Unilateral Deficits in Visual Perception and Learning after Unilateral Inferotemporal Cortex Lesions in Macaques

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Unilateral Deficits in Visual Perception and Learning after Unilateral Inferotemporal Cortex Lesions in Macaques"

Transcription

1 Unilateral Deficits in Visual Perception and Learning after Unilateral Inferotemporal Cortex Lesions in Macaques William H. Merigan 1 and Richard C. Saunders 2 1 Department of Ophthalmology and Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY 14642, USA and 2 Laboratory of Neuropsychology, NIMH, NIH, Bethesda, MD 20892, USA This study adapted the method of partial lesions, combined with controlled fixation, to study the perceptual role of macaque inferotemporal (IT) cortex. Unilateral lesions were made in IT cortex of three monkeys, without section of the corpus callosum, and visual function was tested ipsilateral and contralateral to the lesion. The observed changes were compared to the effects of bilateral lesions of IT cortex in one monkey, the approach used in most previous studies. Unilateral lesions produced far less profound, although more selective, loss on the tested visual abilities than did bilateral lesions. All three monkeys with unilateral lesions showed decreased chromatic sensitivity, but sparing of achromatic sensitivity, and severely disrupted learning and performance of visual matching to sample, and in all cases, the visual loss was contralateral to the site of the lesion. Unexpectedly, the magnitude of the contralateral loss was not increased by later section of the corpus callosum and anterior commissure in one of the monkeys, a lesion that removes interhemisperic input to contralateral from ipsilateral temporal cortex neurons. These results support physiological findings that show that the response of IT cortex neurons is dominated by the contralateral visual field, despite the bilateral activation many IT neurons receive. Comparison to earlier studies of lesions of area V4, which provides input to IT cortex, shows that V4 and IT lesions produce qualitatively different effects. Keywords: color, inferotemporal cortex, learning, matching, macaque, shape Introduction In recent years, the function of retinotopic cortical visual areas, such as V1, V2 and V4, has been studied by making localized lesions, and using controlled fixation to place test stimuli within and outside the visual field region corresponding to the lesioned area. Because this approach involves a within-subject control for non-visual determinants of discrimination, such as motivation, and understanding of the task, it provides a direct comparison of visual function at affected and control regions of the visual field. Using this approach, it has been shown that lesions to area V1 cause devastating visual loss (Merigan et al., 1993), whereas those in areas V2, V4 and MT produce more moderate sensitivity loss, but complete loss of the ability to discriminate certain stimuli (Merigan et al., 1993; Schiller and Lee, 1994; Rudolph, 1997; Merigan, 2000). However, despite the advantages of this approach, with one exception (De Weerd et al., 1999), it has only been used for studies of cortical areas that have highly retinotopic visual organization (Merigan et al., 1993; Newsome and Pare, 1988; Rudolph and Pasternak, 1999). Cortical areas in inferotemporal (IT) cortex (TEO and TE) do not show the precise retinotopy of earlier areas. The excitatory receptive fields of TEO neurons are confined largely to the contralateral hemifield (Boussaoud et al., 1991), and the single unilateral lesion study of TEO (De Weerd et al., 1999), also suggests unilateral organization. On the other hand, many receptive fields of TE neurons are large and bilateral, and one of their most dramatic features is the loose retinotopy they demonstrate (Tanaka et al., 1991). However, it is widely recognized that TE receptive fields extend to slightly greater eccentricities on their contralateral edge (Op De Beeck and Vogels, 2000), and that their response properties are dominated by stimuli placed in the contralateral field (Chelazzi et al., 1998). These physiological responses suggest that unilateral lesions of areas TEO or TE might result in greater contralesional than ipsilesional visual loss. If so, this method might be ideal for examining the effects of damage to IT, since emotional or other non-specific effects of bilateral IT lesions (Huxlin et al., 2000) could contribute to the disruption of visual abilities. In this study, we tested the effects of unilateral IT lesions on those visual abilities we have found to be affected by bilateral IT lesions: luminance and chromatic contrast sensitivity, and the perception of illusory contours (Huxlin et al., 2000). In addition, we examined post-lesion learning and performance of visual matching of stimulus color and shape. Section of the corpus callosum and anterior commissure was added later in one monkey to determine if interhemispheric connections altered the effects of unilateral IT lesions. Materials and Methods Animals and Surgical Procedures Subjects were four adult, female monkeys (Macaca nemestrina) weighing 5 kg. Following initial training and the achievement of stable visual thresholds, brain lesions were made using standard sterile neurosurgical procedures. Through a craniotomy, the dura was opened and the desired tissue removed by suction with a 20- gauge stainless steel tube. For two of the four monkeys lesions were made at a single time, but for the other two monkeys they were made in two stages, to help separate the effects of different components of the lesion. Visual testing was carried out until performance was stable, both before and after all lesions (although, as noted below, sensitivity often improved in subsequent test periods). Experiments were carried out in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (1987). Lesions Monkey A (TEO + TE) received a one-stage unilateral lesion of right IT cortex that included both TEO and TE (von Bonin and Bailey, 1947; Iwai and Mishkin, 1969). The areas removed extended caudally to include the lateral bank of the inferior occipital sulcus (IOS), and rostrally to the tip of the temporal pole. Dorsally, the ablations included the inferior bank, the fundus and the upper bank of the superior temporal sulcus (STS). Ventrally, they extended to the lateral bank of the occipito-temporal sulcus (OTS). Cerebral Cortex V 14 N 8 Oxford University Press 2004; all rights reserved Cerebral Cortex August 2004;14: ; doi: /cercor/bhh045

2 Monkey B (TEO + TE) received a two-stage unilateral lesion: (i) of right area TEO, and (ii) of right area TE. Monkey C (TE + CC) received a two-stage lesion: (i) of left area TE, and (ii) of the posterior corpus callosum and anterior commissure. Monkey D (bil. TEO + TE) (bilateral lesion) served as a comparison for the effects of bilateral IT lesions, and received a one-stage bilateral lesion of areas TEO and TE. Following each lesion, monkeys were tested until performance stabilized, which required 4 months. Behavioral Testing Apparatus and Procedures The monkeys were seated, facing a high-resolution ( ) 17 Nanao video display at a distance of 84 cm. They were tested for trials/day, 5 days per week, until performance was stable. All testing was carried out binocularly, and none of the monkeys had more than 0.5 D of refractive error in either eye. All animals had free access to monkey chow, supplemented regularly with fresh fruit, but they had limited access to fluids, which served as rewards for correct responses during behavioral testing. Visual Tasks and Stimuli Monkeys A (TEO + TE), B (TEO + TE), and C (TE + CC), were tested, with controlled fixation, on the three tasks illustrated in Figure 1, in their right and left visual field, before and after each stage of their unilateral IT lesions. Monkey D (bil. TEO + TE) was tested on the same tasks without fixation control, before and after receiving a bilateral IT lesion. Luminance Contrast Sensitivity The monkeys were trained to discriminate the vertical versus horizontal orientation of small patches of grating stimuli (Gabor) (vertical orientation shown in Fig. 1A) by pressing one of two response buttons on the test panel in front of them. Contrast thresholds were then tested, using this procedure, by varying grating contrast under a staircase procedure that increased contrast with each error, and decreased contrast with each three correct responses. Grating patches were 3 by 3 in size, of moderate spatial frequency (2 c/deg.) and their contrast was windowed by a twodimensional gaussian function with sigma 1.2. For monkeys A (TEO + TE), B (TEO + TE) and C (TE + CC), the Gabor stimuli were presented at an eccentricity of 4 in the lower left or lower right visual fields. For monkey D (bil. TEO + TE), the stimuli were presented at the center of the display. Chromatic Contrast Sensitivity The stimuli (not shown) were Gabor patches of isoluminant grating, identical in size and spatial frequency to the achromatic stimuli described above. Two types of isoluminant chromatic contrast thresholds were measured, with red green modulated gratings, and with tritanopic (approximately yellow blue) gratings. As above, the stimuli were 3 by 3 in size, and were shown at an eccentricity of 4 from the fixation target in the lower right or lower left visual field for monkeys A (TEO + TE), B (TEO + TE), and C (TE + CC), and at the center of the display for monkey D (bil. TEO + TE). Figure 1. Visual tasks and stimuli. The three monkeys given unilateral IT lesions were tested on an orientation discrimination, with controlled fixation, for (A) contrast sensitivity and (B) illusory contour orientation as well as on (C) shape and color matching (shape matching is illustrated). The location of the fixation spot and test stimulus were arranged to place test stimuli in the control or lesioned hemifield. (D) The single monkey given a bilateral IT lesion was tested without fixation control on contrast sensitivity and the orientation of illusory contours. 864 Visual Deficits after Unilateral IT Lesions Merigan and Saunders

