Capacity and contextual constraints on product activation: Evidence from task-irrelevant fact retrieval

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Capacity and contextual constraints on product activation: Evidence from task-irrelevant fact retrieval"

Transcription

1 Q QJEP(A)08802 / Sep 20, 04 (Mon)/ [?? pages 1 Tables 2 Figures 0 Footnotes 3 Appendices]. Centre single caption cf. [no comma] Shortcut keys UK Spelling. [RJ] THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2004, 57A (8), Capacity and contextual constraints on product activation: Evidence from task-irrelevant fact retrieval Elena Rusconi University of Padua, Padua, Italy Giovanni Galfano University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy Viviana Speriani Ospedale S. Camillo, Venezia, Italy Carlo Umiltà University of Padua, Padua, Italy Three experiments tested the limiting conditions of multiplication facts retrieval in a numbermatching task (LeFevre, Bisanz, & Mrkonjic, 1988). By presenting two digits as cue and by requiring participants to decide whether a subsequent numerical target had been present in the pair, we found interference when the target coincided with the product of the cue digits. This was evidence for obligatory activation of multiplication facts. Also, we showed that multiplication facts retrieval occurred even in the absence of any arithmetic context (i.e., a multiplication sign between the cue digits) and did not require processing resources (i.e., the process met the capacity criterion of automaticity; Jonides, 1981), whereas manipulation of the spatial relation between the two operands (cue digits) negatively affected retrieval. The present work appears to be unique in the context of previous similar studies on mental calculation, which invariably adopted an arithmetic task as the primary demand. We identify this difference as the reason for the failure of all previous studies in revealing independence of multiplication facts from attentional resources. Furthermore, we suggest the application of a contextual definition of automaticity to this kind of retrieval, given the fact that it might depend both on association strength and on contextual setting variables. Correspondence should be addressed to Carlo Umiltà, Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università di Padova, via Venezia, 8, 35131, Padova, Italy. carlo.umilta@unipd.it Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by grants from the European Commission (RTN grant HPRN-CT ), MURST, and University of Padua to Carlo Umiltà. We would like to thank Mark Ashcraft, André Vandierendonck, and an anonymous reviewer for their helpful suggestions on earlier drafts of the manuscript The Experimental Psychology Society DOI: /

2 1486 RUSCONI ET AL. Adults are thought to possess well-developed mental representations of simple (i.e., with two single-digit operands) number facts that allow them to retrieve, rather than to calculate, the result of simple arithmetic problems (see, e.g., Ashcraft, 1992; Campbell & Oliphant, 1992). For instance, Ashcraft (1987) proposed that number facts are represented in a network that is similar to the networks for word representations, where the nodes are numbers instead of words, and the links between them represent arithmetic relationships. In general, a network is a graph consisting of nodes that represent physical or conceptual objects and arcs that describe the relationship to other concepts or units. The information is stored in a network by interconnecting nodes with labelled arcs. Upon presentation of an arithmetic problem, activation in the network would automatically spread from activated operands to linked nodes, such as the sum. Individuals who are skilled at arithmetic are assumed to have automatic access to stored arithmetic facts via activation in their network of arithmetic knowledge. Processing strategies other than retrieval are prominent only in Siegler s model (Shrager & Siegler, 1998; Siegler, 1988), which is also the only explicitly developmental model. (Note that nonretrieval strategies are more prominent in children than in adults.) The use of strategies, however, is not incompatible with network retrieval models. Some studies (e.g., LeFevre, Bisanz, & Mrkonjic, 1988; LeFevre & Kulak, 1994; Thibodeau, LeFevre, & Bisanz, 1996; Zbrodoff & Logan, 1986) addressed the issue of whether activation of arithmetic facts is indeed automatic, in the sense that activation of related nodes occurs without intention upon presentation of the appropriate stimuli. For example, in the Zbrodoff and Logan study, participants were asked to verify simple arithmetic equations of the form a + b = c and a b = c. Half of the equations were false, and, more importantly, half of these false equations contained a result that would have been correct if the participant had been asked to perform a different operation. For instance, the authors found that more time was required to reject 4 3=7than 4 3=6. In the former case, the multiplier, multiplicand, and false product were associated through knowledge about addition. Obligatory activation of the addition fact presumably interfered with retrieval of the relevant multiplication fact (also see Winkelman & Schmidt, 1974). Similarly, it took longer to reject 3 +4=12than 3+4=8. These findings, however, provide only limited support for obligatory activation of arithmetic facts because participants had been instructed to perform an arithmetic operation, although a different one. Therefore, the interference effect may reflect processing that is obligatory only if the participant is performing arithmetic with both kinds of operation. Indeed, Zbrodoff and Logan found a reduction or even disappearance of associative confusion when their participants were asked to verify just one kind of operation in a block. LeFevre et al. (1988) provided more convincing evidence that the activation of simple arithmetic facts is obligatory because they showed that it occurs even when mental arithmetic is completely irrelevant to the task at hand. In their study, pairs of numbers were presented (e.g., 5 + 1). After a variable delay, the pair of numbers disappeared, and a target number appeared. If the target was one of the previously presented numbers (e.g., 5), then the participant responded yes by pressing a response key. If the target was not one of the previously presented numbers (e.g., 3), then the participant responded no by pressing another key. It was found that a no item was rejected more slowly when the target was the sum of the presented pair (e.g., and 6) than when the target was arithmetically unrelated (e.g., and 3). That showed that activation of the sum occurs even when mental arithmetic is completely irrelevant to the task at hand and interferes with performance in a number-

