Unequal Weber fractions for the categorization of brief temporal intervals

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Unequal Weber fractions for the categorization of brief temporal intervals"

Transcription

1 Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 21, 72 (5), doi:1.3758/app Unequal Weber fractions for the categorization of brief temporal intervals SIMON GRONDIN Université Laval, Québec, Québec, Canada How constant is the Weber fraction (WF) for brief time intervals? This question was assessed in three experiments with two base durations (BDs), and 1 sec, and with different ways of estimating the WF. In Experiment 1, the psychometric functions were drawn on the basis of 4, 8, or 12 comparison intervals with the shortest to longest duration ranges being kept constant. The results revealed no effect of the number of intervals, but the WF (threshold/bd) was significantly lower at sec. In Experiment 2, the comparison intervals were distributed over three duration ranges. There was no range effect, and the WF was generally lower at sec than at 1 sec. In Experiment 3, one condition allowed a comparison of the BD with the same range between the shortest and longest comparison intervals. Once again, the WF was lower at sec than at 1 sec. Overall, the results reveal (1) that increasing the number of comparison intervals or the duration range does not seem to affect the value of the WF and (2) that the WF is lower at sec than at 1 sec, which is inconsistent with the scalar property of some timing models. Although the question remains open (Ivry & Schlerf, 28), many time perception researchers believe that there is an internal clock that is, a unique central device dedicated to measuring the passage of time (see, e.g., Grondin, 28; Meck, 23). This clock is often described as a pacemaker that emits pulses, whose accumulation in a counter constitutes the basis for the experience of time (Grondin, 21; Killeen & Weiss, 1987). The presence of this clock counter device underpins the scalar expectancy theory (SET), which is a very influential theory in timing research (Wearden, 23). A basic tenet of SET is that the variability of temporal estimates increases proportionally with the magnitude of time (see Gibbon, 1977, 1991; Gibbon, Church, & Meck, 1984). From a psychophysics perspective, this means that the Weber fraction (WF) should remain constant over a wide range of durations. In the information-processing version of SET, variance in a timing task is reported as also belonging to memory and, to some extent, to decisional processes (Gibbon et al., 1984; Meck, 23). There are time perception models that are not based on a pacemaker counter perspective (see Grondin, 21). On the one hand, some models are referred to as being dependent specifically on a modality (Johnston, Arnold, & Nishida, 26) or as coordination-dependent systems (Jantzen, Steinberg, & Kelso, 25). For instance, in the state-dependent networks model, judging duration means being able to recognize spatial patterns of activity (Buonomano, 27; Karmarkar & Buonomano, 27). On the other hand, there is a model, the dynamic attending theory, based on the capacity to synchronize (attunement) the internal, attentional rhythmicity (an oscillatory process) with the temporal structure of environmental stimuli (Jones, 1976; Jones & Boltz, 1989; Large & Jones, 1999). The variability of temporal estimates is often assessed on the basis of some variation of the method of constant stimuli the bisection or the temporal generalization method where intervals of different lengths have to be categorized as short or long. In the bisection task, a short standard and a long standard (shortest and longest of a distribution of intervals) are presented before the presentation of comparison intervals. In the generalization task, only one standard (midpoint of a distribution of comparison intervals) is presented, and a participant decides whether a given interval is identical to the standard (or, as in the present experiments, whether the interval is shorter or longer than the standard). The results can be used to obtain a psychometric function, where the probability of responding long is plotted as a function of the length of the intervals. The steepness of the function indicates the degree of variability. According to SET, when psychometric functions drawn for different duration ranges are normalized by their mean, they superimpose (see Church, 23; Wearden & Lejeune, 28). Some authors have reported that the choice of time intervals that is, the spacing between the data points on the function influences the estimation of variability (Wearden & Ferrara, 1995, 1996; but see Church & Gibbon, 1982). Moreover, this effect may depend on the range of durations under investigation (Grondin, Bisson, & Gagnon, 29). Larger differences between the longest and shortest intervals are reported as leading to higher estimates of sensitivity. Therefore, manipulating the spacing S. Grondin, simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca 21 The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1422