3 Illusory Contour Orientation The monkeys discriminated the horizontal or vertical orientation of illusory contours made from a set of six concentric circles. Figure 1B shows a horizontal illusory contour stimulus. The three center circles were black, and the outer three circles were of progressively lower contrast. Stimuli were 3 in diameter, and were presented at the same locations as described above for grating stimuli. Visual Matching-to-sample After completion of the above testing of visual capacities, the three monkeys with unilateral IT lesions were trained to match shape and color stimuli in a match-to-sample task, in which sample and test stimuli were presented for 0.5 s each, and onset of the test stimulus occurred at offset of the sample stimulus. In alternating sessions the monkeys matched shape stimuli, and color stimuli. As above, the monkeys maintained fixation during testing, and the test stimuli were presented at 4 eccentricity in either the right or left visual field during each test session. Color stimuli were circular patches of highly saturated yellow, green, blue and red, and the shape stimuli tested are illustrated in Figure 8. Stimuli were 3 in diameter and sample and test stimuli were presented at the same location. Histology At the conclusion of testing, the four monkeys were overdosed with Pentobarbital and perfused through the heart with saline, followed by fixative (4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffer, ph 7.4). Their brains were removed, and 50 µm thick frozen coronal sections were obtained. Alternate sections were stained with Cresyl Violet for Nissl substance to map the extent of the lesion. The stained coronal sections were matched with those from a standard normal brain at 0.5 mm levels throughout the antero-posterior and medio-lateral extent of the lesions, and the lesions were then mapped onto these standardized sections. The dorsal lateral geniculate (LGN) and medial pulvinar nuclei were also examined for cell loss and degeneration. Results Histology The ablations were largely as intended. Coronal reconstructions for each animal are shown in Figure 2. Slight damage to the superior temporal gyrus was seen in monkey B (TEO + TE) (arrow in Fig. 2), likely due to reopening the dura to make the second stage lesion. In monkey C (TE + CC), the corpus callosum was transected in its entirety, caudally from the splenium through the rostral part of the genu. The anterior commissure was also cut completely. There was no observable extra damage to neighboring structures, except for possible slight damage to the fornix, where it was transected to access the anterior commissure. Degeneration was found in the ventral lateral part of the medial pulvinar as described previously (Huxlin et al., 2000; Britten et al., 1992; Dean and Weiskrantz, 1974). In all four cases, no degeneration was observed in the LGN. Initial Post-operative Behavior No disruptions of behavioral control were seen in any monkeys after unilateral IT lesions. Following the bilateral lesion, monkey D (bil. TEO + TE) showed severely disrupted visual behavior, which suggested that it did not recognize the technician, other monkeys or familiar food objects (e.g. vitamin tablets or bananas). By 1 week after surgery, its behavior had largely returned to normal, although competitive social interactions with other monkeys did not return to pre-surgery level. Effects of Unilateral and Bilateral IT Lesions on Visual Capacities Figures 3 and 4 show that, unlike bilateral IT lesions, which reduced both luminance and chromatic contrast sensitivity, unilateral IT lesions reduced only chromatic contrast sensitivity. Figure 3 shows the effect of the lesions on luminance contrast sensitivity. The contrast sensitivity of monkey D (bil. TEO + TE) was severely affected by the bilateral IT lesion, dropping by more than a factor of five. However, no significant loss of sensitivity was seen in any of the three monkeys with unilateral lesions in comparison of contra- to ipsi-lesional visual fields (Fig. 3). Furthermore, in no case did a unilateral lesion cause a decrease in sensitivity. Sensitivity either increased when tested after a lesion, or remained stable in a few cases where postlesion testing followed closely after pre-lesion testing (e.g. monkey A in Fig. 5). Subsequent addition of a callosal and anterior commissure section in monkey C (TE + CC) had no effect on contrast sensitivity. Figure 4 shows results for blue yellow chromatic contrast sensitivity. The sensitivity of monkeys with unilateral lesions was reduced log units in the contralesional visual field relative to the ipsilesional field, and that of monkey D (bil. TEO + TE) was reduced 0.4 log units by the bilateral lesion. Addition of section of the callosum and anterior commissure in Monkey C caused no significant change in the relative contralateral versus ipsilateral sensitivity. Figure 5 shows the time course of sensitivity changes in monkey A (TEO + TE) from before placement of a one stage TEO + TE lesion (Pre), across daily measures of sensitivity immediately after the lesion (Session post-lesion), and with final stable thresholds (Post). Both luminance and chromatic sensitivity were initially decreased in the contralateral field, but showed rapid improvement. Luminance sensitivity recovered to values that were not significantly changed from before the lesion. On the other hand, the yellow blue sensitivity of this monkey was decreased sharply after the lesion on the contralateral side, and then showed recovery. Because sensitivity increased in the ipsilateral field, final sensitivity was substantially greater in the ipsilateral than contralateral field. Examination of the separate effects of TEO and TE lesions in monkey B (TEO + TE), Figure 6, revealed that no color sensitivity loss followed the TEO lesion, thus the full loss resulted from the subsequent, TE lesion. That the TE lesion was the basis of the chromatic sensitivity loss was supported by results in monkeys A (TEO + TE) and C (TE), both of whom showed a substantial loss after a lesion of TE alone (monkey C) or after a combined lesion of TEO and TE (monkey A). The contralateral to ipsilateral sensitivity ration was very close to 100% in all three monkeys before the TE lesion. The lesions caused a log unit loss of chromatic sensitivity. Similar results were obtained for red green chromatic sensitivity (not shown). Unilaterally lesioned monkeys showed losses of 0.1 log units and the bilaterally lesioned monkey D showed 0.4 log unit loss. Performance on the illusory contour discrimination is shown in Figure 7. The performance of the unilaterally lesioned monkeys remained near perfect in both ipsi- and contralesional visual fields. Additional section of the corpus callosum and anterior commissure in monkey C (TE + CC) was without effect on this performance. However, the performance of monkey D (bil. TEO + TE), was devastated, dropping from 95% correct to near chance. Cerebral Cortex August 2004, V 14 N 8 865

4 Figure 2. Reconstruction of IT lesions, shown at five coronal levels. The level of each coronal section is illustrated above on lateral and ventral views of the brains. The lesions were largely as intended. There was no LGN degeneration in any of the brains. One brain, 541, had some damage to the superior temporal gyrus (arrows) probably because we had to re-open the dura to extend the lesion. 866 Visual Deficits after Unilateral IT Lesions Merigan and Saunders

