Self-affirmation and affective forecasting: Affirmation reduces the anticipated impact of negative events

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Self-affirmation and affective forecasting: Affirmation reduces the anticipated impact of negative events"

Transcription

1 Self-affirmation and affective forecasting: Affirmation reduces the anticipated impact of negative events Janet V. T. Pauketat, Wesley G. Moons, Jacqueline M. Chen, Diane M. Mackie & David K. Sherman Motivation and Emotion ISSN DOI /s x 1 23

2 Your article is protected by copyright and all rights are held exclusively by Springer Science +Business Media New York. This e-offprint is for personal use only and shall not be selfarchived in electronic repositories. If you wish to self-archive your article, please use the accepted manuscript version for posting on your own website. You may further deposit the accepted manuscript version in any repository, provided it is only made publicly available 12 months after official publication or later and provided acknowledgement is given to the original source of publication and a link is inserted to the published article on Springer's website. The link must be accompanied by the following text: "The final publication is available at link.springer.com. 1 23

3 DOI /s x ORIGINAL PAPER Self-affirmation and affective forecasting: Affirmation reduces the anticipated impact of negative events Janet V. T. Pauketat 1 Wesley G. Moons 2 Jacqueline M. Chen 3 Diane M. Mackie 1 David K. Sherman 1 Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016 Abstract When forecasting how they will feel in the future, people overestimate the impact that imagined negative events will have on their affective states, partly because they underestimate their own psychological resiliency. Because self-affirmation enhances resiliency, two studies examined whether self-affirmation prior to forecasting reduces the extremity of affective forecasts. Participants in self-affirmation conditions completed a values scale or wrote an essay asserting their most important value, whereas participants in the no-affirmation condition asserted a relatively unimportant value. Participants then predicted their affective reactions to a negative or positive imagined event. In both studies, self-affirmation reduced the unpleasant affect expected to result from a negative event, but had no impact on affective forecasts for a positive event. This pattern was mediated by participants cognitive appraisals of the imagined event, but not by differential focus on that event. Results are consistent with self-affirmation activating or enhancing psychological resiliency to counteract immune neglect during affective forecasting of a negative event. & Janet V. T. Pauketat janet.pauketat@psych.ucsb.edu & Wesley G. Moons Wesley@MoonsAnalytics.com Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, USA Moons Analytics, San Diego, USA Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, USA Keywords Affective forecasting Self-affirmation Immune neglect Coping Introduction Asking the boss for a raise, bringing up relationship issues to one s spouse, and getting screened for cancer are all events that people expect to be unpleasant. In fact, people overestimate how negatively they will feel when anticipating such situations compared to how they actually feel when in those situations (Gilbert and Wilson 2007). The current research examines whether self-affirmation is a psychological intervention that can successfully reduce the biased extremity of negative affective forecasts by counteracting the immune neglect process through which affective forecasting can function. The overestimation that occurs in affective forecasting about negative events is partly due to two kinds of underestimation. First, people underestimate the role of psychological coping processes (the psychological immune system) that typically kicks into help us cope with negative events. This failure to take psychological defenses and coping mechanisms into account has been termed immune neglect (Gilbert et al. 1998). We overestimate how upset we will be by a negative work evaluation, for example, because we do not realize that our psychological immune system will kick in and we will cope with the negative feedback from the boss, which will make us feel better. Because people are often unaware of these processes even when faced with an actual threat (Sherman et al. 2009b), it is not surprising that people do not factor in the possible benefits of these coping processes when imagining a threat. Thus, people expect to feel worse than they actually do when making affective forecasts. Importantly, this immune

4 neglect explains the extremity and inaccuracy of affective forecasts only for negative events, because coping processes are less relevant to positive events (compared to capitalizing processes for positive events; Langston 1994; Gilbert et al. 1998). Self-affirmation is part of the psychological immune system (Steele 1988; Gilbert et al. 1998) that people draw on in order to maintain a positive sense of self when they experience threats. Self-affirmation occurs when the selfconcept is bolstered by reminders of alternative self-resources that can be used to cope with a threat (Sherman and Cohen 2006). Thus, the effects of negative evaluation can be buffered by, for example, reminding people of values they hold as important and giving people an opportunity to express themselves in a value-consistent manner (McQueen and Klein 2006). Importantly, self-affirmation is posited as distinct from general positive feeling about the self (Steele and Liu 1988; McQueen and Klein 2006). General positive mood and high self-esteem are other components of the psychological immune system suggesting its effective functioning (Wilson and Gilbert 2008). Thus, to detect self-affirmation effects in countering the immune neglect present in affective forecasts of negative events, both positive mood and self-esteem need to be ruled out as alternative explanations. Self-affirmation reduces psychological and biological responses to stressors, and can make cognitive appraisals of events less threatening and more manageable (Creswell et al. 2005; Sherman et al. 2009a; Sherman et al. 2000). Additionally, cognitive appraisals themselves initiate a coping process within emotional responding to manage stress and threats from negative events (Lazarus and Folkman 1987). Responding affectively to events, negative or positive, is influenced by the attention paid to the event and the consequent reaction, explanation, and adaptation to the event (Wilson and Gilbert 2008). The Attention, Reaction, Explanation, and Adaptation (AREA) model of affective adaptation suggests that emotional responses occur when there is an attention-grabbing, self-relevant, and unexplained event. This emotional response weakens over time with comprehension of the event, which can occur through explanatory processes such as appraisals (Wilson and Gilbert 2008). With appraisals also functioning in the psychological immune system, self-affirmation may serve to weaken affective forecasts and enable affective adaptation to imagined events. How might self-affirmation and cognitive appraisals influence immune neglect and affective forecasts of negative events? First, because self-affirmation activates coping processes, self-affirming prior to affective forecasting could undermine immune neglect by bringing online the cognitive appraisal coping processes (i.e. parts of the psychological immune system) that are typically activated by actual threats. In other words, the coping resources activated by self-affirmation could induce forecasters to appraise potential or imagined negative events as less threatening, just as those resources help people appraise actual threats less negatively. Thus, appraisals are the means by which the coping responses inherent in self-affirmation act to influence affective reactions and affective forecasts. Second, because self-affirmation activates the psychological immune system, increases resiliency, and decreases appraisals of threat when confronted with only a negative or threatening event (Steele 1988; McQueen and Klein 2006; Sherman and Hartson 2011), self-affirmation may also impact affective forecasting of only negative real or imagined events. That is, self-affirmation may impact immune neglect and thus reduce the extremity of forecasts for negative events, but not change forecasting of positive events. Because coping mechanisms are less relevant to positive events, any influence that self-affirmation has via coping mechanisms will temper forecasts of negative events, but will leave affective forecasts of positive events unchanged. We tested this hypothesis in two experiments. Experiment 1 Participants in self-affirmation and no-affirmation conditions forecast their affective reactions to either a negative or positive event. The imagined events were the gain or loss of money events that could reasonably occur to any of the participants. The primary test of our hypothesis rested on the affective forecasts made. If self-affirmation counteracted immune neglect, we expected affective forecasts of negative but not positive events to be more moderate when people self-affirmed compared to when they did not. To gain further insight into the operation of self-affirmation, we measured appraisals as an index of coping mechanisms influencing immune neglect. We also measured baseline affect, positive mood, and relative attention to the event as an index of focalism, another source of affective forecasting bias (Wilson et al. 2000), to investigate alternative explanations. Method Participants and design Sixty-one students (20 men, 41 women) completed the study for partial course credit and were randomly assigned to a 2 (Self-affirmation or No-affirmation) 9 2 (Negative or Positive event) between-subjects design.

