that are not apparent in relation to anxiety symptoms [47,48,99,100]. For instance, MacLeod et al. [99] found that depressed patients, in contrast to patients with panic disorder or healthy controls, generated fewer positive experiences in response to various time-frame cues for both memory recall and future-thinking in a verbal fluency paradigm. In a subsequent study using a related paradigm, depressive symptoms but not anxiety symptoms were associated with a reduction in anticipated future positive experiences [47]. Similarly, Miranda and Mennin [48] reported that a greater propensity to predict that positive events would not happen in the future, and an increased level of certainty about these predictions, was associated with higher depression symptoms, but not anxiety symptoms. Finally, this pattern of results was extended to a pediatric sample of primary school children, in which probability ratings of self-referential future positive events were likewise negatively associated with levels of depression, but not levels of anxiety [100]. There is also evidence of specificity for depression over anxiety with respect to impaired perceptual processing of positively valenced cues and reduced ability to modify behavior as a function of rewards. First, individuals with MDD, as compared to individuals with social anxiety disorder and controls, have been shown to require a higher level of emotional expression in order to identify happy faces [40]. Second, utilizing a probabilistic reward task with a non-clinical sample, Pizzagalli, Jahn, and O Shea [12] found that a reduced response bias toward a more frequently rewarded stimulus was specifically associated with anhedonic symptoms and not with symptoms of anxiety or general distress. These findings were replicated and extended in later studies using both non-clinical [101] and MDD [13] samples. Finally, in the abovementioned study by Forbes and colleagues [51], a reduced propensity to choose high-probability, high-reward options in a gambling task predicted depressive symptoms, but not anxiety symptoms, one year later. Collectively, the empirical evidence summarized in this section supports the notion that anhedonia symptoms are relatively specific to depression over anxiety. State-independence of Anhedonia as a Depressive Endophenotype Relatively few studies in MDD samples have examined whether the symptoms of anhedonia reflect state or trait characteristics. Nevertheless, initial support for the temporal stability of anhedonia has emerged from investigations in which researchers have compared individuals who are actively experiencing clinical symptoms of depression with those who are in a remitted or recovered phase of the illness [43,102,103,104]. In the earliest of these studies, previously depressed patients re-tested during remission continued to demonstrate less frequent endorsement of positive words (non-depressed content adjectives), but not negative words (depressed content adjectives), than control subjects in a self-referent encoding task [102]. As suggested by the authors, these results raise the possibility that reduced positive self-image might represent a vulnerability factor for future depressive episodes. Ramel and colleagues [103] similarly found that remitted depressed participants, but not controls, recalled significantly fewer self-referent positive words following a sad mood induction compared to before the mood induction. The findings in remitted depressed individuals across both of these studies mirror the reduced endorsement and recall of self-referent positive words seen in currently depressed individuals 3 It is of note that Heerey, Bell-Warren and Gold s [94] findings of unimpaired reward sensitivity in schizophrenia are limited by the fact that patients were medicated during testing and smoking status was not taken into account; the latter is a particularly relevant consideration since there are high rates of smoking in schizophrenia [95], and nicotine has been found to increase response bias in the same task used by Heerey and colleagues [96].

10 [105]. In another experiment involving a negative mood induction, remitted subjects with a history of MDD were less likely than controls to associate themselves with happiness on an Implicit Association Test (IAT), irrespective of current mood state [104]. Along these lines, Joormann and Gotlib [43] reported that both currently and formerly depressed individuals failed to demonstrate a positive attentional bias towards happy faces that was characteristic of control participants during a dot-probe task. Interestingly, using a similar task combined with a sad mood induction, these researchers also found that unaffected children of depressed mothers failed to demonstrate an attentional bias towards happy faces [106]. Collectively, these findings demonstrate the existence or persistence of anhedonic symptoms (e.g., lower endorsement and recall of self-referential positive traits, and blunted attentional positivity biases) in fully remitted subjects and in other individuals at increased risk for depression. Accordingly, such results indicate that reduced positive self-image and blunted attentional biases toward incentive stimuli might increase vulnerability to depression and be state-independent. Additional evidence of reduced reactivity to positive cues that continues beyond an active depressive episode comes from psychophysiological studies. For example, in an event-related potential (ERP) study by Nandrino and colleagues [107], first-episode depressed patients exhibited a reduction in P300 amplitude to positive words that was still present after treatment. Since the P300 is thought to index post-identification processes of discriminating and categorizing stimuli [108], these results may reflect state-independent hedonic processing deficits specifically, impairment in the categorization of positive stimuli. In another electroencephalography (EEG) study, remitted depressed patients demonstrated reduced left-sided anterior resting EEG activity [109], mirroring patterns described in currently depressed patients [110]. Given that left prefrontal cortex activity has been associated with approach-related behaviors and appetitive goals [111,112], the findings of left-sided anterior hypoactivation across depressed and remitted individuals may reflect trait-like anhedonia. However, it is important to note that not all studies lend credence to the notion of stateindependence of anhedonic symptoms. For example, in the aforementioned study by Blanchard and colleagues [88], self-reported social anhedonia scores of depressed patients declined over a one-year follow-up in recovered patients, which suggests state dependence of social anhedonia in depression. A similar conclusion was drawn in a more recent study where individuals with current depression, but not those with remitted depression, reported diminished emotional responsiveness (i.e., lower self-reported ratings of happiness and enthusiasm) to anticipated reward, as compared to never depressed individuals [113]. One factor potentially contributing to both sets of results could be that experimental paradigms may need to include negative mood inductions to prime participants in order to see biased processing in a remitted depressed sample [114]. In a study that used such mood induction methods, results were mixed with respect to state-independence of anhedonic symptoms: both currently and remitted depressed youth recalled a significantly smaller amount of positive words than controls following a sad mood induction, but only currently depressed subjects exhibited a reduced endorsement of positive traits as well [115]. Accordingly, although there is some support for the temporal stability of anhedonia, further studies are necessary to determine whether specific components of anhedonia might persist after remission and be independent of illness status. Familial Association of Anhedonia as a Depressive Endophenotype In order to assess whether anhedonia fulfills the criterion of familial association, we turn to studies that have examined anhedonic symptoms in unaffected (e.g., no current or past diagnosis of mental disorder) first-degree relatives of patients with depression as compared to the general population. For example, Le Masurier and colleagues [116] reported that unaffected first-degree relatives of individuals with MDD took significantly longer to make self-referential categorizations of positive personality characteristics than age-matched controls; both groups were faster, however, to categorize positive than negative traits, suggesting a relatively reduced positive bias in the former group. Although a depressed comparison group was not included in this study, depressed patients were slower to identify positive words on a go/no-go task in an earlier study [117], indicating similarities in anhedonia-related information processing between individuals with depression and their unaffected relatives. A reduced positive bias has also been noted in never-disordered daughters of mothers with a history of recurrent MDD as compared to never-disordered daughters of mothers with no history of Axis I psychopathology [106]. In

