Submitted Article Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last?

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Submitted Article Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last?"

Transcription

1 Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy (2011) volume 33, number 3, pp doi: /aepp/ppr019 Submitted Article Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last? Robin Dillaway, Kent D. Messer*, John C. Bernard, and Harry M. Kaiser Robin Dillaway is a graduate of the Department of Food and Resource Economics at the University of Delaware. Kent D. Messer is an associate professor and John C. Bernard is a professor in the Department of Food and Resource Economics, and the Department of Economics, respectively, at the University of Delaware. Harry M. Kaiser is the Gellert Family Professor of Applied Economics and Management at Cornell University. *Correspondence may be sent to: messer@udel.edu. Submitted 18 June 2010; accepted 17 March Abstract Using experimental methods with adult subjects from the mid- Atlantic region of the United States, this research examines both the short- and longer-term impacts of media information on consumer purchasing behavior. Subjects in the treatment group were given food safety information about poultry from a popular consumer magazine. Willingness to pay (WTP) estimates were then elicited for two types of chicken breasts: (1) a leading-brand that was identified in the information treatment as having a high incidence of Campylobacter and Salmonella bacteria; and (2) a lesser known brand, which was reported as being relatively free of harmful bacteria. Results indicated that both negative and positive food safety information significantly impacted consumers WTP for safer chicken compared to the reportedly less-safe leading-brand chicken. These changes in behavior persisted throughout the seven-week study period. Key words: Consumer behavior, food safety, experimental economics, media information. JEL Codes: Q13, D83, C91. Introduction The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) estimates that there are approximately 76 million illnesses, 325,000 hospitalizations, and 5,000 deaths annually caused by food-borne diseases in the United States (Mead et al. 1999). The U.S. Department of Agriculture s Economic Research Service estimates that food-borne illnesses from the top five pathogens affecting humans cost society $6.9 billion annually (Crutchfield and Roberts, 2000). Moreover, a recent study estimates that food-borne illness has a societal cost of $357 billion annually (Roberts, 2007). Clearly, # The Author(s) Published by Oxford University Press, on behalf of Agricultural and Applied Economics Association. All rights reserved. For permissions, please journals.permissions@oup.com. 363

2 Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy food-borne illnesses are a health risk as well as an economic burden on society, yet the effect of food-borne illnesses on consumer behavior is not well understood. The issue of whether and how quickly consumers return to their previous behavior after receiving media food safety information is important to consider when studying the effect of information on consumers purchasing behaviors. However, research on how quickly the impact of food safety information decays is scarce. Accordingly, the focus of this research is to provide a better understanding of consumer reactions to media food safety information over time. Media reports about food safety concerns are relatively common. Consider for example, the 2006 Escherichia coli outbreak in spinach, which resulted in 204 illnesses, 104 hospitalizations, and 3 deaths (Calvin 2007). In September 2006, the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) advised consumers not to eat bagged spinach and expanded the warning the following day to fresh spinach. This resulted in a five-day period during which no fresh spinach was sold in the United States, while California spinach remained off the market for ten days (Calvin 2007). In the longer term, sales of bagged spinach were depressed for months afterward. Compared to the same period from the previous year, retail sales of bagged spinach were down 27% five months after the outbreak (Calvin 2007). Knowing more about the effects of food safety information on consumer behavior is of particular importance to government agencies. Agencies such as the CDC and FDA issue numerous warnings about specific food products. For instance, the CDC reports that in 2006, there were 1,247 food-borne outbreaks (CDC 2008). Given the large number of reported outbreaks, their impact on consumers can be important. Media reports are an important source of information about food safety issues for consumers. The degree of coverage a food safety incident receives is likely to influence consumer decisions. However, little is known about how consumers react to situations where there is an initial burst of media information about the safety of a food product and then relatively little information is provided as the media focuses its attention on other topics, which is common practice, as food safety tends to receive sporadic coverage in most media outlets. This study differs from most of the previous literature in that it seeks to understand both short- and longer-term consumer responses to food safety concerns by eliciting willingness to pay (WTP) for poultry products using experimental economics methods. This study expands upon previous experiments on food safety with its multiple sessions, which allows for an examination of the dynamic processes that exist in the real world. Most previous experiments, (for example, Hayes et al. 1995; Lusk and Schroeder 2002; and Thomsen and McKenzie 2001,) which consist of a single, isolated session, are unable to show consumers changing attitudes and WTP over time. To our knowledge, the seven-week study period used here is the longest used by an experimental study of this kind. Other studies (for example Saghaian 2007; Beach et al. 2008) of consumer response to food safety scares have traditionally relied upon aggregate market data and do not track specific individuals to see how their behavior changes over time, as the experimental framework here allows. Our study involved 110 adult participants who were randomly assigned to either the control group or the treatment group. Each subject participated in three sessions over seven weeks. Participants in the control 364

3 Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last? treatment were given no food safety information. Participants in the food safety treatment were given information from a Consumer Reports magazine article, which stated that during the testing of leading brands of chicken (identified specifically by Consumer Reports as Perdue, Tyson, Foster Farms, and Pilgrim s Pride) 1 high rates of contamination of harmful bacteria were found. Ranger brand chicken, however, was relatively clean of these bacteria (Consumer Reports 2007). Analysis of the WTP data indicated that participants in the treatment group were willing to pay more for the Ranger chicken (a relatively safer product). Results from this study indicated that consumers were strongly affected by food safety information both immediately and over time. Relevant Literature The impact of food safety information on consumers WTP has been well established in both experimental and empirical studies. In general, the experimental studies have used controls available in a laboratory to measure consumers WTP at a single moment in time, while the empirical studies have employed econometric techniques to examine the change in consumer behavior over time. That food safety information affects consumer purchasing decisions is well established in the literature with some studies focusing on the effect of general information alone, others focusing on positive information, others on negative information, and some using a combination of both. The experiments of Hayes et al. (1995) demonstrated that the availability of information can change consumers purchasing habits in response to perceived risk, as consumers in this study were willing to pay a premium for safer food. The authors also found that experimental subjects tended to underestimate the probability of a foodborne illness. Several empirical studies have also shown food demand to be affected by food safety concerns. For instance, Piggitt and Marsh (2004) found a relatively small downward demand shift in response to food safety concerns, while Marsh, Schroeder, and Mintert (2004) found a significant downward demand shift in response to Food Safety Inspection Service recalls. Literature on food product recalls has also shown negative market impacts (for example, Lusk and Schroeder 2002; Thomsen and McKenzie 2001). Studies that have focused on either negative or positive information (for example Lusk et al. 2001) have shown that negative information decreases a consumers WTP, while positive information increases consumers WTP. For example, the experimental results of Lusk et al. (2001) showed a premium involving positive information regarding the tenderness of steak. That is, consumers were willing to pay a premium when given a taste test that included information concerning the tenderness of a steak compared to a taste test with no information. Results of the study indicated that consumers value quality (as indicated by tenderness in this study) in the beef market. In another experimental study concerning consumer WTP, Stenger (2000) showed that information resulted in a significant increase in WTP for vegetables grown without the use of sewage 1 All brand names are described as they were reported in Consumer Reports. The authors make no claims to the accuracy of this information. 365

4 Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy sludge as a fertilizer over their current spending levels for fruits and vegetables, despite consumers perception that the health risk from such a fertilizer treatment was small. Saghaian s (2007) observational study concerning BSE food safety shocks also shows that negative information affects prices downward over a ten-week period. In this study, retail prices lag from a week to ten days behind producer price drops and are less severe. When consumers receive information about food safety, this information is often mixed with both positive and negative messages. Several studies have found that negative information has a stronger effect than positive information. For example, Fox, Hayes, and Shogren s (2002) study of irradiated pork tested favorable versus unfavorable information to determine which had a stronger effect on consumers WTP at the time of purchase. Using three treatments (negative information, positive information, and a balanced treatment involving both positive and negative information), the study demonstrated that providing negative information had a much stronger influence, even when the negative information comes from a nonscientific source. In a study using real-life case reports concerning genetically modified foods, Hu, Zhong, and Ding (2006) found that positive information did not significantly affect participants WTP, but negative information significantly decreased WTP. Several studies have also considered consumers response to food safety information over time. Brown, Cranfield, and Henson (2005) found that WTP drops markedly as participants become more tolerant of risk; their results from a one-session experiment indicated that the WTP of consumers who initially overvalued the risk of food-borne illness tended to increase their WTP as their tolerance for risk increased. The study did not contain a longer-term time component to determine if participants returned to previous levels of tolerance for risk over time. Hammitt and Haninger s (2007) stated-interest survey results indicate that participants WTP is insensitive to duration between one and seven days. Shogren, List and Hayes (2000) conducted an experimental study involving consecutive auctions eliciting participants WTP for food products over a two week period. The authors found that participants WTP changed over time. Given the strong effect that information has regarding immediate consumers food purchasing behavior, food safety concerns can be expected to change behavior over time. Studies that have used observational methodologies have indicated that consumer behavior is affected by the passage of time. Beach et al. (2008) demonstrated in their study of the influence of newspaper stories about avian influenza on Italian poultry sales that negative media information had a persistent effect lasting up to five weeks. The reasons for these variations in the duration of impacts on consumer behavior are difficult to unravel. Saghaian s (2007) observational study of the effect of BSE on U.S. markets found downward price effects during the ten-week study period, forecasted up to 15 weeks. Thomsen, Shiptsova, and Hamm (2006) found that sales of a recalled brand remained depressed for approximately eight to twelve weeks and did not fully recover for four to five months. These results were specific to a particular brand that was affected by a recall and did not pertain to non-branded food commodity markets. 366