5 Figure 3. Luminance contrast sensitivity in the ipsilesional and contralesional visual fields for monkeys A (TEO + TE), B (TEO + TE), and C (TE) after unilateral IT lesions, and before and after a bilateral IT lesion in monkey D (bil. TEO + TE). Results are also shown for monkey C (TE + CC) following addition of section of the anterior commissure and corpus callosum. Error bars show ±SD. Figure 4. Chromatic (yellow blue) contrast sensitivity in the ipsilesional and contralesional visual fields of monkeys with unilateral IT lesions, and before and after a bilateral IT lesion in monkey D. Results are also shown for monkey C following additional section of the anterior commissure and corpus callosum. Error bars show ±SD. Effects of Unilateral IT Lesions on Visual Matching-tosample Performance All three monkeys learned to perform shape (Fig. 8A) and color (Fig. 8B) matching in their ipsilesional fields, with percent correct performance near 90%. Their rate of learning and final performance was similar to that of three other control (nonlesioned) monkeys recently trained on this task (not shown). However, in their contralesional field, they were unable to perform much above 70% correct for the matching of color stimuli, and above chance for the shape shape comparisons. Section of the anterior corpus callosum and anterior commissure in monkey C (TE + CC) did not further exacerbate this deficit in performance. Discussion The most striking result of this study was that unilateral IT cortex lesions produced contralateral visual and perceptual loss, with no loss seen ipsilateral to the lesions. Furthermore, the contralateral effects of unilateral lesions were not exacerbated by sectioning the callosal and anterior commissure fibers that provide input to IT neurons from the ipsilateral visual field. Whereas bilateral IT cortex lesions disrupted all tested visual abilities, unilateral lesions caused no change in luminance contrast sensitivity and the discrimination of illusory contours. The unilateral deficits observed in chromatic vision Figure 5. Time course of (A) luminance and (B) yellow blue contrast sensitivity for monkey A (TEO + TE) before, and immediately after a one-stage lesion of TEO and TE, as well as after performance stabilized. Large open circles show ipsilesional thresholds before the lesion and after stability, and large filled squares show the same results for the contralesional field. Small open circles and the solid curve show daily ipsilesional sensitivity immediately after the lesion, and small filled squares and dotted curves show sensitivity in the contralesional field. appear to parallel the contralateral color vision loss that has been often observed in humans with localized lesions in the lingual and fusiform gyri. Finally, we found that unilateral IT lesions did not disrupt the perception of illusory contours that is reliably disrupted by lesions of cortical area V4 (unpublished), even though V4 provides much of the input to IT cortex. Unilateral IT Lesions Produce Contralesional Visual Loss That unilateral IT lesions caused contralateral loss of visual thresholds and visual matching performance seems at odds with the bilateral extent of some IT receptive fields in the macaque. The portion of the visual field affected by cortical lesions at earlier stages of the macaque visual pathway is always contralateral to the lesion and consistent with the excitatory receptive fields of the damaged neurons. A small lesion in macaque area V1 (Merigan et al., 1993) caused severe loss of vision in a region of the visual field that is likely to encompass only the excitatory centers of receptive fields, and not the much larger receptive field surrounds. Similarly, lesions in macaque area V4 (Merigan, 1996) produced visual loss in a quadrant of the visual field, approaching within 1 2 of the horizontal and vertical meridia (Merigan, 1996; Merigan and Pham, 1998), a region which covers the excitatory receptive Cerebral Cortex August 2004, V 14 N 8 867

6 Figure 6. Ratio of contralesional to ipsilesional yellow blue contrast sensitivity before (hatching) and after (black) a unilateral lesion of area TE. For monkey A, this figure compares results from before to after a single stage lesion of areas TEO and TE. For monkey B, the figure compares results from before to after the TE stage of a two-stage lesion. For monkey C, the figure compares results from before to after a TE lesion. Error bars show ±SD. *P < 0.01, Student s t-test. field centers of the damaged V4 neurons (Gatass et al., 1988). This loss does not include the surrounds of V4 receptive fields, which extend across the vertical and horizontal meridia of the visual field (Desimone et al., 1993). This failure to find visual impairment ipsilateral to V4 lesions suggests that the visual loss that results from such cortical damage is due primarily to inactivation of receptive field centers. The receptive fields of TEO neurons are larger than those of V4 neurons, and although they show coarse retinotopic organization, with upper and lower field representations, they show little extension across the vertical meridian of the visual field (Boussaoud et al., 1991). In the present study, in agreement with our earlier findings with bilateral TEO lesions (Huxlin et al., 2000), we did not find any visual loss after unilateral TEO lesions, presumably because we did not test the learning of simple pattern discriminations (Iwai and Mishkin, 1969; Blake et al., 1977), or the effects of distracting stimuli (De Weerd et al., 1999), which are affected by lesions of TEO. We did find dramatic visual loss in this study following unilateral TE or TEO plus TE lesions in three monkeys. Unlike the receptive fields of TEO neurons, many TE receptive fields extend well across the vertical meridian, and some are bilaterally symmetric (Kobatake and Tanaka, 1994; Op De Beeck and Vogels, 2000). TE receptive fields do show the same falloff in the spatial scale of processing with visual field eccentricity from the fovea that is found in primate visual perception (Cowey and Rolls, 1974; Popovic and Sjostrand, 2001). However, certain stimulus selectivities and response properties (latency, amplitude, etc.) of TE neurons are relatively consistent throughout the receptive field (Sary et al., 1993; Ito et al., 1995). For example, Tovee et al. (1994) found that the response of TE neurons to face stimuli was strongly translation invariant, with no significant change in the neural information Figure 7. Effect of IT lesions on illusory contour discrimination, comparing ipsilesional to contralesional visual fields of monkeys with unilateral IT lesions [monkeys A (TEO + TE), B (TEO + TE), and C (TE)], and comparing pre- to post-bilateral IT lesion in monkey D (bil. TEO + TE). Results are also shown for monkey C (TE + CC) following additional section of the anterior commissure and corpus callosum. Error bars show ±SD. Figure 8. Shape and color-matching performance of the three monkeys with unilateral IT lesions. Lightly hatched bars show performance in the ipsilesional hemifield and darkly hatched bars the contralesional hemifield. Error bars show ±SD. about face identity even with fixation more than 7 left or right of the fovea. However, it is well established that even at the level of area TE, cortical neurons have a bias towards the contralateral visual field in terms of center location and receptive field extent (Boussaoud et al., 1991; Op De Beeck and Vogels, 2000). The most dramatic evidence of contralateral domination of response is seen when two stimuli are presented simultaneously on either side of the vertical meridian, the response of TE neurons is dominated by the stimulus presented in the contralateral visual field (Chelazzi et al., 1998; Sato, 1989). These observations, coupled with the findings of the present study, suggest that the contribution of TE neurons to vision is much more substantial in the contralateral than ipsilateral visual hemifields. Bilateral IT Lesions Cause Substantially More Visual Loss than Unilateral Lesions The one monkey that received bilateral IT lesions in the present study, as well as monkeys with bilateral IT lesions we have studied previously (Huxlin et al., 2000), showed more substantial and widespread visual loss than monkeys with unilateral lesions. In the present study, bilateral lesions affected all tested visual abilities: luminance contrast sensi- 868 Visual Deficits after Unilateral IT Lesions Merigan and Saunders

