Accounting Standards, Implementation Guidance, and Example-Based Reasoning

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Accounting Standards, Implementation Guidance, and Example-Based Reasoning"

Transcription

1 DOI: /j X x Journal of Accounting Research Vol. 45 No. 4 September 2007 Printed in U.S.A. Accounting Standards, Implementation Guidance, and Example-Based Reasoning SHANA CLOR-PROELL AND MARK W. NELSON Received 10 November 2005; accepted 7 September 2006 ABSTRACT This paper examines interpretation of accounting standards that provide implementation guidance via affirmative or counter examples. Based on prior psychology research, we predict that practitioners engage in example-based reasoning such that they are more likely to conclude that their case qualifies for the same treatment as the example. We test our predictions in two experiments in which participants judge the appropriateness of income-statement recognition. Experiment 1 uses Masters of Business Administration (MBA) students and varies example type (affirmative, counter) and case (revenue recognition, expense recognition) in a 2 2 design. Experiment 1 supports our predictions. Experiment 2 uses more experienced practitioners, and varies example type (affirmative, counter, both) in a 1 3 design. Experiment 2 supports the use of example-based reasoning, and indicates that practitioners in the both condition respond as if they had only received an affirmative example. These results have implications for understanding how guidance that accompanies accounting standards can result in aggressive or conservative application of standards. S. C. Johnson Graduate School of Management, Cornell University. We thank Rob Bloomfield, Tom Dyckman, Merle Erickson, Tom Fields, Max Hewitt, Frank Hodge, Jane Kennedy, Bob Libby, Kathy Rupar, Steve Salterio, Nicholas Seybert, William Tayler, an anonymous reviewer, and workshop participants at Cornell University and the University of Washington for helpful comments. We also thank Cornell s Johnson Graduate School of Management for financial support. 699 Copyright C, University of Chicago on behalf of the Institute of Professional Accounting, 2007

2 700 S. CLOR-PROELL AND M. W. NELSON 1. Introduction Accounting standards in the United States currently consist of a mix of principles and rules (SEC [2003]), and the Financial Accounting Standards Board (FASB [2002]), International Accounting Standards Board (IASB [2002]), and Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC [2003]) have recently advocated moving to a more principles-based financial-reporting system that avoids bright-line rules and requires more professional judgment to determine appropriate accounting. Much research suggests that practitioners sometimes employ the latitude inherent in standards to make choices consistent with their incentives, such that regulators need to strike the right balance of incentives to encourage accurate reporting when latitude exists (Nelson [2003]). We examine whether, even when practitioners intend to report accurately, the process by which they deal with latitude might produce inaccurate reporting. We focus on practitioners use of examples when determining appropriate application of standards. In practice, examples can be provided by standard setters as implementation guidance when the standard is issued (e. g., Statement of Financial Accounting Standards (SFAS) No. 106 (FASB [1990]), SFAS No. 123(R) (FASB [2004]), SFAS No. 133(FASB [1998])). Examples also can be provided by standard setters subsequent to issuance (e. g., Emerging Issues Task Force (EITF) 94-3 (FASB [1994]) and EITF (FASB [1999])), by regulators via enforcement actions (e. g., Accounting and Auditing Enforcement Release (AAER) 108 (SEC [1986])), and by the business press. We use the term example-based reasoning to refer to the psychological process by which practitioners apply such examples. Prior psychology research examining similarity judgments (e. g., Tversky [1977]) and priming (e. g., Higgins, Bargh, and Lombardi [1985]) suggests that practitioners may conclude that the treatment illustrated by the example is more appropriate for their situation than is actually justified by the underlying facts. Thus, a practitioner might tend to conclude that a particular accounting treatment is appropriate for their case when presented with implementation guidance that provides an example of acceptable reporting (which we call an affirmative example ), and for the same case might tend to conclude that a particular accounting treatment is not appropriate for their situation when presented with implementation guidance that provides an example of unacceptable reporting (which we call a counter example ). Two features of the accounting setting make it difficult to infer that results from prior psychology research generalize, and also present design challenges for our experiments. First, unlike the generic contexts used in prior psychology research, in which examples are abstract and not intended to communicate decision thresholds, a unique feature of the accounting context is that standard setters may choose specific examples to communicate thresholds or conditions that they deem necessary for

3 EXAMPLE-BASED REASONING 701 a treatment to be appropriate. For instance, an affirmative example in which two facts favor revenue recognition and two do not could communicate to participants that at least 50% of case facts must favor revenue recognition for recognition to be appropriate. Results that a participant is more likely to recognize revenue when presented with that example could be attributed either to the psychological factors we hypothesize or to the participant s case meeting the threshold communicated by the example. To discriminate between psychological and threshold-based explanations, our experiments present participants with affirmative examples in which all facts favor recognition (thus setting a very high threshold for recognition that discourages concluding that recognition is acceptable), and/or with counter examples in which all characteristics favor nonrecognition (thus setting a very low threshold for recognition that encourages concluding that recognition is acceptable). In this design, the psychological and thresholdbased explanations yield opposite results, because recognition is more likely for affirmative examples than counter examples under the psychological explanation, but less likely under the threshold-based explanation. As a consequence, this design biases away from finding support for the psychological effects that we predict, and in fact requires that the effect of example-based reasoning be strong enough to offset any threshold-based reasoning that occurs. A second distinctive feature of the accounting context is that accounting practitioners may have incentives to report inaccurately. Three aspects of our experiments allow us to discriminate between psychological and incentive-based explanations. First, we inform participants that their goal is to report accurately, and we use comprehension checks and pre-example judgments to identify the extent to which participants internalize that goal, so we can compare responses between those who internalize the accuracy goal and those who do not. Second, we gather data both from Masters of Business Administration (MBA) students (for whom setting an accuracy goal might be relatively easier) and from experienced practitioners in accounting and finance to determine if results are robust to potential intrusion of more knowledge of financial reporting and incentives. Third, and most important, we design our experiments to insure that, while reporting incentives may affect the general levels of behavior we observe, they cannot produce the specific directional predictions that underlie our hypothesis tests. We also test for and do not find patterns of results that would be implied if participants judgments were driven by incentives to report inaccurately. We report the results of two experiments. Experiment 1 utilizes a 2 2 between-subjects design in which MBA students judge the appropriateness of income-statement recognition, varying case (revenue, expense) and example type (affirmative, counter). Participants receive a case in which they must determine how likely it is that the transaction in question will qualify for revenue or expense recognition, basing their judgment on either an

4 702 S. CLOR-PROELL AND M. W. NELSON affirmative example in which recognition was allowed or a counter example in which recognition was not allowed. Results from experiment 1 indicate that, for both the revenue- and expense-recognition cases, participants are more likely to believe recognition is appropriate when given an affirmative example than when given a counter example. From the perspective of the net income implied by participants judgments, participants are on average slightly conservative, but example type and case interact, with net income higher when revenue standards use affirmative examples and when expense standards use counter examples. Debriefing data indicate that these results were obtained when participants reported using a similarity-based or priming-based judgment process, but not when participants reported using another judgment process. Experiment 2 focuses on the revenue-recognition context, and examines whether results from experiment 1 generalize to more experienced finance and accounting practitioners. Experiment 2 also investigates whether providing practitioners with both an affirmative and a counter example can reduce the effects of example-based reasoning. Accordingly, experiment 2 utilizes a 1 3 between-subjects design in which practitioners judge the appropriateness of revenue recognition, varying example type (affirmative, counter, both). Results from experiment 2 indicate that the results of experiment 1 generalize to more experienced participants that have several years of post-mba experience in accounting and finance. Results also indicate that providing both affirmative and counter examples is not an effective method of reducing the effects obtained in experiment 1, since practitioners who receive both an affirmative and a counter example respond as if they only attend to the affirmative example. Overall, these results indicate that, even when practitioners are encouraged to report accurately, and even when examples provided in implementation guidance communicate relatively extreme thresholds, reporting judgments are influenced by the psychological processes that underlie application of implementation guidance in ways that may not be intended by standard setters. Our results also indicate that providing a balance of affirmative and counter examples may not be an effective approach to reducing example-based reasoning. Therefore, standard setters may want to consider the directional effect of example-based reasoning (conservative or aggressive) created by the combination of accounting standard and example type included in any implementation guidance that they provide, particularly if latitude in standards and implementation guidance increases with a shift towards a more principles-based regime. The next section provides the background information and develops our predictions. The third and fourth sections describe the experiments used to test our predictions and present the results of each experiment. Section 5 discusses the implications.

