Autonomy Support and Need Satisfaction in the Motivation and Well-Being of Gymnasts

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Autonomy Support and Need Satisfaction in the Motivation and Well-Being of Gymnasts"

Transcription

1 372 JOURNAL OF APPLIED SPORT PSYCHOLOGY, 15: , M GAGNÉ ET AL. Copyright Association for Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology ISSN: print / online DOI: / Autonomy Support and Need Satisfaction in the Motivation and Well-Being of Gymnasts MARYLÈNE GAGNÉ Concordia University RICHARD M. RYAN AND KELLY BARGMANN University of Rochester This study examined the effects of young athletes perceptions of support from coaches and parents on their need satisfaction, motivation, and well-being. Using the framework of selfdetermination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2000b), a 4-week diary study of 33 female gymnasts from the northeastern U.S. was conducted that examined (a) the relations of perceived parent and coach supports to the athlete s enduring and daily motivation and need satisfaction and (b) how daily motivation and psychological need satisfaction during practice affects athletes well-being. Results obtained using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) revealed that, as predicted, daily motivation predicted pre-practice well-being, and that changes in well-being from pre- to post-practice varied systematically with the need satisfaction experienced during practice. Discussion highlights the importance of adult supports for adolescent sport participants to ensure need satisfaction, and the advantages of diary methodologies in sport research. As children and teenagers participation in organized sports has grown, so has awareness of the benefits and hazards associated with it (Martens, 1978; Fox & Biddle, 1988; Whitehead & Corbin, 1997). Although psychological and physical benefits can derive from participation in competitive sport (Frederick & Ryan, 1995; Mandigo & Holt, 2000), for many children and teens, it can also lead to damaged self-esteem and mood disturbances, particularly when youths experience performance pressure from close adults (Brustad, 1988; Davis, 1997; Ommundsen & Vaglum, 1991; Pugh, Wolff, DeFrancesco, Gilley, & Heitman, 2000; Reeve & Deci, 1996). For these reasons, the influence of coaches and parents on children and teen sport participants experiences is an issue of utmost importance in sport psychology. Not surprisingly, there is considerable research on the interaction of parents and coaches with sport participants. For example, parental pressure predicted decreased enjoyment of bas- Received 13 May 2002; accepted 1 November Richard Ryan was supported in part by National Institute of Health Grant Marylène Gagné was supported by a doctoral fellowship from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. Address correspondence to Marylène Gagné, Department of Management, GM , John Molson School of Business, Concordia University, 1455 de Maisonneuve West, Montreal, Quebec, H3G 1M8, Canada. mgagne@jmsb.concordia.ca 372

2 WELL-BEING OF GYMNASTS 373 ketball (Brustad, 1988), and parental and coach positive emotional involvement predicted the enjoyment of soccer (Ommundsen & Vaglum, 1991). Also, having an autocratic coach who provided little feedback decreased intrinsic motivation in Division I college athletes (Amorose & Horn, 2000, 2001), and in swimmers (Black & Weiss, 1992). The present study examined the impact of athletes perceptions of coaches and parents, and training climate, on sport motivation and well-being. We used self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000b) to formulate hypotheses about how parents and coaches interaction styles influence athletes motivation and well-being. The Motivation of Gymnasts Gymnastics is a challenging skill-based sport that requires long hours of practice to attain proficiency, and rigorous training to attain the needed strength and endurance. Accordingly, gymnasts face many challenges, such as intense training, competition, and the maintenance of a physique that is difficult to maintain during adolescence (Davis, 1997; Douillard, 1994). Because of its rigor, and because training starts at an early age, both coaches and parents are typically strongly involved in a gymnast s development, and play critical roles in shaping the athlete s sport-related experience and self-concept. Adults expectations and pressures can lead young athletes to experience stress, pain, and self-disparagement, just as support and encouragement can lead to joy, a sense of challenge, and enhanced self-esteem (Goudas, Biddle, Fox, & Underwood, 1995; Krane, Greenleaf, & Snow, 1997). SDT (Ryan & Deci, 2000a) is relevant to this issue as it predicts how adult pressure can lead athletes to adopt a motivational style that is more controlling, whereas adult autonomy support can foster more self-determined and intrinsic forms of motivation (Frederick & Ryan, 1995; Ryan & Connell, 1989). Furthermore, a core aspect of SDT is the proposition that different forms of motivation range on a continuum of self-determination that have been shown to differentially affect performance and well-being (Ryan & Deci, 2000a). The Continuum of Motivational Styles SDT specifies that people can be motivated for different reasons that can be modeled as lying along a continuum of autonomy. The least autonomous form of motivation is labeled external regulation, and occurs when a person performs activities either to obtain rewards, or to avoid punishments or sanctions. Another form of motivation is introjection, which concerns performance motivated by self-esteem related contingencies. An introjected athlete feels prideful and self-aggrandizing when performing well, and self-disparaging or guilty when doing poorly. A still more autonomous form of motivation is described as identified motivation, when the person experiences an activity as personally valuable or important to the self, such as exercising to maintain one s health. Finally, some activities are intrinsically motivated, a highly autonomous form of motivation in which an activity is engaged because of its inherent satisfactions such as for the fun, interest, or the challenge it offers. These four types of motivation have been studied in many domains of activity including education, religion, work, health care, and sport (see Ryan & Deci, 2000b, for reviews). In each of these domains, the four types have been shown to fall along a continuum of autonomy that has been validated both in children and adults (Ryan & Connell, 1989; Vallerand, 1997), with external regulation being the most controlled form of motivation, and intrinsic motivation the most autonomous. Numerous studies have indicated that the more autonomous the person s motivation, the greater his or her persistence, performance, and well-being (Deci & Ryan, 2000). The relative autonomy of motivation has been found to relate to quality of experience and sport attitudes (Pelletier et al., 1995), and with readiness to initiate exercise and enjoyment (Markland, 1999;

3 374 M. GAGNÉ ET AL. Mullan & Markland, 1997). Goudas, Biddle, and Fox (1994) showed that children in physical education (PE) classes with more autonomous motivational styles expressed more interest in physical activities and less ego involvement (Seifriz, Duda, & Chi, 1992). Goudas, Biddle, and Underwood (1995) also showed that undergraduate PE students in a gymnastics course who experienced greater autonomy in class were more likely to evidence intrinsic interest and report intentions to persist. These and other studies (e.g., Vallerand & Losier, 1999) suggest the applicability of SDT to sport and exercise. In the present study we measured each of these motivational styles and related them to the motivational climate created by parents and coaches, and to gymnasts well-being. The Motivational Climate SDT proposes that pressure and control have negative effects on one s adopted motivational style because they thwart the satisfaction of the basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness. These needs are either fulfilled or thwarted through interactions with key people in a given context, and we label the quality of these interactions the motivational climate. A motivational climate will foster autonomous motivation to the extent that it supports satisfaction of these three psychological needs. To foster autonomous motivation, the person(s) who have the power to influence others (e.g., coaches, parents) should be both involved and autonomy supportive (Grolnick, Deci, & Ryan, 1997). Involvement concerns the extent to which adults care for and devote resources to the children (e.g., accompanying them to practice or meets, speaking with the coaches or with the parents, asking the child about what is going on within the activity). Autonomy support concerns the extent to which adults enable and encourage initiative and choice in the athlete, and share in his/her perspective when solving problems or offering advice (Reeve, 1998; Ryan, 1993). The opposite of autonomy support is control, as when coaches or parents are more directive, authoritarian, and pressuring. Thus, involvement is the extent to which an adult partakes in a child s activity, whereas autonomy support qualifies this involvement in terms of the degree to which the adult controls the child s activity, or supports the child s initiative. According to SDT, involvement typically has positive effects, particularly when combined with autonomy-support (Grolnick, Ryan, & Deci, 1991). This means that a controllingly involved coach or parent will likely foster externally regulated or introjected motivation in an athlete. By contrast, an autonomy-supportive coach or parent will likely foster identified regulation and intrinsic motivation (e.g., Chirkov & Ryan, 2001; Grolnick & Ryan, 1989; Ryan & Grolnick, 1986). In two recent longitudinal studies, the motivational orientation and supportiveness of parents and coaches fostered autonomous motivation in adolescents and in competitive swimmers, which in turn increased their persistence in sports (Fortier, 2000; Pelletier, Fortier, Vallerand, & Brière, 2001). Sarrazin, Vallerand, Guillet, Pelletier, and Curry (2002) also found that task-involving climates (contrasted with ego-involving climates) promoted need satisfaction and negatively predicted drop-out in a 21-month longitudinal study of adolescent handballers. Accordingly, we predicted that our young gymnasts would be more autonomously motivated to the extent that they experienced parents and coaches as positively involved and autonomy-supportive rather than controlling. This hypothesis was tested both in terms of individual differences in perceptions, and in terms of daily training experiences. Effects of Climate and Motivation on Well-Being SDT proposes that autonomous self-regulation leads to increased well-being. We define well-being as psychological functioning characterized by positive experiences and an integrated sense of self within the domain of action (Ryan & Deci, 2001). As such, our choice of indicators reflects this definition of well-being, in that they focus on stability of one s self-

