The Influence of Selective Attention on Auditory Localization in Younger and Older Adults

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Influence of Selective Attention on Auditory Localization in Younger and Older Adults"

Transcription

1 The Influence of Selective Attention on Auditory Localization in Younger and Older Adults by Stephanie Yung A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Department of Psychology University of Toronto Copyright by Stephanie Yung 2016

2 The Influence of Selective Attention on Auditory Localization in Younger and Older Adults Abstract Stephanie Yung Master of Arts Department of Psychology University of Toronto 2016 Auditory localization, the ability to locate sounds, declines with age due to changes in peripheral and central auditory processing. As selective attention is typically preserved in older adults, drawing their attention towards the stimulus before it occurs may improve auditory localization. This study evaluated this hypothesis by precueing younger and older participants towards a likely location of the upcoming target (a broadband noise) with a visual attentional cue. The attentional cues either provided a) correct information about the upcoming target location (valid) b) incorrect information about the upcoming target location (invalid), or c) no information about the upcoming target location (neutral). Participants estimated the target s location on a schematic drawing of the testing environment. Akin to younger adults, older adults made less errors and biases, and faster responses with valid cues. This suggests that older adults can employ selective attention to improve auditory localization, similar to younger adults. ii

3 Acknowledgments I would like to thank my supervisors Dr. Jennifer Campos and Dr. Ian Spence for their patience, guidance, and sharing their expertise with me. I would also like to thank Bruce Haycock for programming the auditory localization task. Last but not least, I would like to thank my external examination committee member, Dr. Claude Alain, for his generosity of time. iii

4 Table of Content Acknowledgments... iii Table of Content... iv 1 Introduction Overview Mechanisms of Auditory Localization Age Differences in Auditory Localization Selective Attention and Auditory Localization Rationale and Hypotheses Method Participants Materials Audiometric Testing Auditory Target Testing Environment Visual Cues and Response Auditory Localization Task Procedure Design Results Absolute Error Signed Error Response Time Discussion Localization Accuracy Response Time Summary of the Results Conclusion References iv

5 List of Figures Figure 1. Diagram of the testing environment in aerial view. Participants sat at equidistance (2.14 m) from the five physical loudspeakers at -90, -28, 0, +28, +90. The loudspeakers were hidden from view by a 240 projection screen. Figure 2. Trial procedure of the auditory localization task (A). This exemplifies a valid trial in the auditory localization task. The attentional cue was centered at -90. The red circle indicates that the target was presented at -90. Note that participants were not shown the placements of the physical loudspeakers. (B) is a screenshot of the auditory localization task during feedback. Figure 3. Mean absolute error (degrees) between the three different attentional cue types collapsed across location and age groups. Error bars represent ±2 SE. Figure 4. Mean absolute error (degrees) between the five locations collapsed across age groups and the attentional cue types. Error bars represent ±2 SE. Figure 5. Mean absolute error (degrees) between the five locations clustered by the three different attentional cue types. Error bars represent ±2 SE. Figure 6. Mean signed error (degrees) between the three different attentional cue types collapsed across location and age groups. Error bars represent ±2 SE. Figure 7. Mean signed error (degrees) between the five locations collapsed across age groups and the attentional cue types. The error bars represent ±2 SE. Figure 8. Mean signed error (degrees) between the five locations clustered by the three different attentional cue types. Error bars represent ±2 SE. Figure 9. Mean response time (ms) between the three different attentional cue types collapsed across location and age groups. Error bars represent ±2 SE. Figure 10. Mean response time (ms) between the five locations collapsed across age groups and the attentional cue types. Error bars represent ±2 SE. Figure 11. Mean response time (ms) between the three different attentional cue types clustered by age groups. Error bars represent ±2 SE. Figure 12. Mean response time (ms) between the five locations clustered by age groups. Error bars represent ±2 SE. v

6 1 Introduction 1.1 Overview Hearing helps humans become spatially aware of their environment. If one hears tires screeching from behind, one can assume that there is a car behind them. Locating a bird may also be easier by listening to its call, similar to following a familiar voice to find a friend in the crowd. In the literature, this ability to locate objects through hearing is termed auditory localization (Briley & Summerfield, 2014; Freigang et al., 2015; Moore, 2013). Despite its importance in everyday life, auditory localization declines with age (Freigang et al., 2015). Even the healthiest older adults with no or mild hearing loss are not as accurate as younger adults in localizing sounds (e.g., Freigang et al., 2014). This suggests that the age differences in auditory localization may be due to changes in the central auditory system (the interactions between the ears and the brain) rather than the peripheral auditory system (ear structures; Freigang et al., 2015). One way to compensate for the declines in auditory localization may be to use selective attention, a higher-order cognitive ability that has been found to be preserved with age (e.g., Groth & Allen., 2000; Guerreiro et al., 2014). Selective attention is the ability to attend to relevant information while ignoring irrelevant information, and this can be used when there is directional knowledge about the upcoming stimulus (Posner, 1980). While studies have showed that selective attention can improve auditory localization in younger adults (e.g., Golob & Holmes, 2011), it is still unclear whether it has the same influence on older adults. Therefore, this study aimed to understand the interaction between auditory localization and attention as we age. Of particular interest was whether older adults can locate a broadband noise target presented in the horizontal plane (left, right, and directly in front of the listener s head) as quickly and as accurately as younger adults when attention is directed towards its upcoming location. Thus, three different types of visual attentional cues provided the target s upcoming location: 1) valid cues, which correctly indicated the upcoming target location; 2) invalid cues, which incorrectly indicated the upcoming target location; and 3) neutral cues, which provided no directional information about the upcoming target location. Based on the current literature (Groth & Allen, 2000; Golob et al., 2002; Golob & Holmes, 2011), correct information about the 1

7 upcoming target location should improve older adults accuracy and response time to a greater extent than younger adults (described further below). To begin, I will first review the mechanisms of auditory localization. Then, I will review the current literature on the age differences in auditory localization and the influence of selective attention on auditory localization with younger adults. Based on the reviewed literature, the hypotheses and the study design will be described. Finally, the results and its interpretations will be discussed. 1.2 Mechanisms of Auditory Localization Two sound cues are used to locate sounds in the horizontal plane (e.g., left, right and directly in front of the listener s head): the interaural time difference (ITD) and the interaural level difference (ILD). The temporal difference in the arrival of the sound to each ear is termed the interaural time difference (Middlebrooks & Green, 1994). For instance, when a sound arrives at the right ear first, its perceived location should be closer to the right side of the listener s head. The difference of the sound intensity in each ear is termed the interaural level difference. For instance, when a sound is louder at the right ear, its perceived location should be closer to the right side of the listener s head (Akeroyd, 2006, 2014; Grothe et al., 2010; Middlebrooks & Green, 1994; Moore, 2013). Auditory localization thus involves the detection of the sound cues in the peripheral auditory system (the structure of the ears), and then the integration of the sound cues in the central auditory system (the interaction between the ears and the brain; Middlebrooks & Green, 1994). Interestingly, not all sounds produce reliable ITDs and ILDs for localization. Broadband and everyday noise, such as speech or bird chirps, rely on the comparisons between ITDs and ILDs in each ear (Akeroyd, 2006, 2014). However, pure-tone noise, such as high- and low-frequency noise, produce either ILDs or ITDs, respectively (Middlebrooks & Green; 1991; Moore, 2013). Therefore, broadband and everyday noise are localized with higher accuracy because they produce ITDs and ILDs, whereas high- and low-frequency noise are harder to localize because they have one or the other (e.g., Abel et al., 2000; Freigang et al., 2015; Middlebrooks & Green; 1991; Moore, 2013; see also Rayleigh, 1907). 2