2 TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION 1423 between data points when the method of constant stimuli is used might have an impact on the verdict regarding the suitability of the Weber function for time. In the present series of experiments, the purpose was to test the constancy of the WF for brief temporal intervals in conditions involving various spacings of intervals and various numbers of points on the psychometric functions. Increasing the number of points was expected to increase the difficulty of the task, especially in conditions in which more points were distributed over a larger range of duration. It is known, for instance, that the perceived duration of intervals is influenced by the global distributional properties of the series of intervals used within a session (Jones & McAuley, 25; McAuley & Jones, 23). In the present study, assessments were based on individual estimates of temporal sensitivity in a very simple basic condition that is, one in which a temporal judgment followed the presentation of a single time interval. Sensitivity was estimated for two very distinct standard durations (2 and 1, msec) and within a range where explicit counting was not expected to provide any benefits ( 1 sec; Grondin, Meilleur-Wells, & Lachance, 1999). In Experiments 1 and 2, the distance between the intervals was kept proportional in the 2- and 1,-msec durations (i.e., it was multiplied by 5). In Experiment 3, one of the spacing conditions was the same in the 2- and 1,-msec conditions. EXPERIMENT 1 Method Participants. Twelve volunteers from Université Laval (5 women and 7 men) took part in the experiment. The average age was 25.3 years. The participants received $3 Canadian for taking part in the experiment. The study was approved by the Comité d éthique de la recherche de l'université Laval. All the participants gave written informed consent prior to the experiment. Apparatus and Stimuli. The intervals to be discriminated were marked by two 1-kHz 2-msec auditory signals. The signals were produced by an IBM PC and were presented binaurally through headphones (Sony MDR-V6). Each participant was seated at a computer in a dimly lit room and was asked to respond either short or long by pressing 1 or 3, respectively, on the computer keypad. Procedure. The experiment consisted of six experimental sessions of 3 4 min for each participant, one session for each experiment condition. A minimum of 1 h was required between sessions. The experiment included two independent variables: standard interval durations, or base durations (2 and 1, msec), and number of comparison intervals (4, 8, or 12). Comparison intervals were separated by 22, 1, or 6 msec for the 2-msec standard, ranging from 167 to 233 msec in the 4-point condition, from 165 to 235 msec in the 8-point condition, and from 167 to 233 msec in the 12-point condition. For the 1,-msec standard, comparison intervals were five times longer that is, separated by 11, 5, or 3 msec, ranging from 835 to 1,165 msec in the 4-point condition, from 825 to 1,175 msec in the 8-point condition, and from 835 to 1,165 msec in the 12-point condition (see Table 1 for a summary). Each session consisted of six blocks, with a 2-sec pause between blocks. Each block contained 18 (4-point condition), 9 (8-point condition), or 6 (12-point condition) randomly ordered presentations of the comparison intervals, for a total of 72 trials per block. Thus, results from the six blocks were combined to trace one psychometric function per condition, where each point is represented by 18, 54, or 36 observations in the 4-, 8-, and 12-point conditions, respectively. Table 1 Shortest and Longest Comparison Intervals in Each Condition in Experiments 1 3 Comparison Intervals (msec) Number of Intervals 2-msec Standard 1,-msec Standard Experiment 1 Different Numbers of Comparison Intervals; Same Proportional Range , , ,165 Experiment 2 Unique Number of Comparison Intervals; Same Proportional Ranges 8 (narrow) ,7 8 (middle) ,14 8 (large) ,21 Experiment 3 Unique Number of Comparison Intervals; Nonproportional Ranges 6 (narrow) ,5 6 (large) ,1 The single-stimulus method was employed (Morgan, Watamaniuk, & McKee, 2). Each trial consisted of the presentation of one interval. At the beginning of each session, the standard (2 or 1, msec) was presented 1 times. The participants were informed that they would have to judge whether later intervals were longer or shorter than the standard (temporal generalization). After the presentation of the standard, the experimental trials began. Each trial consisted of one comparison interval marked by two brief auditory signals (2 msec). The participants had to judge whether the interval was shorter or longer than the standard presented at the beginning of the session. After the participant entered a response, the correct response was indicated on the computer screen. Data analysis. For each participant and for each experimental condition, a 4-, 8-, or 12-point psychometric function was traced, plotting the 4, 8, or 12 comparison intervals on the x-axis and the probability of responding long on the y-axis. The cumulative normal distribution was fitted to the resulting curves. Two indices of performance were estimated for each psychometric function, one for sensitivity and one for the perceived duration. As an indicator of temporal sensitivity, which was the most important issue in the present study, estimates of the standard deviation on the psychometric function were determined. Using one SD (or variance) is a common procedure for expressing temporal sensitivity (Grondin, 28; Killeen & Weiss, 1987). The other dependent variable is the bisection point (BP). The BP can be defined as the x value corresponding to the.5 probability of long responses on the y-axis. Longer perceived durations are reflected by smaller BP values. Results Figure 1 provides an illustration of the grouped data in each experimental condition. Six psychometric functions were constructed for each individual, one for each of the six experimental conditions. For the 4-point conditions, 2 or 1, msec, the goodness of fit was highly satisfactory (mean R 2 values.98). For the 8-point conditions, the mean R 2 value was above.95 at 2 msec and above.92 at 1, msec. For the 12-point conditions, the mean R 2 value was above.92 at 2 msec and above 5 (range from.71 to.96) at 1, msec.

3 1424 GRONDIN R 2 = R 2 = R 2 = Probability of Responding Long R 2 = R 2 = R 2 = ,55 1, ,25 1,75 1,125 1, ,15 1,45 1,75 1,15 1,135 1,165 Comparison Intervals (msec) Figure 1. Psychometric function (pooled results) for the 4- (left), 8- (middle), and 12-point (right) conditions in Experiment 1. Upper panels, standard 2 msec; lower panels, standard 1, msec.

4 TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION 1425 In order to compare directly the sensitivity values at and 1 sec, the WF was computed: WF SD/base duration. Table 2 provides the mean results in each condition. A 2 3 ANOVA with repeated measures revealed that there was a significant duration effect [F(1,11) 157, p, 2.591], with the WF being lower in the -sec than in the 1-sec condition; however, the number of comparison intervals exerted no significant effect [F(2,22).598, p.554, 2.52], and the interaction effect was not significant [F(2,22) 35, p 53, 2.38]. In order to compare directly the perceived duration values at and 1 sec, the constant error (CE) was computed: CE BP base duration. The 2 3 ANOVA with repeated measures revealed no significant effect. EXPERIMENT 2 In this experiment, the spacing between comparison intervals was kept proportional from to 1 sec. However, there were eight comparison intervals in each of three conditions, but the spacing between these intervals differed. In other words, the duration range covered in each condition was now varied. Method Participants. Twelve volunteers from Université Laval (8 women and 4 men) took part in the experiment. The average age was 25.5 years. The participants received $3 Canadian for taking part in the experiment. Apparatus and Stimuli. The materials were the same as those in Experiment 1. Procedure. The participants followed the same procedure as that in Experiment 1, but the comparison intervals in each of the six experimental conditions (2 3) were different. First, there were 8 comparison intervals in each condition. For the 2-msec standard, each comparison interval was separated by 4, 8, or 12 msec (ranging from 186 to 214 msec, from 172 to 228 msec, and from 158 to 242 msec, respectively, as indicated in Table 1). For the 1,-msec standard, comparison intervals were multiplied by 5 that is, were separated by 2, 4, or 6 msec (ranging from 93 to 1,7 msec, from 86 to 1,14 msec, and from 79 to 1,21 msec, respectively). Table 2 Weber Fraction in Each Condition in Experiments 1 3 Number of Intervals 2 msec 1, msec Experiment 1 Different Numbers of Comparison Intervals; Same Proportional Range Experiment 2 Unique Number of Comparison Intervals; Same Proportional Ranges 8 (narrow) (middle) (large) Experiment 3 Unique Number of Comparison Intervals; Nonproportional Ranges 6 (narrow) (large) In other words, the range between the briefest and longest comparison intervals in the narrowest condition is multiplied by two and by three, respectively, in the other two conditions. Results As in Experiment 1, one psychometric function for each of the six experimental conditions was constructed for each individual (see the pooled data in Figure 2). For the 4-msec spacing conditions, the goodness of fit was highly satisfactory (mean R 2 values.96 at 2 msec and.93 at 1, msec). For the 8-msec spacing conditions, the mean R 2 values were above.98 at 2 and 1, msec. For the 12-msec spacing conditions, the mean R 2 values were above.99 at 2 and 1, msec. Table 2 provides the mean WF in each condition. Once again, the WFs were lower at sec than at 1 sec; this observation applies to each of the three spacing conditions. A 2 3 ANOVA with repeated measures revealed that no effect was significant [duration, F(1,11) 29, p.117, 2 8; spacing of intervals, F(2,22) 87, p.594, 2 2; interaction, F(2,22) 189, p 8, 2.133]. Note, however, that the lowest WF at sec was significantly lower than the lowest WF at 1 sec [t(11) 26, p.5]. Finally, the 2 3 ANOVA with repeated measures revealed no significant effect for the CE. EXPERIMENT 3 In this experiment, the spacing of the comparison intervals from to 1 sec was not kept proportional. The parameters were selected in such a way that in one condition, the spread of the distribution of comparison intervals was the same at and 1 sec. Method Participants. Twelve volunteers from Université Laval (6 women and 6 men) took part in the experiment. The average age was 24 years. The participants received $24 Canadian for taking part in the experiment. Apparatus and Stimuli. The materials were the same as those in the previous experiments. Procedure. The experiment consisted of four sessions of 3 4 min for each participant, one session for each experimental condition. A minimum of 1 h was required between sessions. The experiment included two independent variables: base duration (2 and 1, msec) and the distribution of comparison intervals (narrow vs. large). At 2 msec, the comparison intervals lasted 175, 185, 195, 25, 215, and 225 msec (narrow range) and 15, 17, 19, 21, 23, and 25 msec (large range). At 1, msec, the comparison intervals lasted 95, 97, 99, 1,1, 1,3, and 1,5 msec (narrow range) and 9, 94, 98, 1,2, 1,6, and 1,1 msec (large range). Note that there were 1 msec between the shortest and longest comparison intervals in the large condition at 2 msec and in the narrow condition at 1, msec. In all the conditions, there were six comparison intervals. Each session consisted of six blocks, with a 2-sec pause between blocks, and each block contained 72 trials that is, 12 repetitions of each comparison interval. Results One psychometric function for each of the four experimental conditions was constructed for each individual (see the pooled data in Figure 3). At 2 msec, the goodness