4 WELL-BEING OF GYMNASTS 375 concept, feeling an inner energy and spirit (vitality), and being proactive and approach-oriented (PANAS). SDT posits that the fulfillment of the basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness is necessary for well-being to be attained and maintained. When any of these psychological needs is frustrated or blocked, ill being and impoverished engagement is the predicted result. Consistent with this view, Sheldon, Ryan, and Reis (1996) and Reis, Sheldon, Gable, Roscoe, and Ryan (2000) found that both general well-being and day-to-day fluctuations in well-being were directly associated with perceived psychological need satisfaction. In the current study, we tested how variations in experiences of autonomy, competence, and relatedness in gymnastics affected changes in well-being from before to after a practice. We used indices of well-being that we suggest are impacted by the sport experience. One is the athlete s mood, which we assessed using a widely used measure of positive and negative affect developed by Watson, Clark, and Tellegen (1988). Engaging in physical activities has been associated with increased PA but not NA, both within- and between-persons (Clark & Watson, 1988; Watson, 1988). VanLanduyt, Ekkekakis, Panteleimon, Hall, and Petruzzello (2000) found mood improvement for some exerciser participants, and mood deterioration in others. Another focus is subjective vitality (Ryan & Frederick, 1997), or the experience of feeling energetic and alive. Vitality has been shown to positively relate to need fulfillment, and to covary with physical states such as pain and common physical ailments such as colds, flu, and headaches (Ryan & Frederick, 1997). Also, autonomous motivation and need satisfaction have been shown to engender more vitality (Nix, Ryan, Manly, & Deci, 1999; Reis et al., 2000). A third important aspect of well-being concerns self-esteem. Recent research on self-esteem suggests that having stable self-esteem is at least as important as having a high level of selfesteem (Kernis, Cornell, Sun, Berry, & Harlow, 1993). Level and stability have independent effects on positive outcomes, even though they tend to be moderately correlated (Kernis et al., 1993). Consistent with SDT s view that autonomous motivation is associated with more stable self-esteem (Deci & Ryan, 1995), Kernis, Paradise, Whitaker, Wheatman, and Goldman (2000) found that people with more stable self-esteem were more likely to pursue goals for autonomous (identified and intrinsic) relative to controlled (introjected and external) reasons. They also experienced more interest and less tension. Also, children with unstable self-esteem were shown to be less intrinsically motivated (Waschull & Kernis, 1996), and to perceive their father as more controlling and critical (Kernis, Brown, & Brody, 2000). Based on these studies, we expected that gymnasts with higher autonomous motivation and need satisfaction would have higher and more stable self-esteem, PA, and vitality, and lower and more stable NA. We also expected that perceptions of parents and coaches autonomy support would influence level and stability of self-esteem, PA, NA, and vitality similarly. Overview of Study To date, research on sport motivation has mainly used cross-sectional designs that focus on individual differences. However, the ongoing experience of practicing a sport varies from day to day or from practice to practice. On some days, one can feel more confident, energetic, and inspired, whereas on others, one may feel tired, impatient, and insecure. Diary studies (Reis & Gable, 2000) allow one to examine factors that influence this fluctuation. In the present study, we recorded day-to-day motivation to attend practice, experiences of need satisfaction, and well-being. We asked gymnasts to complete short forms before and after each of their practices, for a total of 15 practices that spanned a period of 4 weeks. The data are thus structured across two levels, the first being daily fluctuations of psychological constructs that are nested within gymnasts, who may differ at a second level in terms of experienced support and motivation.

5 376 M. GAGNÉ ET AL. Person-level hypotheses: At the between-person level, analyses were conducted to examine relations between self-reports obtained in the initial questionnaire and aggregated data from the daily reports. Perceptions of autonomy support and involvement from coaches and parents were expected to be related to the adoption of more autonomous forms of motivation and to reports of need satisfaction. Autonomous motivation and need satisfaction, in turn, were expected to have positive effects on PA, self-esteem and vitality, and negative effects on NA. Practice-level hypotheses: We asked gymnasts to report before each practice why they came to their practice that day and to report on their well-being. After each practice, we asked them to report on how their needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness had been satisfied during the practice, and to report on their well-being again. It was expected that incoming motivation would relate to well-being before and after practice, but that need satisfaction during practice would predict of changes in well-being from before to after each practice. We also tested for cross-level interactions, such as the effects of individual differences in overall motivation on the relation between daily motivation and daily need satisfaction with fluctuations in wellbeing. METHOD Participants Forty-five female gymnasts between the ages of 7 to 18 (M = 13 years, SD = 2.35) from a northeastern U.S. competition team agreed (with parents consent) to participate in a diary study on their motivation for gymnastics. The gymnasts were coded on their ability level using a 5 to 9 scale from the classification system of the United States Association of Gymnastics, with level 9 being the highest ability level. The median ability level was 6. The number of years that the gymnasts had been practicing ranged from 1 to 11, with a median of 6, and they reported practicing for an average of 7 hours a week. Procedure All 45 gymnasts completed the initial questionnaire, and 33 completed the diary forms before and after each practice, for a total of 15 practices that spanned 4 weeks during the noncompeting period of the season. Because the club moved to a new location between the administration of the initial questionnaire and the diary study, the remaining 12 gymnasts stopped training at this club and did not complete the diary part. The median number of diaries completed per participant was 13, with a range from 4 to 15. This number of observations is considered to yield moderate power (Kreft & De Leeuw, 1998) and is similar to many other published studies in terms of the ratio of people and days to number of variables studied (e.g, Gable, Reis, & Elliot, 2000; Reis et al., 2000; Sheldon, Ryan, & Reis, 1996). The questionnaires and diaries were administered by trained research assistants who brought completed forms to the laboratory immediately after each collection. All measures were scaled from 1 to 5 to ensure the young gymnasts would use the full scale and wording on some of the items was simplified. The Initial Questionnaire The initial survey was given 1 month prior to the start of the diary study in two group administrations. Gymnasts reported their age, level, years practicing gymnastics, and the number of hours and sessions practiced per week. They also completed the following measures:

6 WELL-BEING OF GYMNASTS 377 Self-regulation for gymnastics. This scale assessed five different motivational regulations for engaging in sport/leisure activities, based on the model set forth by Ryan and Connell (1989) and elaborated in the sport domain by Pelletier et al. (1995). It consisted of 15 statements that answered the question Why do you practice gymnastics? that participants rated using a 1 (completely agree) to 5 (completely disagree) Likert-type scale. Three items assessed intrinsic motivation (e.g., For the pleasure I feel when I practice gymnastics ; α =.73), 3 assessed identified regulation (e.g., It is a good way to get exercise ; α =.63), 3 assessed introjected regulation (e.g., I would feel guilty if I didn t come to practice ; α =.53), 3 assessed external regulation (e.g., I like to win ribbons and trophies at meets ; α =.66), and 3 assessed amotivation (e.g., I m not sure why I still practice gymnastics, I don t seem to be going anywhere with it ; α =.88). Although some of these reliabilities appear to be low, they were calculated with only 3 items, which lowers estimates of reliability. Moreover, these reliabilities are similar to the ones obtained in other research using both children (e.g., Ryan & Connell, 1989; α s ranged from.62.82), and adolescents (e.g., Vallerand, Fortier, & Guay, 1997; α s ranged from.72.87). For some analyses, subscale items were averaged to form an index of relative autonomy, computed from the following formula (Grolnick & Ryan, 1987): 2 (intrinsic motivation) + (identification) (introjection) 2 (external regulation). Children s perception of parents scale. This scale was adapted from Grolnick et al. (1991) and measured children s perceptions of parental autonomy support (6 items, e.g., My parents let me choose what to do when it comes to gymnastics ; α =.69) and involvement (7 items, e.g., My parents don t do as much as they could to support me in gymnastics [reversed] α =.80), on a 1 to 5 Likert-type scale. The scale was also modified to measure perceptions of the coaches autonomy support (α =.79) and involvement (α =.83) by replacing the term parents with coaches. Parent autonomy support and involvement were correlated at.57, and coach autonomy support and involvement were correlated at.79. Attendance. The number of attended practices out of a possible 15 was computed for each gymnast and used as an indicator of behavioral engagement in gymnastics. Measures in the Diary Study Before each practice, gymnasts completed a short form on which they were asked to respond to items in terms of how they felt at this moment, and contained the following scales: Motivation for gymnastics. Six items were adapted from the initial questionnaire to measure gymnasts reasons for coming to practice, rated on a 1 to 5 scale. Answers included 2 external regulation items ( Because I don t want others to get upset with me for not being here and Because I have to be here ), 2 introjection items ( Because I would feel guilty if I didn t come to practice and Because I am hoping to show off my skills today ), 1 identification item ( Because I think it s important to practice to maintain and improve my skills ), and 1 intrinsic motivation item ( Because I feel like I really want to do gymnastics ). A daily motivation index was calculated using the same formula used in the initial questionnaire. Positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). This scale contained 10 items measuring positive affect (e.g., excited; α =.91) and 10 items measuring negative affect (e.g., distressed; α =.91) rated on a 1 (Not at all) to 5 (Extremely) Likert-type scale (Watson et al., 1988). Subscale items were averaged to form daily PA and NA scores. The PANAS has been used and validated with children samples (Melvin & Molloy, 2000; Wilson & Gullone, 1999).