5 Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last? Experimental Design In empirical studies, a key concern is whether all consumers have the same access to information regarding the safety of food products. Additionally, little is known about how consumer behavior changes as information concerning the incident becomes less available over time. To help control for these influences, this study used an experimental economics lab setting. To date, essentially all experimental studies have looked at consumer behavior response to food safety information as captured in a single session, and have not conducted the more difficult task of repeatedly bringing adult subjects to a lab setting to evaluate change in WTP over time. Our experimental design used within-subject comparisons of WTP for two different types of chicken breasts and between-subject comparisons of WTP for two different treatment groups one with food safety information and one without this information. This design was structured to test the following five primary research questions in order to understand how positive and negative information affected participants in both the shortand longer-term: (1) Did consumers have different preferences for the two chicken types when food safety information was not provided? (2) Did consumers have different preferences for the two chicken types when food safety information was provided? (3) Did brand-related food safety information decrease consumers WTP for a relatively less safe product? (4) Did brand-related food safety information increase consumers WTP for a relatively safer product? (5) Did consumers WTP for any type of chicken in either treatment group change over time after no additional food safety information was provided? In this study, 110 adults participated in the experiment. 2 Participants were recruited from a large northeast university s lifelong learning campus (for students aged 50 and over), as well as from the university s staff, and public attendees of an annual event open to the general public. The latter experiments were conducted in the Experimental Economics Laboratory for Policy and Decision Research at the University of Delaware. The study was widely advertised as an experiment in decision-making that would require participation in three sessions. These respective settings were chosen because they provided an easily accessible laboratory setting for participants that were available in the same building, at the same time and day of the week for several weeks. All experimental sessions were conducted between April and July The subject pool was not selected to represent the entire United States or even a regional area. 3 As shown in table 1, to examine short- and longer-term impacts on WTP, the 2 Initially, 119 subjects participated in the first session, nine of whom dropped out of the study. Therefore, 92.4% of the subjects who attended the first session attended all three sessions. 3 The sample population used in this study had many participants over the age of 50. The administrators made every effort to ensure that all participants were fully capable of participating through several rounds of instructions. It was not within the scope of this study to determine if older participants were less capable of operating computers, read more carefully, were more risk averse, were more likely to be exposed to food safety concerns, or were more rigid in their beliefs. These behaviors may be possible with an older population. 367

6 Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy Table 1 Experiment structure Session 1 (1 hour) Part A 1. Participants read instructions 2. Verbal instructions 3. Practice round; WTP bid for pen Part B 1. Participants read instructions 2. Verbal instructions 3. Presentation of food safety information if session is Treatment group 4. Presentation of products to participants 5. WTP bids for leading-brand chicken, Ranger brand chicken, fettuccine pasta, and one dozen eggs Session 2 (7 days later, 15 minutes) Part B only, no additional information 1. Participants read instructions 2. Verbal instructions 3. WTP bids for leading-brand chicken, Ranger brand chicken, fettuccine pasta, and one dozen eggs Session 3 (28 days or 49 days later, 15 minutes) Part B only, no additional information 1. Participants read instructions 2. Verbal instructions 3. WTP bids for leading-brand chicken, Ranger brand chicken, fettuccine pasta, and one dozen eggs experiment consisted of three sessions. In the first session, which lasted one hour, participants WTP was elicited for each of four products. This was followed by a second 15-minute session held at the same time and place one week after the first session. No further food safety information was given to the participants at the second session. Participants repeated part B of the experiment, which involved bidding on the four products. The third session was held 28 days later for half of the participants and 49 days later for the other half. 4 This division was necessary to accommodate 4 A limiting factor for the length of the study was the semester length at the lifelong learning facility, which ended eight weeks after the beginning of this study. Thus, extending the study beyond seven weeks would have likely meant a much lower percentage of participants completing all three sessions. 368

7 Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last? scheduling conflicts, such as a national holiday. Both treatments were equally represented in the different lengths of time for the third sessions. In both treatments, the third session lasted fifteen minutes, and Part B was repeated with no further food safety information presented. While 119 subjects initially participated in the first session, nine of these subjects subsequently dropped out of the study. Therefore, 92.4% of the subjects who attended the first session attended all three sessions. Participants earned $60 in cash and/or products for the experiment. The payments for all participants were $11 in cash and/or products in session 1, $10 in cash and/or products in session 2, and the remaining payment in cash and/or products in session 3. A modification of the sealed-bid English auction mechanism was used to elicit WTP estimates due to its ease of explanation and its demandrevealing and incentive-compatible properties (Davis and Holt 1993; Bernard 2006). Using this auction mechanism, subjects incentives were to bid their highest WTP and therefore reveal their true demand preferences. Compared to second-price auctions, traditional English auctions are better able to measure participants WTP, but have the disadvantage of having bids visible to everyone in the experiment session, which can lead to potential group-effects on individual behavior. Lusk and Shogren s (2007) review of different auction methods also point out the merits of the English auction, as well as the random n th price auction. The sealed-bid English auction retains the benefit of the increasing bid clock while keeping bids hidden (Bernard, 2006). In this study, we used a modification of the traditional English Auction. We refer to this auction as a sealed, random n th bid English Auction. All of the bids were submitted confidentially so that participants were unaware of what other participants in their experimental session were bidding. This research used $10 as the maximum bid since this was the amount of the initial balance given to all participants and was significantly higher than the market price for 1.5 pounds of chicken breasts. In these experiments, the highest bid submitted was $9.90 for Ranger chicken. Participants were informed using both written and oral instructions that the optimal strategy was to bid their actual highest WTP for each product. Therefore, they needed to determine their personal highest WTP for the product being auctioned and submit that amount as their bid. A computer program that employed Excel spreadsheets and programmed using Visual Basic for Applications was used to confidentially record participants WTP (Messer, Kaiser, and Schulze, 2008). Participants were instructed to stop the program when the displayed price reached her/his maximum WTP. The program initially showed a $0.00 price. Subjects wanting to submit a bid of $0 were provided a Withdraw button at this initial screen. Participants wanting to submit bids greater than zero were instructed to start by clicking the Start button. When they did so, the program began increasing the price in one-cent increments at a uniform time interval until the participant clicked a button marked Withdraw. If the Withdraw button was never clicked, the program would record the maximum bid allowed ($1 for the training round and $10 for the four products). The maximum price was always equal to the participants initial balance of funds. To help ensure that participants did not get a sense of what other participants were bidding, it was necessary to allow the clock to run up to the maximum bid of $

8 Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy The price for the item was determined by the highest rejected bid. At the end of each auction, bids were arranged by the administrator s computer program from highest to lowest and the number of purchasers was randomly determined. The subject with the highest bid was the first purchaser, and so on. The number of purchasers was determined randomly by having a volunteer roll a six-sided die with the number of purchasers therefore ranging from one to six for sessions with seven or more participants. 5 For example, if the number of participants in a session was ten, and the result of the die roll was five, the five highest bidders would purchase the product and pay a price equal to the bid of the sixth highest bidder. Prior to conducting the experiments, participants were randomly assigned to either a treatment group (n ¼ 56) that received media-based food safety information, or a control group (n ¼ 54) that received no media information. The average number of participants in a session was nine, with a range from three to sixteen. Verbal and oral instructions were provided to improve the understanding of experiment procedures (see the Review Appendix). Questions were encouraged during all stages of the experiment. Subjects were seated at individual computer terminals equipped with privacy screens so that all decisions would be made confidentially. The first session was divided into two parts (table 1). Following Messer et al. (2011), Part A consisted of a practice round where participants bid on a pen. This practice round helped ensure that participants understood how the bidding process worked and how to stop the computer program at the desired WTP. For the pen, the initial balance provided to each subject was $1. Participants were instructed to bid zero if they valued the product at $0 or less, and were instructed to bid $1 if they valued the product at $1 or greater. If their value for the pen was between $0 and $1, they were instructed to stop the computer program at the price that represented their highest WTP. During the training round, questions were answered to ensure that participants understood the program and procedure. In part B, the initial balance was $10 for each product. Participants were permitted to bid between $0 and $10. In part B, four products were auctioned: 1) Frozen boneless skinless chicken breasts from a leading-brand such as Foster Farms, Perdue, Pilgrim s Pride, or Tyson (approximately pounds). 2) Frozen boneless skinless chicken breasts from Ranger (approximately pounds).6 5 For sessions with less than seven participants, the maximum number of purchasers was k 1, where k was the number of participants. In some cases, a four-sided die was used. The number of purchases was determined randomly using an n th -price auction because of the repeated nature of these experiments. For instance, if a standard second-price auction was used, then after the first session, most subjects would have a good sense of whether their bid for the selected product in the subsequent sessions would be close to the highest bid in their group. The use of an n th -price auction helped ensure that essentially none of the bidders would be off-margin. 6 Ranger brand chicken is only available in the Pacific Northwest. To avoid deceiving subjects, over 70 pounds (nearly 50 packages) of Ranger brand frozen boneless skinless chicken breast were shipped overnight express from Bellingham, Washington, packaged in dry ice and in freezer containers, at a cost of over a thousand dollars. 370

