Mechanisms Mediating Visual Detection in Static Perimetry

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Mechanisms Mediating Visual Detection in Static Perimetry"

Transcription

1 Mechanisms Mediating Visual Detection in Static Perimetry Ronald S. Harwerth,* Earl L. Smith III* and Louis DeSantis\ Purpose. The usual stimuli in static perimetry are white-light luminance increments. However, the specific visual detection mechanisms involved in perimetry are unknown because all classes of neural mechanisms are sensitive to spectrally broadband stimuli. The objective of this study was to determine the relative sensitivities of nonopponent and opponent detecting mechanisms under standard perimetry test conditions. Methods. Using trained rhesus monkey subjects, the relative sensitivities of the vision mechanisms for the detection of perimetry test stimuli were determined through psychophysical measurements of spectral sensitivity at each of the test field locations of the C24-2 threshold program on the Humphrey Field Analyzer (Allergan Humphrey, San Leandro, CA). The spectral sensitivity functions were analyzed by a three-channel model that incorporated independent short-wavelength-sensitive, nonopponent (luminance), and opponent (chromatic) spectral sensitivity mechanisms. Results. The visual detection mechanisms for perimetry thresholds varied as a function of the size and wavelength of the test field. With the perimeter's standard stimulus (Goldmann Size III) and bowl illumination (31.5 asb), the presence of a short-wavelength-sensitive mechanism was clearly evident at all field locations, but its relative sensitivity systematically declined with eccentricity. Under these conditions, the sensitivities of the opponent and nonopponent mechanisms were approximately equal at most field locations. With a larger stimulus (Goldmann Size V), however, the contribution of the opponent spectral sensitivity mechanism was more apparent over most of the central field and the alterations of sensitivity with eccentricity were less pronounced. In contrast, a small test field (Goldmann Size II) appeared to bias detection toward nonopponent mechanisms. Conclusion. The results of these investigations indicate that detection thresholds during perimetry can be effectively biased toward different photopic, visual processing channels through the appropriate selection of size and wavelength of the test stimulus. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993;34: Computerized light-sense perimetry is an important component in the diagnosis of ocular disorders affecting peripheral vision as, for instance, in the differentiation of glaucoma from ocular hypertension. 1 It has, however, been demonstrated that standard perimetric methods (white-light test stimuli superimposed on a white-light adaptation field) are not always sensitive From the *College of Optometry; University of Houston, Houston, and falcon Laboratories, Inc., Fort Worth, Texas. This study was supported in part by research funds from Alcon Laboratories, Inc., Fort Worth, Texas and Public Health Service research giants EY-OJ139 and EY , and core grant EY from the National Eye Institute, Bethesda, Maryland. Presented in part at the annual meeting of the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Sarasola, Florida, May 2, Submitted for publication f une 8, 1992; accepted February 10, Proprietary interest category: N. Reprint requests: Ronald S. Harwerth, College of Optometry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas measures of the state of the underlying retinal neurology. For example, Quigley et al 2 reported that perimetric visual field defects are poorly correlated with neural damage in retinas of glaucoma patients. It has been suggested that static perimetry thresholds are insensitive to early neural damage because white-light test stimuli can be detected by retinal mechanisms that are not affected early in the disease process. 3 ' 4 The validity of this type of explanation must be based on a knowledge of the detection mechanisms involved in perimetry thresholds and the alterations of these specific visual mechanisms by glaucoma. Neither is known, but such information would be of considerable value for the evaluation of perimetry with conventional testing parameters and/or to provide a rational basis for the development of alternative testing strategies. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 1993, Vol. 34, No. 10 Copyright Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 3011

2 3012 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 1993, Vol. 34, No. 10 The visual sensitivities of neural mechanisms can be classified by their spectral response properties as broadband (nonopponent) or chromatic (opponent) mechanisms. 5 These two classes of response properties are considered to originate from different neural combinations of the three cone types. 56 Because either of the two channels could be primary in the detection of perimetry stimuli, the general purpose of our experiments was to describe the relative sensitivities of color vision mechanisms in standard, clinical perimetry. Specifically, the sensitivities of detection mechanisms were assessed by an analysis of their contributions to the increment-threshold spectral sensitivity functions of observers with normal vision, under standard perimetry test conditions. Increment-threshold spectral sensitivity under white-light adaptation seems to depend on the sensitivities of several channels or mechanisms derived from either independent responses or interactions between the responses of retinal cones. 7 " 12 Although there are alternative models, 1314 the three photopic mechanisms most often considered to underlie the increment-threshold spectral sensitivity functions are: an independent short-wavelength-sensitive (SWS) mechanism reflecting the sensitivity of the SWS cones; a color-opponent (chromatic) mechanism derived from differential inputs of the middle-wavelength-sensitive and long-wavelength-sensitive cone classes; and a color nonopponent (luminance) mechanism derived from undifferentiated middle-wavelength-sensitive and long-wavelength-sensitive cone inputs. 8 " 12 It might be presumed that the luminance contrast of a white test field superimposed on a white background would be detected by the nonopponent.mechanisms. However, achromatic light could be detected by either opponent or nonopponent channels 1516 and factors other than the spectral composition of the test and background fields can influence the relative sensitivities of these channels. For example, the intensity of the adaptation field, 12 the size of the test field with respect to the size of the background, 17 the exposure duration of the test field, 1819 and the retinal location of the test field 20 are all factors that affect the balance of sensitivities between opponent and nonopponent channels. Some of the stimulus parameters used in conventional perimetry would cause a bias toward opponent channel detection, but others would bias detection toward nonopponent channels. Considering perimetry with the Humphrey Field Analyzer (Allergan Humphrey, San Leandro, CA) as a specific example, the moderately intense bowl illumination (31.5 asb; 10 cd/m 2 ) and relatively long stimulus duration (200 msec) would seem to favor detection by opponent mechanisms, while the relatively small test field (0.43 degrees for the standard Goldmann Size III stimulus) and extensive adaptation field probably favor detection by the nonopponent mechanisms. In addition, the relative weights of these various factors undoubtedly vary across the visual field. 21 Therefore, it is apparent that the relative roles of these mechanisms in lightsense perimetry must be assessed empirically. SUBJECTS AND METHODS Subjects The subjects for the current experiments were three 5-year-old, male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatto) who had been the subjects of our previous comparisons of the normal visualfieldsof humans and monkeys. 22 The monkey subjects were used in anticipation of studies of experimentally-induced ocular disorders and because the experimental requirements for these investigations would have been formidable for human observers. The complete series of experiments actually required more than 150 visual field measurements involving approximately 125,000 behavioral trials for each subject and were only practical by using surrogate human observers. Conversely, the use of monkeys as alternative subjects has little consequence for the general applicability of the findings because the visual capabilities of macaque monkeys, including perimetric thresholds, match those of human beings. 22 " 25 All of the experimental and animal care procedures adhered to the ARVO statement for the Use of Animals in Ophthalmic and Vision Research. Apparatus The visual fields of the monkeys were measured with a modified Humphrey Visual Field Analyzer that was attached to a small primate testing cubicle (BRS/LVE, Laurel, MD). The principal modifications of the perimeter, which did not alter the standard threshold procedures or data analysis programs, included the following: (1) A light-emitting diode fixation stimulus was placed in Maxwellian view to control the monkeys' head and eye positions. A luminance increment of the light-emitting diode was the test stimulus in at least 30% of the session trials. (2) A microcomputer eye-position monitor (Micromeasurements System 2000, Micromeasurements, Inc., Berkeley, CA), aligned with the fixation stimulus, caused an interruption of the behavioral paradigm if an eye movement greater than approximately 5 occurred during a trial. Infrared light for the eye-position monitor was provided by filtered tungsten lamps (Schott glass RG-715; Jenaer Glaswerk Schott and Gen., Mainz, Germany) mounted to the wall of the primate cubicle. (3) Custom readonly memory computer chips were provided by the perimeter's manufacturer that delayed movement of the projector for 2 seconds after the presentation of