11 this study, a dot-probe task with emotional faces was administered to participants after a negative mood induction. The daughters of mothers with a history of depression failed to show the selective attention to positive facial expressions exhibited by the control daughters; rather, unlike controls, they displayed selective attention to negative facial expressions. Of note, the lack of positivity bias seen in the daughters of mothers with a history of depression was also found in both currently and formerly depressed patients by this same group of researchers using a similar dot-probe paradigm [43]. The mother-daughter findings by Joormann and colleagues complement a previous report on another pediatric sample with depressed versus non-depressed mothers [118]. In this earlier study, high-risk children endorsed significantly fewer positive words and recalled a higher proportion of endorsed negative words than low-risk children in a self-referent encoding task, but only following a negative mood induction. However, a notable strength of the more recent study by Joormann and colleagues [106] is that they obtained diagnostic information on the children and thus could rule out the possibility that current or past mental illness in their high-risk sample might have contributed to their results. In this way, assessment of individuals who have never expressed mental illness enables a more unambiguous deduction that observed findings may represent a vulnerability to depression rather than a consequence of psychopathology. In a further study that took into account history of depression, Farmer and colleagues [119] utilized a sib-pair design and the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) to examine the familiality of various personality dimensions including reward dependence a temperament trait reflecting domains such as social attachment, dependence on the approval of others, and sentimentality [120]. Of note, MDD patients tend to show reduced levels of reward dependence as compared to control individuals [121]. Interestingly, Farmer and colleagues found that never-depressed siblings of depressed probands had higher reward dependence scores than never-depressed siblings of healthy control probands. However, siblings of depressed probands with a history of depression themselves had lower reward dependence scores than either of these groups, indicating the possibility that high reward dependence may act as a protective factor against developing depression. Finally, in the first fmri study to compare asymptomatic juvenile offspring of parents with MDD to those of healthy parents, Monk and colleagues [122] found that high-risk offspring showed reduced nucleus accumbens activation when passively viewing happy faces (and increased nucleus accumbens activation when passively viewing fearful faces) relative to low-risk offspring. The neural responsiveness to positive stimuli found in the high-risk offspring parallels findings of reduced activation in the nucleus accumbens in response to happy faces [123] or reward feedback [60] in adults with MDD. Overall, empirical evidence that anhedonia-related emotional processing biases and reduced positivity biases occur more frequently in unaffected relatives of ill individuals than the general population lends support for the familial association of anhedonia as a depressive endophenotype, and suggests it may represent a risk factor for depression. Heritability of Anhedonia as a Depressive Endophenotype Relatively few studies have examined the heritability of anhedonia (i.e., whether a proportion of its variance can be attributable to genetic variance), and some have investigated this topic in psychiatrically healthy samples or within the context of disorders other than MDD. In addition, the majority of such research has focused on self-report measures and only one study to date has used an objective behavioral measure of hedonic capacity, along with self-report measures, to investigate potential genetic contributions [124]. In this study, the same probabilistic reward task previously mentioned was administered to a small sample (n = 70; 35 twin pairs) of monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs who reported no current or past psychiatric disorder. Model fitting revealed that 46% of the variance in hedonic capacity was accounted for by additive genetic factors, while 54% was accounted for by individual-specific environmental factors [124]. These results extend previous twin studies that have relied solely on self-report measures to assess the heritability of anhedonia. The earliest evidence of genetic influences on hedonic capacity comes from a study in a sample of college undergraduates by Dworkin and Saczynski [125], in which the intraclass correlation coefficients for a scale of hedonic capacity (items selected from the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory and the California Psychological Inventory) were significantly higher for

