Enhanced neural processing of goal-directed actions after active training in 4-month-old. infants.

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Enhanced neural processing of goal-directed actions after active training in 4-month-old. infants."

Transcription

1 Running head:. Enhanced neural processing of goal-directed actions after active training in 4-month-old infants. Running head: Total number of words: 6557

2 2 The current study explores the neural correlates of action perception and its relation to infants active experience performing goal-directed actions. Study 1 provided active training with sticky mittens that enables grasping and object manipulation in pre-reaching 4-month-olds. Following training, EEG was recorded while infants observed images of hands grasping towards (congruent) or away from (incongruent) objects. We demonstrate that brief active training facilitates social perception as indexed by larger amplitude of the P400 ERP component to congruent compared to incongruent trials. Study 2 presented 4-month-old infants with passive training in which they observed an experimenter perform goal-directed reaching actions, followed by an identical ERP session to that used in Study 1. The second study did not demonstrate any differentiation between congruent and incongruent trials. These results suggest that (1) active experience alters the brains response to goal-directed actions performed by others, and that (2) visual exposure alone is not sufficient in developing the neural networks sub-serving goal processing during action observation in infancy. Keywords: grasping, experience, infant, P400, event-related potential, sticky mittens, active training

3 3 During the first year of life, infants need to develop both their motor repertoire and their ability to process the goals of other people in order to learn about the physical world and to interact with others. Early in life these abilities develop in tandem. Behavioral studies demonstrate strong correlations between the ability to perform an action and the ability to process the goal of other peoples reaching (Brune & Woodward, 2007; Daum & Gredebäck, 2011; Gredebäck & Falck-Ytter, in press; Kanakogi & Itakura, 2011; Kochukhova & Gredebäck, 2010) and means-end actions (Gredebäck, Stasiewicz, Falck-Ytter, Rosander, & von Hofsten, 2009; Sommerville & Woodward, 2005). These behavioral tasks have been complemented by studies targeting the neural correlates of action perception in infancy. Many of these studies have focused on mµ desynchronization, thought to reflect sensory-motor activity, during action observation in infants that are already proficient reachers (Cannon, Simpson, Fox, Vanderwert, Woodward, & Ferrari in press; van Elk, van Schie, Hunnius, Vesper, & Bekkering, 2008; Mareshall, Young, & Melzoff, 2011; Nyström, Ljunghammar, Rosander, & von Hoften, 2010; Southgate, Johnson, Osborne, & Csibra, 2009, Upshaw, Bernier, & Sommerville, 2015). To our knowledge only one study has investigated the neural correlates of action perception at the onset of reaching between 4 and 6 months of age. In this study Bakker, Daum, Handl, & Gredebäck (2015) related infants reaching ability to an event related potential (ERP) component, P400, known to index social processing, and more specifically, hypothesized to reflect detection of goal-directed actions and agents early in life (Gredebäck, Melinder, & Daum, 2010; Gredebäck & Daum, 2015; see also, Csibra, Kushnerenko, & Grossman, 2008; de Haan, Johnson, & Halit, 2003; Nelson, Moulson, & Richmond, 2006). Bakker et al. (2015) demonstrated larger P400 amplitudes for power grasps directed towards (congruent trials) than away (incongruent trials) from objects, but only for infants that were able to reach

4 4 proficiently using power grasps (not present in 4-months-olds but observable in 5- and 6- month-olds that have developed good grasping skills). No effect was observed for infants that observed precision grasps, an action that infants are unable to perform at these ages. These results suggest that a close correspondence between the observed action and the action repertoire of the infant is needed in order to promote the perception of goal-directed actions. The same P400 ERP component has previously been shown to be sensitive to gaze direction (Senju, Johnson & Csibra, 2008), pointing hands (Gredebäck, et al., 2010; Melinder, Konijnenberg, Hermansen, Daum, & Gredebäck, 2015), give-me gestures (Bakker, Kaduk, Elsner, Juvrud, & Gredebäck, 2015), and goal-directed pro-social agents (Gredebäck, et al., 2015) in infants. This ERP component is believed to originate in the Superior Temporal Sulcus (STS; Gredebäck & Daum, 2015), an area involved in the perception of biological motion and goal-directed actions (Keysers & Perrett, 2004) such as reaching and looking (Allison, Puce, & McCarthy, 2000). It is often claimed that infants own motor development is a driving force in the development of social perception - that we learn to understand others through an embodied process grounded in action (Gredebäck & Falck-Ytter, 2015; Marshall & Meltzoff, 2011; Uithol & Paulus, 2014). However, the above-mentioned studies suffer from one basic confound that of visual experience. Because we visually attend to our own actions (Flanagan & Johansson, 2003; Rosander & von Hofsten, 2011) a large amount of observational experience comes along naturally with enhanced motor skills. As such, correlational studies cannot isolate the involvement of a participant s own motor proficiency from prolonged visual experience observing the same actions (for adult studies that have demonstrated a causal connection between motor cortex activity and social perception see Cardellicchio, Sinigaglia, & Costantini, 2013; Elsner, D Ausilio, Gredebäck, Falck-Ytter, & Fadiga, 2013; there are also examples of facilitated social cognition following motor learning

5 5 in the absence of visual experience, Casile & Giese, 2006). In order to overcome the motor and visual learning confound in infancy, Sommerville, Woodward and Needham (2005) relied on a behavioral paradigm in which active action experience and passive observational experience were separated. Sommerville and colleagues were able to induce grasping experience in 3-month-old infants that typically are unable to reach in a goal directed and functional manner (von Hofsten, 2004). Through the use of Velcro-covered mittens infants were provided with the opportunity to grasp and lift objects. After a short grasping training with mittens, a habituation paradigm was used to demonstrate that active training facilitates goal processing during action observation. In contrast, 3-month-old infants that were habituated to actions by passively observing others perform them did not demonstrate the same effect. In other words, they were unable to process other peoples actions in terms of goals, even though their visual experience with reaching was similar to those infants who were able to perform the actions in tandem. The same paradigm has been replicated (Gerson & Woodward, 2014) and used to demonstrate that active training facilitates one s own action exploration (Needham, Barrett, & Peterman, 2002; Libertus & Needham, 2010), face perception (Libertus & Needham, 2011) and the efficiency or rationality of others actions (Skerry, Carey, & Spelke, 2013). As in the original study, none of these effects were observed following passive observational training. The current study aims to map the neural correlates of action perception following active training with sticky mittens and compare this to a control condition in which infants are given only visual exposure to similar actions (similar control conditions have previously been used by Gerson & Woodward, 2014; Libertus & Needham, 2010; 2011; Needham, et al., 2002; Skerry et al., 2013; Sommerville, et al., 2005). We hypothesize that the same ERP component demonstrated to index social perception at the onset of functional

6 6 grasping (P400; Bakker, et al., 2015) will differentiate congruent from incongruent grasping actions in pre-reaching infants following training. This effect should only be observable following active training (Experiment 1) and not present following an equivalent amount of visual exposure to goal-directed grasping actions (Experiment 2). This effect should remain when comparing the two experiments directly (larger difference in P400 amplitudes in Experiment 1 than 2). We targeted 4-month-olds because infants of this age do not typically (in the absence of sticky mittens, active training) differentiate goal-directed from non-goal directed actions in this paradigm (Bakker et al., 2015). A result consistent with this hypothesis would demonstrate, for the first time, a neural correlate of action perception that is driven by active motor experience (when controlling for visual experience), demonstrating a causal connection between cortical activity, motor experience, and social perception early in life. It is likely the case that training in general alters brain activity not specifically related to the social domain. In order to assess this we also analyze the ERP components Nc, known to index attention early in life (Richards, 2003), and the P100 (Csibra, et al., 2008) targeting early perceptual processes. We do not formulate any specific hypothesis with respect to these components in the individual experiments, but suggest that any such effect should not be present when comparing the two experiments. In other words, there might be general training effects on both early visual and attentional components but these should not remain when directly comparing active and passive training. EXPERIMENT 1 Participants Fifteen 4-month-old infants (9 girls, mean age 129 days, SD = 5 days) were included in the final sample. Fourteen additional infants were tested but excluded from the final