5 Procedure Following Gilbert et al. (1998), we established baseline levels of affect by asking participants to report the extent to which they felt each of eight emotions in general using 9-point scales anchored by 1 (not at all) and 9 (very much). Participants reports of feeling happy, cheerful, glad, and satisfied and their reverse-scored reports of feeling sad, depressed, down, and glum were averaged into a single baseline affect index (Cronbach s a =.93) with higher scores indicating more positive affect. Affirmation manipulation Following Sherman et al. (2000), Steele and Liu (1983) and Tesser and Cornell (1991) in a well-established affirmation manipulation, participants rank-ordered five values (aesthetic, political, religious, social, and theoretical) in order of personal importance. Participants then saw ten pairs of statements and were asked to assign four agreement points to one or the other of the statements. That is, they had 4 points to allot within each pair based on their agreement with one or the other statement. In the self-affirmation condition, one statement was always consistent with participants highest ranked value (allowing them to assert their value) and the other statement was irrelevant. Participants in the no-affirmation condition assigned points to either a statement that was consistent with their lowest ranked value or an irrelevant statement. Affective forecasts Participants then imagined either the positive or negative event. In the negative event condition, participants were asked to imagine receiving an unexpected bill for $300 from their school. In the positive event condition, participants were asked to imagine receiving an unexpected refund check for $600 from their school. 1 Participants used 9-point scales anchored by 1 (not at all) and 9 (very much) to forecast how they would feel immediately after this imagined event. Participants forecasts of feeling happy, cheerful, glad, and satisfied and their reverse-scored forecasts of feeling sad, depressed, down, and glum were averaged into a single affective forecast index (Cronbach s a =.97) so higher scores indicated more positive affect. 2 This measurement allowed us to create a standard index of affective forecasting that is generalizable across events while allowing for control of baseline affect. Appraisals Participants reported appraisals of the event by responding to How disturbing is this event? and How relieving is this event? using a 9-point scale 1 Money was doubled for the positive event condition in Experiment 1 because negative events generally have more impact than positive events (Taylor 1991). 2 Analyzing positive emotions separately revealed the same pattern as analysis of the composite index. anchored by 1 (not at all) and 9 (very much). After the disturbing item was reverse-scored, the two items were averaged into a single appraisal index (r =.51) with higher scores indicating more positive appraisals of the event. Disturbing and relieving appraisals were assessed to tap coping with or explaining an unexpected event in student life that may either be disturbing and negative (losing $300) or relieving and positive (gaining $600). 3 Focalism Relative focus on the central event was assessed with responses to four questions on 9-point scales anchored by 1 (not at all) and 9 (very much): How much did you focus on the event itself when making your predictions?, How much did the event alone influence your predictions?, Other than the event, how much did you focus on other things that could be going on in your life?, and How much did you consider aspects of the situation unrelated to the event when making your predictions? After reverse-scoring responses to the last two items, all four were averaged into a focalism index (Cronbach s a =.71) with higher scores indicating more focus on the central event. The focalism index was analyzed to examine whether focalism could account for the effect of self-affirmation on affective forecasting. Mood Participants reports, anchored by 1 (not at all) and 9 (very much), of how happy they felt right now and their reverse-scored reports of how sad they felt right now were averaged into a single positive mood index (r =.75) and analyzed to examine whether positive mood could account for the effect of self-affirmation in affective forecasting (Cohen et al. 2000; Sherman et al. 2000). Results and discussion Affective forecasts We subjected the affective forecast index to a 2 (Affirmation: Self-affirmation or No-affirmation) 9 2 (Event valence: Negative or Positive) ANCOVA that controlled for baseline affect. 4 Main effects of affirmation, F(1, 56) = 7.57, p =.008, g 2 p =.12, and event valence, F(1, 56) = 87.49, p \.001, g 2 p =.61, were qualified by a significant interaction, F(1, 56) = 9.53, p =.003, g 2 p =.15 3 Analyzing each appraisal separately in the mediated moderation revealed the same pattern as analysis of the composite index. The relief appraisal (b =.48, p \.001) mediated the affirmation by event valence interaction (b =-.09, p =.187) and the reverse scored disturbing appraisal (b =.24, p =.012) mediated the affirmation by event valence interaction (b =-.14, p =.071). Thus, we maintained a composite of the two items to better simultaneously assess appraisals for both the positive and negative events. 4 The covariate of baseline affect did not interact with the independent variables.

6 Forecast of Positive Affect (Fig. 1). Pairwise comparisons, using the Bonferroni correction, showed that affirmed participants expected to react just as positively to the positive event (M = 8.12, SE =.43) as participants who were not affirmed (M = 8.21, SE =.44), F(1, 56) =.02, p =.886. In contrast, affirmed participants expected to react more positively to the negative event (M = 5.44, SE =.44) than participants who were not affirmed (M = 2.87, SE =.43), F(1, 56) = 17.32, p \.001, g p 2 =.24. Consistent with our expectations, this asymmetrical pattern for positive and negative events suggested that self-affirmation reduced forecast extremity for negative events. We suggest that self-affirmation brings on-line explanatory and construal processes (i.e. appraisals) that specifically counteracted the typical effects of immune neglect on negative forecasts. Appraisals Non-affirmed Affirmed Negative Event Positive Event Event Valence Fig. 1 Affective forecast positivity as a function of affirmation condition and the valence of the imagined event, Experiment 1 To further explore the operation of the psychological immune system consequences of self-affirmation, appraisals of the event were subjected to an ANOVA. Main effects of affirmation, F(1, 57) = 6.75, p =.012, g p 2 =.11, and event valence, F(1, 57) = 52.24, p \.001, g p 2 =.48, were qualified by an affirmation by event valence interaction, F(1, 57) = 9.62, p =.003, g p 2 =.14. Pairwise comparisons, using the Bonferroni correction, showed that the positive event was appraised similarly by affirmed participants (M = 7.53, SE =.46) and participants who were not affirmed (M = 7.77, SE =.47), F(1, 57) =.13, p =.721. In contrast, affirmed participants appraised the negative event as less disturbing (M = 5.60, SE =.47) than participants who were not affirmed (M = 2.93, SE =.47), F(1, 57) = 15.98, p \.001, g p 2 =.22. Thus, the interaction pattern for appraisals mirrored the interaction pattern observed for affective forecasts, further implicating the activation of the psychological immune system. Mediation by appraisals Self-affirmation influenced affective forecasting for the negative event only, so we conducted mediated moderation analyses (see Baron and Kenny 1986; Muller et al. 2005)to test whether appraisals mediated the interactive effect of affirmation and event valence on affective forecasting. 5 Because the affirmation by event valence interaction pattern on the appraisal index matched the interaction pattern on the affective forecast index, one mediation pattern may be that the appraisals were directly related to forecasts despite positivity or negativity of the imagined event. An alternative possibility may be that the appraisal index is relevant only to affective forecasting for the negative event and unrelated to affective forecasting for the positive event (given that appraisals function as a coping mechanism for negative events). The mediated moderation model tested for both possibilities by examining the direct relation of appraisal on forecasting and the appraisal by event valence interaction on forecasting. We confirmed that the affirmation (no affirmation =-1, affirmation = 1) by event valence (negative =-1, positive = 1) interaction significantly predicted the affective forecast index, b =-.24, p =.003, and appraisal index, b =-.28, p =.003. Following Muller et al. (2005), when baseline affect, affirmation, event valence, the affirmation by event valence interaction, appraisal index, and the appraisal by event valence interaction were included as simultaneous predictors of the affective forecast index, the affirmation by event valence interaction was no longer significant, b =-.05, p =.417, but appraisals of the event were significantly related to forecasts, b =.50, p \.001. Further, the event valence by appraisal interaction was also significant, b =-.15, p =.040, reflecting the expected pattern that appraisals were strongly related to forecasts for the negative imagined event, b =.70, p \.001, but not related to forecasts for the positive event, b =.30, p =.054. Thus, affirmation improved appraisals of the negative event itself, consistent with activated coping resources, and appraisals were only related to affective forecasts for the negative event, not positive event. 5 Because the affective forecasts preceded the completion of the focalism items, we chose to also assess appraisals of the imagined event after affective forecasts to make both mediators as comparable as possible. Mediation tests do not require that mediators be measured before dependent variables (MacKinnon 2008; Wood et al. 2007).