7 378 M. GAGNÉ ET AL. Self-esteem. This 10-item scale assessed the gymnasts attitudes towards themselves (e.g., I feel that I have a number of good qualities ; α =.88; Rosenberg, 1965), using a 1 (Strongly disagree) to 5 (Strongly agree) Likert-type scale. Item scores were averaged to form daily selfesteem scores. Subjective vitality scale. Four items from this 7-item scale (e.g., I feel alive and vital ; α =.87) were used to assess the gymnasts daily subjective feelings of vitality on a 1 (Not at all) to 5 (Extremely) Likert-type scale (Ryan & Frederick, 1997). Items were averaged to form daily vitality scores. After each practice, participants completed a second form containing once again the PANAS, self-esteem, and subjective vitality scales. They also completed the following scale: Need satisfaction scale. This scale assessed the extent to which gymnasts felt that their needs for autonomy (7 items, e.g., My coaches helped me choose my own direction during practice ; α =.87), competence (2 items, e.g., I was good at gymnastics ; r =.39), and relatedness (3 items, e.g., I felt like I was part of the team ; α =.89) were satisfied during their practice, using a 1 (Completely disagree) to 5 (Completely agree) Likert-type scale. Subscale items were averaged to form daily autonomy, competence, and relatedness scores. They were highly correlated at.51,.57, and.83, as they have been in other studies (e.g., Deci et al., 2001). RESULTS Person-Level Analyses At this level of analysis, we were interested in examining relations between perceived autonomy support with the different motivational styles and need satisfaction, and between motivational styles and need satisfaction with well-being outcomes. For this purpose we aggregated the diary data across all sessions for each gymnast on each variable (before and after practice data separately), and also computed the average variation on each post-practice well-being variable over the 4-week period of the study by taking the standard deviation of the daily scores for each gymnast (Kernis et al., 1993). Table 1 presents motivation means for the initial questionnaire and the averaged daily reports. Gymnasts scored highest on identification, followed by intrinsic motivation and by introjection in the initial questionnaire. They scored highest on intrinsic motivation, followed by identification and introjection in the daily self-reports. Correlations between the initial and daily measures ranged from.22 to.50, indicating that situational motivation (i.e., practice level) is somewhat related to contextual motivation, but may also be affected by situational influences, as postulated in Vallerand s (1997) hierarchical model of motivation. Correlations between age and motivation revealed that older gymnasts reported being less introjected in the initial questionnaire, r =.33, p <.05, and reported being more identified on a daily basis than younger gymnasts, r =.44, p <.01. Age was unrelated to attendance level. Identified gymnasts also practiced more times per week, r =.32, p <.05. Despite these relations, age and ability level did not moderate the results reported below, and are not further discussed. Correlations were computed between parent and coach autonomy support and involvement with initial and daily motivation scores (see Table 1). These revealed that perceived parent autonomy support was positively related to identified (initial and daily) and intrinsic motiva-

8 WELL-BEING OF GYMNASTS 379 Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations (SD) of Initial/Daily Motivation Variables and Daily Need Satisfaction, and Their Correlations with Perceptions of Parents and Coaches from the Initial Questionnaire (n = 45/33). Mean (SD) Parent autonomy support Parent involvement Coach autonomy support Coach involvement Amotivation 1.80 (.99) / 1.71 (.97).15 / /.13.34* /.03.42** /.07 External regulation 1.80 (.96) / 2.21 (1.15).01 /.26.37** / / /.17 Introjection 3.18 (.88) / 2.65 (1.07).07 / / / /.06 Identification 4.53 (.63) / 4.23 (.88).27+ /.38*.21 /.18.37** / *** /.32 Intrinsic motivation 3.93 (.73) / 4.44 (.52).31* /.13.31* / /.11.34* /.08 R. A. I (2.61) / 6.03 (3.22).26+ /.43**.14 / / /.08 Autonomy 3.83 (1.03).23.37*.54***.60*** Competence 3.73 (.57) * Relatedness 4.08 (.92).37*.35*.42**.50** Note: R. A. I. = Relative autonomy index. + p <.10, * p <.05, ** p <.01, *** p <

9 380 M. GAGNÉ ET AL. tion (initial) motivation, while coach autonomy support was associated with higher identified motivation. Involvement was only related to initial motivation measures. Perceived parent involvement was positively related both to autonomous and controlled forms of motivation, whereas perceived coach involvement was positively related to identified and intrinsic motivation and negatively related to amotivation. It is also worth noting that perceived parent involvement was positively related to the number of times gymnasts practiced during the week, r =.41, p <.01, as was coach autonomy support, r =.29, p <.05. Table 1 also displays the correlations between perceived parent and coach autonomy support and involvement with need satisfaction during practices. Expectably, parent autonomy support and involvement were not strongly related to need satisfaction during practice, but coach autonomy support and involvement were. Thus, coaches may possibly have greater influence on gymnasts practice motivation and well-being than parents. Parent involvement was perceived as relatively less autonomy supportive than the involvement of coaches, as indicated in (a) the correlation between parent autonomy support and involvement of.57, which is not as strong as the correlation of.79 between coach autonomy support and involvement; (b) its similar correlations of parent with both controlled and autonomous forms of motivation; and (c) its low correlation with satisfaction of the need for autonomy. This could mean that at least some of the parents in our sample were perceived as involved in a pressuring way, whereas coaches were perceived as involved in an autonomy supportive way. Table 2 presents correlations between the motivational styles measured in the initial and daily questionnaires and the averaged daily ratings of well-being. Overall, these correlations reveal a pattern where positive well-being outcomes were more positively correlated with autonomous motivational styles, and more negatively correlated with controlling motivational styles, whereas the opposite pattern was true for negative well-being outcomes. Specifically, intrinsic motivation was positively related with average pre- and post-practice PA, whereas controlled forms of motivation (introjected and external) and amotivation were positively related to average pre- and post-practice NA. In light of the ample evidence for separate affective systems for PA and NA, these results are interesting. It appears that PA is associated with more autonomous forms of motivation, whereas NA is associated with more controlled forms of motivation. Instability of PA tended to be positively correlated with external regulation from the initial questionnaire, and amotivation from the diaries. In contrast, instability of NA was only negatively correlated with intrinsic motivation from the diaries. Mean vitality ratings were positively related to intrinsic motivation and negatively to external regulation and amotivation. Instability of vitality was positively associated with external regulation from the initial questionnaire, and with amotivation from the diaries. Means for self-esteem pre- and post-practice were negatively associated with controlling motivational styles both from the initial questionnaire and from the diaries, and were positively associated with intrinsic motivation from the diaries. In contrast, unstable self-esteem was positively associated with introjection and external regulation from the initial questionnaire, and amotivation from the diaries. This supports Deci and Ryan s (1995) hypotheses that being motivated in a controlling manner is associated with more unstable self-esteem. Finally, number of attended practices did not correlate with motivation from the initial questionnaire, but was positively correlated with daily intrinsic motivation, r =.37, p <.05. To prospectively examine the influence of daily motivation on change in daily well-being, we conducted partial correlations between aggregates of daily motivation and aggregates of well-being variables measured post-practice, controlling for aggregates of well-being variables measured pre-practice. Even though these analyses do not meet all criteria for causality, they can give an indication of the likelihood that motivational styles affected patterns of change in well-being during practice. Very few correlations were significant. Intrinsic motivation was

10 WELL-BEING OF GYMNASTS 381 Table 2 Correlations between Initial/Daily Motivation and the Means and Variability of Daily Outcomes (n = 33) Amotivation External Introjected Identified Intrinsic R. A. I. regulation regulation regulation motivation Mean PA B. P..28 / / / /.40*.35* /.68***.29+ /.36* Mean PA A. P..21 /.34*.22 / / / /.54**.41* /.26 SD PA A. P..00 /.33*.36* / / / / /.11 Mean NA B. P..38* /.62***.40* /.33*.44** /.38*.11 / / * /.45* Mean NA A. P..20 /.24.45** / / / /.39*.36* /.37* SD NA A. P..27 / / / / /.34*.12 /.22 Mean vitality B. P..51** /.41*.18 / / / /.67***.30+ /.37* Mean vitality A. P..45** /.49**.41* / / / /.51***.49** /.38* SD vitality A. P..19 /.35*.50** / / / /.12.58*** /.31+ Mean self-esteem B. P..54** /.48**.11 /.24.35* / / /.64***.29+ /.45** Mean self-esteem A. P..56** /.55**.22 / * / / /.63***.42* /.49** SD self-esteem A. P..01 /.36*.37* / / / /.17.54*** /.28+ Note: R. A. I. = Relative Autonomy Index, B. P. = Before Practice, A. P. = After Practice, SD = Standard Deviation. + p <.10, * p <.05, ** p <.01, *** p <