3 Detection Mechanisms in Perimetry WAVELENGTH (NM) FIGURE l. Absorption spectra for the crystalline lenses of the three monkeys. The solid line, representing the absorption spectra of the monkeys' lenses, was derived from a modification of the absorption spectrum of lenses of human subjects of approximately 20 years of age (see references 29 and 30). The absorption spectra were obtained by determining the multiplication factor needed to scale the function for human subjects to fit the difference spectrum between the shortwavelength-sensitive photopigment and the monkey's spectral sensitivity for wavelengths between 420 and 480 nm (shown by the data symbols). The data represented by squares are on a true scale, the the other functions have been displaced upward (circles) or downward (triangles) by 1.0 log unit for clarity of presentation. the visual field stimuli and modified the testing protocol to necessitate discrete behavioral responses for each trial. (4) The timing of the perimeter's testing sequence was controlled and synchronized with the other events of the behavioral procedure by an external laboratory computer. (5) Narrow-band, monochromatic test stimuli were produced by interference filters with 10 nm half-band widths (Oriel Corp., Stratford, CT) located in the optical pathway for the test stimulus, between the light source and the field stop that sets the test field size. It was necessary to calibrate the monochromatic stimuli from measurements of light energy (EG&G model 550 radiometer/photometer, Salem, MA) ahead of the field stop. As a result, the relative stimulus intensities were accurately defined,but it was not possible to obtain absolute calibration of the visual stimuli on the surface of the perimeter bowl. Therefore, the light energies were converted to relative quantum values and, for convenience, an arbitrary constant was used to adjust the monkeys' threshold values into a range compatible with previous experiments using an optical system with more precise calibration. 12 However, because a single value was used for all of the data, the calibration factor does not alter the relative sensitivities across animals,fieldsizes, retinal eccentricity, or adaptation level. (6) In the series of experiments on the effects of retinal adaptation, the bowl illumination was lowered by reducing the lamp voltage through the perimeter's set-up menu. Although the variation in lamp voltage changed the color temperature of the bowi illumination, the adaptation data (see Fig. 7) did not show wavelength-specific effects. Thus, the changes in the color temperature of the adapting illuminance did not cause significant chromatic adaptation. The perimeter bowl illuminance levels, calibrated with the EG&G radiometer/ photometer system, covered the range of asb (10 to 0.6'cd/m 2 ). The monkeys' pupils varied from approximately 6 to 9 mm in diameter over this adaptation range resulting in a variation in retinal illuminance of troland. During the experimental sessions, the monkeys were placed in a custom-made primate chair that provided adjustments for alignment of their eyes at the correct viewing position for the perimeter and allowed access to the juice reward delivery spout. A behavioral response lever was mounted at a convenient waist level. Methods Psychophysical increment thresholds for the central fixation stimulus and the peripheral visual field stimuli were measured with an adaptation of the criterion response-time paradigm used in our previous studies of visual functions in monkeys. 26 The essential components of the procedures for visual field measurements with monkeys, which have been described in more detail, 22 were as follows: the monkeys were trained to press and hold down their response lever to initiate a trial and, subsequently, to release the lever in the presence of a visual stimulus. Because the time of the appearance of the stimulus within a trial was randomly varied, the monkey's responses that were closely correlated with the visual stimulus presentation (i.e., within the 900 msec response interval) were operationally defined as true-positive responses and they were rewarded. Alternatively, lever releases beyond the response interval were defined as false-negative responses. The test stimulus in any trial could be either a luminance increment of the central, fixation stimulus or of a peripheral field stimulus. The locations and intensities of the peripheral test stimuli were determined by the Humphrey Field Analyzer's C24-2 fullthreshold program. The Humphrey Field Analyzer's C24-2 program consists of 54 test field locations in the central visual field that are placed at 6-degree intervals (horizontally and vertically) on Cartesian coordinates. The most central testfieldlocations are at an eccentricity of 3 degrees (on a 45-degree angle into each quad-

4 3014 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 1993, Vol. 34, No. 10 SPECTRAL SENSITIVITY MECHANISMS 1) SW-mech = LOG{SWS}+ SS1 2) NOPP-mech = LOG{LWS + (MWS * K2)} + SS2 3) OPP-mech = LOG{LWS - (MWS * K3)} + SS " WAVELENGTH (nm) WAVENUMBER (I/cm) WAVELENGTH (nm) WAVENUMBER -(I/cm) FIGURE 2. Examples of increment-threshold spectral sensitivity functions (mean ± 1 SD) from a central (left panel) and a peripheral (right panel) visualfieldlocation. The curves superimposed on the data represent the: best-fitting color vision mechanisms derived from the spectral sensitivity model presented at the top of thefigure (see text for details). rant) and the most peripheral locations are at 27 degrees, in the nasal field, or 21 degrees, in the superior, inferior, and temporal fields. Visual field data with monochromatic test stimuli of a single wavelength were obtained in each daily session. On successive days, data were collected for pseudo-randomly ordered wavelengths between 420 and 680 nm, in 20-nm intervals, until two sets of data were acquired for each subject at each wavelength. The mean threshold value from the two sets of data was taken as the attenuation factor for calculating of the relative quantum threshold value. The logarithm of the reciprocal of the quantum threshold (log sensitivity) as a function of the reciprocal of the wavelength of the monochromatic stimulus (wave number) represented the increment-threshold spectral sensitivity function. Data Analysis The relative sensitivities of the color vision mechanisms mediating detection at each field location were determined by a specific curve-fitting analysis based on Stiles' proposition that spectral sensitivity functions represent the upper envelope of multiple independent detection mechanisms. 27 It was assumed that although there may be several potential detection mechanisms at every site in the retina, the mechanism most sensitive to a specific stimulus independently determined the monkey's visual threshold. The empirical spectral sensitivity functions were therefore used to define the mechanisms mediating detection by fitting separate portions of the function from a set of three underlying spectral sensitivity mechanisms, that is, i.e., an SWS mechanism (SWS-mech), a nonopponent mechanism (NOPP-mech), and an opponent mechanism (OPP-mech). Of course, this upper envelope, curve fitting procedure by itself is not sufficient to define the stimulus detection mechanisms. However, extensive investigations of spectral sensitivity functions for foveal vision of monkeys 12 have verified the existence of these three underlying mechanisms and it seems logical to extend the analysis to peripheral retinal locations. In addition, the form of incrementthreshold spectral sensitivity in perimetry is important in understanding the procedure, regardless of the underlying detection mechanisms. Quantitative descriptions for the three spectral sensitivity mechanisms 12 used in the analysis of the increment-threshold functions are presented at the top of Figure 2. Each mechanism was based on one or two cone photopigment absorption functions, but the general conclusions of the analysis are not dependent on

5 Detection Mechanisms in Perimetry 3015 FIGURE 3. The relative sensitivities of the opponent and nonopponent visual detection mechanisms as a function of field location with the Goldmann Size III stimulus and the Humphrey Field Analyzer standard bowl illumination (31.5 asb). The cells of the data matrix represent the 54 test locations of the Humphrey Field Analyzer C24-2 threshold program; the small numbers in the upper left corner of each cell correspond to the perimeter's designation of field locations, that is, spaced at 6-degree intervals (horizontally and vertically) on cartesian coordinates with the most central test field locations at an eccentricity of 3 degrees (on a 45-degree angle into each quadrant) and the most peripheral locations are 27 degrees, in the nasal field, or 21 degrees, in the superior, inferior, and temporal fields. At each field location an "N" or an "O" designates whether the spectral sensitivity data at that location were better fitted (minimum chi-square) with the nonopponent mechanism or opponent mechanism and the statistically significant cases (P < 0.05) are labeled by an asterisk. Examples of the spectral sensitivity data (mean ± 1SD) and best-fitting mechanisms from four representative field locations are also presented. specific cone fundamentals. For ease of computation, the cone functions were derived from the polynomial expressions of Baylor et al 28 with compensation for the spectral absorption of the animal's crystalline lens. Corrections for macular pigment were not included because even the most central test fields were located at an eccentricity (3 degrees) where absorption by the macula lutea approaches nonsignificance The correction for the spectral absorption of each animal's lens was based on the finding by Savage et al 31 that individual variations in the optical density of human lenses differ by a multiplicative factor. Assuming that the same is true for monkeys, the average lens absorption function for human subjects aged approximately 20 years was modified to fit the monkey's spectral sensitivity function. Because the SWS-mech

6 3016 Investigative Ophthalmology 8c Visual Science, September 1993, Vol. 34, No. 10 z w in -6 WAVENUMBER (I/cm) WAVENUMBER (I/cm) FIGURE 4. The relative sensitivities of the opponent and nonopponent visual detection mechanisms as a function of field location with the Goldmann Size V stimulus and the Humphrey Field Analyzer standard bowl illumination (31.5 asb). Other details as in Figure 3. represents the sensitivity of a single class of cones, the lens absorption for wavelengths between 420 and 480 nm should be proportional to the difference spectrum between the sensitivity of the SWS photopigment and the subject's spectral sensitivity at these wavelengths. The spectral sensitivity of the SWS-mech was determined by the average data from ten field locations approximating a 12-degree arc around fixation, but excluding points near the blind spot. The difference spectrum data and the modified absorption spectrum for the lens of each of the monkeys are presented in Figure 1. The lens optical densities at 415 nm (0.98, 1.13, and 1.45, for the 3 monkeys) represent only minor modifications of the data for 20-year-old humans, which can be considered the approximate human age-equivalent of the monkeys. 33 The three cone photopigment functions, corrected for absorption by the lens, were used as constants (SWS, middle-wavelength-sensitive, and long-wavelength-sensitive) in the descriptive spectral sensitivity model (shown in Fig. 2). The interaction values (K2 and K3) and the sensitivity values (SSI, SS2, and SS3) of the photopigment functions that provided the bestfit of the model to the spectral sensitivity data were determined by a Chi-square minimizing routine. In addition, using a reduced Chi-square statistic that compensated for the number of fitting parameters, 34 the algorithm evaluated the total fit to determine the minimum number of mechanisms required to describe the spectral sensitivity function. The differences produced by fitting the data with different numbers of parameters were then assessed from the F-ratios of the reduced Chi-square values, 35 with statistical significance set at a level of P < 0.05.