7 tivity, red green and yellow blue chromatic contrast sensitivity and the perception of illusory contours, whereas unilateral lesion affected only chromatic sensitivity. Similarly non-selective loss of vision has been found previously after bilateral IT lesions (Huxlin et al., 2000). Some visual abilities recover completely (Dean, 1979) or partially (Huxlin et al., 2000) after bilateral IT lesions, but the range of residual visual disruption can remain substantial (Heywood et al., 1995). The relatively preserved sensitivity after unilateral lesions cannot be due to the maintenance of visual function by the interhemispheric pathways that provide the bilateral receptive fields of IT neurons, since section of the corpus callosum and anterior commissure in monkey C did not exacerbate the visual loss. The greater loss after bilateral IT lesions could in part reflect a generalized disturbance of behavior, such as the Kluver Bucy syndrome that is often found after bilateral, but never after unilateral IT lesions (Kluver and Bucy, 1997). A second more speculative possibility is that IT lesions cause a disruption in the appearance of visual stimuli, which makes discriminations difficult. Monkeys may eventually learn to compensate for the changed appearance after unilateral IT lesions by comparing stimulus appearance in the affected and intact visual field. Such a mechanism would depend on the post-lesion visual experience of the monkey. Thus, deficits after unilateral lesions would initially be as great as those following bilateral lesions, but would decline with the use of vision. TEO Lesions Do Not Mimic the Effects of V4 Lesions Curiously, as reported previously for bilateral lesions (Huxlin et al., 2000), lesions of area TEO, which receives input from areas V2 and V4, did not produce reliable effects of V2 and V4 lesions, the disruption of illusory contour discrimination or grouping performance (Merigan et al., 1993; Merigan, 1996). In addition to providing most of the input to TEO (Boussaoud et al., 1991; Rockland et al., 1994), V4 receives a large feedback connection from TEO (Rockland et al., 1994). V4 also provides input to, and receives feedback directly from, TE (Baizer et al., 1991; Felleman and Van Essen, 1991). If the visual effects of V4 lesions depended on the interruption of top-down influences in the ventral pathway, TEO plus TE lesions should cause the same disruption of performance as V4 lesions. Indeed, both V4 and TEO lesions have been reported (De Weerd et al., 1999) to enhance the disruptive effects of visual distractors on visual discrimination, an effect attributed to impaired attention. However, the disruption of grouping by V4 lesions does not appear to be due to altered attention (Merigan, 2000). Nor is visual grouping disrupted by IT lesions (Huxlin et al., 2000), suggesting instead that this ability involves feedforward processing that is not sensitive to loss of feedback from IT cortex. Chromatic Sensitivity Loss with Sparing of Achromatic Vision In the present study, unilateral IT lesions affected only chromatic sensitivity, whereas bilateral lesions affected all tested visual abilities. Color vision loss has previously been reported following bilateral IT lesions in macaques (Heywood et al., 1988) and to persist even following extensive retraining, given to insure maximal recovery from the lesion effects (Huxlin et al., 2000). The severity of this color vision loss is variable from study to study, ranging from apparently complete color blindness (Heywood et al., 1995), to moderate reductions in hue discrimination (Huxlin et al., 2000). Because we found no postlesion degeneration of neurons in the lateral geniculate nucleus, this loss cannot be due to damage to the optic radiations, which can be damaged by IT lesions (Huxlin et al., 2000). Although severe unilateral loss of human color vision, with partial sparing of luminance vision, is commonly reported to follow unilateral cortical lesions, it is not clear if these lesions are comparable to the IT lesions studied in this experiment. Plant (1991) reviewed published cases of unilateral color vision loss, including the studies of Kolmel (1988), and described frequent contralateral, upper quadrant loss of color detection in patients, with substantial sparing of luminance vision. Rizzo and colleagues (Rizzo et al., 1993) tested two patients with color vision loss secondary to cortical lesions and found, like the present study, decreased sensitivity along both red green and blue yellow axes, with relative sparing of achromatic vision. More recently, two patients were described with upper left quadrant color vision loss, as well as loss of form vision, following a localized lesion in extrastriate cortex (Merigan et al., 1997; Gallant et al., 2000). It is still not clear how the locus of the cortical lesion in these cases corresponds to the somewhat better demarcated boundaries of V4, TEO and TE in the macaque. The human lesions are typically found in the lingual and/or fusiform gyri on the ventral surface of the brain. This region appears to be anterior to the location identified as V4 in functional magnetic resonance imaging mapping studies (Sereno et al., 1995; DeYoe et al., 1996; Engel et al., 1997), and could correspond to the color responsive area identified in such studies as V8 (Hadjikhani et al., 1998). If human V8 does indeed correspond to macaque TEO, one might expect hemifield losses after lesions of this area, since TEO contains a hemifield representation (Boussaoud et al., 1991). However, human color loss often involves only the upper visual field quadrant (Plant, 1991). Moreover, unilateral TEO lesions in the present study caused no loss of color vision, this was found only after unilateral TE or TEO plus TE lesions. Thus, at present it appears that the unilateral color vision loss in humans is more profound than that seen in macaques in the present study, and may involve a cortical area anterior to V4, but with quadrant representation of the visual field. Thus, the area damaged in humans who show quadrant color loss may not correspond to that damaged in this study. Learning/Performance of Visual Matching to Sample Was Severely Affected in the Contralesional Visual Field Visual matching performance was severely affected contralateral to the lesion, especially the matching of shapes, on which the performance of all three monkeys was reduced to near chance. In contrast, performance ipsilateral to the lesion was normal. This unilateral loss stands in marked contrast to the purely bilateral visual deficits found in humans after unilateral temporal lobe lesions (Huxlin and Merigan, 1998). Although this species difference could reflect differences in the organization of temporal lobe areas in macaques and humans, it is more likely that the dramatic unilateral loss observed here was due to the type of testing performed. One important difference between the severely disrupted matching and the minimally disrupted orientation discriminations in this study, was that the latter discriminations were all trained and tested until stability before the lesions were made. Several studies have shown that Cerebral Cortex August 2004, V 14 N 8 869