7 Focalism When the focalism index was subjected to an ANOVA, there was no main effect of affirmation, F(1, 57) =.05, p =.825, g p 2 =.00, nor event valence, F(1, 57) =.87, p =.355, g p 2 =.02. However, a significant affirmation by event valence interaction emerged, F(1, 57) = 4.93, p =.030, g p 2 =.08. Pairwise comparisons, using the Bonferroni correction, showed that self-affirmation did not affect participants relative attention to the positive event [Self-affirmation M = 6.25, SE =.37; No-affirmation, M = 5.50, SE =.38; F(1, 57) = 2.03, p =.160] or the negative event [Self-affirmation M = 5.07, SE =.38; Noaffirmation M = 5.98, SE =.38), F(1, 57) = 2.94, p =.092]. Instead, the focalism interaction was driven by affirmed participants paying more relative attention to the positive event than the negative event, F(1, 57) = 5.06, p =.028, g p 2 =.08. The focalism index was not affected by affirmation or event valence. Additionally, the interaction pattern differed from that of the affective forecast index and the appraisal index. Thus, focalism was ruled out as an explanation of the affirmation by event valence interaction effect on affective forecasting. 6 Mood 6 Additional analysis of participants in the negative event condition also showed that when affirmation, appraisals, and focalism were entered simultaneously, only appraisals remained a significant predictor of forecasts, b =.60, p \.001. Furthermore, when standardized appraisal and focalism indices were entered as a repeated measure into a mixed model ANOVA with affirmation and valence as between subjects factors, the two measures were revealed as distinct, with only appraisals being affected by affirmation for the negative event, consistent with our primary reported results. As expected, the lack of significant main effects or interaction on the positive mood index revealed that the affirmation manipulation did not impact current affective states. Affirmed participants reported similar levels of positive affect (M = 6.19, SE =.29) to non-affirmed participants [M = 6.98, SE =.30; F(1, 57) = 3.69, p =.060]. Additionally, neither positive (M = 6.85, SE =.29) nor negative (M = 6.32, SE =.30) event valence affected positive mood, F(1, 57) = 1.66, p =.202, and there was no interaction between affirmation and event valence, F(1, 57) =.01, p =.940. Moreover, there was no correlation between participants affective forecasts and positive mood, r =-.05, p =.726. Overall, these findings are consistent with the idea that self-affirmation weakened the extremity of affective forecasting about a negative event by activating the psychological immune system and reducing bias from immune neglect. The enhanced coping processes that selfaffirmation is known to bring on-line improved appraisals of the negative event and these in turn accounted for the more favorable affective forecasts for imagined negative events. Self-affirmation uniquely impacted affective forecasts for negative rather than positive events, consistent with the immune neglect hypothesis. Although the appraisal index assessed evaluations of the event itself that are relevant to the explanatory process of the affective adaptation model (Wilson and Gilbert 2008) and the forecast reflected anticipated reactions to the event relevant to the reacting and adapting processes of the AREA model, one concern might be that the appraisal measure was not distinct from the forecast measure. If the appraisal measure and forecast measure were empirically indistinguishable, then the two measures would be highly correlated across all conditions. However, the data strongly refute this concern. The measures were associated only when participants produced forecasts for the negative event, indicating that the two measures captured different aspects of the forecasting process. Even in the negative event condition, where appraisal was most strongly related to forecasting, appraisal and forecasting shared only 14 % of variance, indicating that the two measures reflect different constructs. These results support the notion that selfaffirmation can change the way that negative events are perceived. Consequently, affirmation can change how people expect to react to anticipated negative events. Self-affirmation likely influenced affective forecasting by activating coping resources that, in turn, influenced downstream appraisals of the events (Sherman et al. 2009b). Additionally, we expected affirmation to influence forecasting through appraisal. We did not expect affirmation to influence focalism on the central event or participants current mood. The findings were consistent with both those expectations. Experiment 2 In Experiment 2, we sought to replicate the results of Experiment 1 and provide evidence that another resource, self-esteem, could not explain the impact of self-affirmation on affective forecasts. The second experiment closely followed the design and procedures of Experiment 1. However, trait and state self-esteem were assessed as well as mood. Additionally, despite the well-established nature of our affirmation manipulation in Experiment 1, we implemented a different manipulation in order to generalize across methods. Finally, to rule out any possible influence of the different amounts of money gained or lost in Experiment 1, participants imagined losing the same amount of money in the negative event condition as they imagined gaining in the positive condition.

8 Method Participants and design Forty-seven students (22 men, 25 women) at a large Western university were paid $5 to participate and were randomly assigned to a 2 (Affirmation: Self-affirmation or No-affirmation) 9 2 (Event valence: Negative or Positive) between-subjects design. Procedure Participants reported the extent to which they felt happy, cheerful, glad, and satisfied in general using 9-point scales anchored by 1 (not at all) and 9 (very much; Cronbach s a =.93) as a measure of their baseline affect. Affirmation manipulation Participants completed another well-established essay-writing self-affirmation task (McQueen and Klein 2006; Sherman and Cohen 2006; Sherman et al. 2009b). First, they ranked 10 personal values (e.g., sense of humor, social skills, business/money) in order of importance and subsequently completed a writing exercise. Participants in the self-affirmation condition had unlimited time to write an open-ended essay about their most important value. Specifically, they were asked to describe why they ranked this value as most important to them and to describe a time in their life when it was important to them. In contrast, participants in the no-affirmation condition wrote an essay about their 9th ranked value in which they explained why this value might be important to a typical student at their university and imagined a time when this value would be important in the life of a typical student at their school. Affective forecast Participants in the negative event condition imagined receiving an unexpected bill for $300 from their school, whereas participants in the positive event condition imagined receiving an unexpected refund of $300 from their school. Participants then used 9-point scales anchored by 1 (not at all) and 9 (very much) to forecast how happy, cheerful, glad, and satisfied they would feel immediately after the imagined event occurred (Cronbach s a =.99). Appraisals Participants reported appraisals of the event identical to Study 1. The reverse scored appraisal of how disturbing the event was averaged with the appraisal of how relieving the event was so that higher values indicated more positive appraisals of the event (r =.54). Focalism Participants used items identical to Study 1 to report relative attention to the event versus other possible information. After reverse-scoring the items assessing increased attention to other contextual information, the four items were averaged into a focalism index (Cronbach s a =.67) with higher scores indicating more focus on the central event. 7 Self-esteem Participants responded to items from one trait and two state self-esteem measures presented in random order. Ten items from Rosenberg s (1965) trait selfesteem scale (e.g., On the whole, I am satisfied with myself.), as well as seven performance state self-esteem items (e.g., I feel frustrated or rattled by my performance.) and seven social state self-esteem items (e.g., I am worried about what other people think of me.) from Heatherton and Polivy s (1991) state self-esteem measure were completed using 7 point scales anchored by 1 (strongly disagree) and 7 (strongly agree). After reverse-scoring as appropriate, items were averaged into three reliable scales of trait selfesteem (Cronbach s a =.91), performance state self-esteem (Cronbach s a =.90), and social state self-esteem (Cronbach s a =.89). Mood Participants reported their current mood by answering a different question from Experiment 1, in order to generalize across mood measures. Participants responded to the question How would you describe your mood right now? using a 9-point scale anchored by 1 (very bad mood) and 9 (very good mood). Results and discussion Affective forecasts Subjecting the affective forecast index to a 2 (Affirmation) 9 2 (Event valence) ANCOVA that controlled for baseline affect revealed a main effect of event valence, F(1, 42) = 61.77, p \.001, g p 2 =.60, but no main effect of the affirmation manipulation, F(1, 42) = 1.13, p =.293. More importantly, a significant affirmation by event valence interaction emerged once again, F(1, 42) = 5.01, p =.031, g p 2 =.11 (Fig. 2). Pairwise comparisons showed that affirmed participants expected to react just as positively to the positive event (M = 7.19, SE =.57) as participants who were not affirmed (M = 7.87, SE =.57), F(1, 42) =.70, p =.406. In contrast, affirmed participants expected to react more positively to the negative event (M = 3.93, SE =.57) than participants who were not affirmed (M = 2.01, SE =.60), F(1, 42) = 5.33, p =.026, g p 2 =.11. This asymmetrical pattern for positive and negative events suggested that selfaffirmation reduced forecast extremity via immune neglect. 7 The set of appraisal items and the set of focalism items were presented in random order.