7 Detection Mechanisms in Perimetry WAVELENGTH (nm) WAVENUMBER (I/cm) WAVENUMBER (I/cm) WAVENUMBER (I/Cm) FIGURE 5. The relative sensitivities of the opponent and nonopponent visual detection mechanisms as a function of field location with the Goldmann Size II stimulus and the Humphrey Field Analyzer standard bowl illumination (31.5 asb). Other details as in Figure 3. RESULTS The data from the three monkeys were in agreement across all of the experimental conditions and therefore were averaged across animals. Examples of the spectral sensitivity data for a central and a peripheral field location, selected to illustrate differences in the forms of the functions, are presented in Figure 2. The data from a central field location {left panel) were bestfit with three mechanisms; the SWS-mech to fit the sensitivity peak in the short wavelength region of the spectrum, the OPP-mech to fit the majority of the data across the two sensitivity peaks in the middle and longer wavelength region, and the NOPP-mech to fit the few data points in the nm region that are not accounted for by the OPP-mech. In contrast, for the peripheral field location {right panel), the spectral sensitivity function was better described by just two mechanisms, the SWS-mech and the NOPP-mech. In both of these cases, the correlation between the fitted functions and the data provided an indication of the mechanisms mediating detection. These examples also illustrate the two aspects of the data analysis that were considered separately for each field location: (1) the sensitivity and category (opponent or nonopponent interactions) of the mechanism that mediated detection over the middle- and long-wavelength regions of the spectrum (520 to 680 nm); and (2) the sensitivity of the independent SWS-mech that mediated detection over the short-wavelength region of the spectrum ( nm). The results of the analysis of the relative sensitivities of opponent versus nonopponent mechanisms, as a function of perimetric field location, are presented

8 3018 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 1993, Vol. 34, No. 10 BBS ES9 B EB3 BBS! WEI j MjHM WBil BEfll CO jh laajj 2'J i ESI BSI HSi Epi j EB1 EfSI Ed ES9 ESS ESI 1ES3 FIGURE 6. The relative sensitivity of the short-wavelenth-sensitive mechanism as a function of field location. The cells of the data matrix represent the 54 test locations of the Humphrey Field Analyzer C24-2 threshold program; the small numbers in the upper left corner of each cell correspond to the perimeter's designations of field location. The sensitivity data have been normalized to the most central field location in the inferior, nasal visual field and displayed as a gray scale proportional to the relative sensitivity. The numeric relative sensitivity values for each field location are also presented in the corresponding cells of the matrix. (A) Relative sensitivity of the short-wavelenth-sensitive mechanism with the Goldmann Size III test field. (B) Relative sensitivity for white-light stimuli with the Goldmann Size III test stimulus. (C) Relative sensitivity of the short-wavelength mechanism with the Goldmann Size V test stimulus. in Figures 3-5 and 8). The cells of the data matrices represent the 54 test field locations of the Humphrey Field Analyzer C24-2 program. For each field location, an "N" or an "O" designates whether the spectral sensitivity data were better fitted with the NOPPmech or OPP-mech (minimum Chi-square value) and the statistically significant cases (P < 0.05) are labeled by an asterisk. Although the relative contributions of the SWS-mech have not been presented in these matrices, its inclusion as a fitting parameter provided a significantly better description of the functions with Goldmann Size III or V stimuli at all field locations. These properties are also illustrated by the representative examples of the spectral sensitivity data (mean ± 1

9 Detection Mechanisms in Perimetry 3019 LOG BOWL LUMINANCE (ASB) FIGURE 7. Detection thresholds (mean ± 1 SD) as a function of perimeter bowl illuminance for monochromatic test stimuli (Goldmann Size V). Functions are presented for four wavelengths, (A) 460 nm, (B) 520 nm, (C) 580 nm, and (D) 620 nm, at test field coordinates of: 9 degrees, 9 degrees in superior-temporal quadrant (triangles); 9 degrees, 15 degrees in the superior-nasal quadrant {squares); 15 degrees, 15 degrees in the inferior-nasal quadrant {circles); and 3 degrees, 3 degrees in the inferior-temporal quadrant (diamonds)). Data for the most peripheral visual field location (circles) are on a true scale, the other data has been displaced upward by 6.5 log units for clarity of presentation. SD) and the best-fitting spectral sensitivity mechanisms at four field locations. With the standard test stimulus (Goldmann Size III; 0.43 degrees) and bowl luminance (31.5 asb), the spectral sensitivity functions in the central visual field tended to be better described by opponent mechanisms whereas the functions in more peripheral field locations were generally better described by nonopponent mechanisms (Fig. 3). However, the distinctions between mechanisms were hot often statistically significant and, under these conditions, the sensitivities of NOPP-mech and OPP-mech were nearly equal across the whole visual field. Based on these results, it is not possible to specify the detection mechanisms that are most sensitive for a Goldmann Size III test target or to propose a monochromatic stimulus that would bias detection toward a certain mechanism. Visual field measurements with a larger test stimulus, a Goldmann Size V (1.72 degrees), resulted in a somewhat clearer separation of detection mechanisms and the peak in short-wavelength sensitivity was more obvious at all field locations. In the longer wavelength regions of the spectrum, opponent color mechanisms were more sensitive than nonopponent mechanisms over most of the visual field with a substantial proportion being statistically better described by OPP-mech than NOPP-mech functions (Fig. 4). Under these conditions, the separation of color vision mechanisms was sufficiently clear to demonstrate that the sensitivities of the opponent mechanisms were higher than the nonopponent mechanisms for long wavelength stimuli over the majority of the visual field locations. In distinction, with a smaller perimetry test field (Goldmann Size II; 0.21 degrees), the best descriptions of the spectral sensitivity functions at most test locations required an additive combination of the middle- and long-wavelength-sensitive photopigment functions. An additive combinations of cone functions defines a nonopponent mechanism and, as shown by the data of Figure 5, also defined the predominant detection mechanism with this small test field. Interestingly, the majority of the field locations (19 of 29) that were significantly better fitted with the N-OPP function were in the nasal hemifield (Fig. 5). In contrast, with the largest test field (Goldmann Size V) the majority of the significant cases for the OPP function (16 of 25) were in the temporal hemifield (Fig. 4). In our analysis of the spectral sensitivity functions, the sensitivity of the SWS-mech was considered to be an independent detection process (Fig. 2). The SWSmech was not evident with the 0.21-degree test field, even for the most central field locations (Fig. 5), but its inclusion significantly improved the descriptive fits of the data for all field locations, with either the or 1.72-degree test fields. Nevertheless, as illustrated by the insets of Figure 3, the SWS-mech sensitivity with the 0.43-degree test field was quite low at the more eccentric field locations. To illustrate the SWS-mech sensitivity as a function of field location, the data were normalized at the most central field location (3 degrees, 3 degrees) in the inferior, nasal field and are displayed in Figure 6 using a gray scale that was proportional to the logarithm of the relative sensitivity. The numeric relative sensitivity values for the individual field locations are also presented in the cells of the data matrix. As shown in panel A of Figure 6, with the perimeter's standard stimulus size (0.43 degrees) the SWS-mech sensitivity rapidly declined as a function of eccentricity. The decline in SWS-mech sensitivity was, in fact, more rapid than with a white-light stimulus of

10 3020 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 1993, Vol. 34, No WAVELENGTH (nm) FIGURE 8. The relative sensitivities of the opponent and nonopponent visual detection mechanisms as a function of field location with the Goldmann Size V stimulus and a Humphrey Field Analyzer bowl illumination of 4 asb. Other details as in Figure 3. the same size {panel B). In contrast, the change in SWS-mech sensitivity with eccentricity was smaller and similar to the function with the white light stimulus when the SWS-mech sensitivity was measured with a larger diameter test field (1.72 degrees, panel C). The variations in SWS-mech sensitivity as a function of field location (Fig. 6C) indicated that the SWS-mech was reasonably well isolated with the larger test field at all eccentricities within, at least, the centra] 48 degrees of the visual field. For instance, at the most peripheral field locations, the sensitivity of the SWS-mech at 460 nm was approximately 1.0 log unit higher than the sensitivity of either the OPP-mech or NOPP-mech (see Fig. 4). Previous studies 12 have shown that the relative balance in the sensitivities of the three color vision mechanisms are dependent on the level of white-light adaptation. Therefore, to determine how the detection mechanisms were affected by the state of adaptation provided by the bowl illumination, threshold-versusintensity functions were derived for wavelengths of 460, 520, 580, and 620 nm (panels A-D, Fig. 7) using a Goldmann Size V test stimulus. The results from each of the field locations, illustrated by the four examples in Figure 7, were similar in showing a linear relationship between threshold and adaptation level over the range of asb, regardless of the stimulus wavelength. The slopes of the functions, which varied between 0.56 and 0.84 on the log-log coordinate system, did not vary systematically with wavelength, but they are somewhat more shallow than has been found for foveal vision. 12 The linearity of the threshold-versus-intensity relationships indicates that alterations in the adaptation level did not change the detection mechanisms for a Goldmann Size V test stimulus. This outcome is also