8 discriminations learned after IT lesions can be more disrupted than those very thoroughly learned (often termed overlearned ) before the lesions (Gross, 1973). In addition, it may be that visual matching itself is particularly susceptible to disruption by IT cortex lesions (Fuster et al., 1985), as suggested by reports that have emphasized the role of IT in short-term memory (Iversen and Weiskrantz, 1970; Wilson et al., 1972; Dean, 1974). Furthermore, a previous study that combined unilateral IT lesions with callosal section (Vogels et al., 1997), found impaired orientation matching contralesionally, despite prior training on this task, but almost no disruption of orientation discrimination. Our results suggest that future explorations of unilateral temporal lobe damage in humans should also examine visual matching performance. In addition, it may be possible to amplify the severity of unilateral deficits in humans, for diagnostic purposes, by presenting irrelevant distractor stimuli in the unaffected hemifield, in order to maximize cross-midline stimulus competition (Chelazzi et al., 1998). Notes The authors thank Dr Krystel Huxlin for valuable comments on the manuscript. This work was supported in part by grants EY00898 and P30 EY01319 from NIH and by an unrestricted grant from Research to Prevent Blindness. Address correspondence to William Merigan, Department of Ophthalmology, Box 314, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY 14642, USA. billm@cvs.rochester.edu. References Baizer JS, Ungerleider LG, Desimone R (1991) Organization of visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in macaques. J Neurosci 11: Blake L, Jarvis CD, Mishkin M (1977) Pattern discrimination thresholds after partial inferior temporal or lateral striate lesions in monkeys. Brain Res 120: Boussaoud D, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG (1991) Visual topography of area TEO in the macaque. J Comp Neurol 306: Britten KH, Newsome WT, Saunders RC (1992) Effects of inferotemporal cortex lesions on form-from-motion discrimination in monkeys. Exp Brain Res 88: Chelazzi L, Duncan J, Miller EK, Desimone R (1998) Responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex during memory-guided visual search. J Neurophysiol 80: Cowey A, Rolls ET (1974) Human cortical magnification factor and its relation to visual acuity. Exp Brain Res 21: Dean P (1974) The effect of inferotemporal lesions on memory for visual stimuli in rhesus monkeys. Brain Res 77: Dean P (1979) Visual cortex ablation and thresholds for successively presented stimuli in rhesus monkeys: hue. Exp Brain Res 35: Dean P, Weiskrantz L (1974) Loss of preoperative habits in Rhesus monkeys with inferotemporal lesions: recognition failure or relearning deficit? Neuropsychologia 12: Desimone R, Moran J, Schein SJ, Mishkin M (1993) A role for the corpus callosum in visual area V4 of the macaque. Vis Neurosci 10: De Weerd P, Peralta MR 3rd, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG (1999) Loss of attentional stimulus selection after extrastriate cortical lesions in macaques. Nat Neurosci 2: [Erratum appears in Nat Neurosci 2000, 3:409.] DeYoe EA, Carman GJ, Bandettini P, Glickman S, Wieser J, Cox R, Miller D, Neitz J (1996) Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93: Engel SA, Glover GH, Wandell BA (1997) Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. Cereb Cortex 7: Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1:1 47. Fuster JM, Bauer RH, Jervey JP (1985) Functional interactions between inferotemporal and prefrontal cortex in a cognitive task. Brain Res 330: Gallant JL, Shoup RE, Mazer JA (2000) A human extrastriate area functionally homologous to macaque V4. Neuron 27: Gatass R, Sousa APB, Gross CG (1988) Visuotopic organization and extent of V3 and V4 of the macaque. J Neurosci 8: Gross CG (1973) Visual functions of inferotemporal cortex. In: Handbook of Sensory Physiology (Jung R, ed.), pp Berlin: Springer. Hadjikhani N, Liu AK, Dale AM, Cavanagh P, Tootell RB (1998) Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8. Nat Neurosci 1: Heywood CA, Shields C, Cowey A (1988) The involvement of the temporal lobes in colour discrimination. Exp Brain Res 71: Heywood CA, Gaffan D, Cowey A (1995) Cerebral achromatopsia in monkeys. Eur J Neurosci 7: Huxlin KR, Merigan WH (1998) Deficits in complex visual perception following unilateral temporal lobectomy. J Cogn Neurosci 10: Huxlin KR, Saunders RC, Marchionini D, Pham HA, Merigan WH (2000) Perceptual deficits after lesions of inferotemporal cortex in macaques. Cereb Cortex 10: Ito M, Tamura H, Fujita I, Tanaka K (1995) Size and position invariance of neuronal responses in monkey inferotemporal cortex. J Neurophysiol 73: Iversen SD, Weiskrantz L (1970) An investigation of a possible memory defect produced by inferotemporal lesions in the baboon. Neuropsychologia 8: Iwai E, Mishkin M (1969) Further evidence on the locus of the visual area in the temporal lobe of the monkey. Exp Neurol 25: Kluver H, Bucy PC (1997) Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal lobes in monkeys [1939 classical article]. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 9: Kobatake E, Tanaka K (1994) Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral visual pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. J Neurophysiol 71: Kolmel H (1988) Pure homonymous hemiachromatopsia. Findings with neuroopthalmological examination and imaging procedures. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 237: Merigan WH (1996) Basic visual capacities and shape discrimination after lesions of extrastriate area V4 in macaques. Vis Neurosci 13: Merigan WH (2000) Cortical area V4 is critical for certain texture discriminations, but this effect is not dependent on attention. Vis Neurosci 17:1 10. Merigan W, Pham HA (1998) V4 lesions in macaques affect both singleand multiple-viewpoint shape discriminations. Vis Neurosci 15: Merigan WH, Nealey TA, Maunsell JHR (1993) Visual effects of lesions of cortical area V2 in macaques. J Neurosci 13: Merigan WH, Freeman A, Meyers S (1997) Parallel processing streams in human visual cortex. Neuroreport 8: Newsome WT, Pare EB (1988) A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). J Neurosci 8: Op De Beeck H, Vogels R (2000) Spatial sensitivity of macaque inferior temporal neurons. J Comp Neurol 426: Plant GT (1991) Disorders of colour vision in diseases of the nervous system. In: Inherited and acquired color vision deficiencies (Foster DH, ed.), pp Boca Raton: CRC Press. Popovic Z, Sjostrand J (2001) Resolution, separation of retinal ganglion cells, and cortical magnification in humans. Vision Res 41: Rizzo M, Smith V, Pokorny J, Damasio AR (1993) Color perception profiles in central achromatopsia. Neurology 43: Visual Deficits after Unilateral IT Lesions Merigan and Saunders

9 Rockland KS, Saleem KS, Tanaka K (1994) Divergent feedback connections from areas v4 and teo in the macaque. Vis Neurosci 11: Rudolph K, Pasternak T (1999) Transient and permanent deficits in motion perception after lesions of cortical areas MT and MST in the macaque monkey. Cereb Cortex 9: Rudolph KK (1997) Motion and form perception following lesions of areas MT/MST and V4 in the macaque monkey. PhD Thesis, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York. Sary G, Vogels R, Orban GA (1993) Cue-invariant shape selectivity of macaque inferior temporal neurons. Science 260: Sato T (1989) Interactions of visual stimuli in the receptive fields of inferior temporal neurons in awake macaques. Exp Brain Res 77: Schiller PH, Lee K (1994) The effects of lateral geniculate nucleus, area V4, and middle temporal (MT) lesions on visually guided eye movements. Vis Neurosci 11: Sereno MI, Dale AM, Reppas JB, Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB (1995) Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science 268: Tanaka K, Saito H, Fukada Y, Moriya M (1991) Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 66: Tovee MJ, Rolls ET, Azzopardi P (1994) Translation invariance in the responses to faces of single neurons in the temporal visual cortical areas of the alert macaque. J Neurophysiol 72: Vogels R, Saunders RC, Orban GA (1997) Effects of inferior temporal lesions on two types of orientation discrimination in the macaque monkey. Eur J Neurosci 9: von Bonin G, Bailey P (1947) The neocortex of Macaca mulatta. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press. Wilson M, Kaufman HM, Zieler RE, Lieb JP (1972) Visual identification and memory in monkeys with circumscribed inferotemporal lesions. J Comp Physiol Psychol 78: Cerebral Cortex August 2004, V 14 N 8 871

V4 lesions in macaques affect both single- and multiple-viewpoint shape discriminations

V4 lesions in macaques affect both single- and multiple-viewpoint shape discriminations Visual Neuroscience (1998), 15, 359 367. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1998 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238098 $12.50 V4 lesions in macaques affect both single- and multiple-viewpoint shape discriminations

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Retinotopy & Phase Mapping Fani Deligianni B. A. Wandell, et al. Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex, Neuron, 56(2):366-383, 2007 Retinotopy Visual Cortex organised in visual field maps: Nearby neurons have

More information

Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe

Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe How many extrastriate areas are there? LOTS!!! Macaque monkey flattened cortex Why? How do we know this? Topography Functional properties Connections

More information

Neuroscience Tutorial

Neuroscience Tutorial Neuroscience Tutorial Brain Organization : cortex, basal ganglia, limbic lobe : thalamus, hypothal., pituitary gland : medulla oblongata, midbrain, pons, cerebellum Cortical Organization Cortical Organization

More information

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception 1 Virginia R. de Sa Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Lecture 6: Beyond V1 - Extrastriate cortex Chapter 4 Course Information 2 Class web page: http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/

