Evidence for Multiple Representations of Number in the Human Brain. Thesis. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Evidence for Multiple Representations of Number in the Human Brain. Thesis. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for"

Transcription

1 Evidence for Multiple Representations of Number in the Human Brain Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Science in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Frank Joseph Kanayet B. S. Graduate Program in Psychology The Ohio State University 2009 Thesis Committee: Professor John Opfer, Advisor Professor Simon Dennis Professor Wil Cunningham

2 Copyright by Frank Joseph Kanayet 2009

3 Abstract In the adult brain, the horizontal segment of the intraparietal sulcus (HIPS) is known to play a pivotal role in representing magnitudes of numerals. In children, however, prefrontal cortex may play a larger role than HIPS. This thesis tests a hypothesized link between developmental changes in locus of representation and the logarithmic-to-linear shift in representations of numerical magnitude. Participants were presented with number lines and asked to judge the accuracy of linear, logarithmic, or log-linear placements. Consistent with our hypothesis, event-related potentials (ERPs) revealed greatest parietal N1 amplitudes for linear trials and greatest frontal P3 amplitude for logarithmic trials. Additionally, the effects on linear trials were moderated by magnitude: parietal N1 amplitudes decreased with magnitude, while frontal P3 amplitudes increased with magnitude. Together, these results provide evidence for multiple representations of number and show that when linear representations are less entrenched, there is more involvement of the prefrontal cortex. ii

4 Dedicated to Catalina for being my companion and supporting me every step of the way My parents for giving me all the opportunities to grow as a person and pursue my dreams. Last but not least, to all the professors that played an instrumental role in motivating me to pursue a scientific career. iii

5 Acknowledgments First, I would like to thank my advisor John Opfer for all the discussions, thoughtful comments and support during the development and writing of this thesis. I am still amazed about all the transformations this project has been subjected to as a result of my meetings with John. I would also like to thank Wil Cunningham for opening the doors of his lab to me, and for providing his expertise in the conduction of brain imaging research to make this thesis a better product. In addition, I would like to express gratitude to Simon Dennis, Vladimir Sloutsky, and the rest of the members of the ERP reading group for providing a space to discuss issues related to ERP research that improved my understanding of this methodology and of the big picture implications of all the methodological decisions made in this thesis. Similarly, I am grateful to Jay Van Bavel for going with me through all the steps of the research, from plugging cables to data analysis. It would literally had been impossible to get to this point without the countless hours of troubleshooting that Jay and I spent in the ERP lab. I am also very grateful to Laura Nelson and Sima Finy for helping me with data collection and coding, and to Andrew Jahn, for helping with the data analysis. Finally, I would like to thank my parents and brother for being there for me at all times, and my girlfriend Catalina Torrente who motivates me, proofreads everything I write, listens to and discusses my ideas, and accompanies me in every single thing I do. iv

6 Vita February 4, Born Bogotá, Colombia B.S. Psychology, Universidad de los Andes B.S. Philosophy, Universidad de los Andes Presidential Fellowship, The Ohio State University to present Graduate Teaching Associate, Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University Fields of Study Major Field: Psychology Areas of Emphasis: Cognitive Psychology; Developmental Psychology v

7 Table of Contents P a g e Abstract...ii Dedication..iii Acknowledgments iv Vita...v List of Tables...viii List of Figures...ix Evidence for Multiple Representations of Number in the Human Brain..1 How is numeric magnitude represented in the brain?..3 Behavioral evidence from numerical estimation..10 Present study..13 Method.. 15 Participants.15 Task..16 Stimuli...16 Procedure and design ERP recording procedure. 17 Results Behavioral results..19 Electrophysiological results. 20 Discussion 26 vi

8 Future directions 29 Conclusion.. 31 References. 33 Appendix A: Tables 38 Appendix B: Figures vii

9 List of Tables Table Page 1 Table of ANOVA effects for accuracy and median reaction times viii

10 List of Figures Figure Page 1 Experimental procedure. Example of a) linear trials, b) logarithmic trials, and c) log-linear trials. ISI = Intertrial stimulus interval Montage and labels for electrodes used in the experiment. Top is front Area of circles represents the percentage of responses judged as correct by participants for linear trials (green), logarithmic trials (red), and log-linear trials blue) Mean d prime values for each numeral (+ SE). (a) and (b) represent significant differences at p < N1 component of ERP to placement of numbers on number lines. Figure shows the negative amplitude wave in the ms time window after a number was shown in either the linear, logarithmic, or log-linear position. The mean amplitude varies parametrically with the linearity of placement of the number. a) Average waveforms for representative posterior electrode P4 (right parietal) for linear trials (black), logarithmic trials (red) and log-linear trials (blue). b) Current source density topography for linear trials at 200 ms. c) Current source density topography for logarithmic trials at 200 ms 43 6 P3 component of ERP to placement of numbers on number lines. Figure shows the positive amplitude wave in the ms time window after a number was shown in either the linear, logarithmic, or log-linear position. The mean amplitude varies parametrically with the logarithmicity of placement of the number. a) Average waveforms for representative anterior electrode FC4 (right frontal) for linear trials (black), logarithmic trials (red) and log-linear trials (blue). b) Current source density topography for linear trials at 300 ms. c) Current source density topography for logarithmic trials at 300ms.44 ix