7 7 analysis, as they did not obtain a sufficient number of artifact-free trials (n < 10 trials/condition) due to excessive movements or fussiness (reviews of infant ERP studies have noted that a dropout of 50% is standard; Stets, Stahl, & Reid, 2012). Before the experimental session, parents signed a consent form and were given a gift certificate (value approximately 10). The study was approved by the local ethics committee and conducted in accordance with the standards specified in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Methods Procedure and stimuli. Infants participated in two experimental tasks. The experiment began with a behavioral task, here referred as the active experience task. Subsequently, we used EEG to record infants neural responses to sequences of pictures displaying hands and objects. The total duration of the experiment was approximately 30 minutes. Active experience task. Infants were seated on their caregiver s lap, in front of a table, supported at their torso by their parents. A pair of mittens was put on infants hands. The mittens were custom made from ecru colored fabric, modeled from those used in Needham, et al. (2002) and Sommerville et al (2005). The dorsal side of the mittens was transparent allowing infants to see their fingers, while the palmar side was equipped with Velcro covering infant s entire palm. In order to encourage grasping, infants were presented with 9 colorful balls (3 x 3 cm) placed one at a time at a reachable distance from an infant, approximately 10 cm from the infants torso. The balls were covered with the corresponding side Velcro so when in contact they easily adhered to the mittens. In this way, each infant was given an opportunity to lift the ball after making contact with it. In total infants were given 240 seconds to interact with the objects. During this time infants had several

8 8 opportunities to interact with the objects, that is to touch them, manipulate, and lift them up. When infant lifted a ball, he or she was given approximately 5 seconds to explore the object after which the ball was removed from the mitten and placed again in front of the child. A new object was presented as soon as the child showed no interest in lifting up the previous one. During the training, the experimenter interacted with the infant, by smiling, talking, and praising the child when he or she acted upon the object and when he or she lifted the object. After 240 seconds the experimenter took away the objects and removed the mittens from the infant s hands. The training sessions were video recorded and coded for the number of goaldirected action performed by the infant. In this case, a goal-directed action was coded if infant s action resulted in contact with the object, the object was subsequently lifted from the table, and if the action was preceded by a visual fixation on the object. The time interval between gaze at the object and the grasping action could not exceed 3 seconds for it to be counted as a goal-directed action. On average, each infant performed 9.7 goal directed actions (range: 5-16, SD = 3) during the training session. EEG task. The stimuli were presented on a 17-inch computer screen that was rotated 90 degrees (so the vertical extension of the screen was larger than its horizontal counterpart) at a distance of 60 cm from the observing infant. Infants observed sequences of still images of colorful balls appearing on the upper and lower parts of the screen (to avoid lateralization effects) followed by static images of human hands shaped in a whole hand grasp. The hand was directed either toward (congruent condition) or away from the previously presented ball (incongruent condition). The hand did not move towards the object but remained still. Each infant watched both the congruent and incongruent trials presented in randomized order. All trials began with a 100 ms presentation of two rectangles (6 horizontal 5 vertical degrees) displayed at the upper and lower part of the computer screen, 13 vertical degrees apart. This

9 9 was followed by a fixation cross (duration random within the range of ms) at the center of the screen followed by a ball presented (for 240 ms) inside an upper or lower rectangle. The whole trial sequence ended with a picture of a grasping hand performing a power grasp (duration of 1000 ms) without the object being present (Figure 1). Stimuli were presented using the software E-Prime 2.0, E-Studio (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA). These stimuli were repeated until infants demonstrated signs of fussiness. In these instances a distractor image was displayed on the screen along with an attention grabbing sound until infants reoriented their attention to the screen, at which time the stimulus presentation commenced once more. During the EEG recording we kept possible distractions to a minimum. Light conditions in the lab were kept low and a curtain separated the experimenter and EEG equipment from the infant. The experimenter monitored infant s gaze direction through a camera. As soon as an infant got distracted and no longer looked at the stimuli, the experimenter used attention-grabbing pictures with a sound to orient infants attention back to stimuli. The experiment was terminated when an infant was no longer interested in the stimulus. EEG recording and analysis. We used a 128-channel HydroCel Geodesics Sensor Net to record infants EEGs and EOG. The signal was sampled at 250 Hz, vertex-referenced, amplified (EGI Net Amps 300 amplifier, Electric Geodesics, Eugene, OR) and low-pass filtered at 100 Hz. The EEG signal was post-processed using a digital filter ( Hz) and segmented from 550 ms prior to the appearance of the hand (including the last 160 ms of the empty rectangles with the fixation cross and the 240 ms long presentation of the object) until 900 ms after the hand was presented. The electrodes from the most anterior and posterior areas were not included in the final analysis because of high noise caused by poor contact

10 10 with the scalp (which is common in infant ERP studies; see Gredebäck, Melinder, & Daum, 2010, Gredebäck, et al., 2015). In total, 38 electrodes were excluded from the final analyses. Furthermore, the data was inspected manually, and those electrodes contaminated with artifacts were rejected (a standard procedure in infant ERP work; see Hoehl & Wahl, 2012). We used frontal electrodes in order to detect eye movement and eye blinks. The data from the missing channels were interpolated from the surrounding electrodes. The trials included in the final data contained no more than 10% of the artifact-contaminated electrodes. The data were baseline corrected with a 350 ms baseline starting from the onset of the analysis interval. This slightly longer than usual baseline (350 compared to 200 ms in Gredebäck et al., 2010) was chosen in order to obtain more stable ERPs, taking in consideration the very young age of our participants. As this baseline overlaps with the presentation of the object (not informative of condition) this might result in an enhanced negativity over visual areas in the final ERP waveform. This should however be the same for all conditions and experiments in this study and cannot account for any of the effects reported below. All remaining trials were merged to individual averages, separated by condition. Individual averages that contained at least 10 artifact free trials were used to create the grand average. Analyses were performed on six channel areas lower occipital (electrode number 74, 75 [Oz], 82), left posterior temporal (electrode number 65, 66, 67, 69, 70 [01], 71), right posterior temporal (electrode number 76, 77, 83 [02], 84, 89, 90), left central (electrode number 47, 42, 37, 31, 51, 52, 53 [P3], 54), and right central (electrode number 87, 93, 98, 79, 80, 86, 92 [P4], 97) - with a focus on a time interval ranging from ms (labeled P100), 300 ms 400 ms (labeled N290) and 400 ms ms (labeled P400) after the onset of the stimulus. Additionally the analysis on the frontal area (electrode number 5, 6, 7, 12, 13, 20, 29, 30, 36, 104, 105 [C4], 106, 111, 112, 118) was performed on time window ms (labeled Nc) after the onset of the stimulus. The choice of the electrodes for P100 and P400 components

11 11 was based on a similar procedure used in the study by Bakker et al., 2015 and visual inspection of the data for the Nc and N290 component. The average amplitudes within the time window defined above for the P100, N290 and the P400 components for posterior temporal and central areas were statistically compared in a 2 2 repeated measures ANOVA, with condition (congruent, incongruent) and lateralization (left, right) as within-subject factors. The average amplitude within the time window used to assess the lower occipital P100, P400 and the frontal Nc were compared using a repeated measure t-test with averaged amplitude over channels as the dependent variable and condition as the independent variable. In order to check whether the results would replicate the same findings when the time window was matched to previously published work using the same paradigm (Bakker et al. 2015), we performed additional analysis between 300 ms and 600 ms for the component labeled P400. This time window was targeted in the posterior temporal and occipital areas. Figure 1. Left section denoted the type of training used in Experiment 1 and 2. In both instances infants were presented with the same stimulus set during EEG recording,