More information

Commentary on Moran and Desimone's 'spotlight in V4

Commentary on Moran and Desimone's 'spotlight in V4 Anne B. Sereno 1 Commentary on Moran and Desimone's 'spotlight in V4 Anne B. Sereno Harvard University 1990 Anne B. Sereno 2 Commentary on Moran and Desimone's 'spotlight in V4' Moran and Desimone's article

More information

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) What happens beyond the retina? What happens in Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)- 90% flow Visual cortex Information Flow Superior colliculus 10% flow Slide 2 Information

More information

Chapter 7: First steps into inferior temporal cortex

Chapter 7: First steps into inferior temporal cortex BEWARE: These are preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. Chapter 7: First steps into inferior temporal cortex Inferior temporal cortex (ITC)

More information

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception 1 Virginia R. de Sa Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Lecture 5: LGN and V1: Magno and Parvo streams Chapter 3 Course Information 2 Class web page: http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/

More information

LISC-322 Neuroscience. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation

LISC-322 Neuroscience. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation LISC-3 Neuroscience THE VISUAL SYSTEM Central Visual Pathways Each eye sees a part of the visual space that defines its visual field. The s of both eyes overlap extensively to create a binocular. eye both

More information

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization THE VISUAL SYSTEM Higher Visual Processing Martin Paré Assistant Professor Physiology & Psychology Most of the cortex that covers the cerebral hemispheres is

More information

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones Photoreceptors Rods Cones 120 000 000 Dim light Prefer wavelength of 505 nm Monochromatic Mainly in periphery of the eye 6 000 000 More light Different spectral sensitivities!long-wave receptors (558 nm)

More information

The Eye. Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. Today s goals. 5 From eye to brain. Today s reading

The Eye. Cognitive Neuroscience of Language. Today s goals. 5 From eye to brain. Today s reading Cognitive Neuroscience of Language 5 From eye to brain Today s goals Look at the pathways that conduct the visual information from the eye to the visual cortex Marielle Lange http://homepages.inf.ed.ac.uk/mlange/teaching/cnl/

More information

Visual area MT responds to local motion. Visual area MST responds to optic flow. Visual area STS responds to biological motion.

Visual area MT responds to local motion. Visual area MST responds to optic flow. Visual area STS responds to biological motion. Visual area responds to local motion MST V3a V3 Visual area MST responds to optic flow MST V3a V3 Visual area STS responds to biological motion STS Macaque visual areas Flattening the brain What is a visual

More information

C:\Documents and Settings\sstensaas\Desktop\dental visual 2010\VisualPath dental 2010.docVisualPath dental 2010.doc

C:\Documents and Settings\sstensaas\Desktop\dental visual 2010\VisualPath dental 2010.docVisualPath dental 2010.doc Neuroanatomy Suzanne Stensaas April 8, 2010, 10:00-12:00 p.m. Reading: Waxman Ch. 15, Computer Resources: HyperBrain Ch 7 THE VISUAL PATHWAY Objectives: 1. Describe the pathway of visual information from

More information

Required Slide. Session Objectives

Required Slide. Session Objectives Vision: CNS 2018 Required Slide Session Objectives Visual system: CNS At the end of this session, students will be able to: 1. Understand how axons from the eyes travel through the optic nerves and tracts

More information

Supplementary Note Psychophysics:

Supplementary Note Psychophysics: Supplementary Note More detailed description of MM s subjective experiences can be found on Mike May s Perceptions Home Page, http://www.senderogroup.com/perception.htm Psychophysics: The spatial CSF was

More information

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I Monocular Sensory Processes of Vision: Color Vision Mechanisms of Color Processing . Neural Mechanisms of Color Processing A. Parallel processing - M- & P- pathways B. Second

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention Cognitive Neuroscience Attention There are many aspects to attention. It can be controlled. It can be focused on a particular sensory modality or item. It can be divided. It can set a perceptual system.

More information

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!)

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!) Introduction to Physiological Psychology Vision ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ /~ksweeney/psy260.html Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy (the part that s visible to us!)

More information

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye: Problems confronting the visual system: Visual Physiology image contains a huge amount of information which must be processed quickly. image is dim, blurry and distorted. Light levels vary enormously.

More information

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling. Lect21-22: Vision Central Pathways

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling. Lect21-22: Vision Central Pathways CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect02: BOSS Discrete Event Simulator Lect21-22: Vision Central Pathways Chapter 12 of Purves et al., 4e Larry Wittie Computer Science, StonyBrook

More information

1. The responses of on-center and off-center retinal ganglion cells

1. The responses of on-center and off-center retinal ganglion cells 1. The responses of on-center and off-center retinal ganglion cells 2. Responses of an on-center ganglion cell to different light conditions 3. Responses of an on-center ganglion cells to different light

More information

Pathway from the eye to the cortex

Pathway from the eye to the cortex Vision: CNS 2017 Pathway from the eye to the cortex Themes of this lecture Visual information is analyzed in more complicated ways than in the retina. One major pathway from the eye leads to the striate

More information

Area MT Neurons Respond to Visual Motion Distant From Their Receptive Fields

Area MT Neurons Respond to Visual Motion Distant From Their Receptive Fields J Neurophysiol 94: 4156 4167, 2005. First published August 24, 2005; doi:10.1152/jn.00505.2005. Area MT Neurons Respond to Visual Motion Distant From Their Receptive Fields Daniel Zaksas and Tatiana Pasternak

More information

Shape Discrimination Deficits During Reversible Deactivation of Area V4 in the Macaque Monkey

Shape Discrimination Deficits During Reversible Deactivation of Area V4 in the Macaque Monkey Shape Discrimination Deficits During Reversible Deactivation of Area V4 in the Macaque Monkey Pascal Girard, Stephen G. Lomber 1 and Jean Bullier Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition, CNRS-UPS UMR

More information

Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. Lecture 3: Disorders of Perception

Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. Lecture 3: Disorders of Perception Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Lecture 3: Disorders of Perception Sensation vs Perception Senses capture physical energy from environment that are converted into neural signals and elaborated/interpreted

More information

PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study. Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision

PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study. Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision 1. Explain what a hierarchically organised neural system is, in terms of physiological response properties of its neurones.

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

HOMONYMOUS VISUAL FIELD DEFECTS Perimetric findings and corresponding neuro-imaging results

HOMONYMOUS VISUAL FIELD DEFECTS Perimetric findings and corresponding neuro-imaging results Homonymous visual field defects 511 HOMONYMOUS VISUAL FIELD DEFECTS Perimetric findings and corresponding neuro-imaging results JAN SCHILLER 1, TRAUGOTT J. DIETRICH 1, LIESE LORCH 1, MARTIN SKALEJ 2, CHRISTOPH

More information

Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex

Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex David Whitney,* 1 Herbert C. Goltz, 2 Christopher G. Thomas, 1 Joseph S. Gati, 2 Ravi S. Menon, 2 Melvyn A. Goodale 1 1 The Department

More information

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Overview Explain the significance of the turn to the brain in cognitive science Explain Mishkin and Ungerleider s hypothesis that there are two distinct visual systems Outline

More information

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material Supplemental Material Recording technique Multi-unit activity (MUA) was recorded from electrodes that were chronically implanted (Teflon-coated platinum-iridium wires) in the primary visual cortex representing

More information

Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system

Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system Parallel pathways from the retina to the cortex Visual input is initially encoded in the retina as a 2D distribution of intensity. The retinal