9 Forecast of Positive Affect Appraisals Non-affirmed Affirmed Similarly, a main effect of event valence on appraisals emerged, F(1, 43) = 39.70, p \.001, g p 2 =.48, but no main effect of the affirmation was observed, F(1, 43) = 3.25, p =.329. An affirmation by event valence interaction emerged again, F(1, 43) = 8.20, p =.006, g p 2 =.16. Pairwise comparisons showed that the positive event was appraised similarly by affirmed participants (M = 6.75, SE =.53) and participants who were not affirmed (M = 7.75, SE =.53), F(1, 43) = 1.80, p =.187. In contrast, affirmed participants appraised the negative event as less disturbing (M = 4.92, SE =.53) than participants who were not affirmed (M = 2.86, SE =.55), F(1, 43) = 7.25, p =.010, g p 2 =.14. As in Experiment 1, the pattern of appraisals mirrored the pattern of affective forecasting. Mediation by appraisals Negative Event Positive Event Event Valence Fig. 2 Affective forecast positivity as a function of affirmation condition and the valence of the imagined event, Experiment 2 We once again used mediated moderation analysis to test whether appraisals (driven by immune system functioning) mediated the affirmation by event valence effect on participants affective forecasting. We expected to replicate the pattern from Experiment 1 such that appraisals would be more strongly related to forecasts for negative events than forecasts for positive events. We confirmed the affirmation by event valence interaction on affective forecasts, b =-.21, p =.031, and appraisals, b =-.30, p =.006. When baseline affect, affirmation, event valence, the affirmation by event valence interaction, appraisals, and the appraisal by event valence interaction were included as simultaneous predictors of affective forecasts, the affirmation by event valence interaction on affective forecasts was no longer significant, b =-.02, p =.82, but appraisal was significantly related to affective forecasting, b =.58, p \.001. Further, as in Experiment 1, the appraisal by event valence interaction was significant, b =-.24, p =.018, which reflected a stronger association between appraisals and forecasts in the negative event condition, b =.82, p \.001, than in the positive event condition, b =.34, p =.039. Once again, this different pattern for negative and positive events demonstrated the empirical distinctiveness of the appraisal and forecast measures. More importantly, these results bolster the conclusions from Experiment 1 and provide further evidence that affirmation can improve forecasts for negative events specifically by engaging the psychological immune system s appraisal coping processes for explaining negative events. Focalism An identical ANOVA on the focalism measure showed that there were no significant main effects of affirmation, F(1, 43) =.01, p =.940, or valence F(1, 43) =.05, p =.833 on focalism. There was also no significant affirmation by event valence interaction, F(1, 43) = 3.96, p =.053. Pairwise comparisons showed that self-affirmation did not significantly affect focalism about the positive event [Self-affirmation M = 6.81, SE =.45; No-affirmation, M = 5.88, SE =.45; F(1, 43) = 2.18, p =.147] or the negative event (Self-affirmation M = 5.81, SE =.45; Noaffirmation M = 6.68, SE =.47), F(1, 43) = 1.79, p =.188). Moreover, there was no significant difference between negative and positive events for affirmed participants, F(1, 43) = 1.55, p =.221, or for non-affirmed participants, F(1, 43) = 2.48, p =.. There was no statistically significant effect of affirmation or event valence on focalism in this analysis. Thus, focalism was ruled out as an alternative explanation of the effect of affirmation on affective forecasting. 8 Self-esteem As expected, there were no main effects of affirmation on reported trait self-esteem (Self-affirmation M = 5.57, SE =.21; No-affirmation M = 5.28, SE =.22; F(1, 43) =.91, p =.346), state performance self-esteem [Selfaffirmation M = 5.26, SE =.24; No-affirmation M = 5.07, SE =.24; F(1, 43) =.32, p =.575], or on 8 As in Experiment 1, analysis of participants in the negative event condition showed that when affirmation, appraisals, and focalism were entered simultaneously, only appraisals remained a significant predictor of forecasts, b =.82, p \.001.

10 state social self-esteem [Self-affirmation M = 4.22, SE =.26; No-affirmation M = 3.82, SE =.27; F(1, 43) = 1.09, p =.303]. Further, there were no affirmation by event valence interactions on any of the three self-esteem measures [trait F(1, 43) =.14, p =.710; state performance F(1, 43) =.21, p =.649; state social F(1, 43) =.41, p =.528]. Together, these results indicate that self-esteem could not account for the effects of self-affirmation on affective forecasts. 9 Mood Once again, a 2 (Affirmation) 9 2 (Event valence) ANOVA on the positive mood index revealed that the affirmation manipulation did not impact positive mood [Self-affirmation M = 6.75, SE =.33; No-affirmation M = 6.35, SE =.34; main effect of affirmation F(1, 43) =.70, p =.401; main effect of valence F(1, 43) =.15, p =.703; affirmation by event valence interaction F(1, 43) =.10, p =.750]. Moreover, there was no correlation between participants affective forecasts and positive mood, r =.10, p =.507. General discussion Results from two studies show that self-affirmation can reduce the extremity of affective forecasting for negative imagined events. The pattern of affective forecasting and the full mediation by participants cognitive appraisals of the event suggest that self-affirmation activated psychological resources for coping. These resources influenced how people viewed the threatening event, in line with theoretical conceptions of self-affirmation and affective adaptation and coping (Steele 1988; McQueen and Klein 2006; Wilson and Gilbert 2008). When self-affirmation occurred, people viewed the event as less disturbing and, as a consequence, anticipated feeling better in reaction to it. That is, the activation of coping resources that typically occur with self-affirmation appeared to counter the neglect of coping resources typical of affective forecasting for negative events. Although the current findings are consistent with the interpretation that self-affirmation reduces bias in forecasting from immune neglect because of the activation of coping resources, we have no direct evidence of that activation of resources. Providing such evidence is not an easy task. Direct measurement of the coping processes activated 9 In a separate sample, no statistically significant effect of the affirmation manipulation was found on self-esteem or mood even when participants completed the measures immediately after the affirmation manipulation. by self-affirmation could make the putative impact of the self-affirmation manipulation obvious to participants. Unfortunately, such measurement could also impair the effectiveness of a self-affirmation manipulation, which functions best when its purpose is concealed (Sherman, Cohen et al. 2009). Because of this, we relied on the logic that affirmation-induced coping resources would be reflected in appraisal measures that are typically used to assess individuals perceptions of psychological resources and situational demands (Tomaka et al. 1993). Specifically, we assessed appraisals that change as a function of the coping processes that self-affirmation activates. This is also why we focused on appraisals that would be relatively more important to the negative event. This strategy allowed us to demonstrate the impact that self-affirmation had on reducing bias related to immune neglect. It is also possible that we failed to detect an effect of self-affirmation on forecasting for positive events because we used a composite measure of affect. However, examinations of each single affect item that constituted that measure also failed to show an affirmation effect. Thus, it is not the case that the effect was lost in the averaging process. Similarly, the composite appraisal measure may be less informative than the individual items that constitute it. However, examining the composite is the most theoretically sound approach in order to capture the potential variations in both positive and negative appraisals for both positive and negative hypothetical events. Appraisals of relief work well for positive events but not negative events; appraisals of being disturbed work well for negative events but not positive events. Therefore, their composite is best able to capture the full variation in appraisals that forecasters may make across negative and positive scenarios. Additionally, the high values of the forecasts for the positive event may suggest a ceiling effect that prevented self-affirmation from increasing positive forecasts. However, if self-affirmation was reducing bias in forecasting for positive events as for negative events, we would expect to see more muted forecasts of positive affect, rather than the positivity that we found or even more extreme positivity. Indeed, previous research has demonstrated that a pleasure paradox exists whereby the cognitive resources used to explain a positive event reduce people s actual and forecasted positive emotions (Wilson et al. 2005; Wilson and Gilbert 2005). We do not see this effect for forecasts for the positive event following our affirmation, suggesting that affirmation did not reduce the extremity of forecasts for positive events. Another possible explanation for self-affirmation s lack of effect on affective forecasting for positive events comes from the AREA model of affective adaptation (Wilson and Gilbert 2008) summarized earlier. This model holds that