11 7 How the brain reacts to differences between the logarithmic and linear placements of numbers on number lines. Around 200 ms after presentation of a number line placement, a strong N1 component emerges in parietal cortex with the identification of the linear placement being correct. 100 ms later, in frontal cortex, a strong P3 component emerges with the recognition of the logarithmic placement being erroneous. a) Difference waveforms (logarithmic minus linear) for representative right posterior electrode P4 (red), and representative right frontal electrode (blue). b) Current source density topography for N1 (200 ms). c) Current source density topography for P3 (300 ms) 45 8 N1 component of ERP to placement of numbers on number lines for linear trials that were judged as correct by participants. Figure shows the negative amplitude wave in the ms time window after the placement of the hatch mark for small numbers (less than 500) and large numbers (greater than 500). a) Average waveforms for representative posterior electrode P4 (right parietal) for correct linear trials with small numbers after the presentation of a hatch mark (blue) and for large numbers after the presentation of a hatch mark (red). b) Current source density topography for small numbers at 200 ms. c) Current source density topography for large numbers at 200 ms.46 9 P3 component of ERP to placement of numbers on number lines for linear trials that were judged as correct by participants. Figure shows the positive amplitude wave in the ms time window after the placement of the hatch mark for small numbers (less than 500) and large numbers (greater than 500). a) Average waveforms for representative anterior electrode F7 (left frontal) for correct linear trials with small numbers after the presentation of a hatch mark (blue) and for large numbers after the presentation of a hatch mark (red). b) Current source density topography for small numbers at 300 ms. c) Current source density topography for large numbers at 300 ms N1 component of ERP to presentation of Arabic numerals. Figure shows the negative amplitude wave in the ms time window after the presentation of small numbers (less than 500) and large numbers (greater than 500). a) Average waveforms for representative posterior electrode P7 (left parietal) for trials with small numbers (blue) and for large numbers (red). b) Current source density topography for small numbers at 200 ms. c) Current source density topography for large numbers at 200 ms P3 component of ERP to presentation of Arabic numerals. Figure shows the positive amplitude wave in the ms time window after the presentation of small numbers x

12 (less than 500) and large numbers (greater than 500). a) Average waveforms for representative posterior electrode P7 (left parietal) for trials with small numbers (blue) and for large numbers (red). b) Current source density topography for small numbers at 350 ms. c) Current source density topography for large numbers at 350 ms...49 xi

13 Evidence for Multiple Representations of Number in the Human Brain Whether a pollster determining the sampling process for a presidential election poll, a parishioner telling the time by counting the tolls of a church bell, or a child figuring out how much candy she had received on this Halloween versus a previous one, mental representations of numerical magnitude are important for projecting the future, monitoring the present, and learning from the past. Moreover, this ability to code our experiences numerically must scale consistently regardless of the shape, size, sensory modality or context in which particular numeric magnitudes are presented. To understand how humans and other animals represent numerical magnitudes, neuroscientists have examined numeric coding in the brain. In this research, two prominent brain areas have been implicated: the prefrontal cortex and the horizontal segment of the intraparietal Sulcus (HIPS). Most studies have shown that HIPS plays a major role in numeric representation (Dehaene, Piazza, Pinel, Cohen, 2003 for a review). Thus, a dominant view regarding the role of HIPS is that magnitude is coded in this area as an abstract, notation-independent representation (Dehaene, Dehaene-Lambertz, & Cohen, 1998; Libertus, Woldroff & Brannon, 2007; Plodowski et al., 2003). However, studies that use different populations have found different results. For example, single cell recordings in monkeys (Nieder & Miller, 2004; Nieder & Merten, 2007) and fmri studies in children (Ansari et al. 2005; Ansari & Dhital, 2006) have shown stronger effects in the prefrontal cortex compared to HIPS. Additionally, fmri studies have shown greater activation of prefrontal cortex for tasks where children and adults are asked to count dots or sequences of taps (Cantlon, Brannon, Carter & Pelphrey, 2006; Venkatraman et al., 2005) compared to tasks that involve numerals. These findings 1