12 12 demonstrated on the right section of the figure. Numbers mark the duration of each image in the stimulus sequence (ms). Results and discussion The average number of presented trials for both conditions was 85. On average, infants provided 35 artifact-free trials: 16 (range: 10-33) for the congruent and 18 (range: 10-29) for the incongruent condition. The analysis revealed a significant difference in the P400 amplitudes for congruent and incongruent trials in the posterior temporal and lower occipital areas (Figure 2). More specifically, for the posterior temporal area a main effect of congruency was observed with higher amplitudes in the congruent (4.5 µv) than in the incongruent (0.45 µv) condition, F(1,14) = , p =.006, η 2 = There was no main effect of hemisphere, F(1,14) =.056, p =.817, nor was there an interaction effect between condition and hemisphere, F(1,14) =.784, p =.391. ERP amplitudes for the lower central occipital P400 demonstrate higher amplitudes for congruent (2.6 µv) than incongruent trials (- 2.6 µv), t(14) = 3.68, p =.002, d = Additionally the analysis for P400 component within left and right central area were performed but did not show any significant difference between the congruent (-1.8 µv) and incongruent (-0.4 µv) trials, t(14) = 1.835, p =.09. The analysis within the posterior temporal area for N290 ERP component did not reveal a main effect of congruency F(1,14) = 2.215, p =.159 nor did it reveal a main effect of hemisphere, F(1,14) =.915, p =.355, or an interaction between congruency and hemisphere, F(1,14) =.185, p =.674. No significant difference for N290 within the lower occipital area was observed t(14) = 1.491, p =.158.

13 13 Similar to the previous component, the analysis within the posterior temporal area for P100 ERP component did not reveal a main effect of congruency F(1,14) = 3.004, p =.103 or a main effect of hemisphere, F(1,14) = 1.009, p =.332, or interaction between congruency and hemisphere, F(1,14) =.012, p =.916. No significant difference for P100 within the lower occipital area was observed t(14) = 1.110, p =.286. The results demonstrate differential neural activity within the Nc component (Figure 3) located in the frontal area with more negatively pronounced congruent trials (-3 µv) than incongruent trials (1.34 µv), t(14) = , p =.002, d = The additional analysis on the P400 component within the time window previously used by Bakker et al. (2015; ms) replicated the findings from the time chosen ( ms) based on the visual inspection of the data. The analysis revealed a significant difference in the P400 amplitudes for congruent (2.5 µv in the posterior temporal area and 2.6 µv in the lower occipital) and incongruent ( µv in the posterior temporal area and µv in the lower occipital) trials in the posterior temporal (F(1,14) = 9.133, p =.009, η 2 = 0.61) and lower occipital areas t(14) = 3.41, p =.004, d = There was no main effect of hemisphere (F(1,14) =.007, p =.934, nor was there an interaction between condition and hemisphere, F(1,14) = 1.009, p =.332 in posterior temporal region. These results demonstrate that active training with goal-directed actions in pre-reaching infants results in changes at the neural level that allow coding of observed goal-directed actions. More specifically, experience grasping and lifting objects elicited substantial differences between congruent and incongruent manual actions in both posterior temporal, occipital and frontal sites. The differential ERP responses include both the hypothesized P400 known to be involved in the processing of goal-directed actions (Bakker, et al., 2015; Gredebäck, et al., 2010; Melinder et al., 2015) and ERP component Nc known to index selective attention (Richards, 2003). No differences were observed for ERP component P100

14 14 or for P400 over central cites. Given Experiment 1 alone it is difficult to assess the nature of this training effect. Experiment 2 provides another form of training in which infants observe an experimenter perform the same reaching and grasping actions, followed by ERP measures of the P100, the P400 and the Nc during observation of goal-directed grasping actions (identical to that used in Experiment 1). If active training with sticky mittens is needed for action perception to emerge than no differentiation between goal-directed (congruent) and non-goal directed (incongruent) trials are expected following passive observation. Figure 2. Illustrate P400 ERP waveforms for the two experiments (Active training; Experiment 1 and Passive observation; Experiment 2) separated for occipital and posterior

15 15 temporal cites (channels marked with black included in the analysis). Solid black lines represent the congruent condition and dotted grey lines represent incongruent condition. Figure 3. Illustrate Nc ERP waveforms for the two experiments (Active training; Experiment 1 and Passive observation; Experiment 2) (channels marked with black included in the analysis). Solid black lines represent the congruent condition and dotted grey lines represent incongruent condition. Figure 4. Topographical map of the wave forms (active training group) at 600 ms after stimulus onset. Congruent condition is positioned on the left and incongruent on the right. EXPERIMENT 2

16 16 In the Experiment 2 infants were presented with the same stimulus material as in Experiment 1. The only difference between the two experiments was that the active training in Experiment 1 was replaced by passive observation of goal-directed actions performed by the experimenter. The time and the number of goal-directed actions that infants observed were kept constant across experiments. Participants Thirty 4-month-old infants were included in the study. The final sample consisted of 15 infants (7 girls, mean age 126 days, SD = 7 days). Additional, fifteen infants were not included in the final analysis due to an insufficient number of artifact-free trials (n < 10). Similar to Experiment 1, before participation in the experiment, the parents were informed about the procedure, purpose of the study and signed a consent form. They received a voucher with an approximate value of 10. Methods Procedure and stimuli. Similar to Experiment 1, infants performed two tasks: first they took part in passive observation training followed by the EEG recording. This design allowed us to compare the neural activity of infants attending to the same ERP stimuli after different training opportunities. All aspects of the study were identical to Experiment 1 with one exception: during training infants observed goal-directed reaching actions that mimicked the average active object engagement from Experiment 1. Each infant attended to 10 goaldirected actions (compared to 9.7 in Experiment 1). The timing of the events matched the 240 seconds of active training. All the grasping events were performed in the similar way; the experimenter looked at the child and if necessary brought child s attention to the action by initiating a contact by speaking directly to a child (greeting or calling by the name). The

17 17 experimenter extended the hand and lifted the object up. While holding the object, the experimenter praised her own successful reach in the same way as during the active training e.g. the experimenter said: well done, good job etc. Consequently, the experimenter put away the object on the table. In the event that the infants did not show interest in the used object, the experimenter exchanged it for a new one and started from the beginning. If the infant did not attend to a grasping event the action was repeated. Results and discussion On average, infants observed 87 trials in total and provided an average of 26 artifactfree trials: 12 (range: 10-27) for the congruent and 14 (range: 10-22) for the incongruent condition. The analysis were performed on P100, N290, P400, and Nc components and revealed no significant differences between conditions or lateralization. The analysis over posterior temporal area for P400 component (mean amplitude 0.23 µv for congruent and 1.12 µv for incongruent condition) did not reveal a main effect of congruency (F(1,14) =.284, p =.602), a main effect of hemisphere F(1,14) = 2.394, p =.146, nor an interaction between condition and hemisphere F(1,14) =.299, p =.593. The analysis for the P400 component over the lower occipital area (mean amplitude for the congruent trials 3.15 µv and 1.10 µv for the incongruent trails) did not reveal a significant difference between the conditions (t(14) =.836, p =.417). Additional analysis for P400 component over central (left, right) area did not indicate significant difference between the congruent (-1.73 µv) and incongruent trials (-1.46 µv), t(14) =.124, p =.730. Analysis of the P100 component over the posterior temporal area did not reveal a main effect of congruency (mean amplitude -5.9 µv for congruent and -4.5 µv for incongruent condition), F(1,14) = 1.304, p =.273. The analysis did not show a main effect of hemisphere F(1,14) =.666, p =.428, nor an interaction between condition and hemisphere F(1,14) =

