Making remembering more memorable

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Making remembering more memorable"

Transcription

1 Memory ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: Making remembering more memorable Marcus L. Leppanen & Keith B. Lyle To cite this article: Marcus L. Leppanen & Keith B. Lyle (2018) Making remembering more memorable, Memory, 26:7, , DOI: / To link to this article: Published online: 01 Jan Submit your article to this journal Article views: 109 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at

2 MEMORY 2018, VOL. 26, NO. 7, Making remembering more memorable* Marcus L. Leppanen and Keith B. Lyle Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, USA ABSTRACT Memory retrieval is a cognitive operation that itself can be remembered or forgotten, with potentially important consequences. To study memory for prior remembering, we had participants first study target words (e.g., bark) alongside semantically related cue words (e.g., dog). Then, on Test 1, participants retrieved targets in response to either the study cue or a changed cue that was semantically related to a homograph of the target (e.g., birch). Finally, on Test 2, participants retrieved all targets in response to the original study cues, and participants judged whether targets were previously retrieved on Test 1. As in previous research, cue change on Test 1 rendered target retrievals less memorable, suggesting context changes harm memory for prior remembering. We hypothesised that the negative effect of context change could be ameliorated by reminding participants of the original study cues during Test 1. We had participants either retrieve (Experiments 1 and 3, Ns = 46 and 62) or view (Experiment 2, N = 118) the study cue following each target retrieval. Reminding significantly reduced the negative effect of cue change, with self-generation being especially potent. This indicates that reminding can make remembering more memorable in the face of context change. ARTICLE HISTORY Received 27 June 2017 Accepted 21 December 2017 KEYWORDS Memory for prior remembering; reminding; context; cued recall Think about the last memory you retrieved before reading this paper. Was it a memory you had retrieved many times before (e.g., of an important conversation you had with a colleague last week and have been thinking about ever since) or one you were recalling for the first time? Recalling previous occasions of memory retrieval has been called remembering prior remembering (Arnold & Lindsay, 2002). Failure to remember prior remembering can have various consequences. In everyday life, for example, forgetting prior remembering might lead people to repeatedly share the same memory with a conversational partner an experience that can elicit emotional reactions ranging from mere embarrassment to serious doubt about one s cognitive health. A more dramatic example of what can happen when people forget prior remembering is provided by Schooler (2001), who documented cases in which victims of childhood sexual abuse forgot instances of remembering their abuse. This led individuals to falsely believe that they had repressed memories for long periods of time an unsettling distortion of selfknowledge. In addition to personal consequences, remembering prior remembering also has implications for research on studies of involuntary and intrusive memories (Bernsten, 2010), which often rely on participants self-reports of whether and how often events have been remembered. Unless people document their recollections immediately after they happen, self-reports will necessarily have a retrospective aspect and be dependent on people s ability to remember prior remembering (for related commentary, see Hintzman, 2011). Forgetting prior remembering therefore poses a threat to the validity of research on involuntary and intrusive remembering (for related discussion, see Takarangi, Strange, & Lindsay, 2014). The variety of potential consequences associated with forgetting prior remembering motivates us to understand the circumstances under which it is more or less likely to occur. Prior research indicates that one cause of forgetting prior remembering is change in retrieval context over time. Evidence comes from the dominant laboratory paradigm used to study memory for prior remembering (Arnold & Lindsay, 2002). This paradigm comprises a study phase followed by two distinct test phases. First, participants are presented with semantically related cue-target pairs (e.g., hand palm, dog bark). Test 1 comprises cued recall of target words. Cues for some targets are the same as at study (e.g., hand is the cue for palm) while cues for other targets are changed to a word that is semantically related to a homograph of the target (e.g., dog is changed to birch). Other targets are not tested at all in this phase. Test 2 comprises a second round of cued recall. All cues are the same as at study and all targets are tested. The manipulation of cue identity on Test 1 CONTACT Marcus L. Leppanen marcus.leppanen@louisville.edu Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, 2301 S. 3rd Street, Louisville, KY 40292, USA *Results of this research were presented, in part, at the fifty-sixth annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group

3 MEMORY 961 means that, for the targets that were tested, there is either a semantic change in retrieval context between tests or there is not. After attempting retrieval of each target on Test 2, participants are asked whether the target was also retrieved on Test 1; this constitutes the critical test of memory for prior remembering. The key finding is that participants are less likely to remember retrieving targets on Test 1 when the cue changed between tests versus remained the same (sometimes called the forgot-it-allalong effect), suggesting that semantic changes in retrieval context harm memory for prior remembering (Arnold & Lindsay, 2002, 2005; Raymaekers, Peters, Smeets, Abidi, & Merckelbach, 2011). Negative effects of retrieval-context change are not particularly surprising if we assume, as Arnold and Lindsay (2002) did, that memory for prior remembering is governed by the same principles that govern memory for other types of events. One of these principles is that events are more likely to be remembered when retrieval contexts closely resemble encoding contexts (e.g., Godden & Baddeley, 1975; Smith, Glenberg, & Bjork, 1978; Tulving & Thomson, 1973). In Arnold and Lindsay s(2002) procedure, the critical to-be-remembered events are instances of memory retrieval that occur during Test 1. These are encoded in the specific context of Test 1 cues and, presumably, neural activity associated with acts of retrieval is bound, via medial-temporal-lobe processes (Squire & Zola, 1996), to Test 1 cues. The retrieval context for these acts is established by presentation of Test 2 cues. The reason changed cues (versus ones that are the same as on Test 1) are often ineffective at providing access to previously encoded retrieval acts is presumably that changed cues were not present during Test 1 and therefore were not bound to retrieval-related neural activity that occurred on Test 1, essentially violating the encoding-specificity principle (Tulving & Thomson, 1973). The encoding-specificity perspective described above suggests a means of increasing memory for prior remembering, at least under the conditions typically investigated using Arnold and Lindsay s (2002) procedure. In that procedure, as previously stated, Test 2 cues for all targets match study-phase cues. For example, if bark is initially studied with the cue dog, then the Test 1 cue may be either dog or birch, but the Test 2 cue will invariably be dog. Given this setup, suppose individuals were reminded of the study cue (i.e., dog) after retrieving bark during Test 1. Although reminding could happen on both sameand changed-cue trials, it is on changed-cue trials that it would presumably be most impactful because it would provide an opportunity to bind the study cue to the act of memory retrieval. This opportunity otherwise does not exist on changed-cue trials because the study cue never appears (unlike on same-cue trials, wherein the study cue and retrieval act may be bound together during the cued-recall attempt itself). Reminders could allow study cues, when presented on Test 2, to effectively cue memory for Test 1 retrievals, even when retrievals initially occurred in the context of a changed cue. Hence, our proposal is that reminding people of the study cue immediately following the Test 1 act of remembering might improve memory for prior remembering in the face of retrieval-context change (provided the study cue is reinstated on Test 2). A hypothetical real-world analogy might make our proposal easier to understand. Imagine you have an interesting experience with a waiter at Restaurant A (the encoding/study context). You later tell the story of that experience to a friend at Restaurant B, without dwelling on contextual details specific to Restaurant A (analogous to a changed Test 1 retrieval context). Later still, you and the same friend end up together at Restaurant A. Being back in Restaurant A might make you think of the experience you had with the waiter there (analogous to Test 2 retrieval in the original study context). Due to the change in retrieval context between the restaurants, you might not remember previously retrieving the story. Consequently, you might launch into a retelling of a story your friend has already heard. Such an accidental retelling would be less likely, we argue, if you had been specifically reminded of Restaurant A soon after you initially retrieved the story in Restaurant B (analogous to a study-cue reminder during Test 1 retrieval). We chose to examine the role of reminding in memory for prior remembering because it seems likely that, when remembering occurs under conditions of retrievalcontext change in everyday settings, it is sometimes accompanied by a reminder of the original context and sometimes not. For example, events encoded in an upsetting emotional context might be remembered in a calmer emotional context after the crisis has passed. When recalling the events, someone who experienced them with us may or may not be present to remind us of our original emotional state. A thorough understanding of the effects of retrieval-context change on memory for prior remembering should therefore take into account the presence or absence of reminders. The proposal that reminding might influence memory for prior remembering is consonant with recent arguments that reminding has wide-ranging implications for human cognition (Benjamin & Ross, 2010; Hintzman, 2011; Jacoby & Wahlheim, 2013; Putnam, Sungkhasettee, & Roediger, 2017; Wahlheim & Jacoby, 2013). As the cited authors have pointed out, when current events remind people of previous ones, it can have various beneficial effects, such as improving the quality of recency judgments, producing proactive facilitation (versus interference), and protecting people from the misinformation effect. If reminding also improves memory for prior remembering, it would support Arnold and Lindsay s (2002) assumption that remembering prior remembering operates on the same principles as remembering other types of information and it would provide additional evidence for what Jacoby and Wahlheim called the importance of looking back for human cognition.