5 EXAMPLE-BASED REASONING Background and Hypotheses Prior accounting research has investigated various aspects of auditors use of examples. 1 Salterio [1996] provides evidence that, when presented with a set of precedents that uniformly indicated a particular treatment, auditors are more likely to rely on precedents that they perceive to be similar to their case, regardless of source of precedent or client preference. Extending this work, Salterio and Koonce [1997] provide evidence that, when presented with a set of conflicting precedents, auditors are more likely to rely on precedents that favor their client s preferred treatment, similar to other research indicating significant effects of incentives when standards do not clearly specify a particular reporting alternative (e. g., Hackenbrack and Nelson [1996], Kadous, Kennedy, and Peecher [2003], Nelson, Elliott, and Tarpley [2002]). In contrast to these studies, our focus is not on the extent to which reporting preferences affect practitioners reliance on examples. 2 Rather, our focus is on determining whether practitioners use of examples might be affected by the psychological processes underlying example-based reasoning, holding constant that practitioners intend to report accurately. Specifically, we investigate whether practitioners who apply implementation guidance may tend to believe that an example included in that guidance is more relevant to their own situation than actually may be the case. Two streams of psychology research underlie our hypotheses. First, an extensive literature in psychology examines similarity comparisons and classification judgments. A simple model developed by Tversky [1977] provides a useful framework. In that model, the similarity between two items, A and B, isafunction of the features that A and B share, less the features that A and B do not share. Prior research suggests that the weight associated with shared features is often overstated, and the weight associated with unshared features is often understated when unshared features are diagnostic (Frederick and Libby [1986], Smith and Kida [1991], Church [1991], Bamber, Ramsay, and Tubbs [1997]) but overstated when they are not (Nisbett, Zukier, and Lemley [1981], Hackenbrack [1992], Glover [1997], Hoffman and Patton [1997], Shelton [1999]). Assuming the features of an example are all relevant, prior research thus suggests that the similarity between the 1 A related literature examines analogical reasoning, whereby decision makers solve problems by identifying an analog that is structurally similar to their decision problem and mapping elements of the analog to their own problem (Novick [1988], Holyoak and Thagard [1997]). Prior accounting research has examined this type of analogical reasoning in tax, auditing, and managerial accounting settings (Marchant [1989], Marchant et al. [1991, 1993], Matsumura and Vera-Munoz [2005]), providing evidence about the circumstances in which accountants are most likely to identify and apply solutions implied by an analog. 2 We use the term example rather than precedent because implementation guidance may also consist of examples that describe hypothetical transactions for the purpose of providing implementation guidance.

6 704 S. CLOR-PROELL AND M. W. NELSON example and the transaction under consideration is likely to be overstated, which could encourage practitioners to conclude that the treatment indicated by the example is appropriate for their transaction. A second relevant psychology literature involves priming, whereby some stimulus activates associations in memory that affect subsequent processing of information. Prior research on priming in the attitude and impressionformation literatures has found that the priming of an applicable construct, such as an evaluative tone, can increase the likelihood that the construct is used to process a subsequent stimulus (Higgins, Bargh, and Lombardi [1985], Levin, Schneider, and Gaeth [1998]). If an example establishes such an evaluative tone, an example for which recognition was allowed (not allowed) could lead practitioners to conclude that there is a higher likelihood of recognition (nonrecognition) for the transaction under consideration, regardless of the specific facts of the transaction. Key for our purposes is that both of the example-based processes described above (similarity assessment and priming) offer the same directional prediction about practitioners applying implementation guidance that includes examples: practitioners believe the treatment used in an example is more appropriate for their situation than is actually the case, because they overstate the similarity between their case and the example and/or because the example primes them to focus on the outcome indicated in the example. If a case is being compared with an affirmative example, practitioners are more likely to conclude that the accounting treatment used in the example is allowable for the case. In contrast, if the case is being compared with a counter example, practitioners are more likely to conclude that the accounting treatment is not allowable for the case. Thus, example-based reasoning implies a main effect of example type (affirmative/counter) in judged appropriateness of accounting treatment: H1: Practitioners provided with an affirmative (counter) example are more (less) likely to conclude that an accounting treatment is appropriate. The practical effect on net income of support for H1 depends on whether the accounting treatment in question involves revenue or expense recognition. Income is higher when revenues are recognized and lower when expenses are recognized. Therefore, if H1 is supported, lower income should result from providing counter examples to guide revenue-recognition decisions and affirmative examples to guide expense-recognition decisions, since those are the circumstances that discourage revenue recognition and encourage expense recognition. Likewise, support for H1 implies that higher income should result from providing affirmative examples to guide revenue-recognition decisions and counter examples to guide expenserecognition decisions. This reasoning suggests that H1 can be restated as an interaction between example type (affirmative/counter) and case (revenue/expense) in predicting the income resulting from example-based reasoning:

7 EXAMPLE-BASED REASONING 705 H2: The net income implied by practitioners reporting judgments for a revenue (expense) case is higher after receiving an affirmative (counter) example than after receiving a counter (affirmative) example. The example-based reasoning perspective also suggests that more accurate reporting might be encouraged by providing an equal mix of both affirmative and counter examples. If practitioners tend to overweight shared features when making similarity comparisons, overstated similarity assessments between the practitioner s case and an affirmative example could be counterbalanced by overstated similarity assessments between the practitioner s case and a counter example. If practitioners are influenced by priming, the positive prime provided by the affirmative example is counterbalanced by the negative prime provided by the counter example. 3 Therefore, in addition to replicating the results of experiment 1 with more experienced practitioners, experiment 2 tests the following hypothesis concerning the use of both affirmative and counter examples in a revenue case: H3: Practitioners provided with an affirmative (counter) example are more (less) likely to conclude that an accounting treatment is appropriate than are practitioners provided with both an affirmative and a counter example. 3. Experiment METHOD Design Overview. Experiment 1 employs a 2 2 between-subjects design, crossing two example conditions (Affirmative/Counter) and two case conditions (Revenue/Expense). Affirmative examples describe the facts of an accounting transaction that is likely to qualify for revenue/expense recognition. Counter examples describe the facts of an accounting transaction that is not likely to qualify for revenue/expense recognition. The revenue case requires a recognition determination for a bill-and-hold sale. The expense recognition case requires a technological feasibility determination for a computer software product Participants. One hundred twenty-five MBA students enrolled in an MBA-level intermediate-accounting course participated voluntarily in this study. Using MBA students as participants allowed us to enhance experimental power by reducing intrusion from experienced practitioners reporting incentives and their knowledge of existing specific standards and precedents. Data collection occurred at the start of the course (January, 3 Salterio and Koonce [1997] provide evidence that auditors provided with mixed precedents attend more to the precedents supporting the reporting position favored by their client. We focus on circumstances where a practitioner s preference is accurate application of the standard (rather than attaining a favored reporting position).

8 706 S. CLOR-PROELL AND M. W. NELSON 2005), before coverage of bill-and-hold sales or software capitalization, to ensure that participants were familiar with financial accounting and basic revenue- and expense-recognition standards but lacked knowledge of the specific accounting guidance relevant to our experiment. Experiment 1 participants have an average of five years of work experience and are an average of 29 years old. Seventy-three percent are male. The students all self-selected to take at least one elective financialaccounting course that prepares them for financial-reporting or financialanalysis responsibilities Manipulation of Case. The purpose of the case manipulation is to vary the direction of the net-income implications of participants judgments, with recognition resulting in higher net income for the revenue case and lower net income for the expense case. We choose to examine bill-and-hold sales and software development cases because extant accounting guidance (AAER 108 (SEC [1986]) and Staff Accounting Bulletin (SAB) 101 (SEC [1999]) for bill-and-hold sales; SFAS No. 86 (FASB [1985]) for software) indicates multiple characteristics that affect the likelihood that recognition is appropriate. In both cases, participants determine the probability that a transaction by Capital Auto Parts, Inc. (CAP) qualifies for revenue (or expense) recognition. Participants given the revenue case receive facts relating to a recent bill-and-hold sale, and must consider whether the seller (CAP) should recognize (versus defer) revenue for a product that the buyer has purchased for delivery in the following period. Participants given the expense case receive facts relating to CAP s recently developed computer software product, and must consider whether CAP should recognize as expense (versus capitalize) software-development costs (according to SFAS No. 86 (FASB [1985]), software costs can be capitalized after technological feasibility has been reached). While the revenue and expense cases may differ in a number of respects that influence participants assessments of the probability that recognition is appropriate, the important difference for our purposes is the directional effect of case on net income revenue recognition increases income while expense recognition decreases net income. Each case provides four key facts relating to the transaction in question. Two facts support concluding that recognition is appropriate and another two facts support concluding that recognition is inappropriate, so it is unclear from only the case materials whether recognition is appropriate. Facts provided in the revenue case are adapted from AAER 108 (SEC [1986]); facts provided in the expense case are adapted from SFAS No. 86 (FASB [1985]). The order of facts and whether a given fact supports recognition is balanced between subjects. 4 Pilot tests confirmed that the facts are similar 4 Specifically, to balance any order effects, facts presented in each case appear in 16 different orders. The orders are selected so that each fact appears in each position an equal number of times. Each fact is either in favor of or against recognition an equal number of times. Each