18 18.000, p =.991. Finally, the t-test analysis for lower occipital area for P100 did not reveal significantly different amplitudes between congruent (-6.1 µv) and incongruent trials (- 5.7 µv), (t(14) = , p =.309). The analysis of the Nc component did not demonstrate significant differences in neural activity between congruent (- 0.2 µv) and incongruent trials (- 0.6 µv), t(14) =.355, p =.309. The results from Experiment 2 demonstrate that the effects observed in Experiment 1 was related to active experience and that pure visual exposure to similar actions is not sufficient to elicit the same effect. In general, these findings support the notion that active sticky mittens training enhance social networks dedicated to the processing of goal-directed actions. In order to further isolate the ERP components that make a strong contribution to the sticky mittens effect direct comparisons between ERP components from Experiment 1 and 2 were performed. The analysis over posterior temporal area for P400 component when using the time window between 300 and 600 ms after stimulus onset (mean amplitude -0.6 µv for congruent and 0.4 µv for incongruent condition) did not reveal a main effect of congruency (F(1,14) =.452, p =.512), a main effect of hemisphere F(1,14) = 2.718, p =.121, nor an interaction between condition and hemisphere F(1,14) = 2.483, p =.499. The analysis for the P400 component over the lower occipital area (mean amplitude for the congruent trials 1.4 µv and 0.3 µv for the incongruent trails), did not reveal a significant difference between the conditions (t(14) =.626, p =.541). Additional analysis for P400 component over central (left, right) area did not indicate significant differences between the congruent (-1.4 µv) and incongruent trials (-2.4 µv), t(14) =.-.069, p =.946. STATISTICAL COMPARISON OF EXPERIMENT 1 AND 2

19 19 In order to examine the effect of the training across experiments (active training in Experiment 1 and passive training in Experiment 2) we performed additional analysis on the difference score between conditions in the P400 component located over the posterior temporal area. The analysis revealed a significant difference as a function of the experience that infants acquired prior to EEG testing, demonstrating a larger relative P400 amplitude for the active training (3.9 µv) than passive training condition (-1.1 µv), F(1,28) = 6.381, p =.017, η 2 = These findings demonstrate that active but not the passive training influenced neural responses to congruent and incongruent grasping. We performed additional analyses on the difference score between conditions for two components that were found significant after active training experience. The analysis for P400 component over the lower occipital area did not reveal significantly different neural activity when comparing active and passive training F(1,28) = 2.979, p =.095. We did not find significant difference between active and passive training when comparing difference score between conditions within Nc component F(1,28) = 0.80, p =.780. No correlation between the number of goal directed actions during active training and the amplitude of the P400 ERP component was observed (b =.166, t(14) = 526, p =.608). Additional analysis was performed on the number of trials presented in each condition and experiment. The rationale for this analysis is to assess potential differences in general attention and motivation to participate in the EEG session following active and passive training. As the experiment continues until infants become too fussy to provide additional artifact-free trials, the number of presented trials directly relate to how long infants were attending to the stimuli. An ANOVA with condition and type of experience as factors did not demonstrate any significant differences in the attention between conditions F(1,28) =.771, p =.395 or studies, F(1,28) =.235, p =.635, for descriptive statistics see each individual experiment. The analysis within the posterior temporal area for P100 ERP component did not

20 20 reveal a main effect of congruency F(1,14) = 3.004, p =.103 neither main effect of hemisphere, F(1,14) = 1.009, p =.332, nor interaction effect between congruency and hemisphere, F(1,14) =.012, p =.916. The direct comparison between the two experiments demonstrates that infants were equally attentive in both experiments and that the posterior temporal P400 has a central role as a key marker of active action experience following training. This effect can be elicited in 4-month-old infants, one to two months before a similar neural correlate of active action experience emerges alongside the typical development of goal-directed reaching. The fact that observed goal-directed actions produce an enhanced P400 relative to observed non goaldirected action, but only in infants that receive active sticky mittens training, suggests that infants, through sticky mittens training, learn to encode the goal of other peoples actions. GENERAL DISCUSSION The aim of the current study was three-fold. First, we aimed to contribute to a better understanding of how infants become proficient in encoding other people s goal-directed actions and the neural mechanisms involved in this process. Second, we aimed to examine which type of experience (active versus passive) more strongly influences processing of others action at a neural level. Finally, given that our design to training infants to perform an action that they are not able to perform in the absence of training, we aimed to look at the causal connection between action production and action perception. Our findings demonstrate that brief experience producing goal-directed grasping actions, at an age when functional grasping has not yet emerged, enhances the neural circuitry dedicated to processing goal-directed actions performed by others. The neural correlate that indexes the training induced action perception was the P400 ERP component. This neural signature has previously been associated with the processing of goal-directed

21 21 actions in humans (Senju, et al., 2008; Melinder, et al., 2015; Gredebäck, et al., 2010; Bakker, et al., 2015) and animate agents (Gredebäck et al., 2015). In the light of these prior papers we believe that infants, through active training, begin to process manual actions as goal directed and come to expect hands to be directed towards objects in the environment. We have previously argued (Bakker et al., 2015) that seeing an object appearing either at the top or the bottom of the screen heightens attention to this location. When a grasping hand later appears in the middle of the screen, infants fixate this location and covert attention is distributed along the reaching axis. If the axis of attention overlaps with the previously cued location then the amplitude of the P400 is enlarged and the action is perceived as goal directed. In essence, the effect observed here with respect to P400 is thought to represent a priming effect similar to what is demonstrated in infants and adults using eye tracking (Daum & Gredebäck, 2011; Daum, Ulber, & Gredebäck, 2013) and response time measures (Driver et al., 1999; Friesen & Kingstone, 1998). Only four minutes of active reaching and grasping training, with an average of 10 object-directed actions was sufficient to elicit priming and social perception networks typically developing 1-2 months later in life (Bakker et al., 2015). The speed of this neural reorganization or enhancement is quite remarkable, particularly given the young age of these infants. Though lacking in spatial resolution it has been argued that the ERP component P400 originates from the STS, an area know to be involved in social perception and the processing of goal-directed actions (Allison, Puce, & McCarthy, 2000; Keysers & Parrett, 2004). Both fmri studies of STS activation in adults and ERP studies of P400 amplitudes in infants demonstrate similar signatures. First of all, both are sensitive to faces (for STS see Yang, Rosenblau, Keifer, & Pelphrey, 2015; for P400 see de Haan, Pascalis, & Johnson, 2002; Leppänen, Moulson, Vogel-Farley, & Nelson, 2007), the cued direction of gaze (for STS see Perrett et al., 1985; for P400 see Senju et al., 2008), the goal directedness of human

22 22 hands (for STS see Bahnemann, Dziobek, Perhn, Wolf, & Heekeren, 2010; for P400 see Bakker, et al., 2015; Gredebäck et al., 2010; Melinder, et al., 2015), and point-light walkers (for STS see Grossman, et al., 2000; P400, Reid, Hoehl, Landt, & Striano, 2008). Secondly, the infant P400 is closely related to the adult ERP component N170 (de Haan, et al., 2002; Nelson et al., 2006) documented in source localization and joint fmri/eeg studies to originate from the STS (Puce, Allison, Bentin, Gore, & McCarthy, 1998; Itier & Taylor, 2004; Dalrymple et al., 2011). Our results demonstrate for the first time that P400 is engaged in the establishment of the action production action perception link, and suggest that the STS is the source of this activation. We believe that the role of the STS area is to detect the goal-directed actions from visual input. Relating to theoretical model of action perception timeline proposed by Gredebäck and Daum (2015), the STS would be responsible for the very first step of action processing, that is, the initial modulation of attention that occurs within the first approx. 500 ms from the moment of detection of the agent or cue. In the current study, this first step of processing would be the summation of attentional shifts from the object and the congruent hand, indexed by P400 component. Additionally, we argue that the brief training provided by the sticky mittens, although helpful in enhancing the detection of incoming information and drawing infants attention in the direction of the object, is not sufficient to build up a strong motor representation of the observed action. We believe that for more advanced processing (involving for example the ability to predict the goal of others actions) infants need strong and stable motor plans that are built up during regular motor development (Gredebäck & Melinder, 2010; Gredebäck & Falck-Ytter, 2015) associated with massive training (Adolphs et al., 2012).