9 Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last? 3) Eggs (one dozen, size large). 4) Fettuccine pasta (one pound). All original packaging information was removed from the products prior to the experiment to control for any reaction individuals might have to the packaging. Both types of chicken were displayed in clear, one-gallon sealed freezer bags. Participants were informed that if they purchased the chicken, we would package the chicken in ice, if desired, to ensure that the chicken would remain frozen until they returned home. 7 Before the bidding began, an administrator walked around the room and displayed each of the products. Participants were asked not to touch the products, since the products would be distributed after the experiment to the purchasers, but they were permitted to visually inspect each of them as closely as they wished. To prevent potential order effects, the order in which the products were displayed was determined using a Latin squares design. 8 Participants bid on all four products, but only one product was actually purchased similar to the procedures designed by Bernard, Zhang, and Gifford (2006). The purchased product was randomly predetermined using a four-sided die and was written on an index card. The card was sealed inside an envelope, which was opened by a volunteer after all of the auctions were completed. Prior studies have shown that, in a multiple-round auction such as this one, randomly determining the product that is purchased helps to elicit WTP among participants (see Lusk, Feldkamp, and Schroeder (2004); Hayes et al. (1995); and Messer et al. (2010)). This procedure compensates for the potential that a participant purchased a product in one session and therefore decreased her or his WTP in subsequent sessions. It was possible for the same product to be binding in more than one session. The binding product in this experiment was randomly determined for each session in an effort to reduce the potential effects across sessions caused by a subject receiving a product in one session and having to bid again for the same product in subsequent sessions. These procedures were used since this experiment involved multiple sessions with the same products. In such cases, there is a potential that participants who purchase a product in one session will bid lower than their true WTP in subsequent sessions since they already have the product. However, this effect was minimized since the actual quantity of chicken sold during the experiments was small. Therefore, it was important to make the binding auctions random in order to minimize this type of effect on participant WTP. After bids for all four products were collected, the predetermined product was distributed to purchasers and used to calculate cash earnings. 7 All of the chicken was frozen so that consumers would have less concern about potential food safety issues related to the administrator s handling of the chicken, and to minimize concerns that the chicken would need to be eaten immediately. This latter reason was important in the case that participants were not returning to their homes immediately after the research, as well as to ensure that the potential useful life of this product was as long as possible, so that consumers WTP would not be significantly affected by the amount of chicken that they might have just recently purchased at a grocery store prior to the session. 8 A Latin Squares design is used here as a method of varying the order of products. For instance, if in the first session, products A, B, and C are introduced in that order, in the next session, they will be introduced in the order B, C, A. In the following session they will be in the order C, A, B, and so on. 371

10 Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy At the end of the experiment a short survey was then distributed in each session to collect demographic data, as well as information about subjects beliefs about the safety of food products, and whether subjects received additional food safety information between sessions of the experiment. The information provided to the treatment group about the two chicken products is included in appendix 1. Results The participants WTP bids for chicken are displayed in the four panels of figure 1, which shows the demand for each chicken type in each session and in each treatment group. The figures plot WTP on the y-axis against the percentage of participants willing to pay at a given price on the x-axis. Figure 1 WTP for approximately pounds of chicken breasts, by type and treatment 372

11 Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last? The legends of each figure provide the mean and standard deviation related to each demand curve. To answer the research questions outlined in the experimental design section, the participants responses were analyzed in several different ways. Normality tests were performed on WTP data for the leading-brand chicken and the Ranger brand of chicken using STATA s sktest, which measures skewness and kurtosis to test for normality. The results of the skewness and kurtosis tests indicated that the data were not normally distributed. Thus, the WTP bids were analyzed with nonparametric Wilcoxon rank-sum tests to analyze differences between the two treatment groups (table 2). Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were used to test for statistical significance within experiment sessions and within chicken types. Additionally, two-limit Tobit regression models were also used to explain participants WTP for chicken. A two-limit Tobit was required, since participants bids were constrained at $0 and $10 in the experiment. The model included data from all three sessions, for both chicken types from both the control and treatment groups. This model thus included six observations from each of the 103 non-vegetarian subjects bidding three times each for two brands of chicken. A random-effects model was used since each participant had multiple observations in the panel data set. Marginal-effect coefficients were calculated to translate the regression coefficients into WTP. The model included an information variable measuring the impact of the food safety information in the study on participants WTP. The variable info was also interacted with all other independent variables to find the effects of food safety information on subgroups. Table 3 provides descriptive statistics on the variables in the following model: wtp_chicken ¼ b 0 + b 1 (info) + b 2 (ranger) + b 3 (ranger*info) + b 4 (days) + b 5 (days 2 ) + b 6 (days*info) + b 7 (days 2 *info) + b 8 (ranger*days) + b 9 (ranger*days 2 ) + b 10 (ranger*info*days) + b 11 (ranger*info*days 2 ) + b 12 (age) + b 13 (age*info) + b 14 (female) + b 15 (female*info) + b 16 (children) + b 17 (children*info) + b 18 (primary_shopper) + b 19 (primary_shopper*info) + b 20 (education) + b 21 (education*info) + b 22 (nonwhite)+ b 23 (nonwhite*info)+ b 24 (p_chickensafe) + b 25 (p_chickensafe*info) + b 26 (income)+ b 27 (income*info)+ b 28 (eat_chicken_often)+ b 29 (eat_chicken_often*info) + 1. The variables p_chickensafe and p_chickensafe*info were constructed using survey responses from participants indicating whether they considered chicken to be a safe product. A Hausman test for endogeneity indicated that the p_chickensafe variable was endogenous. The residual term was a significant predictor of the dependent variable ( p ¼ 0.023). Therefore, instrumental variables were constructed for p_chickensafe and p_chickensafe*info using auxiliary regressions of these variables on all exogenous variables in model. Tests of Research Questions The first research question was, Did consumers have different preferences for the two chicken types when food safety information was not provided? A visual inspection of panels A and C in figure 1 suggests that the demand curves for leading-brand chicken were slightly higher than the demand curves for the Ranger brand. As can be seen in table 2, panel A, on average in the three sessions, participants were willing to pay between 373

12 Table 2 Sets of tests of differences in WTP with Wilcoxon significance tests. H 0 : WTP for Ranger brand - WTP for Leading-brand 5 0 Panel Control Prob> z Panel Treatment Prob> z A Session ¼ B ¼ Session ¼ ¼ Session ¼ ¼ All Sessions ¼ NA ¼ 0.55 NA Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy H 0 : WTP for Treatment group WTP for Control group ¼ 0 Leading-brand Prob. z Ranger brand Prob. z 374 C Session ¼ D ¼ Session ¼ ¼ Session ¼ ¼ All Sessions ¼ NA ¼ 0.18 NA H 0 : WTP for Session 1 WTP for Session 2 ¼ 0 Leading-brand Prob. z Ranger brand Prob. z E Control ¼ F ¼ Treatment ¼ ¼ H 0 : WTP for Session 2 WTP for Session 3 ¼ 0 Leading-brand Prob. z Ranger brand Prob. z G Control ¼ H ¼ Treatment ¼ ¼ H 0 : WTP for Session 1 WTP for Session 3 ¼ 0 Leading-brand Prob. z Ranger brand Prob. z I Control ¼ J ¼ Treatment ¼ ¼

13 Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last? Table 3 Variables in the Tobit model. Variable Mean Standard deviation Minimum Maximum wtp_chicken - WTP for both leading-brand and Ranger chicken. info - 1 for the treatment group and 0 otherwise. ranger - 1 for WTP for Ranger and 0 otherwise. days - Number of days since the first session. days 2 - Number of days since the first session squared. age-19if, 20; 24.5 if between 20 and 29; 34.5 if between 30 and 39,...; 84.5 if between 80 and 89. female- 1 for female and 0 otherwise. children- 1 if participant has children under 18 living at home and 0 otherwise. primary_shopper - 1 if participant is primary shopper in household and 0 otherwise. education- 0 if less than college degree, 1 if college degree, and 2 if more than a college degree. nonwhite - 1 if nonwhite racial designation and 0 otherwise. p_chickensafe - Instrumental variable constructed from participant responses whether they consider chicken safe. income- Categorical household annual income variable ranging from 0 ($0 - $39,999) to 10 (over $200,000) by $40,000 increments. eat_chicken_often - 0 if participant eats chicken never; 1 if rarely; 2 if once a month; 3 if several times a month; 4 if once a week; and 5 if several times a week $0.15 and $0.29 less for a pound and a half of the chicken breasts. However, these differences were not statistically significant in any session. For the pooled data used in the two-limit Tobit analysis, the coefficient on the variable ranger was again negative (-0.273) and not statistically significant (p ¼ 0.216) (table 4). Recall that participants were given information which stated that Ranger brand was safer. The second question, Did consumers have different 375