12 MZ twin pairs (.63) than DZ twin pairs (.41). Subsequent twin studies similarly reported intraclass correlation coefficients that were significantly higher for MZ than DZ twin pairs (e.g., [126,127]), lending support for the notion that genetic factors contribute to the phenotypic expression of anhedonia. In one of these studies, at least one member of each twin pair had schizophrenia, and anhedonia levels were rated in a semistructured clinical interview [126]; the other study used an anhedonic subscale of a schizotypy Self-Report Questionnaire with a population-based twin registry [127]. Of note, an important limitation of these early studies is that they did not include model fitting on the variance-covariance matrices for MZ and DZ twin pairs to estimate the specific contributions of additive genetic, dominant genetic, common environment, and non-shared environment/measurement error. In subsequent twin studies where investigators did perform such model fitting, heritability estimates of hedonic capacity range from 22% to 67% [128,129,130,131,132,133,134,135]. This broad range of heritability estimates might be partially accounted for by the fact that various self-report measures might be assessing different facets of hedonic capacity or might have different psychometric characteristics (e.g., validity, reliability). Further research, including the evaluation of anhedonia across monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs discordant for depression, and the utilization of objective measures of anhedonia in these samples, is necessary to obtain a more explicit understanding of heritability estimates for anhedonia. Nevertheless, results from twin studies to date suggest that genetic variance accounts for a considerable proportion of the variance in anhedonia. Importantly, given that heritability of MDD is estimated to be between 31% and 42% [136], heritability estimates for anhedonia may be even higher than those for MDD, which underscores a potential advantage of using an endophenotypic approach. Cosegregation of Anhedonia as a Depressive Endophenotype There is a notable lack of studies investigating anhedonia across affected and unaffected relatives of depressed individuals. However, in the aforementioned sib-pair study conducted by Farmer and colleagues [119], siblings of depressed individuals who had a history of depression themselves had significantly lower reward dependence scores compared with never-depressed siblings of depressed individuals. Results from this study support the notion that within families of depressed individuals, anhedonia may occur more frequently in family members who are affected with the illness than in family members who are unaffected by the illness. Nevertheless, additional studies are clearly needed to further investigate the cosegregation of anhedonia. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN OBJECTIVE LABORATORY MEASUREMENTS OF ANHEDONIA Given that self-report measures of anhedonic symptoms are inherently subjective and vulnerable to reporting biases, the development of more objective measures of anhedonia is well warranted. As a first step in this direction, our laboratory recently developed a probabilistic reward task to provide an objective measure of reward responsiveness [12]. The task, which was adapted from a prior paradigm described by Tripp and Alsop [137], involves a differential reinforcement schedule to obtain an objective measurement of an individual s ability to adapt behavior as a function of reinforcement history [12]. In this task, participants are briefly presented with one of two stimuli and asked to determine which stimulus appeared on a computer screen. Importantly, the two stimuli are physically very similar and presented very briefly (100 ms), making the differentiation quite difficult. Critically, unbeknownst to the participants, correct identification of one stimulus is followed by reward feedback (e.g., Correct!! You won 5 cents! ) three times more frequently than correct identification of the other stimulus. Under this experimental setting, healthy subjects quickly develop a robust response bias toward the more frequently rewarded stimulus [12,137,138]. Notably, the probabilistic nature of the task prevents participants from being able

13 to use the outcome of a single trial to deduce which stimulus is more profitable; rather, participants must integrate reinforcement history over time to perform most successfully on the task. Importantly, based on results from initial studies, the psychometric properties of this task appear promising. For example, in two separate studies, the test-retest reliability for response bias over approximately 38 days was [12,139]. Moreover, as mentioned above, the heritability of reward responsiveness in a twin study using this task was estimated to be approximately 48% [124]. Furthermore, studies utilizing the probabilistic reward task with participants presumed to be impaired in reinforcement learning such as individuals with affective disorders [7,140] provide evidence of construct validity. A reduced response bias towards the more frequently rewarded stimulus has been described in unmedicated patients with MDD [13], medicated euthymic patients with bipolar disorder [141], and students with elevated depressive symptoms [12]. In the study with unmedicated MDD patients, trial-by-trial probability analyses indicated that this group had a reduced response bias toward the more frequently rewarded stimulus in the absence of immediate reward, but was responsive to single rewards. Moreover, as noted earlier, this dysfunction was correlated with anhedonic symptoms (r =.52, p <.05) and not with symptoms of anxiety or general distress. Importantly, performance on the probabilistic reward task is also associated with activation of reward-related neural regions, in particular the dacc and caudate. Participants who fail to develop a response bias towards the more frequently rewarded stimulus have been found to show significantly lower activation in caudate and dacc regions in response to reward feedback than those who develop such a response bias [139]. Furthermore, activation of dacc regions specifically correlates with this learning ability (r =.40, p <.03). These findings are in line with previous studies that have demonstrated a link between dacc region activation and the ability to integrate reinforcement history over time (e.g., [66] as well as studies implicating the caudate in learning action-reward contingencies (e.g., [76]). Finally, given that DA is believed to play a key role in reward-related learning [142], two studies have been conducted to examine whether pharmacological manipulations affecting DA either indirectly [96] or directly [143] would influence development of response bias in the probabilistic reward task. In the first case, healthy nonsmokers were administered a single dose of transdermal nicotine (7-14mg) in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover design; nicotine was found to increase response bias towards the more frequently rewarded stimulus [96]. Further studies will be needed to determine whether the mechanisms underlying these findings are similar to those emerging from animal studies, in which nicotine activates the presynaptic nicotinic receptors on mesocorticolimbic DA neurons to increase appetitive responding [144]. In the second case, healthy participants who received a single 0.5 mg dose of the D2/3 agonist pramipexole two hours prior to completing the probabilistic reward task demonstrated impaired reinforcement learning as compared to those who received placebo [143]. Here again, future studies are necessary to explore whether the mechanisms underlying these findings emulate related animal data [145] and reflect pramipexole-induced activation of DA autoreceptors and corresponding reductions in phasic DA bursts. In spite of the need for studies directly assessing DA signaling in humans, it is important to emphasize that impaired reinforcement learning in the pramipexole group could be simulated by decreased reward-related presynaptic DA signaling in a neural network model of striato-cortical function in subsequent analyses of this dataset [146]. In sum, evidence supports the psychometric properties of the probabilistic reward task, and accordingly, its potential usefulness to objectively measure specific reward-related dysfunctions. SUMMARY AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS The overarching goal of this chapter was to review human literature pointing to the potential utility of applying an endophenotypic approach to depression research. In doing so, we specifically focused on anhedonia, which is emerging as one of the most promising endophenotypes of depression (e.g., [7,12,13]). In line with this conceptualization, we discussed empirical evidence suggesting that anhedonia meets the criteria of biological and clinical plausibility, familial association, and heritability; mixed or limited findings exist for state-independence and specificity, and few studies have addressed cosegregation. The research presented in this chapter supports a strong relationship between anhedonia and depression from both clinical and biological viewpoints. In particular, individuals with depression are