5 1426 GRONDIN R 2 = R 2 = R 2 = Probability of Responding Long R 2 = R 2 = R 2 = ,1 1,3 1,5 1, ,2 1,6 1,1 1, ,3 1,9 1,15 1,21 Comparison Intervals (msec) Figure 2. Psychometric function (pooled results) for the narrow (left), middle (middle), and large (right) spacing conditions in Experiment 2. Upper panels, standard 2 msec; lower panels, standard 1, msec.

6 TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION Probability of Responding Long 1. R 2 = R 2 = R 2 = R 2 = ,1 1,3 1, , 1,5 1,1 Comparison Intervals (msec) Figure 3. Psychometric function (pooled results) for the narrow (left) and large (right) spacing conditions in Experiment 3. Upper panels, standard 2 msec; lower panels, standard 1, msec. of fit was highly satisfactory (mean R 2 values.98 in the narrow- and the large-range conditions). At 1, msec, the mean R 2 values were 6 and.96 in the narrow- and large-range conditions, respectively. Table 2 provides the mean WF in each experimental condition. A 2 2 ANOVA with repeated measures revealed that there was a significant duration effect [F(1,11) 945, p.11, 2 62], with the WF being once again lower in the -sec than in the 1-sec condition; the range of comparison intervals exerted no significant effect [F(1,11) 164, p 85, 2.13], and the interaction effect was not significant [F(1,11) 3.128, p.15, 2 21]. Moreover, a t test indicated that the best performance at sec was significantly better than the best performance at 1 sec [t(11) 3.163, p.1; see Figure 4]. Finally, the 2 2 ANOVA with repeated measures revealed no significant effect for the CE. DISCUSSION The present series of experiments generated findings regarding not only the potential spacing effect of temporal sensitivity on estimates, but also the impact of using different numbers of comparison intervals when thresholds

7 1428 GRONDIN Weber Fraction Large Narrow 1 1 Standard Duration (sec) Figure 4. Mean Weber fraction in the narrow- and large-range conditions for each standard duration in Experiment 3. have to be estimated. Most important, the experiments offer a critical look at Weber s law for the discrimination of brief temporal intervals. Method Effects In these experiments, the purpose was to adopt a classical method for estimating sensitivity (threshold) to time intervals, as well as to manipulate different parameters in order to assess the extent to which the threshold estimates varied and, thus, the impact of the experimental manipulations on Weber s law. The first step involved checking whether the number of intervals chosen for building the psychometric functions would influence the threshold estimates. Depending on the nature of time representation in memory, different impacts might be expected at and 1 sec. Indeed, increasing the number of comparison intervals might have a greater impact in the 1-sec range than in the -sec range, since the intervals would be longer. However, the results do not support this hypothesis. Moreover, both the effect size for the number-of-interval condition and the duration number-of-interval interaction were very small. Of course, this conclusion regarding the number-of-interval issue is not definitive, for it is based on the acceptance of a null hypothesis. Nevertheless, it is reasonable to believe that the number of intervals on psychometric functions has a negligible effect on temporal sensitivity estimates, for increasing the number of intervals in an experimental session does not seem to cause any overload in memory, even with longer intervals. Strictly speaking, the data from Experiments 2 and 3 indicate that the spacing of comparison intervals has no effect on estimates of temporal sensitivity, at either or 1 sec. This finding is inconsistent with the data in Wearden and Ferrara (1995, 1996). Moreover, preliminary lab results indicate some spacing effect as well, with a narrower range leading to a lower threshold at sec (Grondin et al., 29). In Experiment 3, the tendency of the results indicates that, at 1 sec, narrower spacing decreases sensitivity (higher threshold). Indeed, in this experiment, the individual R 2 values were lowest (and the thresholds highest) in the 1-sec and narrow condition. It is not excluded that the dissimilarities between the present results and those in Wearden and Ferrara (1995, 1996) rely on subtle methodological differences. (1) Wearden and Ferrara used bisection, not the generalization variant used here; (2) the spacing effects in their experiments involved different spacings of comparisons between the short and long standards (e.g., linear vs. logarithmic), not different numbers of linearly spaced stimuli, as in the present investigation, or different linear spacings; and (3) their theoretical focus was on whether people were averaging all the stimuli in the bisection experiment together or using the short and long standards. As well, although a kind of generalization task was used here and in Ferrara, Lejeune, and Wearden (1997; see also Wearden & Grindrod, 23, for similar results), the method in each set of experiments differed since, on the one hand, the task required saying (yes or no) whether the intervals was like the standard, whereas it was a shorter/ longer response that was requested from the participants in the present experiments. 1 This distinction may account for the different findings, considering that a part of variance in the information- processing version of SET is actually located at the decision process level (Wearden & Grindrod, 23). The Weber Fraction Apart from highlighting a potential spacing effect, the results in all of the conditions in the present experiments point in the same direction: There were unequal WFs for the different duration ranges, and the WF values were smaller at sec than at 1 sec. This is a violation of Weber s law, which states that the ratio of threshold to magnitude should remain constant. Of critical importance is the fact that the inequality of the WF is due not to a higher value at sec, but to a higher value at 1 sec. Indeed, what is usually observed in psychophysics is an increase of the WF for lower magnitudes of a sensory continuum, a phenomenon easily accounted for by the generalized form of Weber s law. The present findings are therefore totally inconsistent with this generalized version. If anything, they are exactly the opposite. Such a result is consistent with the one reported by Lavoie and Grondin (24), where the Weber fraction was higher at 2 sec than at sec. The latter result might have been due to an increase of the Weber fraction with an interval longer than 1 sec. Several studies have shown an increase of the ratio when durations were longer than 2 sec (Getty, 1975; see Fraisse, 1978, for a brief overview, and Bizo, Chu, Sanabria, & Killeen, 26, for animal timing data). The literature on the WF for the discrimination of tempi which involves the discrimination of empty time intervals indicates that Weber s law holds when intervals range from 4 to 1,45 msec (Ehrlé & Samson, 25; Halpern & Darwin, 1982). Beyond this range, when single intervals are presented, the WF can be reduced if an explicit counting strategy is adopted (Grondin et al., 1999). In the present experiments, the WF inequality occurred within the narrow - to 1-sec range.