14 suggest that over the course of development, experience with numerical magnitudes may cause the prefrontal cortex to play a successively less prominent role (and HIPS a successively more prominent role) in numeric magnitude processing. To explain this developmental trend, we propose here a novel theory about how the brain develops a mature representation of numeric magnitude. The theory holds that: (1) at any given age, the brain represents numeric magnitudes using both a logarithmically-compressed code and a linear code, with the probability of a number being processed by the linear code increasing with age and experience; and (2) logarithmic-coding is predominantly processed in frontal areas, whereas linear coding is predominantly processed in parietal areas. The origin of this hypothesis stems from behavioral studies on the development of numeric representations (Booth & Siegler, 2006; Opfer & Siegler, 2007; Opfer & Thompson, 2008; Siegler & Opfer, 2003; Siegler, Thompson, & Opfer, in press; Thompson & Opfer, 2008; Thompson & Opfer, in press). In these studies, children and adults were asked to estimate the position of numbers on a blank line with the endpoints labeled 0 and 100, 0 and 1000, or 0 and This estimation task is particularly revealing about representations of numeric value because it transparently reflects the ratio characteristics of the number system. Overall, younger children s estimates typically follow Fechner s Law and increase logarithmically with actual value, whereas older children s estimates increase linearly. At any given age, however, individual children use both logarithmic and linear representations of number, depending on numerical context. That is, for very large numeric contexts (e.g., on and number lines), children s estimates increase logarithmically; however, the same children will use linear representations when estimating the magnitudes of numbers for small numeric contexts (e.g., on number lines). If our developmental theory is correct, it should be possible to identify two different patterns in the brain for 2

15 situations that are consistent with both the logarithmic and linear representations, thereby providing neural correlates for the logarithmic-to-linear shift hypothesis. Finally, the theory predicts that even adult brains are likely to use the logarithmic code for representing large numbers, much as children do for a wider range of numbers. To test our theory, we asked participants to judge whether a number had been accurately marked on the number line, and we evaluated the components of Event- Related Potentials (ERP) within 1s after participants were given a number to estimate and 1s after shown a target position for that number. By evaluating ERP components related to numeric estimation in the number line task, we were able to test several predictions derived from our developmental hypothesis. Specifically, we were able to provide a novel test of whether subjects expected positions on numbers on number lines to increase linearly, logarithmically, both, or neither with numeric value, and we were able to test whether these expectations were related to the topography of numerical processing, and to previous experience with the numbers to-be-estimated. In the following sections I will do a background review of some of the most significant findings that inform the work in this thesis. Initially I will review some of the major findings regarding the way numeric magnitude is represented in the brain. Next, I will review some behavioral data that reveals some clues about how numeric magnitude is represented in the brain both in adults and in children. Finally, I will end with a description of the studies presented here and of the general purpose they are designed to fulfill. How is numeric magnitude represented in the brain? With the improvement in neuroimaging techniques, there has been an increase in the number of studies that try to find neural correlates for the processing of numbers in the brain. Among the techniques that have yielded important findings are the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) studies, single cell recording, and ERP. 3

16 As it would be expected of domains as complex as number and numerosity processing, many areas of the brain are involved. Therefore, an attempt to localize a single brain area as the place where everything is processed is very likely a major simplification. However, a robust finding in experiments that use fmri techniques is that the HIPS area plays a key role in numeric magnitude processing (Dehaene et al., 2003, for a review). Some of the tasks that have yielded high activation of HIPS are distance effects in comparisons of one-digit (Ansari et al. 2005; Chochon et al., 1999; Pinel et al., 2004; Venkatraman et al. 2005) and two-digit numbers (Pinel et al., 2001; Wood, Nuerk & Willmes, 2006), approximate addition and subtraction tasks (Chochon et al., 1999, Dehaene et al. 1999; Lee, 2000; Simon et al. 2002; Venkatraman et al. 2005), response to changes in numerosity in habituation tasks (Cantlon et al., 2006; Piazza et al., 2004), and recognition of meaning of signed numbers by hearing impaired adults (Masataka et al., 2005). It is worth noting that these studies use different tasks, stimuli, and populations, thus showing that the general importance of HIPS for magnitude processing. Moreover, Dehaene et al. (2003) propose that this area is number specific and it is the place for an abstract, notation-independent representation of numeric magnitude. In single cell recording studies with monkeys, Nieder & Miller (2004) found populations of neurons in the posterior parietal cortex that are selectively sensitive to different numerosities. Moreover, they found that the tuning curves of these populations of neurons are increasingly overlapping, providing a viable mechanism in the brain for the analog non-symbolic logarithmic representation discussed above. This finding of numerons in the posterior parietal cortex is consistent with the claim that HIPS is a number specific area. Another imaging technique that has produced interesting results regarding numerical representation in the brain is the ERP. Similar to what has been presented 4