9 EXAMPLE-BASED REASONING 707 in terms of the extent to which they provide important information that does or does not support recognition. Each case is also accompanied by a statement of basic revenue- or expenserecognition principles (adapted from Kieso, Weygandt, and Warfield [2004]). The statement was designed to provide participants with a simplified version of an accounting standard that contains some latitude. This statement was identified as the relevant standard, but participants were also informed that various aspects affect the likelihood that recognition is appropriate, and that no individual aspect guarantees that recognition is appropriate Manipulation of Example. An important feature of the accounting setting is that standard setters provide examples as implementation guidance to communicate information to practitioners. Consequently, any example provided in a standard could be used by a practitioner in at least two ways: It could provide the basis for an example-based comparison, or it could impart information about some decision threshold. For instance, if an affirmative example contains two positive facts (consistent with recognition) and two negative facts (inconsistent with recognition), it can be viewed as indicating the decision threshold of two elements are sufficient for recognition to be allowed. Practitioners applying that example to a mixed-fact case could conclude that recognition is allowed, either because they are applying example-based reasoning or because the case satisfies the decision threshold communicated by the example. As our focus is on example-based reasoning, we wanted to use examples that only support our hypothesis when an example-based process is used, and not when a decision threshold is being applied. Therefore, the affirmative (counter) example used in the experiment describes a situation in which all four of the key facts relating to the transaction support recognition (nonrecognition). The decision threshold that can be extrapolated from these examples is recognition is likely to be allowed (disallowed) when all four elements are present (absent), which biases against concluding that recognition is (is not) justified for a case that has a mix of positive and negative elements. Thus, to the extent the examples communicate a decision threshold, the threshold biases away from our hypothesis, because the affirmative example sets a high threshold for recognition (all four features present) and therefore discourages recognition, while the counter example sets a low threshold for recognition (all four features not present) and therefore encourages recognition. However, to the extent participants pursue example-based reasoning as we hypothesize, they are more likely to fact is not always followed or preceded by the same other fact. The orders are selected so that each fact is sometimes consistent with and sometimes in opposition to each other fact in the case. Finally, the orders are selected so that facts indicating recognition are always followed by facts indicating nonrecognition and vice versa. Two recognition facts never appear next to each other and two nonrecognition facts never appear next to each other.

10 708 S. CLOR-PROELL AND M. W. NELSON conclude recognition is justified in the affirmative example case than in the counter example case Materials and Procedure. Appendices 1 and 2 include task and case information, the standard, the affirmative example, and the counter example used in the revenue and expense treatments, respectively. Participants first read a case that considers the appropriate accounting treatment for either a bill-and-hold sale (Revenue case) or costs related to the development of a computer software product (Expense case). Each case contains background financial information and the specific facts of the transaction in question. The background financial information is based on the CAP case used by Nelson, Smith, and Palmrose [2005] and adapted from Braun [2001] and Libby and Kinney [2000]. The background information is intended to indicate to participants that the treatment of the transaction in question has a relatively large financial impact on the profitability of the company. Specifically, recognizing revenue or expense from the transaction in question results in a change in net income that is almost 5% of the prior year s net income and 0.5% of the prior year s total assets. Participants are provided a case and a relevant standard (but not an example that provides implementation guidance). Participants are informed that they have an incentive to report net income accurately rather than conservatively or aggressively, and asked to indicate their incentive to reinforce it and provide data for a comprehension check. Participants also are informed that, given the case materials and standard, they should think that the likelihood of qualifying for recognition is 50%. Participants are then asked to indicate on a 100-point scale the probability that the transaction in the case qualifies for revenue or expense recognition. The lower (upper) end of the scale is marked numerically as 0 (100) and the description associated with the number is definitely does NOT qualify ( definitely qualifies ). This judgment serves as a pretest baseline to assess the effect of examples, and also provides evidence about whether participants internalized instructions that conveyed an accuracy goal and a 50% pre-example likelihood of the case qualifying for recognition. 6 Next, participants are asked to assume that the standard is accompanied by an example that is intended to provide further guidance. The example is 5 The affirmative and counter examples are consistent with each other, because the affirmative example indicates that all positive attributes allow recognition and the counter example indicates that all negative attributes do not allow recognition. Thus, the processes that we investigate are distinct from the effects of counter-factual reasoning as investigated by Heiman [1990], Koonce [1992], and Kadous, Krische, and Sedor [2005], in which considering inconsistent evidence debiases optimism or overconfidence. 6 By encouraging participants to hold a prior belief around 50% and then eliciting this prior belief, our design enables us to decrease the amount of noise in the pretest judgment while still allowing participants to set their own prior, which has been found to play an important role in prior belief-revision research (McMillan and White [1993]). It also minimizes the risk of ceiling or floor effects.

11 EXAMPLE-BASED REASONING 709 either an affirmative example or counter example as dictated by the treatment condition. After reviewing this information, participants are again asked to indicate the probability that the transaction in the CAP case qualifies for recognition as revenue or as expense, depending on the treatment condition. The experiment concludes with participants completing a short debriefing questionnaire. 3.2 RESULTS Comprehension Checks. The case materials indicate to participants that they should have an incentive to report net income accurately (i.e., unbiased) rather than conservatively (i.e., understated) or aggressively (i.e., overstated). The results of a comprehension-check question reveal that 10 participants (8%) do not indicate they have an accuracy incentive. Dropping these participants from the analysis does not affect the results that follow, so all analyses include these participants. The case materials also indicate that, prior to receiving an affirmative or counter example, there is a 50% probability that the transaction qualifies for recognition. The results of the pre-example judgment provide evidence about the extent to which participants internalize the 50% prior. A majority of participants (77) select 50% as the pre-example judgment, but a significant minority (48) do not. Dropping these participants from the analysis does not affect the results that follow, so all analyses include these participants Hypothesis Tests. H1 predicts a main effect for participants judgments of the probability that the transaction in the case qualifies for recognition, with participants judgments higher when presented with an affirmative example than when presented with a counter example. Mean judgments are shown in table 1, panel A for pre-example judgments, post-example judgments, and the difference between pre- and post-example judgments. As shown in table 1, panel B, a general linear model (GLM) with post-example judgment as the dependent variable indicates a significant main effect of example type (F = 11.37, p = 0.001), but an insignificant main effect of case (F = 1.17, p = 0.194), and an insignificant interaction between example type and case (F = 1.13, p = 0.289). 8, 9 This result supports H1 by indicating that 7 All analyses are presented using the post-example judgment as the dependent variable. Results are similar if the dependent variable is the difference between the pre- and post-example judgments, and when pre-example judgment is included as a covariate. In addition, using an indicator variable set to one if the pre-example judgment is 50% and zero otherwise reveals that the covariate does not significantly interact with example type or case, indicating that results do not depend on whether participants initial judgment deviated from 50%. When the pre-example judgment is the dependent variable, neither the main effects nor the interaction are significant. 8 Order is not significant in any analyses, so it is dropped from analyses and is not discussed further. 9 All data in both experiments are analyzed parametrically since the assumptions of normality and equal variance are met. However, the same results of hypothesis tests are obtained when rank-transformed data are used to perform nonparametric analyses.

12 710 S. CLOR-PROELL AND M. W. NELSON TABLE 1 Results of Experiment 1: Judged Probability that Transaction Qualifies for Recognition Panel A: Cell means Revenue Case Expense Case Collapsed across Case Pre- Post- Pre- Post- Pre- Postexample example example example example example Example Type N Judgment Judgment Diff. N Judgment Judgment Diff. Judgment Judgment Diff. Affirmative Counter Collapsed by type Panel B: Test of H1 Sum of Mean Source df Squares Square F Value p-value Case 1 1, , Example 1 8, , Case Example Error , Participants in experiment 1 judge the probability that the transaction in the case qualifies for revenue or expense recognition. This table reports descriptive statistics and hypothesis tests about participants judgments. Participants receive a basic revenue (expense) recognition standard and apply it to a case that always contains an equal mix of facts supporting recognition and facts supporting nonrecognition. Participants make pre-example judgments (based on only the case information and general standard) and post-example judgments (after receiving either an affirmative example in which all facts favor recognition and recognition is allowed, or a counter example where all facts favor nonrecognition and recognition is not allowed), with the example serving as implementation guidance that supplements the standard.