3 AUTOMATICITY AND PRODUCT RETRIEVAL 1487 matching task. This interference effect was present at brief delays between cue and target (60 and 120 ms) but not at longer delays (180, 240, and 480 ms), indicating either that the automatic activation of arithmetic facts is short-lived or that its effects can be inhibited at longer intervals. Very importantly, obligatory activation did not depend on the presence of an addition sign between the initial pair of numbers, nor was it restricted to presentation in an arabic format. That is, interference occurred even when the addition sign was omitted (e.g., 5 1 and 6), or the target was a word (six). Thibodeau et al. (1996) tried to demonstrate the interference effect of obligatory activation of arithmetic facts in a number matching task by using multiplication facts. However, the results were not fully convincing because the condition without the multiplication sign was missing. Thus, the interference effect for multiplication is in need of further investigation. In a recent study, Galfano, Rusconi, and Umiltà (2003) addressed this question and demonstrated the presence of an obligatory activation of multiplication facts even in the absence of the multiplication sign in a number-matching task. In particular, they explored the possibility of obtaining an automatic activation of the nodes (i.e., the mental representations corresponding to numbers) that precede or follow, in the multiplication table, the product node (i.e., the mental representation of the product number). Their findings suggest that multiplication facts are stored in a highly related network in which activation spreads automatically from the product node to adjacent nodes (i.e., to the preceding and subsequent multiples of both the first and the second operand of the initial pair). The notion of obligatory activation is a central component of theories about automatic processes (e.g., Kahneman & Chajczyk, 1983; Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1981). However, obligatory activation is not the only criterion of automaticity. In particular, Jonides (1981) proposed three criteria of automaticity. The first criterion is capacity (or load insensitivity). If a process, like accessing arithmetic facts, is automatic, it should not require processing resources. Therefore, it should be less impaired than a controlled process by a concomitant, resourceconsuming task. The second criterion is resistance to suppression. An automatic process should be more difficult to suppress than a controlled process. The third criterion is expectancy. An automatic process should be triggered by presentation of the relevant information, regardless of the participant s expectations about the task to be performed. Often, the last two criteria are subsumed under the single criterion of intentionality. Previous studies (e.g., LeFevre et al., 1988, as regards expectation; Zbrodoff & Logan, 1986, as regards resistance to suppression) tested two criteria only. To our knowledge, the criterion of capacity has yet to be tested in the context of task-irrelevant activation of arithmetic facts. It has been tested, instead, in the context of explicit recall (in production tasks) or recognition (in verification tasks) of arithmetic facts by adopting dual-task paradigms (e.g., De Rammelaere, Stuyven, & Vandierendonck, 1999, 2001; Lee & Kang, 2002; Lemaire, Abdi, & Fayol, 1996). The underlying rationale is very simple: If production and verification of an arithmetic fact were limited by the amount of available attentional resources, then requiring a participant to perform a secondary resource-demanding task during retrieval or verification should cause performance impairment with respect to baseline. It is likely that the greater the extent to which a process is limited by available resources, the more susceptible it should be to disruption by a resource-demanding task. Another issue that is presently debated in the literature, and that will be addressed in the present study, is the exact role played by verbal processes in elementary arithmetic. There can

4 1488 RUSCONI ET AL. be little doubt that verbal processes are involved, for example, in understanding the operands of arithmetic problems or in producing a response. The debated issue concerns the format in which arithmetic facts are stored in long-term memory. Dehaene and his colleagues (e.g., Dehaene, 1992; Dehaene, Spelke, Pinel, Stanescu, & Tsivkin, 1999) have proposed that arithmetic facts that have been learned by rote at school, especially the addition and multiplication tables, are stored in long-term memory in a verbal format. Also, they would be retrieved as automatic verbal associations. In contrast, most subtraction and division problems, which are not commonly learned by rote (e.g., Dehaene & Cohen, 1997), would be solved through mental manipulations of the quantities represented by the operands. However some simple subtraction and division problems would be memorized in a verbal format like multiplication problems. And, like multiplication and addition problems, their results would be retrieved as automatic verbal associations. Moreover, in a recent study, Lee and Kang (2002) have suggested that arithmetic knowledge might be related in a specific manner to workingmemory subsystems. In their study, simultaneous articulatory suppression significantly delayed the performance in solving multiplication but not subtraction problems. Holding an image in mind had the opposite effect. The purpose of Experiment 1 was twofold. First, we tried to replicate, by using the product and in the absence of a multiplication sign, the interference effect that Galfano et al. (2003) found with the nodes adjacent to it, therefore complementing the Thibodeau et al. (1996) study. Second, we tried to determine whether this effect, if present, satisfies the capacity criterion of automaticity and whether it is reduced, or even eliminated, when participants have to perform a resource-demanding secondary task. We addressed the question of the existence of an obligatory verbal access to arithmetic knowledge in Experiment 2, in which both a number-matching and a verification task have been employed, respectively, with multiplication and with multiplication and addition facts. Three secondary tasks have been adopted: Two of them are supposed to tap on attentional resources (backward subtraction and random spatial tapping) and one on the phonological loop (articulatory suppression). The presence of two primary tasks (a direct and an indirect arithmetical task), allowed us to distinguish between two kinds of associative effects, both attributed to retrieval of arithmetic stored facts, and to test the efficacy of our secondary-task manipulation. With Experiment 3 we attempted to qualify the concept of automatic retrieval by manipulating the stimulus arrangement, in order to investigate the effects of an unusual visuo-spatial presentation on the activation of arithmetic facts. EXPERIMENT 1 Our first experiment was divided into two conditions. We asked participants to perform a number-matching task only in one condition, and a number-matching task with a concomitant random spatial tapping (Experiment 1A) or backward subtraction (Experiment 1B) in the other. The order of the two conditions was counterbalanced between participants. In the case of the number-matching task only, we expected to find a significant slowing of reaction times (interference) when the target corresponded to the product of the cue digits rather than being unrelated to them. Given the absence of any arithmetical hint, this would be a stronger

5 evidence for automaticity of product activation when arithmetic is irrelevant or even interfering for the task at hand, as Thibodeau et al. (1996) interposed a multiplication sign between their cue digits. In the case of combined random tapping, or backward counting, and the number-matching task, if the task-irrelevant activation of multiplication facts satisfies the capacity criterion of automaticity, it should not be affected by any one of the concomitant tasks. Backward subtraction and random spatial tapping task, indeed, have classically been assumed to heavily tap on attentional resources (e.g., Logie, Gilhooly, & Wynn, 1994). In the number-matching task, product interference emerges from a task-irrelevant cognitive operation, therefore, under dual-task condition we might expect a reduction, or even a total disappearance of interference, if interference in a single-task condition depended on the availability of spared resources. Lavie (1995; Lavie & Cox, 1997) has shown that the locus of the selective attention filter in the spatial domain can be changed as a function of the perceptual/cognitive load of the task at hand. She found that interference effects in the flanker paradigm could be eliminated by increasing task difficulty and thus force participants to adopt an attentional set completely focused on the target-defining attributes. By applying this notion to a numbermatching paradigm, one may argue that the interference effect results from the task not imposing a heavy perceptual/cognitive load on the participants attentional state. Therefore, assuming a nearly total engagement of processing resources because of the execution of a concomitant task, the interference effect should be eliminated. If, instead, performance in the number-matching task is impaired by a concomitant backward subtraction task but not by a random spatial tapping, the disruption would be unambiguously attributed to a structural competition, as backward subtraction implies numbers exactly like product retrieval. A selective interference by random spatial tapping, on the contrary, would probably reveal an involvement of the spatial sketch-pad in multiplication facts retrieval. Method Participants AUTOMATICITY AND PRODUCT RETRIEVAL 1489 A total of 23 undergraduates from the University of Padua (most of them enrolled in humanistic faculties) participated in the experiment as volunteers. Their age ranged from 22 to 36 years, with a mean of 25.3 years. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naive to the purpose of the experiment. Apparatus, stimuli, and procedure Experiments 1A and 1B. Primary task. All the participants performed the primary task in two sessions: one in the single-task condition and the other in the dual-task condition. The order of the conditions was counterbalanced between participants. A secondary random spatial tapping task was assigned to one group of participants (Experiment 1A), and a secondary backward subtraction task was assigned to the other group (Experiment 1B). The basic experimental paradigm was that of the number matching for multiplication facts. Each trial included an initial cue (e.g., 9 8) and a subsequent target (e.g., 75). Ties (e.g., 4 4) were excluded, because they appear to have an easier access to the memory store than do other problems (e.g., Graham & Campbell, 1992). There were six types of stimuli; three belonging to the nonmatching category and three belonging to the matching category. Nonmatching/product stimuli consisted of targets that were correct products of the cue (e.g., 3 8 and 24). Nonmatching/unrelated stimuli had the same cues as the product stimuli but the target was