11 emotional reactions to positive events might be mitigated by explanation and adaptation, just like emotional reactions to negative events. However, the model also states that affective adaptation, or subduing of an emotional reaction, is unnecessary if there is less uncertainty about the event itself. Thus, cognitive appraisal processes are not engaged, and the original emotional response is maintained. We suggest that students receiving a reimbursement from their university either quickly explain it as a benefit or at the very least, experience less uncertainty about and less need to explain this positive event. Thus, cognitive appraisals are not engaged as explanatory coping mechanisms, and there is no reduction in extremity of positive forecasts. Students learning that they must pay more to the university in the case of the negative event however, may experience uncertainty about why the university requires their money. They need to engage appraisals to cope with the unexpected and uncertain negative event, and the affirmation makes this possible. Therefore, we see reduced bias in forecasts for the negative event. To our knowledge, this is the first time that the implications of self-affirmation and appraisal coping processes on the immune neglect bias in affective forecasting have been explored and these results and mechanisms are ripe for further investigation. Finally, the current studies do not include groups of participants who actually experienced the negative or positive events that the affective forecasters imagined. This makes it unwise to draw strong conclusions about the accuracy of the forecasts that participants made. Nevertheless, the vast majority of studies indicate that errors in affective forecasts are due to overestimation, rather than underestimation, of affective reactions, making forecasts more extreme than experiences (see Wilson and Gilbert 2003). Thus, it seems reasonable to assume that using selfaffirmation to reduce the extremity of negative affective forecasts will also increase the accuracy of such forecasts; however, this remains a question open for research. These findings extend the conceptualization of self-affirmation theory, which has typically focused on how people cope with current threats and stressors, to situations where people contemplate imagined threats. In this study, an actual active threat was not necessary to observe the positive impact of self-affirmation. We argue that this provides a novel context in which self-affirmation can buffer against threats. We suggest that self-affirmation prior to an imagined threat is an effective intervention for reducing bias in affective forecasting, particularly if the threat could potentially happen in the future. For example, we may spend the week before asking our boss for a raise imagining that event (i.e. the threat). The event is imagined at this point, but could become real in the future. Our use of imagining losing or gaining tuition money may have had similar real potential for the students in these studies. Understandably, researchers often focus on how people deal with threats in the present, but cognitive representations of anticipated threats can also be influenced by selfaffirmation processes. Additionally, the focus on loss or gain could apply to any potential context in which we stand to lose or gain anything self-relevant, especially as concern for loss and gain is central to such motivational processes as regulatory focus in preventing or promoting outcomes (Higgins 1998). While these studies are the first (that we are aware of) to investigate whether self-affirmation can impact imagined events, there is some evidence that imagined threats function similarly to real threats in judgments (Skitka 2002). Additionally, other imagined or hypothetical events and emotions have been shown to affect emotional, social and motivational processes (e.g. Moons et al. 2015; Crisp and Turner 2009; Katzir et al. 2010). Perhaps self-affirmation may be bounded to affect only imagined events that are self-relevant or unexpected and negative, as we find. However, this suggests that future work should more closely examine how self-affirmation impacts construals of imagined, current, future, and past negative events. A more comprehensive understanding of how self-affirmation shapes such construals could reveal that simply affirming a central value makes asking for a raise, addressing marital issues, and visiting a physician less upsetting than people typically expect. References Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2000). When beliefs yield to evidence: Reducing biased evaluation by affirming the self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, Creswell, J. D., Welch, W. T., Taylor, S. E., Sherman, D. K., Gruenewald, T. L., & Mann, T. (2005). Affirmation of personal values buffers neuroendocrine and psychological stress responses. Psychological Science, 16, Crisp, R. J., & Turner, R. N. (2009). Can imagined interactions produce positive perceptions? Reducing prejudice through simulated social contact. American Psychologist, 64, Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science, 317, Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in

12 experimental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 1 41). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Katzir, M., Eyal, T., Meiran, N., & Kessler, Y. (2010). Imagined positive emotions and inhibitory control: The differentiated effect of pride versus happiness. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, Langston, C. A. (1994). Capitalizing on and coping with daily-life events: Expressive responses to positive events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1987). Transactional theory and research on emotions and coping. European Journal of Personality, 1, MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. New York: Taylor & Francis Group/Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. McQueen, A., & Klein, W. M. P. (2006). Experimental manipulation of self-affirmation: A systematic review. Self and Identity, 5, Moons, W. G., Chen, J. M., & Mackie, D. M. (2015). Stereotypes: A source of bias in affective and empathic forecasting. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. doi: / Muller, D., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2005). When moderation is mediated and mediation is moderated. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Sherman, D. K., Bunyan, D. P., Creswell, J. D., & Jaremka, L. M. (2009a). Psychological vulnerability and stress: The effects of self-affirmation on sympathetic nervous system responses to naturalistic stressors. Health Psychology, 28, Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of selfdefense: Self-affirmation theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp ). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Sherman, D. K., Cohen, G. L., Nelson, L. D., Nussbaum, A. D., Bunyan, D. P., & Garcia, J. (2009b). Affirmed yet unaware: Exploring the role of awareness in the process of self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, Sherman, D. K., & Hartson, K. A. (2011). Reconciling self-protection with self-improvement: Self-affirmation theory. In M. D. Alicke & C. Sedikides (Eds.), Handbook of self-enhancement and selfprotection (pp ). New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Sherman, D. A. K., Nelson, L. D., & Steele, C. M. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten the self? Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, Skitka, L. J. (2002). Do the means always justify the ends, or do the ends sometimes justify the means? A value protection model of justice reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 21, pp ). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Steele, C. M., & Liu, T. J. (1983). Dissonance processes as selfaffirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, Tesser, A., & Cornell, D. P. (1991). On the confluence of selfprocesses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27, Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kelsey, R. M., & Leitten, C. L. (1993). Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, Wilson, T. D., Centerbar, D. B., Kermer, D. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). The pleasures of uncertainty: Prolonging positive moods in ways people do not anticipate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp ). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press. Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting: Knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Explaining away: A model of affective adaptation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, Wood, R. E., Goodman, J. S., Beckmann, N., & Cook, A. (2007). Mediation testing in management research: A review and proposals. Organizational Research Methods, 11,

Self-Affirmation As A Stress BUffer. Denice Higareda

Self-Affirmation As A Stress BUffer. Denice Higareda Self-Affirmation As A Stress BUffer Denice Higareda Context Stress Impairs Cognitive Function Carmen Sandi In two or three Things tocolumns Know Self-affirmation The act of acknowledging one's values Experimental

More information

The Role of Self-affirmation and Self-expansion on State Self-esteem. Research Thesis

The Role of Self-affirmation and Self-expansion on State Self-esteem. Research Thesis The Role of Self-affirmation and Self-expansion on State Self-esteem Research Thesis Presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements for graduation with research distinction in Psychology in the undergraduate

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

GOLDSMITHS Research Online Article (refereed)

GOLDSMITHS Research Online Article (refereed) GOLDSMITHS Research Online Article (refereed) Davies, Martin F. Irrational beliefs and unconditional self-acceptance. II. Experimental evidence linking two key features of REBT Originally published in

More information

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance 1 Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Erica Behrens Winona State University Abstract This study examines the effects of self-consciousness

More information

Working for the Self or Working for the Group: How Self- Versus Group Affirmation Affects Collective Behavior in Low-Status Groups

Working for the Self or Working for the Group: How Self- Versus Group Affirmation Affects Collective Behavior in Low-Status Groups Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 96, No. 1, 183 202 0022-3514/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013068 Working for the Self or Working for the Group:

More information

The Group as a Resource: Reducing Biased Attributions for Group Success and Failure via Group Affirmation

The Group as a Resource: Reducing Biased Attributions for Group Success and Failure via Group Affirmation The Group as a Resource: Reducing Biased Attributions for Group Success and Failure via Group Affirmation David K. Sherman Zoe Kinias Brenda Major Heejung S. Kim University of California, Santa Barbara

More information

Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers

Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers Marium Din 1, Samra Afzal 2 1,2 National University of Modern Languages, Islamabad, Pakistan dr.mariumdin@gmail.com, afzalsamra@yahoo.com

More information

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY Volume 7, Number 4 Submitted: August 27, 2001 Resubmitted (first): November 13, 2001 Resubmitted (second): November 15, 2001 Resubmitted (third): November 21, 2001

More information

Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins

Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins Columbia University ABSTRACT Where does value come from? I propose a new answer to this classic question. People experience

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Aspects of Affective Forecasting

Aspects of Affective Forecasting Aspects of Affective Forecasting Then: The Peak-End Model of Memory for Experienced Utility Psychology 466: Judgment & Decision Making Instructor: John Miyamoto 11/21/2017: Lecture 09-1 Note: This Powerpoint

More information

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE Personal Report JOHN SMITH 2017 MySkillsProfile. All rights reserved. Introduction The EIQ16 measures aspects of your emotional intelligence by asking you questions

More information

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Adam Hahn 1 Bertram Gawronski 2 Word count: 20,754 excluding acknowledgements, abstract,

More information

Contents. Chapter. A Closer Look at You. Section 17.1 Your Personality Section 17.2 Develop a Positive Attitude. Chapter 17 A Closer Look at You

Contents. Chapter. A Closer Look at You. Section 17.1 Your Personality Section 17.2 Develop a Positive Attitude. Chapter 17 A Closer Look at You Chapter 17 A Closer Look at You Chapter 17 A Closer Look at You Contents Section 17.1 Your Personality Section 17.2 Develop a Positive Attitude 1 Section 17.1 Your Personality Personality develops from

More information

Post-Hoc Rationalism in Science. Eric Luis Uhlmann. HEC Paris. (Commentary on Jones and Love, "Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment?