14 Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy Table 4 Two-limit Tobit results, WTP for chicken. Marginal effect Standard error P> z constant info ranger ranger*info days days days*info days 2 *info ranger*days ranger*days ranger*info*days ranger*info*days age age*info female female*info children children*info primary_shopper primary_shopper*info education education*info nonwhite nonwhite*info p_chickensafe p_chickensafe*info income income*info eat_chicken_often eat_chicken_often*info Sum of all info coefficients F test statistic Wald chi 2 ¼ Prob. chi 2 ¼ Log likelihood ¼ Left-censored observations 79 Uncensored observations 455 Right-censored observations 0 Note: Since some of the survey questions were left blank by subjects, some observations were not included in the final model, reducing the total number of observations to 534. preferences for the two chicken types when food safety information was provided, contrasted WTP in the information treatment for the two chicken types. Inspection of figure 1 shows a marked increase in WTP between leading-brand chicken ( panel D) and Ranger chicken ( panel B). These differences in average WTP ranged from $0.36 to $0.94, which were statistically significant in session 1 ( p ¼ 0.000), session 2 ( p ¼ 0.036), and session 3(p ¼ 0.012) (table 2, panel B). This result was further supported by the Tobit model results where the increase in WTP for Ranger from the food safety information was $1.24 when all other factors were held constant 376

15 Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last? (p ¼ 0.000). All of this evidence suggested that the null hypothesis can be rejected, as participants in the treatment group were willing to pay more for Ranger chicken than for leading-brand chicken. This response to the positive information for Ranger chicken 9 corroborated results found in Lusk et al. (2001) and Stenger (2000). The third research question, Did brand-related food safety information decrease consumers WTP for a relatively less safe product, related to whether information affects WTP for chicken. Since all variables were interacted with the information variable in the Tobit analysis, the derivative of info was calculated by adding the coefficient for info and all variables interacted with info. A model-specification F-test indicated that the info variable had a coefficient of and was significant at the p, 0.01 significance level (table 4). The effect of information was most pronounced in the lowering of WTP for leading-brand chicken. For instance, as shown in panel C of table 2, participants who were given food safety information from Consumer Reports were willing to pay, on average, $0.65 less in the first session for leading-brand chicken than participants who were not given this information. This difference was statistically significant (p ¼ ) (table 2, panel C). The $0.56 and $0.58 average differences in the second ( p ¼ ) and third ( p ¼ ) sessions, respectively, were not significant at the p.05 significance level, but were significant at the 0.10 level. Taken together, this suggested that the answer to the third research question was that consumers do reduce their WTP for the less safe product after receiving negative food safety news. The results for leading-brand chicken were consistent with the literature (see Hayes et al and Messer et al. 2011) in that negative information decreased WTP. Research question 4, Did brand-related food safety information increase consumers WTP for a relatively safer product, reveals the result that food safety information for Ranger chicken had a significant positive effect. Table 4 indicates that the Tobit model results for the variable ranger*info are highly significant with a coefficient of All else held constant, average WTP for Ranger chicken was $1.23 higher. Table 2 (panel B) also indicated that participants who received food safety information were willing to pay a premium for Ranger chicken when compared to leadingbrand chicken in all three sessions. In contrast, the information treatment did not increase participants WTP for Ranger chicken by session, as none of the differences between any of the three sessions were statistically significant (table 2, panel D). Thus, consumers will pay more for a safer product after learning of positive food safety information. Furthermore, consumers WTP for this product will not decrease like it does for the less safe brand of chicken. However, this result is not differentiable by session, which suggests that time is not a factor in consumer willingness to pay a premium for a safer product. The fifth research question, Did consumers WTP for any type of chicken in either treatment group change over time after no additional food safety information was provided, tested the longer-term effect on WTP. As shown in figure 1, average WTP for both types of chicken in both treatment groups was highest in the first session. However, tests of these differences 9 While the information about Ranger brand chicken was not universally positive, as some level of bacteria contamination was detected, the Consumer Reports article clearly presented Ranger as the relatively safer alternative for chicken consumers. 377

16 Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy (table 2, panels E-J), show that the only differences that were statistically significant were the $0.76 and $0.83 decrease in WTP for Ranger chicken in the information treatment (p ¼ and p ¼ , respectively). The two-limit Tobit model indicates that none of the eight variables that included days were statistically significant. The variable, days, measured the number of days that had elapsed since the first session. This passage of time could potentially have affected participants in several ways: participants may have forgotten about the information they learned over time; participants desire for chicken may also have changed over time based on whether they had recently purchased chicken; participants may also have changed their mind or had second thoughts since the previous sessions. The insignificance of the days variables demonstrates that there was no significant change in participants WTP over time for leading-brand or Ranger brand chicken in either treatment group. 10 A more differentiated analysis is available from the Wilcoxon significance tests shown in table 2. The results of these tests indicate that WTP for Ranger was significantly different between sessions 1 and 2 in the treatment group ( panel F). Session 1 WTP for Ranger is also significantly different from session 3 for the treatment group ( panel J). However, WTP for Ranger is not significantly different between sessions 2 and 3 in the treatment group (panel H). None of the differences in WTP for Ranger between any of the sessions is significantly different in the control group. WTP for leading-brand is not significantly different from session to session for either the treatment or the control group (panels E, G, and I). These results indicate that the premium for Ranger is short-lived and disappears by session 2, while the negative effect on WTP for leading-brand remains over all sessions. In addition to treatment dummy variables, several demographic variables were also included in the Tobit regression model. As is common in experimental studies, gender, age, income, and education variables were included as explanatory variables of WTP (Bernard, Zhang, and Gifford 2006; Fox, Hayes, and Shogren 2002; Hobbs et al. 2005; Hu, Zhong, and Ding 2006; Lusk et al. 2001; Lusk, Feldkamp, and Schroeder 2004). Since some research has found the presence of children in participants households to influence WTP, the number of children in each household was included as an explanatory variable (Bernard, Zhang, and Gifford 2006; Hu, Zhong, and Ding 2006; Kanter, Messer, and Kaiser 2009; Lusk et al. 2001; Messer et al. 2011). Following previous studies, race (Bernard, Zhang, and Gifford 2006), whether a subject considers themselves the primary shopper in the household (Kanter, Messer, and Kaiser 2009), and 10 Survey results indicate that 24 subjects (21.8% of the total sample) reported receiving outside information between experimental sessions that affected their bidding during the course of the experiment. Of these 24 subjects, 9 were in the control group and 15 were in the treatment group. While subjects were not given any information about Ranger brand chicken other than the safety information provided in the Consumer Reports article, simple internet searches about this brand would reveal to consumers that Ranger is also free range and the chickens are raised without the use of hormones or antibiotics. To test whether subjects researched Ranger brand chicken and subsequently bid higher in subsequent sessions, any positive responses were compared with bids that were at least 10% higher for Ranger brand chicken. Of the subjects who responded that they were affected by outside information, only 4.6% of subjects bid at least 10% higher for Ranger in session two than in session one, 6.4% of subjects bid at least 10% higher in session three than in session two, and 5.5% bid at least 10% higher in session three than in session one. Therefore, the potential impact of information gained outside of the experiment is likely minimal. 378

17 Do Consumer Responses to Media Food Safety Information Last? the frequency which subjects reported that they eat chicken (Messer et al. 2011) were also included as explanatory variables. Many results from the above mentioned variables are worth noting. In the Tobit model, children*info, p_chickensafe, and eat_chicken_often*info were highly significant independent variables. The variables children, primary_shopper, education*info, p_chickensafe*info, and income were also significant. Participants in the treatment group with children (children*info) under 18 living at home were willing to pay $2.55 less on average for chicken. Those in the control group (children) were willing to pay $0.93 more, on average. Those participants in the treatment group who considered chicken to be a safe product were willing to pay $0.96 more on average. Interestingly, those participants in the treatment group who ate chicken often (eat_chicken_often*info) were willing to pay $1.16 more on average. This result was consistent with the results of Payne et al. (2009), who found frequent consumers of beef were less affected by negative food safety information. Participants who consume chicken more frequently are also more likely to have been exposed to similar food safety warnings concerning chicken in the past. This may also be a factor in the lack of significance of this variable for the control group and such a strongly significant result for thetreatmentgroup.thesignificanceofthevariableprimary_shopper indicated that participants in the control group who considered themselves to be the primary shopper in the household were willing to pay $1.05 more on average than those who did not consider themselves to be the primary shopper. The significance of the impact of product safety information and education (education*info) indicated that more highly educated participants in the treatment group were willing to pay, on average, $0.43 less ($0.65 less + $0.22 from the coefficient for the education variable). The significance of the income variable (income) indicated that for every dollar of household income that participants in the control group reported, participants were willing to pay $0.007 more for chicken. The remaining demographic variables were statistically insignificant. Conclusion This experimental study demonstrated that a combination of negative and positive information regarding food safety has a long-lasting impact on demand. One hundred ten adults participated in this research to test the effects of negative and positive information over time on consumer purchasing behavior (as measured by WTP). Participants were asked to repeat the experiment twice after the initial session to measure changes in their WTP over time, extending out to seven weeks. During each session, WTP data were collected on both leading-brand and Ranger brand chicken. Demographic data were also obtained from the participants. The food safety information used in the study came from a 2007 Consumer Reports magazine article stating that leading-brand chicken, specifically identified as Perdue, Tyson, Foster Farms, and Pilgrim s Pride, frequently contained harmful bacteria. The article also stated that another brand, Ranger, was relatively free of harmful bacteria. Results from this study indicated that consumers are willing to change their purchasing behaviors to avoid unsafe products. Both positive and negative information had an effect on consumers WTP. Consumers were willing to pay less for the leading-brand chicken after they received negative food safety information compared to a control group that did not 379