4 962 M. L. LEPPANEN AND K. B. LYLE Our prediction that a reminder-based intervention will increase memory for prior remembering rests on the assumption that Arnold and Lindsay s (2002) procedure does not ordinarily involve a high rate of involuntary reminding. People are often reminded of past events involuntarily (Hintzman, 2011). One could imagine that, whenever people retrieve a target from the study phase, they are automatically reminded of the study cue, even if the Test 1 cue is different (e.g., being reminded of dog when bark is retrieved in response to birch). This sort of involuntary reminding might be expected based on the associativesymmetry hypothesis (Kahana, 2002), according to which A B word pairs (such as dog-bark) are represented holistically in memory and therefore B can bring to mind A, just as A can bring to mind B. If involuntary reminding normally occurs during Test 1 target retrieval, then explicit reminders of study cues during Test 1 would be redundant and there would be little reason to expect them to improve memory for prior remembering. We reasoned, however, that involuntary reminding is probably uncommon in Arnold and Lindsay s (2002) procedure, at least when Test 1 cues are changed from study. The changed-cue condition was intentionally developed to create a situation in which participants would think about target words differently on Test 1 than during study (i.e., participants were intended to think about the targets homographs on Test 1). We reasoned that activation of a target s homograph with its distinct conceptual and semantic features is unlikely to prompt spontaneous retrieval of the study cue (e.g., when thought of as part of a tree, bark is unlikely to remind people of the sound a dog makes). We therefore assumed that bringing reminding under task control (Jacoby & Wahlheim, 2013) would be necessary to make participants think of study cues during Test 1. The present experiments Three experiments (conducted in the order of report) utilised Arnold and Lindsay s (2002) basic procedure, but with additions to the Test 1 phase (see Figure 1 for a graphical depiction of our methodology). Our central question was whether the negative effect of Test 1 cue change on memory for prior remembering could be ameliorated by reminding participants of the study cue during their first attempt at retrieving a target. Typically, the Test 1 phase in studies utilising Arnold and Lindsay s procedure only requires participants to recall targets, with either the same cue as at study or a changed cue. To bring reminding under task control in the present research, we added a follow-up task to each Test 1 cued-recall attempt. The exact nature of this task differed across experiments. Details are provided in the Introduction and Method section of each experiment. In all experiments, we expected to obtain the typical finding, whereby memory for prior remembering would be poorer when Test 1 and Test 2 retrieval cues differed versus remained the same. However, we furthermore expected memory for prior remembering of changed-cue targets to be improved when participants were reminded of the study cue during Test 1 versus when they were not. The same pattern might be expected for same-cue targets, if the association between study cue and Test 1 retrieval were strengthened by retrieving the study cue, but prior research indicates that memory for prior remembering of same-cue targets is, on average, quite high (e.g., Arnold & Lindsay, 2002), potentially making increases negligible. Experiment 1 In our first experiment, we examined the effect of having participants remind themselves of study cues during Test 1 by prompting participants to recall the study cue following each Test 1 retrieval act. We assumed each participant would be able to recall some study cues but not others, allowing us to observe the impact of self-generated reminding within-participants. We furthermore assumed that when participants were able (versus unable) to bring the study cue to mind, it would create or strengthen an association between the study cue and the memory retrieval. The Test 2 phase was the same as in prior research, with participants recalling targets in response to cues from the study phase and then indicating whether they had retrieved each target on Test 1. We hypothesised that memory for prior remembering of changed-cue targets would be better following successful retrieval of study cues versus unsuccessful retrieval of study cues. Method Participants Participants were 46 undergraduates (33 women; aged years, M = 19.5) who earned course credit for participating. The procedure was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Louisville. Materials A set of 113 homographs were selected as target words (selection criteria are explained in Arnold & Lindsay, 2002). Four of these words served as primacy buffers and 4 as recency buffers in all experiments, leaving 105 critical pairs for analysis. Cue-target pairs in the study phase were formed by pairing one of two possible cues with each target. Critically, each target word was a homograph and each cue corresponded to one of two possible meanings for a given target (e.g., dog and birch were cues for the target bark). Assignment of cue to target was counterbalanced in the study phase such that each cue served equally often as the study cue. During Test 1, 35 targets were assigned to each of 3 conditions: tested with the study cue (same-cue trials), tested with the other possible cue (changed-cue trials), and not tested. Assignment of targets to Test 1 treatment was counterbalanced such that each target received each treatment for an equal

5 MEMORY 963 Figure 1. Visual representation of the experimental paradigm. Modified procedure from Arnold and Lindsay (2002). The sequence of events for a changedcue target are depicted. (A) The study phase was the same in all three experiments. (B) On all Test 1 trials, participants attempted cued recall of studied targets. (C) Cued recall was followed with an instruction to either recall the study cue (Experiments 1 and 3) or type the study cue that was provided on the screen (Experiment 2). In Experiment 3, prior to the instruction to recall the study cue, participants were informed whether the study cue was the same as or different than the just-presented test cue. (D) Test 2 was the same in all three experiments. See text for additional details. number of participants. All targets were tested with the study cue on Test 2, regardless of Test 1 treatment. Procedure Participants were tested in groups of up to five. Each participant completed a version of the Arnold and Lindsay (2002) procedure on an individual computer. The procedure was modified to be run without requiring an experimenter to record participants recall responses. During the study phase participants viewed 105 critical cue-target word pairs (e.g., hand-palm, birch-bark), preceded by 4 primacy buffers and followed by 4 recency buffers. Critical pairs were presented in a new random