8 TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION 1429 Why the WFs are unequal at and 1 sec remains a difficult question. One may argue that longer intervals occupy a larger space in working memory, which would exceed the temporal capacity of working memory (Gilden & Marusich, 29; Lavoie & Grondin, 24). However, in such a case, increasing the number of comparison intervals, as in Experiment 1, should have led to more damage (increased WF) at 1 sec than at sec, which was not the case. On the other hand, the unequal WF might be due not to a high value at 1 sec, but to a low value at sec, which implies an interval range that is closer to the one that one has to deal with in normal speech. Perhaps the greater amount of experience with such intervals in the speech context leads to better discrimination in this range. 2 Implications for the Study of Time That Weber s law does not hold is not specific to the study of psychological time. Somewhat along the line proposed by Holway and Pratt (1936), Masin (29) reported a large series of results, based on multiple sensory contin ua, showing different sorts of violation of Weber s law (see also Cobb, 1932). A failure of Weber s law has an important impact on the study of time. As was noted in the introduction, one popular contemporary theory of time perception, SET, is founded on the scalar property that is, on the fact that the variability increases proportionally with the magnitude of time (Wearden, 23). The pacemaker counter device described earlier is the basis of SET and is reported to conform to some form of Weber s law (Killeen & Weiss, 1987). The proportional view of SET, or of Weber s law, allows positing that a common timekeeping mechanism (a single internal clock) is used for different values on the temporal continuum. Breaking the continuum might mean abandoning this unifying principle. This possibly opens the door to a fragmented and less simple view in which multiple timing mechanisms could be argued to be at play. Indeed, such a view is consistent with pharmacological evidence supporting the hypothesis that there are two distinct timing mechanisms underlying the processing of temporal intervals, one in the subsecond and one in the second range (see Rammsayer, 28). The first one would be a subcortical (or sensory-based or automatic) timing mechanism like Karmarkar and Buonomano s (27) state-dependent network model, for instance whereas the second one requires the support of more cognitive resources. The present data do not indicate what the appropriate mechanism for very brief intervals is, or what the mechanisms for intervals in the - and 1-sec range would be. However, they do indicate that the viability of the singleclock hypothesis requires the identification of an additional, nontemporal source of variance that exerts more influence at 1 sec than at sec. It is usually recognized that the sensory effects associated with the marking process cause more variance at sec than at 1 sec. So, this could not be the source of that additional variance. If it exists, therefore, the additional variance source is probably located at a higher, cognitive level of time processing. Returning to SET s information-processing framework, it is not excluded that the decisional processes are the correct location. AUTHOR NOTE This research was made possible by a research grant awarded by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. This study was presented at the 5th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, held in Boston in November 29. I thank Nicolas Bisson for his help in this project and two anonymous reviewers for their very helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. Correspondence should be addressed to S. Grondin, École de psychologie, Université Laval, 2325 rue des Bibliothèques, Québec, QC, G1V A6 Canada ( simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca). REFERENCES Bizo, L. A., Chu, J. Y. M., Sanabria, F., & Killeen, P. R. (26). The failure of Weber s law in time perception and production. Behavioural Processes, 71, doi:1.116/j.beproc5.11 Buonomano, D. V. (27). The biology of time across different scales. Nature Chemical Biology, 3, Church, R. M. (23). A concise introduction to the scalar timing theory. In W. H. Meck (Ed.), Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing (pp. 3-22). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Church, R. M., & Gibbon, J. (1982). Temporal generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, doi:1.137/ Cobb, P. W. (1932). Weber s law and Fechnerian muddle. Psychological Review, 39, doi:1.137/h73327 Ehrlé, N., & Samson, S. (25). Auditory discrimination of anisochrony: Influence of the tempo and musical backgrounds of listeners. Brain & Cognition, 58, doi:1.116/j.bandc Ferrara, A., Lejeune, H., & Wearden, J. H. (1997). Changing sensitivity to duration in human scalar timing: An experiment, a review, and some possible explanations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 5B, doi:1.18/ Fraisse, P. (1978). Time and rhythm perception. In E. Carterette & M. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception VIII (pp ). New York: Academic Press. Getty, D. J. (1975). Discrimination of short temporal intervals: A comparison of two models. Perception & Psychophysics, 18, 1-8. Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, Gibbon, J. (1991). Origins of scalar timing. Learning & Motivation, 22, doi:1.116/23-969(91)915-z Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. In J. Gibbon & L. Allen (Eds.), Timing and time perception (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 423, pp ). New York: New York Academy of Sciences. doi:1.1111/j tb23417.x Gilden, D. L., & Marusich, L. R. (29). Contraction of time in attention- deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 23, doi:1.137/a14553 Grondin, S. (21). From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time. Psychological Bulletin, 127, doi:1.137/ Grondin, S. (28). Methods for studying psychological time. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp ). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group. Grondin, S. (21). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, Grondin, S., Bisson, N., & Gagnon, C. (29). Temporal processing, range effect, and Weber fraction. In M. Elliott & S. Antonijevic (Eds.), Fechner Day 29: Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp ). Galway, Ireland: International Society for Psychophysics. Grondin, S., Meilleur-Wells, G., & Lachance, R. (1999). When to start explicit counting in a time-intervals discrimination task: A critical point in the timing process by humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 25, doi:1.137/