Consumers Response to the 2006 Foodborne Illness Outbreak Linked to Spinach

Consumers Response to the 2006 Foodborne Illness Outbreak Linked to Spinach Consumers Response to the 2006 Foodborne Illness Outbreak Linked to Spinach 34 Carlos Arnade, carnade@ers.usda.gov Linda Calvin, lcalvin@ers.usda.gov Fred Kuchler, fkuchler@ers.usda.gov Consumers responded

More information

The Role of Media in Shaping the Consumers Food Risk Perception and Behavior: A Case Study of Spinach Recall

The Role of Media in Shaping the Consumers Food Risk Perception and Behavior: A Case Study of Spinach Recall The Role of Media in Shaping the Consumers Food Risk Perception and Behavior: A Case Study of Spinach Recall Arbindra Rimal 1 *, Benjamin Onyango, 1 Dragan Miljkovic, 2 and William Hallman, 2 *Contact

More information

Hamburger Pork Chop Deli Ham Chicken Wing $7.76 $5.53 $4.52 $4.02 $2.45 $2.79 $3.10 $3.51

Hamburger Pork Chop Deli Ham Chicken Wing $7.76 $5.53 $4.52 $4.02 $2.45 $2.79 $3.10 $3.51 FooDS FOOD DEMAND SURVEY Volume 3, Issue 12: April 15, 2016 About the Survey FooDS tracks consumer preferences and sentiments on the safety, quality, and price of food at home and away from home with particular

More information

An Evaluation of the 2015 Outbreak of Avian Influenza in the U.S.

An Evaluation of the 2015 Outbreak of Avian Influenza in the U.S. An Evaluation of the 2015 Outbreak of Avian Influenza in the U.S. Lei Gao Department of Agricultural Economics Texas A&M University padmagao@tamu.edu James Richardson Department of Agricultural Economics

More information

Hamburger Pork Chop Deli Ham Chicken Wing $9.05 $5.31 $4.52 $3.93 $2.45 $2.43 $2.17 $3.22

Hamburger Pork Chop Deli Ham Chicken Wing $9.05 $5.31 $4.52 $3.93 $2.45 $2.43 $2.17 $3.22 FooDS FOOD DEMAND SURVEY Volume 4, Issue 4: August 15, 2016 About the Survey FooDS tracks consumer preferences and sentiments on the safety, quality, and price of food at home and away from home with particular

More information

Attitudes, Acceptance, and Consumption: The Case of Beef Irradiation

Attitudes, Acceptance, and Consumption: The Case of Beef Irradiation Attitudes, Acceptance, and Consumption: The Case of Beef Irradiation Senhui He, Stanley Fletcher, and Arbindra Rimal Food contamination by microorganisms, such as pathogenic bacteria and parasites, causes

More information

Victoria Salin, Senarath Darmasena, Alex Wong, and Ping Luo Texas A&M University Contact:

Victoria Salin, Senarath Darmasena, Alex Wong, and Ping Luo Texas A&M University Contact: Food Product Recalls in the USA, 2000-2003 Research Update presented at 2005 Annual Meeting of the Food Distribution Research Society, Washington, DC, Oct. 19, 2005 Victoria Salin, Senarath Darmasena,

More information

Survey Results:

Survey Results: Survey 12-05 Results: 1. Meat 2. Alternatives 3. Food safety Panelists responding: 1119 Number of responses 1. What are your preferences when making a purchasing decision for meat and meat alternatives?

More information

IFIC Foundation Survey

IFIC Foundation Survey IFIC Foundation Survey Research with Consumers To Test Perceptions and Reactions To Various Stimuli and Visuals Related to Bioengineered Foods June 2018 1 Introduction Research Objectives Measure consumers

More information

MILLENNIALS AND ORANGE JUICE CONSUMPTION

MILLENNIALS AND ORANGE JUICE CONSUMPTION MILLENNIALS AND ORANGE JUICE CONSUMPTION Yan Heng, PhD and Lisa House, PhD Presented to the International Citrus Beverage Conference Florida Agricultural Market Research Center Food and Resource Economics

More information

FooDS Food Demand Survey

FooDS Food Demand Survey FooDS Food Demand Survey Volume 1: First Year Summary About the Survey FooDS tracks consumer preferences and sentiments on the safety, quality, and price of food at home and away from home with particular

More information

Gender Effects in Private Value Auctions. John C. Ham Department of Economics, University of Southern California and IZA. and

Gender Effects in Private Value Auctions. John C. Ham Department of Economics, University of Southern California and IZA. and Gender Effects in Private Value Auctions 2/1/05 Revised 3/3/06 John C. Ham Department of Economics, University of Southern California and IZA and John H. Kagel** Department of Economics, The Ohio State

More information

VIOLATIONS AND POTENTIAL SOLUTIONS

VIOLATIONS AND POTENTIAL SOLUTIONS VIOLATIONS AND POTENTIAL SOLUTIONS Food Safety Forum Tuesday, July 17, 2018 FOODBORNE ILLNESS RISK FACTORS While the food supply in the United States is one of the safest in the world, the Centers for

More information

The Strategic Marketing Institute Working Paper

The Strategic Marketing Institute Working Paper The Strategic Marketing Institute Working Paper The Market for Organic and Fortified Eggs William A. Knudson 2-0104 January 2004 Introduction Eggs are an important part of the American diet. According

More information

FooDS Food Demand Survey Issue 12: April 15, 2014

FooDS Food Demand Survey Issue 12: April 15, 2014 FooDS Food Demand Survey Issue 12: April 15, 2014 About the Survey FooDS tracks consumer preferences and sentiments on the safety, quality, and price of food at home and away from home with particular

More information

CHAPTER V: Conclusions

CHAPTER V: Conclusions CHAPTER V: Conclusions The traditional methods of meat demand analysis have always been based on aggregate annual, quarterly, or monthly time-series data of prices and the attendant quantities purchased

More information

Motivating Consumers to Respond Appropriately to Food Recalls. William K. Hallman, Ph.D. Director Food Policy Institute Rutgers University

Motivating Consumers to Respond Appropriately to Food Recalls. William K. Hallman, Ph.D. Director Food Policy Institute Rutgers University Motivating Consumers to Respond Appropriately to Food Recalls William K. Hallman, Ph.D. Director Food Policy Institute Rutgers University 10/18/2013 The Food Policy Institute The Food Policy Institute

More information

The role of training in experimental auctions

The role of training in experimental auctions AUA Working Paper Series No. 2010-2 February 2010 The role of training in experimental auctions Andreas Drichoutis Department of Economics University of Ioannina, Greece adrihout@cc.uoi.gr Rodolfo M. Nayga,

More information

CONSUMER RESEARCH: PLANT-BASED MARKET - BRAZIL

CONSUMER RESEARCH: PLANT-BASED MARKET - BRAZIL CONSUMER RESEARCH: PLANT-BASED MARKET - BRAZIL Plant-Based Proteins: Here to Stay The plant-based protein market is growing exponentially in Brazil, with products launching in natural food stores, big

More information

Section Processing

Section Processing Section 10000 Processing Table of Contents 10100 Introduction 10200 Participation 10300 Submit Requests in Pounds, Not Cases 10400 Approved Processors 10500 TDA Processing Contracts 10600 Value Pass-Through

More information

Predicting Food Prices from Estimated Implicit Values of Nutritional Characteristics

Predicting Food Prices from Estimated Implicit Values of Nutritional Characteristics University of Tennessee, Knoxville Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Research Reports AgResearch 1-1986 Predicting Food Prices from Estimated Implicit Values of Nutritional Characteristics

More information

Understanding Consumers Processing of Online Review Information

Understanding Consumers Processing of Online Review Information Understanding Consumers Processing of Online Review Information Matthew McNeill mmcneil@clemson.edu Nomula Siddarth-Reddy snomula@clemson.edu Dr. Tom T. Baker Clemson University School of Marketing 234

More information

Food Safety for Restaurants: How to Prevent Foodborne Illness, Food Contamination & Lawsuits

Food Safety for Restaurants: How to Prevent Foodborne Illness, Food Contamination & Lawsuits Food Safety for Restaurants: How to Prevent Foodborne Illness, Food Contamination & Lawsuits Foodborne illness causes an estimated 7.8 million sicknesses and about 3,000 deaths in the U.S. annually. In

More information

Hamburger Pork Chop Deli Ham Chicken Wing $7.89 $5.47 $4.61 $4.25 $2.79 $2.29 $2.82 $3.55

Hamburger Pork Chop Deli Ham Chicken Wing $7.89 $5.47 $4.61 $4.25 $2.79 $2.29 $2.82 $3.55 FooDS FOOD DEMAND SURVEY Volume 2, Issue 12: April 14, 2015 About the Survey FooDS tracks consumer preferences and sentiments on the safety, quality, and price of food at home and away from home with particular

More information

May 6, Division of Dockets Management (HFA-305) Food and Drug Administration 5630 Fishers Lane, Rm Rockville, MD 20852

May 6, Division of Dockets Management (HFA-305) Food and Drug Administration 5630 Fishers Lane, Rm Rockville, MD 20852 May 6, 2016 Division of Dockets Management (HFA-305) Food and Drug Administration 5630 Fishers Lane, Rm. 1061 Rockville, MD 20852 Submitted electronically via www.regulations.gov Re: Docket No. FDA-2014-N-1207