6 964 M. L. LEPPANEN AND K. B. LYLE order for each participant. Each homographic target was paired with one of two possible cues (e.g., bark was paired with either dog or birch). On each trial a cue-target pair was presented in the centre of the screen for 2000 ms. The pair was then replaced with a sentence containing the cue word and three asterisks where the target word could logically go (e.g., I heard a dog *** next door). The sentence appeared on screen alone for 3500 ms and then the target word appeared above the sentence for an additional 1000 ms. After offset of the sentence and target, there was a 1000 ms inter-trial interval before the next trial. After the study phase, participants advanced immediately to the Test 1 phase. This phase comprised a cuedrecall test for two-thirds of the studied targets. Participants were informed that some targets would be cued with the original cue (same-cue targets), while others would be cued with a previously unseen word that was semantically related to a target (changed-cue targets). In the latter case, they were to respond with a target from the study phase. If participants could not recall a target, they were instructed to type pass. Participants were instructed that they would also be asked to retrieve the cue they originally studied with the target and were given an example of how to respond in the case of cues that remained the same and those that changed. If participants could not recall the cue from the study phase they were instructed to type pass. During each Test 1 trial participants first performed cued recall of a target. Cue-target pairs consisted of a cue and the first and last letters of the target word separated by dashes (e.g., birch b k). Participants were instructed to type the entire word. Following each cued-recall attempt, participants were given feedback on whether their recall was correct or incorrect. Then, on a separate screen, participants were instructed to Type the cue from the STUDY phase below and press (ENTER). After pressing the ENTER key the next trial began. After completion of Test 1, participants worked on a distractor task for five minutes. Participants then began Test 2, which participants were informed was another cued-recall test. Participants were informed they would be attempting to recall all the targets they had originally studied. Participants were furthermore told that all targets would be cued with words from the study phase. Participants responded to the cued-recall portion of each Test 2 trial identically as during Test 1. As during Test 1, following each cuedrecall attempt, participants were given feedback on whether their recall was correct or incorrect. However, feedback on Test 2 differed, with participants being shown the target when cued recall was incorrect. After retrieving a target participants were also asked: Did you retrieve this target during the first test? Yes or No and were told to respond irrespective of the cue the target had been paired with on Test 1. If a participant had not correctly retrieved a target on Test 1, or if they had responded with pass, they were instructed to respond No. After the Test 2 phase, participants were debriefed and dismissed. Results Subsections labelled Cued Recall of Targets and Effect of Cue Change on Memory for Prior Remembering are common to the Results sections of all three experiments. These subsections report effects typically obtained in Arnold and Lindsay s (2002) procedure. All dependent measures and analyses referenced in those subsections were the same across experiments, unless otherwise noted. Note that, because analyses were conditionalised on the presence of certain response types, and participants sometimes gave no responses of a given type, degrees of freedom sometimes differ between tests. Cued recall of targets We first examined cued recall of targets on Tests 1 and 2.Rememberthat,onTest1,cuesforsometargetswere the same as in the study phase, cues for others were changed, and still other targets were not tested at all. This manipulation had sensible effects on cued-recall performance. On Test 1, the proportion of targets recalled was significantly higher on same-cue trials (M =.80) than changed-cue trials (M =.69),t(45) = 6.16, p <.001, d =.95. We next examined the proportion of targets recalled on Test 2 (on which all cues were the same as in the study phase) as a function of how targets were treated on Test 1 (same-cue, changed-cue, or not tested) using a repeated-measures one-way ANOVA, which yielded a significant result, F(2, 90) = 6.90, p =.002, η 2 =.133. Recall did not differ between targets assigned to the same-cue or changed-cue conditions (Ms =.84 and.82, respectively), t(45) = 1.09, p =.28, d =.18, but these levels of recall were significantly higher than for targets that had not been tested (M =.79), smallest t(45) = 2.26, p =.03, d =.33. Superior recall of previously tested versus nottested targets is consistent with the idea that testing is an active process that increases the accessibility of memories (e.g., Roediger & Karpicke, 2006; van den Broek et al., 2016). Effect of cue change on memory for prior remembering We next analysed judgments of prior remembering as a function of retrieval-context stability or change. Table 1 shows the proportion (and raw number) of targets judged to have been retrieved on Test 1 when memory for prior remembering was queried on Test 2. Following Arnold and Lindsay (2002), we analysed proportion correct for only those targets successfully retrieved on both tests. Proportions correct were submitted, as in Arnold and Lindsay (2002), to a repeated-measures oneway ANOVA with Test 1 cue (same or changed) as the within-participants factor. Participants were significantly more likely to correctly indicate they had retrieved targets assigned to the same-cue condition (M =.93)

7 MEMORY 965 Table 1. Mean number of targets and mean proportion of targets judged as recalled as a function of recall status on Test 1 and Test 2 for Experiment 1. Test 1/Test 2 recall status Number of targets Proportion judged as recalled on Test 1 Same cue* Recalled/Recalled (.01) Recalled/Not recalled (.09) Not recalled/not (.05) recalled Not recalled/recalled (.06) Changed cue* Recalled/Recalled (.03) Recalled/Not recalled (.06) Not recalled/not (.04) recalled Not recalled/recalled (.05) Not tested* NA/Recalled (.06) NA/Not recalled (.03) Notes: NA = Not applicable. * Test 1 context. Numbers in parentheses are standard errors of the mean. Lines in bold are those for which statistical analyses are reported in the text. than the changed-cue condition (M =.71), F(1, 45) = 65.42, p <.001, η 2 =.592, confirming that retrieval-context change harms memory for prior remembering. Study-cue retrieval and its effect on memory for prior remembering We next analysed performance regarding the novel feature of this experiment: The requirement that participants retrieve the study cue after attempting target retrieval on Test 1. We first examined study-cue retrieval, contingent on correct retrieval of the target, as a function of whether the same cue or a changed cue appeared on Test 1. Proportion correct cue retrieval was much higher on same-cue trials (M =.86) than changed-cue trials (M =.22), t(37) = 21.93, p <.001, d = On nearly half of changed-cue trials (M =.49), participants erroneously recalled the Test 1 cue when asked for the study cue, suggesting that participants had difficulty monitoring the source of their memories for Test 1 cues (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993). Finally, we addressed our central question: Was the negative effect of Test 1 cue change on memory for prior remembering ameliorated by self-generated retrieval of study cues? We submitted judgments of prior remembering to a 2 (Test 1 cue: same or changed) 2 (study-cue retrieval: success or failure) within-subjects ANOVA. The main effect of Test 1 cue was significant, F(1, 33) = 47.21, p <.001, η 2 =.589, reflecting the previously documented effect whereby judgments of prior remembering were more often correct when Test 1 cues were kept the same (M =.90) versus changed (M =.77). The main effect of study cue retrieval accuracy was also significant, F(1, 33) = 51.79, p <.001, η 2 =.611. Participants were more likely to remember prior remembering following retrieval success (M =.95) versus failure (M =.72). Critically, the interaction was also significant, F(1, 33) = 61.60, p <.001, η 2 =.651. Decomposing the interaction (see Figure 2), we Figure 2. Proportion correct judgments of prior remembering in Experiment 1. Proportion of correct judgments of prior remembering in Experiment 1 as a function of Test 1 cue type and successful or unsuccessful study-cue retrieval. Error bars represent one standard error of the mean. found that the negative effect of Test 1 cue change was significant when participants failed to retrieve the study cue (Ms =.87 and.59 in the same- and changed-cue conditions, respectively), t(40) = 9.07, p <.001, d = 1.51, but there was no negative effect of cue change when participants successfully retrieved the study cue (Ms =.94 and.97 in the same- and changed-cue conditions, respectively), t(37) = 1.39, p =.174, d =.25. For both trial types, retrieving the study cue during Test 1 improved memory for prior remembering compared to when the cue was not retrieved, same-cue t(40) = 2.68, p =.01, d =.52; changedcue t(37) = 8.99, p <.001, d = Unique contribution of reminding to memory for prior remembering Our finding that memory for prior remembering was greater for targets whose study cue was retrieved (versus not) could conceivably be due to item effects because it could be that targets whose Test 1 retrieval was remembered just happened to be ones with particularly memorable study cues. To argue against this possibility, we sought to show that successful retrieval of study cues in the changed-cue condition was positively related to memory for prior remembering, even after controlling for the ease of remembering targets prior retrieval in the same-cue condition. Our analysis was modelled after analyses reported in other recent examinations of the effects of reminding (Jacoby & Wahlheim, 2013; Putnametal., 2017; Wahlheim&Jacoby,2013). We used hierarchical multiple regression to predict proportion correct judgments of prior remembering for each target in the changed-cue condition, collapsed across participants. In the first step, we entered each target s proportion of correct judgments of prior remembering in the samecue condition. In the second step, we entered the proportion of changed-cue trials on which the study cue was retrieved. Only study-cue retrieval predicted a significant amount of the variance in memory for prior remembering in the changed-cue condition (step 1 ΔR 2 =.004, p =.541, step 2 ΔR 2 =.16, p <.001).