6 1490 RUSCONI ET AL. unrelated to either digit in the cue (e.g., 3 8 and 49). For both product and unrelated stimuli, the target did not match or include either number of the digit cue. Nonmatching/filler stimuli had a double-digit number in the cue and a single-digit number in the target, so that participants saw nonmatching trials that included double-digit cues and also single-digit targets that required a no response (e.g., 48 7 and 9). Matching/target-balancing stimuli had the same targets as those used in the nonmatching product trials, so that participants saw cases in which the double-digit number in the cue matched the target (e.g., 7 53 and 53). In matching/cue-balancing stimuli there were the same cues of nonmatching/product stimuli. This condition was included so that participants saw matching trials that consisted of two single-digit numbers in the cue (4 3 and 3). Matching/filler stimuli had a double-digit number in the cue (e.g., 97 3 and 3), so that participants saw trials in which the single digit in the cue matched the target (the opposite happening for target-balancing stimuli). The experimental list contained 10 stimuli for each type (see Appendix A), presented at three different stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs). In total, there were 180 stimuli for each block. The experimental session was divided into two blocks (single-task and dual-task conditions), differing, apart from the requirement of performing the secondary task, only for the randomized order of stimuli. None of the number cues or targets was 0 or 1, in order not to elicit back-up strategies instead of direct access to a result (e.g., Baroody, 1983). Combinations of cues and targets that may evoke activation on the basis of some relation among the items other than multiplication (e.g., addition or counting: 5 7 and 12 or 3 4 and 5) were discarded. Participants were required to respond no if the one- or two-digit target did not match any of the cue numbers (e.g., 6 9 and 54 or 6 75 and 8) and to respond yes if the target matched either number in the cue (e.g., 6 9 and 9 or 2 89 and 89). An IBMcompatible 486 computer connected to a 15-inch colour VGA monitor controlled timing of events and generated stimuli. Display background was black, and stimuli appeared in white. Each trial began with a 100-ms 500-Hz tone as warning signal. At the same time, the fixation point (a hash sign) appeared for 400 ms. Then the cue appeared at the centre of the screen and was visible for 60 ms. It was then replaced by a 40-ms backward mask consisting of seven hash signs. Following the mask, a blank screen was presented for a variable time interval (20 ms, 170 ms, or 300 ms), therefore producing three different SOAs (120 ms, 270 ms, and 400 ms). Each SOA occurred with the same frequency. The target was then presented at the centre of the screen (see Figure 1). Participants were required to decide whether the Figure 1. Experimental procedure for the number-matching task in Experiments 1 and 2.

7 AUTOMATICITY AND PRODUCT RETRIEVAL 1491 target matched one of the digits in the cue and to press the appropriate key (e.g., 1 for yes, and 3 for no)on the numerical key-pad on the computer keyboard as fast as possible. Reaction time and accuracy were recorded, and, at the end of each trial, participants were given a visual feedback concerning their performance. They sat at an approximate distance of 50 cm from the screen. Each single digit measured 50 mm in height and 30 mm in width, and the two digits in the cue were always separated by three spaces. Note that there was no multiplication sign between the two digit cues. Half of the participants were instructed to respond yes by pressing a key with their right index finger and no by pressing another key with their right middle finger; the other half received opposite instructions. Note that the presence of the interference effect was indexed by significantly slower reaction times for nonmatching/product trials than for nonmatching/unrelated trials. We hypothesized that participants, upon presentation of the cue (e.g., 9 3) activate a set of possible targets that includes the actual number presented (e.g., 9, 3), as well as associated numbers (e.g., 27). Therefore, they should be slower to respond no when the target was the product (i.e., 9 3 and 27) than when the target was unrelated to the pair (e.g., 9 3 and 28). For what concerns the effect of SOA, we might expect the same pattern of results as that found by LeFevre and her colleagues (LeFevre et al., 1988; LeFevre & Kulak, 1994) that is, interference with the shorter SOA (120 ms), which declines and/or disappears when the SOA becomes longer (270 ms and 400 ms). Alternatively, based on previous data from our own laboratory (Galfano et al., 2003), we might expect a significant interference effect also at the longer SOAs. Experiment 1A. Secondary task: Random spatial tapping. In the concomitant task, 12 participants had to press with their left index finger six different keys in a random sequence, with a constant 1-s rhythm, paced by a metronome that continued throughout the entire dual-task block. The keys belonged to the numerical key-pad of a keyboard positioned on a table to the participant s left and connected to an I-Mac processor. They were the 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6 keys, isolated from the neighbour keys by a cardboard screen and covered with white labels. After practising for about 3 minutes, participants were instructed to maintain their gaze fixed to the centre of the PC screen, in order to begin performing their primary task. The putatively random sequence of tapped keys was recorded in a SimpleText file on the I-Mac. During the whole session, the experimenter was present for assuring that participants adhered to the instructions (constant rhythm, random generation, and gaze fixed to the centre of the screen). Experiment 1B. Secondary task: Backward subtraction. In the concomitant task, 11 participants had to count backward by three, starting from a three-digit number provided by the experimenter, and with a constant 1.8-s rhythm paced by a metronome, which continued throughout the entire dual-task block. The articulation of a three-digit number requires time: This is the reason why we chose a slower pace with respect to the random spatial tapping. Using two-digit numbers instead would allow a faster pace. However, as the primary task lasted for a few minutes, this would have required frequent interruptions of the task, as the participant would reach one-digit numbers in about one minute from backward subtraction. Experimental instructions explicitly required participants to pronounce aloud the result of every subtraction, so that their performance was under control. During each dual-task block, which lasted about 9 minutes, the experimenter provided a total of three starting numbers (one every 3 minutes, alternating odd and even numbers), in order to maintain constant the articulation time for each number word. After practising for about 3 minutes, participants were instructed to maintain their gaze fixed to the centre of the screen, in order to begin performing their primary task, while the experimenter pronounced aloud the first three-digit number. Performance on the subtraction task was manually recorded by the experimenter, who also reminded the participant to be accurate, if more than three consecutive results were wrong. A response was not considered wrong if it was three steps away from a previous wrong answer (e.g., in the response sequence 100, 97, 93, 90, thereisonlyoneerror).