11 Detection Mechanisms in Perimetry 3021 demonstrated by a comparison of the data shown in Figure 4, which were obtained for a 31.5-asb adaptation level to those for a lower adaptation level (4 asb) presented in Figure 8. With the lower level of adaptation, the SW-mech was isolated for test wavelengths below 500 nm, even at the most peripheral field locations, and the opponent mechanisms tended to dominate detection at the longer wavelengths throughout most of the central visual field. On the basis of these results, it may be concluded that, with the 1.72-degree test stimulus, the relative balance of the visual detection mechanisms are consistent across adaptation states, down to very low levels of photopic vision. DISCUSSION The results of these experiments have demonstrated that the visual detection mechanisms in static perimetry vary as a function of the size and wavelength of the test field. Whether any of these variations are applicable to the improvement of diagnostic procedures for ocular disorders would depend on the specific effects of the disease process and the goals of the perimetric measurement. For example, under the standard testing conditions of the Humphrey Field Analyzer (Goldmann Size III test field superimposed on a 31.5-asb background), specific detection mechanisms cannot be specified because the sensitivities of all of the detection mechanisms are approximately equal (Fig. 2). It therefore is unlikely that very selective deficits would alter visual thresholds under these standard conditions, but they are probably optimal for the assessment of deep scotorrias because any of the mechanisms would be effective in detecting the test stimulus. As demonstrated by the spectral sensitivity functions with larger and smaller stimulus field diameters, static perimetry thresholds may be differentially biased toward specific detection mechanisms by the appropriate selection of test field wavelength and/or test field size. These procedures could, therefore, be used to develop testing strategies for the specific neural deficits associated with various retinal diseases. In this respect, it is apparent that any single set of standard test parameters designed to bias detection toward a single mechanism would not be appropriate for general use because specific defects in each of the three color vision mechanisms have been associated with retinal diseases of one form or another. For example, evidence has been presented for specific deficits in the sensitivity of the SWS-mech in many retinal diseases of the inner and outer retina, 36 ' 3 ' including glaucoma. 38 " 41 Alternatively, psychophysical and morphologic data have been reported that suggest that the nonopponent or luminance mechanisms are differentially affected in incipient glaucoma. 42 " 44 Other diseases, especially optic neuritis, have been shown, however, to cause selective losses in the opponent or chromatic mechanisms. 45 ' 46 A selective vulnerability of the SWS-mech in early glaucoma has been suggested by several recent demonstrations that perimetry with monochromatic, blue light can reveal glaucomatous field defects that were not present with standard white light stimuli. 39 " 41 In each case, isolation of the SWS-mech was accomplished by auxiliary lighting of the perimeter bowl to provide a relatively intense yellow or orange adaptation field. However, the results of the present experiments (Figs 4 and 7B) indicate that, with the largest test field diameter, adequate isolation of the shortwavelength-mechanism can be achieved with the standard white light background. The use of achromatic, white adaptation is an advantage because factors that affect the overall level of adaptation, such as senile miosis, would uniformly affect the entire sensitivity function and, therefore, maintain a constant isolation of the short-wavelength-sensitive mechanism (Figs 7 and 8). On the other hand, color specific effects of the crystalline lens from aging or disease will affect the accuracy of sensitivity measurements with short wavelength light and the lens absorption of each patient should be empirically determined. 3 li32 ' 40>47 In this report, we described a new method of determining the optical density of the lens that takes advantage of the relative isolation of the SWS-mech with the standard white-light adaptation used in perimetry. The procedure used standard perimetry thresholds with liionochromatic blue light and an analysis procedure that provided a spectral correction factor for sensitivity measurements with any monochromatic stimulus. The visual detection mechanisms with a small diameter perimetric test field (Goldmann Size II; 0.21 degrees) could be described as nonopponent channels at essentially all field eccentricities (Fig. 5). The nonopponent nature of the spectral sensitivity mechanisms was defined by the additive combination of the middle- and long-wavelength-sensitive photopigment functions needed to obtain the best fit of the psychophysical functions. However, in contrast to opponent mechanisms, the shape of a spectral sensitivity function may not be sufficient to identify nonopponent mechanisms 48 or even a specific part of the geniculostriate pathway. Nonopponent neurons have been identified in both the magnocellular and parvocellular divisions of the lateral geniculate nucleus 15 ' 49 and within the parvocellular pathway, neurons with nonopponent response characteristics can be either unitary or associated with the short- wavelength-sensitive mechanism in the blue/yellow opponent channels. 50 The stimulus conditions and methods of data analysis in the current experiments were not specific enough to assume that detection would be confined to any one of these channels and, consequently, it is not certain

12 3022 Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 1993, Vol. 34, No. 10 that the use of small monochromatic test fields in perimetry would bias detection toward the broadband or M-pathway mechanisms. Isolation of the opponent-color vision channels, using a large test field and monochromatic light, is straightforward. The findings of the current experiments are in agreement with a large number of previous investigations 7 " 1217 ~ 20>51 that have demonstrated opponent channel spectral sensitivity for middle- to long-wavelength stimuli, in the presence of white-light adaptation. It was also expected that large test fields would be more effective than small stimuli for achieving a distinct isolation of these channels at peripheral retinal locations. 20 In fact, the failure to attain unequivocal isolation of the opponent mechanisms at the most peripheral field locations was probably because the 1.72-degree (Goldmann Size V) test field was not large enough at these eccentricities. In any case, the use of the largest of the perimeter's test fields, in conjunction with a monochromatic test light of nm, apparently would provide independent detection by opponent mechanisms over the major portion of the central visual field. Therefore, it would be expected that diseases of the retina or optic nerve that preferentially affect neurons in the red/green chromatic channels would show selective deficits under these test conditions. Overall, the results of these experiments have suggested testing conditions that may have application in the development of methodology for perimetry that is more sensitive to the effects of early disease states than the current, standard procedures. Of course, the validity of each of the sets of conditions must be subjected to further investigations in defined disease states. For example, the correlations of specific testing strategies could be determined throughout the natural timecourse of induced pathology, such as experimental glaucoma in monkeys. Nevertheless, even procedures developed through investigations of valid monkey models will need to be subjected to investigations on human patients to define their clinical efficacy. Key Words perimetry, visual fields, color vision, psychophysics, monkeys Acknowledgments The authors thank Christian Kuether for design and construction of the primate testing apparatus, James Rezabek for design and construction of the computer interface system, John Feaster for assistance with the statistical data analysis, Enita Torres for assistance with graphic presentation of the results, Dr. C.A.Johnson for providing the Vision Pathways data conversion programs, and Steve Myers and Allergan Humphrey for custom ROM chips for the Humphrey Field Analyzer. References 1. Anderson DR: Perimetry With and Without Automation. St Louis: C.V Mosby; 1987: Quigley HA, Dunkelberger GR, Green WR. Retinal ganglion cell atrophy correlated with automated perimetry in human eyes with glaucoma. Am] Ophthalmol. 1989; 107: Quigley HA, Sanchez RM, Dunkelberger GR, L'Hernault NL, Baginski TA. Chronic glaucoma selectively damages large optic nerve fibers. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1987;28: Lachenmayr BJ, Airaksinen PJ, Drance SM, Wijsman K. Correlation of retinal nerve-fiber-layer loss, changes at the optic nerve head and various psychophysical criteria in glaucoma. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 1991; 229: Shapley R. Visual sensitivity and parallel retinocortical channels. Annu Rev Psychol. 1990;41: DeValois RL, Abramov I, Jacobs GH. Analysis of response patterns of LGN cells. J Opt Soc Am. 1966;56: Sperling HG, Harwerth RS. Red-green cone interactions in increment- threshold spectral sensitivity of primates. Science. 1971; 172: Ingling CR, Tsou BH-P. Orthogonal combinations of the three visual channels. Vision Res. 1977; 17: Kranda K, King-Smith PE. Detection of colored stimuli by independent linear systems. Vision Res. 1979; 19: King-Smith PE, Kranda K. Photopic adaptation in the red-green spectral range. Vision Res. 1981; 21: Thornton JE, Pugh EN. Red/green opponency at detection threshold. Science. 1983;219: Kalloniatis M, Harwerth RS. Spectral sensitivity and adaptation characteristics of cone mechanisms under white light adaptation. J Opt Soc Am [A). 1990;7: Hood DC, Finkelstein MA. A case for the revision of textbook models of color vision: the detection and appearance of small brief lights. In: Mollon JD, Sharpe LT, eds. Colour Vision Physiology and Psychophysics. New York: Academic Press; 1983: Sperling HG, Wright AA, Mills SL. Color vision following intense green light exposure: data and a model. Vision Res. 1991; 31: Creutzfeldt OA, Lee BB, Elepfandt A. A quantitative study of chromatic organisation and receptive fields of cells in the lateral geniculate body of the rhesus monkey. Exp Brain Res. 1979;35: Schiller PH, Logothetis NK, Charles ER. Role of the color-opponent and broad-band channels in vision. VisNeurosci. 1990;5: Foster DH, Snelgar RS. Test andfieldspectral sensitivities of colour mechanisms obtained on small white backgrounds: Action of unitary opponent-colour process? Vision Res. 1983;23: King-Smith PE, Carden D. Luminance and opponent color contributions to visual detection and to tern-