More information

Table Egg Industry Outlook

Table Egg Industry Outlook Agricultural Finance Table Egg Industry Outlook Executive summary Table egg prices have experienced increased volatility over the past two years. Avian flu decimated layer flocks in 05, and in response,

More information

Assessing authorized labeling as a marketing tool: an empirical study of Japan

Assessing authorized labeling as a marketing tool: an empirical study of Japan 2012 International Conference on Economics, Business and Marketing Management IPEDR vol.29 (2012) (2012) IACSIT Press, Singapore Assessing authorized labeling as a marketing tool: an empirical study of

More information

WATERMELON PRESENTATION Domestic A & U Study. rose research, llc. November, 2008

WATERMELON PRESENTATION Domestic A & U Study. rose research, llc. November, 2008 WATERMELON PRESENTATION Domestic A & U Study rose research, llc. November, 2008 Study Purpose Understand the dynamics operating in the watermelon category - - among purchasers (respondents who have purchased

More information

David Fikes, FMI VP Communica6ons & Consumer Affairs

David Fikes, FMI VP Communica6ons & Consumer Affairs Solving X in the New Consumer Value Equa:on of Food Purchase Decisions Consumer Beliefs, Knowledge and Willingness-to-Pay for Sustainability-Related Poultry Produc:on Prac:ces David Fikes, FMI VP Communica6ons

More information

What s in a Name? Using IRI Scanner Data to Evaluate Retail Food Labeling for Shell Eggs. Janet G. Peckham

What s in a Name? Using IRI Scanner Data to Evaluate Retail Food Labeling for Shell Eggs. Janet G. Peckham What s in a Name? Using IRI Scanner Data to Evaluate Retail Food Labeling for Shell Eggs Janet G. Peckham janet.peckham@fda.hhs.gov Emily Galloway emily.galloway@!fda.hhs.gov U.S. Food and Drug Administration

More information

Discussion Paper Series

Discussion Paper Series DEPARTMENT OF FOOD ECONOMICS Discussion Paper Series Beef Consumption, Risk Perception and Consumer Demand for Traceability along the Beef Chain by Mary McCarthy & Jennifer Barton Agribusiness Discussion

More information

E. coli O157:H7 - Multistate Outbreak Associated with Hazelnuts, 2010

E. coli O157:H7 - Multistate Outbreak Associated with Hazelnuts, 2010 Introduction This series focuses on investigations of outbreaks caused by commercially distributed food items and detected through pathogen specific surveillance. The etiologic agents often are Salmonella,

More information

Eggs and egg products: Consumers attitudes, perceptions and behaviours

Eggs and egg products: Consumers attitudes, perceptions and behaviours Eggs and egg products: Consumers attitudes, perceptions and behaviours Fjerkrækongress 2013 6 Februar 2013 Vincent Guyonnet, DVM, Ph.D, Dipl. ACPV Scientific Advisor The International Egg Commission vincent@internationalegg.com

More information

Consumer Perceptions of Food Safety: An emphasis on Meat

Consumer Perceptions of Food Safety: An emphasis on Meat Consumer Perceptions of Food Safety: An emphasis on Meat Edgar Chambers IV, Ph.D. Sensory Analysis Center, Kansas State University Consumer Perception Consumer beliefs and attitudes are important and often

More information

Outbreak Alert! Trends in Foodborne Illness Outbreaks in the United States ( )

Outbreak Alert! Trends in Foodborne Illness Outbreaks in the United States ( ) 5 th MEETING PAN AMERICAN COMMISSION ON FOOD SAFETY (COPAIA) Rio de Janeiro, Brazil, June 10, 2008 Provisional Agenda Item 5 COPAIA5/5 (Eng.) May, 28 th 2008 ORIGINAL: ENGLISH Outbreak Alert! Trends in

More information

Multistate Foodborne Outbreaks: Investigation and Communication Process

Multistate Foodborne Outbreaks: Investigation and Communication Process National Center for Emerging and Zoonotic Infectious Diseases Multistate Foodborne Outbreaks: Investigation and Communication Process Matthew Wise, MPH, PhD Deputy Branch Chief for Outbreak Response Laura

More information

Online Appendix A. A1 Ability

Online Appendix A. A1 Ability Online Appendix A A1 Ability To exclude the possibility of a gender difference in ability in our sample, we conducted a betweenparticipants test in which we measured ability by asking participants to engage

More information

Economics 345 Applied Econometrics

Economics 345 Applied Econometrics Economics 345 Applied Econometrics Lab Exam Version 1: Solutions Fall 2016 Prof: Martin Farnham TAs: Joffré Leroux Rebecca Wortzman Last Name (Family Name) First Name Student ID Open EViews, and open the

More information

FARMERS MARKET GENERAL INFORMATION

FARMERS MARKET GENERAL INFORMATION FARMERS MARKET GENERAL INFORMATION Cass County Food Code 3 201.11 Compliance with Food Law. (A) FOOD shall be obtained from sources that comply with LAW. (B) FOOD prepared in a private home may not be

More information

FooDS Food Demand Survey Issue 9: January 17, 2014

FooDS Food Demand Survey Issue 9: January 17, 2014 FooDS Food Demand Survey Issue 9: January 17, 2014 About the Survey FooDS tracks consumer preferences and sentiments on the safety, quality, and price of food at home and away from home with particular

More information

Inventory... 3 Inventory Types... 3 Contracting Entities Single Inventory Management... 3 FIFO... 4

Inventory... 3 Inventory Types... 3 Contracting Entities Single Inventory Management... 3 FIFO... 4 Section 8000 Inventory and Records Table of Contents Inventory... 3 Inventory Types... 3 Contracting Entities... 3 Single Inventory Management... 3 FIFO... 4 School Food Authorities (SFAs) That Act as

More information

From Farm to Fork: How Spatial-Temporal Data can Accelerate Foodborne Illness Investigation in a Global Food Supply Chain

From Farm to Fork: How Spatial-Temporal Data can Accelerate Foodborne Illness Investigation in a Global Food Supply Chain From Farm to Fork: How Spatial-Temporal Data can Accelerate Foodborne Illness Investigation in a Global Food Supply Chain Kun Hu 1, Stefan Edlund 1, Matthew Davis 1, James Kaufman 1 1 Accelerate Discovery

More information

Chicken: Myths and Facts

Chicken: Myths and Facts Chicken: Myths and Facts University of Delaware Bud Malone Gary VanWicklen Dan Bautista Stephen Collier University of Maryland Nick Zimmermann Nat Tablante Jennifer Timmons Inma Estevez The following poster

More information

SETTING THE STAGE. News in Review November 2012 Teacher Resource Guide E. COLI HEALTH HAZARD

SETTING THE STAGE. News in Review November 2012 Teacher Resource Guide E. COLI HEALTH HAZARD News in Review November 2012 Teacher Resource Guide E. COLI HEALTH HAZARD SETTING THE STAGE In September 2012, the Canadian Food Inspection Agency (CFIA) issued a notice informing Canadians not to eat

More information

VOLUME 5 ISSUE 4 AMBER WAVES ECONOMIC RESEARCH SERVICE/USDA. George Doyle, GettyImages

VOLUME 5 ISSUE 4 AMBER WAVES ECONOMIC RESEARCH SERVICE/USDA. George Doyle, GettyImages 24 VOLUME 5 ISSUE 4 George Doyle, GettyImages ECONOMIC RESEARCH SERVICE/USDA U.S. Consumers Had Short-Term Response to First BSE Announcements Fred Kuchler, fkuchler@ers.usda.gov Abebayehu Tegene, ategene@ers.usda.gov

More information

An Online Cost Calculator for Estimating the Economic Cost of Illness Due to Shiga Toxin- Producing E. coli (STEC) O157 Infections

An Online Cost Calculator for Estimating the Economic Cost of Illness Due to Shiga Toxin- Producing E. coli (STEC) O157 Infections United States Department of Agriculture Economic Research Service Economic Information Bulletin Number 28 An Online Cost Calculator for Estimating the Economic Cost of Illness Due to Shiga Toxin- Producing

More information

HEALTH TRANS OMEGA-3 OILS BALANCE GOOD FAT PROTEIN OBESITY USAGE HABITS

HEALTH TRANS OMEGA-3 OILS BALANCE GOOD FAT PROTEIN OBESITY USAGE HABITS HEALTH TRANS OMEGA-3 OILS BALANCE GOOD FAT PROTEIN OBESITY USAGE HABITS think 15TH ANNUAL consumer attitudes about nutrition Insights into Nutrition, Health & Soyfoods eat Consumer Attitudes about Nutrition

More information

Based On A National Survey Among Adults. Conducted On Behalf Of: Synthetic Biology Project The Woodrow Wilson International Center For Scholars

Based On A National Survey Among Adults. Conducted On Behalf Of: Synthetic Biology Project The Woodrow Wilson International Center For Scholars Awareness & Impressions Of Synthetic Biology A Report Of Findings Based On A National Survey Among Adults Conducted On Behalf Of: Synthetic Biology Project The Woodrow Wilson International Center For Scholars

More information

2012 Emerging Trends and Key Issues Report Product Recall & Contamination Risk Management CONTENTS

2012 Emerging Trends and Key Issues Report Product Recall & Contamination Risk Management CONTENTS 2012 Emerging Trends and Key Issues Report Product Recall & Contamination Risk Management The 2012 Emerging Trends and Key Issues Product Recall & Contamination Risk Management Report is published annually

More information

Test date Name Meal Planning for the Family Study Sheet References: Notes in class, lectures, labs, assignments

Test date Name Meal Planning for the Family Study Sheet References: Notes in class, lectures, labs, assignments Test date Name Meal Planning for the Family Study Sheet References: Notes in class, lectures, labs, assignments Food for Today - Chapters 3.1, 3.2, 12.1,2,3 World of Food - Chapters 3, 5, 8 1. Know the

More information

P9mX$*yXOjxeEK#S. Consumer Demand for Low-Acrylamide-Forming Potato Products: Evidence from Lab Auctions. Jonathan R. McFadden and Wallace E.