23 23 The effects of active motor experience were also visible in more frontal ERP components, in this case the Nc, known to index selective attention (Richards, 2003). This component is known to be sensitive to surprising events. In our case the increased negativity of Nc is more strongly elicited for congruent actions. It is possible that infants may see congruent actions as more interesting as these trials signal that the action will be accomplished. Studies that have presented similar stimuli to infants (e.g., hands pointing, grasping, gaze, or goal directed non-human animate agents; see introduction), do not report Nc differences. One suggestion is that this effect is directly related to active training and as such is less visible in other studies that have assessed P400 amplitudes at the onset of functional grasping and pointing later in life (Bakker, et al., 2015; Gredebäck, et al., 2010; Melinder, et al., 2015). Interpretations of the Nc effects should however be made with caution as no differential effect (comparing Nc between the two experiments) could be observed. Based on this, another suggestion is likely: that the temporal-parietal P400 and the centro-frontal Nc creates a functional network that arises from active training with one s own reaching, grasping and object manipulation, involved in the encoding of goal-directed manual actions (P400) and subsequent modulation of attention (Nc). Considering that prior studies that examined older infants, did not report Nc effects, it is possible that the attentional component becomes less prominent later in life, once the motor plan needed to perform functional goal-directed actions develops. It is clear that more studies need to target the Nc component with respect to observation of goal-directed actions in order to validate our alternative explanations. The involvement of P400 is however more clearly expressed, both with its presence in Experiment 1, absence in Experiment 2 and through direct comparisons between two experiments. We further suggest that this P400 effect is maintained throughout the first year of life and that in adults it is expressed as an N , a transition similar to that previously

24 24 documented for observation of congruent and incongruent pointing (Gredebäck, et al., 2010; Melinder et al., 2015). As demonstrated in the statistical comparison between Experiments 1 and 2 the training effect is only found following active training, in which infants obtain firsthand experience grasping for and manipulating objects. Passive observation training of others actions does not generate the same, or any, enhancement in neural activity in ERP component P400. This finding speaks for the unique role of active experience in shaping our ability to make sense of other people and the world at large. One important caveat of our study is necessary to mention. Our experiments are designed to compare active and passive experience as they naturalistically occur in the real world. Specifically, during active training infants experience a first-person visual perspective on their own actions, whereas during passive training infants adopt a third-party perspective on others actions. It is possible that first-person observational experience would be more influential in activating the networks responsible for action understanding. An alternate control for the active experience condition would be to present infants with videos with of another infants first-person reaching experience. However because of the pragmatic challenges of using such an approach with very young, and given the confounds inherent in comparing video and live experiences, this approach was not our first choice. Although, our data support the supremacy of active training over passive training in enhancing processing of goal-directed actions, we acknowledge that both types of experiences (active and passive) contribute to social perception processes in infants everyday lives. The literature provides evidence that visual learning facilitates encoding of other people s goals that cannot be rejected (Biro, Verschoor, & Coenen, 2011; Henrichs, Elsner, Elsner, Wilkinson, Gredeback, 2014; Green, Li, Lockman, Gredebäck, in press). However, our data shows that active experience generates a stronger signal and more in-depth processing in social perception networks, creating a foundation for social learning very early

25 25 in life. We believe that the first-person experience is uniquely powerful as it gives simultaneous visual and motor experience. Thus, it is possible that actions that are only visually accessible may be gained with some time lag. This speculation requires more investigation and may be a subject for the further studies. Together, our findings suggest that brief active reaching and grasping experience provides a causal facilitation of social networks and attentional priming with respect to goaldirected manual actions. We learn about others through active engagement with the world, through an embodied process that uses available first-hand experience reaching and grasping for objects to perceive, encode, and interpret second-hand information obtained whilst observing others reach and grasp for objects. This active training enhances a social network involving P400, assumed to originate in STS and to be responsible for the detection and processing of goal-directed actions via priming. This study provides the first causal connection between neural networks dedicated to social processing and active action experience during the first 6 months of life. Acknowledgements We are grateful to all the parents and their children participating in this study. Additionally, we would like to thank Kahl Hellmer and Tori Wesevich for their help in participants recruitment, as well as Malin Karstens for her excellent assistance with the data collection. This work was financed by an ERC StG grant (CACTUS ) and a Wallenberg Academy Fellow grant from Knut and Alice Wallenberg foundation (KWA ).

26 26 REFERENCES Adolph, K. E., Cole, W. G., Komati, M., Garciaguirre, J. S., Badaly, D., Lingeman, J. M.,... & Sotsky, R. B. (2012). How do you learn to walk? Thousands of steps and dozens of falls per day. Psychological science, 23(11), Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4(7), doi: /s (00) Bakker, M., Daum, M. M., Handl, A., & Gredebäck, G. (in press). Neural correalates of action perception at the onset of functional grasping. Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, nsu 119 Bakker, M., Kaduk, K., Elsner, C., Juvrud, J., & Gustaf, G. (2015). The neural basis of nonverbal communication-enhanced processing of perceived give-me gestures in 9- month-old girls. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 59. Bahnemann, M., Dziobek, I., Prehn, K., Wolf, I., & Heekeren, H. R. (2010). Sociotopy in the temporoparietal cortex: common versus distinct processes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(1), Biro, S., Verschoor, S., & Coenen, L. (2011). Evidence for a unitary goal concept in 12- month-old infnats. Developmental Science, 14, doi: /j x Brune, C. W., & Woodward, A. L. (2007). Social cognition and social responsiveness in 10- month-old infants. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8(2),

27 27 Cannon, E. N., Simpson, E. A., Fox, N. A., Vanderwert, R. E., Woodward, A. L., & Ferrari, P. F. (in press). Relations between infants' emerging reach-grasp competence and event-related desyncronization in EEG. Developmental Science. Cardellicchio, P., Sinigaglia, C., & Costantini, M. (2013). Grasping affordances with the other's hand: A TMS study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(4), Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16(1), doi: /j.cub Csibra, G., Kushnerenko, E., & Grossman, T. (2008). Electrophysiological methods in studying infant cognitive development. In C. A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience (pp ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Daum, M. M., & Gredeback, G. (2011). The development of grasping comprehension in infancy: covert shifts of attention caused by referential actions. Experimental Brain Research, 208(2), Daum, M. M., Ulber, J., & Gredebäck, G. (2013). The Development of Pointing Perception in Infancy: Effects of Communicative Signals on Covert Shifts of Attention. Developmental Psychology, 49(10), Dalrymple, K. A., Oruc, I., Duchaine, B., Pancaroglu, R., Fox, C. J., Iaria, G.,... Barton, J. J. S. (2011). The anatomic basis of the right face-selective N170 IN acquired prosopagnosia: A combined ERP/fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual cognition, 6(5), de Haan, M., Johnson, M. H., & Halit, J. (2003). Development of face-sensitive event-related