8 966 M. L. LEPPANEN AND K. B. LYLE Discussion Like previous research (e.g., Arnold & Lindsay, 2002), Experiment 1 showed that changes in retrieval context causepeopletoforgetpriorremembering. When retrieval cues on Test 1 and Test 2 differed, versus remained the same, participants were less likely to remember Test 1 retrievals. Our novel prediction was confirmed, in that reminding participants of the study cue immediately after Test 1 retrieval on changed-cue trials increased memory for prior remembering. Analysing memory for prior remembering in the changed-cue condition on a trial-by-trial basis, we found that memory was greater when participants successfully brought the study cue to mind following Test 1 target retrieval versus when they failed to do so. The beneficial effect of reminding completely offset the negative effect of retrieval-context change. Isolating cases in which study-cue retrieval was successful, memory for prior remembering was no worse in the changed-cue condition than in the same-cue condition and was, in fact, numerically better in the former than the latter (see Figure 2). These results were not likely due to item effects and instead suggest that bringing study cues to mind following acts of retrieval on Test 1 made a unique contribution to the subsequent memorability of retrievals. Although reminding was powerful in the changed-cue condition, it was infrequent. Participants successfully recalled the study cue on only a minority of trials. Although we cannot say definitively why this was so, we found that participants often provided the Test 1 cue when asked for the study cue and this suggests that source confusion may have played a role. Participants may have misattributed their just-formed memory of the Test 1 cue to the study phase of the procedure. It is also possible that retroactive interference was involved, since the changed-cue condition involves pairing targets with a new cue immediately before testing memory for an old cue. Whatever its cause, the fact that study-cue recall was infrequent in the changed-cue condition, even though participants were actively attempting to retrieve the cues, bolsters our supposition, stated in the Introduction, that spontaneous or involuntary reminding of study cues in response to changed Test 1 cues is probably rare. Theoretically, we contend that study-cue reminders allow study cues to be bound to Test 1 retrieval acts via medial-temporal-lobe-dependent associative processes (Squire & Zola, 1996). When those same cues reappear on a test of memory for prior remembering, they can effectively cue retrieval for earlier retrieval acts. This is especially important in the changed-cue condition, which otherwise provides little or no opportunity to bind study cues to Test 1 retrieval acts. In the same-cue condition, retrieval acts occur in response to the presentation of study cues, providing an initial opportunity for binding. A reminder of the study-cue immediately subsequent to retrieval can provide a second opportunity. Experiment 2 In Experiment 1, reminding of study cues depended on participants being able to retrieve cues from memory. When participants were able to successfully remind themselves of the study cues, it had a pronounced facilitative effect on memory for prior remembering, but, unfortunately, retrieval attempts often failed. Therefore, in our second experiment, we jettisoned the requirement that participants actively retrieve study cues and instead simply re-presented the study cue following each cued-recall trial on Test 1. Participants were required to type the study cue using the computer keyboard (essentially, a copy response). This was the same motor response as in Experiment 1, but it followed a passive presentation in Experiment 2 versus active retrieval in Experiment 1. We hoped that reminding-via-presentation would increase memory for prior remembering similarly to reminding-via-retrieving, but with the advantage that participants would be able to view all study cues, versus retrieving only a minority. Another methodological difference between Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 is that, in Experiment 2, the reminder condition was compared in between-participants fashion to a condition in which participants were not reminded of study cues in any way during Test 1. This no-reminder control condition was essentially identical to the procedure developed by Arnold and Lindsay (2002, Experiment1). Method Participants Ninety-three individuals were assigned to the reminder condition (62 female; aged years, M = 20.0) and 56 to the control condition (45 female; aged years, M = 21.5). All individuals earned course credit for participating. The procedure was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Louisville. Procedure The only procedural difference between Experiments 1 and 2 lay in what happened following each target-retrieval attempt on Test 1. In the control condition, there was no activity following target-retrieval attempts. In the reminder condition, participants were instructed that after each target-retrieval attempt, they would be shown the cue word that had been paired with the target during the study phase and they should type it on the computer keyboard. On each trial in Test 1, after participants had either retrieved a target and typed it in or typed pass, the study cue and a response box were displayed, with the instructions: Please type the word below in the box and press (ENTER). Data were collected in the reminder and control conditions in separate semesters. Initially, we were interested solely in the reminder condition, anticipating that it

9 MEMORY 967 would yield statistically indistinguishable memory for prior remembering regardless of whether Test 1 trials were same-cue or changed-cue (replicating what we found in Experiment 1 given reminding-via-retrieval). As reported under Results, however, we found that memory for prior remembering in the reminding-viapresentation condition was, contra our expectations, significantly poorer following changed-cue testing versus same-cue testing. We then ran the control condition to assess whether the effect of retrieval-context change was reliably different in the reminder condition than in a standard no-reminder condition. Participants in the two conditions were drawn from the same participant pool and run in the same laboratory by similarly trained research assistants. When collecting data in both conditions, we ran participants throughout the entire semester and a greater availability of participants in one semester than the other led to oversampling in the reminder condition compared to the control condition. Results Cued recall of targets We examined cued recall of targets on Tests 1 and 2 in the same manner as in Experiment 1, but taking into account the between-participants factor of condition. We analysed the proportion of targets recalled on Test 1 via a 2 (cue type: same or changed) 2 (condition: reminder or control) mixed-design ANOVA. The main effect of cue type was significant, F(1, 110) = , p <.001, η 2 =.575, with greater recall on same-cue trials (M =.84) than changed-cue trials (M =.72). The main effect of condition was also significant, F(1, 147) = 14.58, p <.001, η 2 =.09, with greater recall in the control condition (M =.81) than the reminder condition (M =.74). The effect of condition was unanticipated and we consider its implications in the Discussion section below. The two-way interaction was not significant, p =.834. We analysed the proportion of targets recalled on Test 2 via a 3 (Test 1 treatment: same-cue, changed-cue, or not tested) 2 (condition: reminder or control) mixed-design ANOVA. The main effect of Test 1 treatment was significant, F(2, 294) = 25.79, p <.001, η 2 =.149. Recall was, on average, identical for targets assigned to the same- and changedcue conditions (M =.86), and that level of recall was significantly higher than for targets that had not been tested (M =.81), smallest t(148) = 6.32, p <.001, d =.63. The main effect of condition was also significant, F(1, 147) = 10.83, p =.001, η 2 =.069, with greater recall in the control condition (M =.88) than the reminder condition (M =.83). The effect of condition mirrors the one obtained on Test 1 and indicates that, on both tests, participants in the control condition unexpectedly recalled more targets than participants in the reminder condition. The two-way interaction was not significant, F(2, 294) = 2.95, p =.054, η 2 =.02. Effect of cue change on memory for prior remembering Table 2 shows the proportion (and raw number) of targets judged to have been retrieved on Test 1 when memory for prior remembering was queried on Test 2. We analysed proportion correct as a function of cue change, while also considering the between-participants factor of condition. Proportion correct was analysed via a 2 (Test 1 cue: same or changed) 2 (condition: reminder or control) mixed-design ANOVA. Results are depicted in Figure 3. While the main effect of Test 1 cue was significant, F(1, 147) = 88.26, p <.001, η 2 =.375, the main effect of condition was not significant, F(1, 147) = 3.16, p =.078, η 2 =.021. The key result was the significant two-way interaction, F(1, 147) = 8.49, p =.004, η 2 =.055. As shown in Figure 3, correct judgments of prior remembering for same-cue targets did not differ significantly between the reminder and control conditions (Ms =.91 and.92, respectively), p =.811, but correct judgments of prior remembering for changed-cue targets were significantly more likely in the reminder condition than the control condition (Ms =.82 and.73, respectively), t(110) = 2.63, p =.01, d =.37, indicating that reminders of the study cue increased memory for prior remembering in the face of retrievalcontext change. Retrieval-context change (versus stability) nonetheless significantly impaired memory for prior remembering in both conditions, smallest t(92) = 6.16, p <.001, d =.71. The magnitude of this impairment, Table 2. Mean number of targets and mean proportion of targets judged as recalled as a function of recall status on Test 1 and Test 2 for Experiment 2. Proportion judged as Test 1/Test 2 recall status Number of targets recalled on Test 1 Reminder condition Same cue* Recalled/Recalled (.01) Recalled/Not recalled (.05) Not recalled/not recalled (.03) Not recalled/recalled (.04) Changed cue* Recalled/Recalled (.02) Recalled/Not recalled (.04) Not recalled/not recalled (.04) Not recalled/recalled (.03) Not tested* NA/Recalled (.03) NA/Not recalled (.03) Control condition Same cue* Recalled/Recalled (.01) Recalled/Not recalled (.08) Not recalled/not recalled (.05) Not recalled/recalled (.06) Changed cue* Recalled/Recalled (.03) Recalled/Not recalled (.06) Not recalled/not recalled (.05) Not recalled/recalled (.04) Not tested* NA/Recalled (.05) NA/Not recalled (.04) Notes: NA = Not applicable. * Test 1 context. Numbers in parentheses are standard errors of the mean. Lines in bold are those for which statistical analyses are reported in the text.