13 Detection Mechanisms in Perimetry 3023 poral and spatial integration. J Opt Soc Am. 1976;66:7O Smith EL, Levi DM, Manny RE, Harwerth RS, White JM. The relationship between binocular rivalry and strabismic suppression. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1985;26: Kuyk TK. Spectral sensitivity of the peripheral retina to large and small stimuli. Vision Res. 1982; 22: Mullen KT. Color vision as a post-receptoral specialization of the central visual field. Vision Res. 1991:31: Han.verth RS, Smith EL, DeSantis L. Behavioral perimetry in monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993;34: DeValois RL, Morgan HC, Poison MC, Mead WR, Hull EM. Psychophysical studies of monkey vision. I. Macaque luminosity and color vision tests. Vision Res. 1974;H: DeValois RL, Morgan HC, Snodderly DM. Psychophysical studies of monkey vision. III. Spatial luminance contrast sensitivity tests of macaque and human observers. Vision Res. 1974; 14: Harwerth RS, Smith EL. Rhesus monkey as a model for normal vision of humans. Am J Optom Physiol Optics. 1985; 62: Harwerth RS, Boltz RL, Smith EL. Psychophysical evidence for sustained and transient channels in the monkey visual system. Vision Res. 1980; 20: Stiles WS. Color vision: the approach through increment threshold sensitivity. Proc Nail Acad Sci USA 1959:45: Baylor DA, Nunn BJ, Schnapf JL. Spectral sensitivity of cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. J Physiol (Lond). 1987; 390: Moreland JD, Bhatt P. Retinal distribution of macular pigment. In: Verriest G, ed. Color Vision Deficiencies VII. Boston MA: Dr. W Junk Publishers; 1984: Werner JS, Donnelly SK, Kliegl R. Aging and human macular pigment density. Vision Res. 1987; 27: Savage GL, Haegerstrom-Portnoy G, Adams AJ, Hewlett SE. Age changes in the optical density of human ocular media. Clin Vis Sci. 1993; 8: van Norren DV, VossJJ. Spectral transmission of the human ocular media. Vision Res. 1974; 14: Boothe RG, Dobson V, Teller DY. Postnatal development of vision in human and nonhuman primates. Ann Rev Neurosci. 1985; 8: Bevington PR. Data Reduction and Data Analysis for the Physical Sciences. New York: McGraw Hill; 1969: Johnson NL, Leone FC. Statistics and Experimental Design in Engineering and the Physical Sciences. Vol I. New York: John Wiley and Sons; 1964: Adams AJ. Chromatic and luminosity processing in retinal disease. AmJ Optom Physiol Optics. 1982; 59: Greenstein VC, Hood DC, Ritch R, Steinberger D, Carr RE. S (blue) cone pathway vulnerability in retinitis pigmentosa, diabetes and glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1989;30: Heron G, Adams AJ, Husted R. Central visual fields for short wavelength sensitivity pathways in glaucoma and ocular hypertension. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1988;29: Hart WM, Silverman SE, Trick GL, Nesher R, Gordon MO. Glaucomatous visual field damage. Luminance and color-contrast sensitivities. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1990;31: Sample PA, Weinreb RN. Color perimetry for assessment of primary open- angle glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1990;31: Johnson CA, Adams AJ, Casson EJ, Quigg JM: Can short wavelength sensitivity losses predict the development of glaucomatous visual field defects. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1991; 32(suppl): Tyler, CW. Specific defects of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1981;20: Berton ME, Wilson TW, Wilson R, Spaeth GL, Krupin T. Temporal contrast sensitivity loss in primary openangle glaucoma and glaucoma suspects. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1991:32: Glovinski Y, Quigley HA, Dunkelberger GR. Retinal ganglion cell loss is size dependent in experimental glaucoma, invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1991; 32: King-Smith PE, Lubow M, Benes SC. Selective damage to chromatic mechanisms in neuro-ophthalmic diseases. I. Review of published evidence. Doc Ophthalmol. 1984;58: Roggeveen HCJ, De Winter APCM, Went LN. Color vision studies in families with dominant optic atrophy. In: Veriest G, ed. Color Vision Deficiencies VIII. Boston, MA: Dr WJunk Publishers; 1987: Johnson CA, Adams AJ, Lewis RA. Evidence for a neural basis of age-related visual field loss in normal observers. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1989; 30: Finkelstein MA, Hood DC. Detection and discrimination of small brief lights: variable tuning of opponent channels. Vision Res. 1984; 24: Dreher B, Fukada Y, Rodieck RW. Identification, classification and anatomical segregation of cells with X- like and Y-like properties in the lateral geniculate nucleus of old-world primates. J Physiol (Lond). 1976;258: Gouras P. Precortical physiology of colour vision. In: Gouras P, ed. Vision and Visual. Dysfunction. Vol 6. The Perception of Colour. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; 1991: Drum B, Armaly MF, Huppert WE. Chromatic and achromatic sensitivity in glaucoma. In: Drum B, Verriest G, eds. Color Vision Deficiencies IX. Boston, MA: Dr WJunk Publishers; 1989:

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I Monocular Sensory Processes of Vision: Color Vision Mechanisms of Color Processing . Neural Mechanisms of Color Processing A. Parallel processing - M- & P- pathways B. Second

More information

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3 eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3 retinal specialization fovea: highest density of photoreceptors, aimed at where you are looking -> highest acuity optic disk: cell-free area, where

More information

T he retinal ganglion cells of different sizes have distinct

T he retinal ganglion cells of different sizes have distinct 604 CLINICAL SCIENCE Agreement between frequency doubling perimetry and static perimetry in eyes with high tension glaucoma and normal tension glaucoma S Kogure, Y Toda, D Crabb, K Kashiwagi, F W Fitzke,

More information

TUMBLING E RESOLUTION PERIMETRY IN GLAUCOMA

TUMBLING E RESOLUTION PERIMETRY IN GLAUCOMA Tumbling E resolution perimetry in glaucoma 179 TUMBLING E RESOLUTION PERIMETRY IN GLAUCOMA FERGAL A. ENNIS 1, ROGER S. ANDERSON 1, WINSTON S. McCLEAN 1 and SIMON J.A. RANKIN 2 1 Vision Science Research

More information

Spectral sensitivity and color discrimination changes in glaucoma and glaucoma-suspect patients

Spectral sensitivity and color discrimination changes in glaucoma and glaucoma-suspect patients Spectral sensitivity and color discrimination changes in glaucoma and glaucoma-suspect patients Anthony J. Adams, Rosemary Rodic, Roger Husted,* and Robert Stamper Color vision changes may occur early

More information

CHAPTER 10 NON-CONVENTIONAL PERIMETRY

CHAPTER 10 NON-CONVENTIONAL PERIMETRY 193 CHAPTER 10 NON-CONVENTIONAL PERIMETRY INTRODUCTION Static Standard Automated Perimetry (SAP, alternatively called white-on-white perimetry), which uses a white Goldmann size III stimulus presented

More information

Color vision deficiency in retinitis pigmentosa

Color vision deficiency in retinitis pigmentosa International Congress Series 1282 (25) 684 688 www.ics-elsevier.com Color vision deficiency in retinitis pigmentosa Rokiah Omar a, *, Stephan Dain b, Peter Herse b a Department of Optometry, Faculty of

More information

CHAPTER 11 KINETIC PERIMETRY WHAT IS KINETIC PERIMETRY? LIMITATIONS OF STATIC PERIMETRY LOW SPATIAL RESOLUTION

CHAPTER 11 KINETIC PERIMETRY WHAT IS KINETIC PERIMETRY? LIMITATIONS OF STATIC PERIMETRY LOW SPATIAL RESOLUTION 205 CHAPTER 11 KINETIC PERIMETRY WHAT IS KINETIC PERIMETRY? LIMITATIONS OF STATIC PERIMETRY LOW SPATIAL RESOLUTION Static perimetry is currently the most commonly used type of perimetry. With static perimetry,

More information

Characteristics of Frequency-of-Seeing Curves in Normal Subjects, Patients With Suspected Glaucoma, and Patients With Glaucoma

Characteristics of Frequency-of-Seeing Curves in Normal Subjects, Patients With Suspected Glaucoma, and Patients With Glaucoma Characteristics of Frequency-of-Seeing Curves in Normal Subjects, Patients With Suspected Glaucoma, and Patients With Glaucoma Balwantray C. Chauhan* James D. Tompkins,-\ Raymond P. LeBlanc* and Terry

More information

Glaucomatous Visual Field Damage

Glaucomatous Visual Field Damage Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, Vol. 31, No. 2, February 199 Copyright Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Glaucomatous Visual Field Damage Luminance and Color-Conrrasr Sensitivities

More information

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function VISION Vol. 20, No. 2, 67 71, 2008 Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function Keizo SHINOMORI Department of Information Systems Engineering, Kochi University of Technology

More information

Correlation of Blue Chromatic Macular Sensitivity with Optic Disc Change in Early Glaucoma Patients

Correlation of Blue Chromatic Macular Sensitivity with Optic Disc Change in Early Glaucoma Patients Correlation of Blue Chromatic Macular Sensitivity with Optic Disc Change in Early Glaucoma Patients Yoshio Yamazaki, Kenji Mizuki, Fukuko Hayamizu and Chizuru Tanaka Department of Ophthalmology, Nihon

More information

Normal and amblyopic contrast sensitivity functions in central and peripheral retinas

Normal and amblyopic contrast sensitivity functions in central and peripheral retinas Normal and amblyopic contrast sensitivity functions in central and peripheral retinas Joseph Thomas Contrast sensitivity functions (CSF's)for temporally modulated sine wave gratings were established at

More information

Perimetric testing is used clinically to detect visual field

Perimetric testing is used clinically to detect visual field Quantifying Effects of Retinal Illuminance on Frequency Doubling Perimetry William H. Swanson, Mitchell W. Dul, and Susan E. Fischer PURPOSE. To measure and quantify effects of variation in retinal illuminance

More information

2. METHODS. 2.1 Apparatus

2. METHODS. 2.1 Apparatus Pupillary light reflex associated with melanopsin and cone photorecetors Sei-ichi Tsujimura, 1 Katsunori Okajima, 2 1 Faculty of Sciences and Engineering, Kagoshima University, Japan 2 Faculty of Environment

More information

STANDARD AUTOMATED PERIMETRY IS A GENERALLY

STANDARD AUTOMATED PERIMETRY IS A GENERALLY Comparison of Long-term Variability for Standard and Short-wavelength Automated Perimetry in Stable Glaucoma Patients EYTAN Z. BLUMENTHAL, MD, PAMELA A. SAMPLE, PHD, LINDA ZANGWILL, PHD, ALEXANDER C. LEE,