28 28 potentials during infancy: a review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 51, de Haan, M., Pascalis, O., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Specialization of neural mechanisms underlying face recognition in human infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(2), Elsner, C., D'Ausilio, A., Gredeback, G., Falck-Ytter, T., & Fadiga, L. (2013). The motor cortex is causally related to predictive eye movements during action observation. Neuropsychologia, 51(3), Flanagan, J. R., & Johansson, R. S. (2003). Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 14(424), Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orientating is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), Gerson, S. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2014). Learning from their own actions: the unique effect of producing actions on infants' action understanding. Child Development, 85(1), Gredebäck, G., & Daum, M. (in press). The Microstructure of Action Perception in Infancy: Decomposing the Temporal Structure of Social Information Processing. Child Development Perspectives. Gredebäck, G., & Falck-Ytter, T. (2015). Eye movements during action observation. Perspectives on Psychological Science. Gredebäck, G., Stasiewicz, D., Falck-Ytter, T., Rosander, K., & von Hofsten, C. (2009). Hand configuration and goal type modulate goal directed gaze shifts in 14-monthold infants. Developmental Psychology, 45, Gredebäck, G., Kaduk, K., Bakker, M., Gottwald, J., Ekberg, T., Elsner, C., Reid, V., & Kenward, B. (2015). The neuropsychology of infants pro-social preferences.

29 29 Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, Gredebäck, G., Melinder, A., & Daum, M. M. (2010). The development and neural basis of pointing comprehension. Social Neuroscience, 5(5-6), Green, D., Li, Q., Lockman, J., & Gredebäck, G. (2014). Culture influences action understanding in infancy: A comparative study of action prediction in Chinese and Swedish infants. Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), Henrichs, I., Elsner, C., Elsner, B., Wilkinson, N., & Gredeback, G. (2014). Goal certainty modulates infants' goal-directed gaze shifts. Developmental Psychology, 50, doi: /a Hoehl, S., & Wahl, S. (2012). Recording infant ERP data for cognitive research. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37(3), Itier, R. J., & Taylor, M. J. (2004). Source analysis of the N170 to faces and objects. Neuroreport, 15(8), Kanakogi, Y., & Itakura, S. (2011). Developmental correspondence between action prediction and motor ability in early infancy. Nature Communications, 2, 341. Keysers, C., & Perrett, D. I. (2004). Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), doi: / Kochukhova, O., & Gredebäck, G. (2010). Preverbal Infants Anticipate That Food Will Be Brought to the Mouth: An Eye Tracking Study of Manual Feeding and Flying Spoons. Child Development, 81(6),

Reflexive Spatial Attention to Goal-Directed Reaching

Reflexive Spatial Attention to Goal-Directed Reaching Reflexive Spatial Attention to Goal-Directed Reaching Alexis A. Barton (aabarton@indiana.edu) Bennett I. Bertenthal (bbertent@indiana.edu) Samuel Harding (hardinsm@indiana.edu) Department of Psychological

More information

The development and neural basis of referential gaze perception

The development and neural basis of referential gaze perception SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE, 2006, 1 (34), 220234 The development and neural basis of referential gaze perception Atsushi Senju, Mark H. Johnson, and Gergely Csibra Birkbeck College, University of London, London,

More information

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze Psychology, 2014, 5, 451-460 Published Online April 2014 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.55055 Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift

More information

The Neural Basis of Somatosensory Remapping Develops in Human Infancy

The Neural Basis of Somatosensory Remapping Develops in Human Infancy Current Biology, Volume 24 Supplemental Information The Neural Basis of Somatosensory Remapping Develops in Human Infancy Silvia Rigato, Jannath Begum Ali, José van Velzen, and Andrew J. Bremner Supplemental

More information

Development of goal-directed gaze shift based on predictive learning

Development of goal-directed gaze shift based on predictive learning 4th International Conference on Development and Learning and on Epigenetic Robotics October 13-16, 2014. Palazzo Ducale, Genoa, Italy WePP.1 Development of goal-directed gaze shift based on predictive

More information

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus) Introduction 17 subjects were tested to measure effects of a Brain.fm focus session on cognition. With 4 additional subjects, we recorded EEG data during baseline and while

More information

Developmental Psychology Event-related potentials discriminate familiar and unusual goal outcomes in 5-montholds

Developmental Psychology Event-related potentials discriminate familiar and unusual goal outcomes in 5-montholds Developmental Psychology Event-related potentials discriminate familiar and unusual goal outcomes in 5-montholds and adults --Manuscript Draft-- Manuscript Number: Full Title: Abstract: Article Type: Keywords:

More information

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential Title of Thesis Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential 2014 September Yang Weiping The Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology (Doctor s Course)

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 827 831. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research BRIEF REPT Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the

More information

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Submitted to Current Biology Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Anna Plodowski, Rachel Swainson, Georgina M. Jackson, Chris Rorden and Stephen R. Jackson School of Psychology

More information

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln 1 DATA MANAGEMENT Backups Storage Identification Analyses 2 Data Analysis Pre-processing Statistical Analysis

More information

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG Outline of Talk Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials Shafali Spurling Jeste Assistant Professor in Psychiatry and Neurology UCLA Center for Autism Research and Treatment Basic definitions and

More information

Taking control of reflexive social attention

Taking control of reflexive social attention Cognition 94 (2005) B55 B65 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Taking control of reflexive social attention Jelena Ristic*, Alan Kingstone Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia,

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

WORK EXPERIENCE AND EDUCATION

WORK EXPERIENCE AND EDUCATION Curriculum Vitae GUSTAF GREDEBÄCK December 6 st, 2017 Professor Chair of Developmental Psychology Manager of the Uppsala Child and Baby Lab Department of Psychology, Uppsala University Box 1225, 751 42

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences from the N170 adaptation effect Xiao-hua Cao 1, Chao Li 1, Carl M Gaspar 2, Bei Jiang 1 1. Department of Psychology,Zhejiang Normal

More information

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Infant and Child Development Inf. Child Dev. 10: 39 45 (2001) DOI: 10.1002/icd.245 Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Olivier Pascalis a, *, Elisabeth

More information

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials Chinese Science Bulletin 2007 Science in China Press Springer-Verlag Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials SONG Yan 1, PENG

More information

Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier

Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier S. Bentin, D. Carmel / Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 197 COGNITION Cognition 85 (2002) 197 202 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Discussion Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier

More information

The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology?

The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology? ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 13(2), 117 122 Copyright 2001, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology? Patrick R. Green Department

More information

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention University of New Mexico UNM Digital Repository Psychology ETDs Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2-9-2010 Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention David B.

More information

Attentional control and reflexive orienting to gaze and arrow cues

Attentional control and reflexive orienting to gaze and arrow cues Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 964-969 Attentional control and reflexive orienting to gaze and arrow cues JELENA RISTIC University of California, Santa Barbara, California ALISSA WRIGHT University

More information

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:296 301 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0353-4 BRIEF REPORT Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Risa Sawaki & Steven J. Luck Published online: 20 December 2012 #

More information

Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain

Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain Motivating Questions Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D. Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory COGS1 class How do our brains perceive the mental states of others

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source

More information

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study Armita Faghani Jadidi, Raheleh Davoodi, Mohammad Hassan Moradi Department of Biomedical Engineering Amirkabir University

More information

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14 CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines Assignment 5: Neural mechanisms of object-based attention Due: Friday, April 14 This Assignment is a guided reading of the 2014 paper, Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based

More information

E. J. H. Jones 1*, K. Venema 2, R. Earl 2, R. Lowy 2,4, K. Barnes 3, A. Estes 2,4, G. Dawson 5 and S. J. Webb 2,3,6*

E. J. H. Jones 1*, K. Venema 2, R. Earl 2, R. Lowy 2,4, K. Barnes 3, A. Estes 2,4, G. Dawson 5 and S. J. Webb 2,3,6* Jones et al. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders (2016) 8:7 DOI 10.1186/s11689-016-9139-8 RESEARCH Reduced engagement with social stimuli in 6-month-old infants with later autism spectrum disorder:

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

Prof. Greg Francis 7/8/08

Prof. Greg Francis 7/8/08 Attentional and motor development IIE 366: Developmental Psychology Chapter 5: Perceptual and Motor Development Module 5.2 Attentional Processes Module 5.3 Motor Development Greg Francis Lecture 13 Children

More information

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention.