10 968 M. L. LEPPANEN AND K. B. LYLE remembering was assessed only for items successfully retrieved on both tests. Although participants in the control condition were able to recall a somewhat greater number of targets, what matters for our purposes is that these participants remembered prior remembering in the face of retrieval-context change more poorly than participants who received reminders. Experiment 3 Figure 3. Proportion correct judgments of prior remembering in Experiment 2. Proportion of correct judgments of prior remembering in Experiment 2 as a function of condition and Test 1 cue type. Error bars represent one standard error of the mean. however, was about half as large in the reminder condition (M diff =.10) as in the control condition (M diff =.19). Discussion Under conditions of retrieval-context change, memory for prior remembering was significantly better when participants were reminded (versus not) of each target s study cue immediately after target retrieval on Test 1. Reminding was achieved in Experiment 2 by re-presenting the study cue on the screen and having participants type it on the keyboard. Participants were instructed before the test began that the cue they were being asked to type had been paired with the target they had attempted to retrieve during the study phase. We theorised that this sort of reminding would provide an opportunity to bind study cues to retrieval acts. It appears, though, that reminding participants of a target s study cue after retrieving the target in response to a different test cue is not mnemonically equivalent to retrieving the target in response to the study cue. Even with reminding, changed-cue retrieval events were significantly less memorable than same-cue retrieval events. Hence, reminding-via-presentation reduced the negative effect of retrieval-context change on memory for prior remembering, but did not eliminate it. This contrasts with the finding in Experiment 1 that target retrievals on changed-cue trials were as likely to be remembered as those on same-cue trials, provided participants were able to recall study cues following retrieval. An unexpected finding in this experiment was that cued recall of targets was poorer in the reminder condition than the control condition. This was true on both Tests 1 and 2. A possible explanation is that, for participants in the reminder condition, viewing and typing study cues during Test 1 produced output interference, which has previously been observed in paired-associate recall procedures (Roediger & Schmidt, 1980). It may also be that the result is spurious and unrelated to the experimental manipulation. In any event, it is unclear that differences in recall performance change the conclusions we would draw from this experiment, since memory for prior In Experiment 3, we sought to replicate Experiment 1 in showing that the negative effect of retrieval-context change on memory for prior remembering is completely eliminated when participants recall study cues immediately following an act of retrieval. Two considerations made us feel a replication attempt was warranted, especially given recent intense interest in the replicability of psychological findings (e.g., Open Science Collaboration, 2015). First, the negative effect of retrieval-context change on memory is usually quite robust and, in Experiment 2, we found that it endured, albeit in diminished form, when we externally presented the study cue to participants on every trial. Second, the beneficial effect of study-cue recall was based on a small number of observations because successful recall was rare on changed-cue trials. For either or both of these reasons, one might question the reliability of the finding. Rather than conducting an exact replication, we changed the procedure in the hope of increasing studycue recall on changed-cue trials. We found that participants in Experiment 1 often mistook the Test 1 cue for the study cue. That is, when asked to recall the study cue, participants reported the Test 1 cue. This is understandable from a source-monitoring perspective (Johnson et al., 1993), because, having just seen the Test 1 cue, participants may have experienced source confusion and thought the just-seen cue had also appeared in the study phase, not unlike what happens when eyewitnesses are exposed to post-event misinformation (see Loftus, 2005, for a review). We reasoned that participants might be better able to resolve this source confusion if they were explicitly alerted to instances in which the Test 1 cue was not the study cue and they would therefore need to search their memories for a cue word other than the justpresented one. We therefore presented participants with a statement following each cued-recall attempt on Test 1 that specified whether the cue they had just seen was the same as the study cue or different. We hoped this would encourage participants to thoroughly search for study cues on changed-cue trials, thereby increasing the likelihood of study-cue recall and providing us with more opportunities to observe the effect of study-cue recall on memory for prior remembering. Even if recall remained infrequent, we could test whether this relatively rare event was as potent in Experiment 3 as it was in Experiment 1.

11 MEMORY 969 Method Participants Participants were 62 undergraduates (42 female; aged years, M = 20.1) who earned course credit for participating. The procedure was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Louisville. Procedure Experiment 3 was procedurally identical to Experiment 1 except for the method of reminding during Test 1. After attempting to retrieve each target on Test 1, participants received one of two informational messages that appeared on the computer screen. On same-cue trials, participants saw: This cue remained the SAME. Please type the cue from the STUDY phase below and press (ENTER). On changed-cue trials, participants saw: This cue was DIFFER- ENT than the one you studied. Please type the cue from the STUDY phase below and press (ENTER). Results Cued recall of targets The proportion of targets correctly recalled on Test 1 was significantly higher on same-cue trials (M =.80) than changed-cue trials (M =.68), t(61) = 9.34, p <.001, d = A repeated-measures ANOVA on Test 2 target recall revealed a significant effect of Test 1 treatment, F(2, 122) = 11.71, p <.001, η 2 =.161. Unlike in Experiments 1 and 2, the proportion of targets correctly recalled differed significantly, but only slightly, between targets assigned to samecue (M =.84) and changed-cue trials (M =.82), t(61) = 2.30, p =.025, d =.30. Recall of targets that had not previously been tested (M =.79) was significantly lower than both types of previously tested targets, smallest t(61) = 2.59, p =.01, d =.34. Table 3. Mean number of targets and mean proportion of targets judged as recalled as a function of recall status on Test 1 and Test 2 for Experiment 3. Test 1/Test 2 recall status Number of targets Proportion judged as recalled on Test 1 Same cue* Recalled/Recalled (.01) Recalled/Not recalled (.07) Not recalled/not (.05) recalled Not recalled/recalled (.05) Changed cue* Recalled/Recalled (.03) Recalled/Not recalled (.05) Not recalled/not (.04) recalled Not recalled/recalled (.04) Not tested* NA/Recalled (.04) NA/Not recalled (.03) Notes: NA = Not applicable. * Test 1 context. Numbers in parentheses are standard errors of the mean. Lines in bold are those for which statistical analyses are reported in the text. Effect of cue change on memory for prior remembering Table 3 shows the proportion (and raw number) of targets judged to have been retrieved on Test 1 when memory for prior remembering was queried on Test 2. Participants were significantly more likely to correctly indicate that they had retrieved targets assigned to same-cue trials (M =.92) than changed-cue trials (M =.68), F(1, 61) = , p <.001, η 2 =.629. Study-cue retrieval and its effect on memory for prior remembering We first examined retrieval of study cues as a function of whether the same cue or a changed cue appeared on Test 1. Proportion correct cue retrieval was significantly higher on same-cue trials (M =.90) than changed-cue trials (M =.28), t(57) = 24.61, p <.001, d = Recall on changed-cue trials was numerically higher than in Experiment 1 (by cross-experimental comparison), but only slightly and non-significantly so, t(94) = 1.47, p =.144, d =.24. Finally, we examined whether the negative effect of Test 1 cue change on memory for prior remembering was eliminated by reminding participants of the study cue. We submitted judgments of prior remembering to a 2 (Test 1 cue: same or changed) 2 (study-cue retrieval: success or failure) within-participants ANOVA. There were significant main effects of Test 1 cue, F(1, 45) = 28.64, p <.001, η 2 =.389, and study-cue retrieval, F(1, 45) = 40.73, p <.001, η 2 =.475, but these were superseded by a significant interaction, F(1, 45) = 45.34, p <.001, η 2 =.502. As shown in Figure 4, and mirroring the result from Experiment 1, the interaction arose because the negative effect of Test 1 cue change was significant when participants failed to retrieve study cues (Ms =.89 and.59 for same and changed cues, respectively), t(51) = 7.64, p <.001, d = 1.06, but was absent when participants successfully retrieved study cues (Ms =.92 and.94 for same and changed cues, respectively), t(55) =.216, p =.174, d =.04. Retrieval success (versus failure) significantly increased memory for prior remembering following changed-cue testing, t(54) = 9.82, p <.001, d = 1.35, while the increase was only numerical following same-cue testing, t(52) = 1.02, p =.314, d =.16. Unique contribution of reminding to memory for prior remembering To examine the unique contribution of reminding, above and beyond potential item effects, to memory for prior remembering, we conducted the same hierarchical multiple regression analysis as in Experiment 1. Study-cue retrieval predicted a significant amount of the variance in memory for prior remembering in the changed-cue condition, above and beyond a significant contribution from memory for prior remembering of targets in the same-