8 Different environments can also affect the sound cues and subsequent auditory localization (e.g., Kopčo et al., 2007). Most everyday environments are reverberant, which means there are surfaces that reflect sound waves (e.g., classrooms, concert halls, caves). Environments that absorb or weaken sound reflections are anechoic (e.g., sound booth). Broadband noise stimuli have been found to be localized with high accuracy in semi-reverberant and anechoic environments (Abel et al., 2000; Dobreva et al., 2011; Kopčo et al., 2007); however, pure-tone noise stimuli are localized most accurately in environments with anechoic properties (Ahveninen et al., 2014; Brungart et al., 2011; Freigang et al., 2014). Freigang et al. (2015) explained that pure-tone noise may be hard to localize in non-anechoic environments because they have fewer sound cues, thus the integrity of the pure-tone noise may be more modulated by acoustical properties such as reverberation. For instance, Abel et al. (2000) found that high-frequency noise was harder to localize in a semi-reverberant environment because the reflective surfaces may have increased the sound intensity differences between each ear (ILDs). In other words, listeners perceived the high-frequency noise as farther away from its actual location because the sound reflections may have made the sound waves in each ear louder than they actually were. Few studies have examined auditory localization in fully reverberant environments (Ahveninen et al., 2014; Kopčo et al., 2007). Understandably, these environments may introduce uncontrollable factors as discussed above (see also Zahorik et al., 2005, for how reverberation may bias distance perception). However, most sounds in everyday life are heard in reverberant environments. Thus, the localization of pure-tone noise in simple environments such as anechoic or semi-reverberant environments may not be as generalizable to real-life settings. To improve our knowledge of auditory localization in realistic settings, this study aimed to extend the current literature by using a broadband noise in a typically, reverberant indoor setting. 1.3 Age Differences in Auditory Localization Most studies on auditory localization have focused on younger adults (e.g., Ahveninen et al., 2014; Akeroyd, 2006; Arnott & Alain, 2002; Brungart et al., 2011; Golob & Holmes, 2011; Middlebrooks & Green, 1991; Savel, 2009; Stevens & Newman, 1936; Tiippana et al., 2011; Wood & Bizley, 2015), but when older adults are compared to younger adults, older adults localized sounds less accurately (Abel et al., 2000; Dobreva et al., 2011; Freigang et al., 2014). For instance, Abel et al. (2000) asked younger and older adults to localize broadband noise and 3

9 high- and low-frequency noise from different arrays of speakers positioned in the horizontal plane. Listeners localized the sounds by pressing buttons that corresponded with each speaker. Ultimately, they found that the localization estimates of the sounds presented from the lateral positions were less accurate in older adults than in younger adults. Further, overall localization accuracy was higher by 15% in younger listeners compared to older listeners. This was also found by Freigang et al. (2014) who presented high- and low-frequency noises positioned in the horizontal plane; sounds presented more laterally were more difficult to localize than sounds presented towards the center, and the overall accuracy of the younger listeners was 21% higher than older listeners. The mechanisms that account for the age differences in auditory localization have not been elucidated. Dobreva et al. (2011) noted that it is especially difficult to disentangle the central and peripheral auditory changes that may contribute to problems with auditory localization in healthy older adults. For instance, even the healthiest older adult may have slight hearing difficulty (especially in the higher frequencies) due to functional changes in the peripheral auditory system. However, Abel et al. (2000) and Freigang et al. (2014) did not find strong evidence that higher hearing thresholds, as measured by audiometric tests, were associated with poorer localization accuracy. On the other hand, Freigang et al. (2015) reviewed that Freigang et al. s 2014 study may not have found strong associations between hearing thresholds and auditory localization performances because they tested older adults under 65 years of age. According to Freigang et al. (2015), the biggest changes in peripheral hearing manifest after 65 years. Nevertheless, Freigang et al. (2015) reviewed that the age-related changes in the central auditory system may better explain localization problems in healthy older adults. Specifically, functional changes (e.g., loss of hair cells in the cochlea) can lead to temporal jitter in afferent neuronal signal processing, and subsequently reduce the transmission of precise temporal information of the sound cues (p. 361; see also Pichora-Fuller & Schneider, 1998). This suggests that older adults may locate sounds less accurately because the integration of the sound cues from each ear is delayed. As Dobreva et al. (2011) mentioned, the interaction between peripheral auditory processing and central auditory processing are indeed difficult to disentangle. Due to structural declines in the 4

10 peripheral auditory system, the temporal processing of the central auditory system suffers (Freigang et al., 2015). However, one way to control for some of the effects associated with peripheral deficits is to test older adults with pure-tone hearing thresholds that are comparable to younger adults under ideal conditions (i.e. in a sound booth). Therefore, the current study recruited older adults who are at least 65 years of age with normal hearing (as defined by <30 db for frequencies from 0.25, 0.50, 0.75, 1, 2, 3, and 4kHz; International Organization for Standardization (ISO), 2000). 1.4 Selective Attention and Auditory Localization Selective attention may compensate for the age-related changes in the peripheral and central auditory systems. Selective attention is the cognitive ability to attend to relevant information while ignoring irrelevant information (Groth & Allen, 2000). There are numerous theories on how attention is deployed, but most posit that there are attentional processing resources that are differentially distributed based on the task demands (Feng & Spence, 2013). For example, in Eriksen and St. James s zoom lens analogy (1986), if the size of the attended field is large, the processing resources within this field will be distributed more sparsely. While this ensures the target(s) will be detected, accuracy and/or response time to the target(s) may be impaired. On the other hand, if the size of the attended field is smaller, the processing resources will become focused on a denser area; thus, the responses to the target(s) within this field may be more accurate and/or faster. The zoom lens analogy has been traditionally applied to visual information (e.g., Madden et al., 1994; Madden, 2007), demonstrating that older adults respond to visual targets more slowly and less accurately when they attend to a larger field than a smaller field. However, when older adults have prior knowledge about the upcoming target location, they respond faster and more accurately. Thus, prior knowledge about the target may decrease the size of the attended field and ensure the processing resources are organized to respond to the targets faster and more accurately. In the context of auditory localization tasks, younger adults seem to benefit from employing selective attention (e.g., Golob & Holmes, 2011). In a series of auditory localization experiments, Rhodes (1987) informed younger adults that an upcoming sound would have a high chance of 5

11 appearing in the same location as it did during the previous trial (but not always). Therefore, the previous trial location precued the likely location of the upcoming trial. Rhodes (1987) found that the responses of the precued locations were faster and more accurate than non-cued locations. Since then, more studies have demonstrated the benefit of precueing in improving auditory localization in various planes in younger adults and in those with visual impairments (e.g., Collins & Schirillo, 2013; Bausenhart et al., 2007; Föcker et al., 2010; Golob & Holmes, 2011; Hiscock et al., 1999; Lee & Spence, 2015; Teder-Sälejärvi & Hillyard, 1999; Rorden & Driver, 2001). To the best of my knowledge, the influence of selective attention on auditory localization has not been examined in older adults. Because the ability to attend to cued visual targets and to ignore distracting information is preserved in older adults (Guerreiro et al., 2014; Groth & Allen, 2000), older adults may also improve in auditory localization when the sound source is precued. In other words, older adults may compensate for peripheral and central auditory changes in localizing sounds with top-down control through selective attention. Thus, to extend the literature on the influence of selective attention on auditory localization and aging, this study adopted Posner s (1980) cueing paradigm. Posner s (1980) cueing paradigm is composed of providing directional knowledge about the upcoming target. Three types of attentional cues are given: 1) Valid cues, provided on 60% of the trials, gave correct information about the upcoming target location; 2) Invalid cues, provided on 20% of the trials, gave incorrect information about the upcoming target location, and 3) neutral cues, provided on 20% of the trials, gave no information about the upcoming target location. Golob et al. (2002) also adopted Posner s cueing paradigm in an auditory localization task with younger adults. Sounds were presented from three locations (0, -90 (left) and +90 (right)). Before the sounds were presented, participants were verbally given the attentional cues. Valid cues correctly indicated the sound location (e.g., left and the sound was presented from the left), invalid cues provided incorrect information about the sound location (e.g., right when the sound was presented from the left). Neutral cues did not provide any directional information (e.g., go ). Golob et al. (2002) found that participants localized sounds after valid cues faster than after neutral and invalid cues. However, they did not report differences in localization accuracy 6

12 between the attentional cues. In another study conducted by Golob and Holmes (2011), they tasked younger adults to localize sounds presented at -90, -45, 0, +45, and +90 with a target/non-target paradigm. Ultimately, Golob and Holmes (2011) found that younger adults localized targets faster than non-targets. They also reported that the localization accuracy for targets presented at the lateral positions was higher than the non-targets at the lateral positions. As Eriksen and James (1986) suggested, prior information about the target location may have helped focus the distribution of the attentional processing resources together, allowing the localization responses to be faster and more accurate. 1.5 Rationale and Hypotheses The influence of selective attention on auditory localization in older adults has not been extensively studied. To fill this gap in knowledge, younger and older adults completed an auditory localization task modified with Posner s (1980) cueing paradigm. The auditory target was a broadband noise, presented from one of five hidden loudspeakers located directly ahead (0 ) and to the left (-28 and -90 ) and right (+28 and +90 ) of the listener s head in a nonsound attenuated laboratory. Prior to the onset of the target, attentional cues were visually presented to the participants to a) provide correct information about the upcoming target location (valid), b) provide incorrect information about the upcoming target location (invalid), or c) provide no information about the upcoming target location (neutral). Based on the reviewed literature (Freigang et al., 2014, 2015; Golob et al., 2002; Golob & Holmes, 2011), this study evaluated the following hypotheses: 1) Younger adults will localize the target more accurately and faster than older adults overall. 2) The target will be localized more accurately and faster after valid cues than after neutral and invalid cues. 3) The target presented at -90 and +90 will be localized less accurately than those at -28, 0, and