13 EXAMPLE-BASED REASONING 711 the judged probability of recognition is higher when provided an affirmative example than when provided a counter example. Another way to consider the effects of example-based reasoning is to focus on the effect of recognition judgments on net income. H2 predicts that example type and case interact in determining income-recognition judgments, such that the probability of making an income-increasing judgment for a revenue (expense) case is greater after receiving an affirmative (counter) example than after receiving a counter (affirmative) example. Participants judge the probability that the transaction in question qualifies for recognition of revenue or expense, so to analyze H2, the dependent variable needs to be converted to the probability that the recognition judgment is income increasing. In the revenue case the dependent variable does not require a transformation because judging a higher probability of qualifying for revenue recognition is equivalent to judging a higher probability of increasing income. However, in the expense case, judging a higher probability of qualifying for expense recognition implies a lower probability of increasing income. Therefore, we subtract the probability of qualifying for expense recognition from 100 to obtain the probability of making an income-increasing judgment. The transformed means are presented in table 2, panel A for pre-example judgments, post-example judgments, and the difference between pre- and post-example judgments, and are shown in figure 1 for post-example judgments and the difference between pre- and post-example judgments. Using the transformed post-example judgment, a GLM procedure indicates a significant case by example interaction (F = 11.37, p = 0.001). Table 2, panel B provides the complete analysis of variance (ANOVA) table. Focusing only on the revenue case, simple-effects tests indicate that income-increasing judgments are significantly more probable when participants are provided an affirmative example than when they are provided a counter example (t = 3.07, p = 0.001). Focusing only on the expense case, simple-effects tests indicate that income-increasing judgments are significantly more probable when participants are provided a counter example than when they are provided an affirmative example (t = 1.67, p = 0.049). 10 These results support H2. The main effect for case is insignificant (F = 0.03, p = 0.858), as is the main effect for example (F = 1.13, p = 0.289) Alternatively, focusing only on affirmative examples, simple-effects tests indicate that income-increasing judgments are significantly more probable when participants are provided a revenue case than an expense case (t = 2.31, p = 0.011). Focusing only on counter examples, simple-effects tests indicate that income-increasing judgments are significantly more probable when participants are provided an expense case than a revenue case (t = 2.46, p = 0.008). 11 We make no prediction about whether the absolute differences between pre- and postexample judgments differ between case and example types. The data reported in table 2, panel A suggest a larger absolute difference for the revenue case than for the expense case and for affirmative examples than for counter examples, which implies that participants react

14 712 S. CLOR-PROELL AND M. W. NELSON Panel A: Cell means TABLE 2 Results of Experiment 1: Implied Income Effects Revenue Case Expense Case Collapsed across Case Pre- Post- Pre- Post- Pre- Postexample example example example example example Example Type N Judgment Judgment Diff. N Judgment Judgment Diff. Judgment Judgment Diff. Affirmative Counter Collapsed by type Panel B: Test of H2 Sum of Mean Source df Squares Square F Value p-value Case Example Case Example 1 8, , Error , Simple effect of example for revenue-recognition case: Standard p-value Parameter Estimate Error t Value (One-Tailed) Revenue Simple effect of example for expense-recognition case: Standard p-value Parameter Estimate Error t Value (One-Tailed) Expense Participants in experiment 1 judge the probability that the transaction in the case qualifies for revenue or expense recognition. This table transforms results reported in table 1 to report descriptive statistics and hypothesis tests about the probability of making an income-increasing judgment implied by participants judgments. For the revenue case the means indicate the probability of qualifying for revenue recognition. For the expense case the means indicate 100 the probability of qualifying for expense recognition. Participants receive a basic revenue (expense) recognition standard and apply it to a case that always contains an equal mix of facts supporting recognition and facts supporting nonrecognition. Participants make pre-example judgments (based on only the case information and general standard) and post-example judgments (after receiving either an affirmative example in which all facts favor recognition and recognition is allowed, or a counter example where all facts favor nonrecognition and recognition is not allowed), with the example serving as implementation guidance that supplements the standard.

15 EXAMPLE-BASED REASONING 713 FIG. 1. Results: Experiment 1. Participants in experiment 1 judge the probability that the transaction in the case qualifies for revenue or expense recognition. Participants receive a basic revenue (expense) recognition standard and apply it to a case that always contains an equal mix of facts supporting recognition and facts supporting nonrecognition. Participants make pre-example judgments (based on only the case information and general standard) and post-example judgments (after receiving either an affirmative example in which all facts favor recognition and recognition was allowed, or a counter example where all facts favor nonrecognition and recognition is not allowed), with the example serving as implementation guidance that supplements the standard. This figure shows mean judgments of the appropriateness of making an income-increasing judgment. For the revenue case the means indicate the probability of qualifying for revenue recognition. For the expense case the means indicate 100 the probability of qualifying for expense recognition. Difference is mean post-judgment less mean pre-judgment Testing Alternative Explanations Effect of reporting threshold implied by example. One alternative explanation for the results is that an example provided as implementation more strongly to examples in the revenue case than in the expense case, and to affirmative examples than to counter examples. To test this possibility, we perform an ANOVA in which the absolute value of the difference is the dependent variable and case and example type are the independent variables. The results reveal insignificant main effects for case (F = 0.99, p = 0.322) and example type (F = 0.72, p = 0.398), and an insignificant interaction (F = 0.18, p = 0.674). This analysis indicates that participants do not react significantly differently to examples in the revenue and expense cases and do not react significantly differently to affirmative and counter examples.

16 714 S. CLOR-PROELL AND M. W. NELSON guidance could communicate a threshold necessary for a particular accounting treatment to be allowed. As discussed previously, the examples used in experiment 1 are relatively extreme, with affirmative examples indicating the accounting treatment is appropriate when all of the four facts in the example favor the treatment, and counter examples indicating the accounting treatment is not appropriate when none of the four facts in the example favor the treatment. To the extent that these examples communicate a threshold, affirmative examples communicate a high threshold for recognition (i.e., implying if have all facts favoring recognition, can recognize ), while counter examples communicate a low threshold (i.e., implying if don t have any facts favoring recognition, do not recognize ). Therefore, threshold-based reasoning biases against support for our psychological explanation, because practitioners are likely to judge recognition as more appropriate when provided a counter example (that implies a low threshold) than an affirmative example (that implies a high threshold), such that income is lower when affirmative examples are provided in the revenue case and when counter examples are provided in the expense case. In fact, threshold-based reasoning would produce a main effect of example type on judged appropriateness of recognition that is opposite to that which was predicted by H1 and supported by our results, and would produce an interaction between example type and case on the income resulting from participants judgments that is opposite to that predicted by H2 and supported by our data. Thus, the data do not support a threshold-based explanation for the results Effect of nonaccuracy incentives. Application of an example to a reporting decision can also be affected by preferences that a practitioner has for particular effects on net income. For example, many practitioners might be accustomed to preferring income-increasing alternatives. We can test in two ways for an effect of practitioners perceptions of reporting incentives. First, recall that our instructions are designed to convey an accuracy incentive along with a case that suggests a 50% probability of qualifying for recognition. To the extent that participants knowledge of incentives or reporting practices affects their judgment within the experiment, we expect their pre-example judgment to deviate from 50%. Of the 48 participants who do not select 50% as their pre-example judgment, 27 receive the revenue case and provide a mean pre-example judgment of 34.8% likelihood of revenue recognition, and 21 receive the expense case and provide a mean pre-example judgment of 55.6% likelihood of expense recognition, indicating a slight preference for conservative reporting among those who deviate from a 50% pre-example judgment. However, as discussed in footnote 7, results of hypothesis tests do not depend on whether participants express a 50% pre-example judgment. Thus, analyses of pre-example judgments do not suggest an intrusion of knowledge or reporting incentives that affects our results. Second, our design allows us to test for an incentive-based pattern of post-example judgments. Participants who perceive an income-increasing

17 EXAMPLE-BASED REASONING 715 incentive tend to recognize revenue and not expense, and participants who perceive an income-decreasing incentive tend to recognize expense and not income. Therefore, incentive-based reasoning would produce a main effect of case and no main effect or interaction between case and example type both on judged appropriateness of recognition and on the income resulting from participants judgments. Results of tests of H1 and H2 show no main effect for case, but rather the predicted main effect for example type for H1 and the predicted interaction between example type and case for H2. Thus, the data do not support an incentive-based explanation for the results Effect of individual facts indicating recognition or nonrecognition. Two of the four facts presented in the revenue and expense cases support recognition, while the other two do not support recognition. Whether a particular fact supports recognition is manipulated between subjects in four combinations of facts. To ensure that the results are not driven by any specific combination of facts, we analyze the transformed post-example judgments in a model that includes example, case, and a four-level categorical pattern variable. The analysis reveals an insignificant three-way interaction between case, example, and pattern (F = 0.43, p = 0.855), and our predicted significant interaction between case and example (F = 9.44, p = 0.003), indicating that our primary result does not depend on the specific fact pattern provided to participants Evidence About Process Used to Make Judgment. To provide insight about process, a debriefing question elicits participants perception of the process they use to make their recognition judgment. Participants are asked to choose one of three alternatives that best describes how they use the example provided in the standard to decide whether to recognize revenue or expense. One option describes a similarity-based process: I based my judgment on the amount of similarity that existed between the CAP case and the example provided in the standard. I thought the CAP case was somewhat similar to the example, so I thought it was more likely that the CAP case should be treated similar to how the example was treated. A second option describes a priming-based process: Regardless of the degree of similarity between the CAP case and the example, I based my decision on whether revenue (expense) recognition was allowed for the example. If revenue (expense) recognition was allowed for the example, I concluded it was more likely that revenue (expense) recognition would be allowed in general. If revenue (expense) recognition was not allowed in the example, I concluded it was less likely that revenue (expense) recognition would be allowed in general. A third option was Other (please explain). The order of options 1 and 2 is balanced between subjects. One participant does not indicate which process he uses to make his judgment. Sixty-two (50%) of the remaining 124 participants indicate they use a similarity-based process, 44 participants (35%) indicate a primingbased process, and 18 participants (15%) indicate another process. Including a three-level categorical variable for judgment process in our analysis of the income implied by post-example judgments, we find a significant