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention. Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention. All behaviors unfold over time; therefore, our ability to perceive and adapt our behavior according to the temporal constraints

More information

INVESTIGATION COGNITIVE AVEC LES EFRPS (EYE-FIXATION-RELATED POTENTIALS) Thierry Baccino CHART-LUTIN (EA 4004) Université de Paris VIII

INVESTIGATION COGNITIVE AVEC LES EFRPS (EYE-FIXATION-RELATED POTENTIALS) Thierry Baccino CHART-LUTIN (EA 4004) Université de Paris VIII INVESTIGATION COGNITIVE AVEC LES EFRPS (EYE-FIXATION-RELATED POTENTIALS) Thierry Baccino CHART-LUTIN (EA 4004) Université de Paris VIII MESURES OCULAIRES PRELIMINARY QUESTIONS TO EFRPS Postulate: Individual

More information

The Other-Race Effect and its Influences on the Development of Emotion Processing

The Other-Race Effect and its Influences on the Development of Emotion Processing University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 2009 The Other-Race Effect and its Influences on the Development of Emotion Processing Alexandra Monesson

More information

Perception of Faces and Bodies

Perception of Faces and Bodies CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Perception of Faces and Bodies Similar or Different? Virginia Slaughter, 1 Valerie E. Stone, 2 and Catherine Reed 3 1 Early Cognitive Development Unit and 2

More information

An Embodied Account of Action Prediction

An Embodied Account of Action Prediction Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences 107 An Embodied Account of Action Prediction CLAUDIA ELSNER ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSIS UPPSALA 2015 ISSN

More information

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition RESEARCH ARTICLE A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition Sameer Sethi 1 *, Dr. Simon Rigoulot 2, Dr. Marc D. Pell 3 1 Faculty of Science, McGill University,

More information

neurons: how kids learn

neurons: how kids learn mirror neurons: how kids learn Table of Contents 1 2 mirror neurons The Neuron What is a Mirror Neuron Research language development Mimicry Mapping 3 actions and intentions Understanding Intentions 4

More information

AUTOMATIC ATTENTIONAL SHIFTS BY GAZE, GESTURES, AND SYMBOLS

AUTOMATIC ATTENTIONAL SHIFTS BY GAZE, GESTURES, AND SYMBOLS Psychologia, 2010, 53, 27 35 AUTOMATIC ATTENTIONAL SHIFTS BY GAZE, GESTURES, AND SYMBOLS Wataru SATO 1), Takanori KOCHIYAMA 2), Shota UONO 1), and Sakiko YOSHIKAWA 1) 1) Kyoto University, Japan, 2) Advanced

More information

Spatial Attention to Social Cues is not a Monolithic Process

Spatial Attention to Social Cues is not a Monolithic Process Spatial Attention to Social Cues is not a Monolithic Process Samuel M. Harding (hardinsm@indiana.edu) Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, 1101 E. 10 th Street Bloomington, IN 47405 USA Ty W.

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Attentional Blink Paradigm

Attentional Blink Paradigm Attentional Blink Paradigm ATTENTIONAL BLINK 83 ms stimulus onset asychrony between all stimuli B T D A 3 N P Z F R K M R N Lag 3 Target 1 Target 2 After detection of a target in a rapid stream of visual

More information

Motor System Activation Predicts Goal Imitation in 7-Month-Old Infants

Motor System Activation Predicts Goal Imitation in 7-Month-Old Infants 632231PSSXXX10.1177/0956797616632231Filippi et al.motor System Recruitment Predicts Goal Imitation research-article2016 Psychological Science OnlineFirst, published on April 12, 2016 as doi:10.1177/0956797616632231

More information

A Computational Model For Action Prediction Development

A Computational Model For Action Prediction Development A Computational Model For Action Prediction Development Serkan Bugur 1, Yukie Nagai 3, Erhan Oztop 2, and Emre Ugur 1 1 Bogazici University, Istanbul, Turkey. 2 Ozyegin University, Istanbul, Turkey. 3

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Paul E. Hemeren (paul.hemeren@his.se) University of Skövde, School of Humanities and Informatics

More information

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1 The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1 Psycholinguistics / Neurolinguistics Happy Hour!!! Tuesdays 7/11, 7/18, 7/25 5:30-6:30 PM @ the Boone Center

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

Why do we look at people's eyes?

Why do we look at people's eyes? Journal of Eye Movement Research 1(1):1, 1-6 Why do we look at people's eyes? Elina Birmingham University of British Columbia Walter Bischof University of Alberta Alan Kingstone University of British Columbia

More information

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso Control of visuo-spatial attention Emiliano Macaluso CB demo Attention Limited processing resources Overwhelming sensory input cannot be fully processed => SELECTIVE PROCESSING Selection via spatial orienting

More information

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces N. F. Troje* and D. Kersten *Max-Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik, Spemannstr. 38, 72076 Tübingen, Germany Department of Psychology, University

More information

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society Event-Related Potentials Reveal Perceptual Simulation of Color in Language Comprehension Aitao Lu (Center for Studies

More information

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction Proceedings of the First International Conference on Complex Medical Engineering (CME2005) May 15-18, 2005, Takamatsu, Japan (Organized Session No. 20). Paper No. 150, pp. 715-719. Event-Related Potentials

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Automatic attentional shifts by gaze, gestures, and symbols. Benesse Corporation), Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University,

Automatic attentional shifts by gaze, gestures, and symbols. Benesse Corporation), Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Psychologia 1 Automatic attentional shifts by gaze, gestures, and symbols Wataru Sato 1, Takanori Kochiyama 2, Shota Uono 3, and Sakiko Yoshikawa 4 Institutional affiliation 1 Department of Comparative

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2008, 8 (2), 195-210 doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.2.195 The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies MIKA

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The number line effect reflects top-down control Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),862-868???-??? The number line effect reflects top-down control JELENA RISTIC University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

The Gaze Cueing Paradigm with Eye Tracking Background Set-up Lab

The Gaze Cueing Paradigm with Eye Tracking Background Set-up Lab iworx Physiology Lab Experiment Experiment HP-17 The Gaze Cueing Paradigm with Eye Tracking Background Set-up Lab Note: The lab presented here is intended for evaluation purposes only. iworx users should

More information

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Cognitive Brain Research 18 (2004) 255 272 Research report Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Audrey Duarte a, *, Charan Ranganath

More information

Infants perceiving and acting on the eyes: Tests of an evolutionary hypothesis

Infants perceiving and acting on the eyes: Tests of an evolutionary hypothesis J. Experimental Child Psychology 85 (2003) 199 212 Journal of Experimental Child Psychology www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp Infants perceiving and acting on the eyes: Tests of an evolutionary hypothesis Teresa

More information

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION INTRODUCTION Research on the Deaf Brain is beginning to provide a new evidence base for policy and practice in relation to intervention with deaf children. This talk outlines the multi-channel nature of

More information

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return David J. Prime and Pierre Jolicoeur Abstract & Here we examined the relationship between inhibition of return (IOR) and response-selection

More information

This presentation is the intellectual property of the author. Contact them for permission to reprint and/or distribute.