13 4) Improvements in localization accuracy and response time associated with valid cues will be proportionally greater in older adults than in younger adults. 2 Method 2.1 Participants Sixteen younger adults (M = 22.19, SD = 2.43; 5 males) and fifteen older adults (M = 68.13, SD = 3.46; 3 males) were recruited by flyers posted at the University of Toronto and Toronto Rehabilitation Institute. Prospective participants were screened over the telephone for health conditions that could impair attention, auditory localization abilities, and/or performance on the pointing response task (e.g. diagnosed hearing loss, arthritis in the hand or arm). Eligible participants were then brought into the lab and completed audiometric testing in a sound booth to ensure their pure-tone air conduction thresholds were less than or equal to 25 db (for younger adults) or 30 db (for older adults) for frequencies from 0.25 to 4 khz in both ears (procedure described in section 2.2.1). Older adults were also screened for cognitive impairment with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MOCA); those with a score of less than or equal to 25 (out of 30 points) were excluded. Participants were compensated $10 per hour. This study was approved by the research ethics board at Toronto Rehabilitation Institute. 2.2 Materials Audiometric Testing The audiometric test was conducted with a Grason-Stadler 61 Clinical Audiometer and Sennheiser headphones in a dimly lit sound booth. The heard/not-heard detection paradigm was used. The right ear (or the self-reported better ear) was always tested first. The test began at 1000 Hz with the sound intensity set at 40 db. If participants responded to the tone by saying yes through the headset, the intensity was lowered from 40 db to 30 db. The intensity always lowered in increments of 10 db until there was no response. When a response was given, the intensity was always increased by 5 db HL. Once three responses ( yes ) to a particular intensity were made, the intensity was recorded. After testing 1000 Hz, the lower frequencies (250, 500, and 500 Hz) were tested, and then the higher frequencies (2000, 3000, and 4000 Hz), all of which began at the sound intensity of 40 db. Once the testing of the right ear or the self-reported 8

14 better ear was finished, the other ear was tested. The audiometric test took approximately 10 minutes to complete Auditory Target The auditory target in the auditory localization task was a broadband Gaussian noise generated in Audacity. The target was presented at 60 db from one of five loudspeakers (Meyer-sound MP- 4XP) hidden behind a projector screen in the testing environment (see section 2.2.3). The center loudspeaker was located straight ahead of the participant at 0, and the lateral loudspeakers were at -90, -28 (left), and +90 and +28 (right). Participants sat 2.14 m away from the loudspeakers. See Figure 1. In the practice blocks, the target was presented at 70 db to emphasize learning the trial procedure Testing Environment The study was conducted in a laboratory located within Toronto Rehabilitation Institute s Challenging Environment Assessment Laboratory under dim lighting conditions. The acoustical properties of the laboratory were measured with a Norsonic NOR140 sound level meter, serial number The reverberation times in the laboratory were within the range deemed acceptable by ANSI standards for a teaching environment, and the average background sound level was 43 db(a) with no equipment operating (Campos et al., 2016) Visual Cues and Response On a Samsung Galaxy Tab 10.1, a schematic top-down drawing of the testing environment was shown to the participants (see Figure 2). The green circle on the schematic drawing represented the walls of the laboratory, and participants localized the target by using a tablet stylus (Adonit Jot Mini stylus) to tap on a location on the circle to represent the perceived target location. At the start of each trial, the location of the participant was represented with a red fixation cross in the centre of the circle. The valid and invalid cues resembled a green pie slice that originated at the participant s location and extended to the wall, highlighting an area of 30 surrounding one of the five loudspeakers. For neutral cued trials, there was no pie slice; instead, the fixation cross was coloured green to indicate to the participants that they will hear the target soon. 9

15 Participants also received feedback about their response. After the target location was indicated on the circle, a red arrow and the angular direction information were shown to help ensure that their response reflected their intended response. The red arrow originated from the participant s location on the schematic drawing, and pointed towards the tapped location. The angular direction information of the tapped location was shown at the bottom of the tablet screen. See Figure 2. The brightness setting was optimized by the electronic touch tablet to ensure the visual cues were consistently visible. The tablet was placed horizontally on a stand that was positioned directly in front of the participant (see Figure 1). The height of the stand was adjusted for each participant to ensure they maintained an upright head position, and that the tablet would not occlude the speaker at 0. A foam armrest was provided to reduce physical discomforts such as fatigue Auditory Localization Task The auditory localization task was completed on the electronic touch tablet. The task consisted of five blocks of 100 trials each (500 trials total). Based on the previous studies (e.g., Golob et al., 2002; Posner, 1980), each block presented 60 valid cues, 20 neutral cues, and 20 invalid cues. The target was presented at each location (-90, -28, 0, +28, +90 ) equally often. The trials within each block were randomized. There were also two practice blocks, each composed of 10 trials (6 valid trials, 2 neutral trials and 2 invalid trials). The trial procedure of the practice blocks was identical to the experimental blocks. 10

16 Figure 1. Diagram of the testing environment in aerial view. Participants sat at equidistance (2.14 m) from the five physical loudspeakers at -90, -28, 0, +28, +90. The loudspeakers were hidden from view by a 240 projection screen. 2.3 Procedure Participants first completed the audiometric test to ensure the minimum hearing criteria were met. Older participants also completed the MOCA to ensure the cognitive criteria were met. Participants who did not meet the hearing and/or cognitive criteria did not complete the study. Before the auditory localization task, participants were instructed that the target will be presented from the walls of the laboratory; therefore, the main task was to indicate the direction of the target, relative to their sitting position, on the schematic drawing. Participants were also instructed to locate the target as quickly and as accurately as they can, and to maintain their gaze towards the fixation cross on the tablet without moving their head or body. Participants completed the two practice blocks to first familiarize themselves with the trial procedure. At the beginning of each trial, a red fixation cross appeared on the center of the schematic drawing. The participant tapped the red fixation cross to continue the trial and held the stylus in 11

17 position until the target was heard. After tapping the red fixation cross, the attentional cue was presented for 250 milliseconds. The interval between the offset of the cue and the onset of the target was 600 milliseconds. The target was then played for 500 ms from one of the five loudspeakers. When the target finished playing, the fixation cross transformed into a white X to prompt the participant s response. The participant then tapped on the circumference of the circle to estimate their perceived location of the target. The maximum allowed response time window was 7 seconds. After their response, a red arrow appeared as feedback. The feedback extended from the center to the touched location on the schematic drawing for 1 second. Simultaneously, the exact angular position in degrees corresponding to the response was shown at the bottom of the tablet screen. To indicate the end of the current trial, the white X changed back into a red fixation cross. The next trial began when the participant placed the stylus back on the red fixation cross. See Figure 2 for the trial procedure. Each practice block took less than one minute to complete. Each experimental block took around 10 minutes to complete. There were mandatory breaks after every two experimental blocks. Additional breaks were offered at the end of each block. At the end of the auditory localization task, the experimenter debriefed and compensated the participants. 12

18 Figure 2. Trial procedure of the auditory localization task (A). This exemplifies a valid trial in the auditory localization task. The attentional cue was centered at -90. The red circle indicates that the target was presented at -90. Note that participants were not shown the placements of the physical loudspeakers. (B) is a screenshot of the auditory localization task during feedback. 2.4 Design A 2 x 3 x 5 mixed-factorial design was used. The independent variables included age group (younger vs. older adults), types of attentional cues (valid vs. neutral vs. invalid), and location (-90 vs. -28 vs. 0 vs. +28, vs. +90 ). Age group was a between-participant factor, whereas the types of attentional cues and location were within-participant factors. The dependent variables included localization accuracy, which was quantified as 1) absolute error (degrees), and 2) signed error (degrees), and response time (ms). 13

19 3 Results For each dependent variable (i.e., absolute error, signed error, response time), a 2 (age group) x 3 (attentional cue) x 5 (location) mixed-factorial ANOVA was conducted. Thereafter, a Bonferonni post-hoc test was conducted to examine pairwise differences for each significant main and interaction effects. No trials were excluded from the analyses. 3.1 Absolute Error Absolute error, defined as the magnitude of unsigned error, was calculated by obtaining the absolute angular difference between the perceived target location and the actual target location. The closer the value is to 0, the closer the perceived target location was to the actual target location. For example, if the target was at -90, and the participant responded -85, the absolute error of that trial would be 5. This would indicate the response was off by 5 (smaller magnitude of error), whereas if they responded +15, the response would be off by 75 (larger magnitude of error). The ANOVA showed a significant main effect of attentional cue, F(2, 58) = 61.78, p <.01, η 2 p =.68. The post-hoc test indicated that the targets preceded by valid cues had smaller mean absolute error than for targets preceded by neutral (p <.01) and invalid cues (p <.01; Figure 3). There was also a significant main effect of location, F(4, 116) = 45.16, p <.01, η 2 p =.61. The post-hoc test indicated that the mean absolute error of targets at -28 and +28 were higher than the targets at -90, +90, and 0 (p <.01 for all comparisons; see Figure 4). 14