More information

THE VISUAL PATHWAY FOR DENTAL STUDENTS

THE VISUAL PATHWAY FOR DENTAL STUDENTS Neuroanatomy Suzanne S. Stensaas, Ph.D. February 16, 2012 Objectives: THE VISUAL PATHWAY FOR DENTAL STUDENTS A. Draw the expected visual fields seen in classic lesions of the nerve, chiasm, thalamus, optic

More information

Parallel streams of visual processing

Parallel streams of visual processing Parallel streams of visual processing RETINAL GANGLION CELL AXONS: OPTIC TRACT Optic nerve Optic tract Optic chiasm Lateral geniculate nucleus Hypothalamus: regulation of circadian rhythms Pretectum: reflex

More information

The Receptive Fields of Inferior Temporal Cortex Neurons in Natural Scenes

The Receptive Fields of Inferior Temporal Cortex Neurons in Natural Scenes The Journal of Neuroscience, January 1, 2003 23(1):339 348 339 The Receptive Fields of Inferior Temporal Cortex Neurons in Natural Scenes Edmund T. Rolls, Nicholas C. Aggelopoulos, and Fashan Zheng University

More information

Adventures into terra incognita

Adventures into terra incognita BEWARE: These are preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. Chapter VI. Adventures into terra incognita In primary visual cortex there are neurons

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Hillyard(1973) Dichotic Listening Task N1 component enhanced for attended stimuli Supports early selection Effects of Voluntary

More information

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017 V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8 Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017 Topography: mapping of objects in space onto the visual cortex contralateral representation

More information

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni VISUAL AGNOSIA -damage to the extrastriate visual regions (occipital, parietal and temporal lobes) disrupts recognition of complex visual stimuli

More information

Vision II. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota

Vision II. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota Vision II Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota 1 Ganglion Cells The axons of the retinal ganglion cells form the optic nerve and carry visual information into the brain. 2 Optic

More information

The Visual System. Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004

The Visual System. Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004 The Visual System Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004 Phone: 343-4538 Email: vivien.casagrande@mcmail.vanderbilt.edu Office: T2302 MCN Required Reading Adler s Physiology of the Eye Chapters

More information

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision Biological Bases of Behavior 6: Vision Sensory Systems The brain detects events in the external environment and directs the contractions of the muscles Afferent neurons carry sensory messages to brain

More information

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m. Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Wednesday, 16 March

More information

Neuroanatomy, Text and Atlas (J. H. Martin), 3 rd Edition Chapter 7, The Visual System, pp ,

Neuroanatomy, Text and Atlas (J. H. Martin), 3 rd Edition Chapter 7, The Visual System, pp , Normal CNS, Special Senses, Head and Neck TOPIC: FACULTY: LECTURE: READING: RETINA and CENTRAL VISUAL PATHWAYS P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center Friday, 20

More information

Interhemispheric visual interaction in a patient with posterior callosectomy

Interhemispheric visual interaction in a patient with posterior callosectomy Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 597 604 Interhemispheric visual interaction in a patient with posterior callosectomy S.R. Afraz, L. Montaser-Kouhsari, M. Vaziri-Pashkam, F. Moradi School of Intelligent Systems,

More information

Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning

Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning Just One View: Invariances in Inferotemporal Cell Tuning Maximilian Riesenhuber Tomaso Poggio Center for Biological and Computational Learning and Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences Massachusetts

More information

Lecture 4: Lesions and neurological examination of visual function in extrastriate visual cortex

Lecture 4: Lesions and neurological examination of visual function in extrastriate visual cortex BEWARE: These are only highly preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. In the meantime, please interpret with caution. Feedback is welcome at

More information

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1 Announcements exam 1 this Thursday! review session: Wednesday, 5:00-6:30pm, Meliora 203 Bryce s office hours: Wednesday, 3:30-5:30pm, Gleason https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zdw7pvgz0um M Cells M cells

More information

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3 eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3 retinal specialization fovea: highest density of photoreceptors, aimed at where you are looking -> highest acuity optic disk: cell-free area, where

More information

Attentional Control of Visual Perception: Cortical and Subcortical Mechanisms

Attentional Control of Visual Perception: Cortical and Subcortical Mechanisms Attentional Control of Visual Perception: Cortical and Subcortical Mechanisms R. Desimone, M. Wessinger, L. Thomas, et al. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 1990 55: 963-971 Access the most recent version

More information

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection The New Yorker (2/7/5) Selective Attention Perception and awareness are necessarily selective (cell phone while driving): attention gates access to awareness Selective attention is deployed via two modes

More information

Retinotopy and Functional Subdivision of Human Areas MT and MST

Retinotopy and Functional Subdivision of Human Areas MT and MST The Journal of Neuroscience, August 15, 2002, 22(16):7195 7205 Retinotopy and Functional Subdivision of Human Areas MT and MST Alexander C. Huk, Robert F. Dougherty, and David J. Heeger Department of Psychology,

More information

Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex

Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 2382-2386, March 1996 Neurobiology Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex EDGAR A. DEYOE*, GEORGE J. CARMANt, PETER BANDETTINIt,

More information

Cognitive Modelling Themes in Neural Computation. Tom Hartley

Cognitive Modelling Themes in Neural Computation. Tom Hartley Cognitive Modelling Themes in Neural Computation Tom Hartley t.hartley@psychology.york.ac.uk Typical Model Neuron x i w ij x j =f(σw ij x j ) w jk x k McCulloch & Pitts (1943), Rosenblatt (1957) Net input:

More information

Image Formation and Phototransduction. By Dr. Abdelaziz Hussein Lecturer of Physiology

Image Formation and Phototransduction. By Dr. Abdelaziz Hussein Lecturer of Physiology Image Formation and Phototransduction By Dr. Abdelaziz Hussein Lecturer of Physiology Vision Vision is a complex process through which an image of the external environment is formed on the photosensitive

More information

Mental imagery: Against the Nihilistic Hypothesis. Giorgio Ganis a,c William L. Thompson a. Harvard University, Department of Psychology

Mental imagery: Against the Nihilistic Hypothesis. Giorgio Ganis a,c William L. Thompson a. Harvard University, Department of Psychology Kosslyn et al., p. 1 Mental imagery: Against the Nihilistic Hypothesis Stephen M. Kosslyn a,b Giorgio Ganis a,c William L. Thompson a a Harvard University, Department of Psychology b Massachusetts General

More information

COMPUTATIONAL NEUROIMAGING OF HUMAN VISUAL CORTEX

COMPUTATIONAL NEUROIMAGING OF HUMAN VISUAL CORTEX Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1999. 22:145 73 Copyright c 1999 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved COMPUTATIONAL NEUROIMAGING OF HUMAN VISUAL CORTEX Brian A. Wandell Neuroscience Program and Department of Psychology,

More information

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing C Contextual Influences in Visual Processing TAI SING LEE Computer Science Department and Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Synonyms Surround influence;

More information

The Neuroscience of Vision II

The Neuroscience of Vision II Striate Cortex (V1) is not Sufficient for Seeing The Neuroscience of Vision II Hubel and Wiesel conclude their 1968 paper by Where else is Visual Processing Performed? Beyond V1 An important step in identifying

More information

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2 Name: ID # ID: A Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2 Multiple Choice (38 questions, 1 point each) Identify the letter of the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

More information

Models of Attention. Models of Attention

Models of Attention. Models of Attention Models of Models of predictive: can we predict eye movements (bottom up attention)? [L. Itti and coll] pop out and saliency? [Z. Li] Readings: Maunsell & Cook, the role of attention in visual processing,

More information

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience Levels of organization Central Nervous System 1m 10 11 neurons Neural systems and neuroanatomy Systems 10cm Networks 1mm Neurons 100μm 10 8 neurons Professor Daniel Leeds

More information

Sensorimotor Functioning. Sensory and Motor Systems. Functional Anatomy of Brain- Behavioral Relationships

Sensorimotor Functioning. Sensory and Motor Systems. Functional Anatomy of Brain- Behavioral Relationships Sensorimotor Functioning Sensory and Motor Systems Understanding brain-behavior relationships requires knowledge of sensory and motor systems. Sensory System = Input Neural Processing Motor System = Output

More information

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J. The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem By Andrew Laguna, S.J. Outline I. Introduction II. The Visual System III. What is the Binding Problem? IV. Possible Theoretical Solutions

More information

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:.