18 716 S. CLOR-PROELL AND M. W. NELSON three-way interaction among case, example, and process (F = 2.28, p = 0.041). Further analyses indicate that the three-way interaction occurs because the interaction between case and example is present for participants who indicate a priming- or similarity-based process, but not present for participants who indicate another process. 12 This analysis suggests that a selfreported similarity-based or priming-based process is consistent with our results. Participants who self-reported some other process provide a different pattern of results, indicating that the process variable is at least somewhat informative as to the process underlying our results. 3.3 DISCUSSION The results of experiment 1 provide support for example-based reasoning, in that participants who receive an affirmative example are more likely to conclude that revenue or expense recognition is appropriate for the case than are participants who receive a counter example. As a consequence, income is higher (lower) when affirmative examples are provided for revenue- (expense-) recognition cases than when counter examples were provided. These results are different from those that would be predicted by thresholdbased or incentive-based reasoning. Additional analysis reveal that participants who report using either a similarity-based process or a priming-based process provide results that are consistent with our predictions. 4. Experiment 2 In experiment 1, student participants are used to enhance experimental power by avoiding intrusion from experienced practitioners knowledge of existing standards and precedents. However, it is also important to determine the extent to which results generalize to circumstances where experienced practitioners existing knowledge combines with provided examples when applying implementation guidance. Therefore, while we do not posit particular effects of experienced practitioners knowledge, one purpose of experiment 2 is to generalize the results of H1 and H2 to a more experienced population. The pattern of results in experiment 1 also suggests a potential means for encouraging accurate reporting. Specifically, practitioners could be provided with both affirmative and counter examples. As predicted by H3, so long as practitioners focus equally on both types of examples, effects of example-based reasoning are counterbalanced. We focus on only the revenue case in experiment 2 because the results of experiment 1 indicate 12 Specifically, if we exclude the 18 participants who self-reported an other process, such that process is now a two-level indicator variable discriminating between a priming- and a similarity-based process, we find an insignificant three-way interaction among case, example, and process; a significant case by example interaction; and insignificant main effects of process, case, and example. The interaction between case and example is present for both the similaritybased- and priming-based-process participants when analyzed separately.

The Importance of Information Order and Management Explanation when Performing Analytical Procedures in Audit Planning

The Importance of Information Order and Management Explanation when Performing Analytical Procedures in Audit Planning The Importance of Information Order and Management Explanation when Performing Analytical Procedures in Audit Planning Leslie Berger University of Waterloo PhD Student First Year Summer Paper September

More information

Covariation Judgments and Selective Sampling in Auditing: An Experimental Study

Covariation Judgments and Selective Sampling in Auditing: An Experimental Study Covariation Judgments and Selective Sampling in Auditing: An Experimental Study Ananda R. Ganguly University of Illinois Department of Accountancy 1206 S. Sixth St. Champaign, IL 61820 aganguly@uiuc.edu

More information

The effect of competing pressures and outcome knowledge on auditors qualitative materiality judgments

The effect of competing pressures and outcome knowledge on auditors qualitative materiality judgments The effect of competing pressures and outcome knowledge on auditors qualitative materiality judgments J. Owen Brown PhD student Department of Accounting and Information Systems Virginia Tech University

More information

ISA 540, Auditing Accounting Estimates, Including Fair Value Accounting Estimates, and Related Disclosures Issues and Task Force Recommendations

ISA 540, Auditing Accounting Estimates, Including Fair Value Accounting Estimates, and Related Disclosures Issues and Task Force Recommendations Agenda Item 1-A ISA 540, Auditing Accounting Estimates, Including Fair Value Accounting Estimates, and Related Disclosures Issues and Task Force Recommendations Introduction 1. Since the September 2016

More information

American Accounting Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Accounting Review.

American Accounting Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Accounting Review. The Effect of Quality Assessment and Directional Goal Commitment on Auditor's Acceptance of Client-Preferred Accounting Methods Author(s): Kathryn Kadous, S. Jane Kennedy and Mark E. Peecher Source: The

More information

IAASB Main Agenda (September 2005) Page Agenda Item. Analysis of ISA 330 and Mapping Document

IAASB Main Agenda (September 2005) Page Agenda Item. Analysis of ISA 330 and Mapping Document IAASB Main Agenda (September 2005 Page 2005 1869 Agenda Item 4-D.1 Analysis of ISA 330 and Mapping Document 1. Exhibit 1 sets out statements in ISA 330 that use the present tense to describe auditor actions,

More information

Does Information about Material Weaknesses Facilitate Auditors Fraud Detection?

Does Information about Material Weaknesses Facilitate Auditors Fraud Detection? Does Information about Material Weaknesses Facilitate Auditors Fraud Detection? Jacqueline S. Hammersley Terry College of Business J.M. Tull School of Accounting University of Georgia 238 Brooks Hall Athens,

More information

Assurance Engagements Other than Audits or Review of Historical Financial Statements

Assurance Engagements Other than Audits or Review of Historical Financial Statements Issued December 2007 International Standard on Assurance Engagements Assurance Engagements Other than Audits or Review of Historical Financial Statements The Malaysian Institute Of Certified Public Accountants

More information

INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS 3000 ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS OTHER THAN AUDITS OR REVIEWS OF HISTORICAL FINANCIAL INFORMATION CONTENTS

INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS 3000 ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS OTHER THAN AUDITS OR REVIEWS OF HISTORICAL FINANCIAL INFORMATION CONTENTS INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS 3000 ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS OTHER THAN AUDITS OR REVIEWS OF HISTORICAL FINANCIAL INFORMATION (Effective for assurance reports dated on or after January 1,

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

CHECKLIST FOR EVALUATING A RESEARCH REPORT Provided by Dr. Blevins

CHECKLIST FOR EVALUATING A RESEARCH REPORT Provided by Dr. Blevins CHECKLIST FOR EVALUATING A RESEARCH REPORT Provided by Dr. Blevins 1. The Title a. Is it clear and concise? b. Does it promise no more than the study can provide? INTRODUCTION 2. The Problem a. It is clearly

More information

Accounting judgments on terms of likelihood under IFRS: Korea and Australia

Accounting judgments on terms of likelihood under IFRS: Korea and Australia Agenda Paper 12 Joint research project- Preliminary results Accounting judgments on terms of likelihood under IFRS: Korea and Australia Youngmi Seo Korea Accounting Standards Board (KASB) Eric Lee Australian

More information

THE UNIVERSITY OF IOWA 6A:287 - EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH IN ACCOUNTING FALL 2001

THE UNIVERSITY OF IOWA 6A:287 - EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH IN ACCOUNTING FALL 2001 THE UNIVERSITY OF IOWA 6A:287 - EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH IN ACCOUNTING FALL 2001 Richard M. Tubbs Office: W276 PBB Office Phone: 335-0848 Office Hours: By appointment. Class Time and Site: 2:30-5:00 MF C250

More information

Information Pursuit Bias in Auditors: The Effects of Litigation Risk and Auditor Experience

Information Pursuit Bias in Auditors: The Effects of Litigation Risk and Auditor Experience Information Pursuit Bias in Auditors: The Effects of Litigation Risk and Auditor Experience William B. Tayler Assistant Professor Emory University William_Tayler@bus.emory.edu Steven D. Smith Assistant

More information

American Accounting Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Accounting Review.