Basic definition and Classification of Anhedonia. Preclinical and Clinical assessment of anhedonia.

Basic definition and Classification of Anhedonia. Preclinical and Clinical assessment of anhedonia. Basic definition and Classification of Anhedonia. Preclinical and Clinical assessment of anhedonia. Neurobiological basis and pathways involved in anhedonia. Objective characterization and computational

More information

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Risk factors for the development and outcome of childhood psychopathology SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Chapter 147 In this chapter I present a summary of the results of the studies described in this thesis followed

More information

CNTRICS Final Task Selection: Long-Term Memory

CNTRICS Final Task Selection: Long-Term Memory Schizophrenia Bulletin vol. 35 no. 1 pp. 197 212, 2009 doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn134 Advance Access publication on October 16, 2008 CNTRICS Final Task Selection: Long-Term Memory John D. Ragland 1,2, Roshan

More information

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way Mortality and morbidity attributable to use of addictive substances in the United States. The Association of American Physicians from 1999 60 million tobacco smokers in the U.S. 14 million dependent on

More information

We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists. International authors and editors

We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists. International authors and editors We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists 4,000 116,000 120M Open access books available International authors and editors Downloads Our

More information

Cognitive, affective, & social neuroscience

Cognitive, affective, & social neuroscience Cognitive, affective, & social neuroscience Time: Wed, 10:15 to 11:45 Prof. Dr. Björn Rasch, Division of Cognitive Biopsychology University of Fribourg 1 Content } 5.11. Introduction to imaging genetics

More information

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives)

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Recognize that behavior is multi-factorial with genetic components Understand how multi-factorial traits are studied. Explain the terms: prevalence, incidence,

More information

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H PSYC H O PAT H O LO G Y : PSYC H I AT R I C P R O B LEMS A N D T H E A S SO C I

More information

The Neurobiology of Addiction

The Neurobiology of Addiction The Neurobiology of Addiction Jodi Gilman, Ph.D. Center for Addiction Medicine Massachusetts General Hospital Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry Harvard Medical School What is Addiction? commonly

More information

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group 1 Supplemental Data Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group Additional inclusion criteria for the major depressive disorder group were: age of onset of

More information

BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION

BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION TREVOR.W. ROBBINS Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge Many drugs of abuse, including stimulants such as amphetamine and cocaine, opiates

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for affectively valenced and neutral stimuli in depression: Evidence from a novel matching task

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for affectively valenced and neutral stimuli in depression: Evidence from a novel matching task COGNITION AND EMOTION 2011, 25 (7), 12461254 BRIEF REPORT Memory for affectively valenced and neutral stimuli in depression: Evidence from a novel matching task Ian H. Gotlib 1, John Jonides 2, Martin

More information

The Adolescent Developmental Stage

The Adolescent Developmental Stage The Adolescent Developmental Stage o Physical maturation o Drive for independence o Increased salience of social and peer interactions o Brain development o Inflection in risky behaviors including experimentation

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

The roles of valuation and reward processing in cognitive function and psychiatric disorders

The roles of valuation and reward processing in cognitive function and psychiatric disorders Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences The final publication is available from the New York Academy of Sciences via http://dx.doi.org/xx The roles of valuation and reward processing in cognitive function

More information

What makes us ill?

What makes us ill? www.unifr.ch/psycho/en/research/psycli What makes us ill? What makes us ill? Looking for vulnerability factors for mental illness Prof. Dr. Chantal Martin-Soelch In the framework of the burden of mental

More information

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

A model of the interaction between mood and memory INSTITUTE OF PHYSICS PUBLISHING NETWORK: COMPUTATION IN NEURAL SYSTEMS Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 12 (2001) 89 109 www.iop.org/journals/ne PII: S0954-898X(01)22487-7 A model of the interaction between

More information

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H

More information

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Master Thesis Christa Nijnens September 16, 2009 University of Utrecht Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience Department of Neuroscience

More information

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver NRSC January 28, 2010 Costs: Health, Crime, Productivity Costs in billions of dollars (2002) $400 $350 $400B legal

More information

CONTROVERSIES AND NEW DIRECTIONS

CONTROVERSIES AND NEW DIRECTIONS BIPOLAR Introduction DISORDER IN CHILDHOOD AND EARLY ADOLESCENCE Introduction MELISSA P. D ELBELLO, DAVID AXELSON, and BARBARA GELLER CONTROVERSIES AND NEW DIRECTIONS Although the existence and diagnostic

More information

Acute Stress Selectively Reduces Reward Sensitivity

Acute Stress Selectively Reduces Reward Sensitivity Acute Stress Selectively Reduces Reward Sensitivity The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation Published Version

More information

Diagnosis of Mental Disorders. Historical Background. Rise of the Nomenclatures. History and Clinical Assessment

Diagnosis of Mental Disorders. Historical Background. Rise of the Nomenclatures. History and Clinical Assessment Diagnosis of Mental Disorders History and Clinical Assessment Historical Background For a long time confusion reigned. Every selfrespecting alienist, and certainly every professor, had his own classification.