9 143 GRONDIN Halpern, A. R., & Darwin, C. J. (1982). Duration discrimination in a series of rhythmic events. Perception & Psychophysics, 31, Holway, A. H., & Pratt, C. C. (1936). The Weber-ratio for intensive discrimination. Psychological Review, 43, doi:1.137/ h59748 Ivry, B., & Schlerf, J. (28). Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, doi:1.116/j.tics8 Jantzen, K. J., Steinberg, F. L., & Kelso, J. A. S. (25). Functional MRI reveals the existence of modality and coordination-dependent timing networks. NeuroImage, 25, Johnston, A., Arnold, D. H., & Nishida, S. (26). Spatially localized distortions of event time. Current Biology, 16, Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: Toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83, doi:1.137/33-295x Jones, M. R., & Boltz, M. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychological Review, 96, doi:1.137/33-295x Jones, M. R., & McAuley, J. D. (25). Time judgments in global temporal contexts. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, Karmarkar, U. R., & Buonomano, D. V. (27). Timing in the absence of clocks: Encoding time in neural network states. Neuron, 53, doi:1.116/j.neuron7.1 Killeen, P. R., & Weiss, N. A. (1987). Optimal timing and the Weber function. Psychological Review, 94, doi:1.137/33-295x.9455 Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How we track time-varying events. Psychological Review, 16, Lavoie, P., & Grondin, S. (24). Information processing limitations as revealed by temporal discrimination. Brain & Cognition, 54, doi:1.116/j.bandc4.39 Masin, S. (29). The (Weber s) law that never was. In M. Elliott & S. Antonijevic (Eds.), Fechner Day 29: Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp ). Galway, Ireland: International Society for Psychophysics. McAuley, J. D., & Jones, M. R. (23). Modeling effects of rhythmic context on perceived duration: A comparison of interval and entrain- ment approaches to short-interval timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29, doi:1.137/ Meck, W. H. (ED.) (23). Functional and neural mechanisms of internal timing. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Morgan, M. J., Watamaniuk, S. N. J., & McKee, S. P. (2). The use of an implicit standard for measuring discrimination thresholds. Vision Research, 4, doi:1.116/s ()93-6 Rammsayer, T. H. (28). Neuropharmacological approaches to human timing. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp ). Bingley, U.K.: Emerald Group. Wearden, J. H. (23). Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and mind II (pp ). Goettingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber. Wearden, J. H., & Ferrara, A. (1995). Stimulus spacing effects in temporal bisection by humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48B, doi:1.18/ Wearden, J. H., & Ferrara, A. (1996). Stimulus range effects in temporal bisection by humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49B, doi:1.18/ Wearden, J. H., & Grindrod, R. (23). Manipulating decision processes in the human scalar timing system. Behavioural Processes, 61, Wearden, J. H., & Lejeune, H. (28). Scalar properties in human timing: Conformity and violations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, doi:1.18/ NOTES 1. Thank you to one anonymous reviewer for pointing out the series of differences reported in this paragraph. 2. Thank you to the other anonymous reviewer for proposing this clarification sentence, as well as other wording adjustments adopted in the final version of the article. (Manuscript received December 23, 29; revision accepted for publication February 2, 21.)

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS Simon Grondin, Vincent Laflamme, Nicolas Bisson, Emi Hasuo, Tsuyoshi Kuroda Université Laval, Québec, Canada simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca Abstract

More information

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE Isabelle Guay and Simon Grondin Université Laval, Québec, Canada Email: simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca ABSTRACT

More information

Little to be Expected from Auditory Training for Improving Visual Temporal Discrimination

Little to be Expected from Auditory Training for Improving Visual Temporal Discrimination 95 ARTICLE Little to be Expected from Auditory Training for Improving Visual Temporal Discrimination Simon Grondin, Nicolas Bisson, Caroline Gagnon Pierre-Luc Gamache and Andrée-Anne Matteau Abstract It

More information

Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection?

Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection? Learning and Motivation 33, 10 31 (2002) doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1097, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection? Lorraine G. Allan McMaster University,

More information

Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach

Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach PQJE178995 TECHSET COMPOSITION LTD, SALISBURY, U.K. 6/16/2006 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 0000, 00 (0), 1 17 Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection

More information

SCALAR TIMING (EXPECTANCY) THEORY: A COMPARISON BETWEEN PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION. Abstract

SCALAR TIMING (EXPECTANCY) THEORY: A COMPARISON BETWEEN PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION. Abstract SCALAR TIMING (EXPECTANCY) THEORY: A COMPARISON BETWEEN PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION Hannes Eisler and Anna D. Eisler Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden he@psychology.su.se

More information

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis Journal of Sound and Vibration Manuscript Draft Manuscript Number: JSV-D-10-00826 Title: Does filled duration illusion occur for very short time intervals? Article Type: Rapid Communication Keywords: time

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Episodic temporal generalization: A developmental study

Episodic temporal generalization: A developmental study THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2005, 58A (4), 693 704 Episodic temporal generalization: A developmental study T. McCormack Queen s University, Belfast, Northern Ireland, UK J. H. Wearden

More information

DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS?

DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS? DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS? Emi Hasuo, Yoshitaka Nakajima, and Kazuo Ueda Graduate School of Design, Kyushu University -9- Shiobaru, Minami-ku, Fukuoka, - Japan

More information

When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur?

When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur? THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (10), 1709 1724 When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur? J. H. Wearden Keele University, Keele, UK N. P. M. Todd and L.

More information

Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation

Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation Emily A. Williams (Emily.A.Williams@Manchester.ac.uk) Andrew J. Stewart (Andrew.J.Stewart@Manchester.ac.uk) Luke A. Jones (Luke.Jones@Manchester.ac.uk)

More information

Decision processes in models of timing

Decision processes in models of timing Acta Neurobiol Exp 2004, 64: xxx-xxx NEU OBIOLOGI E EXPE IMENT LIS Decision processes in models of timing John H. Wearden Review Department of Psychology, Manchester University, Manchester, M13 9PL, UK,

More information

Temporal Generalization in 3- to 8-Year-Old Children

Temporal Generalization in 3- to 8-Year-Old Children Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 80, 271 288 (2001) doi:10.1006/jecp.2001.2629, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Temporal Generalization in 3- to 8-Year-Old Children Sylvie Droit-Volet

More information

The perception of empty and filled time intervals by pigeons

The perception of empty and filled time intervals by pigeons pqjb04b-06.qxd 12/29/04 11:00 AM Page 31 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2005, 58B (1), 31 45 The perception of empty and filled time intervals by pigeons Andrew Miki and Angelo Santi