American Accounting Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Accounting Review. Knowledge Structure and the Estimation of Conditional Probabilities in Audit Planning Author(s): Mark W. Nelson, Robert Libby, Sarah E. Bonner Source: The Accounting Review, Vol. 70, No. 1 (Jan., 1995),

More information

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity Measurement & Variables - Initial step is to conceptualize and clarify the concepts embedded in a hypothesis or research question with

More information

Interpersonal Affect, Accountability and Experience in Auditor Fraud Risk Judgments and the Processing of Fraud Cues

Interpersonal Affect, Accountability and Experience in Auditor Fraud Risk Judgments and the Processing of Fraud Cues Kennesaw State University DigitalCommons@Kennesaw State University Faculty Publications 2018 Interpersonal Affect, Accountability and Experience in Auditor Fraud Risk Judgments and the Processing of Fraud

More information

ORDER EFFECTS ON AUDITOR MATERIALITY JUDGMENTS: THE IMPACT OF QUALITATIVE INFORMATION

ORDER EFFECTS ON AUDITOR MATERIALITY JUDGMENTS: THE IMPACT OF QUALITATIVE INFORMATION ORDER EFFECTS ON AUDITOR MATERIALITY JUDGMENTS: THE IMPACT OF QUALITATIVE INFORMATION Charles A. Brown Assistant Professor School of Business Penn State Erie Erie, PA 16563-1400 814-898-6432 cab51@psu.edu

More information

Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis

Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis EFSA/EBTC Colloquium, 25 October 2017 Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis Julian Higgins University of Bristol 1 Introduction to concepts Standard

More information

Contribution. We extend previous research by investigating

Contribution. We extend previous research by investigating The Effects of Environmental Attitudes, Perceived Environmental Information Importance, Corporate Environmental Performance, and Independent Assurance on Investor Judgments William N. Dilla, Iowa State

More information

International Standard on Auditing (UK) 530

International Standard on Auditing (UK) 530 Standard Audit and Assurance Financial Reporting Council June 2016 International Standard on Auditing (UK) 530 Audit Sampling The FRC s mission is to promote transparency and integrity in business. The

More information

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Uri Hasson (uhasson@princeton.edu) Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 USA Geoffrey P. Goodwin (ggoodwin@princeton.edu)

More information

IAASB Main Agenda (March 2005) Page Agenda Item. DRAFT EXPLANATORY MEMORANDUM ISA 701 and ISA 702 Exposure Drafts February 2005

IAASB Main Agenda (March 2005) Page Agenda Item. DRAFT EXPLANATORY MEMORANDUM ISA 701 and ISA 702 Exposure Drafts February 2005 IAASB Main Agenda (March 2005) Page 2005 579 Agenda Item 13-F DRAFT EXPLANATORY MEMORANDUM ISA 701 and ISA 702 Exposure Drafts February 2005 Introduction This memorandum provides some background to, and

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

Experimental Design. Dewayne E Perry ENS C Empirical Studies in Software Engineering Lecture 8

Experimental Design. Dewayne E Perry ENS C Empirical Studies in Software Engineering Lecture 8 Experimental Design Dewayne E Perry ENS 623 Perry@ece.utexas.edu 1 Problems in Experimental Design 2 True Experimental Design Goal: uncover causal mechanisms Primary characteristic: random assignment to

More information

DATA GATHERING. Define : Is a process of collecting data from sample, so as for testing & analyzing before reporting research findings.

DATA GATHERING. Define : Is a process of collecting data from sample, so as for testing & analyzing before reporting research findings. DATA GATHERING Define : Is a process of collecting data from sample, so as for testing & analyzing before reporting research findings. 2012 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Measurement Measurement: the assignment

More information

More Precise versus Less Precise Accounting Standards: The Effect of Auditor Judgment Frameworks in Constraining Aggressive Reporting

More Precise versus Less Precise Accounting Standards: The Effect of Auditor Judgment Frameworks in Constraining Aggressive Reporting More Precise versus Less Precise Accounting Standards: The Effect of Auditor Judgment Frameworks in Constraining Aggressive Reporting Ann G. Backof Assistant Professor McIntire School of Commerce University

More information

Auditing Standards and Practices Council

Auditing Standards and Practices Council Auditing Standards and Practices Council Philippine Standard on Assurance Engagements 3000 (Revised) ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS OTHER THAN AUDITS OR REVIEWS OF HISTORICAL FINANCIAL INFORMATION PHILIPPINE STANDARD

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information

A REEXAMINATION OF THE DILUTION OF AUDITOR MISSTATEMENT RISK ASSESSMENTS: An

A REEXAMINATION OF THE DILUTION OF AUDITOR MISSTATEMENT RISK ASSESSMENTS: An A REEXAMINATION OF THE DILUTION OF AUDITOR MISSTATEMENT RISK ASSESSMENTS: An experimental study of the impact of client information type, workload, and PCAOB guidance on dilution. Suzanne M. Perry A Dissertation

More information

Technical Specifications

Technical Specifications Technical Specifications In order to provide summary information across a set of exercises, all tests must employ some form of scoring models. The most familiar of these scoring models is the one typically

More information

January 2, Overview

January 2, Overview American Statistical Association Position on Statistical Statements for Forensic Evidence Presented under the guidance of the ASA Forensic Science Advisory Committee * January 2, 2019 Overview The American

More information

International Framework for Assurance Engagements

International Framework for Assurance Engagements FRAMEWORK March 2015 Framework International Framework for Assurance Engagements Explanatory Foreword The Council of the Malaysian Institute of Accountants has approved this International Framework for

More information

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 6.1 Research Design Research is an organized, systematic, data based, critical, objective, scientific inquiry or investigation into a specific problem, undertaken with the

More information

SRI LANKA AUDITING STANDARD 530 AUDIT SAMPLING CONTENTS

SRI LANKA AUDITING STANDARD 530 AUDIT SAMPLING CONTENTS SRI LANKA AUDITING STANDARD 530 AUDIT SAMPLING (Effective for audits of financial statements for periods beginning on or after 01 January 2014) CONTENTS Paragraph Introduction Scope of this SLAuS... 1

More information

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2014, 42(7), 1105-1116 Society for Personality Research http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2014.42.7.1105 ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN

More information

Regression Discontinuity Analysis

Regression Discontinuity Analysis Regression Discontinuity Analysis A researcher wants to determine whether tutoring underachieving middle school students improves their math grades. Another wonders whether providing financial aid to low-income

More information

Framing the frame: How task goals determine the likelihood and direction of framing effects

Framing the frame: How task goals determine the likelihood and direction of framing effects McElroy, T., Seta, J. J. (2007). Framing the frame: How task goals determine the likelihood and direction of framing effects. Judgment and Decision Making, 2(4): 251-256. (Aug 2007) Published by the Society

More information

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing? Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC How are we doing? Prepared by Maura McCool, M.S. Office of Research, Evaluation and Assessment Metropolitan Community Colleges Fall 2003 1 General Education Assessment

More information

Prompting the Benefit of the Doubt: The Joint Effect of Auditor-Client Social Bonds and Measurement Uncertainty on Audit Adjustments

Prompting the Benefit of the Doubt: The Joint Effect of Auditor-Client Social Bonds and Measurement Uncertainty on Audit Adjustments Prompting the Benefit of the Doubt: The Joint Effect of Auditor-Client Social Bonds and Measurement Uncertainty on Audit Adjustments Steven J. Kachelmeier Ben W. Van Landuyt 2016 Illinois Audit Doctoral

More information

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation The description and presentation of results involves a number of topics. These include scales of measurement, descriptive statistics used to summarize

More information

An Understanding of Role of Heuristic on Investment Decisions

An Understanding of Role of Heuristic on Investment Decisions International Review of Business and Finance ISSN 0976-5891 Volume 9, Number 1 (2017), pp. 57-61 Research India Publications http://www.ripublication.com An Understanding of Role of Heuristic on Investment

More information

The Effect of Auditors' Ethics on Their Detection of Creative Accounting Practices: A Field Study

The Effect of Auditors' Ethics on Their Detection of Creative Accounting Practices: A Field Study International Journal of Business and Management; Vol. 8, No. 13; 2013 ISSN 1833-3850 E-ISSN 1833-8119 Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education The Effect of Auditors' Ethics on Their Detection

More information

IAASB Main Agenda (February 2007) Page Agenda Item PROPOSED INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON AUDITING 530 (REDRAFTED)

IAASB Main Agenda (February 2007) Page Agenda Item PROPOSED INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON AUDITING 530 (REDRAFTED) IAASB Main Agenda (February 2007) Page 2007 423 Agenda Item 6-A PROPOSED INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON AUDITING 530 (REDRAFTED) AUDIT SAMPLING AND OTHER MEANS OF TESTING CONTENTS Paragraph Introduction Scope

More information

Mantel-Haenszel Procedures for Detecting Differential Item Functioning

Mantel-Haenszel Procedures for Detecting Differential Item Functioning A Comparison of Logistic Regression and Mantel-Haenszel Procedures for Detecting Differential Item Functioning H. Jane Rogers, Teachers College, Columbia University Hariharan Swaminathan, University of

More information

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Hidehito Honda (hito@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Kimihiko Yamagishi (kimihiko@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Graduate School of Decision Science

More information

MMI 409 Spring 2009 Final Examination Gordon Bleil. 1. Is there a difference in depression as a function of group and drug?