8 1492 RUSCONI ET AL. Results Experiment 1A Performance in the number-matching task (single- and dual-task conditions). All analyses reported here and in the corresponding sections of Experiments 2 and 3 refer to nonmatching/product and nonmatching/unrelated trials that is, to the crucial manipulation of our experimental design. For the first group of participants, mean correct reaction times (RTs) were submitted to an analysis of variance (ANOVA) with a between-participants factor, order of conditions (single task/dual task vs. dual task/single task), and three within-participants factors, condition (single task vs. dual task), SOA (120 ms vs. 270 ms vs. 400 ms) and target type (product vs. unrelated). It revealed significant main effects of condition, F(1, 10) = 20.03, p <.001, SOA, F(2, 20) = 18.87, p <.0001, and target type, F(1, 10) = 10.55, p =.009. Mean RT in the singletask condition (number matching task: M = 692 ms, SE = 38) was 181 ms faster than mean RT in the dual-task condition (number matching + random tapping: M = 873 ms, SE = 70). RT decreased as SOA increased (120-ms SOA: M = 862 ms, SE = 56; 270-ms SOA: M = 750 ms, SE = 52; 400-ms SOA: M = 737 ms, SE = 42). That presumably reflects the well-known temporal warning effect (see, e.g., Niemi & Näätänen, 1981) and was not considered further in the present and in the subsequent discussions. Responses to product targets required, on average, 814 ms (SE = 55) to be executed, whereas responding to unrelated targets required 752 ms (SE = 45; 62 ms of interference effect). The crucial Condition Target Type interaction was far from significance (F < 1), which does not allow one to accept the hypothesis of a difference between the interference effect found in the single-task condition (67 ms) and that found in the dual-task condition (57 ms). The mean error rate was about 2%. A 2 (order of blocks) 2 (condition) 3 (SOA) 2 (target type) ANOVA on the number of errors revealed only a significant main effect of condition, F(1, 10) = 9.35, p <.01, indicating a lower accuracy in the dual-task condition than in the single-task condition, and of target type, F(1, 10) = 5.00, p <.05, indicating lower accuracy on product trials than on unrelated trials. The Condition Target Type interaction was not significant (F < 1). Performance in the secondary task (random spatial tapping). We assumed that the two conditions differed for the amount of resources available to execute the primary task. However, the nonsignificant difference in the interference effect between the single-task condition and the dual-task condition might be due to the fact that participants did not comply with the instructions concerning the secondary task. Participants performance in randomization tasks is commonly assessed by measures of stereotypy (e.g., frequency of adjacent items in an ordinal sequence or preferential selection of particular pairs), and usage of the response alternatives (e.g., whether each item in the response set is equally likely to be selected). In the present dual-task conditions, the mean redundancy (R) score (see Towse & Neil, 1998) was 1.45% (range: %), the mean Coupon score was (range: ), the mean Guttman s Null Score Quotient (NSQ) was 3.09% (range: %) and the mean Turning Point Index (TPI) was 89.29% (range: %).

9 AUTOMATICITY AND PRODUCT RETRIEVAL 1493 Experiment 1B Performance in the number-matching task (single- and dual-task conditions). A 2 (order of conditions) 2 (condition) 3 (SOA) 2 (target type) ANOVA on RTs revealed significant main effects of condition, F(1, 9) = 8.47, p =.02, SOA, F(2, 18) = 33.24, p <.0001, and target type, F(1, 9) = 5.57, p =.04. The mean latency of response for the single-task condition (number-matching task: M = 660 ms, SE = 40) was about 206 ms faster than the mean latency for the dual-task condition (number matching + backward subtraction: M = 866 ms, SE = 73). RT decreased as SOA increased (120-ms SOA: M = 839 ms, SE = 59; 270-ms SOA: M = 762 ms, SE = 55; 400-ms SOA: M = 688 ms, SE = 44). Responses to product targets required on average 789 ms (SE = 57) to be executed, while responding to unrelated targets required 737 ms (SE = 47; 52 ms of interference effect). The crucial Condition Target Type interaction was far from significance (F < 1), which does not allow one to accept the hypothesis of a difference between the mean interference effect found in the single-task condition (65 ms) and that found in the dual-task condition (40 ms). A further ANOVA with two between-participants factors, group (tapping vs. subtraction) and order of execution (single/dual vs. dual/single), and three within-participant factors, condition (single- vs. dual-task condition), SOA (120, 270, and 400 ms) and target type (product vs. unrelated), was executed, with RT data from both the first and the second group, to assess the hypothesis of a quantitative difference between the two dual-task manipulations. In particular, the 40-ms interference effect found with the concomitant backward-subtraction task might be significantly lower than the 57-ms effect found with random spatial tapping. The ANOVA revealed significant main effects of condition, F(1, 19) = 23.66, p <.0001, SOA, F(2, 38) = 46.08, p <.0001, and target type, F(1, 19) = 15.42, p =.0009, but no main effect of the two between-participants factors (Fs < 1). No two-way or three-way interactions reached significance, including the crucial Group Target Type, and Group Condition Target Type interactions (Fs < 1). The mean error rate was 4%, and a 2 (order of conditions) 2 (condition) 3 (SOA) 2 (target type) ANOVA on the number of errors revealed only a significant main effect of condition, F(1, 9) = 25.06, p <.001, showing a lower accuracy in the dual-task condition than in the single-task condition, and of target type, F(1, 9) = 12.18, p =.007, showing lower accuracy with product trials than unrelated trials. The Condition Target Type interaction was not significant (F < 1). Performance on the secondary task (backward subtraction). The mean percentage of errors was 4%, identical to the mean percentage of errors for the primary task. During task performance, the experimenter noted on preprinted tables calculations and errors produced by the participant. The participant showing the best performance was accurate on 99% of counts, the participant showing the worst performance was accurate on 87% of counts. A qualitative classification of errors indicated a substantial homogeneity, with a predominance of subtractions by 1, 2, or 4 and hundred or decade substitutions. Discussion The present experiment confirms and extends the Thibodeau et al. (1996) study regarding the obligatoriness of product retrieval. In a number-matching task we found significantly slower

10 1494 RUSCONI ET AL. RTs to nonmatching/product than to unrelated targets, even in the absence of any arithmetical cue (e.g., a multiplication sign between the cue digits). We further assumed that, if the interference effect was still present in spite of the need to perform a resource-consuming concomitant task, that would fulfil the capacity criterion of automaticity. Thus, there would be evidence that the activation of multiplication facts is both obligatory and resource independent. That in turn would be evidence that the activation of multiplication facts is automatic, according to the criteria proposed by Jonides (1981). Our results provide clear evidence in favour of a truly automatic activation for multiplication facts. The interference effect is significant and present in both the single-task and the dual-task conditions, and its time course does not differ between the two conditions. The lack of interaction between SOA and target type is consistent with other data from our laboratory on neighbour nodes (Galfano et al., 2003), but contradicts other studies that employed the same number-matching task (e.g., LeFevre et al., 1988; Thibodeau et al., 1996). At the moment, we are not able to identify the reason for this difference. An analysis of the studies on multiplication facts points to the selection of stimuli as a potentially critical factor. The rapid decrease (if not the disappearance or even the inversion) of the interference effect in the Thibodeau et al. study might be due to the presence of a higher percentage of product targets (20% vs. 15% in our experiments) and the inclusion of 5-related facts, which have an easier access than other multiplication facts (Campbell & Graham, 1985). The relative accessibility and frequency of critical targets may have produced an earlier activation peak and earlier activation decay. Further experimental work is necessary to test the plausibility of this provisional explanation. The interference found in the dual-task conditions was smaller than that found in the single task, even if the Condition Target Type interaction was far from significance. Assuming that our experiment lacked statistical power and that this difference is real, it might be attributed to the slower RTs in the dual-task conditions. It has been proposed (e.g., De Jong, Liang, & Lauber, 1994) that, when an automatically activated representation is not task relevant it decays rapidly, and its effect on RTs varies as a function of the relative response speed. That is, with longer RTs the probability to detect the effects of automatic activation decreases. In the present experiment, the temporal interval between cue onset and selection of the response was substantially longer in dual-task conditions, which may explain the apparent decrease in magnitude of the interference effect. However, this interpretation was not sustained by a significant interaction between target type and SOA. As at the shortest SOA RTs were significantly longer than at the longest SOA, we should have registered a larger interference effect at the longest SOA. At any rate, it is clear that the activation of the product occurs here and even under very unfavourable conditions. However, we have to be cautious in concluding that processing resources play no role in the retrieval of arithmetic knowledge. In fact, retrieval could be only partially automatic. Rather surprisingly, we did find a significant worsening of performance on the primary task with a concomitant backward subtraction but no significant reduction of the interference effect. Given the fact that both number matching and the activation of arithmetic knowledge require processing of number representations, we must consider at least two possibilities, besides the one that invokes lack of statistical power. The first is that the backward subtraction task actually did not require processing resources. We show in Experiment 2 that this was not