More information

Time flies when we read taboo words

Time flies when we read taboo words Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2010, 17 (4), 563-568 doi:10.3758/pbr.17.4.563 Time flies when we read taboo words JASON TIPPLES University of Hull, Hull, England Does time fly or stand still when one is

More information

Perception of the duration of auditory and visual stimuli in children and adults

Perception of the duration of auditory and visual stimuli in children and adults Q283 QJEP(A)42 / Jun, 4 (Tue)/ [22 pages 3 Tables 3 Figures 2 Footnotes Appendices].. Centre single caption. cf. [no comma]. disk edited RJ,. Graphics from word/xcel into cdraw. Please check THE QUARTERLY

More information

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 BLL R. BROWN AND THOMAS J. REBBN2 UNlVERSTY OF LOUSVLLE This experiment required Ss to make Markov-generated histoforms that were

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Fundamentals of Psychophysics Fundamentals of Psychophysics John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3045! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Visual neuroscience physiology stimulus How do we see the world? neuroimaging

More information

Categorical Perception

Categorical Perception Categorical Perception Discrimination for some speech contrasts is poor within phonetic categories and good between categories. Unusual, not found for most perceptual contrasts. Influenced by task, expectations,

More information

ABOUT THE INFLUENCE OF PERCEIVING DISGUSTED, HAPPY AND SAD FACES ON TIME PERCEPTION

ABOUT THE INFLUENCE OF PERCEIVING DISGUSTED, HAPPY AND SAD FACES ON TIME PERCEPTION ABOUT THE INFLUENCE OF PERCEIVING DISGUSTED, HAPPY AND SAD FACES ON TIME PERCEPTION Noémie de la Sablonnière, Vincent Laflamme, Katherine Labonté and Simon Grondin Université Laval, Québec, Canada noemie.de-la-sablonniere.1@ulaval.ca

More information

Rhythm Categorization in Context. Edward W. Large

Rhythm Categorization in Context. Edward W. Large Rhythm Categorization in Context Edward W. Large Center for Complex Systems Florida Atlantic University 777 Glades Road, P.O. Box 39 Boca Raton, FL 3343-99 USA large@walt.ccs.fau.edu Keywords: Rhythm,

More information

ON THE ROLE OF IMPUTED VELOCITY IN THE AUDITORY KAPPA EFFECT. Molly J. Henry. A Thesis

ON THE ROLE OF IMPUTED VELOCITY IN THE AUDITORY KAPPA EFFECT. Molly J. Henry. A Thesis ON THE ROLE OF IMPUTED VELOCITY IN THE AUDITORY KAPPA EFFECT Molly J. Henry A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling Green State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the

More information

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? Keyed. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 55B (1), 1 25 Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? J.H. Wearden, A. Parry, and L. Stamp University of

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University; Graduate School of Economics, Osaka University syamane@kindai.ac.jp Abstract We examined

More information

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities David P. McGovern, Andrew T. Astle, Sarah L. Clavin and Fiona N. Newell Figure S1: Group-averaged learning

More information

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2003 Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2003 Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination Psychology 4165, Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination Weight Discrimination Performance Probability of "Heavier" Response 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 50.0 100.0 150.0 200.0 250.0 Weight of Test Stimulus (grams)

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Timing in the Absence of Clocks: Encoding Time in Neural Network States

Timing in the Absence of Clocks: Encoding Time in Neural Network States Article Timing in the Absence of Clocks: Uma R. Karmarkar 1 and Dean V. Buonomano 2,3,4, * 1 University of California, Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, 230 Barker Hall #3190, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA

More information

CONTEXTUAL CHANGE EFFECTS IN RETROSPECTIVE AND PROSPECTIVE DURATION JUDGMENT PARADIGMS USING MUSICAL PIECES. Breanne Kay Smith

CONTEXTUAL CHANGE EFFECTS IN RETROSPECTIVE AND PROSPECTIVE DURATION JUDGMENT PARADIGMS USING MUSICAL PIECES. Breanne Kay Smith CONTEXTUAL CHANGE EFFECTS IN RETROSPECTIVE AND PROSPECTIVE DURATION JUDGMENT PARADIGMS USING MUSICAL PIECES by Breanne Kay Smith A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree

More information

EFFECT OF SPEED AND ACCURACY INSTRUCTIONS ON CONFIDENCE JUDGMENTS FOR MAGNITUDE ESTIMATES*.

EFFECT OF SPEED AND ACCURACY INSTRUCTIONS ON CONFIDENCE JUDGMENTS FOR MAGNITUDE ESTIMATES*. EFFECT OF SPEED AND ACCURACY INSTRUCTIONS ON CONFIDENCE JUDGMENTS FOR MAGNITUDE ESTIMATES*. Ana Garriga-Trillo C. Díaz-Berciano I. Serrano UNED CES San Pablo CEU UCM Madrid, Spain Madrid, Spain Madrid,

More information

STUDY OF THE BISECTION OPERATION

STUDY OF THE BISECTION OPERATION STUDY OF THE BISECTION OPERATION Sergio Cesare Masin University of Padua, Italy ABSTRACT Using the bisection procedure subjects divided initial sensory intervals in several intervals. The psychophysical

More information

Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns

Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns Perception & Psychophysics 1986, 40 (2), 69-73 Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns PETER J. ESSENS TNO Institute for Perception, Soesterberg, The Netherlands In two reproduction experiments,

More information

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Ethan D. Montag Rochester Institute of Technology Munsell Color Science Laboratory Chester F. Carlson Center for Imaging Science

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Fall 2001 Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Fall 2001 Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination Psychology 4165, Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination Weight Discrimination Performance Probability of "Heavier" Response 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 50.0 100.0 150.0 200.0 250.0 Weight of Test Stimulus (grams)

More information

Auditory Scene Analysis

Auditory Scene Analysis 1 Auditory Scene Analysis Albert S. Bregman Department of Psychology McGill University 1205 Docteur Penfield Avenue Montreal, QC Canada H3A 1B1 E-mail: bregman@hebb.psych.mcgill.ca To appear in N.J. Smelzer

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE Åke Hellström Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, S-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden Email: hellst@psychology.su.se Abstract Participants compared successive

More information

Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs

Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs Perception is the conscious experience of things and

More information

Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals

Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals Perception & Psychophysics 1993, 54 (3), 383-394 Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals SIMON GRONDIN Uniuersite Laurentienne, Sudbury, Ontario, Canada

More information

Chapter 2: Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges

Chapter 2: Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges Chapter 2: Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges JOHN H. WEARDEN Abstract Scalar timing (or scalar expectancy) theory, SET, was originally developed as an explanation