More information

Exploring the brain and behavioral mechanisms of MBIs for depression and anxiety

Exploring the brain and behavioral mechanisms of MBIs for depression and anxiety Exploring the brain and behavioral mechanisms of MBIs for depression and anxiety Day: Thursday 12th July 2018 Time: 10:45 12:00 Track: Working Mechanisms The goal of this symposium is to share results

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation

Cover Page. The handle  holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/32078 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Author: Pannekoek, Nienke Title: Using novel imaging approaches in affective disorders

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content 1 Supplementary Online Content Miller CH, Hamilton JP, Sacchet MD, Gotlib IH. Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging of major depressive disorder in youth. JAMA Psychiatry. Published online September

More information

Emotion Explained. Edmund T. Rolls

Emotion Explained. Edmund T. Rolls Emotion Explained Edmund T. Rolls Professor of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford and Fellow and Tutor in Psychology, Corpus Christi College, Oxford OXPORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Contents 1 Introduction:

More information

MH ID: Misuse of Psychotropic Medications. Jarrett Barnhill MD UNC School of Medicine Jarrett November 19, 2013

MH ID: Misuse of Psychotropic Medications. Jarrett Barnhill MD UNC School of Medicine Jarrett November 19, 2013 MH ID: Misuse of Psychotropic Medications Jarrett Barnhill MD UNC School of Medicine Jarrett Barnhill@med.unc.edu November 19, 2013 Goals To explore the complex relationship between challenging behaviors

More information

CIMR. What is the CIMR? May In this issue. Quarterly Newsletter. Volume 1, Issue 2

CIMR. What is the CIMR? May In this issue. Quarterly Newsletter. Volume 1, Issue 2 What is Department of : What is the? The has recently been established at University Health Network to promote the understanding of depression and mood disorders; to develop innovative evidence-based therapeutic

More information

Chronic irritability in youth that may be misdiagnosed as bipolar disorder. Ellen Leibenluft, M.D.

Chronic irritability in youth that may be misdiagnosed as bipolar disorder. Ellen Leibenluft, M.D. Chronic irritability in youth that may be misdiagnosed as bipolar disorder Ellen Leibenluft, M.D. Chief, Section on Bipolar Spectrum Disorders Emotion and Development Branch National Institute of Mental

More information

Concurrent Disorders

Concurrent Disorders Concurrent Disorders Dr. Christy Sutherland MD CCFP dipabam Medical Director, PHS Community Services Methadone/Buprenorphine 101 Workshop April 1, 2017 Overview Introduction Epidemiology Treatment Principles

More information

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences University of Lethbridge Research Repository OPUS Faculty Research and Publications http://opus.uleth.ca Lalumière, Martin 2005 Behavioral genetics: The study of differences Lalumière, Martin L. Department

More information

Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP

Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP Disclosures This speaker has no conflicts of interest to disclose Objectives Define drug abuse and addiction Identify the

More information

Understanding Addiction and Its Impact on the Brain. SDSMA Webinar Matthew Stanley, DO

Understanding Addiction and Its Impact on the Brain. SDSMA Webinar Matthew Stanley, DO Understanding Addiction and Its Impact on the Brain SDSMA Webinar Matthew Stanley, DO Estimated Economic Cost to Society Due to Substance Abuse and Addiction: Illegal drugs: Alcohol: Tobacco: $181 billion/year

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

BroadcastMed Bipolar, Borderline, Both? Diagnostic/Formulation Issues in Mood and Personality Disorders

BroadcastMed Bipolar, Borderline, Both? Diagnostic/Formulation Issues in Mood and Personality Disorders BroadcastMed Bipolar, Borderline, Both? Diagnostic/Formulation Issues in Mood and Personality Disorders BRIAN PALMER: Hi. My name is Brian Palmer. I'm a psychiatrist here at Mayo Clinic. Today, we'd like

More information

The Role of Smoking in Cocaine. Addiction

The Role of Smoking in Cocaine. Addiction The Role of Smoking in Cocaine Addiction Luca Colnaghi Eric Kandel Laboratory Columbia University in the City of New York Department of Neuroscience Index 1- The Brain, memory, metaplasticity 2- Cocaine

More information

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives)

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Recognize that behavior is multi-factorial with genetic components Understand how multi-factorial traits are studied. Explain the terms: incidence, prevalence,

More information

Neurobiology of Addiction

Neurobiology of Addiction Neurobiology of Addiction Tiffany Love, Ph.D. Department of Psychiatry The University of Utah What is Addiction? Addiction is a chronic, relapsing, and treatable brain disorder. Compulsive drug seeking

More information

Serotonin and Dopamine Interactions. Atanas Stankov Computational Psychiatry Seminar Translational Neuromodeling Unit

Serotonin and Dopamine Interactions. Atanas Stankov Computational Psychiatry Seminar Translational Neuromodeling Unit Serotonin and Dopamine Interactions Atanas Stankov Computational Psychiatry Seminar Translational Neuromodeling Unit 02.05.2014 Outline NT Review Opponent/complement Interactions DA-5HT opponency L-Dopa