The effects of free recall testing on subsequent source memory

The effects of free recall testing on subsequent source memory MEMORY, 2010, 18 (4), 385393 The effects of free recall testing on subsequent source memory Gene A. Brewer, Richard L. Marsh, Joseph T. Meeks, and Arlo Clark-Foos University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA

More information

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005,?? 12 (?), (5),???-??? 852-857 BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval LARRY L. JACOBY, YUJIRO SHIMIZU,

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Remindings influence the interpretation of ambiguous stimuli

Remindings influence the interpretation of ambiguous stimuli Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:107 113 DOI 10.3758/s13423-013-0476-2 BRIEF REPORT Remindings influence the interpretation of ambiguous stimuli Jonathan G. Tullis & Michael Braverman & Brian H. Ross & Aaron

More information

Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment Hypothesis: Comment on Belli and Reply to Tversky and Tuchin

Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment Hypothesis: Comment on Belli and Reply to Tversky and Tuchin Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1989, Vol. 118, No. 1,92-99 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Im 0096-3445/89/S00.7 Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment

More information

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

The spacing and lag effect in free recall The spacing and lag effect in free recall Michael J. Kahana, Bradley R. Wellington & Marc W. Howard Center for Complex Systems and Department of Psychology Brandeis University Send correspondence to: Michael

More information

Involuntary and Voluntary Memory Sequencing Phenomena

Involuntary and Voluntary Memory Sequencing Phenomena 5 Involuntary and Voluntary Memory Sequencing Phenomena An Interesting Puzzle for the Study of Autobiographical Memory Organization and Retrieval Jennifer Talarico and John H. Mace Introduction One of

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Cognitive Mechanisms Underlying Recovered-Memory Experiences of Childhood Sexual Abuse Elke Geraerts, 1,2 D. Stephen Lindsay, 3 Harald Merckelbach, 2 Marko Jelicic,

More information

Elizabeth Loftus. Lost in the mall study 1992

Elizabeth Loftus. Lost in the mall study 1992 Lost in the mall study 1992 -Subject was told 4 stories from his past, including one that was made up about him being lost in the mall at age 5. -Over the next two weeks, he jotted down whatever memories

More information

Shayne Loft, Michael S. Humphreys, & Susannah J. Whitney. The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia

Shayne Loft, Michael S. Humphreys, & Susannah J. Whitney. The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia Memory Access Task Interference 1 Running Head: Memory Access Task Interference Control of Access to Memory: The use of Task Interference as a Behavioral Probe Shayne Loft, Michael S. Humphreys, & Susannah

More information

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (6), 1067-1073 Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources MOSHE NAVEH-BENJAMIN University of Missouri,

More information

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Uri Hasson (uhasson@princeton.edu) Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 USA Geoffrey P. Goodwin (ggoodwin@princeton.edu)

More information

Presentation of cues during recall impairs recall of non-cued items (part-set cuing

Presentation of cues during recall impairs recall of non-cued items (part-set cuing GODBOLE, NAMRATA, M.A. Assessing Temporal Associations in Recall: A New Take on the Strategy Disruption Account of the Part-Set Cuing Effect. (2012) Directed by Dr. Peter F. Delaney. 54 pp. Presentation

More information

Source memory and the picture superiority effect

Source memory and the picture superiority effect Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2007 Source memory and the picture superiority effect Noelle L. Brown Louisiana State University and Agricultural and

More information

When Misinformation Improves Memory: The Effects of Recollecting Change

When Misinformation Improves Memory: The Effects of Recollecting Change 672268PSSXXX10.1177/0956797616672268Misinformation and RemindingsPutnam et al. research-article2016 Research Article When Misinformation Improves Memory: The Effects of Recollecting Change Psychological

More information

Probabilistic Retroactive Interference: The Role of Accessibility Bias in Interference Effects

Probabilistic Retroactive Interference: The Role of Accessibility Bias in Interference Effects Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 136, No. 2, 200 216 0096-3445/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.200 Probabilistic Retroactive

More information

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Anna V. Fisher (fisher.449@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Cuing Effects in Short-term Recall. Gerald Tehan. University of Southern Queensland. Michael S. Humphreys. University of Queensland

Cuing Effects in Short-term Recall. Gerald Tehan. University of Southern Queensland. Michael S. Humphreys. University of Queensland Cuing Effects in Short-term Recall Cuing in Short-term Recall 1 Gerald Tehan University of Southern Queensland Michael S. Humphreys University of Queensland Mailing Address: Gerry Tehan Faculty of Sciences

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

Information and cue-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states

Information and cue-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states Information and cue-priming effects on tip-of-the-tongue states Psycholinguistics 2 Contents: Introduction... 1 Pilot Experiment... 2 Experiment... 3 Participants... 3 Materials... 4 Design... 4 Procedure...

More information

Memory in Everyday Life. Lesson 5

Memory in Everyday Life. Lesson 5 Memory in Everyday Life Lesson 5 I. Eyewitness testimony and the malleability [influence] of memory A. Memory does not work like a video recorder: People do not encode or retrieve every aspect of an event

More information

Memory for flip-flopping: Detection and recollection of political contradictions By: Adam L. Putnam, Christopher N. Wahlheim, Larry L.

Memory for flip-flopping: Detection and recollection of political contradictions By: Adam L. Putnam, Christopher N. Wahlheim, Larry L. Memory for flip-flopping: Detection and recollection of political contradictions By: Adam L. Putnam, Christopher N. Wahlheim, Larry L. Jacoby Putnam, A. L., Wahlheim, C. N., & Jacoby, L. L. (2014). Memory

More information

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Mem Cogn (2011) 39:1222 1231 DOI 10.3758/s13421-011-0105-0 Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Dawn M. McBride & Jaclyn K. Beckner & Drew

More information

Journal of Memory and Language

Journal of Memory and Language Journal of Memory and Language 68 (2013) 333 349 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Journal of Memory and Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jml ethinking familiarity: emember/know

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Elaborative Retrieval: Do Semantic Mediators Improve Memory? Melissa Lehman and Jeffrey D. Karpicke Online First Publication, March 4,

More information

The Regression-Discontinuity Design

The Regression-Discontinuity Design Page 1 of 10 Home» Design» Quasi-Experimental Design» The Regression-Discontinuity Design The regression-discontinuity design. What a terrible name! In everyday language both parts of the term have connotations

More information

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Memory. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Memory. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin AQA A Level Psychology Topic Companion Memory Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin AQA A LEVEL Psychology topic companion: MEMORY Page 2 Contents Memory The multi-store model 3 Types of long-term memory 9 The working

More information

A dissociation between causal judgment and outcome recall

A dissociation between causal judgment and outcome recall Journal Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 2005,?? 12 (?), (5),???-??? 950-954 A dissociation between causal judgment and outcome recall CHRIS J. MITCHELL, PETER F. LOVIBOND, and CHEE YORK GAN University

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society Title Testing the Myth of the Encoding-Retrieval Match Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9f23v8bz Journal Proceedings

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information

Spontaneous Trait Inferences Are Bound to Actors Faces: Evidence From a False Recognition Paradigm

Spontaneous Trait Inferences Are Bound to Actors Faces: Evidence From a False Recognition Paradigm Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 83, No. 5, 1051 1065 0022-3514/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.5.1051 Spontaneous

More information

Are awareness questionnaires valid? Investigating the use of posttest questionnaires for assessing awareness in implicit memory tests

Are awareness questionnaires valid? Investigating the use of posttest questionnaires for assessing awareness in implicit memory tests Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (1), 53-64 doi: 10.3758/MC.36.1.53 Are awareness questionnaires valid? Investigating the use of posttest questionnaires for assessing awareness in implicit memory tests Terrence

More information

Age-Related Deficits in Reality Monitoring of Action Memories

Age-Related Deficits in Reality Monitoring of Action Memories Psychology and Aging Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 2008, Vol. 23, No. 3, 646 656 0882-7974/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013083 Age-Related Deficits in Reality Monitoring of Action

More information

Memory (1) Visual Sensory Store. Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory

Memory (1) Visual Sensory Store. Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory Memory (1) Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) Model of Memory Visual Sensory Store It appears that our visual system is able to hold a great deal of information but that if we do not attend to this information

More information

How cue-dependent is memory?: Internal reinstatement and cueing effects in recognition and source memory

How cue-dependent is memory?: Internal reinstatement and cueing effects in recognition and source memory Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2006 How cue-dependent is memory?: Internal reinstatement and cueing effects in recognition and source memory Jeffrey

More information

Psychology Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d e. 2. a b c d e 14. a b c d e

Psychology Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d e. 2. a b c d e 14. a b c d e 1 7 Psychology 3450 - Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A Name: MUN ID: Indicate your answer by crossing out the appropriate letter. Answer 20 questions only. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Distinctiveness and the Recognition Mirror Effect: Evidence for an Item-Based Criterion Placement Heuristic

Distinctiveness and the Recognition Mirror Effect: Evidence for an Item-Based Criterion Placement Heuristic Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2005, Vol. 31, No. 6, 1186 1198 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1186

More information

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Hidehito Honda (hito@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Kimihiko Yamagishi (kimihiko@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Graduate School of Decision Science

More information

Psychology 2019 v1.3. IA2 high-level annotated sample response. Student experiment (20%) August Assessment objectives

Psychology 2019 v1.3. IA2 high-level annotated sample response. Student experiment (20%) August Assessment objectives Student experiment (20%) This sample has been compiled by the QCAA to assist and support teachers to match evidence in student responses to the characteristics described in the instrument-specific marking

More information

Misled Subjects May Know More Than Their Performance Implies

Misled Subjects May Know More Than Their Performance Implies Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1989, Vol. 15, No. 2, 246-255 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. O278-7393/89/S0O.75 Subjects May Know More

More information

Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting

Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2012, 65 (11), 2231 2257 Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting Michael J. Serra and Benjamin D. England Department

More information

Attributions of Familiarity in Amnesia: Evidence From a Fame Judgment Task

Attributions of Familiarity in Amnesia: Evidence From a Fame Judgment Task Neuropsychology 1993, Vol. 7, No. 4, 510-518 In (he public domain Attributions of Familiarity in Amnesia: Evidence From a Fame Judgment Task Laird S. Cermak, Mieke Verfaellie, Tracy Butler, and Larry L.

More information

Running Head: MODALITY EFFECTS IN FALSE RECALL 1. Modality Effects in False Recall: Dissociations Between Short- and Long-Term Memory

Running Head: MODALITY EFFECTS IN FALSE RECALL 1. Modality Effects in False Recall: Dissociations Between Short- and Long-Term Memory Running Head: MODALITY EFFECTS IN FALSE RECALL 1 Modality Effects in False Recall: Dissociations Between Short- and Long-Term Memory Grace M. Glowniak University of Michigan 2 Abstract In recent research,

More information

Why misinformation is more likely to be recognised over time: A source monitoring account

Why misinformation is more likely to be recognised over time: A source monitoring account Why misinformation is more likely to be recognised over time: A source monitoring account Peter Frost Southern New Hampshire University Melissa Ingraham and Beth Wilson Rivier College Citation: Frost,

More information

The efficiency of binding spontaneous trait inferences to actorsõ faces

The efficiency of binding spontaneous trait inferences to actorsõ faces Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 549 562 Journal of Experimental Social Psychology www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp The efficiency of binding spontaneous trait inferences to actorsõ faces

More information

THE FORMATION OF FALSE MEMORIES LOFTUS AND PECKRILL (1995)

THE FORMATION OF FALSE MEMORIES LOFTUS AND PECKRILL (1995) THE FORMATION OF FALSE MEMORIES LOFTUS AND PECKRILL (1995) Bell Ringer Eyewitness Testimony Prior to viewing this clip, what is your opinion on eyewitness accounts? Why? MEMORY Your memory is your mind

More information

Aging and Maintaining Intentions Over Delays: Do It or Lose It

Aging and Maintaining Intentions Over Delays: Do It or Lose It Psychology and Aging Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 18, No. 4, 823 835 0882-7974/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.823 Aging and Maintaining Intentions Over

More information

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework Modified from Organizing Scientific Thinking Using the QuALMRI Framework Written by Kevin Ochsner and modified by others. Based on a scheme devised by

More information

Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk.

Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk. chunking Increasing the amount of information that can be held in short-term memory by grouping related items together into a single unit, or chunk. clustering Organizing items into related groups during

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Michael Romano (mrr2@nyu.edu) Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place New York, NY 1000 USA Abstract Traditional

More information

Psych 136S Review Questions, Summer 2015

Psych 136S Review Questions, Summer 2015 Psych 136S Review Questions, Summer 2015 For each paper you should be able to briefly summarize the methods and results and explain why the results are important. The guided summary for the Roediger et

More information

Retrieval of Autobiographical Information

Retrieval of Autobiographical Information Retrieval of Autobiographical Information Erica C. Yu and Scott Fricker Bureau of Labor Statistics 2 Massachusetts Avenue, NE Washington, DC 20212 Introduction Survey questions about past behavior often

More information

Can déjà vu result from similarity to a prior experience? Support for the similarity hypothesis of déjà vu

Can déjà vu result from similarity to a prior experience? Support for the similarity hypothesis of déjà vu Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2009, 16 (6), 1082-1088 doi:10.3758/pbr.16.6.1082 Can déjà vu result from similarity to a prior experience? Support for the similarity hypothesis of déjà vu ANNE M. CLEARY,

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (3), 490-494 Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness GINO CAMP, DIANE PECHER, and HENK G. SCHMIDT Erasmus University Rotterdam,

More information

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Memory & Cognition 1974, Vol. 2, No.3, 533 538 Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Ss were required to learn linear

More information

Singers sometimes find it difficult to recall old song lyrics because of all the new songs they have learned.

Singers sometimes find it difficult to recall old song lyrics because of all the new songs they have learned. PROACTIVE AND RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE PRACTICE When the aisles of a shopping mart are changed, you may instinctively start walking towards the old shelves instead of the new ones. Singers sometimes find

More information

How Many Colors Can You Remember? Capacity is about Conscious vs unconscious memories

How Many Colors Can You Remember? Capacity is about Conscious vs unconscious memories Science B44 Lecture 18 Visual Memory Memory 1. Afterimage, persistence, iconic sensory memory 2. Conscious vs unconscious memories 3. Short and long term memories 4. Where are memories seen 5. Flashbulb

More information

A strategy disruption component to retrieval-induced forgetting

A strategy disruption component to retrieval-induced forgetting Memory & Cognition 2006, 34 (1), 102-111 A strategy disruption component to retrieval-induced forgetting MICHAEL D. DODD University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada ALAN D. CASTEL Washington University,

More information

False Recognition: Words and Images

False Recognition: Words and Images The Huron University College Journal of Learning and Motivation Volume 51 Issue 1 Article 3 2013 False Recognition: Words and Images Tristan Bell Follow this and additional works at: http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/hucjlm

More information

Mechanisms of Memory: Can we distinguish true from false memories?