20 Absolute Error (Degrees) Absolute Error (Degrees) Valid Neutral Invalid Attentional Cue Type Figure 3. Mean absolute error (degrees) between the three different attentional cue types collapsed across location and age groups. Error bars represent ±2 SE Azimuth of Sound Location (Degrees) Figure 4. Mean absolute error (degrees) between the five locations collapsed across age groups and the attentional cue types. Error bars represent ±2 SE. 15

21 Absolute Error (Degrees) There was a significant interaction effect between attentional cue and location, F(8, 232) = 10.05, p <.01, η 2 p =.26. The post-hoc test indicated that the valid cues had the lower mean absolute error compared to neutral and invalid cues at all locations (p <.01) except at +90, where the mean absolute error of valid cues was not significantly different from neutral cues (p >.05). The post-hoc test also indicated that the mean absolute error of neutral cues was not statistically different than invalid cues for targets presented at -90 (p >.05) and +28 (p >.05). See Figure Valid Neutral Invalid Azimuth of Sound Location (Degrees) Figure 5. Mean absolute error (degrees) between the five locations clustered by the three different attentional cue types. Error bars represent ±2 SE. There were no other significant effects (p >.05), which suggested that younger and older adults performed similarly in absolute error across the different attentional cue and location trials. 3.2 Signed Error Signed error was calculated by obtaining the angular difference between the perceived target location and the actual target location. Thus, the directional bias of the error was indicated by a positive or negative value. A positive value indicated a rightward bias of the error relative to the 16

22 Signed Error (Degrees) physical target location, and a negative value indicated a leftward bias. For example, if the target was at -90, and the perceived target was at -80, the signed error would be +10. This value would indicate that the perceived response was off by 10, and that it was localized towards the right side of the actual target location. The closer the value is to 0, the closer the perceived target location was made from its actual target location. The ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of attentional cue, F(2, 58) = 4.97, p <.01, η 2 p =.15. The post-hoc test indicated that the targets preceded by valid cues had less leftward bias compared to invalid cues (p <.01). No other pairwise comparisons between the attentional cues were significant (p >.05). This indicated that the leftward biases for valid cues were comparable to neutral cues, and that the neutral cues were comparable to invalid cues (Figure 6) Valid Neutral Invalid Attentional Cue Type Figure 6. Mean signed error (degrees) between the three different attentional cue types collapsed across location and age groups. Error bars represent ±2 SE. There was also a significant main effect of location, F(4, 116) = 69.16, p <.01, η p 2 =.71. The post-hoc test showed that all of the locations had different mean signed error from each other (p <.01) except for 0 vs. +90 (p >.05; Figure 7). 17

23 Signed Error (Degrees) Azimuth of Sound Location (Degrees) Figure 7. Mean signed error (degrees) between the five locations collapsed across age groups and the attentional cue types. The error bars represent ±2 SE. There was a significant interaction between attentional cue and location, F(8, 232) = 25.98, p <.01, η p 2 =.48. The post-hoc test showed that the targets preceded by valid cues had smaller mean signed error compared to neutral and invalid cues at all locations (p <.05) except for 0 and +90 (p >.05). At 0 and +90, the leftward biases associated with valid and neutral cues were comparable. The post-hoc test also showed that at -28, +28, and +90, the mean signed error associated with neutral and invalid cues were comparable. See Figure 8. No other pairwise comparisons were significant (p <.05), which indicated that older adults had mean signed error comparable to younger adults across the different attentional cue and location trials. 18

24 Signed Error (Degrees) Valid Neutral Invalid Azimuth of Sound Locations (Degrees) Figure 8. Mean signed error (degrees) between the five locations clustered by the three different attentional cue types. Error bars represent ±2 SE. 3.3 Response Time Response time (ms) was the time after the target was presented to the moment the participant responded on the schematic drawing by tapping the tablet with the stylus. The lower the response time, the faster the target was localized. The ANOVA showed a significant main effect of attentional cue, F(2, 58) = 79.02, p <.01, η 2 p =.73. The post-hoc test indicated that the responses after valid cues were faster than after neutral (p <.01) and invalid cues (p <.01). However, the response times for neutral and invalid cues were not significantly different from each other, p >.05 (Figure 9). 19

25 Response Time (ms) Valid Neutral Invalid Attentional Cue Type Figure 9. Mean response time (ms) between the three different attentional cue types collapsed across location and age groups. Error bars represent ±2 SE. There was a significant main effect of location, F(2, 58) = 79.02, p <.01, η p 2 =.73. The post-hoc test indicated that the response times for targets at the right-side locations (+28 and +90 ) were faster than the targets at directly ahead (0 ) and the left-side locations (-90 and -28 ), p <.01 for all comparisons. Meanwhile, the response times for the right-side target locations did not differ from each other, p >.05. The response times for the targets presented on the left-side and directly ahead also did not differ statistically from each other, p >.05. See Figure

26 Response Time (ms) Azimuth of Sound Location (Degrees) Figure 10. Mean response time (ms) between the five locations collapsed across age groups and the attentional cue types. Error bars represent ±2 SE. Lastly, there were significant interaction effects with the between-subject factor, age group. The interaction between age group and attentional cue was small but significant, F(2, 58) = 3.93, p =.03, η p 2 =.12. The post-hoc test indicated that for older adults, the response times after valid cues were faster than invalid cues, and that invalid cues were faster than neutral cues, p <.01 for all comparisons. In younger adults, response times after valid cues were faster than neutral and invalid trials, p <.01; however, response times between neutral and invalid cues did not differ from each other, p >.05 (Figure 11). 21

27 Response Time (ms) Younger Adults Older Adults Valid Neutral Invalid Attentional Cue Type Figure 11. Mean response time (ms) between the three different attentional cue types clustered by age groups. Error bars represent ±2 SE. There was also a small, but significant interaction between age group and location, F(4, 116) = 2.93, p =.02, η p 2 =.09. The post-hoc test showed that younger adults responded to the targets on the left-side locations slower than targets on the right-side locations and directly ahead, p <.01. Older adults responded slower to the targets on the left-side locations and directly ahead than the targets on the right-side locations, p <.01. See Figure

28 Response Time (ms) Younger Adults Older Adults Azimuth of Sound Location (Degrees) Figure 12. Mean response time (ms) between the five locations clustered by age groups. Error bars represent ±2 SE. 4 Discussion Literature has shown that older adults typically localize sounds less accurately than younger adults due to age-related changes in the peripheral and central auditory system (e.g., Freigang et al., 2014). Thus, this study investigated whether selective attention may compensate for the deficits in auditory localization in older adults (e.g., Golob et al., 2002; Golob & Holmes, 2011). In a typical non-sound attenuated laboratory, the auditory target, a broadband noise, was presented at one of five hidden locations in the horizontal plane: -90, -28, 0, +28, and +90. Selective attention was manipulated by visually cueing the target location, and that was either 1) valid, in that attention was directed towards the upcoming target location, 2) invalid, in that attention was directed towards another location, or 3) neutral, in that no directional information was provided. Localization performance was then assessed by measuring 1) absolute error 23

29 (degrees) and 2) signed error (degrees), and 3) response time (ms). The next sections will discuss localization accuracy and response time separately, and then considered together at the end. 4.1 Localization Accuracy Contrary to my initial hypothesis, younger adults were not more accurate at localizing the broadband noise than were older adults overall. This was unexpected because the age differences in the previous studies were quite large (i.e., 15% difference in accuracy between younger and older adults in Abel et al. s (2000) study, and 21% in Freigang et al. s (2014) study). This suggests that broadband noise is easy to localize accurately for both younger and older adults, and it might be easy because broadband noise contains more sound cues (ITDs and ILDs; Middlebrooks & Green, 1991). In fact, previous studies found that the overall localization accuracy of broadband noise was near ceiling unlike pure-tone noise, which was localized less accurately (Abel et al., 2000; Dobreva et al., 2000). If this study compared broadband noise with pure-tone noise, which contains less sound cues, age differences in localization accuracy may be apparent. This study also did not find age differences in localization accuracy across the different types of attentional cues. Broadband noise preceded by valid cues had lower localization errors and smaller biases compared to neutral cues and invalid cues in both younger and older adults. This supported the hypothesis that the broadband noise was localized more accurately with valid cues; however, this did not support the hypothesis that the valid cues improved localization accuracy proportionally more in older adults than in younger adults. As proposed previously, the broadband noise may be easy to localize for both younger and older adults, therefore further information about its location may not significantly improve accuracy. These findings compliment the literature on selective attention and aging (e.g., Groth & Allen, 2000), though. Because older and younger adults had similar localization errors and biases across the types of attentional cues, this suggests that older adults were as capable as younger adults in employing selective attention to improve performance (e.g., Geuerreiro et al., 2014). Specifically, older adults can distribute attentional processing resources based on prior knowledge of the sound and respond more accurately (e.g., Groth & Allen, 2000), similar to younger adults (Golob et al., 2002; Golob & Holmes, 2011). Based on the zoom-lens analogy (Eriksen & St. James, 24