More information

Behavioral measurement of background adaptation in infants. RONALD M. HANSEN

Behavioral measurement of background adaptation in infants. RONALD M. HANSEN Volume 21 Number 4 Reports 625 11. Ehinger B: Biogenic monoamines as transmitters in the retina. In Transmitters in the Visual Process, Bonting SL, editor. New York, 1976, Pergaman Press, p. 145. 12. Dowling

More information

Fluctuations on the Humphrey and Octopus Perimeters

Fluctuations on the Humphrey and Octopus Perimeters May 987 Vol. 28/ Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science A Journal of Dosic and Clinical Research Articles Fluctuations on the and Perimeters Randall S. Drenton and William A. Argus Fluctuation of

More information

11/23/17. Post receptoral pathways for color vision: How is colour coded? Colour Vision 2 - post receptoral

11/23/17. Post receptoral pathways for color vision: How is colour coded? Colour Vision 2 - post receptoral Colour Vision II: The post receptoral basis of colour vision and acquired color vision deficiencies Prof. Kathy T. Mullen McGill Vision Research (H4.14) Dept. of Ophthalmology kathy.mullen@mcgill.ca Colour

More information

Seeing Color. Muller (1896) The Psychophysical Axioms. Brindley (1960) Psychophysical Linking Hypotheses

Seeing Color. Muller (1896) The Psychophysical Axioms. Brindley (1960) Psychophysical Linking Hypotheses Muller (1896) The Psychophysical Axioms The ground of every state of consciousness is a material process, a psychophysical process so-called, to whose occurrence the state of consciousness is joined To

More information

3/16/2018. Perimetry

3/16/2018. Perimetry Perimetry The normal visual field extends further away from fixation temporally and inferiorly than superiorly and nasally. From the center of the retina this sensitivity decreases towards the periphery,

More information

Vision Seeing is in the mind

Vision Seeing is in the mind 1 Vision Seeing is in the mind Stimulus: Light 2 Light Characteristics 1. Wavelength (hue) 2. Intensity (brightness) 3. Saturation (purity) 3 4 Hue (color): dimension of color determined by wavelength

More information

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye: Problems confronting the visual system: Visual Physiology image contains a huge amount of information which must be processed quickly. image is dim, blurry and distorted. Light levels vary enormously.

More information

Image Processing in the Human Visual System, a Quick Overview

Image Processing in the Human Visual System, a Quick Overview Image Processing in the Human Visual System, a Quick Overview By Orazio Gallo, April 24th, 2008 The Visual System Our most advanced perception system: The optic nerve has 106 fibers, more than all the

More information

Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension

Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension Christopher W. Tyler Temporal modulation sensitivity for a 5 flickering field was measured in central and peripheral retina

More information

MOVE IT OR LOSE IT: THE ROLE OF KINETIC VISUAL FIELDS

MOVE IT OR LOSE IT: THE ROLE OF KINETIC VISUAL FIELDS MOVE IT OR LOSE IT: THE ROLE OF KINETIC VISUAL FIELDS Course Objectives Review the visual field Review types of perimetry Discuss advantages and disadvantages of different types of visual field testing

More information

The color of night: Surface color categorization by color defective observers under dim illuminations

The color of night: Surface color categorization by color defective observers under dim illuminations Visual Neuroscience ~2008!, 25, 475 480. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2008 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238008 $25.00 doi:10.10170s0952523808080486 The color of night: Surface color categorization

More information

Foveal cone photopigment bleaching in central serous retinopathy

Foveal cone photopigment bleaching in central serous retinopathy Foveal cone photopigment bleaching in central serous retinopathy Stephen A. Burns, Ann E. Elsner, and Louis A. Lobes, Jr. Color-matching techniques were used to follow the course of central serous retinopathy

More information

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited by law: any public performance or display,

More information

Neuroscience - Problem Drill 13: The Eye and Visual Processing

Neuroscience - Problem Drill 13: The Eye and Visual Processing Neuroscience - Problem Drill 13: The Eye and Visual Processing Question No. 1 of 10 needed, (3) Pick the answer, and (4) Review the core concept tutorial as needed. 1. Which of the following statements

More information

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision Biological Bases of Behavior 6: Vision Sensory Systems The brain detects events in the external environment and directs the contractions of the muscles Afferent neurons carry sensory messages to brain

More information

Visual Fields Shawn L. Cohen, M.D. Part 2 of 4. Definitions / Tables (Part 2 of 2) Static Perimetry (Humphrey, Octopus)

Visual Fields Shawn L. Cohen, M.D. Part 2 of 4. Definitions / Tables (Part 2 of 2) Static Perimetry (Humphrey, Octopus) Visual Fields Shawn L. Cohen, M.D. Part 2 of 4 Definitions / Tables (Part 2 of 2) Static Perimetry (Humphrey, Octopus) Normal Visual Field: Components: General Information Reliability Indices Raw Data

More information

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!)

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!) Introduction to Physiological Psychology Vision ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ /~ksweeney/psy260.html Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy (the part that s visible to us!)

More information

The frequency-doubling illusion occurs when a low-spatialfrequency

The frequency-doubling illusion occurs when a low-spatialfrequency Sensitivity and Specificity of Frequency Doubling Perimetry in Neuro-ophthalmic Disorders: A Comparison with Conventional Automated Perimetry Michael Wall, 1,2 Richard K. Neahring, 1 and Kimberly R. Woodward

More information

Responses of Primate Retinal Ganglion Cells to Perimetric Stimuli METHODS

Responses of Primate Retinal Ganglion Cells to Perimetric Stimuli METHODS Glaucoma Responses of Primate Retinal Ganglion Cells to Perimetric Stimuli William H. Swanson, 1 Hao Sun, 2 Barry B. Lee, 3,4 and Dingcai Cao 5 PURPOSE. Perimetry is used clinically to assess glaucomatous

More information

Subjective Color Perception. An Honors Thesis (10 499) Elisabeth J. Barker Binnig. Thesis Director Dr. Darrell Butler

Subjective Color Perception. An Honors Thesis (10 499) Elisabeth J. Barker Binnig. Thesis Director Dr. Darrell Butler An Honors Thesis (10 499) by Elisabeth J. Barker Binnig Thesis Director Dr. Darrell Butler (advisor's signature) Ball State University Muncie, Indiana September 1987 - .- 1 When certain black and white

More information

A MULTI-STAGE COLOR MODEL REVISITED: IMPLICATIONS FOR A GENE THERAPY CURE FOR RED-GREEN COLORBLINDNESS

A MULTI-STAGE COLOR MODEL REVISITED: IMPLICATIONS FOR A GENE THERAPY CURE FOR RED-GREEN COLORBLINDNESS Abstract for Chapter A MULTI-STAGE COLOR MODEL REVISITED: IMPLICATIONS FOR A GENE THERAPY CURE FOR RED-GREEN COLORBLINDNESS Katherine Mancuso 1, Matthew C. Mauck 2, James A. Kuchenbecker 1, Maureen Neitz

More information

Electronic Letters to: Electronic letters published: Multiple Mechanisms in the VEP 23 July von :24

Electronic Letters to: Electronic letters published: Multiple Mechanisms in the VEP 23 July von :24 QUICK SEARCH: Author: Go [advanced] Keyword(s): HOME HELP FEEDBACK SUBSCRIPTIONS ARCHIVE SEARCH Year: Vol: Page: Electronic Letters to: Visual Neurophysiology: Givago S. Souza, Bruno D. Gomes, Cézar A.

More information

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the Some transformations of color information from lateral geniculate nucleus to striate cortex Russell L. De Valois*, Nicolas P. Cottaris, Sylvia D. Elfar*, Luke E. Mahon, and J. Anthony Wilson* *Psychology

More information

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Javier R. Movellan Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0515 movellan@inc.ucsd.edu Thomas

More information

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* Perception & Psychophysics 1973. Vol. 13. So. 2.356-360 Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* ANGEL VASSILEV+ Institu te of Physiology. Bulgarian Academy of Sciences.

More information

Wavelength discrimination at detection threshold

Wavelength discrimination at detection threshold K. T. Mullen and J. J. Kulikowski Vol. 7, No. 4/April 199/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 733 Wavelength discrimination at detection threshold K. T. Mullen Physiological Laboratory, Downing Street, Cambridge CB3 3EG,

More information

A new visual field test in empty sella syndrome: Rarebit perimetry

A new visual field test in empty sella syndrome: Rarebit perimetry European Journal of Ophthalmology / Vol. 18 no. 4, 2008 / pp. 628-632 A new visual field test in empty sella syndrome: Rarebit perimetry G.F. YAVAS 1, T. KÜSBECI 1, O. ESER 2, S.S. ERMIS 1, M. COŞAR 2,

More information

Central Visual Fields For Short Wavelength Sensitive Pathways in Glaucoma and Ocular Hypertension

Central Visual Fields For Short Wavelength Sensitive Pathways in Glaucoma and Ocular Hypertension Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, Vol. 29, No. 1, January 1988 Copyright Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Central Visual Fields For Short Wavelength Sensitive Pathways in

More information

The Midget and Parasol Channels

The Midget and Parasol Channels The visual and oculomotor systems Peter H. Schiller, year 2006 The Midget and Parasol Channels MIDGET SYSTEM PARASOL SYSTEM or Neuronal response profile ON OFF ON OFF time Midget system cones ON OFF ON