11 AUTOMATICITY AND PRODUCT RETRIEVAL 1495 the case. The second possible explanation appeals to cognitive and neuropsychological models of number cognition. According to the triple-code model (Dehaene, 1992; Dehaene & Cohen, 1995), not all types of arithmetic operation are handled by the same functional and neural system. Familiar multiplication problems are often learned by rote and would be solved through the retrieval of automatic verbal associations. In contrast, other problems, particularly subtraction problems, would not be systematically memorized and would require the manipulation of the quantities represented by the operands instead. Neuropsychological evidence points to the existence of two dissociable types of arithmetic routines verbal and semantic (e.g., Dehaene & Cohen, 1997). Our data fit well with the functional separability of different arithmetic operations, as the retrieval of multiplication facts was not impaired by the execution of a concomitant subtraction task. According to Dehaene (1992), a prerequisite for the retrieval of multiplication facts is that arabic operands be first encoded in a verbal format. One wonders, of course, how our participants could efficiently retrieve the product of two digits flashed on a screen, if an interfering task was loading their phonological loop. If Dehaene s hypothesis were correct, results should have revealed effects of a structural verbal competition on the spontaneous activation of multiplication facts. That did not happen. However, we cannot define our backward subtraction task as a proper concomitant verbal task, given the slow rate imposed by the choice of three-digit numbers. The hypothesis of a verbal structural interference was addressed in Experiment 2. EXPERIMENT 2 In Experiment 2, all participants took part in eight sessions, equally distributed between two days on consecutive weeks (e.g., if a participant performed the first four sessions on Thursday, he or she completed the remaining four on the next Thursday). Number matching was performed as the primary task in four out of the eight sessions, and verification of simple arithmetical problems was performed as the primary task in the other four sessions. Except for the two single-task conditions (number matching or verification only), a concomitant task was performed during each session: backward subtraction, random spatial tapping, or articulatory suppression. The number-matching task has already been described for Experiment 1, and its basic characteristics remained the same in the present experiment. However, stricter criteria were adopted in the selection of the stimulus set, as described in the following section. As in the previous experiment, we were interested in analysing the interference effect under different conditions that is, during the execution of tasks that required attentional resources (spatial random tapping and backward subtraction) and during a task selectively interfering with verbal processing (articulatory suppression). As regards the effects of secondary tasks requiring attentional resources, we expected to replicate the findings we observed in the previous experiment. In particular, we expected to find a significant interference effect (related trials slower than unrelated trials) both in the single-task condition and in the conditions with concomitant spatial random tapping and backward subtraction. The condition with concomitant articulatory suppression was introduced in order to test the hypothesis of obligatory access to the verbal code for the retrieval of multiplication facts (e.g., Dehaene & Cohen, 1997). Were this hypothesis correct, a concomitant task interfering with the articulatory loop

12 1496 RUSCONI ET AL. should disrupt the retrieval process, which in turn would result in a significant reduction, or disappearance of the interference effect. If, on the other hand, interference persisted even with concomitant articulatory suppression, then this would imply that multiplication facts can be accessed without necessarily employing a verbal code. In addition to number matching, we introduced as a primary task the arithmetic verification task that Zbrodoff and Logan (1986) adopted to test the intentionality criterion in simple mental arithmetic. In the mixed block condition of their Experiments 1 and 2, participants verified simple multiplication and addition problems; half of the equations were true and half were false. Half of the false equations, defined as associative lures, would have been true, had participants performed the alternative arithmetic operation (e.g., = 28; 4 7 = 11), and the remaining false equations, called nonassociative lures, would not have been true under any conventional arithmetic operation (e.g., = 26; 4 7 = 13). Nonassociative lures were responded to faster and more accurately than associative lures, showing the presence of an associative confusion effect (i.e., a cost in RT and error rate to associative vs. nonassociative lures). By manipulating the relevance of the irrelevant operation, the authors demonstrated that this effect is only partially automatic. Indeed, intending to perform just one operation (pure block condition) reduced its magnitude. We were not interested in the effect of our dualtask conditions on the verification task per se. Rather, we were interested in the associative confusion effect: Being only partially automatic (Zbrodoff & Logan, 1986), the associative confusion effect should disappear or significantly decrease when participants are performing a concomitant random spatial tapping or a concomitant backward subtraction. Moreover, if arithmetic facts were obligatorily retrieved by accessing their verbal code, a structural competition effect should emerge with concomitant articulatory suppression, thus eliminating the associative confusion deriving from product associative lures (i.e., during the verification of addition problems). We expected a different pattern as regards the influence of the concomitant resourcedemanding tasks on the associative confusion effect (in the verification task) and on the interference effect (in number-matching task). In particular, we predicted that the interference effect would remain significant at least in three out of the four conditions in which number matching was performed that is, in the single-task condition and even in the presence of resource-consuming concomitant tasks (backward subtraction and spatial random tapping) as in Experiment 1. By contrast, we expected the associative confusion effect to be present in the single-task condition but not in the conditions with concomitant backward subtraction and random spatial tapping. According to the triple-code model (e.g., Dehaene & Cohen, 1995), neither the interference effect in the number-matching task nor the associative confusion effect in the verification task should be reliable in the conditions with concomitant articulatory suppression. To summarize, in designing Experiment 2, we introduced three modifications. First, we added another primary task. This was aimed at showing that the dual-task manipulations used in Experiment 1 selectively influence an associative effect (observed under conditions of task-relevant retrieval), leaving the interference effect (observed under conditions of taskirrelevant retrieval) relatively unchanged. In doing so, we empirically tested the arguments of task ineffectiveness and/or lack of power that might be invoked as alternative accounts for the results of Experiment 1. Second, we adopted, as an additional concomitant task, the classical articulatory suppression task at 1-s rate (e.g., Logie et al., 1994) to test the