30 1986), the valid attentional cue may have allowed participants to direct attentional processing resources towards the attended location more efficiently. Consequently, localization responses were more accurate than when the attentional processing resources were focused at the wrong location or across the entire field (e.g., from -90 to +90 ). The current study also did not find age differences in localization accuracy across the five locations, indicating that younger and older adults made similar localization errors and biases at -90, -28, 0, +28, and +90. Particularly, this study found that localizing the broadband noise presented at -28 and +28 had greater errors and leftward and rightward biases, respectively (see Figure 7). This did not support the hypothesis that the broadband noise at -90 and +90 would be localized the least accurately, and is inconsistent with the previous literature (e.g., Freigang et al., 2014; 2015). Specifically, previous studies showed that localization accuracy declined as sounds were presented more laterally (Abel et al., 2000; Dobreva et al., 2011; Freigang et al., 2014). The answer to why the broadband noise presented at -90 and +90 were easier to localize than at -28 and +28 is not clear. One possibility is that the sound cues of the broadband noise at -90 and +90 were more clearly attenuated at one ear than the other ear. For instance, if the sound pressure level (ILD) was stronger in the left ear than in the right ear, there may be greater certainty that the sound was located on the left side. At -28 and +28, the differences of the sound pressure level between the left and right ear may be smaller; thus, the perceived location may be harder to determine. However, this does not completely explain the discrepancy from the literature. It is possible that the reverberation in the testing environment also influenced the perceived differences of the sound pressure level and arrival time. Previous studies were conducted in anechoic and semi-reverberant environments (e.g., Freigang et al., 2014); thus, the perceived sound cues were not influenced by acoustical factors such as reverberation. In this study, the reverberation times may have enhanced the sound cues at -90 and +90, thus improving localization accuracy at these locations, but reduced the sound cues at -28 and +28, therefore lowering localization accuracy at these locations. However, this study cannot ascertain this occurrence or specify its influence due to limitations in the current design (e.g., changes in the 25

31 sound pressure levels of the broadband noise were not measured). Future directions may include replicating this study in an acoustically-controlled environment (i.e., sound booth) to ascertain the differences between the lateral sound locations. 4.2 Response Time As predicted, this study showed that younger adults localized the broadband noise faster than older adults overall. This suggests that younger adults can detect and identify the location of the broadband noise on the schematic drawing faster than older adults. But because younger and older adults had comparable hearing thresholds (which may associate with detecting the sound cues) and similar localization accuracy (which may associate with integrating the sound cues to identify the location), differences in motor reflexes and/or familiarity with the electronic devices may better explain this finding. For instance, younger adults may have responded faster because they have better motor control of their hand/arm. Younger and older adults also had different patterns of response times with the attentional cues. While both groups localized broadband noise faster after valid cues, older adults responded slower after neutral cues than after invalid cues, and younger adults responded similarly between neutral and invalid cues (see Figure 11). Older adults may have found that any directional information is helpful, regardless of whether it was correct or incorrect, compared to not receiving directional information at all when it comes to responding quickly. These findings compliment the attention literature (e.g., Groth & Allen, 2000), such that responses after valid cues are known to be faster than responses after invalid cues and neutral cues (e.g., Posner, 1987; Golob et al., 2002; Madden et al., 1994; Spence & Driver, 1997), and extend the current literature on attention to auditory localization in older adults. However, it is unclear why older adults responded slower after neutral cues than after invalid cues, as studies have shown that responses after invalid cues are typically slower than after neutral cues (e.g., Golob et al., 2002; Groth and Allen, 2000). One possibility, based on the zoom-lens analogy (Eriksen and St. James, 1986), is that the attentional processing resources were spread too sparsely across the testing environment in neutral-cued trials. When the attentional cue was invalid, the processing resources were at least focused together, which may have allowed the older adults to shift to the new attended location more efficiently. Meanwhile, 26

32 whether the processing resources were focused together or spread apart may not have mattered to younger adults, who may have compensated for this with better motor and/or cognitive processes (i.e., quickly re-positioning their hand to tap the correct location and/or attending to the more relevant location). Finally, this study found that younger and older adults might have variable response times between the locations, which were not expected. In fact, previous studies on auditory localization have not reported age differences in localizing sounds in terms of response time (Abel et al., 2000; Dobreva et al., 2011; Freigang et al., 2014). In this study, both groups localized the broadband noise on the right side (+28 and +90 ) faster than on the left side (-28 and -90 ), but interestingly, older adults also responded slower when the broadband noise was presented directly ahead (0 ; see Figure 12). Since attentional cues did not influence response times between the locations, it is unlikely that the size of the attended field or the distribution of the processing resources played any role in this finding. This may instead indicate the right side of the tablet was easier to respond quickly than on the left side of the tablet. For older adults, quickly pinpointing a location close to 0 on the tablet may also be difficult. 4.3 Summary of the Results Overall, the findings suggest that older adults are able to modulate attentional processing resources to improve auditory localization performances, similar to younger adults. Specifically, when older adults direct their attention towards the upcoming target location, they may localize the target faster and with less errors and less biases that are comparable to younger adults. However, this study found that older adults responded slower to targets that were preceded by neutral cues, suggesting that some directional information may be better than no directional information. Some of the discrepancy from the reviewed literature may be due to using broadband noise and not accounting for acoustical factors in the environment and other agerelated sensory and cognitive changes. A follow-up study would be to include high- and lowfrequency noise, and replicate the design in an anechoic environment. This will expand on the findings on selective attention in auditory localization and aging, and address potential acoustical confounds in the environment. 27

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions Binaural Hearing Why two ears? Locating sounds in space: acuity is poorer than in vision by up to two orders of magnitude, but extends in all directions. Role in alerting and orienting? Separating sound

More information

Effect of spectral content and learning on auditory distance perception

Effect of spectral content and learning on auditory distance perception Effect of spectral content and learning on auditory distance perception Norbert Kopčo 1,2, Dávid Čeljuska 1, Miroslav Puszta 1, Michal Raček 1 a Martin Sarnovský 1 1 Department of Cybernetics and AI, Technical

More information

Trading Directional Accuracy for Realism in a Virtual Auditory Display

Trading Directional Accuracy for Realism in a Virtual Auditory Display Trading Directional Accuracy for Realism in a Virtual Auditory Display Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham, I-Fan Lin, and Tim Streeter Hearing Research Center, Boston University 677 Beacon St., Boston, MA 02215

More information

Sound Localization PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 31. Audition

Sound Localization PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 31. Audition Sound Localization PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 31 Physics and psychology. Audition We now have some idea of how sound properties are recorded by the auditory system So, we know what kind of information

More information

Sound localization psychophysics

Sound localization psychophysics Sound localization psychophysics Eric Young A good reference: B.C.J. Moore An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing Chapter 7, Space Perception. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 233-267 (2004). Sound localization:

More information

CLASSROOM AMPLIFICATION: WHO CARES? AND WHY SHOULD WE? James Blair and Jeffery Larsen Utah State University ASHA, San Diego, 2011

CLASSROOM AMPLIFICATION: WHO CARES? AND WHY SHOULD WE? James Blair and Jeffery Larsen Utah State University ASHA, San Diego, 2011 CLASSROOM AMPLIFICATION: WHO CARES? AND WHY SHOULD WE? James Blair and Jeffery Larsen Utah State University ASHA, San Diego, 2011 What we know Classroom amplification has been reported to be an advantage

More information

The Effects of Action on Perception. Andriana Tesoro. California State University, Long Beach

The Effects of Action on Perception. Andriana Tesoro. California State University, Long Beach ACTION ON PERCEPTION 1 The Effects of Action on Perception Andriana Tesoro California State University, Long Beach ACTION ON PERCEPTION 2 The Effects of Action on Perception Perception is a process that

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

EFFECTS OF TEMPORAL FINE STRUCTURE ON THE LOCALIZATION OF BROADBAND SOUNDS: POTENTIAL IMPLICATIONS FOR THE DESIGN OF SPATIAL AUDIO DISPLAYS

EFFECTS OF TEMPORAL FINE STRUCTURE ON THE LOCALIZATION OF BROADBAND SOUNDS: POTENTIAL IMPLICATIONS FOR THE DESIGN OF SPATIAL AUDIO DISPLAYS Proceedings of the 14 International Conference on Auditory Display, Paris, France June 24-27, 28 EFFECTS OF TEMPORAL FINE STRUCTURE ON THE LOCALIZATION OF BROADBAND SOUNDS: POTENTIAL IMPLICATIONS FOR THE

More information

Hearing in the Environment

Hearing in the Environment 10 Hearing in the Environment Click Chapter to edit 10 Master Hearing title in the style Environment Sound Localization Complex Sounds Auditory Scene Analysis Continuity and Restoration Effects Auditory

More information

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean?