More information

Effects of Enucleation of the Fixating Eye on Strobismic Amblyopio in Monkeys

Effects of Enucleation of the Fixating Eye on Strobismic Amblyopio in Monkeys Effects of Enucleation of the Fixating Eye on Strobismic Amblyopio in Monkeys Ronald S. Horwerrh,* Earl L. Smith, III,* Gary C. Duncan,* M. L. J. Crawford,t and Gunrer K. von Noorden^: The effects of enucleation

More information

In recent years, considerable interest. Short wavelength automated perimetry. Physiological Aspects of SWAP. John M. Wild

In recent years, considerable interest. Short wavelength automated perimetry. Physiological Aspects of SWAP. John M. Wild Short wavelength automated perimetry John M. Wild Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales, UK ABSTRACT. Short Wavelength Automated Perimetry (SWAP) utilizes a blue

More information

High sensitivity rod photoreceptor input to blue-yellow color opponent pathway in macaque retina

High sensitivity rod photoreceptor input to blue-yellow color opponent pathway in macaque retina High sensitivity rod photoreceptor input to blue-yellow color opponent pathway in macaque retina Greg D. Field 1, Martin Greschner 1, Jeffrey L. Gauthier 1, Carolina Rangel 2, Jonathon Shlens 1,3, Alexander

More information

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1 Announcements exam 1 this Thursday! review session: Wednesday, 5:00-6:30pm, Meliora 203 Bryce s office hours: Wednesday, 3:30-5:30pm, Gleason https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zdw7pvgz0um M Cells M cells

More information

Construction of the Visual Image

Construction of the Visual Image Construction of the Visual Image Anne L. van de Ven 8 Sept 2003 BioE 492/592 Sensory Neuroengineering Lecture 3 Visual Perception Light Photoreceptors Interneurons Visual Processing Ganglion Neurons Optic

More information

Ganglion cell complex scan in the early prediction of glaucoma

Ganglion cell complex scan in the early prediction of glaucoma Original article in the early prediction of glaucoma Ganekal S Nayana Super Specialty Eye Hospital and Research Center, Davangere, Karnataka, India Abstract Objective: To compare the macular ganglion cell

More information

Electrodiagnostics Alphabet Soup

Electrodiagnostics Alphabet Soup Nathan Lighthizer, O.D., F.A.A.O Assistant Professor, NSUOCO Chief of Specialty Care Clinics Chief of Electrodiagnostics Clinic What is electrodiagnostics testing? Visual Pathway Basic Understanding VEP

More information

THE STRUCTURE-FUNCTION JUNCTION

THE STRUCTURE-FUNCTION JUNCTION THE STRUCTURE-FUNCTION JUNCTION Craig Thomas, O.D. 3900 West Wheatland Road Dallas, Texas 75237 972-780-7199 thpckc@yahoo.com Paul M. Karpecki, O.D., FAAO 120 N Eagle Creek Drive # 431 Lexington, KY 40509

More information

Human colour perception and its adaptation

Human colour perception and its adaptation Network: Computation in Neural Systems 7 (1996) 587 634. Printed in the UK Human colour perception and its adaptation Michael A Webster Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, NV 89557-0062,

More information

The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect. absolute threshold. Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information.

The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect. absolute threshold. Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information. absolute threshold The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect accommodation Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information. acuity Sharp perception or vision audition

More information

Diagnosing open-angle glaucoma may be particularly

Diagnosing open-angle glaucoma may be particularly A New Look at Selective Perimetry What is its role in clinical practice? BY MURRAY FINGERET, OD Diagnosing open-angle glaucoma may be particularly challenging when damage is mild or early. Although most

More information

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus Perceived color of light is determined by 3 characteristics (properties of electromagnetic energy): 1. Hue: the spectrum (wavelength) of light (color)

More information

Perimetry Phobia: Don t fear the field Savory Turman, COMT, CPSS

Perimetry Phobia: Don t fear the field Savory Turman, COMT, CPSS Perimetry Phobia: Don t fear the field Savory Turman, COMT, CPSS I have no financial interest in this presentation. Who am I? Where am I? What am I? The anatomy of the visual field Purpose of Visual Field

More information

Contrast sensitivity and retinal ganglion cell responses in the primate

Contrast sensitivity and retinal ganglion cell responses in the primate PSYCHOLOGY NEUROSCIENCE Psychology & Neuroscience, 2011, 4, 1, 11-18 DOI: 10.3922/j.psns.2011.1.003 Contrast sensitivity and retinal ganglion cell responses in the primate Barry B. Lee 1,2 and Hao Sun

More information

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones Photoreceptors Rods Cones 120 000 000 Dim light Prefer wavelength of 505 nm Monochromatic Mainly in periphery of the eye 6 000 000 More light Different spectral sensitivities!long-wave receptors (558 nm)

More information

Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 8 November 1972)

Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 8 November 1972) J. Physiol. (1973), 232, pp. 149-162 149 With 8 text-figures Printed in Great Britain PSYCHOPHYSICAL EVIDENCE FOR SUSTAINED AND TRANSIENT DETECTORS IN HUMAN VISION BY J. J. KULIKOWSKI AND D. J. TOLHURST*

More information

The Evolution of Fundus Perimetry

The Evolution of Fundus Perimetry The Evolution of Fundus Perimetry Company Profile CenterVue designs and manufactures highly automated medical devices for the diagnosis and management of ocular pathologies, including those that represent

More information

Multifocal Electroretinograms in Patients with Branch Retinal Artery Occlusion

Multifocal Electroretinograms in Patients with Branch Retinal Artery Occlusion Multifocal Electroretinograms in Patients with Branch Retinal Artery Occlusion Shigeru Hasegawa, Akira Ohshima, Yuuki Hayakawa, Mineo Takagi, and Haruki Abe PURPOSE. To investigate the usefulness of second-order

More information

Baseline Visual Field Characteristics in the Ocular Hypertension Treatment Study

Baseline Visual Field Characteristics in the Ocular Hypertension Treatment Study Baseline Visual Field Characteristics in the Ocular Hypertension Treatment Study Chris A. Johnson, PhD, 1 John L. Keltner, MD, 2 Kimberly E. Cello, BS, 2 Mary Edwards, BS, 2 Michael A. Kass, MD, 3 Mae

More information

7. Sharp perception or vision 8. The process of transferring genetic material from one cell to another by a plasmid or bacteriophage

7. Sharp perception or vision 8. The process of transferring genetic material from one cell to another by a plasmid or bacteriophage 1. A particular shade of a given color 2. How many wave peaks pass a certain point per given time 3. Process in which the sense organs' receptor cells are stimulated and relay initial information to higher

More information

Balance between pattern and flicker sensitivities in

Balance between pattern and flicker sensitivities in British Journal of Ophthalmology, 1984, 68, 31-315 Balance between pattern and flicker sensitivities in the visual fields of ophthalmological patients D. REGAN AND D. NEIMA From the Department of Ophthalmology,

More information

Psy393: Cognitive Neuroscience. Prof. Anderson Department of Psychology Week 3

Psy393: Cognitive Neuroscience. Prof. Anderson Department of Psychology Week 3 Psy393: Cognitive Neuroscience Prof. Anderson Department of Psychology Week 3 The Eye: Proof for the existence of God? And then there was light Optics Perception Absorption Eye is receiver not sender Plato

More information

Study of Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer Thickness Within Normal Hemivisual Field in Primary Open-Angle Glaucoma and Normal-Tension Glaucoma

Study of Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer Thickness Within Normal Hemivisual Field in Primary Open-Angle Glaucoma and Normal-Tension Glaucoma Study of Retinal Nerve Fiber Layer Thickness Within Normal Hemivisual Field in Primary Open-Angle Glaucoma and Normal-Tension Glaucoma Chiharu Matsumoto, Shiroaki Shirato, Mai Haneda, Hiroko Yamashiro

More information

IUSO Bill Swanson PubMed Papers June 28, 2012

IUSO Bill Swanson PubMed Papers June 28, 2012 1. Malik R, Swanson WH, Garway-Heath DF. 'Structure-function relationship' in glaucoma: past thinking and current concepts. Clin Experiment Ophthalmol. 2012 May;40(4):369-80. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-9071.2012.02770.x.

More information

Introduction to Physiological Psychology

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Introduction to Physiological Psychology Vision ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ksweeney/psy260.html This class n Sensation vs. Perception n How light is translated into what we see n Structure

More information

Test-retest variability in visual field testing using frequency doubling technology

Test-retest variability in visual field testing using frequency doubling technology European Journal of Ophthalmology / Vol. 17 no. 2, 2007 / pp. 203-207 Test-retest variability in visual field testing using frequency doubling technology A. HORANI, S. FRENKEL, E.Z. BLUMENTHAL Department

More information

Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color tuning properties

Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color tuning properties Visual Neuroscience (2004), 21, 275 282. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2004 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238004 $16.00 DOI: 10.10170S0952523804213426 Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color

More information

Spatial summation of blue-on-yellow light increments and decrements in human vision

Spatial summation of blue-on-yellow light increments and decrements in human vision Vision Research 40 (2000) 989 1000 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Spatial summation of blue-on-yellow light increments and decrements in human vision Angel Vassilev a, *, Margarita Zlatkova a, Velichko

More information

Science & Technologies

Science & Technologies STANDARD COMPUTERIZED PERIMETRY IN FUNCTION OF DIAGNOSTIC GLAUCOMA Iljaz Ismaili, 1 Gazepov Strahil, 2, Goshevska Dashtevska Emilija 1 1 University Eye Clinic,Skopje 2 Clinical Hospital, Shtip Abstract

More information

What do we perceive?