13 AUTOMATICITY AND PRODUCT RETRIEVAL 1497 hypothesis of an obligatory translation into a verbal code for retrieving the product of two single-digit numbers (e.g., Dehaene & Cohen, 1997). Third, we opted for a within-participants design, which should enhance sensitivity to experimental manipulations and statistical power. Method Participants A total of 17 undergraduates from the University of Padua (most of them enrolled in humanistic faculties) participated in the experiment as paid volunteers. Their age ranged from 22 to 29 years, with a mean of 24.5 years. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naive to the purpose of the experiment. None had participated in the previous experiment. Apparatus, stimuli, and procedure Primary tasks. Number matching task. All the participants performed number matching as primary task in four sessions: one in the single-task condition and the others in each of the three dual-task conditions. The order of conditions was roughly counterbalanced between participants, and, for each participant, it was the same as the order of conditions in the verification task. The basic experimental paradigm was the same as that used in the previous experiment. A total of 11 stimuli for each of the six target conditions were created, and additional criteria were used to select the stimulus set, besides those used in Experiment 1. First, we selected only unrelated targets having the same parity as the product targets (i.e., an odd product was coupled with an odd unrelated target, and an even product was coupled with an even target), thus increasing their semantic similarity. Second, the average magnitude of unrelated and product targets was nearly the same that is, five of the unrelated targets were obtained by adding two units to the product and six of them by subtracting two units. This would eliminate the possibility that product targets were rejected more slowly because they were closer to the cue digits (also see Galfano et al., 2003). Furthermore, in the present experiment, if the product contained one of the single digits presented in the cue, its corresponding two-digit unrelated target also presented a partial matching. That was allowed in order to have a fair number of stimuli that met the new selection criteria. Apparatus and procedure were the same as those in Experiment 1. The only exception was the presence of only one SOA (120 ms) instead of three, as in the previous experiment SOA did not interact with any of the other factors in the experimental design. The total number of trials was 132, resulting from two repetitions of the entire set of 66 stimuli reported in Appendix B. Verification task. The stimuli were simple equations of the form a + b = c and a b = c and are reported in Appendix B. The same pairs of digits that had been presented as critical cues in the matching task were used as pairs of operands in the verification task. Half of the trials were true equations, and half were false equations. Addition and multiplication problems were equally distributed in the two conditions. For addition problems, 11 true equations were formed such that c was the sum of a and b (e.g., = 11), 11 associative lures were formed such that c was the product of a and b (e.g., = 28), and 11 nonassociative lures were formed such that c was neither the sum nor the product of a and b (e.g., = 26). Similarly, for the multiplication problems, 11 true equations, 11 associative lures (e.g., 4 7 = 11), and 11 nonassociative lures (e.g., 4 7 = 13) were created. In both the addition and multiplication problems, the proposed results for the nonassociative lures were chosen to match the distance between correct results and associative lures. Average distance between lures and sums for addition problems was in the associative condition and in the nonassociative condition; average distance

14 1498 RUSCONI ET AL. between lures and products for multiplication problems was in the associative condition and in the nonassociative condition. The stimuli appeared centred on the screen located in front of the participants. Each single digit measured 50 mm in height and 30 mm in width. The operands and the operator were always separated by a single space. On each trial, participants first saw a fixation hash sign lasting for 500 ms, serving as a warning signal. This was accompanied by a 100-ms, 500-Hz tone. After fixation offset, the operation appeared and remained visible for 2,500 ms or until a response was made, whichever came first. The allowed response keys were the same as those for the number-matching task. A total of 8 participants responded false by pressing the 1 key and true by pressing the 3 key; the others did the opposite. In each of the four sessions with verification task as primary task, the participants received a block of 172 trials. Within each block, 22 true equations were repeated four times, and 44 lures were repeated two times each. The order of trials was randomized independently during each block. Participants were told that they had to verify simple arithmetic equations by pressing one of two response keys and were asked to be as quick and accurate as possible. Secondary tasks. Random spatial tapping. In order to render the random spatial tapping more demanding than in Experiment 1A, participants had to press with their left index finger nine different keys in a random sequence, with a constant 1-s rhythm, paced by a metronome that continued through the entire dual-task block. As in Experiment 1A, the keys belonged to the numerical key-pad of a different keyboard from that used for performing the primary task. They were the 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, and 9 keys, isolated from the neighbour keys by a cardboard screen and covered with white labels. The procedure was identical to that described for Experiment 1A. Backward subtraction. Apparatus, stimuli, and procedure were the same as those in Experiment 1. Articulatory suppression. Our participants were asked to repeat aloud the word bah at a constant rhythm of 1 s, paced by a metronome, while performing the primary task. The experimenter was always present to monitor the correct execution of the task. Results Performance in the primary tasks Number matching. Mean correct RTs were submitted to a two-way repeated measures ANOVA with condition (single task vs. concomitant tapping vs. concomitant subtraction vs. concomitant suppression) and target type (product vs. unrelated) as within-participant factors. It revealed significant main effects of condition, F(3, 48) = 12.92, p <.0001 (mean RTs of 619 ms for the single-task condition, 667 ms for the condition with concomitant tapping, 751 ms for the condition with concomitant backward subtraction, and 618 ms for the condition with articulatory suppression), and target type, F(1, 16) = 8.74, p =.009 (mean RTs of 678 ms for the product and 650 ms for unrelated targets). The Condition Target Type interaction was not significant (F < 1). However, in order to test our specific hypotheses more directly, we performed separate planned comparisons for target type in each condition. In the single-task condition there was a significant interference effect of 18 ms: product, M = 628 ms, SE = 20; unrelated, M = 610 ms, SE = 19; t(16) = 1.83, p =.04, one-tailed. A significant 25-ms interference effect was present in the condition with concomitant random spatial tapping (product, M = 679 ms, SE = 21; unrelated, M = 654 ms, SE = 21), t(16) = 1.87, p =.04, one-tailed, and in

The role of response selection and input monitoring in solving simple arithmetical products

The role of response selection and input monitoring in solving simple arithmetical products Journal Memory & Cognition 2005,?? 33 (?), (8),???-??? 1472-1483 The role of response selection and input monitoring in solving simple arithmetical products MAUD DESCHUYTENEER and ANDRÉ VANDIERENDONCK

More information

2 3 primes naming 6 : Evidence from masked priming

2 3 primes naming 6 : Evidence from masked priming Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2009, 71 (3), 471-480 doi:10.3758/app.71.3.471 2 3 primes naming 6 : Evidence from masked priming JAVIER GARCÍA-ORZA, JESÚS DAMAS-LÓPEZ, ANTONIO MATAS, AND JOSÉ MIGUEL

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1040-1050 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1040 Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching ERINA SAEKI AND SATORU

More information

The role of phonological and executive working memory resources in simple arithmetic strategies

The role of phonological and executive working memory resources in simple arithmetic strategies EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2007, 19 (6), 910 933 The role of phonological and executive working memory resources in simple arithmetic strategies Ineke Imbo and André Vandierendonck Ghent