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean? 3-D Sound and Spatial Audio What do these terms mean? Both terms are very general. 3-D sound usually implies the perception of point sources in 3-D space (could also be 2-D plane) whether the audio reproduction

More information

IN EAR TO OUT THERE: A MAGNITUDE BASED PARAMETERIZATION SCHEME FOR SOUND SOURCE EXTERNALIZATION. Griffin D. Romigh, Brian D. Simpson, Nandini Iyer

IN EAR TO OUT THERE: A MAGNITUDE BASED PARAMETERIZATION SCHEME FOR SOUND SOURCE EXTERNALIZATION. Griffin D. Romigh, Brian D. Simpson, Nandini Iyer IN EAR TO OUT THERE: A MAGNITUDE BASED PARAMETERIZATION SCHEME FOR SOUND SOURCE EXTERNALIZATION Griffin D. Romigh, Brian D. Simpson, Nandini Iyer 711th Human Performance Wing Air Force Research Laboratory

More information

INTRODUCTION TO PURE (AUDIOMETER & TESTING ENVIRONMENT) TONE AUDIOMETERY. By Mrs. Wedad Alhudaib with many thanks to Mrs.

INTRODUCTION TO PURE (AUDIOMETER & TESTING ENVIRONMENT) TONE AUDIOMETERY. By Mrs. Wedad Alhudaib with many thanks to Mrs. INTRODUCTION TO PURE TONE AUDIOMETERY (AUDIOMETER & TESTING ENVIRONMENT) By Mrs. Wedad Alhudaib with many thanks to Mrs. Tahani Alothman Topics : This lecture will incorporate both theoretical information

More information

Aging and Hearing Loss: Why does it Matter?

Aging and Hearing Loss: Why does it Matter? Aging and Hearing Loss: Why does it Matter? Kathy Pichora-Fuller Professor, Psychology, University of Toronto Adjunct Scientist, Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, University Health Network Adjunct Scientist,

More information

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Hearing-aids Induce Plasticity in the Auditory System: Perspectives From Three Research Designs and Personal Speculations About the

More information

21/01/2013. Binaural Phenomena. Aim. To understand binaural hearing Objectives. Understand the cues used to determine the location of a sound source

21/01/2013. Binaural Phenomena. Aim. To understand binaural hearing Objectives. Understand the cues used to determine the location of a sound source Binaural Phenomena Aim To understand binaural hearing Objectives Understand the cues used to determine the location of a sound source Understand sensitivity to binaural spatial cues, including interaural

More information

Reference: Mark S. Sanders and Ernest J. McCormick. Human Factors Engineering and Design. McGRAW-HILL, 7 TH Edition. NOISE

Reference: Mark S. Sanders and Ernest J. McCormick. Human Factors Engineering and Design. McGRAW-HILL, 7 TH Edition. NOISE NOISE NOISE: It is considered in an information-theory context, as that auditory stimulus or stimuli bearing no informational relationship to the presence or completion of the immediate task. Human ear

More information

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS 19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 27 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS PACS: 43.66.Pn Seeber, Bernhard U. Auditory Perception Lab, Dept.

More information

Binaural processing of complex stimuli

Binaural processing of complex stimuli Binaural processing of complex stimuli Outline for today Binaural detection experiments and models Speech as an important waveform Experiments on understanding speech in complex environments (Cocktail

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Angular Resolution of Human Sound Localization

Angular Resolution of Human Sound Localization Angular Resolution of Human Sound Localization By Simon Skluzacek A senior thesis submitted to the Carthage College Physics & Astronomy Department in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Bachelor

More information

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL?

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL? 1 Psychoacoustics What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL? The decibel (db ) is a logarithmic unit of measurement used to express the magnitude of a sound relative to some reference level. Decibels

More information

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths Jennifer J. Lentz a Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington,

More information

Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation

Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation Mitchell L. Day 1,2 * and Bertrand Delgutte 1,2,3 1 Department of Otology and Laryngology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA.

More information

Localization 103: Training BiCROS/CROS Wearers for Left-Right Localization

Localization 103: Training BiCROS/CROS Wearers for Left-Right Localization Localization 103: Training BiCROS/CROS Wearers for Left-Right Localization Published on June 16, 2015 Tech Topic: Localization July 2015 Hearing Review By Eric Seper, AuD, and Francis KuK, PhD While the

More information

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear Pitch & Binaural listening Review 25 20 15 10 5 0-5 100 1000 10000 25 20 15 10 5 0-5 100 1000 10000 Part I: Auditory frequency selectivity Tuning

More information

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA) Comments Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA) Is phase locking to transposed stimuli as good as phase locking to low-frequency

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Volume 19, 2013 http://acousticalsociety.org/ ICA 2013 Montreal Montreal, Canada 2-7 June 2013 Psychological and Physiological Acoustics Session 4aPPb: Binaural Hearing

More information

Sound Workshop. What is sound Longitudinal Waves Frequency and pitch Hearing ranges Sounds in solids, liquids and gases Sound in a vacuum

Sound Workshop. What is sound Longitudinal Waves Frequency and pitch Hearing ranges Sounds in solids, liquids and gases Sound in a vacuum Sound Workshop a. b. c. d. e. f. g. h. i. j. k. l. What is sound Longitudinal Waves Frequency and pitch Hearing ranges Sounds in solids, liquids and gases Sound in a vacuum Echoes Ultrasound Loudspeakers

More information

Hearing. Figure 1. The human ear (from Kessel and Kardon, 1979)

Hearing. Figure 1. The human ear (from Kessel and Kardon, 1979) Hearing The nervous system s cognitive response to sound stimuli is known as psychoacoustics: it is partly acoustics and partly psychology. Hearing is a feature resulting from our physiology that we tend

More information

Perceptual Plasticity in Spatial Auditory Displays

Perceptual Plasticity in Spatial Auditory Displays Perceptual Plasticity in Spatial Auditory Displays BARBARA G. SHINN-CUNNINGHAM, TIMOTHY STREETER, and JEAN-FRANÇOIS GYSS Hearing Research Center, Boston University Often, virtual acoustic environments

More information

Attention and Scene Perception

Attention and Scene Perception Theories of attention Techniques for studying scene perception Physiological basis of attention Attention and single cells Disorders of attention Scene recognition attention any of a large set of selection

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

3-D SOUND IMAGE LOCALIZATION BY INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES AND THE MEDIAN PLANE HRTF. Masayuki Morimoto Motokuni Itoh Kazuhiro Iida

3-D SOUND IMAGE LOCALIZATION BY INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES AND THE MEDIAN PLANE HRTF. Masayuki Morimoto Motokuni Itoh Kazuhiro Iida 3-D SOUND IMAGE LOCALIZATION BY INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES AND THE MEDIAN PLANE HRTF Masayuki Morimoto Motokuni Itoh Kazuhiro Iida Kobe University Environmental Acoustics Laboratory Rokko, Nada, Kobe, 657-8501,

More information

Infant Hearing Development: Translating Research Findings into Clinical Practice. Auditory Development. Overview

Infant Hearing Development: Translating Research Findings into Clinical Practice. Auditory Development. Overview Infant Hearing Development: Translating Research Findings into Clinical Practice Lori J. Leibold Department of Allied Health Sciences The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Auditory Development

More information

Consonant Perception test

Consonant Perception test Consonant Perception test Introduction The Vowel-Consonant-Vowel (VCV) test is used in clinics to evaluate how well a listener can recognize consonants under different conditions (e.g. with and without

More information

User Guide. For Jacoti Hearing Center Pro Version 1.0. Manufacture Year 2017

User Guide. For Jacoti Hearing Center Pro Version 1.0. Manufacture Year 2017 User Guide For Jacoti Hearing Center Pro Version 1.0 Manufacture Year 2017 Revision 26/10/2017 Table of contents 1_ Intended Use... 3 1.1_ Requirements... 3 1.2_ Headphones... 3 1.3_ Startup... 4 1.4_

More information

Lindsay De Souza M.Cl.Sc AUD Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Lindsay De Souza M.Cl.Sc AUD Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: Do Personal FM Systems Improve Speech Perception Ability for Aided and/or Unaided Pediatric Listeners with Minimal to Mild, and/or Unilateral Hearing Loss? Lindsay De Souza M.Cl.Sc AUD