11 382 M. GAGNÉ ET AL. negatively correlated with post-practice NA, pr =.34, p <.05, and amotivation was negatively correlated with post-practice self-esteem, pr =.35, p <.05. We repeated these analyses correlating well-being variables with parent and coach autonomy support and involvement, and with aggregates of need satisfaction during practice. None of the correlations were significant. Practice-Level Analyses Having examined the between-persons findings, some questions arise. Even though we know that levels of affect, vitality, and esteem averaged across days were related to average levels of motivation coming into the practice, between-persons analyses tell us nothing of whether they fluctuated together during practices. Because it is also not appropriate to conduct regression analyses on aggregated data with so few cases, nor is it appropriate to conduct regression analyses using the daily measures as cases because they are non-independent observations, we carried out a series of hierarchical linear models to examine the links between need satisfaction, motivation, and well-being at the daily level. Using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM; Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992) with maximum likelihood estimation, we analyzed if daily well-being varied as a function of daily experiences of motivation and need satisfaction. HLM treats person-level effects as random by using regressions at two levels simultaneously to calculate error terms. A significant random effect indicates that the individual intercepts and slopes are not homogeneous, and that some individual difference variable may be able to explain the fluctuation in slopes across people. To test this, we examine cross-level interactions, or the influence of between-subject effects (e.g., perceived coach autonomy support) on, for example, within-subject slopes between need satisfaction and well-being. Practice-level well-being was estimated using the following equation: WB ij = β 0j + β 1j X ij + e ij (1) where β 0 refers to person (j) s well-being during an average practice; β 1 represents a maximum likelihood estimate of person (j) s slope estimating well-being from daily motivation and need satisfaction variables; X ij represents the motivation or need satisfaction value on each day (i) for each person (j); and e ij is residual error. Person-level effects were estimated using the following equations: β 0j = γ 00 + γ 01 W j + u 0j (2) β 1j = γ 10 + γ 11 W j + u 1j (3) where γ 00 refers to the sample s mean well-being across practices; γ 01 represents the average variation in well-being means derived from a variable measured at the trait level, such as perceived coach involvement; γ 10 represents the average well-being slope across the sample estimated from daily motivation or need satisfaction [1]; γ 11 represents the average variation of the well-being slopes estimated from a variable measured at the trait level, such as perceived parental autonomy support; W j represents the value of a trait measured in the initial questionnaire for person (j); and u 0j and u 1j represent the average error. When the random effect (i.e., u ) was significant, thus indicating heterogeneous slopes or intercepts across gymnasts, moderator variables, like perceived parent autonomy support, were added into the second level equations to examine if they could explain the variance. Note that when we estimated the effects of need satisfaction on well-being after practice, controlling for well-being before practice, the parameters γ 10 and γ 11 indicate pre-practice well-being intercepts and slopes, while new parameters

12 WELL-BEING OF GYMNASTS 383 are added for predictor variables intercepts (γ 20 ) and slopes (γ 21 ). Our analyses were done using HLM 5 (Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong, & Congdon, 2000) with the predictors centered around their respective person-level mean in order to observe how unit changes in the predictors were related to changes in outcomes for each individual. First, we tested whether incoming motivation would relate to well-being measured before practice. Analyses were done by regressing the outcome variables measured before each practice separately on each motivational style and relative autonomous motivation. Results, presented in Table 3, indicate that unit increases in relative autonomous motivation marginally led to a.04 unit increase in PA, and significantly led to a.04 unit decrease in NA, a.05 unit increase in vitality, and a.04 increase in daily self-esteem. Most of the estimated intercepts and the slopes were heterogeneous (i.e., u 0 and u 1 were significant), but none of the variables assessed in the initial questionnaire significantly moderated these effects. Analyses with intrinsic and identified motivation as predictors also yielded significant results. Unit increases in intrinsic motivation led to a.26 increase in PA, a.10 decrease in NA, a.34 increase in vitality, and a.13 increase in selfesteem. Unit increases in identified motivation in turn led to.18 increases in PA and vitality, and a marginally significant.09 increase in self-esteem. Again, most of the estimated intercepts and slopes were heterogeneous, but none of the measures in the initial questionnaire could explain the variability. The other motivational styles were not related to changes in well-being, meaning that, overall, autonomous motivational styles were shown to positively relate to wellbeing on a daily basis, and it appears that intrinsic motivation was a better predictor than identification. 1 Next, analyses were conducted to examine whether incoming motivation would relate to changes in well-being outcomes from before to after practice. Pre-practice levels of well-being were entered into the equation to control for initial level when predicting post-practice wellbeing. These analyses all yielded non-significant results. Since incoming motivation did not influence changes in well-being, something during practice might have affected changes in well-being. So we conducted analyses to examine the effects of need satisfaction during the practice on changes pre- to post-practice. Each of the needs was entered in separate equations to avoid multicollinearity problems because they were highly correlated with one another, and results are presented in Table 3. Increases in daily need satisfaction predicted increases in PA, vitality, and self-esteem, whereas NA was in no way influenced by daily need satisfaction. More specifically, unit increases in perceived autonomy support from coaches predicted a.11 unit increase in PA, a.25 unit increase in vitality, and a.12 unit increase in self-esteem. Unit increases in feelings of competence predicted a.24 unit increase in PA, a.24 increase in vitality, and a.11 increase in self-esteem. Finally, unit increases in relatedness to peers predicted a.19 unit increase in PA, a.34 unit increase in vitality, and a.18 unit increase in self-esteem. All of the intercepts and slopes were again heterogeneous, but moderating effects were only found for change in self-esteem. Coach involvement as rated by the gymnasts in the initial questionnaire moderated the slope between pre-practice and post-practice self-esteem (i.e., variations in β 1, or γ 11 ), and this effect was homogeneous across gymnasts; the self-esteem slope was flatter for gymnasts who reported high coach involvement (1 standard deviation above the 1. Additional analyses were conducted on each well-being measure, one set entering both intrinsic and identified motivation as predictors (r =.21), and a second set entering intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (r =.18). In the first set, intrinsic motivation yielded a bigger effect than identification for PA, selfesteem, and vitality, while neither significantly predicted NA. In the second set, intrinsic motivation yielded a bigger effect than extrinsic motivation for PA, self-esteem, and vitality, while neither significantly predicted NA.

13 384 M. GAGNÉ ET AL. Table 3 Unstandardized (group centered) Results from the HLM Analyses on Well-being Before Practice and Change in Well-being (n = 33) Positive affect Negative affect Vitality Self-esteem Intercept γ *** 1.82*** 4.07*** 4.19*** R. A. I. Slope γ **.05*.04** Intrinsic motivation γ 10.26***.10*.34***.13** Identified regulation γ 10.18**.03.18**.09+ Intercept γ *** 1.71*** 3.82*** 4.15*** Autonomy a γ 10.15**.15*.11*.18** γ 20.11*.04.25**.12* Competence γ 10.14**.14*.14**.20*** γ 20.24***.04.24***.11** Relatedness γ 10.16**.14*.12*.18** γ 20.19**.05.34***.18*** Note: R. A. I. = Relative Autonomy Index. a γ 10 = Slope of outcome before practice, γ 20 = Slope of need satisfaction after practice. + p <.10, * p <.05, ** p <.01, *** p <.001 grand mean) than for gymnasts who reported lower coach involvement (1 standard deviation below the grand mean). However, coach involvement did not influence the relation between need satisfaction and self-esteem (i.e., γ 21 was ns). Specifically, when need for autonomy was entered, self-esteem changed by.21 points across the sample, and coach involvement had an effect of γ 11 =.17, p <.05, which means that for gymnasts who reported lower coach involvement, self-esteem changed by.38 points compared to.04 for gymnasts who reported higher coach involvement. When need for competence was entered, self-esteem changed on average by.22 points, and coach involvement had an effect of γ 11 =.14, p <.05, which means that for gymnasts who reported lower coach involvement, self-esteem changed by.36 points compared to.08 for gymnasts who reported higher coach involvement. When need for relatedness was entered, average change in self-esteem was.20, and coach involvement had an effect of γ 11 =.17, p <.05, which means that for gymnasts who reported lower coach involvement, selfesteem changed by.37 points compared to.03 points for gymnasts who reported higher coach involvement. DISCUSSION The goal of this study was to examine the effects of perceived parent and coach autonomy support on the motivation and well-being of gymnasts. Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000) proposes that the satisfaction of the needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness is essential to sustained and healthy motivation, and that these needs are most likely to be fulfilled under contexts that support people s need for autonomy. Young gymnasts participated in a questionnaire study, and a subset of them participated in a diary study examining perceptions of their parents and coaches autonomy support, their motivation, need satisfaction, and well-being. Diary studies have the advantage of gathering data at a time close to when people experience targeted events and are therefore less subject to memory biases. Having multiple records of a person s experiences also has the advantage of increas-