More information

Cognition in Parkinson's Disease and the Effect of Dopaminergic Therapy

Cognition in Parkinson's Disease and the Effect of Dopaminergic Therapy Cognition in Parkinson's Disease and the Effect of Dopaminergic Therapy Penny A. MacDonald, MD, PhD, FRCP(C) Canada Research Chair Tier 2 in Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging Assistant Professor

More information

Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders

Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders Review Paper Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders Andrée M. Cusi, BSc; Anthony Nazarov, BSc; Katherine Holshausen, BSc; Glenda M. MacQueen, MD, PhD;

More information

Sex Differences in Depression in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis

Sex Differences in Depression in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis 171 Sex Differences in Depression in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis Andrae J. Laws, McNair Scholar, Penn State University Faculty Research Advisor Dr. Peter A. Arnett, Associate Professor of Psychology

More information

Disease Modification in Schizophrenia: Overview of the Issues. ISCTM February 18 th 2014 Ravi Anand, MD Switzerland

Disease Modification in Schizophrenia: Overview of the Issues. ISCTM February 18 th 2014 Ravi Anand, MD Switzerland Disease Modification in Schizophrenia: Overview of the Issues ISCTM February 18 th 2014 Ravi Anand, MD Switzerland Need for a New Treatment Paradigm in Schizophrenia Sixty years after approval for the

More information

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers.

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers. Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers. 1 This work is a result of the combined efforts of myself and my research advisor, Dr.

More information

CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT

CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT Informal Tes3ng Personality Tes3ng Assessment Procedures Ability Tes3ng The Clinical Interview 3 Defining Clinical Assessment The process of assessing the client through

More information

Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging

Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging Angelica Morales, PhD Postdoctoral Fellow Developmental Brain Imaging Lab Oregon Health & Science University No Financial Disclosures The

More information

Talk 2. Neurocognitive differences in children with or without CU- traits 05/12/2013. Psychological. Behavioural

Talk 2. Neurocognitive differences in children with or without CU- traits 05/12/2013. Psychological. Behavioural Neurocognitive differences in children with or without CU- traits Prof Essi Viding Developmental Risk and Resilience Unit, Psychology and Language Sciences, UCL e.viding@ucl.ac.uk Talk 2. Environment Psychological

More information

Psych3BN3 Topic 4 Emotion. Bilateral amygdala pathology: Case of S.M. (fig 9.1) S.M. s ratings of emotional intensity of faces (fig 9.

Psych3BN3 Topic 4 Emotion. Bilateral amygdala pathology: Case of S.M. (fig 9.1) S.M. s ratings of emotional intensity of faces (fig 9. Psych3BN3 Topic 4 Emotion Readings: Gazzaniga Chapter 9 Bilateral amygdala pathology: Case of S.M. (fig 9.1) SM began experiencing seizures at age 20 CT, MRI revealed amygdala atrophy, result of genetic

More information

EEG based biomarkers in pediatric neuropsychiatry: ADHD autism (ASD)

EEG based biomarkers in pediatric neuropsychiatry: ADHD autism (ASD) EEG based biomarkers in pediatric neuropsychiatry: ADHD autism (ASD) Neuropsychologist PhD Geir Ogrim NORWAY geir.ogrim@so-hf.no Affiliations Neuroteam, Child psychiatry service, Østfold Hospital Trust

More information

Research Questions and Survey Development

Research Questions and Survey Development Research Questions and Survey Development R. Eric Heidel, PhD Associate Professor of Biostatistics Department of Surgery University of Tennessee Graduate School of Medicine Research Questions 1 Research

More information

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia In general, sensory processing as well as selective attention impairments are common amongst people with schizophrenia. It is important to note that experts have in

More information

Schizophrenia. Nikita Verma 2017 Page 1

Schizophrenia. Nikita Verma 2017 Page 1 Schizophrenia It is a severe psychiatric disorder with symptoms of emotional instability, detachment from reality and withdrawal into self. It is an umbrella term used to outline a range of different psychiatric

More information

Onset and recurrence of depressive disorders: contributing factors

Onset and recurrence of depressive disorders: contributing factors SUMMARY People with depressive disorders frequently come to see their general practitioner (GP) as these conditions are highly prevalent. In the Netherlands, 19% of the general population experiences a

More information

Knowing How Gamblers Think: Improving Treatment Outcomes

Knowing How Gamblers Think: Improving Treatment Outcomes Knowing How Gamblers Think: Improving Treatment Outcomes Jon E. Grant, JD, MD, MPH Professor University of Minnesota School of Medicine Minneapolis, MN Disclosure Information I have the following financial

More information

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Outline Neuroanatomy of emotion Critical conceptual, experimental design, and interpretation issues in neuroimaging research Fear and anxiety Neuroimaging research

More information

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, 2017 The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain David Reyher, MSW, CAADC Behavioral Health Program Director Alvernia University Defining Addiction

More information

5/6/2008. Psy 427 Cal State Northridge Andrew Ainsworth PhD

5/6/2008. Psy 427 Cal State Northridge Andrew Ainsworth PhD Psy 427 Cal State Northridge Andrew Ainsworth PhD Some Definitions Personality the relatively stable and distinctive patterns of behavior that characterize an individual and his or her reactions to the

More information

Mapping Neural circuitry of Risk and Resilience for Suicidal Behavior and Mood Disorders

Mapping Neural circuitry of Risk and Resilience for Suicidal Behavior and Mood Disorders Mapping Neural circuitry of Risk and Resilience for Suicidal Behavior and Mood Disorders J. John Mann, MD Paul Janssen Professor of Translational Neuroscience in Psychiatry and Radiology Columbia University