More information

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The number line effect reflects top-down control Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),862-868???-??? The number line effect reflects top-down control JELENA RISTIC University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Decomposing the Mean: Using Distributional Analyses to Provide a Detailed Description of Addition and Multiplication Latencies

Decomposing the Mean: Using Distributional Analyses to Provide a Detailed Description of Addition and Multiplication Latencies Decomposing the Mean: Using Distributional Analyses to Provide a Detailed Description of Addition and Multiplication Latencies Marcie Penner-Wilger (mpwilger@connect.carleton.ca) Institute of Cognitive

More information

The role of working memory in carrying and borrowing

The role of working memory in carrying and borrowing Psychological Research (27) 71: 467 483 DOI 1.17/s426-6-44-8 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Ineke Imbo Æ Andre Vandierendonck Æ Evie Vergauwe The role of working memory in carrying and borrowing Received: 1 October

More information

Visuospatial priming of the mental number line

Visuospatial priming of the mental number line Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Cognition 106 (2008) 770 779 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Visuospatial priming of the mental number line Ivilin Stoianov *, Peter Kramer, Carlo Umiltà, Marco

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan University of Alberta The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm by Shawn Tan A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 396 409 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.396

More information

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (3), 490-494 Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness GINO CAMP, DIANE PECHER, and HENK G. SCHMIDT Erasmus University Rotterdam,

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003)

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003) Cognition 97 (2005) 89 97 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003) Filip Van Opstal a, Bert Reynvoet b, Tom Verguts a, * a Department

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article Research Article VISUAL SEARCH REMAINS EFFICIENT WHEN VISUAL WORKING MEMORY IS FULL Geoffrey F. Woodman, Edward K. Vogel, and Steven J. Luck University of Iowa Abstract Many theories of attention have

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience Working memory 1 What is working memory? Brief, immediate memory for information we are currently processing. Closely related to attention: attending to something is often

More information

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 B87 Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized Wim Gevers*,

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks Memory & Cognition 2005, 33 (6), 1085-1095 Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks IRING KOCH and WOLFGANG PRINZ Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Munich, Germany

More information

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Automatic detection - 1 Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1986, 24 (6), 431-434 SIU L. CHOW The University of Wollongong,

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (6), 1111-1116 doi:10.3758/pbr.15.6.1111 Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm Mei-Ching Lien Oregon

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the 1 Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 835-54. doi:10.1037/a0012726

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

Priming or executive control? Associative priming of cue encoding increases switch costs in the explicit task-cuing procedure

Priming or executive control? Associative priming of cue encoding increases switch costs in the explicit task-cuing procedure Journal Memory & Cognition 2006,?? 34 (?), (6),???-??? 1250-1259 Priming or executive control? Associative priming of cue encoding increases switch costs in the explicit task-cuing procedure GORDON D.

More information

Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression

Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression Supplementary Material Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression 1, 2, * Bahador Bahrami Petra Vetter 1, 3 Eva Spolaore 1, 4 Silvia Pagano 1, 4 Brian Butterworth 1,3 Geraint Rees 1,

More information

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources

Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (6), 1067-1073 Older adults associative deficit in episodic memory: Assessing the role of decline in attentional resources MOSHE NAVEH-BENJAMIN University of Missouri,

More information

A systems neuroscience approach to memory

A systems neuroscience approach to memory A systems neuroscience approach to memory Critical brain structures for declarative memory Relational memory vs. item memory Recollection vs. familiarity Recall vs. recognition What about PDs? R-K paradigm

More information

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Memory & Cognition 1974, Vol. 2, No.3, 533 538 Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Ss were required to learn linear

More information

On the Autonomy of Mental Processes: A Case Study of Arithmetic

On the Autonomy of Mental Processes: A Case Study of Arithmetic Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1986, Vol. 115, No. 2, 118-130 Copyright 1986 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-3445/86/100.75 On the Autonomy of Mental Processes: A Case

More information

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Psychon Bull Rev (2017) 24:1651 1657 DOI 10.3758/s13423-017-1226-7 BRIEF REPORT Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Sébastien De Schrijver 1 & Pierre Barrouillet 1 Published

More information

Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments

Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2010, 17 (2), 219-223 doi:10.3758/pbr.17.2.219 Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments ROLF REBER University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway PASCAL WURTZ

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-Signal Paradigm

Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-Signal Paradigm Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2011, Vol. 37, No. 2, 392 404 2010 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021800 Balancing Cognitive Demands:

More information

Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm

Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm VISUAL COGNITION, 2002, 9 (4/5), 421 457 Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm Peter Wühr and Jochen Müsseler Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research,

More information

Control processes in voluntary and explicitly cued task switching

Control processes in voluntary and explicitly cued task switching THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 Vol. 67, No. 10, 1944 1958, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.879390 Control processes in voluntary and explicitly cued task switching Michael

More information

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Alexa Morcom Edinburgh SPM course 2017 Thanks to Rik Henson, Thomas Wolbers, Jody Culham, and the SPM authors for slides Overview Categorical designs Factorial designs

More information

Automaticity of Number Perception

Automaticity of Number Perception Automaticity of Number Perception Jessica M. Choplin (jessica.choplin@vanderbilt.edu) Gordon D. Logan (gordon.logan@vanderbilt.edu) Vanderbilt University Psychology Department 111 21 st Avenue South Nashville,

More information

Cross-modal attentional deficits in processing tactile stimulation

Cross-modal attentional deficits in processing tactile stimulation Perception & Psychophysics 2001, 63 (5), 777-789 Cross-modal attentional deficits in processing tactile stimulation ROBERTO DELL ACQUA and MASSIMO TURATTO University of Padua, Padua, Italy and PIERRE JOLICŒUR

More information

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

The spacing and lag effect in free recall The spacing and lag effect in free recall Michael J. Kahana, Bradley R. Wellington & Marc W. Howard Center for Complex Systems and Department of Psychology Brandeis University Send correspondence to: Michael

More information

Spontaneous Trait Inferences Are Bound to Actors Faces: Evidence From a False Recognition Paradigm

Spontaneous Trait Inferences Are Bound to Actors Faces: Evidence From a False Recognition Paradigm Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 83, No. 5, 1051 1065 0022-3514/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.5.1051 Spontaneous

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning GSTF Journal of Psychology (JPsych) Vol. No., August 5 Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning Hironori Oto 79 Received 6 Jul 5 Accepted 9 Aug 5 Abstract This paper

More information

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task? Bachelor s Thesis Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a sequential movement task? Nils Hatger Prof. Dr. Ing. Willem Verwey Jonathan Barnhoorn, PhD student BMS Faculty/Cognitive

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

Cognition. Mid-term 1. Top topics for Mid Term 1. Heads up! Mid-term exam next week

Cognition. Mid-term 1. Top topics for Mid Term 1. Heads up! Mid-term exam next week Cognition Prof. Mike Dillinger Mid-term 1 Heads up! Mid-term exam next week Chapters 2, 3, of the textbook Perception, Attention, Short-term memory The lectures are to help you digest the chapters; exams

More information

Learning at any rate: action effect learning for stimulus-based actions

Learning at any rate: action effect learning for stimulus-based actions Psychological Research (2011) 75:61 65 DOI 10.1007/s00426-010-0288-1 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Learning at any rate: action effect learning for stimulus-based actions Roland Pfister Andrea Kiesel Joachim Hoffmann