Post-Hoc Rationalism in Science. Eric Luis Uhlmann. HEC Paris. (Commentary on Jones and Love, Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment? Post-Hoc Rationalism in Science Eric Luis Uhlmann HEC Paris (Commentary on Jones and Love, "Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment? On the Explanatory Status and Theoretical Contributions of Bayesian

More information

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about?

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about? 2 Asking and answering research questions What s it about? (Social Psychology pp. 24 54) Social psychologists strive to reach general conclusions by developing scientific theories about why people behave

More information

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Uri Hasson (uhasson@princeton.edu) Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 USA Geoffrey P. Goodwin (ggoodwin@princeton.edu)

More information

Stress, Health, & Coping. Radwan Banimustafa MD

Stress, Health, & Coping. Radwan Banimustafa MD Stress, Health, & Coping Radwan Banimustafa MD Introduction: What Is Stress? Stress a negative emotional state occurring in response to events that are perceived as taxing or exceeding a person s resources

More information

Anticipated Emotions as Guides to Choice

Anticipated Emotions as Guides to Choice 210 VOLUME 10, NUMBER 6, DECEMBER 2001 Anticipated Emotions as Guides to Choice Barbara A. Mellers 1 and A. Peter McGraw Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio Abstract When

More information

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect Abstract This article reports the effects of hedonic versus utilitarian consumption goals on consumers choices between

More information

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals SOCIAL INFLUENCE 2011, 6 (1), 15 21 The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals Esther S. Jeong 1, Yue Shi 1, Anna Baazova 1, Christine

More information

Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1. Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him. Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2

Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1. Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him. Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2 Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1 Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2 1 University of California, Berkeley 2 Cornell University Author Note

More information

SOCI 323 Social Psychology

SOCI 323 Social Psychology SOCI 323 Session 7 Theories of Attribution Lecturer: Dr. Peace Mamle Tetteh, Department of Sociology Contact Information: ptetteh@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of Continuing and Distance Education

More information

Positive Mood and Future-Oriented Self-Evaluation

Positive Mood and Future-Oriented Self-Evaluation Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 29, No. 4, December 2005 ( C 2005) DOI: 10.1007/s11031-006-9011-3 Positive Mood and Future-Oriented Self-Evaluation Ben Gervey, 1 Eric R. Igou, 2 and Yaacov Trope 1,3 Published

More information

Thomas G. Plante & Jesus Mejia

Thomas G. Plante & Jesus Mejia Psychometric Properties of the Santa Clara Brief Compassion Scale Thomas G. Plante & Jesus Mejia ogy ISSN 0031-2789 DOI 10.1007/s11089-016-0701-9 1 23 Your article is protected by copyright and all rights

More information

Stress Prevention in 6 Steps S T E P 3 A P P R A I S E : C O G N I T I V E R E S T R U C T U R I N G

Stress Prevention in 6 Steps S T E P 3 A P P R A I S E : C O G N I T I V E R E S T R U C T U R I N G Stress Prevention in 6 Steps S T E P 3 A P P R A I S E : C O G N I T I V E R E S T R U C T U R I N G 6 steps overview 1. Assess: Raising Awareness 2. Avoid: Unnecessary stress; problem solving 3. Appraise

More information

AFFIRMATION AND WELL-BEING 1

AFFIRMATION AND WELL-BEING 1 AFFIRMATION AND WELL-BEING 1 Spontaneous Self-Affirmation is Associated with Psychological Well-being: Evidence from a U.S. National Adult Survey Sample AFFIRMATION AND WELL-BEING 2 Abstract Emerging evidence

More information

Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members

Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members Margaret E. Brooks, Eyal Grauer, Erin E. Thornbury, and Scott Highhouse Bowling Green State University The scientist-practitioner

More information

Combating the Negative Effects of Stereotypes: Improving Minority Performance with a Values- Affirmation Intervention

Combating the Negative Effects of Stereotypes: Improving Minority Performance with a Values- Affirmation Intervention Combating the Negative Effects of Stereotypes: Improving Minority Performance with a Values- Affirmation Intervention Valerie Purdie-Vaughns Columbia University The Project of Inclusive Leadership Research

More information

Validating Measures of Self Control via Rasch Measurement. Jonathan Hasford Department of Marketing, University of Kentucky

Validating Measures of Self Control via Rasch Measurement. Jonathan Hasford Department of Marketing, University of Kentucky Validating Measures of Self Control via Rasch Measurement Jonathan Hasford Department of Marketing, University of Kentucky Kelly D. Bradley Department of Educational Policy Studies & Evaluation, University

More information

EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY. Pages Social Identity 4:22

EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY. Pages Social Identity 4:22 EVALUATE SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY Pages 106 108 Social Identity 4:22 HENRI TAJFEL S SOCIAL IDENTITY THEORY Individuals strive to improve their self image by trying to enhance their selfesteem, based on:

More information

THE UNDESIRED SELF AND EMOTIONAL EXPERIENCE: A LATENT VARIABLE ANALYSIS

THE UNDESIRED SELF AND EMOTIONAL EXPERIENCE: A LATENT VARIABLE ANALYSIS THE UNDESIRED SELF AND EMOTIONAL EXPERIENCE: A LATENT VARIABLE ANALYSIS By: Ann G. Phillips, Paul J. Silvia, and Matthew J. Paradise Phillips, A. G., Silvia, P. J., & Paradise, M. J. (2007). The undesired

More information

WHAT IS SELF? MODULE-IV OBJECTIVES 16.1 CONCEPT OF SELF. What is Self? Self and Personality. Notes

WHAT IS SELF? MODULE-IV OBJECTIVES 16.1 CONCEPT OF SELF. What is Self? Self and Personality. Notes What is Self? MODULE-IV 16 WHAT IS SELF? Self is focus of our everyday behaviour and all of us do have a set of perceptions and beliefs about ourselves. This kind of self concept plays important role in

More information

Regression Discontinuity Analysis

Regression Discontinuity Analysis Regression Discontinuity Analysis A researcher wants to determine whether tutoring underachieving middle school students improves their math grades. Another wonders whether providing financial aid to low-income

More information

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Heather Dudley What influences people s willingness to interact with someone of a different cultural background from

More information

Perceived discrimination and academic resilience: A study of Albanian immigrant adolescents in Greece

Perceived discrimination and academic resilience: A study of Albanian immigrant adolescents in Greece Perceived and academic resilience: A study of Albanian immigrant adolescents in Greece Frosso Motti-Stefanidi, Vassilis Pavlopoulos Department of Psychology, University of Athens, Greece Jelena Obradović,

More information

The Wellbeing Course. Resource: Mental Skills. The Wellbeing Course was written by Professor Nick Titov and Dr Blake Dear

The Wellbeing Course. Resource: Mental Skills. The Wellbeing Course was written by Professor Nick Titov and Dr Blake Dear The Wellbeing Course Resource: Mental Skills The Wellbeing Course was written by Professor Nick Titov and Dr Blake Dear About Mental Skills This resource introduces three mental skills which people find

More information

Emotional Intelligence (EI)

Emotional Intelligence (EI) Emotional Intelligence (EI) During Fall semester 0 surveys were collected on students enrolled in RTM 90 using the EI short form of the standardized instrument and available online. A copy of the form

More information

Chapter 2: Purpose in Life

Chapter 2: Purpose in Life Chapter 2: Purpose in Life Multiple Choice 1. Yalom identified four topics for existential psychology. They did not include: a. purpose in life. b. choice and responsibility. c. change and impermanence.