More information

An Introduction to Translational Neuroscience Approaches to Investigating Autism

An Introduction to Translational Neuroscience Approaches to Investigating Autism An Introduction to Translational Neuroscience Approaches to Investigating Autism Gabriel S. Dichter, PhD Departments of Psychiatry & Psychology, UNC-Chapel Hill Carolina Institute for Developmental Disabilities

More information

5th Mini-Symposium on Cognition, Decision-making and Social Function: In Memory of Kang Cheng

5th Mini-Symposium on Cognition, Decision-making and Social Function: In Memory of Kang Cheng 5th Mini-Symposium on Cognition, Decision-making and Social Function: In Memory of Kang Cheng 13:30-13:35 Opening 13:30 17:30 13:35-14:00 Metacognition in Value-based Decision-making Dr. Xiaohong Wan (Beijing

More information

Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems

Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Advance Access published August 16, 2013 Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems Shamay-Tsoory, Simone G. Department of Psychology,

More information

Reward Systems: Human

Reward Systems: Human Reward Systems: Human 345 Reward Systems: Human M R Delgado, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ, USA ã 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Introduction Rewards can be broadly defined as stimuli of positive

More information

Genetics of psychiatric disorders Dr Radwan Banimustafa

Genetics of psychiatric disorders Dr Radwan Banimustafa Genetics of psychiatric disorders Dr Radwan Banimustafa Schizophrenia Is a chronic relapsing psychotic disorder which affects young population and interfere with: - Thoughts - Perception - Volition - Behavior

More information

Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making

Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making A Unifying Computational Neuroscience Approach Edmund T. Rolls University of Oxford Department of Experimental Psychology Oxford England OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Contents

More information

Neuroimaging in Clinical Practice

Neuroimaging in Clinical Practice Neuroimaging in Clinical Practice John Gabrieli Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences & Martinos Imaging Center at the McGovern Institute for Brain Research, MIT Disclosures Neither I nor my spouse/partner

More information

Childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventative help:

Childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventative help: Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families Childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventative help: Understanding the link between childhood maltreatment and long-term

More information

Running head: EMOTION REGULATION MODERATES PERFECTIONISM 1. Depression in College Students. Jessica Drews. Faculty Advisor: Scott Pickett

Running head: EMOTION REGULATION MODERATES PERFECTIONISM 1. Depression in College Students. Jessica Drews. Faculty Advisor: Scott Pickett Running head: EMOTION REGULATION MODERATES PERFECTIONISM 1 Emotion Regulation Difficulties as a Moderator of the Relationship between Perfectionism and Depression in College Students Jessica Drews Faculty

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2014 Vol. 28, No. 6, 1090 1099, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.866936 BRIEF REPORT Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder James E. Sorenson,

More information

Negative symptoms: Clinical assessments, biomarkers and the role of reward processing. James Gold Maryland Psychiatric Research Center

Negative symptoms: Clinical assessments, biomarkers and the role of reward processing. James Gold Maryland Psychiatric Research Center Negative symptoms: Clinical assessments, biomarkers and the role of reward processing James Gold Maryland Psychiatric Research Center Financial Disclosure I have financial relationships to disclose: Employee

More information

GAP e comorbidità psichiatrica. Eugenio Aguglia. Università di Catania, Dipartimento di Medicina Clinica e Sperimentale

GAP e comorbidità psichiatrica. Eugenio Aguglia. Università di Catania, Dipartimento di Medicina Clinica e Sperimentale GAP e comorbidità psichiatrica Eugenio Aguglia Università di Catania, Dipartimento di Medicina Clinica e Sperimentale The DSM 5 chapter Addictive Disorders includes gambling disorder as the sole condition

More information

Disclosure Information I have the following financial relationships to disclose:

Disclosure Information I have the following financial relationships to disclose: Gambling and Other Addictive Disorders Jon E. Grant, JD, MD, MPH Professor University of Chicago Disclosure Information I have the following financial relationships to disclose: My research is supported

More information

Reviews and Overviews. Mechanisms of Psychiatric Illness

Reviews and Overviews. Mechanisms of Psychiatric Illness Reviews and Overviews Mechanisms of Psychiatric Illness A Critical Appraisal of Neuroimaging Studies of Bipolar Disorder: Toward a New Conceptualization of Underlying Neural Circuitry and a Road Map for

More information

Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions

Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions Anneke. Goudriaan 1,2, Ph.D. Ruth van Holst 1,2, M.Sc. Dick

More information

The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability

The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability Review Feature Review The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability Jan Peters and Christian Büchel Department of Systems Neuroscience, University Medical Centre Hamburg-Eppendorf,

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Reinforcement altered Training phase RTs.

Supplementary Figure 1. Reinforcement altered Training phase RTs. Supplementary Figure 1. Reinforcement altered Training phase RTs. To investigate if there were any effects of reinforcement on Simon task performance, the slope of RTs within each block was quantified

More information

MOLECULAR EPIDEMIOLOGY Afiono Agung Prasetyo Faculty of Medicine Sebelas Maret University Indonesia

MOLECULAR EPIDEMIOLOGY Afiono Agung Prasetyo Faculty of Medicine Sebelas Maret University Indonesia MOLECULAR EPIDEMIOLOGY GENERAL EPIDEMIOLOGY General epidemiology is the scientific basis of public health Descriptive epidemiology: distribution of disease in populations Incidence and prevalence rates

More information

Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis

Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis Roxann Roberson-Nay, PhD Virginia Commonwealth University Department of Psychiatry The SAD PD link Don Klein proposed

More information

Cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling

Cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling Cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling TNU seminar on Pathological Gambling, 2.11.2012 Jakob Heinzle Translational Neuromodeling Unit (TNU), Institute for Biomedical Engineering (IBT),