14 WELL-BEING OF GYMNASTS 385 ing greatly the power of statistical analyses, although one needs to control for person-related and time-related sources of variance. HLM can handle these issues by allowing to simultaneously test within-person and between-persons effects. Disadvantages of this technique include the increased time and operating costs, and the difficulty of getting long-term commitment from research participants. The results of this study nicely demonstrate the value of this methodology. When aggregating daily-level data, we found that perceptions of parent and coach autonomy support and involvement influenced the quality of the gymnasts motivation. The more autonomy supportive and involved parents and coaches were perceived to be by the gymnasts, the more autonomously motivated the gymnasts were. We were also interested in the question of whether daily motivation and need satisfaction would influence changes in well-being during practice. Partial correlations between well-being variables and aggregates of daily motivation and need satisfaction revealed no significant patterns of influence. Thus, the conclusions one would draw from such between-persons analyses on aggregated data would be that incoming motivation and need satisfaction during practice do not have a strong influence on changes in well-being. However, using HLM made it possible to examine the day-to-day influence of such variables on changes in well-being during practice. In the present study, it allowed us to find that incoming motivation only influenced well-being reported before practice, but that need satisfaction during practice had an overriding effect on change in well-being from before to after the practice. Perceived parent and coach autonomy support and involvement measured in the initial questionnaire had no moderating effects on such relations. Only in one case did we find that coach involvement (which was perceived as fairly non-controlling as attested by its high correlation with autonomy support) influenced the extent to which self-esteem fluctuated during the course of a practice. Gymnasts who perceived that coaches were highly involved in their training had more stable self-esteem than those who perceived coaches as uninvolved. Person-level analyses revealed that perceived parent and coach autonomy support had an influence on the gymnasts adoption of more autonomous forms of motivation. Interestingly, parent involvement also increased the likelihood of adopting more controlled forms of motivation, whereas coach involvement did not. It appeared that coaches involvement was perceived by the athletes as being less pressuring than the athletes own parents involvement. However, these results were not replicated when analyses were done at the daily level. Perceived parent and coach involvement and autonomy support did not moderate the effects of daily need satisfaction on improvements in well-being. Only in the case of self-esteem did we find that coach involvement had a positive effect on the stability of gymnasts self-esteem. Our results mainly focused on examining the effects of perceived autonomy support and need satisfaction on athletes well-being. One additional result showed that parent involvement and autonomy support, and autonomous motivation, had effects on attendance to practice. This result offers interesting support to previous research that has shown that autonomous regulation not only influences the quality of one s experience of sport participation, but also behavioral involvement (Pelletier et al., 2001; Sarrazin et al., 2002). On the theoretical side, the results of this study support self-determination theory s proposal that autonomy support serves to satisfy psychological needs that are necessary for sustained autonomous motivation. We found this effect at two levels. First, perceived parent and coach autonomy support was associated with more autonomous motivation toward gymnastics. Second, daily need satisfaction during practice led to increased well-being. Studies have found similar results in other domains, such as education (Vallerand et al., 1997), organizations (Deci et al., 2001), and behavioral health (Turner, Irwin, Tschann, & Millstein, 1993). The present study was limited by some design factors. First, our participants were children and adolescent females. The age range was wide, which raises the issue of whether age would

15 386 M. GAGNÉ ET AL. moderate the effects reported above, but we found no such effects. Butler (1989) similarly reported that children as young as first graders easily lose their intrinsic motivation in competitive contexts. Although we can not speculate about the generalizability of our results to different age groups and to male athletes, we must note that the literature reviewed herein includes many studies that used children and adults of all ages, of both genders, of different ability levels, and in different sports, with congruent findings. Moreover, research on the role of gender in sports reveals that gender does not have a unilateral effect on motivation; the effect is sport and context specific (Gill, 1999). Nonetheless, future studies would need to replicate the present results in older populations, with male athletes, and with larger samples. Second, the measure of need satisfaction led to problems of multicollinearity that precluded teasing apart each need s contribution to explaining the variance in each outcome variable. Although it is possible to separate the effects of the needs in more controlled settings (e.g., Goudas, Biddle, & Underwood, 1995; Oppenheimer, Stet, & Versteeg, 1986) and to look at the individual effects of each need (e.g., Deci, Hodges, Pierson, & Tomassone, 1992; Gagné, Senécal, & Koestner, 1997), they tend to function in unison in natural settings (e.g., Baard, Deci, & Ryan, 2000; Deci et al., 2001; Gagné, 2003). Future studies may focus more on the individual contributions of each of these needs. Third, we assessed gymnasts perceptions of their parents and coaches autonomy support. Future studies should obtain reports from multiple sources to help establish construct validation. It would also be useful to manipulate coaching styles in future studies, something that has been done in other domains. For example, Williams, Gagné, Ryan, and Deci (2002) trained physicians to be autonomy supportive with patients and found that patients became more autonomously motivated to abstain from smoking. Similarly, Deci, Connell, and Ryan (1989) trained managers to be more autonomy supportive with employees and found that employees in turn became more satisfied at work and more trustful of the organization. Thus, an intervention study could be designed to train coaches to be autonomy supportive toward their athletes and then look at effects on motivation, well-being, and performance. Related to this issue is the question of pressures experienced by the coaches to have high performing athletes. Pelletier, Séguin-Lévesque, and Legault (2002) found that school teachers who experience pressure to comply with a curriculum and live up to performance standards become less autonomy supportive with students. Examination of the context for coaches may thus contribute to our understanding of how and why coaches adopt autonomy-supportive or controlling styles. The implications of this study to the training of young athletes are clear. The way training is carried out has an influence on the well-being of athletes, and perhaps on their participation. Training contexts where coaches support the autonomy of athletes by listening to their concerns and affording them some choice, where athletes feel well connected to teammates, and where they receive some positive competence feedback, are likely to help athletes experience sustained positive emotions, be more energized, and have higher and more stable self-esteem. Using an autonomy supportive style to coach athletes may have direct benefits on how the athlete feels in terms of competence and autonomy, but also this style may influence the climate of the training environment, including how athletes interact with one another. That is, athletes may feel not only that they are talented and can make informed choices, but that they are also part of a cohesive group of like-minded people. We also suggest that when athletes train for autonomous reasons and when their needs are supported by parents and coaches during training, they might train in a manner that will decrease the risk for injury and burnout. Longitudinal studies could allow the testing of this hypothesis. In the meantime, the present diary study supports the importance of providing supports for athletes psychological needs as an integral part of training, and offers an interestingly rich set of new ideas that can be further explored through research.

Human Motivation and Emotion

Human Motivation and Emotion Human Motivation and Emotion 46-332-01 Dr. Fuschia Sirois Lecture 7 Sept. 28, 2006 Lecture 8 Oct. 3, 2006 Types of Motivation INTRINSIC strive inwardly to be competent and self-determining in their quest

More information

Running head: PREDICTING PARTICIPATION IN OPTIONAL PHYSICAL. A Prospective Study of Participation in Optional School Physical Education Using a

Running head: PREDICTING PARTICIPATION IN OPTIONAL PHYSICAL. A Prospective Study of Participation in Optional School Physical Education Using a Running head: PREDICTING PARTICIPATION IN OPTIONAL PHYSICAL EDUCATION A Prospective Study of Participation in Optional School Physical Education Using a Self-Determination Theory Framework Address for

More information

that fosters self-determined motivation among athletes. Coaching style influences athlete

that fosters self-determined motivation among athletes. Coaching style influences athlete BURRELL, CHELSEA J., M.S. The Relationship of Perceived Autonomy-Supportive Coaching Behavior with Motivation Among High School Athletes. (2016) Directed by Dr. Diane Gill. 94 pp. Recent literature suggests

More information

Ian M. Taylor, 1 Nikos Ntoumanis, 2 Martyn Standage, 3 and Christopher M. Spray 1

Ian M. Taylor, 1 Nikos Ntoumanis, 2 Martyn Standage, 3 and Christopher M. Spray 1 Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 010, 3, 99-10 010 Human Kinetics, Inc. Motivational Predictors of Physical Education Students Effort, Exercise Intentions, and Leisure-Time Physical Activity: A

More information

Self-determination theory and work motivation

Self-determination theory and work motivation Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 331-362 (2005) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/job.322 Self-determination theory and work motivation

More information

Testing a self-determination theory-based teaching style intervention in the exercise domain

Testing a self-determination theory-based teaching style intervention in the exercise domain European Journal of Social Psychology Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 375 388 (2008) Published online 18 September 2007 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).463 Testing a self-determination theory-based

More information

The Dynamic Processes of Influence Between Contextual and Situational Motivation: A Test of the Hierarchical Model in a Science Education Setting 1

The Dynamic Processes of Influence Between Contextual and Situational Motivation: A Test of the Hierarchical Model in a Science Education Setting 1 The Dynamic Processes of Influence Between Contextual and Situational Motivation: A Test of the Hierarchical Model in a Science Education Setting 1 Geneviève L. Lavigne and Robert J. Vallerand 2 Laboratoire

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

MOTIVATION OF FEMALE SECONDARY STUDENTS TOWARDS PARTICIPATION IN PHYSICAL ACTIVITIES IN HONG KONG

MOTIVATION OF FEMALE SECONDARY STUDENTS TOWARDS PARTICIPATION IN PHYSICAL ACTIVITIES IN HONG KONG physical education / Tjelesna i zdravstvena kultura Eric C.K. Tsang Department of Health and Physical Education The Hong Kong Institute of Education, Hong Kong Original scientific paper MOTIVATION OF FEMALE

More information

Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2

Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2 Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2 Gherdai Hassel a, Jeffrey John Milroy a, and Muhsin Michael Orsini a Adolescents who engage in regular physical

More information

A Test of Self-Determination Theory in the Exercise Domain

A Test of Self-Determination Theory in the Exercise Domain A Test of Self-Determination Theory in the Exercise Domain JEMMA EDMUNDS 1 Coventry University United Kingdom NIKOS NTOUMANIS AND JOAN L. DUDA University of Birmingham United Kingdom In accordance with

More information

THE EFFECTS OF WIN:LOSS [W:L] ON INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATION AND PERFORMANCE

THE EFFECTS OF WIN:LOSS [W:L] ON INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATION AND PERFORMANCE THE EFFECTS OF WIN:LOSS [W:L] ON INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATION AND PERFORMANCE Presented By: Stephanie P. Bailey SUNY Old Westbury Faculty Mentor: Lorenz S. Neuwirth, Ph.D. Discussion Points Background Overview

More information

Creating a Positive Motivational Climate: Strategies for Division I Women s Tennis Coaches

Creating a Positive Motivational Climate: Strategies for Division I Women s Tennis Coaches UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones Spring 2012 Creating a Positive Motivational Climate: Strategies for Division I Women s Tennis Coaches Mark Roberts University of Nevada,

More information

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation in Sport and Physical Activity A Review and a Look at the Future