Mechanisms of Memory: Can we distinguish true from false memories? Mechanisms of Memory: Can we distinguish true from false memories? Lila Davachi D. Cohen (1996) Dept of Psychology & Center for Neural Science New York University AAAS Judicial Seminar on Neuroscience

More information

AP PSYCHOLOGY 2012 SCORING GUIDELINES

AP PSYCHOLOGY 2012 SCORING GUIDELINES AP PSYCHOLOGY 2012 SCORING GUIDELINES Question 2 For each of the following pairs of terms, explain how the placement or location of the first influences the process indicated by the second. Rods, peripheral

More information

The effects of emotional arousal on item and associative memory

The effects of emotional arousal on item and associative memory Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2013 The effects of emotional arousal on item and associative memory Leslie Ann Butler Louisiana State University and

More information

I Haven t Thought About This for Years! Dating Recent Recalls of Vivid Memories

I Haven t Thought About This for Years! Dating Recent Recalls of Vivid Memories APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 20: 33 42 (2006) Published online 13 September 2005 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.1153 I Haven t Thought About

More information

Retrieval of Concepts in Script-Based Texts and Narratives: The Influence of General World Knowledge

Retrieval of Concepts in Script-Based Texts and Narratives: The Influence of General World Knowledge Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2002, Vol. 28, No. 4, 780 790 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.4.780

More information

A dual-process account of the list-length and strength-based mirror effects in recognition q

A dual-process account of the list-length and strength-based mirror effects in recognition q Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 231 248 Journal of Memory and Language www.elsevier.com/locate/jml A dual-process account of the list-length and strength-based mirror effects in recognition q

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

ORIOLE MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVIEWS (ROUND 1) Editor Decision Letter

ORIOLE MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVIEWS (ROUND 1) Editor Decision Letter 1 ORIOLE MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVIEWS (ROUND 1) Editor Decision Letter Thank you for submitting your manuscript to JCR. Your manuscript was read by three experts who provided constructive and complementary

More information

MEMORY STORAGE. There are three major kinds of storage:

MEMORY STORAGE. There are three major kinds of storage: MEMORY Jill Price was capable of remembering everything that happened last year and several years ago. Memory is the ability to store and retrieve information over time. Memories are the residue of those

More information

Assessing the influence of recollection and familiarity in memory for own- vs. other-race faces

Assessing the influence of recollection and familiarity in memory for own- vs. other-race faces Iowa State University From the SelectedWorks of Christian A. Meissner, Ph.D. 2009 Assessing the influence of recollection and familiarity in memory for own- vs. other-race faces Jessica L Marcon, University

More information

The role of test structure in creating false memories

The role of test structure in creating false memories Journal Memory & Cognition 2006,?? 34 (?), (5),???-??? 1026-1036 The role of test structure in creating false memories JENNIFER H. COANE and DAWN M. MCBRIDE Illinois State University, Normal, Illinois

More information

Do all these faces look familiar? Can you name them all? Why is it difficult to recall names even though you can recognize them? More generally, why

Do all these faces look familiar? Can you name them all? Why is it difficult to recall names even though you can recognize them? More generally, why Do all these faces look familiar? Can you name them all? Why is it difficult to recall names even though you can recognize them? More generally, why do we forget things? Learning Causes Forgetting: Interference

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

Why is dispersion of memory important*

Why is dispersion of memory important* What is memory* It is a web of connections Research has shown that people who lose their memory also lose the ability to connect things to each other in their mind It is these connections that let us understand

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

The effects of a levels-of-processing manipulation on false recall

The effects of a levels-of-processing manipulation on false recall Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2000, 7 (1), 158-162 The effects of a levels-of-processing manipulation on false recall MATTHEW G. RHODES and JEFFREY S. ANASTASI Francis Marion University, Florence, South

More information

Journal of Memory and Language

Journal of Memory and Language Journal of Memory and Language 63 (2010) 149 157 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Memory and Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jml Testing promotes eyewitness accuracy

More information

Is it possible to give a philosophical definition of sexual desire?

Is it possible to give a philosophical definition of sexual desire? Issue 1 Spring 2016 Undergraduate Journal of Philosophy Is it possible to give a philosophical definition of sexual desire? William Morgan - The University of Sheffield pp. 47-58 For details of submission

More information

Testing Promotes Eyewitness Accuracy with a Warning Implications for Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility

Testing Promotes Eyewitness Accuracy with a Warning Implications for Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility Psychology Publications Psychology 8-2010 Testing Promotes Eyewitness Accuracy with a Warning Implications for Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility Ayanna K. Thomas Tufts University John B. Bulevich Rhode

More information

PERCEPTUAL CONDITIONS AFFECTING EASE OF ASSOCIATION

PERCEPTUAL CONDITIONS AFFECTING EASE OF ASSOCIATION Journal of Experimental Psychology 1972, Vol. 93, No. 1, 176-180 PERCEPTUAL CONDITIONS AFFECTING EASE OF ASSOCIATION PETER G. ARNOLD AND GORDON H. BOWER 2 Stanford University Four experiments replicated

More information

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Psychon Bull Rev (2017) 24:1651 1657 DOI 10.3758/s13423-017-1226-7 BRIEF REPORT Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Sébastien De Schrijver 1 & Pierre Barrouillet 1 Published

More information

Highlighting Effect: The Function of Rebuttals in Written Argument

Highlighting Effect: The Function of Rebuttals in Written Argument Highlighting Effect: The Function of Rebuttals in Written Argument Ryosuke Onoda (ndrysk62@p.u-tokyo.ac.jp) Department of Educational Psychology, Graduate School of Education, The University of Tokyo,

More information

Binding of Independent Contexts in Source Memory

Binding of Independent Contexts in Source Memory Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2015 Binding of Independent Contexts in Source Memory Samantha Nicole Spitler Louisiana State University and Agricultural

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

Is task interference in event-based prospective memory dependent on cue presentation?

Is task interference in event-based prospective memory dependent on cue presentation? Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (1), 139-148 doi: 10.3758/MC.36.1.139 Is task interference in event-based prospective memory dependent on cue presentation? SHAYNE LOFT University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland,

More information

Picking Co*on Ac,vity. Picking Cotton on 60 Minutes (http://www.cbsnews.com/stories/2009/03/06/60minutes/main shtml)

Picking Co*on Ac,vity. Picking Cotton on 60 Minutes (http://www.cbsnews.com/stories/2009/03/06/60minutes/main shtml) Picking Co*on Ac,vity Picking Cotton on 60 Minutes (http://www.cbsnews.com/stories/2009/03/06/60minutes/main4848039.shtml) Forgetting Forgetting is a result of either: 1. Encoding Failure 2. Storage Decay

More information

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 BLL R. BROWN AND THOMAS J. REBBN2 UNlVERSTY OF LOUSVLLE This experiment required Ss to make Markov-generated histoforms that were

More information

Interviewing Witnesses: The Effect of Forced Confabulation on Event Memory

Interviewing Witnesses: The Effect of Forced Confabulation on Event Memory Law Hum Behav (2007) 31:463 478 DOI 10.1007/s10979-006-9081-5 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Interviewing Witnesses: The Effect of Forced Confabulation on Event Memory Kathy Pezdek Kathryn Sperry Shana M. Owens Published

More information

Using the past to predict the future

Using the past to predict the future Journal Memory & Cognition 2005,?? 33 (?), (6),???-??? 1096-1115 Using the past to predict the future MICHAEL R. DOUGHERTY, PETRA SCHECK, and THOMAS O. NELSON University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland

More information

The testing effect for mediator final test cues and related final test cues in online and laboratory experiments

The testing effect for mediator final test cues and related final test cues in online and laboratory experiments Coppens et al. BMC Psychology (2016) 4:25 DOI 10.1186/s40359-016-0127-2 RESEARCH ARTICLE The testing effect for mediator final test cues and related final test cues in online and laboratory experiments

More information

Verbal Descriptions of Faces From Memory: Are They Diagnostic of Identification Accuracy?

Verbal Descriptions of Faces From Memory: Are They Diagnostic of Identification Accuracy? Journal of Applied Psychology 1985, Vol. 70, No. 4, 619-626 Copyright 1985 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0021-9010/85/S00.75 Verbal Descriptions of Faces From Memory: Are They Diagnostic

More information

3. Read the study by Grant. Underline psychology key words and add them to your glossary. 4. Make detailed notes on the study

3. Read the study by Grant. Underline psychology key words and add them to your glossary. 4. Make detailed notes on the study Getting ready to study Psychology: 1. Read the study by Loftus and Palmer. Underline psychology key words and look up what they mean. Get yourself a small exercise book and start to make a glossary. 2.

More information

Contextual dependence of the production of study-time effect in free recall. Takeo Isarida (Shizuoka University)

Contextual dependence of the production of study-time effect in free recall. Takeo Isarida (Shizuoka University) 1 Contextual dependence of the production of study-time effect in free recall Takeo Isarida (Shizuoka University) An episodic-memory trace of an event consists of information about both the focal elements

More information

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Unsworth, Nash] On: 13 January 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 918847458] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

The Intermixed Blocked Effect in Human Perceptual Learning Is Not the Consequence of Trial Spacing

The Intermixed Blocked Effect in Human Perceptual Learning Is Not the Consequence of Trial Spacing Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2008, Vol. 34, No. 1, 237 242 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.237

More information