P9mX$*yXOjxeEK#S. Consumer Demand for Low-Acrylamide-Forming Potato Products: Evidence from Lab Auctions. Jonathan R. McFadden and Wallace E. Padua 2017 Abstract Submission I want to submit an abstract for: Conference Presentation Corresponding Author Wallace Huffman E-Mail whuffman@iastate.edu Affiliation Iowa State Univesity Co-Author/s Name

More information

STATISTICS 8 CHAPTERS 1 TO 6, SAMPLE MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS

STATISTICS 8 CHAPTERS 1 TO 6, SAMPLE MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS STATISTICS 8 CHAPTERS 1 TO 6, SAMPLE MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS Circle the best answer. This scenario applies to Questions 1 and 2: A study was done to compare the lung capacity of coal miners to the lung

More information

The Importance of Taste for Food Demand and the Experienced Taste Effect of

The Importance of Taste for Food Demand and the Experienced Taste Effect of 1 The Importance of Taste for Food Demand and the Experienced Taste Effect of Healthy Labels * - An experiment on potato chips and bread Linda Thunström a and Jonas Nordström b Abstract This paper quantitatively

More information

2017 FOOD & HEALTH SURVEY A Focus on Older Adults Funded by

2017 FOOD & HEALTH SURVEY A Focus on Older Adults Funded by A Focus on Older Adults 50-80 TABLE OF CONTENTS Background & Methodology 3 Demographics 4 Healthy Eating 7 Food Confusion 23 Packaged Foods 32 Food Values 37 Background The International Food Information

More information

Simplified Modeling Framework for Microbial Food-Safety Risk Assessments

Simplified Modeling Framework for Microbial Food-Safety Risk Assessments Food Safety and Inspection Service Simplified Modeling Framework for Microbial Food-Safety Risk Assessments Michael Williams Risk Assessment and Analytics Staff Food Safety and Inspection Service, USDA

More information

Food Safety Risk Perception Gap Between Consumers and Processors of Specialty Meat

Food Safety Risk Perception Gap Between Consumers and Processors of Specialty Meat Food Safety Risk Perception Gap Between Consumers and Processors of Specialty Meat William E. Nganje and Thomas Taban Authors are Assistant Professor and McNair Fellow at the Department of Agribusiness

More information

FDA Foodborne Illness Risk Factor Study Data Collection Form

FDA Foodborne Illness Risk Factor Study Data Collection Form APPENDIX O 2105 Data Collection Wake County Facility ID# Sample # QA FDA Foodborne Illness Risk Factor Study Data Collection Form Date: Time In: Time Out: Inspector: Establishment: Manager: Physical Address:

More information

A STUDY ON PATRON PROCURING ATTITUDE TOWARDS RTE (READY TO EAT) FOODS WITH REFERENCE TO COIMBATORE CITY

A STUDY ON PATRON PROCURING ATTITUDE TOWARDS RTE (READY TO EAT) FOODS WITH REFERENCE TO COIMBATORE CITY A STUDY ON PATRON PROCURING ATTITUDE TOWARDS RTE (READY TO EAT) FOODS WITH REFERENCE TO COIMBATORE CITY *KOWSALYA. S ** K. SATHYAPRIYA * Asst. Professor, Department of Commerce (CA), Sri Krishna Arts &

More information

Nutritional Labeling. University of Connecticut. Emily J. Williams University of Connecticut - Storrs,

Nutritional Labeling. University of Connecticut. Emily J. Williams University of Connecticut - Storrs, University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Honors Scholar Theses Honors Scholar Program Spring 5-8-2011 Nutritional Labeling Emily J. Williams University of Connecticut - Storrs, emjw718@gmail.com

More information

Analysis and Interpretation of Data Part 1

Analysis and Interpretation of Data Part 1 Analysis and Interpretation of Data Part 1 DATA ANALYSIS: PRELIMINARY STEPS 1. Editing Field Edit Completeness Legibility Comprehensibility Consistency Uniformity Central Office Edit 2. Coding Specifying

More information

Food Safety and Inspection Service Research Priorities

Food Safety and Inspection Service Research Priorities Food Safety and Inspection Service Research Priorities David Goldman, MD, MPH Assistant Administrator Science Food Safety and Inspection Service 1 Technology Workshop on Food Safety and National Defense

More information

FOOD SCIENCE: AN ECOLOGICAL APPROACH Special Topic: Food Safety & Bioterrorism Jill M. Merrigan

FOOD SCIENCE: AN ECOLOGICAL APPROACH Special Topic: Food Safety & Bioterrorism Jill M. Merrigan FOOD SCIENCE: AN ECOLOGICAL APPROACH Special Topic: Food Safety & Bioterrorism Jill M. Merrigan The CDC, Center for Disease Control and Prevention states that food-related diseases affect tens of millions

More information

Nancy Sittler Region Of Waterloo Public Health

Nancy Sittler Region Of Waterloo Public Health Nancy Sittler Region Of Waterloo Public Health Canadian Institute of Public Health Inspectors Annual Educational Conference June 20-23, 2010 Guelph, ON Presentation Overview Background Significance Aim

More information

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX 2 Web Appendix A: Panel data estimation approach As noted in the main

More information

Testing for non-response and sample selection bias in contingent valuation: Analysis of a combination phone/mail survey

Testing for non-response and sample selection bias in contingent valuation: Analysis of a combination phone/mail survey Whitehead, J.C., Groothuis, P.A., and Blomquist, G.C. (1993) Testing for Nonresponse and Sample Selection Bias in Contingent Valuation: Analysis of a Combination Phone/Mail Survey, Economics Letters, 41(2):

More information

GATS Highlights. GATS Objectives. GATS Methodology

GATS Highlights. GATS Objectives. GATS Methodology GATS Objectives GATS Highlights The Global Adult Tobacco Survey (GATS) is a global standard for systematically monitoring adult tobacco use (smoking and smokeless) and tracking key tobacco control indicators.

More information

DENTAL BENEFITS: A BRIDGE TO ORAL HEALTH & WELLNESS

DENTAL BENEFITS: A BRIDGE TO ORAL HEALTH & WELLNESS R GUARDIAN WORKPLACE BENEFITS STUDY SM 5TH ANNUAL DENTAL BENEFITS: A BRIDGE TO ORAL HEALTH & WELLNESS Dental benefits utilization, especially for preventive services, contributes to better oral health

More information

Meeting Consumers Expectations

Meeting Consumers Expectations Meeting Consumers Expectations February 19, 2008 Savannah, GA Gale Prince gale@galeprince.com Health Benefits Nutrition 64% of us are overweight 30% are obese 20% are on a diet Consumers are making the

More information

Determinants of Unsafe Hamburger Cooking Behavior

Determinants of Unsafe Hamburger Cooking Behavior Determinants of Unsafe Hamburger Cooking Behavior Katherine Ralston, USDA Economic Research Service YoLanda Starke, North Carolina Agricultural and Technical State University Kofi Adu-Nyako, North Carolina

More information

Protect the quality and safety of your food

Protect the quality and safety of your food Protect the quality and safety of your food Description of tool This tool provides useful information about protecting the quality and safety of food. School teachers can use this information while teaching

More information

Food Safety Training

Food Safety Training Food Safety Training Good Personal Hygiene Wash hands frequently, especially after eating, drinking, smoking, touching your face, nose, ears, hair, handling waste, using the restroom, coughing or sneezing,

More information

Blue or Red? How Color Affects Consumer Information Processing in Food Choice

Blue or Red? How Color Affects Consumer Information Processing in Food Choice Blue or Red? How Color Affects Consumer Information Processing in Food Choice Meng Shen Ph.D. Candidate Food and Resource Economics Department University of Florida Email: caassm@ufl.edu Zhifeng Gao Associate

More information

Prepared by Wake County Environmental Services, Environmental Health & Safety Division

Prepared by Wake County Environmental Services, Environmental Health & Safety Division Wake County, North Carolina Report on the Occurrence of Foodborne Illness Risk Factors in Selected Institutional Foodservice, Restaurant, and Retail Food Store Facility Types 2015 Prepared by Wake County

More information

FRESH FRUIT JUICES-A STUDY ON CONSUMER PREFERENCE AND DEMOGRAPHIC PROFILING

FRESH FRUIT JUICES-A STUDY ON CONSUMER PREFERENCE AND DEMOGRAPHIC PROFILING FRESH FRUIT JUICES-A STUDY ON CONSUMER PREFERENCE AND DEMOGRAPHIC PROFILING Pooja Boga, Tejaswini Khanolkar ABSTRACT Consumer buying behavior is a generic term which explains consumer which explains consumer