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation in Sport and Physical Activity A Review and a Look at the Future CHAPTER 3 Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation in Sport and Physical Activity A Review and a Look at the Future ROBERT J. VALLERAND Motivation has been repeatedly reported as a key element of athletes success

More information

Psychology of Sport and Exercise

Psychology of Sport and Exercise Psychology of Sport and Exercise 13 (2012) 51e59 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Psychology of Sport and Exercise journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport Perceived coach-autonomy

More information

Testing Self-Determined Motivation as a Mediator of the Relationship Between Psychological Needs and Affective and Behavioral Outcomes

Testing Self-Determined Motivation as a Mediator of the Relationship Between Psychological Needs and Affective and Behavioral Outcomes Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 2007, 29, 645-663 2007 Human Kinetics, Inc. Testing Self-Determined Motivation as a Mediator of the Relationship Between Psychological Needs and Affective and Behavioral

More information

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155 24 Motivation Internalized Motivation in the Classroom Kennon M. Sheldon The motivation that students bring to a classroom setting is critical in determining how much, and how well, they learn. This activity

More information

Self Determination Theory. Overview

Self Determination Theory. Overview Self Determination Theory Bron: http://www.selfdeterminationtheory.org Overview People are centrally concerned with motivation -- how to move themselves or others to act. Everywhere, parents, teachers,

More information

Coaching Climates and the Destructive Effects of Mastery- Avoidance Achievement Goals on Situational Motivation

Coaching Climates and the Destructive Effects of Mastery- Avoidance Achievement Goals on Situational Motivation JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2006, 28, 69-92 2006 Human Kinetics, Inc. Coaching Climates and the Destructive Effects of Mastery- Avoidance Achievement Goals on Situational Motivation David E.

More information

EXPLORING MOTIVATIONAL STATES IN AN INTERNATIONAL SAMPLE OF COMPETITIVE SPRINGBOARD AND PLATFORM DIVERS Saara Haapanen

EXPLORING MOTIVATIONAL STATES IN AN INTERNATIONAL SAMPLE OF COMPETITIVE SPRINGBOARD AND PLATFORM DIVERS Saara Haapanen EXPLORING MOTIVATIONAL STATES IN AN INTERNATIONAL SAMPLE OF COMPETITIVE SPRINGBOARD AND PLATFORM DIVERS Saara Haapanen Master s Thesis in Sport and Exercise Psychology Autumn 2009 Department of Sport Sciences

More information

Does College Environment Have the Same Effect on All Students?

Does College Environment Have the Same Effect on All Students? Does College Environment Have the Same Effect on All Students? Relationship among High School Engagement, College Environment, and First-year Engagement Yiran Dong James S. Cole Indiana University Bloomington

More information

The Why and How of Goal Pursuits: Effects of Global Autonomous Motivation and. Perceived Control on Emotional Well-being. E.

The Why and How of Goal Pursuits: Effects of Global Autonomous Motivation and. Perceived Control on Emotional Well-being. E. The Why and How of Goal Pursuits: Effects of Global Autonomous Motivation and Perceived Control on Emotional Well-being E. Gaëlle Hortop A Thesis in the Department of Psychology Presented in Partial Fulfillment

More information

Basic Needs and Well-Being: A Self-Determination Theory View

Basic Needs and Well-Being: A Self-Determination Theory View Suggested APA style reference: Lynch, M. (2010). Basic needs and well-being: A self-determination theory view. Retrieved from http://counselingoutfitters.com/vistas/vistas10/article_70.pdf Article 70 Basic

More information

Motivating children in and out of school: Research findings and practical implications

Motivating children in and out of school: Research findings and practical implications Motivating children in and out of school: Research findings and practical implications The Question Does promoting motivation toward activities in school lead to increased motivation toward activities

More information

Internalized Motivation in the Classroom

Internalized Motivation in the Classroom Internalized Motivation in the Classroom Motivation Exercise 20-30 min. The motivation that students bring to a classroom setting is critical in determining how much, and how well, they learn. This activity

More information

How to Become a Persevering Exerciser? Providing a Clear, Future Intrinsic Goal in an Autonomy-Supportive Way

How to Become a Persevering Exerciser? Providing a Clear, Future Intrinsic Goal in an Autonomy-Supportive Way JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2004, 26, 232-249 2004 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. How to Become a Persevering Exerciser? Providing a Clear, Future Intrinsic Goal in an Autonomy-Supportive

More information

Facilitating Motivation, Performance and Wellbeing

Facilitating Motivation, Performance and Wellbeing Facilitating Motivation, Performance and Wellbeing Research and Interventions Using Self-Determination Theory Richard M. Ryan Professor of Psychology, Psychiatry and Education Director of Clinical Training

More information

Perceived motivational climate, need satisfaction and indices of well-being in team sports: A longitudinal perspective Reinboth, Michael; Duda, Joan

Perceived motivational climate, need satisfaction and indices of well-being in team sports: A longitudinal perspective Reinboth, Michael; Duda, Joan Perceived motivational climate, need satisfaction and indices of well-being in team sports: A longitudinal perspective Reinboth, Michael; Duda, Joan DOI: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2005.06.002 Document Version

More information

The Reasons And Motivation For Pre-Service Teachers Choosing To Specialise In Primary Physical Education Teacher Education

The Reasons And Motivation For Pre-Service Teachers Choosing To Specialise In Primary Physical Education Teacher Education Australian Journal of Teacher Education Volume 39 Issue 5 Article 1 2014 The Reasons And Motivation For Pre-Service Teachers Choosing To Specialise In Primary Physical Education Teacher Education Sharna

More information

THE SELF-CONCORDANCE MODEL: THE EFFECTS OF AUTONOMY, EFFORT AND GOAL PROGRESS ON SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING IN THE US AND RUSSIA

THE SELF-CONCORDANCE MODEL: THE EFFECTS OF AUTONOMY, EFFORT AND GOAL PROGRESS ON SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING IN THE US AND RUSSIA Dmitry D. Suchkov THE SELF-CONCORDANCE MODEL: THE EFFECTS OF AUTONOMY, EFFORT AND GOAL PROGRESS ON SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING IN THE US AND RUSSIA BASIC RESEARCH PROGRAM WORKING PAPERS SERIES: PSYCHOLOGY WP

More information

How to Move On and Keep Moving : Preparing Student-Athletes for Life after College Sports

How to Move On and Keep Moving : Preparing Student-Athletes for Life after College Sports How to Move On and Keep Moving : Preparing Student-Athletes for Life after College Sports Erin Reifsteck, Ph.D. Dr. Erin Reifsteck is a post-doctoral research fellow with IPAHW. She received her Ph.D.

More information

Sports Science & Coaching Volume 4 Number

Sports Science & Coaching Volume 4 Number An Intra-Individual Analysis of Players Perceived Coaching Behaviours, Psychological Needs, and Achievement Goals by C.K. John Wang, Koon Teck Koh and Nikos Chatzisarantis Reprinted from International

More information

Motivation Motivation

Motivation Motivation This should be easy win What am I doing here! Motivation Motivation What Is Motivation? Motivation is the direction and intensity of effort. Direction of effort: Whether an individual seeks out, approaches,

More information

Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs

Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2014 Image source 1 Reeve (2009, pp. 142-143) Psychological need An inherent source

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2014 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 when people find

More information

FINAL REPORT RBT1507. Contents. Dr Charlotte Kerner and Dr Victoria Goodyear

FINAL REPORT RBT1507. Contents. Dr Charlotte Kerner and Dr Victoria Goodyear FINAL REPORT RBT1507 The use of electronic lifestyle activity monitors with adolescents: Implications for motivation, need satisfaction and integration into physical education Dr Charlotte Kerner and Dr

More information

Predicting Self-determined Motivation of Elite Female Volleyball Players from Leadership Styles of Coaches in Iran

Predicting Self-determined Motivation of Elite Female Volleyball Players from Leadership Styles of Coaches in Iran ISSN 1750-9823 (print) International Journal of Sports Science and Engineering Vol. 05 (2011) No. 04, pp. 219-224 Predicting Self-determined Motivation of Elite Female Volleyball Players from Leadership

More information

Self-determined work motivation predicts job outcomes, but what predicts self-determined work motivation?

Self-determined work motivation predicts job outcomes, but what predicts self-determined work motivation? Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1109 1115 Brief Report Self-determined work motivation predicts job outcomes, but what predicts self-determined work

More information

C H A P T E R. Leadership. Chapter 9: Leadership

C H A P T E R. Leadership. Chapter 9: Leadership C H A P T E R 9 Leadership Chapter 9: Leadership A boss creates fear, a leader confidence. A boss fixes blame, a leader corrects mistakes. A boss knows all, a leader asks questions. A boss makes work

More information

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use ACM, forthcoming. This is the author s version of the work. It is posted here by permission of ACM for your personal use. Not for redistribution. The definitive version will be published soon. Citation:

More information

From Environmental Factors to Outcomes: A Test of an Integrated Motivational Sequence 1

From Environmental Factors to Outcomes: A Test of an Integrated Motivational Sequence 1 Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 28, No. 4, December 2004 ( C 2004) DOI: 10.1007/s11031-004-2387-z From Environmental Factors to Outcomes: A Test of an Integrated Motivational Sequence 1 Frederick M. E. Grouzet,

More information

Motivation as an independent and a dependent variable in medical education

Motivation as an independent and a dependent variable in medical education R. A. KUSURKAR, TH. J. TEN CATE, M. VAN ASPEREN& G. CROISET University Medical Center Utrecht, The Netherlands, Motivation as an independent and a dependent variable in medical education Medical Teacher

More information

Self-determination Theory as a Grand Theory of Motivation in EFL Classroom

Self-determination Theory as a Grand Theory of Motivation in EFL Classroom Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research Volume 4, Issue 6, 2017, pp. 153-164 Available online at www.jallr.com ISSN: 2376-760X Self-determination Theory as a Grand Theory of Motivation in

More information

Identify and discuss the gaps in conventional wisdom around motivation. Discuss self-determination theory and our basic psychological needs

Identify and discuss the gaps in conventional wisdom around motivation. Discuss self-determination theory and our basic psychological needs MOTIVATION SCIENCE THEORY AND APPLICATION LEARNING OBJECTIVES Identify and discuss the gaps in conventional wisdom around motivation Discuss self-determination theory and our basic psychological needs

More information

PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE?

PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE? Submitted: February 25, 2017 Revision: March 30, 2017 Accepted: April 27, 2017 PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE? Masataka Takebe Koji Murata Graduate

More information

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1060 1066 Brief Report Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2013 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 3 Psychological

More information

Title: Autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the optimal functioning of adult male and female sport participants: A test of basic needs theory

Title: Autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the optimal functioning of adult male and female sport participants: A test of basic needs theory Editorial Manager(tm) for Motivation and Emotion Manuscript Draft Manuscript Number: MOEM1R Title: Autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the optimal functioning of adult male and female sport participants:

More information

Effects of an intervention based on self-determination theory on self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation

Effects of an intervention based on self-determination theory on self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation Psychology and Health Vol. 24, No. 1, January 2009, 29 48 Effects of an intervention based on self-determination theory on self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation Nikos L.D. Chatzisarantis

More information

FURTHER VALIDATION OF THE MOTIVATION TOWARD THE ENVIRONMENT SCALE

FURTHER VALIDATION OF THE MOTIVATION TOWARD THE ENVIRONMENT SCALE 10.1177/0013916502250753 ENVIRONMENT Villacorta ARTICLE et al. / VALIDATION AND BEHAVIOR OF MTE / July SCALE 2003 FURTHER VALIDATION OF THE MOTIVATION TOWARD THE ENVIRONMENT SCALE MARK VILLACORTA received

More information

The Power of Positive Engagement to Motivate the Poor Performer. Disclosure. Opening Skit Questions

The Power of Positive Engagement to Motivate the Poor Performer. Disclosure. Opening Skit Questions The Power of Positive Engagement to Motivate the Poor Performer Anita Cleven, PharmD, RPh Huy Hoang, PharmD, RPh Disclosure In accordance with the ACPE s and ACCME s Standards for Commercial Support, anyone

More information

Follow this and additional works at: Part of the Exercise Science Commons

Follow this and additional works at:   Part of the Exercise Science Commons The College at Brockport: State University of New York Digital Commons @Brockport Senior Honors Theses Master's Theses and Honors Projects 12-8-2016 Who Matters? The Perceived Motivational Climates Created

More information

Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI)

Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) Scale Description The Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) is a multidimensional measurement device intended to assess participantsõ subjective experience related to

More information

The effect of causality orientations and positive competenceenhancing feedback on intrinsic motivation: A test of additive and interactive effects

The effect of causality orientations and positive competenceenhancing feedback on intrinsic motivation: A test of additive and interactive effects The effect of causality orientations and positive competenceenhancing feedback on intrinsic motivation: A test of additive and interactive effects Author Hagger, Martin S., Koch, Severine, Chatzisarantis,

More information

Between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Examination of reasons for academic study based on the theory of internalization

Between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Examination of reasons for academic study based on the theory of internalization Japanese Psychological Research 1997, Volume 39, No. 2, 98 108 Between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Examination of reasons for academic study based on the theory of internalization TOSHIHIKO HAYAMIZU

More information

Motivational Predictors of Young Adolescents' Participation in an Outdoor Adventure Course: A Self-determination Theory Approach

Motivational Predictors of Young Adolescents' Participation in an Outdoor Adventure Course: A Self-determination Theory Approach Journal of Adventure Education and Outdoor Learning Vol 4(1), 57-65, 2004 Motivational Predictors of Young Adolescents' Participation in an Outdoor Adventure Course: A Self-determination Theory Approach

More information

Motivation & Emotion. Extrinsic motivation. Outline Extrinsic motivation. James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2017

Motivation & Emotion. Extrinsic motivation. Outline Extrinsic motivation. James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2017 Motivation & Emotion Extrinsic motivation James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2017 Image source 1 Outline Extrinsic motivation Quasi-needs IM vs. EM Expected and tangible rewards

More information

Self-determination Theory and the psychology of exercise

Self-determination Theory and the psychology of exercise International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology Vol. 1, No. 1, March 2008, 79103 Self-determination Theory and the psychology of exercise Martin Hagger a * and Nikos Chatzisarantis b a School of

More information

INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONAL ORIENTATIONS: A STUDY AMONGTHE COLLEGE STUDENTS

INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONAL ORIENTATIONS: A STUDY AMONGTHE COLLEGE STUDENTS INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONAL ORIENTATIONS: A STUDY AMONGTHE COLLEGE STUDENTS V.R. Rajesh, Ph.D. Research Scholar, Department of Education, Institute of Advanced Study in Education (Autonomous),

More information

AUTONOMY-SUPPORTIVE VERSUS PSYCHOLOGICALLY CONTROLLING PARENTING POSITIVELY PREDICTS STRATEGIES OF PRODUCTIVE LEARNING

AUTONOMY-SUPPORTIVE VERSUS PSYCHOLOGICALLY CONTROLLING PARENTING POSITIVELY PREDICTS STRATEGIES OF PRODUCTIVE LEARNING ISSN 2073-2244 www.alb-shkenca.org Copyright Institute Alb-Shkenca AKTET Journal of Institute Alb-Shkenca Revistë Shkencore e Institutit Alb-Shkenca AUTONOMY-SUPPORTIVE VERSUS PSYCHOLOGICALLY CONTROLLING

More information

Most clients willingly come to us,

Most clients willingly come to us, THE ROLE OF MOTIVATION IN BEHAVIOR CHANGE How Do We Encourage Our Clients To Be Active? by Wendy M. Rodgers, Ph.D., and Christina C. Loitz, M.S. LEARNING OBJECTIVES Understand the basics of self-determination

More information

This is the prepublication version of the following manuscript:

This is the prepublication version of the following manuscript: Running Head: ACADEMIC MOTIVATION CONTINUUM 1 Evidence of a Continuum Structure of Academic Self-Determination: A Two-Study Test Using a Bifactor-ESEM Representation of Academic Motivation David Litalien

More information

c) Redraw the model and place on it relevant attributions for each of the four boxes.

c) Redraw the model and place on it relevant attributions for each of the four boxes. CHAPTER 6: Attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence, and leadership Practice questions - text book pages 107-108 1) a) Figure 6.21 partly illustrates Weiner s model of attribution. Explain the

More information

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science Ghali Hassan Faculty of Education, Queensland University of Technology, Australia Abstract The purpose of this exploratory study was

More information

Athlete burnout is a syndrome that can result from

Athlete burnout is a syndrome that can result from Temporal Ordering of Motivational Quality and Athlete Burnout in Elite Sport CHRIS LONSDALE 1 and KEN HODGE 2 1 University of Western Sydney, Penrith, NSW, AUSTRALIA; and 2 University of Otago, Dunedin,

More information

PSYCHOLOGY OF SPORT & EXERCISE

PSYCHOLOGY OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY OF SPORT & EXERCISE MOTIVATION IN SPORT What we already know?? Remember (NEW)? B F Skinner Operant conditioning Motivation comes through reinforcement Positive (adding nice rewards) Negative

More information

ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition. Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation

ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition. Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation 1 Learning Objectives Based on Chapter 2 of the ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th ed., this session describes

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological needs & implicit motives Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2016 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 3 Psychological

More information

On the cued activation of situational motivation q

On the cued activation of situational motivation q Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 41 (2005) 482 487 www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp On the cued activation of situational motivation q Catherine F. Ratelle a, *, Mark W. Baldwin b, Robert J. Vallerand

More information

A New View on Teaching Motivation Self-determination Theory. MA Wen-ying, LIU Xi. Changchun University, Changchun, China.