This presentation is the intellectual property of the author. Contact them for permission to reprint and/or distribute. Modified Combinatorial Nomenclature Montage, Review, and Analysis of High Density EEG Terrence D. Lagerlund, M.D., Ph.D. CP1208045-16 Disclosure Relevant financial relationships None Off-label/investigational

More information

PAPER Baby steps: investigating the development of perceptual motor couplings in infancy

PAPER Baby steps: investigating the development of perceptual motor couplings in infancy Developmental Science 18:2 (2015), pp 270 280 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12226 PAPER Baby steps: investigating the development of perceptual motor couplings in infancy Carina C.J.M. de Klerk, 1 Mark H. Johnson,

More information

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Sleep Deprivation and Selective Attention An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Logan T. Trujillo, PhD 1 ; Steve Kornguth,

More information

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014)

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014) Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014) Christian Graulty, Orestis Papaioannou, Phoebe Bauer, Michael Pitts & Enriqueta Canseco-Gonzalez, Reed College. Funded by the Murdoch

More information

Enhanced visual perception near the hands

Enhanced visual perception near the hands Enhanced visual perception near the hands Bachelor thesis Marina Meinert (s0163430) Supervisors: 1 st supervisor: Prof. Dr. Ing. W. B. Verwey 2 nd supervisor: Dr. M. L. Noordzij External supervisor: Dr.

More information

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization THE VISUAL SYSTEM Higher Visual Processing Martin Paré Assistant Professor Physiology & Psychology Most of the cortex that covers the cerebral hemispheres is

More information

Neuroscience Tutorial

Neuroscience Tutorial Neuroscience Tutorial Brain Organization : cortex, basal ganglia, limbic lobe : thalamus, hypothal., pituitary gland : medulla oblongata, midbrain, pons, cerebellum Cortical Organization Cortical Organization

More information

The Categorical Representation of Facial Expressions by 7-Month- Old Infants

The Categorical Representation of Facial Expressions by 7-Month- Old Infants The Categorical Representation of Facial Expressions by 7-Month- Old Infants The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters.

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Rui Cao (caorui.beilia@gmail.com) Thomas A. Busey (busey@indiana.edu) Robert M. Nosofsky (nosofsky@indiana.edu) Richard

More information

Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland. Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013

Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland. Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013 Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013 Summary of EEG findings The intent of the EEG monitoring was to see which

More information

Using mu rhythm perturbations to measure mirror neuron activity in infants

Using mu rhythm perturbations to measure mirror neuron activity in infants 1 Using mu rhythm perturbations to measure mirror neuron activity in infants Pär Nyström 1, Therese Ljunghammar 1, Kerstin Rosander 1, Claes von Hofsten 1 1 Department of Psychology, Uppsala University,

More information

The Effects of Action on Perception. Andriana Tesoro. California State University, Long Beach

The Effects of Action on Perception. Andriana Tesoro. California State University, Long Beach ACTION ON PERCEPTION 1 The Effects of Action on Perception Andriana Tesoro California State University, Long Beach ACTION ON PERCEPTION 2 The Effects of Action on Perception Perception is a process that

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information

Experiment HP-13: The Gaze Cue Paradigm

Experiment HP-13: The Gaze Cue Paradigm Experiment HP-13: The Gaze Cue Paradigm Background During almost all social interactions, people s eyes convey information about their direction of attention as well as their emotional and mental status.

More information

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Hearing-aids Induce Plasticity in the Auditory System: Perspectives From Three Research Designs and Personal Speculations About the

More information

Visual Selection and Object Perception

Visual Selection and Object Perception Visual Selection and Object Perception Dima Amso Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychobiology, WMC Cornell University 1300 York Ave. Box 140 New York, NY 10028 dia2004@med.cornell.edu Abstract - We

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box. Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany

Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box. Department of Neurology, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany When in infancy does the fear bias develop? Tobias Grossmann 1 & Sarah Jessen 2 1 Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box 400400, Charlottesville, VA 22904, U.S.A. 2

More information

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 834-839 Brief Reports Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex Jeffrey S. Johnson, Geoffrey F. Woodman, Elsie Braun, and Steven

More information

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Session 4 Virtual Lesion Approach I Alexandra Reichenbach MPI for Biological Cybernetics Tübingen, Germany Today s Schedule Virtual Lesion Approach : Study Design Rationale

More information

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention Tineke Grent- t-jong 1,2, Marty G. Woldorff 1,3* PLoS BIOLOGY 1 Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University,

More information

On the role of response conflicts and stimulus position for hemispheric differences in global/local processing: an ERP study

On the role of response conflicts and stimulus position for hemispheric differences in global/local processing: an ERP study On the role of response conflicts and stimulus position for hemispheric differences in global/local processing: an ERP study Gregor Volberg, Ronald Hübner Universität Konstanz, Fachbereich Psychologie,

More information

Behavioral and Electrophysiological Indices of Memory in Typically Developing and Hypoxic-Ischemic Injured Infants

Behavioral and Electrophysiological Indices of Memory in Typically Developing and Hypoxic-Ischemic Injured Infants Infancy, 19(1), 28 52, 2014 Copyright International Society on Infant Studies (ISIS) ISSN: 1525-0008 print / 1532-7078 online DOI: 10.1111/infa.12032 Behavioral and Electrophysiological Indices of Memory

More information

Implied body action directs spatial attention

Implied body action directs spatial attention Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (6), 1437-1443 doi:10.3758/app.72.6.1437 Brief Reports Implied body action directs spatial attention Will M. Gervais University of Denver, Denver, Colorado

More information

The effects of one s actions upon hidden objects 1. Understanding the effects of one s actions upon hidden objects and the development of

The effects of one s actions upon hidden objects 1. Understanding the effects of one s actions upon hidden objects and the development of The effects of one s actions upon hidden objects 1 Understanding the effects of one s actions upon hidden objects and the development of search behaviour in 7-month-old infants. Richard J O Connor a &

More information

Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception of ambiguous stimuli

Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception of ambiguous stimuli R175B BC JP To printer - need negs for pgs, 3,, and Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,?? (?),???-??? Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception

More information

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION Neural Signal Processing for Functional Neuroimaging Neuroscience Statistics Research Laboratory Massachusetts General Hospital Harvard Medical School/MIT Division

More information

Acta Psychologica 141 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica

Acta Psychologica 141 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica Acta Psychologica 141 (2012) 270 275 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/ locate/actpsy Gaze cues influence memory but not for long Michael

More information

Selective Attention Supports Working Memory Maintenance by Modulating Perceptual Processing of Distractors

Selective Attention Supports Working Memory Maintenance by Modulating Perceptual Processing of Distractors Selective Attention Supports Working Memory Maintenance by Modulating Perceptual Processing of Distractors Kartik K. Sreenivasan and Amishi P. Jha Abstract & Selective attention has been shown to bias

More information

Goal prediction in 2-year-old children with and without autism spectrum disorder: An eyetracking study: Goal prediction in...

Goal prediction in 2-year-old children with and without autism spectrum disorder: An eyetracking study: Goal prediction in... See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/322942013 Goal prediction in 2-year-old children with and without autism spectrum disorder:

More information

Social perception in the infant brain: gamma oscillatory activity in response to eye gaze

Social perception in the infant brain: gamma oscillatory activity in response to eye gaze doi:10.1093/scan/nsm025 SCAN (2007) 2, 284 291 Social perception in the infant brain: gamma oscillatory activity in response to eye gaze Tobias Grossmann, Mark H. Johnson, Teresa Farroni, and Gergely Csibra

More information