MMI 409 Spring 2009 Final Examination Gordon Bleil. 1. Is there a difference in depression as a function of group and drug? MMI 409 Spring 2009 Final Examination Gordon Bleil Table of Contents Research Scenario and General Assumptions Questions for Dataset (Questions are hyperlinked to detailed answers) 1. Is there a difference

More information

Item Analysis Explanation

Item Analysis Explanation Item Analysis Explanation The item difficulty is the percentage of candidates who answered the question correctly. The recommended range for item difficulty set forth by CASTLE Worldwide, Inc., is between

More information

Understanding Your Coding Feedback

Understanding Your Coding Feedback Understanding Your Coding Feedback With specific feedback about your sessions, you can choose whether or how to change your performance to make your interviews more consistent with the spirit and methods

More information

How Racism Correlates with Perceptions and Attributions of Healthcare Disparities

How Racism Correlates with Perceptions and Attributions of Healthcare Disparities University of Tennessee, Knoxville Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange University of Tennessee Honors Thesis Projects University of Tennessee Honors Program 5-2017 How Racism Correlates with

More information

INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON AUDITING (NEW ZEALAND) 530

INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON AUDITING (NEW ZEALAND) 530 Issued 07/11 INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON AUDITING (NEW ZEALAND) 530 Audit Sampling (ISA (NZ) 530) Issued July 2011 Effective for audits of historical financial statements for periods beginning on or after

More information

DON M. PALLAIS, CPA 14 Dahlgren Road Richmond, Virginia Telephone: (804) Fax: (804)

DON M. PALLAIS, CPA 14 Dahlgren Road Richmond, Virginia Telephone: (804) Fax: (804) DON M. PALLAIS, CPA 14 Dahlgren Road Richmond, Virginia 23233 Telephone: (804) 784-0884 Fax: (804) 784-0885 Office of the Secretary PCAOB 1666 K Street, NW Washington, DC 20006-2083 Gentlemen: November

More information

September audit deficiency trends. acuitas, inc. concluding thoughts. pcaob s audit deficiencies by audit standard.

September audit deficiency trends. acuitas, inc. concluding thoughts. pcaob s audit deficiencies by audit standard. September 2015 home executive summary audit deficiencies pcaob inspections methodology are still significant description of a deficiency audit deficiency trends fvm deficiencies impairment deficiencies

More information

USE AND MISUSE OF MIXED MODEL ANALYSIS VARIANCE IN ECOLOGICAL STUDIES1

USE AND MISUSE OF MIXED MODEL ANALYSIS VARIANCE IN ECOLOGICAL STUDIES1 Ecology, 75(3), 1994, pp. 717-722 c) 1994 by the Ecological Society of America USE AND MISUSE OF MIXED MODEL ANALYSIS VARIANCE IN ECOLOGICAL STUDIES1 OF CYNTHIA C. BENNINGTON Department of Biology, West

More information

Fiona Campbell. ISA 315 (Revised) Responding to Comments to the Exposure Draft. Deputy Chair of the IAASB and Chair of the ISA 315 Task Force

Fiona Campbell. ISA 315 (Revised) Responding to Comments to the Exposure Draft. Deputy Chair of the IAASB and Chair of the ISA 315 Task Force ISA 315 (Revised) Responding to Comments to the Exposure Draft IAASB Meeting March 12 th, 2019 Fiona Campbell Deputy Chair of the IAASB and Chair of the ISA 315 Task Force Responses to ED-315 Broad concerns

More information

The Myers Briggs Type Inventory

The Myers Briggs Type Inventory The Myers Briggs Type Inventory Charles C. Healy Professor of Education, UCLA In press with Kapes, J.T. et. al. (2001) A counselor s guide to Career Assessment Instruments. (4th Ed.) Alexandria, VA: National

More information

Experimental Psychology

Experimental Psychology Title Experimental Psychology Type Individual Document Map Authors Aristea Theodoropoulos, Patricia Sikorski Subject Social Studies Course None Selected Grade(s) 11, 12 Location Roxbury High School Curriculum

More information

IAASB Exposure Draft, Proposed ISAE 3000 (Revised), Assurance Engagements Other Than Audits or Reviews of Historical Financial Information

IAASB Exposure Draft, Proposed ISAE 3000 (Revised), Assurance Engagements Other Than Audits or Reviews of Historical Financial Information Tel +44 (0) 20 7694 8871 Fax +44 (0) 20 7694 8429 8 Salisbury Square DX 38050 Blackfriars London EC4Y 8BB sylvia.smith@kpmgifrg.com United Kingdom Technical Director International Auditing and Assurance

More information

AP Psychology -- Chapter 02 Review Research Methods in Psychology

AP Psychology -- Chapter 02 Review Research Methods in Psychology AP Psychology -- Chapter 02 Review Research Methods in Psychology 1. In the opening vignette, to what was Alicia's condition linked? The death of her parents and only brother 2. What did Pennebaker s study

More information

TRANSLATION. Montreal, April 11, 2011

TRANSLATION. Montreal, April 11, 2011 TRANSLATION Montreal, April 11, 2011 Technical Director International Auditing and Assurance Standards Board 545 Fifth Avenue, 14 th Floor New York, New York 10017 USA Dear Madam/Sir: Please find enclosed

More information

CONNERS K-CPT 2. Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

CONNERS K-CPT 2. Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. CONNERS K-CPT 2 Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. Assessment Report Name/ID: Jen Sample / 334 Age: 5 Gender: Female Birth Date: June 30, 2008 Grade: Administration

More information

An Investigation of Factors Influencing Causal Attributions in Managerial Decision Making

An Investigation of Factors Influencing Causal Attributions in Managerial Decision Making Marketing Letters 9:3 (1998): 301 312 1998 Kluwer Academic Publishers, Manufactured in The Netherlands An Investigation of Factors Influencing Causal Attributions in Managerial Decision Making SUNDER NARAYANAN

More information

Concepts that are initially formed of a stimulus

Concepts that are initially formed of a stimulus Determinants of Product Evaluation: Effects of the Time Interval between Knowledge of a Product's Country of Origin and Information about Its Specific Attributes SUNG-TAI HONG ROBERT S. WYER, JR.* Concepts

More information

Basis for Conclusions: ISA 230 (Redrafted), Audit Documentation

Basis for Conclusions: ISA 230 (Redrafted), Audit Documentation Basis for Conclusions: ISA 230 (Redrafted), Audit Documentation Prepared by the Staff of the International Auditing and Assurance Standards Board December 2007 , AUDIT DOCUMENTATION This Basis for Conclusions

More information

Several studies have researched the effects of framing on opinion formation and decision

Several studies have researched the effects of framing on opinion formation and decision Attitudes 1 Framing Effects on Attitudes Toward Pedophiles By: Larissa Smage, Advisor--- Joan Riedle The effects of framing techniques on attitudes toward a pedophile were investigated. Framing involves

More information

ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR

ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR CHAPTER 6 ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR Several studies were done as part of the UTDFP that were based substantially on subjective data, reflecting travelers beliefs, attitudes, and intentions.

More information

PROPOSED LANGUAGE TO REVISE STANDARD 3.04 OF THE ETHICS CODE

PROPOSED LANGUAGE TO REVISE STANDARD 3.04 OF THE ETHICS CODE PROPOSED LANGUAGE TO REVISE STANDARD 3.04 OF THE ETHICS CODE The Council of Representatives of the American Psychological Association (APA) made the following request to the Ethics Committee in the implementing

More information

14 February Tower Old Broad Street London EC2N 1HQ United Kingdom t + 44 (0) f + 44 (0)

14 February Tower Old Broad Street London EC2N 1HQ United Kingdom t + 44 (0) f + 44 (0) 14 February 2008 Jim Sylph International Auditing and Assurance Standards Board 545 5th Avenue, 14th Floor New York, New York 10017 USA Edcomments@ifac.org Tower 42 25 Old Broad Street London EC2N 1HQ

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

FAQ: Heuristics, Biases, and Alternatives

FAQ: Heuristics, Biases, and Alternatives Question 1: What is meant by the phrase biases in judgment heuristics? Response: A bias is a predisposition to think or act in a certain way based on past experience or values (Bazerman, 2006). The term

More information

INFORMATION QUANTITIY AND ORDER IN STUDENTS TAX RESEARCH JUDGMENTS

INFORMATION QUANTITIY AND ORDER IN STUDENTS TAX RESEARCH JUDGMENTS INFORMATION QUANTITIY AND ORDER IN STUDENTS TAX RESEARCH JUDGMENTS Alexander M.G. Gelardi Associate Professor University of St. Thomas 1 Received: April 11 2009 Accepted: June 30, 2010 Abstract The Belief