More information

Chapter 4: The self. What s it about?

Chapter 4: The self. What s it about? Chapter 4: The self What s it about? (Social Psychology pp. 95 140) People construct the self-concept in much the same way as they form impressions of others. According to self-perception theory, they

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46 (2010) 361 366 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Social Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Report Dissonance

More information

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site SEUNGHO CHO shcho72@gmail.com LAURA RICHARDSON WALTON lwalton@comm.msstate.edu Mississippi

More information

Running head: AFFECTIVE FORECASTING AND OBJECTIFICATION 1

Running head: AFFECTIVE FORECASTING AND OBJECTIFICATION 1 Running head: AFFECTIVE FORECASTING AND OBJECTIFICATION 1 The Effect of Demographics, Body Shame, Ambivalent Sexism, and Enjoyment of Sexualization on Anticipated Emotion in Response to Sexual Objectification

More information

The Effects of Uncertainty on Interpersonal Relations. in Terms of Prolonged Satisfaction Ratings. John Smith. Roanoke College

The Effects of Uncertainty on Interpersonal Relations. in Terms of Prolonged Satisfaction Ratings. John Smith. Roanoke College Running head: UNCERTAINTY AND INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS 1 The Effects of Uncertainty on Interpersonal Relations in Terms of Prolonged Satisfaction Ratings John Smith Roanoke College UNCERTAINTY AND INTERPERSONAL

More information

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) 1 VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) Thank you for providing us with the opportunity to revise our paper. We have revised the manuscript according to the editors and

More information

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2014, 42(7), 1105-1116 Society for Personality Research http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2014.42.7.1105 ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN

More information

Chapter 11 Nonexperimental Quantitative Research Steps in Nonexperimental Research

Chapter 11 Nonexperimental Quantitative Research Steps in Nonexperimental Research Chapter 11 Nonexperimental Quantitative Research (Reminder: Don t forget to utilize the concept maps and study questions as you study this and the other chapters.) Nonexperimental research is needed because

More information

INDIVIDUALS ARE COPING ALL THE TIME.

INDIVIDUALS ARE COPING ALL THE TIME. Coping Strategies INDIVIDUALS ARE COPING ALL THE TIME. COPING (CONTENDING) Coping: Is the behavior that protects us from becoming psychologically and physiologically disorganized. Usually incorporates

More information

Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The. Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp ). Blackwell Publishing.

Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The. Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp ). Blackwell Publishing. Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp. 864-867). Blackwell Publishing. Self-Compassion Self-compassion is an open-hearted way

More information

ATTITUDE CHANGE FROM AN IMPLIED THREAT TO ATTITUDINAL FREEDOM 1

ATTITUDE CHANGE FROM AN IMPLIED THREAT TO ATTITUDINAL FREEDOM 1 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1968, Vol. 8, No. 4, 324-330 ATTITUDE CHANGE FRO AN IPLIED THREAT TO ATTITUDINAL FREEDO 1 JOHN SENSENIG AND JACK W. BREH Duke University College students were

More information

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect. David K. Sherman

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect. David K. Sherman 1 Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect David K. Sherman University of California, Santa Barbara Traci Mann University of California,

More information

Mental contrasting of counterfactual fantasies attenuates disappointment, regret, and resentment

Mental contrasting of counterfactual fantasies attenuates disappointment, regret, and resentment Motiv Emot (2018) 42:17 36 DOI 10.1007/s11031-017-9644-4 ORIGINAL PAPER Mental contrasting of counterfactual fantasies attenuates disappointment, regret, and resentment Nora Rebekka Krott 1 Gabriele Oettingen

More information

Depression. Northumberland, Tyne and Wear NHS Trust (Revised Jan 2002) An Information Leaflet

Depression. Northumberland, Tyne and Wear NHS Trust (Revised Jan 2002) An Information Leaflet Depression Northumberland, Tyne and Wear NHS Trust (Revised Jan 2002) An Information Leaflet practical ldren 1 7XR isle, d n. ocial These are the thoughts of two people who are depressed: I feel so alone,

More information

Bouncing back from setbacks

Bouncing back from setbacks Bouncing back from setbacks The development of human resiliency is none other than the process of healthy human development. (Benard, B. 2004, Resiliency: What we have learned. p. 9) What began as a quest

More information

SUBSTITUTABILITY OF SELF-HANDICAPPING 1. Limitations on the Substitutability of Self-Protective Processes: Self-Handicapping is Not

SUBSTITUTABILITY OF SELF-HANDICAPPING 1. Limitations on the Substitutability of Self-Protective Processes: Self-Handicapping is Not SUBSTITUTABILITY OF SELF-HANDICAPPING 1 Limitations on the Substitutability of Self-Protective Processes: Self-Handicapping is Not Reduced by Related-Domain Self-Affirmations Sean M. McCrea University

More information

!!!!!!! !!!!!!!!!!!!

!!!!!!! !!!!!!!!!!!! Running head: VISUAL ADVERTISING ON SELF-REFERENCING AND EMPATHY 1 Effects of Visual Advertising on Self-Referencing and Empathy Towards Health Conditions Christine Cao and Jennifer Ball University of

More information

Detecting Suspect Examinees: An Application of Differential Person Functioning Analysis. Russell W. Smith Susan L. Davis-Becker

Detecting Suspect Examinees: An Application of Differential Person Functioning Analysis. Russell W. Smith Susan L. Davis-Becker Detecting Suspect Examinees: An Application of Differential Person Functioning Analysis Russell W. Smith Susan L. Davis-Becker Alpine Testing Solutions Paper presented at the annual conference of the National

More information

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect Motiv Emot (2006) 30:165 169 DOI 10.1007/s11031-006-9001-5 ORIGINAL PAPER Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect David K. Sherman Traci

More information

Social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) proposes

Social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) proposes I Continue to Feel So Good About Us: In-Group Identification and the Use of Social Identity Enhancing Strategies to Reduce Intragroup Dissonance Demis E. Glasford John Jay College and CUNY Graduate Center

More information

ANSWERS TO EXERCISES AND REVIEW QUESTIONS

ANSWERS TO EXERCISES AND REVIEW QUESTIONS ANSWERS TO EXERCISES AND REVIEW QUESTIONS PART THREE: PRELIMINARY ANALYSES Before attempting these questions read through Chapters 6, 7, 8, 9 and 10 of the SPSS Survival Manual. Descriptive statistics

More information

Managing emotions in turbulent and troubling times. Professor Peter J. Jordan Griffith Business School

Managing emotions in turbulent and troubling times. Professor Peter J. Jordan Griffith Business School Managing emotions in turbulent and troubling times Professor Peter J. Jordan Griffith Business School Overview Emotions and behaviour Emotional reactions to change Emotional intelligence What emotions

More information

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy Volume 17, Number 2, Summer 1999 CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

More information

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH The following document provides background information on the research and development of the Emergenetics Profile instrument. Emergenetics Defined 1. Emergenetics

More information

Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self-Affirming Implementation Intention

Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self-Affirming Implementation Intention Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self-Affirming Implementation Intention ARMITAGE, Christopher J and ARDEN, Madelynne A Available from

More information

Self-Handicapping Variables and Students' Performance

Self-Handicapping Variables and Students' Performance Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern SoTL Commons Conference SoTL Commons Conference Mar 12th, 4:00 PM - 5:45 PM Self-Handicapping Variables and Students' Performance Lugenia Dixon

More information

Organizational. Behavior 15th Global Edition. Chapter. Robbins and Judge. Emotions and Moods 3-0

Organizational. Behavior 15th Global Edition. Chapter. Robbins and Judge. Emotions and Moods 3-0 Organizational 4 Behavior 15th Global Edition Chapter Robbins and Judge Emotions and Moods 3-0 Why Were Emotions Ignored in OB? The Myth of Rationality Emotions were seen as irrational A well-run organization

More information

Identify the relationship among psychological stress, coping and physical illness Approaches to help with stress

Identify the relationship among psychological stress, coping and physical illness Approaches to help with stress Identify the relationship among psychological stress, coping and physical illness Approaches to help with stress What is stress? o nonspecific response of the body to any demand made upon it What is a

More information

Performance in competitive Environments: Gender differences

Performance in competitive Environments: Gender differences Performance in competitive Environments: Gender differences Uri Gneezy Technion and Chicago Business School Muriel Niederle Harvard University Aldo Rustichini University of Minnesota 1 Gender differences