More information

B. G. Shinn-Cunningham Hearing Research Center, Departments of Biomedical Engineering and Cognitive and Neural Systems, Boston, Massachusetts 02215

B. G. Shinn-Cunningham Hearing Research Center, Departments of Biomedical Engineering and Cognitive and Neural Systems, Boston, Massachusetts 02215 Investigation of the relationship among three common measures of precedence: Fusion, localization dominance, and discrimination suppression R. Y. Litovsky a) Boston University Hearing Research Center,

More information

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization Acoust. Sci. & Tech. 24, 2 (23) PAPER The role of low components in median plane localization Masayuki Morimoto 1;, Motoki Yairi 1, Kazuhiro Iida 2 and Motokuni Itoh 1 1 Environmental Acoustics Laboratory,

More information

9.3 Sound The frequency of sound Frequency and pitch pitch Most sound has more than one frequency The frequency spectrum

9.3 Sound The frequency of sound Frequency and pitch pitch Most sound has more than one frequency The frequency spectrum 9.3 Sound Like other waves, sound has frequency, wavelength, amplitude, and speed. Because sound is part of your daily experience, you already know its properties but by different names. You may never

More information

HEARING CONSERVATION PROGRAM

HEARING CONSERVATION PROGRAM CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, CHICO HEARING CONSERVATION PROGRAM PREPARED BY THE OFFICE OF ENVIRONMENTAL HEALTH AND SAFETY REVISED June 2008 TABLE OF CONTENTS Section Page 1.0 Introduction... 1-1 2.0 Exposure

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Chapter 17 Sound Sound and Hearing. Properties of Sound Waves 1/20/2017. Pearson Prentice Hall Physical Science: Concepts in Action

Chapter 17 Sound Sound and Hearing. Properties of Sound Waves 1/20/2017. Pearson Prentice Hall Physical Science: Concepts in Action Pearson Prentice Hall Physical Science: Concepts in Action Chapter 17 Sound Standing Waves in Music When the string of a violin is played with a bow, it vibrates and creates standing waves. Some instruments,

More information

My child with a cochlear implant (CI)

My child with a cochlear implant (CI) My child with a cochlear implant (CI) Introduction Introduce yourself and your child, and thank the participants for coming. State the goals of the meeting: Get to know the people who will support your

More information

REFERRAL AND DIAGNOSTIC EVALUATION OF HEARING ACUITY. Better Hearing Philippines Inc.

REFERRAL AND DIAGNOSTIC EVALUATION OF HEARING ACUITY. Better Hearing Philippines Inc. REFERRAL AND DIAGNOSTIC EVALUATION OF HEARING ACUITY Better Hearing Philippines Inc. How To Get Started? 1. Testing must be done in an acoustically treated environment far from all the environmental noises

More information

TOWN OF FAIRFIELD PUBLIC HEALTH NURSING. MANUAL: School Health APPROVED BY: Board of Health School Medical Advisor

TOWN OF FAIRFIELD PUBLIC HEALTH NURSING. MANUAL: School Health APPROVED BY: Board of Health School Medical Advisor TOWN OF FAIRFIELD PUBLIC HEALTH NURSING MANUAL: School Health APPROVED BY: Board of Health School Medical Advisor POLICY: Audiometric Screening DATE REVISED AND APPROVED: 8-14-95, 3-13-06, 7-2015 I. Purpose

More information

Hear Better With FM. Get more from everyday situations. Life is on

Hear Better With FM. Get more from everyday situations. Life is on Hear Better With FM Get more from everyday situations Life is on We are sensitive to the needs of everyone who depends on our knowledge, ideas and care. And by creatively challenging the limits of technology,

More information

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 1. Laboratory session: Measuring thresholds

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 1. Laboratory session: Measuring thresholds Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear Week 1 Laboratory session: Measuring thresholds What s the most commonly used piece of electronic equipment in the audiological clinic? The Audiometer And what is

More information

Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults

Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults Daniel Fogerty Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of South Carolina, Columbia, South

More information

A Comparison of Baseline Hearing Thresholds Between Pilots and Non-Pilots and the Effects of Engine Noise

A Comparison of Baseline Hearing Thresholds Between Pilots and Non-Pilots and the Effects of Engine Noise DOT/FAA/AM-05/12 Office of Aerospace Medicine Washington, DC 20591 A Comparison of Baseline Hearing Thresholds Between Pilots and Non-Pilots and the Effects of Engine Noise Dennis B. Beringer Howard C.

More information

Science - Year 4. Sound Block 4S. Listen Up! Session 2 Resource Pack

Science - Year 4. Sound Block 4S. Listen Up! Session 2 Resource Pack Science - Year 4 Sound Block 4S Listen Up! Session 2 Resource Pack Original resource copyright Hamilton Trust, who give permission for it to be adapted as wished by individual users. We refer you to our

More information

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly Auditory Temporal Processes 1 Ben-Artzi, E., Babkoff, H., Fostick, L. (2011). Auditory temporal processes in the elderly. Audiology Research, 1, 21-23 ***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully

More information

Auditory System & Hearing

Auditory System & Hearing Auditory System & Hearing Chapters 9 and 10 Lecture 17 Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Spring 2015 1 Cochlea: physical device tuned to frequency! place code: tuning of different

More information

HearIntelligence by HANSATON. Intelligent hearing means natural hearing.

HearIntelligence by HANSATON. Intelligent hearing means natural hearing. HearIntelligence by HANSATON. HearIntelligence by HANSATON. Intelligent hearing means natural hearing. Acoustic environments are complex. We are surrounded by a variety of different acoustic signals, speech

More information

ESD 113 Sound to Harbor Head Start/ECEAP Procedure 620 Page 1 of 6 HEALTH SCREENINGS PROCEDURE. Health Section

ESD 113 Sound to Harbor Head Start/ECEAP Procedure 620 Page 1 of 6 HEALTH SCREENINGS PROCEDURE. Health Section Page 1 of 6 HEALTH SCREENINGS PROCEDURE 1. Head Start children will be screened within 45 days of enrollment and ECEAP children will be screened within 90 days of enrollment. ECEAP children at Head Start

More information

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the 1 Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 835-54. doi:10.1037/a0012726

More information

The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors

The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors PAPER The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors Waka Fujisaki 1;2;* and Makio Kashino 2; { 1 Graduate School of Humanities and Sciences, Ochanomizu University, 2 1 1 Ootsuka, Bunkyo-ku,

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

HST.723J, Spring 2005 Theme 3 Report

HST.723J, Spring 2005 Theme 3 Report HST.723J, Spring 2005 Theme 3 Report Madhu Shashanka shashanka@cns.bu.edu Introduction The theme of this report is binaural interactions. Binaural interactions of sound stimuli enable humans (and other

More information

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2004 Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude Tim Skinner University of South Florida Follow this and

More information

THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING

THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING Vanessa Surowiecki 1, vid Grayden 1, Richard Dowell

More information

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction Linda LeBlanc Trumpet Behavioral Health Laura Grow University of British Columbia Importance of Effective Teaching

More information

SPHSC 462 HEARING DEVELOPMENT. Overview Review of Hearing Science Introduction

SPHSC 462 HEARING DEVELOPMENT. Overview Review of Hearing Science Introduction SPHSC 462 HEARING DEVELOPMENT Overview Review of Hearing Science Introduction 1 Overview of course and requirements Lecture/discussion; lecture notes on website http://faculty.washington.edu/lawerner/sphsc462/

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Lin FR, Ferrucci L. Hearing loss and falls among older adults in the United States. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(4):369-371. eappendix. Audiometric Assessment and Other Study

More information

Communication with low-cost hearing protectors: hear, see and believe

Communication with low-cost hearing protectors: hear, see and believe 12th ICBEN Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem Communication with low-cost hearing protectors: hear, see and believe Annelies Bockstael 1,3, Lies De Clercq 2, Dick Botteldooren 3 1 Université

More information

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080 Perceptual segregation of a harmonic from a vowel by interaural time difference in conjunction with mistuning and onset asynchrony C. J. Darwin and R. W. Hukin Experimental Psychology, University of Sussex,

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

Influence of acoustic complexity on spatial release from masking and lateralization

Influence of acoustic complexity on spatial release from masking and lateralization Influence of acoustic complexity on spatial release from masking and lateralization Gusztáv Lőcsei, Sébastien Santurette, Torsten Dau, Ewen N. MacDonald Hearing Systems Group, Department of Electrical

More information

CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT

CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT PACS:..Hy Furuya, Hiroshi ; Wakuda, Akiko ; Anai, Ken ; Fujimoto, Kazutoshi Faculty of Engineering, Kyushu Kyoritsu University

More information

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Laboratory Equipment & Test Procedures. Motor learning and control historically has built its discipline through laboratory research. This has led to the