More information

Results of the 2016 Gender Equality in the Legal Profession Survey

Results of the 2016 Gender Equality in the Legal Profession Survey Results of the 2016 Gender Equality in the Legal Profession Survey October 2016 INTRODUCTION A Florida Bar Special Committee was appointed by President Bill Schifino at the beginning of the 2016-17 Bar

More information

Examining differences between two sets of scores

Examining differences between two sets of scores 6 Examining differences between two sets of scores In this chapter you will learn about tests which tell us if there is a statistically significant difference between two sets of scores. In so doing you

More information

Paper presentation: Preliminary Guidelines for Empirical Research in Software Engineering (Kitchenham et al. 2002)

Paper presentation: Preliminary Guidelines for Empirical Research in Software Engineering (Kitchenham et al. 2002) Paper presentation: Preliminary Guidelines for Empirical Research in Software Engineering (Kitchenham et al. 2002) Who? Eduardo Moreira Fernandes From? Federal University of Minas Gerais Department of

More information

April A. Working with Individuals at risk for Suicide: Attitudes and Approach

April A. Working with Individuals at risk for Suicide: Attitudes and Approach April 2007 SUICIDE RISK ASSESSMENT WORKING GROUP MENTAL HEALTH & ADDICTIONS SERVICES CORE COMPETENCIES Core Competencies for Assessing and Managing Suicide Risk (Reference: Core Competencies of American

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Auditor Mindsets and Audits of Complex Estimates. Emily E. Griffith University of Georgia

Auditor Mindsets and Audits of Complex Estimates. Emily E. Griffith University of Georgia Auditor Mindsets and Audits of Complex Estimates Emily E. Griffith University of Georgia eegriff@uga.edu Jacqueline S. Hammersley University of Georgia jhammers@uga.edu Kathryn Kadous Emory University

More information

MEASUREMENT, SCALING AND SAMPLING. Variables

MEASUREMENT, SCALING AND SAMPLING. Variables MEASUREMENT, SCALING AND SAMPLING Variables Variables can be explained in different ways: Variable simply denotes a characteristic, item, or the dimensions of the concept that increases or decreases over

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN

Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN Vs. 2 Background 3 There are different types of research methods to study behaviour: Descriptive: observations,

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) Passalacqua, S. A., & Harwood, J. (2012). VIPS communication skills training for paraprofessional dementia caregivers: An intervention to increase person-centered dementia

More information

Sawtooth Software. The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? RESEARCH PAPER SERIES

Sawtooth Software. The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? RESEARCH PAPER SERIES Sawtooth Software RESEARCH PAPER SERIES The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? Dick Wittink, Yale University Joel Huber, Duke University Peter Zandan,

More information

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT Van Der Velde / Guide to Business Research Methods First Proof 6.11.2003 4:53pm page 1 Part I PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT Van Der Velde / Guide to Business Research Methods First Proof 6.11.2003 4:53pm

More information

DRAFT (Final) Concept Paper On choosing appropriate estimands and defining sensitivity analyses in confirmatory clinical trials

DRAFT (Final) Concept Paper On choosing appropriate estimands and defining sensitivity analyses in confirmatory clinical trials DRAFT (Final) Concept Paper On choosing appropriate estimands and defining sensitivity analyses in confirmatory clinical trials EFSPI Comments Page General Priority (H/M/L) Comment The concept to develop

More information

Preliminary Conclusion

Preliminary Conclusion 1 Exploring the Genetic Component of Political Participation Brad Verhulst Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics Virginia Commonwealth University Theories of political participation,

More information

545 Fifth Avenue, 14th Floor Tel: +1 (212) New York, New York Fax: +1 (212) Internet:

545 Fifth Avenue, 14th Floor Tel: +1 (212) New York, New York Fax: +1 (212) Internet: Committee INTERNATIONAL FEDERATION OF ACCOUNTANTS 545 Fifth Avenue, 14th Floor Tel: +1 (212) 286-9344 New York, New York 10017 Fax: +1 (212) 856-9420 Internet: http://www.ifac.org Ethics Committee Meeting

More information

SELF-OBSERVATION AS A SOURCE OF PAIN PERCEPTION 1

SELF-OBSERVATION AS A SOURCE OF PAIN PERCEPTION 1 Journal of ersonality and Social sychology 1968, Vol. 5, No. 3, 205-209 SELF-OBSERVATION AS A SOURCE OF AIN ERCETION 1 RICHARD J. BANDLER, Jit., GEORGE R. MAUARAS, AND DARYL J. BEM Carnegie-Mellon University

More information

Different Scales for Different Frames: The Role of Subjective Scales and Experience in Explaining Attribute Framing Effects

Different Scales for Different Frames: The Role of Subjective Scales and Experience in Explaining Attribute Framing Effects 1 Different Scales for Different Frames: The Role of Subjective Scales and Experience in Explaining Attribute Framing Effects CHRIS JANISZEWSKI* TIM SILK ALAN D. J. COOKE September, 2002 Forthcoming Journal

More information

Wendy J. Reece (INEEL) Leroy J. Matthews (ISU) Linda K. Burggraff (ISU)

Wendy J. Reece (INEEL) Leroy J. Matthews (ISU) Linda K. Burggraff (ISU) INEEL/CON-98-01115 PREPRINT Estimating Production Potentials: Expert Bias in Applied Decision Making Wendy J. Reece (INEEL) Leroy J. Matthews (ISU) Linda K. Burggraff (ISU) October 28, 1998 October 30,

More information

AN EXAMINATION OF ISSUES RELATED TO PROFESSIONAL SKEPTICISM IN AUDITING

AN EXAMINATION OF ISSUES RELATED TO PROFESSIONAL SKEPTICISM IN AUDITING AN EXAMINATION OF ISSUES RELATED TO PROFESSIONAL SKEPTICISM IN AUDITING by ERIN BURRELL NICKELL M.S.A. University of Central Florida, 2007 B.S. University of Florida, 2004 A dissertation submitted in partial

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Hydro One Networks Inc. 483 Bay Street, Toronto, Ontario M5G 2P5. July 7, Attention: Consultation Secretariat

Hydro One Networks Inc. 483 Bay Street, Toronto, Ontario M5G 2P5. July 7, Attention: Consultation Secretariat Hydro One Networks Inc. 483 Bay Street, Toronto, Ontario M5G 2P5 July 7, 2017 Attention: Consultation Secretariat Workplace Safety & Insurance Board Consultation Secretariat 200 Front Street West, 17 th

More information

Goal-setting for a healthier self: evidence from a weight loss challenge

Goal-setting for a healthier self: evidence from a weight loss challenge Goal-setting for a healthier self: evidence from a weight loss challenge Séverine Toussaert (NYU) November 12, 2015 Goals as self-disciplining devices (1) 1. Goals are a key instrument of self-regulation.

More information

Confirmation Bias. this entry appeared in pp of in M. Kattan (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Medical Decision Making.

Confirmation Bias. this entry appeared in pp of in M. Kattan (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Medical Decision Making. Confirmation Bias Jonathan D Nelson^ and Craig R M McKenzie + this entry appeared in pp. 167-171 of in M. Kattan (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Medical Decision Making. London, UK: Sage the full Encyclopedia

More information

Published by: PIONEER RESEARCH & DEVELOPMENT GROUP ( 108

Published by: PIONEER RESEARCH & DEVELOPMENT GROUP (  108 Stress Coping Strategies By It Professionals In South India V. Sreecharan 1, S. Gautami 2, V. Satish Kumar 3 1,2,3 Department of Management Studies, RIIMS, Tirupati, Chittoor(Dist.), Andhra Pradesh ABSTRACT

More information

Volume 36, Issue 3. David M McEvoy Appalachian State University

Volume 36, Issue 3. David M McEvoy Appalachian State University Volume 36, Issue 3 Loss Aversion and Student Achievement David M McEvoy Appalachian State University Abstract We conduct a field experiment to test if loss aversion behavior can be exploited to improve

More information

When Rewards Encourage Professional Skepticism (And When They Do Not) Joseph F. Brazel North Carolina State University

When Rewards Encourage Professional Skepticism (And When They Do Not) Joseph F. Brazel North Carolina State University When Rewards Encourage Professional Skepticism (And When They Do Not) Joseph F. Brazel North Carolina State University jfbrazel@ncsu.edu Justin Leiby University of Georgia jleiby@uga.edu Tammie J. Schaefer

More information

DRAFT FOR PUBLIC COMMENT Guidance Note for ESS9 Financial Intermediaries

DRAFT FOR PUBLIC COMMENT Guidance Note for ESS9 Financial Intermediaries The Guidance Notes provide guidance for the Borrower on the application of the Environmental and Social Standards (ESSs), which form part of the World Bank s 2016 Environmental and Social Framework. The

More information