A New View on Teaching Motivation Self-determination Theory. MA Wen-ying, LIU Xi. Changchun University, Changchun, China. Sino-US English Teaching, January 2016, Vol. 13, No. 1, 33-39 doi:10.17265/1539-8072/2016.01.006 D DAVID PUBLISHING A New View on Teaching Motivation Self-determination Theory MA Wen-ying, LIU Xi Changchun

More information

Motivational Predictors of Coping With Academic Examination

Motivational Predictors of Coping With Academic Examination Motivational Predictors of Coping With Academic Examination Julie Doron, Yannick Stephan, Christophe Maïano, Christine Le Scanff To cite this version: Julie Doron, Yannick Stephan, Christophe Maïano, Christine

More information

Birmingham, UK b Public Health, Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of

Birmingham, UK b Public Health, Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of This article was downloaded by: [University of Birmingham] On: 15 August 2011, At: 03:27 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Climate and Motivation for Women Athletes in Palestine

Climate and Motivation for Women Athletes in Palestine Climate and Motivation for Women Athletes in Palestine by Shima Younes, Lori Ciccomascolo & Minsuk Shim, University of Rhode Island Abstract The purpose of this study was to investigate the factors that

More information

A Modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to Include an Assessment of Amotivation

A Modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to Include an Assessment of Amotivation JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2004, 26, 191-196 2004 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. A Modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to Include an Assessment of Amotivation

More information

Running head: ANTECEDENTS OF PERCEIVED COACH BEHAVIORS 1. Antecedents of Perceived Coach Interpersonal Behaviors: The Coaching Environment and

Running head: ANTECEDENTS OF PERCEIVED COACH BEHAVIORS 1. Antecedents of Perceived Coach Interpersonal Behaviors: The Coaching Environment and Running head: ANTECEDENTS OF PERCEIVED COACH BEHAVIORS Antecedents of Perceived Coach Interpersonal Behaviors: The Coaching Environment and Coach Psychological Well- and Ill-Being Stebbings, J. and Taylor,

More information

Cross-Diagnostic Disorders of Cognition/Motivation The Why and the What. George Garibaldi ISCTM, February 2014

Cross-Diagnostic Disorders of Cognition/Motivation The Why and the What. George Garibaldi ISCTM, February 2014 Cross-Diagnostic Disorders of Cognition/Motivation The Why and the What George Garibaldi ISCTM, February 2014 Disclosure: The presenter is an employee and share holder of F. Hoffmann La Roche The contents

More information

The Paradox of Received Social Support

The Paradox of Received Social Support PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Paradox of Received Social Support The Importance of Natalya C. Maisel 1 and Shelly L. Gable 2 1 University of California, Los Angeles, and 2 University of California,

More information

Workplace Wellbeing: Stop Telling Me What To Do. Nicole Fallowfield, CWP, CIC Principal Director Of Health Risk Management

Workplace Wellbeing: Stop Telling Me What To Do. Nicole Fallowfield, CWP, CIC Principal Director Of Health Risk Management Workplace Wellbeing: Stop Telling Me What To Do Nicole Fallowfield, CWP, CIC Principal Director Of Health Risk Management 1 A Little Background On The Topic Health Coaching Stages of change Knowledge leads

More information

The relationship between, style leadership coaches and achievement motivation female athletes fitness field of Bojnoord city

The relationship between, style leadership coaches and achievement motivation female athletes fitness field of Bojnoord city Journal of Novel Applied Sciences Available online at www.jnasci.org 2014 JNAS Journal-2014-3-5/557-561 ISSN 2322-5149 2014 JNAS The relationship between, style leadership coaches and achievement motivation

More information

The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation

The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation Running head: SELF-EFFICACY, FEEDBACK PERCEPTION AND INTRINSIC MOTIVATION The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation Group 16 Lindy Wijsman

More information

Manipulating Achievement Motivation in Physical Education By Manipulating the Motivational Climate

Manipulating Achievement Motivation in Physical Education By Manipulating the Motivational Climate European Journal of Sport Science, vol. 2, issue 1 2002 by Human Kinetics Publishers and the European College of Sport Science Manipulating Achievement Motivation in Physical Education By Manipulating

More information

Research on self-determination in physical education: key findings and proposals for future research

Research on self-determination in physical education: key findings and proposals for future research This article was downloaded by: [University of Gent] On: 07 January 2014, At: 09:13 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Models for Involvement in Physical Activity

Models for Involvement in Physical Activity Models for Involvement in Physical Activity Why do people embrace or do not embrace a physical active lifestyle? 1 Typical Undergrad Complaints What s the point? Aren t these all the same? What does this

More information

Motivation & Emotion. Outline Intrinsic & extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic-extrinsic motivations & goal-setting. Intrinsic motivation

Motivation & Emotion. Outline Intrinsic & extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic-extrinsic motivations & goal-setting. Intrinsic motivation Motivation & Emotion Intrinsic-extrinsic motivations & goal-setting Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2014 Image source 1 Outline Intrinsic & extrinsic motivation Intrinsic

More information

Basic Psychological Needs: Satisfaction, Frustration and Entrepreneurial Intention

Basic Psychological Needs: Satisfaction, Frustration and Entrepreneurial Intention Basic Psychological Needs: Satisfaction, Frustration and Entrepreneurial Intention Arif Hassan 1 Ibrahim Al-Jubari 2 International Islamic University Malaysia Abstract The presented study was proposed

More information

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question Trivia Question Where did win one for the gipper come from? Chapter 9 What are the 3 components of motivation? 3 major categories of motivation. Major theories of motivation. How the theories are applied

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Within-Person Relationships Among Daily Self-Esteem, Need Satisfaction, and Authenticity Whitney L. Heppner, 1 Michael H. Kernis, 1 John B. Nezlek, 2 Joshua Foster,

More information

Self-determination, stage of readiness to change for exercise, and frequency of physical activity in young people

Self-determination, stage of readiness to change for exercise, and frequency of physical activity in young people European Journal of Sport Science, December 2006; 6(4): 231 243 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Self-determination, stage of readiness to change for exercise, and frequency of physical activity in young people AMANDA

More information

(2014) : 19 (1) ISSN

(2014) : 19 (1) ISSN Van den Berghe, Lynn and Vansteenkiste, Maarten and Cardon, Greet and Kirk, David and Haerens, Leen (2014) Research on selfdetermination in physical education : key findings and proposals for future research.

More information

Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS

Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS INTEREST and ENJOYMENT TASK VALUE GOALS (Purposes for doing) INTRINSIC vs EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION EXPECTANCY BELIEFS SELF-EFFICACY SELF-CONCEPT (Self-Esteem) OUTCOME

More information

The Relationship between leadership styles of coaches with self-determination and burn-out of the Iranian elite female Volleyball players

The Relationship between leadership styles of coaches with self-determination and burn-out of the Iranian elite female Volleyball players The Relationship between leadership styles of coaches with self-determination and burn-out of the Iranian elite female Volleyball players Shirin Zardoshtian Member, (Ph.D) Razi University, Iran Rasool

More information

I ll eat what she s eating can the university student experience influence eating behaviours?

I ll eat what she s eating can the university student experience influence eating behaviours? Snapshots of Doctoral Research at University College Cork 2011 I ll eat what she s eating can the university student experience influence eating behaviours? Ciana Aspell Food Business and Development,

More information

Predictors of Avoidance of Help-Seeking: Social Achievement Goal Orientation, Perceived Social Competence and Autonomy

Predictors of Avoidance of Help-Seeking: Social Achievement Goal Orientation, Perceived Social Competence and Autonomy World Applied Sciences Journal 17 (5): 637-642, 2012 ISSN 1818-4952 IDOSI Publications, 2012 Predictors of Avoidance of Help-Seeking: Social Achievement Goal Orientation, Perceived Social Competence and

More information

Procrastination, Motivation, & Flow

Procrastination, Motivation, & Flow Andrews University Digital Commons @ Andrews University Honors Theses Undergraduate Research 3-28-2016 Procrastination, Motivation, & Flow Reginald Desrosiers Andrews University, rjd@andrews.edu This research

More information

Self-Determination of Contextual Motivation, Inter-Context Dynamics and Adolescents Patterns of Sport Participation over Time

Self-Determination of Contextual Motivation, Inter-Context Dynamics and Adolescents Patterns of Sport Participation over Time Self-Determination of Contextual Motivation, Inter-Context Dynamics and Adolescents Patterns of Sport Participation over Time Julie Boiché, Philippe Sarrazin To cite this version: Julie Boiché, Philippe

More information

Self-Regulation of Physical Education Teacher Education Students' Attitudes Towards Exercise and Diet

Self-Regulation of Physical Education Teacher Education Students' Attitudes Towards Exercise and Diet Boise State University ScholarWorks Kinesiology Faculty Publications and Presentations Department of Kinesiology 4-1-2013 Self-Regulation of Physical Education Teacher Education Students' Attitudes Towards

More information

Motivation & Emotion. Extrinsic motivation & goal-setting. Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2016.

Motivation & Emotion. Extrinsic motivation & goal-setting. Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2016. Motivation & Emotion Extrinsic motivation & goal-setting Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2016 Image source 1 Outline Extrinsic motivation Quasi-needs IM vs. EM Expected

More information

PETTLEP IMAGERY AND VIDEO-OBSERVATION: A MOTIVATION CASE STUDY OF FOUR BADMINTON PLAYERS PETTLEP and intrinsic motivated badminton performance

PETTLEP IMAGERY AND VIDEO-OBSERVATION: A MOTIVATION CASE STUDY OF FOUR BADMINTON PLAYERS PETTLEP and intrinsic motivated badminton performance 239 PETTLEP IMAGERY AND VIDEO-OBSERVATION: A MOTIVATION CASE STUDY OF FOUR BADMINTON PLAYERS PETTLEP and intrinsic motivated badminton performance FERNANDO RAMIREZ RAMIREZ, DAVE SMITH, PAUL HOLMES Manchester

More information

An Application of the Trans-Contextual Model of Motivation in Elementary School Physical Education

An Application of the Trans-Contextual Model of Motivation in Elementary School Physical Education The Physical Educator Vol. 72 pp. 123 141 2015 PEDAGOGY - ELEMENTARY PHYSICAL EDUCATION An Application of the Trans-Contextual Model of Motivation in Elementary School Physical Education Yannis Ntovolis,

More information