Cortical Organization. Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. Cortical Organization Functionally, cortex is classically divided into 3 general types: 1. Primary cortex:. - receptive field:. 2. Secondary cortex: located immediately adjacent to primary cortical areas,

More information

Pathologies of postchiasmatic visual pathways and visual cortex

Pathologies of postchiasmatic visual pathways and visual cortex Pathologies of postchiasmatic visual pathways and visual cortex Optic radiation: anatomy Pathologies of the postchiamsatic visual pathways and visual cortex Characterized by homonymous hemianopsia. This

More information

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception EDGE DETECTION Edge Detectors Slide 2 Convolution & Feature Detection Slide 3 Finds the slope First derivative Direction dependent Need many edge detectors for all orientation Second order derivatives

More information

Computational Cognitive Neuroscience of the Visual System Stephen A. Engel

Computational Cognitive Neuroscience of the Visual System Stephen A. Engel CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Computational Cognitive Neuroscience of the Visual System Stephen A. Engel University of Minnesota ABSTRACT Should psychologists care about functional magnetic

More information

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study Chinese Science Bulletin 2006 Vol. 51 No. 5 536 541 DOI: 10.1007/s11434-006-0536-3 Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study QU Zhe 1,2, SONG Yan 3 & DING Yulong

More information

Cortical Visual Symptoms

Cortical Visual Symptoms 대한안신경의학회지 : 제 6 권 Supplement 2 ISSN: 2234-0971 Jeong-Yoon Choi Department of Neurology, Seoul National University Bundang Hospital, Seongnam, Korea Jeong-Yoon Choi. MD. PhD. Department of Neurology, Seoul

More information

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function VISION Vol. 20, No. 2, 67 71, 2008 Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function Keizo SHINOMORI Department of Information Systems Engineering, Kochi University of Technology

More information

PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX AND VISUAL AWARENESS

PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX AND VISUAL AWARENESS PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX AND VISUAL AWARENESS Frank Tong The primary visual cortex (V) is probably the best characterized area of primate cortex, but whether this region contributes directly to conscious

More information

Cortex and Mind Chapter 6

Cortex and Mind Chapter 6 Cortex and Mind Chapter 6 There are many aspects to attention. It can be controlled. It can be focused on a particular sensory modality or item. It can be divided. It can set a perceptual system. It has

More information

25/09/2012. Capgras Syndrome. Chapter 2. Capgras Syndrome - 2. The Neural Basis of Cognition

25/09/2012. Capgras Syndrome. Chapter 2. Capgras Syndrome - 2. The Neural Basis of Cognition Chapter 2 The Neural Basis of Cognition Capgras Syndrome Alzheimer s patients & others delusion that significant others are robots or impersonators - paranoia Two brain systems for facial recognition -

More information

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15 s PSY 200 Greg Francis Lecture 06 How do you recognize your grandmother? Action potential With enough excitatory input, a cell produces an action potential that sends a signal down its axon to other cells

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the Macaque

Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the Macaque Cerebral Cortex March 2008;18:477-499 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm061 Advance Access publication June 4, 2007 Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the Macaque Leslie G. Ungerleider 1, Thelma W. Galkin 2, Robert

More information

Key questions about attention

Key questions about attention Key questions about attention How does attention affect behavioral performance? Can attention affect the appearance of things? How does spatial and feature-based attention affect neuronal responses in

More information

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Baran Çürüklü Department of Computer Science and Engineering Mälardalen University Västerås S-721 23, Sweden Abstract. In this paper

More information

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Frank Tong Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Office: Room 3-N-2B Telephone: 609-258-2652 Fax: 609-258-1113 Email: ftong@princeton.edu Graduate School Applicants

More information

35 Gedney Rd

35 Gedney Rd Ryan E.B. Mruczek home address: phone, e-mail, and website: 35 Gedney Rd 401.573.3022 Lawrence, NJ 08648 rmruczek@unr.edu http://wolfweb.unr.edu/~rmruczek/ Academic Employment Research Scientist Department

More information

Neural basis of shape representation in the primate brain

Neural basis of shape representation in the primate brain Martinez-Conde, Macknik, Martinez, Alonso & Tse (Eds.) Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 154 ISSN 79-6123 Copyright r 26 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved CHAPTER 16 Neural basis of shape representation

More information

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Selective Attention Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form,

More information

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences Marc Pomplun 1, Yueju Liu 2, Julio Martinez-Trujillo 2, Evgueni Simine 2, and John K. Tsotsos 2 1 Department

More information

fmri and visual brain function Dr. Tessa Dekker UCL Institute of Ophthalmology

fmri and visual brain function Dr. Tessa Dekker UCL Institute of Ophthalmology fmri and visual brain function Dr. Tessa Dekker UCL Institute of Ophthalmology 6 th February 2018 Brief history of brain imaging 1895 First human X-ray image 1950 First human PET scan - uses traces of

More information

Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex

Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex Robert Desimone Laboratory of Neuropsychology, NIMH, 49 Convent Drive, Building 49, Room 1B80, Bethesda, MD 20892-4415, USA

More information

Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and. visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex

Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and. visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex Articles in PresS. J Neurophysiol (July 8, 2009). doi:10.1152/jn.00102.2009 Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex Kaoru Amano (1, 2), Brian

More information

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited by law: any public performance or display,

More information

fmri and visual brain function

fmri and visual brain function fmri and visual brain function Dr Elaine Anderson (presented by Dr. Tessa Dekker) UCL Institute of Ophthalmology 28 th February 2017, NEUR3001 Brief history of brain imaging 1895 First human X-ray image

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

Supporting Online Material

Supporting Online Material Supporting Online Material Materials and Methods Electrophysiological recordings and stimulation We recorded the activity from single neurons that were located primarily in the CM nucleus, but also in

More information

Effects of Attention on MT and MST Neuronal Activity During Pursuit Initiation

Effects of Attention on MT and MST Neuronal Activity During Pursuit Initiation Effects of Attention on MT and MST Neuronal Activity During Pursuit Initiation GREGG H. RECANZONE 1 AND ROBERT H. WURTZ 2 1 Center for Neuroscience and Section of Neurobiology, Physiology and Behavior,

More information

The perirhinal cortex and long-term familiarity memory

The perirhinal cortex and long-term familiarity memory THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2005, 58B (3/4), 234 245 The perirhinal cortex and long-term familiarity memory E. T. Rolls, L. Franco, and S. M. Stringer University of Oxford, UK To analyse

More information

fmri adaptation: a tool for studying visual representations in the primate brain. Zoe Kourtzi 1, Kalanit Grill-Spector 2

fmri adaptation: a tool for studying visual representations in the primate brain. Zoe Kourtzi 1, Kalanit Grill-Spector 2 fmri adaptation: a tool for studying visual representations in the primate brain. Zoe Kourtzi 1, Kalanit Grill-Spector 2 1 Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, 2 Stanford University 1 x.1 Adaptation

More information

Report. Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1

Report. Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1 Current Biology 19, 1356 1361, August 25, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.059 Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1 Report Jason Fischer

More information