More information

Cochlear implant patients localization using interaural level differences exceeds that of untrained normal hearing listeners

Cochlear implant patients localization using interaural level differences exceeds that of untrained normal hearing listeners Cochlear implant patients localization using interaural level differences exceeds that of untrained normal hearing listeners Justin M. Aronoff a) Communication and Neuroscience Division, House Research

More information

MOVEMENT PREPARATION LAB. Name: Score: Activity I: Predictability of the correct response choice & Influence of Pre cueing

MOVEMENT PREPARATION LAB. Name: Score: Activity I: Predictability of the correct response choice & Influence of Pre cueing MOVEMENT PREPARATION LAB Name: Score: Activity I: Predictability of the correct response choice & Influence of Pre cueing Introduction: Research evidence has consistently shown that as the predictability

More information

Unit 4: Sensation and Perception

Unit 4: Sensation and Perception Unit 4: Sensation and Perception Sensation a process by which our sensory receptors and nervous system receive and represent stimulus (or physical) energy and encode it as neural signals. Perception a

More information

Bone Conduction Microphone: A Head Mapping Pilot Study

Bone Conduction Microphone: A Head Mapping Pilot Study McNair Scholars Research Journal Volume 1 Article 11 2014 Bone Conduction Microphone: A Head Mapping Pilot Study Rafael N. Patrick Embry-Riddle Aeronautical University Follow this and additional works

More information

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1 Vision and Action Our ability to move thru our environment is closely tied to visual perception. Simple examples include standing one one foot. It is easier to maintain balance with the eyes open than

More information

Recommended Procedure. Industrial Audiometry

Recommended Procedure. Industrial Audiometry Recommended Procedure Industrial Audiometry Date: November 2012 Date for review: November 2018 General foreword This document presents a Recommended Procedure by the British Society of Audiology (BSA).

More information

Masker-signal relationships and sound level

Masker-signal relationships and sound level Chapter 6: Masking Masking Masking: a process in which the threshold of one sound (signal) is raised by the presentation of another sound (masker). Masking represents the difference in decibels (db) between

More information

L G Davison*, BSc C Barlow, PhD, MIOA M Ashmore, RHAD, MIOA R Weinstein, RHAD Southampton Solent University Faculty of Maritime and Technology

L G Davison*, BSc C Barlow, PhD, MIOA M Ashmore, RHAD, MIOA R Weinstein, RHAD Southampton Solent University Faculty of Maritime and Technology Effectiveness of a New Tablet Based Hearing Screening System Compared to Traditional Audiometry, Among a Cohort of Individuals With a High Incidence of Hearing Loss August 7, 213 L G Davison*, BSc C Barlow,

More information

ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION

ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION ABSTRACT Forty-four research participants completed a study that evaluated speech understanding in noise while wearing S Series hearing aids. One of the experimental questions asked if directional benefit

More information

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Volume 19, 2013 http://acousticalsociety.org/ ICA 2013 Montreal Montreal, Canada 2-7 June 2013 Psychological and Physiological Acoustics Session 2pPPb: Speech. Attention,

More information

Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles

Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (5), 945-949 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.5.945 Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles Xingshan Li and Gordon D. Logan Vanderbilt

More information

AMBCO 1000+P AUDIOMETER

AMBCO 1000+P AUDIOMETER Model 1000+ Printer User Manual AMBCO 1000+P AUDIOMETER AMBCO ELECTRONICS 15052 REDHILL AVE SUITE #D TUSTIN, CA 92780 (714) 259-7930 FAX (714) 259-1688 WWW.AMBCO.COM 10-1004, Rev. A DCO 17 008, 11 13 17

More information

Minimum Audible Angles Measured with Simulated Normally-Sized and Oversized Pinnas for Normal-Hearing and Hearing- Impaired Test Subjects

Minimum Audible Angles Measured with Simulated Normally-Sized and Oversized Pinnas for Normal-Hearing and Hearing- Impaired Test Subjects Minimum Audible Angles Measured with Simulated Normally-Sized and Oversized Pinnas for Normal-Hearing and Hearing- Impaired Test Subjects Filip M. Rønne, Søren Laugesen, Niels S. Jensen and Julie H. Pedersen

More information

Spatial processing in adults with hearing loss

Spatial processing in adults with hearing loss Spatial processing in adults with hearing loss Harvey Dillon Helen Glyde Sharon Cameron, Louise Hickson, Mark Seeto, Jörg Buchholz, Virginia Best creating sound value TM www.hearingcrc.org Spatial processing

More information

Lauer et al Olivocochlear efferents. Amanda M. Lauer, Ph.D. Dept. of Otolaryngology-HNS

Lauer et al Olivocochlear efferents. Amanda M. Lauer, Ph.D. Dept. of Otolaryngology-HNS Lauer et al. 2012 Olivocochlear efferents Amanda M. Lauer, Ph.D. Dept. of Otolaryngology-HNS May 30, 2016 Overview Structural organization Responses Hypothesized roles in hearing Olivocochlear efferent

More information

Improve localization accuracy and natural listening with Spatial Awareness

Improve localization accuracy and natural listening with Spatial Awareness Improve localization accuracy and natural listening with Spatial Awareness While you probably don t even notice them, your localization skills make everyday tasks easier: like finding your ringing phone

More information

Lecture 9: Sound Localization

Lecture 9: Sound Localization Lecture 9: Sound Localization Localization refers to the process of using the information about a sound which you get from your ears, to work out where the sound came from (above, below, in front, behind,

More information

TIPS FOR TEACHING A STUDENT WHO IS DEAF/HARD OF HEARING

TIPS FOR TEACHING A STUDENT WHO IS DEAF/HARD OF HEARING http://mdrl.educ.ualberta.ca TIPS FOR TEACHING A STUDENT WHO IS DEAF/HARD OF HEARING 1. Equipment Use: Support proper and consistent equipment use: Hearing aids and cochlear implants should be worn all

More information

A Novel Software Solution to Diagnose the Hearing Disabilities In Human Beings

A Novel Software Solution to Diagnose the Hearing Disabilities In Human Beings A Novel Software Solution to Diagnose the Hearing Disabilities In Human Beings Prithvi B S Dept of Computer Science & Engineering SVIT, Bangalore bsprithvi1992@gmail.com Sanjay H S Research Scholar, Jain

More information

9.3 Sound. The frequency of sound. pitch - the perception of high or low that you hear at different frequencies of sound.

9.3 Sound. The frequency of sound. pitch - the perception of high or low that you hear at different frequencies of sound. 9.3 Sound Like other waves, sound has frequency, wavelength, amplitude, and speed. Because sound is part of your daily experience, you already know its properties but by different names. You may never

More information

Enhanced visual perception near the hands

Enhanced visual perception near the hands Enhanced visual perception near the hands Bachelor thesis Marina Meinert (s0163430) Supervisors: 1 st supervisor: Prof. Dr. Ing. W. B. Verwey 2 nd supervisor: Dr. M. L. Noordzij External supervisor: Dr.

More information

Emil discovers the world of sounds

Emil discovers the world of sounds Emil discovers the world of sounds Emil discovers the world of sounds Imprint: Bundesinnung der Hörakustiker (biha) KdöR, [Federal Guild of Hearing Aid Professionals] Wallstrasse 5 55122 Mainz, Germany

More information

On the influence of interaural differences on onset detection in auditory object formation. 1 Introduction

On the influence of interaural differences on onset detection in auditory object formation. 1 Introduction On the influence of interaural differences on onset detection in auditory object formation Othmar Schimmel Eindhoven University of Technology, P.O. Box 513 / Building IPO 1.26, 56 MD Eindhoven, The Netherlands,

More information

Recommended Procedure. Industrial Audiometry

Recommended Procedure. Industrial Audiometry Recommended Procedure Date of version: September Date for review: September 2018 Recommended Procedure General foreword This document presents a Recommended Procedure by the British Society of Audiology

More information

Sensation and Perception

Sensation and Perception 1 Sensation and Perception DR. ARNEL BANAGA SALGADO, Doctor of Psychology (USA) FPM (Ph.D.) Psychology (India) Doctor of Education (Phl) Master of Arts in Nursing (Phl) Master of Arts in Teaching Psychology

More information

How high-frequency do children hear?

How high-frequency do children hear? How high-frequency do children hear? Mari UEDA 1 ; Kaoru ASHIHARA 2 ; Hironobu TAKAHASHI 2 1 Kyushu University, Japan 2 National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology, Japan ABSTRACT

More information

ADHEAR The new bone-conduction hearing aid innovation

ADHEAR The new bone-conduction hearing aid innovation ADHEAR The new bone-conduction hearing aid innovation MED-EL has world-wide launched a new kind of hearing aid, ADHEAR, for people who have an hearing impairment and want to prevent surgery. This little

More information