What do we perceive? THE VISUAL SYSTEM Aditi Majumder What do we perceive? Example: Switch off the light in room What we perceive Not only the property of the scene But also that of the visual system Our perception is filtered

More information

Suprathreshold spatial frequency detection and binocular interaction in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia

Suprathreshold spatial frequency detection and binocular interaction in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia Suprathreshold spatial frequency detection and binocular interaction in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia Dennis M. Levi, Ronald S. Harwerth, and Ruth E. Manny We have investigated suprathreshold

More information

Supplementary Appendix

Supplementary Appendix This appendix has been provided by the authors to give readers additional information about their work. Supplement to: Edwards TL, Jolly JK, MacLaren RE, et al.. N Engl J Med 206;374:996-8. DOI: 0.056/NEJMc50950

More information

Behavioral Perimetry in Monkeys

Behavioral Perimetry in Monkeys Behavioral Perimetry in Monkeys Ronald S. Harwerth,* Earl L. Smith HI,* and Louis DeSantisf Purpose. Normative data on the systematic changes in visual sensitivity as a function of retinal eccentricity

More information

Method for comparing visual field defects to local RNFL and RGC damage seen on frequency domain OCT in patients with glaucoma.

Method for comparing visual field defects to local RNFL and RGC damage seen on frequency domain OCT in patients with glaucoma. Method for comparing visual field defects to local RNFL and RGC damage seen on frequency domain OCT in patients with glaucoma. Donald C. Hood 1,2,* and Ali S. Raza 1 1 Department of Psychology, Columbia

More information

FLICKER-TOP PERIMETRY IN NORMALS AND PATIENTS WITH OCULAR HYPERTENSION AND EARLY GLAUCOMA

FLICKER-TOP PERIMETRY IN NORMALS AND PATIENTS WITH OCULAR HYPERTENSION AND EARLY GLAUCOMA Flicker-TOP perimetry 59 FLICKER-TOP PERIMETRY IN NORMALS AND PATIENTS WITH OCULAR HYPERTENSION AND EARLY GLAUCOMA MANUEL GONZÁLEZ DE LA ROSA, JAVIER RODRÍGUEZ and MANUEL RODRÍGUEZ Hospital Universitario

More information

LEA Color Vision Testing

LEA Color Vision Testing To The Tester Quantitative measurement of color vision is an important diagnostic test used to define the degree of hereditary color vision defects found in screening with pseudoisochromatic tests and

More information

Factors Influencing Normal Perimetric Thresholds Obtained Using the Humphrey Field Analyzer

Factors Influencing Normal Perimetric Thresholds Obtained Using the Humphrey Field Analyzer Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, Vol. 33, No. 3, March 1992 Copyright Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Factors Influencing Normal Perimetric Thresholds Obtained Using the

More information

THE CHROMATIC ANTAGONISMS OF OPPONENT PROCESS THEORY ARE NOT THE SAME AS THOSE REVEALED IN STUDIES OF DETECTION AND DISCRIMINATION

THE CHROMATIC ANTAGONISMS OF OPPONENT PROCESS THEORY ARE NOT THE SAME AS THOSE REVEALED IN STUDIES OF DETECTION AND DISCRIMINATION THE CHROMATIC ANTAGONISMS OF OPPONENT PROCESS THEORY ARE NOT THE SAME AS THOSE REVEALED IN STUDIES OF DETECTION AND DISCRIMINATION J.D. MOLLON and C.R. CAVONIUS (Cambridge, U. K./Dortmund, F. R.G.) ABSTRACT

More information

Stable Receptive Field Structure of Color Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex under Adapting and Non-adapting Conditions

Stable Receptive Field Structure of Color Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex under Adapting and Non-adapting Conditions Stable Receptive Field Structure of Color Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex under Adapting and Non-adapting Conditions Bevil R. Conway, Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston MA 02115.

More information

The early stages in the processing of chromatic information by

The early stages in the processing of chromatic information by Contribution of S opponent cells to color appearance Russell L. De Valois*, Karen K. De Valois*, and Luke E. Mahon *Psychology Department and Vision Science Group, University of California, Berkeley, CA

More information

NERVE FIBER LAYER THICKNESS IN NORMALS AND GLAUCOMA PATIENTS

NERVE FIBER LAYER THICKNESS IN NORMALS AND GLAUCOMA PATIENTS Nerve fiber layer thickness in normals and glaucoma patients 403 NERVE FIBER LAYER THICKNESS IN NORMALS AND GLAUCOMA PATIENTS HIROTAKA SUZUMURA, KAYOKO HARASAWA, AKIKO KOBAYASHI and NARIYOSHI ENDO Department

More information

Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system

Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system Vision Research 38 (1998) 3057 3068 Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system Clifton M. Schor *, Mark Edwards, David R. Pope School of Optometry, Uni ersity of California,

More information

Comparison of Visual Field Measurement with Heidelberg Edge Perimeter and Humphrey Visual Field Analyzer in Patients with Ocular Hypertension

Comparison of Visual Field Measurement with Heidelberg Edge Perimeter and Humphrey Visual Field Analyzer in Patients with Ocular Hypertension ORIGINAL PAPERS Adv Clin Exp Med 2016, 25, 5, 937 944 DOI: 10.17219/acem/62142 Copyright by Wroclaw Medical University ISSN 1899 5276 Kamil Kaczorowski 1, B E, Małgorzata Mulak 1, A, C, E, Dorota Szumny

More information

Perimetric Defects and Ganglion Cell Damage: Interpreting Linear Relations Using a Two-Stage Neural Model

Perimetric Defects and Ganglion Cell Damage: Interpreting Linear Relations Using a Two-Stage Neural Model Perimetric Defects and Ganglion Cell Damage: Interpreting Linear Relations Using a Two-Stage Neural Model William H. Swanson, 1,2 Joost Felius, 2 and Fei Pan 1 From the 1 Glaucoma Institute, SUNY State

More information

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus Perceived color of light is determined by 3 characteristics (properties of electromagnetic energy): 1. : the spectrum (wavelength) of light (color) 2.

More information

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY AND PSYCHOPHYSICS IN OCULAR HYPERTENSION AND GLAUCOMA: EVIDENCE FOR DIFFERENT PATHOMECHANISMS IN EARLY GLAUCOMA

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY AND PSYCHOPHYSICS IN OCULAR HYPERTENSION AND GLAUCOMA: EVIDENCE FOR DIFFERENT PATHOMECHANISMS IN EARLY GLAUCOMA ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY AND PSYCHOPHYSICS IN OCULAR HYPERTENSION AND GLAUCOMA: EVIDENCE FOR DIFFERENT PATHOMECHANISMS IN EARLY GLAUCOMA SIMON T. RUBEN I, ROGER A. HITCHINGS I, FRED FITZKE2 and GEOFFREY B. ARDEN2

More information

STRUCTURE & FUNCTION An Integrated Approach for the Detection and Follow-up of Glaucoma. Module 3a GDx

STRUCTURE & FUNCTION An Integrated Approach for the Detection and Follow-up of Glaucoma. Module 3a GDx STRUCTURE & FUNCTION An Integrated Approach for the Detection and Follow-up of Glaucoma Module 3a GDx Educational Slide Deck Carl Zeiss Meditec, Inc. November 2005 1 Structure & Function Modules Module

More information

COLOUR CONSTANCY: A SIMULATION BY ARTIFICIAL NEURAL NETS

COLOUR CONSTANCY: A SIMULATION BY ARTIFICIAL NEURAL NETS OLOUR ONSTANY: A SIMULATION BY ARTIFIIAL NEURAL NETS enrikas Vaitkevicius and Rytis Stanikunas Faculty of Psychology, Vilnius University, Didlaukio 47, 257 Vilnius, Lithuania e-mail: henrikas.vaitkevicius@ff.vu.lt

More information

LISC-322 Neuroscience. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation

LISC-322 Neuroscience. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation LISC-3 Neuroscience THE VISUAL SYSTEM Central Visual Pathways Each eye sees a part of the visual space that defines its visual field. The s of both eyes overlap extensively to create a binocular. eye both

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

Translating data and measurements from stratus to cirrus OCT in glaucoma patients and healthy subjects

Translating data and measurements from stratus to cirrus OCT in glaucoma patients and healthy subjects Romanian Journal of Ophthalmology, Volume 60, Issue 3, July-September 2016. pp:158-164 GENERAL ARTICLE Translating data and measurements from stratus to cirrus OCT in glaucoma patients and healthy subjects

More information

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity S. B. Stevenson *, L. K. Cormack Vision Research 40, 2881-2884. (2000) * University of Houston College of Optometry, Houston TX 77204

More information

Visual Deficits in Amblyopia

Visual Deficits in Amblyopia Human Amblyopia Lazy Eye Relatively common developmental visual disorder (~2%) Reduced visual acuity in an otherwise healthy and properly corrected eye Associated with interruption of normal early visual

More information

VISUAL FIELDS. Visual Fields. Getting the Terminology Sorted Out 7/27/2018. Speaker: Michael Patrick Coleman, COT & ABOC

VISUAL FIELDS. Visual Fields. Getting the Terminology Sorted Out 7/27/2018. Speaker: Michael Patrick Coleman, COT & ABOC VISUAL FIELDS Speaker: Michael Patrick Coleman, COT & ABOC Visual Fields OBJECTIVES: 1. Explain what is meant by 30-2 in regards to the Humphrey Visual Field test 2. Identify the difference between a kinetic

More information