More information

MODELS FOR THE ADJUSTMENT OF RATING SCALES 1

MODELS FOR THE ADJUSTMENT OF RATING SCALES 1 MODELS FOR THE ADJUSTMENT OF RATING SCALES 1 Gert Haubensak and Peter Petzold Justus Liebig University of Giessen, Germany gert.haubensak@psychol.uni-giessen.de Abstract In a category rating experiment

More information

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION Karin Zimmer, R. Duncan Luce and Wolfgang Ellermeier Institut für Kognitionsforschung der Universität Oldenburg, Germany Institute

More information

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08 Auditory Perception - Detection versus Discrimination - Localization versus Discrimination - - Electrophysiological Measurements Psychophysical Measurements Three Approaches to Researching Audition physiology

More information

Does Rapid Auditory Stimulation Accelerate an Internal Pacemaker? Don t Bet on It

Does Rapid Auditory Stimulation Accelerate an Internal Pacemaker? Don t Bet on It Timing & Time Perception 1 (2013) 65 76 brill.com/time 1734 Does Rapid Auditory Stimulation Accelerate an Internal Pacemaker? Don t Bet on It Bruno H. Repp 1,, Hannah B. Mendlowitz 2 and Michael J. Hove

More information

SHORT REPORT Subsymmetries predict auditory and visual pattern complexity

SHORT REPORT Subsymmetries predict auditory and visual pattern complexity Perception, 2013, volume 42, pages 1095 1100 doi:10.1068/p7614 SHORT REPORT Subsymmetries predict auditory and visual pattern complexity Godfried T Toussaint, Juan F Beltran Faculty of Science, New York

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

Some methodological aspects for measuring asynchrony detection in audio-visual stimuli

Some methodological aspects for measuring asynchrony detection in audio-visual stimuli Some methodological aspects for measuring asynchrony detection in audio-visual stimuli Pacs Reference: 43.66.Mk, 43.66.Lj Van de Par, Steven ; Kohlrausch, Armin,2 ; and Juola, James F. 3 ) Philips Research

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08 Auditory Perception - Detection versus Discrimination - Localization versus Discrimination - Electrophysiological Measurements - Psychophysical Measurements 1 Three Approaches to Researching Audition physiology

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shikakeology: Designing Triggers for Behavior Change: Papers from the 2013 AAAI Spring Symposium The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University;

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay*

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay* Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay* JACOB BECK and BRUCE AMBLER University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 other extreme, when no masking

More information

Why ``Sound s Are Jud ged Longer T han Ligh ts : Application of a Model of th e Internal Clock in Hu m ans

Why ``Sound s Are Jud ged Longer T han Ligh ts : Application of a Model of th e Internal Clock in Hu m ans Ó THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1998, 51B (2), 97± 120 Why ``Sound s Are Jud ged Longer T han Ligh ts : Application of a Model of th e Internal Clock in Hu m ans J. H. Wearden, H. Edwards,

More information

Three methods for measuring perception. Magnitude estimation. Steven s power law. P = k S n

Three methods for measuring perception. Magnitude estimation. Steven s power law. P = k S n Three methods for measuring perception 1. Magnitude estimation 2. Matching 3. Detection/discrimination Magnitude estimation Have subject rate (e.g., 1-10) some aspect of a stimulus (e.g., how bright it

More information

Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration. John Wearden Keele University U.K.

Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration. John Wearden Keele University U.K. Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration John Wearden Keele University U.K. In this talk I want to illustrate the use of the techniques of production, reproduction, and verbal estimation

More information

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific?

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Temporal Binding and Internal Clocks: Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Richard Fereday (FeredayR@cardiff.ac.uk), Marc J. Buehner (BuehnerM@cardiff.ac.uk) Cardiff University, School of Psychology,

More information

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion Perception & Psychophysics 1980.28 (5). 465-470 Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion GREGORY M. CORSO Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia 30332 A pair of pure-tone sine

More information

Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures

Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this

More information

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 916-920 Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion JOHN PREDEBON University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia Two experiments

More information

Experimental and Computational Analyses of Strategy Usage in the Time-Left Task

Experimental and Computational Analyses of Strategy Usage in the Time-Left Task Experimental and Computational Analyses of Strategy Usage in the Time-Left Task Ben Meijering (B.Meijering@student.rug.nl) Hedderik van Rijn (D.H.van.Rijn@rug.nl) Department of Artificial Intelligence

More information

LIFETIME OF MEMORY FOR SELF-GENERATED INFORMATION

LIFETIME OF MEMORY FOR SELF-GENERATED INFORMATION LIFETIME OF MEMORY FOR SELF-GENERATED INFORMATION Lutz Munka and Christian Kaernbach Universität Leipzig, Germany munka@uni-leipzig.de, christian@kaernbach.de Abstract In experiments concerning the lifetime

More information

Stimulus Range Effects in Temporal Bisection by Humans

Stimulus Range Effects in Temporal Bisection by Humans q THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1996, 49B (1), 24± 44 Stimulus Range Effects in Temporal Bisection by Humans J.H. Wearden Ma nchester University, Ma nchester, U.K. A. Ferrara University

More information

How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled?

How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 21, 72 (7), 1839-1853 doi:1758/app.72.7.1839 How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled? MIDORI TOKITA

More information

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks*

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks* The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks* ARNOLD D. WELLt University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 Multidimensional stimuli, which could vary on one, two, or all three

More information

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2015 Laboratory 1 Noisy Representations: The Oblique Effect

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2015 Laboratory 1 Noisy Representations: The Oblique Effect Psychology 4165, Laboratory 1 Noisy Representations: The Oblique Effect Probability of Clockwise Response 0.0 0.4 0.8 Orientation Discrimination at 0.0 Deg 0 deg -10-5 0 5 10 Orientation of Test Stimulus

More information

How Emotional Auditory Stimuli Modulate Time Perception

How Emotional Auditory Stimuli Modulate Time Perception Emotion Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 7, No. 4, 697 704 1528-3542/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.697 How Emotional Auditory Stimuli Modulate Time Perception

More information

Lecture 12: Psychophysics and User Studies

Lecture 12: Psychophysics and User Studies ME 327: Design and Control of Haptic Systems Autumn 2018 Lecture 12: Psychophysics and User Studies Allison M. Okamura Stanford University Reminders The last ~20 minutes of today s lecture will be in 520-145

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information ten Oever and Sack 10.1073/pnas.1517519112 SI Materials and Methods Experiment 1. Participants. A total of 20 participants (9 male; age range 18 32 y; mean age 25 y) participated

More information

THE PRESENTATION OF LONG TERM DURATION OF BODY MOVEMENT IN IMPRESSIONIST ARTWORKS DIFFERENTLY DISTORT THE PERCEPTION OF TIME

THE PRESENTATION OF LONG TERM DURATION OF BODY MOVEMENT IN IMPRESSIONIST ARTWORKS DIFFERENTLY DISTORT THE PERCEPTION OF TIME THE PRESENTATION OF LONG TERM DURATION OF BODY MOVEMENT IN IMPRESSIONIST ARTWORKS DIFFERENTLY DISTORT THE PERCEPTION OF TIME Francisco Carlos Nather and José Lino Oliveira Bueno Department of Psychology,

More information

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias Lewis O. Harvey, Jr. Department of Psychology University of Colorado Boulder, Colorado The Brain (Observable) Stimulus System (Observable) Response System

More information

PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES?

PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES? PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS AND ERGONOMICS SOCIETY 8th ANNUAL MEETING 88 PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES? J. E. Thropp,

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

The Color of Similarity

The Color of Similarity The Color of Similarity Brooke O. Breaux (bfo1493@louisiana.edu) Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, Lafayette, LA 70504 USA Michele I. Feist (feist@louisiana.edu) Institute

More information

The perception of time is fundamental to our

The perception of time is fundamental to our Time perception: the bad news and the good William J. Matthews 1 and Warren H. Meck 2 Time perception is fundamental and heavily researched, but the field faces a number of obstacles to theoretical progress.

More information

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 J'OURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2, 446-452 (1963) Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 A visual configuration, or pattern, has qualities over and above those which can be specified

More information

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings Perception & Psychophysics 1980,27 (6),574-578 The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings ALISON BOWLING and WILLIAM LOVEGROVE University oftasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia 700/

More information

CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT

CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT PACS:..Hy Furuya, Hiroshi ; Wakuda, Akiko ; Anai, Ken ; Fujimoto, Kazutoshi Faculty of Engineering, Kyushu Kyoritsu University

More information

Consciousness The final frontier!

Consciousness The final frontier! Consciousness The final frontier! How to Define it??? awareness perception - automatic and controlled memory - implicit and explicit ability to tell us about experiencing it attention. And the bottleneck

More information

INTRODUCTION. Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA Electronic mail:

INTRODUCTION. Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA Electronic mail: Level discrimination of sinusoids as a function of duration and level for fixed-level, roving-level, and across-frequency conditions Andrew J. Oxenham a) Institute for Hearing, Speech, and Language, and

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Acta Psychologica xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Acta Psychologica xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica Acta Psychologica xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy Timing of multiple overlapping intervals: How many clocks

More information

Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception

Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception Sundeep Teki Evaluating the roles of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum in time perception Auditory Cognition Group Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging SENSORY CORTEX SMA/PRE-SMA HIPPOCAMPUS BASAL

More information

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity S. B. Stevenson *, L. K. Cormack Vision Research 40, 2881-2884. (2000) * University of Houston College of Optometry, Houston TX 77204

More information

Classic experiments in sensation and perception

Classic experiments in sensation and perception 1 Classic experiments in sensation and perception PSYC420/PSYC921: Advanced Topics in Cognitive Psychology Summary We will use the course to develop and design a number of classic experiments into online

More information

Karl von Vierordt: His work and his legacy. John Wearden Keele University, UK

Karl von Vierordt: His work and his legacy. John Wearden Keele University, UK Karl von Vierordt: His work and his legacy John Wearden Keele University, UK This talk Consists of 3 parts (of unequal length) Firstly, I want to talk (briefly) about Vierordt s work in physiology and

More information

Visual Transformation of Size

Visual Transformation of Size Journal ol Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1975, Vol. 1, No. 3, 214-220 Visual Transformation of Size Glaus Bundesen and Axel Larsen Copenhagen University, Denmark To investigate

More information

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2016 Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2016 Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness Psychology 4165, Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness Linear-Linear Linear-Linear 0.002 0.005 0.020 0.050 0.200 0.500 2 5 10 20 50 100 200 0.002 0.005 0.020 0.050 0.200 0.500 Lab Overview Loudness is a

More information

Psychophysics & a brief intro to the nervous system

Psychophysics & a brief intro to the nervous system Psychophysics & a brief intro to the nervous system Jonathan Pillow Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017 Lec. 3 Outline for today: psychophysics Weber-Fechner Law Signal Detection

More information

so that a respondent may choose one of the categories to express a judgment about some characteristic of an object or of human behavior.

so that a respondent may choose one of the categories to express a judgment about some characteristic of an object or of human behavior. Effects of Verbally Labeled Anchor Points on the Distributional Parameters of Rating Measures Grace French-Lazovik and Curtis L. Gibson University of Pittsburgh The hypothesis was examined that the negative

More information

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES Varinthira Duangudom and David V Anderson School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Georgia Institute of Technology Atlanta, GA 30332

More information

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) 1 Outline Method of Adjustment Method of Limits Method of Constant Stimuli Probit Analysis 2 Method of Constant Stimuli A set of equally spaced levels of the stimulus

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Detection Theory: Sensory and Decision Processes

Detection Theory: Sensory and Decision Processes Detection Theory: Sensory and Decision Processes Lewis O. Harvey, Jr. Department of Psychology and Neuroscience University of Colorado Boulder The Brain (observed) Stimuli (observed) Responses (observed)

More information

Laboratory exercise: Adaptive tracks and psychometric functions

Laboratory exercise: Adaptive tracks and psychometric functions AUDL 47 / PLING 34 Auditory Perception Laboratory exercise: Adaptive tracks and psychometric functions The purpose of this assignment is familiarise you with the notion of psychometric functions and adaptive

More information

Task-oriented Bayesian inference in interval timing: People use their prior reproduction experience to calibrate time reproduction

Task-oriented Bayesian inference in interval timing: People use their prior reproduction experience to calibrate time reproduction Task-oriented Bayesian inference in interval timing: People use their prior reproduction experience to calibrate time reproduction Zhenguang Cai (Zhenguang.cai@uea.ac.uk) School of Psychology, University

More information

Do We Have a Common Mechanism for Measuring Time in the Hundreds of Millisecond Range? Evidence From Multiple-Interval Timing Tasks

Do We Have a Common Mechanism for Measuring Time in the Hundreds of Millisecond Range? Evidence From Multiple-Interval Timing Tasks J Neurophysiol 99: 939 949, 28. First published December 19, 27; doi:1.1152/jn.1225.27. Do We Have a Common Mechanism for Measuring Time in the Hundreds of Millisecond Range? Evidence From Multiple-Interval

More information