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: Patterns of selective interference in subitizing and counting

The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: Patterns of selective interference in subitizing and counting Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (4), 675-681 The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: Patterns of selective interference in subitizing and counting LANA M. TRICK University of Guelph,

More information

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2009, Vol. 35, No. 2, 434 445 2009 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014529 Influence of Visual Stimulus

More information

On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink

On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 723-728 On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink Pau l E. Dux Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee and Irina M. Harris University

More information

Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, in press. Physical similarity (and not quantity representation) drives perceptual comparison of

Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, in press. Physical similarity (and not quantity representation) drives perceptual comparison of Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, in press Physical similarity (and not quantity representation) drives perceptual comparison of numbers: Evidence from two Indian notations (in press; Psychonomic Bulletin

More information

Intentional versus unintentional use of contingencies between perceptual events

Intentional versus unintentional use of contingencies between perceptual events University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 1996 Intentional versus unintentional use of contingencies

More information

Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration

Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:1694 1709 DOI 10.3758/s13414-011-0135-5 Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration Tomonari Shimomura & Takatsune Kumada Published

More information

Time causes forgetting from working memory

Time causes forgetting from working memory Psychon Bull Rev (2012) 19:87 92 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0192-8 BRIEF REPORT Time causes forgetting from working memory Pierre Barrouillet & Annick De Paepe & Naomi Langerock Published online: 20 December

More information

Awareness in contextual cuing with extended and concurrent explicit tests

Awareness in contextual cuing with extended and concurrent explicit tests Memory & Cognition 28, 36 (2), 43-415 doi: 1.3758/MC.36.2.43 Awareness in contextual cuing with extended and concurrent explicit tests ANDREA C. SMYTH AND DAVID R. SHANKS University College London, London,

More information

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1994, 47A (1) 201-205 Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing Harold Pashler University of

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (3), 504-513 Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks Christian Frings Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany and Peter Wühr Friedrich

More information

Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation

Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (7), 1791-1802 doi:10.3758/app.72.7.1791 Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation SANDRA A J. THOMSON,

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

Short-Term Memory Demands of Reaction-Time Tasks That Differ in Complexity

Short-Term Memory Demands of Reaction-Time Tasks That Differ in Complexity Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 190, Vol. 6, No. 2, 375-39 Short-Term Memory Demands of Reaction-Time Tasks That Differ in Complexity Gordon D. Logan Erindale College,

More information

Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning

Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning Sequential similarity and comparison effects in category learning Paulo F. Carvalho (pcarvalh@indiana.edu) Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University 1101 East Tenth Street Bloomington,

More information

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Mem Cogn (2011) 39:1222 1231 DOI 10.3758/s13421-011-0105-0 Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Dawn M. McBride & Jaclyn K. Beckner & Drew

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? Keyed. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 55B (1), 1 25 Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? J.H. Wearden, A. Parry, and L. Stamp University of

More information

DOCTORATE OF PHILOSOPHY Dec 2008 Committee: Richard B. Anderson, Advisor Dale Klopfer Yiwei Chen Barbara Moses

DOCTORATE OF PHILOSOPHY Dec 2008 Committee: Richard B. Anderson, Advisor Dale Klopfer Yiwei Chen Barbara Moses THE INFLUENCE OF REPRESENTATIONAL PROCESSES ON THE NUMERICAL DISTANCE EFFECT Neil D. Berg A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling Green State University in partial fulfillment of the

More information

To See or Not to See: The Visual Component of Complex Mental Arithmetic

To See or Not to See: The Visual Component of Complex Mental Arithmetic To See or Not to See: The Visual Component of Complex Mental Arithmetic Wendy Ann Deslauriers (wadeslau@connect.carleton.ca) Centre for Applied Cognitive Research, Institute of Cognitive Science, Carleton

More information

Binding across space and time in visual working memory

Binding across space and time in visual working memory Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (3), 292-303 doi:10.3758/mc.38.3.292 Binding across space and time in visual working memory PAUL JOHAN KARLSEN University of Tromsø, Tromsø, Norway RICHARD J. ALLEN University

More information

Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty

Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty Perception & Psychophysics 2008, 70 (8), 1435-1443 doi: 10.3758/PP.70.8.1435 Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty ZHE CHEN University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand

More information

Masked Primes Can Be Genuinely Semantically Processed

Masked Primes Can Be Genuinely Semantically Processed Masked Primes Can Be Genuinely Semantically Processed A Picture Prime Study Eva Van den Bussche, Karolien Notebaert, and Bert Reynvoet Department of Psychology, University of Leuven, Belgium Abstract.

More information

Numbers and Space: A Computational Model of the SNARC Effect

Numbers and Space: A Computational Model of the SNARC Effect Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 1, 32 44 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.32

More information

Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed

Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed Manard et al. BMC Neuroscience 2014, 15:7 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed Marine Manard 1,2, Delphine Carabin

More information

Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements. Influence Spatial-Numerical Associations

Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements. Influence Spatial-Numerical Associations Running head: CODING STRATEGIES IN NUMBER SPACE Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements Influence Spatial-Numerical Associations Oliver Lindemann 1, 2, Juan M. Abolafia 1,3, Jay Pratt 4

More information

Chapter 6: Absolute or relative position?

Chapter 6: Absolute or relative position? The nature of positional errors Experiment 2 showed that transpositions between groups tend to maintain the same position within a group. Such interpositions were as common as adjacent transpositions.

More information

Dissociating inhibition of return from endogenous orienting of spatial attention: Evidence from detection and discrimination tasks

Dissociating inhibition of return from endogenous orienting of spatial attention: Evidence from detection and discrimination tasks PCGN158810 TECHSET COMPOSITION LTD, SALISBURY, U.K. 2/17/2006 COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0), 1 18 Dissociating inhibition of return from endogenous orienting of spatial attention: Evidence from

More information

This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.

This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Treccani B., Cubelli R., Della Sala S., Umiltà, C. (2009). Flanker and Simon

More information

Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences

Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 970-976 Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences Darryl W. Schneider Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee Exploring the hierarchical control relationship

More information

Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck

Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 729-734 Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck PAUL E. DUX AND RENÉ MAROIS Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee The attentional blink

More information

Telling Lies: The Irrepressible Truth? Emma J. Williams, Lewis A. Bott, Michael B. Lewis and John Patrick. School of Psychology, Cardiff University

Telling Lies: The Irrepressible Truth? Emma J. Williams, Lewis A. Bott, Michael B. Lewis and John Patrick. School of Psychology, Cardiff University Telling Lies 1 RUNNING HEAD: TELLING LIES Telling Lies: The Irrepressible Truth? Emma J. Williams, Lewis A. Bott, Michael B. Lewis and John Patrick School of Psychology, Cardiff University School of Psychology

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

Taking control of reflexive social attention

Taking control of reflexive social attention Cognition 94 (2005) B55 B65 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Taking control of reflexive social attention Jelena Ristic*, Alan Kingstone Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia,

More information