More information

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies Emma ter Mors 1,2, Mieneke Weenig 1, Naomi Ellemers 1, Dancker Daamen 1 1 Leiden University,

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Loss Aversion Is an Affective Forecasting Error Deborah A. Kermer, 1 Erin Driver-Linn, 2 Timothy D. Wilson, 1 and Daniel T. Gilbert 2 1 University of Virginia and

More information

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Subhrasikha Naik Senior Study and research chronic diseases Participate in gathering data for Project Cope, which is focused

More information

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1995 The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the

More information

Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel. University of Miami. Jonah Berger. University of Pennsylvania

Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel. University of Miami. Jonah Berger. University of Pennsylvania Facebook Therapy 1 Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel University of Miami Jonah Berger University of Pennsylvania Word Count: 1000 Contact Information: Jonah

More information

EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIP AMONG SELF-AFFIRMATION, SELF- CONCEPT CLARITY AND REDUCED DEFENSIVENESS TO THREATS. Jennifer Lynne Cerully

EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIP AMONG SELF-AFFIRMATION, SELF- CONCEPT CLARITY AND REDUCED DEFENSIVENESS TO THREATS. Jennifer Lynne Cerully EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIP AMONG SELF-AFFIRMATION, SELF- CONCEPT CLARITY AND REDUCED DEFENSIVENESS TO THREATS Jennifer Lynne Cerully B.S. in Computer Science and Decision Science, Carnegie Mellon University,

More information

Few people would disagree with the idea that role models

Few people would disagree with the idea that role models PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN Marx, Roman / FEMALE ROLE MODELS Female Role Models: Protecting Women s Math Test Performance David M. Marx Jasmin S. Roman Harvard University Recent theory and

More information

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels A peer-reviewed electronic journal. Copyright is retained by the first or sole author, who grants right of first publication to Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation. Permission is granted to distribute

More information

TAT INTERPERSONAL DECENTERING AND SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING. James Nixon, B.S. Sharon Rae Jenkins, Ph. D. Brenton LaBrie, B.A.

TAT INTERPERSONAL DECENTERING AND SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING. James Nixon, B.S. Sharon Rae Jenkins, Ph. D. Brenton LaBrie, B.A. TAT INTERPERSONAL DECENTERING AND SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING James Nixon, B.S. Sharon Rae Jenkins, Ph. D. Brenton LaBrie, B.A. Abstract This study examined social understanding, defined as better attention to

More information

Egocentrism, Event Frequency, and Comparative Optimism: When What Happens Frequently Is More Likely to Happen to Me

Egocentrism, Event Frequency, and Comparative Optimism: When What Happens Frequently Is More Likely to Happen to Me 10.1177/0146167203256870 PERSONALITY Chambers et al. AND / EGOCENTRISM SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY AND EVENT BULLETIN FREQUENCY ARTICLE Egocentrism, Event Frequency, and Comparative Optimism: When What Happens Frequently

More information

Thank you very much for your guidance on this revision. We look forward to hearing from you.

Thank you very much for your guidance on this revision. We look forward to hearing from you. 1 PAINTED BUNTING MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) We would like to submit a revision of our manuscript. As you requested, we have run new studies that employ additional

More information

EVALUATION OF CHALLENGE STRESSORS: EVIDENCE FROM ISLAMIC AZAD UNIVERSITY

EVALUATION OF CHALLENGE STRESSORS: EVIDENCE FROM ISLAMIC AZAD UNIVERSITY EVALUATIO OF CHALLEGE STRESSORS: EVIDECE FROM ISLAMIC AZAD UIVERSITY DaryoushQorbani Department of Management, Germi Branch, Islamic Azad Unversity, Germi, Iran Mohammad Feizi 12 Department of Management,

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 4, Number 1 Submitted: August 20, 2008 Revisions: October 16, 2008 Accepted: October 17, 2008 Publication Date: October 20, 2008 Start Today or the Very

More information

Emotion Coaching. A tool to help you to work successfully with young people with SEN&D

Emotion Coaching. A tool to help you to work successfully with young people with SEN&D Emotion Coaching A tool to help you to work successfully with young people with SEN&D Created by Shane Dangar and Ellen Collard, Young People s Champions, Engagement and Participation team, Somerset County

More information

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity Measurement & Variables - Initial step is to conceptualize and clarify the concepts embedded in a hypothesis or research question with

More information

What Affects the Way Individuals Cope with Stress?

What Affects the Way Individuals Cope with Stress? LP 8D hardiness 1 What Affects the Way Individuals Cope with Stress? Hardiness (Commitment, Control and Challenge) Optimism versus pessimism (see chapter 3) Coping options o Change environments o Change

More information

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: SOCIAL AFFILIATION CUES PRIME HELP-SEEKING INTENTIONS 1 This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: Rubin, M. (2011). Social

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

The Paradox of Received Social Support

The Paradox of Received Social Support PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Paradox of Received Social Support The Importance of Natalya C. Maisel 1 and Shelly L. Gable 2 1 University of California, Los Angeles, and 2 University of California,

More information

The Power to Change Your Life: Ten Keys to Resilient Living Robert Brooks, Ph.D.

The Power to Change Your Life: Ten Keys to Resilient Living Robert Brooks, Ph.D. The Power to Change Your Life: Ten Keys to Resilient Living Robert Brooks, Ph.D. The latest book I co-authored with my colleague Dr. Sam Goldstein was recently released. In contrast to our previous works

More information

Supporting Information for How Public Opinion Constrains the U.S. Supreme Court

Supporting Information for How Public Opinion Constrains the U.S. Supreme Court Supporting Information for How Public Opinion Constrains the U.S. Supreme Court Christopher J. Casillas Peter K. Enns Patrick C. Wohlfarth Christopher J. Casillas is a Ph.D. Candidate in the Department

More information

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45 (2009) 927 932 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Social Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Report Cognitive

More information

Made available courtesy of the American Psychological Association:

Made available courtesy of the American Psychological Association: Contextual neglect, self-evaluation, and the frog-pond effect. By: Ethan Zell and Mark D. Alicke Zell, E., & Alicke, M. D. (2009). Contextual neglect, self-evaluation, and the frog-pond effect. Journal

More information

Do I Belong? It Depends on When You Ask

Do I Belong? It Depends on When You Ask Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 17, No. 4, 432 436 1099-9809/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0025455 Do I Belong? It Depends on When You Ask

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1

Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1 Running Head: STEREOTYPE THREAT AND THE RACIAL ACHIEVEMENT GAP 1 Stereotype Threat and the Racial Achievement Gap: Race, Salience of Race, and Perception of Race as Possible Contributing Factors Scientist

More information

The Association between Free Will Beliefs and Stereotypes: People's Belief in Fatalism Promotes Gender Stereotypes

The Association between Free Will Beliefs and Stereotypes: People's Belief in Fatalism Promotes Gender Stereotypes The Association between Free Will Beliefs and Stereotypes: People's Belief in Fatalism Promotes Gender Stereotypes Takumi Watanabe, University of Tokyo, Japan Kaori Karasawa, University of Tokyo, Japan

More information

How do People Really Think about Climate Change?

How do People Really Think about Climate Change? How do People Really Think about Climate Change? #LSESunstein Professor Cass Sunstein Robert Walmsley University Professor at Harvard Law School. Former Administrator of the White House Office of Information

More information

(TEST BANK for Organizational Behavior Emerging Knowledge Global Reality 7th Edition by Steven McShane, Mary Von Glinow)

(TEST BANK for Organizational Behavior Emerging Knowledge Global Reality 7th Edition by Steven McShane, Mary Von Glinow) Organizational Behavior Emerging Knowledge Global Reality 7th Edition McShane Test Bank Completed download: https://testbankreal.com/download/organizational-behavior-emergingknowledge-global-reality-7th-edition-test-bank-mcshane-glinow/

More information

Awareness of implicit bias : what motivates behavior change?

Awareness of implicit bias : what motivates behavior change? Honors Theses Psychology Spring 2012 Awareness of implicit bias : what motivates behavior change? Courtney R. Sanford Noah K. Henry-Darwish Penrose Library, Whitman College Permanent URL: http://hdl.handle.net/10349/1163

More information