More information

Typologies of Alcoholics. Thomas F. Babor and Roger E. Meyer, Section Editors

Typologies of Alcoholics. Thomas F. Babor and Roger E. Meyer, Section Editors Typologies of Alcoholics II Thomas F. Babor and Roger E. Meyer, Section Editors Overview Thomas F. Babor and Roger E. Meyer Readers whose curiosity is likely to be aroused by the concept of alcoholic typologies

More information

Altruistic Behavior: Lessons from Neuroeconomics. Kei Yoshida Postdoctoral Research Fellow University of Tokyo Center for Philosophy (UTCP)

Altruistic Behavior: Lessons from Neuroeconomics. Kei Yoshida Postdoctoral Research Fellow University of Tokyo Center for Philosophy (UTCP) Altruistic Behavior: Lessons from Neuroeconomics Kei Yoshida Postdoctoral Research Fellow University of Tokyo Center for Philosophy (UTCP) Table of Contents 1. The Emergence of Neuroeconomics, or the Decline

More information

The Practitioner Scholar: Journal of Counseling and Professional Psychology 1 Volume 5, 2016

The Practitioner Scholar: Journal of Counseling and Professional Psychology 1 Volume 5, 2016 The Practitioner Scholar: Journal of Counseling and Professional Psychology 1 Assessing the Effectiveness of EMDR in the Treatment of Sexual Trauma Shanika Paylor North Carolina Central University and

More information

The Brain, Behavior and Addiction National Family Dialogue January 27, 2010 Presenter: Flo Hilliard, MSH University of Wisconsin-Madison

The Brain, Behavior and Addiction National Family Dialogue January 27, 2010 Presenter: Flo Hilliard, MSH University of Wisconsin-Madison The Brain, Behavior and Addiction National Family Dialogue January 27, 2010 Presenter: Flo Hilliard, MSH University of Wisconsin-Madison Attitudes about addiction and recovery throughout history Disease?

More information

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related?

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related? Shackman, Afterword Q2 1 Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related? Alexander J. Shackman Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, and Maryland Neuroimaging

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Anna SORKIN¹, Avi PELED 2, Daphna WEINSHALL¹ 1 Interdisciplinary Center for Neural Computation, Hebrew University of Jerusalem,

More information

How rational are your decisions? Neuroeconomics

How rational are your decisions? Neuroeconomics How rational are your decisions? Neuroeconomics Hecke CNS Seminar WS 2006/07 Motivation Motivation Ferdinand Porsche "Wir wollen Autos bauen, die keiner braucht aber jeder haben will." Outline 1 Introduction

More information

Depressive disorders in young people: what is going on and what can we do about it? Lecture 1

Depressive disorders in young people: what is going on and what can we do about it? Lecture 1 Depressive disorders in young people: what is going on and what can we do about it? Lecture 1 Professor Alasdair Vance Head, Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics University of Melbourne

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Emotion Processing in Persons at Risk for Schizophrenia

Emotion Processing in Persons at Risk for Schizophrenia Schizophrenia Bulletin doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn085 Schizophrenia Bulletin Advance Access published July 21, 2008 Emotion Processing in Persons at Risk for Schizophrenia Laura K. Phillips 1 3 and Larry J.

More information

Understanding Addiction

Understanding Addiction Understanding Addiction How Addiction Hijacks the Brain Addiction involves craving for something intensely, loss of control over its use, and continuing involvement with it despite adverse consequences.

More information

Suicide Risk and Melancholic Features of Major Depressive Disorder: A Diagnostic Imperative

Suicide Risk and Melancholic Features of Major Depressive Disorder: A Diagnostic Imperative Suicide Risk and Melancholic Features of Major Depressive Disorder: A Diagnostic Imperative Robert I. Simon, M.D.* Suicide risk is increased in patients with Major Depressive Disorder with Melancholic

More information

Issues That Should Not Be Overlooked in the Dominance Versus Ideal Point Controversy

Issues That Should Not Be Overlooked in the Dominance Versus Ideal Point Controversy Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 3 (2010), 489 493. Copyright 2010 Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. 1754-9426/10 Issues That Should Not Be Overlooked in the Dominance Versus

More information

Critically evaluate theories of causation of depression. This essay explores two of the main theories of causation of depression, which are the

Critically evaluate theories of causation of depression. This essay explores two of the main theories of causation of depression, which are the Critically evaluate theories of causation of depression This essay explores two of the main theories of causation of depression, which are the cognitive model and the biological model. It gives a definition

More information

Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD. Lei Zhang. Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center

Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD. Lei Zhang. Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center Running head: NEUROBEHAVIORAL MECHANISMS OF FEAR GENERALIZATION 1 Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD Lei Zhang Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center Running head:

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. Authors Year Patients Male gender (%) Mean age (range) Adults/ Children

More information

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman BIOMED 509 Executive Control Cambridge University Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan UNM SOM JL Brigman 4-7-17 Symptoms and Assays of Cognitive Disorders Symptoms of Cognitive Disorders Learning & Memory

More information

Chapter Three BRIDGE TO THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES

Chapter Three BRIDGE TO THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES Chapter Three BRIDGE TO THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES Developmental Psychopathology: From Infancy through Adolescence, 5 th edition By Charles Wenar and Patricia Kerig When do behaviors or issues become pathologies?

More information

Biological Risk Factors

Biological Risk Factors Biological Risk Factors Ms Angelina Crea Provisional Psychologist Academic Child Psychiatry Unit Royal Children s Hospital Professor Alasdair Vance Head Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics

More information