More information

CDC - June 23, Salmonella Panama Infections Linked to Cantaloupe. Salmonella

CDC - June 23, Salmonella Panama Infections Linked to Cantaloupe. Salmonella Salmonella Investigation Update: Multistate Outbreak of Salmonella Panama Infections Linked to Cantaloupe Case Count Map (/salmonella/panama0311/062311/map.html) Epi Curve (/salmonella/panama0311/062311/epi.html)

More information

Purchasing Patterns for Nutritional-Enhanced Foods: The case of Calcium-Enriched Orange Juice

Purchasing Patterns for Nutritional-Enhanced Foods: The case of Calcium-Enriched Orange Juice Purchasing Patterns for Nutritional-Enhanced Foods: The case of Calcium-Enriched Orange Juice By Alla Golub, James J Binkley, and Mark Denbaly Dept of Agricultural Economics, Purdue University; (Golub

More information

SECTION 1. Introducing Simply Beef and the Beef Alternative Merchandising program

SECTION 1. Introducing Simply Beef and the Beef Alternative Merchandising program The SIMPLYBEEF guide to BEEF ALTERNATIVE MERCHANDISING AM SECTION 1 Introducing Simply Beef and the Beef Alternative Merchandising program This section includes target audience information and valuable

More information

Consumer Knowledge of Selected Nutrient Content of Nine Fresh Meat Cuts

Consumer Knowledge of Selected Nutrient Content of Nine Fresh Meat Cuts Journal of Agribusiness 17,2(Fall 1999):161 168 1999 Agricultural Economics Association of Georgia Consumer Knowledge of Selected Nutrient Content of Nine Fresh Meat Cuts Alvin Schupp, Robert Downer, Jeffrey

More information

An Analysis of Nutritional Label Use in the Southern United States

An Analysis of Nutritional Label Use in the Southern United States An Analysis of Nutritional Label Use in the Southern United States Patricia E. McLean-Meyinsse Results from a random telephone survey of 1,41 grocery shoppers in the South suggest that 0 percent of them

More information

USDA Foods 101. Food Distribution Program

USDA Foods 101. Food Distribution Program USDA Foods 101 Food Distribution Program 1 Acknowledgement Statement You understand and acknowledge that the training you are about to take does not cover the entire scope of the program; and You are responsible

More information

Food Consumption Data in Microbiological Risk Assessment

Food Consumption Data in Microbiological Risk Assessment 1972 Journal of Food Protection, Vol. 67, No. 9, 2004, Pages 1972 1976 Copyright, International Association for Food Protection Food Consumption Data in Microbiological Risk Assessment LEILA M. BARRAJ*

More information

Evaluation of Safe Food-Handling Instructions on Raw Meat and Poultry Products

Evaluation of Safe Food-Handling Instructions on Raw Meat and Poultry Products 1321 Journal of Food Protection, Vol. 63, No. 10, 2000, Pages 1321 1325 Copyright, International Association for Food Protection Evaluation of Safe Food-Handling Instructions on Raw Meat and Poultry Products

More information

Food Safety Performance Standards: an Epidemiologic Perspective

Food Safety Performance Standards: an Epidemiologic Perspective Food Safety Performance Standards: an Epidemiologic Perspective Institute t of Medicine i Food dforum Meeting Rajal Mody, MD MPH LCDR US Public Health Service Enteric Diseases Epidemiology Branch Centers

More information

The cost-benefit of cholinesterase inhibitors in mild to moderate dementia: a willingness-topay

The cost-benefit of cholinesterase inhibitors in mild to moderate dementia: a willingness-topay The cost-benefit of cholinesterase inhibitors in mild to moderate dementia: a willingness-topay approach Wu G, Lanctot K L, Herrmann N, Moosa S, Oh P I Record Status This is a critical abstract of an economic

More information

MN 400: Research Methods. PART II The Design of Research

MN 400: Research Methods. PART II The Design of Research MN 400: Research Methods PART II The Design of Research 1 MN 400: Research Methods CHAPTER 6 Research Design 2 What is Research Design? A plan for selecting the sources and types of information used to

More information

The Role of USDA s Food Safety and Inspection Service to Ensure Foodborne Disease Control and Prevention

The Role of USDA s Food Safety and Inspection Service to Ensure Foodborne Disease Control and Prevention The Role of USDA s Food Safety and Inspection Service to Ensure Foodborne Disease Control and Prevention David Goldman, MD, MPH Assistant Administrator Office of Public Health Science Agricultural Outlook

More information

PERSPECTIVE A HEALTHY 2017 FOOD & HEALTH SURVEY

PERSPECTIVE A HEALTHY 2017 FOOD & HEALTH SURVEY A HEALTHY PERSPECTIVE Healthy is often at the heart of our discussions about food. Still, how Americans think about healthy in the context of dietary decisions remains hotly debated. Defining Healthy Health-promoting

More information

Executive Summary. About St. Jude Children s Research Hospital. Mission Statement

Executive Summary. About St. Jude Children s Research Hospital. Mission Statement Justin Jones Introduction to Marketing Marketing Plan Final April 24, 2012 Executive Summary St. Jude Children s Research Hospital is a non-profit company that provides unprecedented service for families

More information

Canada s Food Supply: A Preliminary Examination of Changes,

Canada s Food Supply: A Preliminary Examination of Changes, Canada s Food Supply: A Preliminary Examination of Changes, 1992-2002 Canada's Food Guide to Healthy Eating, released in 1992, is a key nutrition education tool for Canadians aged four years and over.

More information

MULTIPLE CHOICE. Choose the one alternative that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1) 1) A) B) C) D)

MULTIPLE CHOICE. Choose the one alternative that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1) 1) A) B) C) D) Exam Name MULTIPLE CHOICE. Choose the one alternative that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1) 1) A) B) C) D) Decide whether or not the conditions and assumptions for inference with

More information

Taking the Mystery Out of TEFAP. Rosie Krueger Child Nutrition Programs Vermont Agency of Education. Diversity & Inclusion

Taking the Mystery Out of TEFAP. Rosie Krueger Child Nutrition Programs Vermont Agency of Education. Diversity & Inclusion Taking the Mystery Out of TEFAP Rosie Krueger Child Nutrition Programs Vermont Agency of Education Today we will learn: What USDA Foods are What TEFAP is How Vermont s USDA Foods for TEFAP are selected

More information

Signature Sponsorship Opportunities. Western New York s BEST

Signature Sponsorship Opportunities. Western New York s BEST Signature Sponsorship Opportunities Western New York s BEST 1 October 24, 2017 Salvatore s Italian Garden s 6461 Transit Road, Buffalo, NY 14043 The March of Dimes CARES The mission of the March of Dimes

More information

CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 3.1 Introduction 3.1 Methodology 3.1.1 Research Design 3.1. Research Framework Design 3.1.3 Research Instrument 3.1.4 Validity of Questionnaire 3.1.5 Statistical Measurement

More information

RECALL INDEX Q2 2018

RECALL INDEX Q2 2018 RECALL INDEX Recall Index Consumer product recalls decreased by 21% to 57 the lowest quarter since 2015 and second lowest since 2004. Recalled units decreased 26% to about 6.3 million below 13 of the last

More information

Article begins on next page

Article begins on next page Public Perceptions of Labeling Genetically Modified Foods Rutgers University has made this article freely available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. [https://rucore.libraries.rutgers.edu/rutgers-lib/46835/story/]

More information

Thailand. Poultry and Products Annual

Thailand. Poultry and Products Annual THIS REPORT CONTAINS ASSESSMENTS OF COMMODITY AND TRADE ISSUES MADE BY USDA STAFF AND NOT NECESSARILY STATEMENTS OF OFFICIAL U.S. GOVERNMENT POLICY Required Report - public distribution Date: GAIN Report

More information

SHOPPING FOR HEALTH 2009

SHOPPING FOR HEALTH 2009 SHOPPING FOR HEALTH 2009 WHAT IT TAKES TO EAT HEALTHY Cary Silvers Director of Consumer Insights Whole Grains Council 4.21.09 Cross-roads at the Supermarket Higher food prices Down economy Low consumer

More information

Overview of USDA Inspection

Overview of USDA Inspection Overview of USDA Inspection Dr. Douglas Powell Professor Department of Diagnostic Medicine/Pathobiology Kansas State University dpowell@ksu.edu bites.ksu.edu barfblog.com http://barfblog.com/2013/05/food-safety-apology-i-texas-health-type-says-sorry-to-family/

More information

Reducing household food and drink waste Date labels and storage guidance Project findings. May 2011

Reducing household food and drink waste Date labels and storage guidance Project findings. May 2011 Reducing household food and drink waste Date labels and storage guidance Project findings May 2011 Presentation outline Background Methodology Recommendations and evidence Potential impact Food safety

More information

The Effects of High Hydrostatic Pressure on Meats

The Effects of High Hydrostatic Pressure on Meats The Effects of High Hydrostatic Pressure on Meats Errol V. Raghubeer Avure Technologies Inc. Kent, WA 98032 September, 2007 Introduction High hydrostatic pressure is an innovative technology that has become

More information