17 above, studies using ERP in adults have found a notation-independent representation in the HIPS area (Libertus et al. 2007; Szucs & Csepe, 2005). Moreover, there are four major components that have been associated with magnitude processing (Deahene, 1996). The first ERP component that shows significant differences when comparing close numbers vs. far numbers is an occipito-temporal N1 component ( ms) related to number identification. The second component is the transition between the N1 and the P2p (second posterior positivity) in parieto-occipitotemporal electrodes. The third component corresponds to the amplitude of this second positivity ( ms). These last two components were related to magnitude comparison. Finally, the P3 ( ms) component was modulated by the distance effect and was related to the beginning of response preparation. A source localization analysis showed that the main location for processing this information was also located in the right IPS. Similarly, other studies have found effects in the N1 and P3 components that reflected processing of magnitude (Ullsperger, 1995) even when it is not taskrelevant (Schwarz & Heinze, 1998). Therefore, there is convergence in the findings obtained with fmri and ERP methodologies. In one of the very few ERP studies that has attempted to compare children and adults in a number comparison task, Temple and Posner (1998) found that 5-year-old children already present a distance effect for symbolic and non-symbolic magnitudes and that the effects concentrated around the same components and electrodes studied by Dehaene. However, in this study they did not use source localization analysis, therefore it is not easy to draw conclusions about the exact localization of the effects they found. Taken together, the evidence from ERP studies consistently shows that for adults and children, the N1, P2p and P3 components have a strong relation with the processing 5

18 of magnitudes using both numerals and numerosities. Additionally, the general finding is that these effects are stronger in temporo-parietal electrodes that are close to HIPS. The overwhelming amount of evidence that relates HIPS to the processing of numerosity is also consistent with the general properties of the IPS as a place critical for the processing of continuous magnitudes like luminosity and size (Pinel et al., 2004; Shuman & Kanwisher, 2004). Therefore, the recruitment of the areas that process analog magnitudes like space to process numeric magnitudes provides a simple interpretation for the findings reported above. However, it is clear that HIPS is not the only brain area that plays a central role in number processing. Another area that has been involved with several tasks of number processing is the prefrontal cortex. Generally speaking, the prefrontal cortex is more active in children than adults (Ansari et al., 2005; Ansari & Dhital, 2006; Cantlon et al., 2006, Kaufmann et al., 2006) and more active in studies that use numerosities like dots, sounds or sequences of taps, than in tasks that involve numerals (Cantlon et al., 2006, Kansaku et al., 2006; Venkatraman et al., 2005). The two critical points from these findings regarding the prefrontal cortex is the importance it plays in counting discrete numerosities and the fact that although it is still active in adults, it is more active in children. Similarly, in Nieder and Miller s (2004) single cell recording study, although there were numerons located in the posterior parietal cortex, the majority of the neurons were located in the prefrontal cortex. Because the largest concentration of these numerons was found in the prefrontal cortex, this study suggests that, at least in macaque monkeys, the processing of numerosities has a very important prefrontal component. As it was the case in the fmri studies reviewed in the previous paragraph, the studies that use numerosities, instead of numerals, tend to show more activation in the prefrontal cortex. 6

19 Recently, an ERP study with 3-month-old infants using a habituation paradigm found that number deviants generated activity in right inferior frontal areas (Izard, Dehaene-Lambertz & Dehaene, 2008). Therefore, from a developmental standpoint, it seems that there is converging evidence of a prefrontal to parietal shift in the processing of numeric magnitude that merits being explored further. Additionally, there are no ERP studies that have used a number line estimation task. As we foreshadowed in the beginning paragraphs, this task has shown important developmental differences, thus it is possible that by using this task we can identify some differences that are relevant to the issue at hand. Furthermore, because it is still an open question if the number line task will elicit the components normally related with magnitude processing, it is of interest to develop a characterization of the brain activity associated to this task and its relations to other number comparison tasks. Because the research on the neural correlates of number processing has been focused in posterior parietal regions, it is still not clear what is the precise role that is being played by the prefrontal cortex. One hypothesis that is consistent with what is known about the prefrontal cortex is that the increase in activation in studies that use numerosities instead of numerals, could signal domain-general attentional demands or processes of response selection that get active in the presence of more difficult tasks. Consistent with this hypothesis, it would be expected to find less activation in prefrontal cortex for numerals than for numerosities because we have experienced throughout our lives extensive training with numerals and its process could be more automatic. Similarly, this would explain the results in children because it is reasonable to assume that the tasks used are more difficult for children than for adults. Likewise, the single cell recording results could be explained from this perspective because the use of a delayed match to sample task typically demands resources from prefrontal cortex. Moreover, because most of the studies mentioned above that have found effects in the 7

20 prefrontal cortex were comparisons that used exclusively numerical stimuli, there is a confound between the numeric distance effect and task demands. Therefore, there is no way to test whether the results are due to attentional demands or number-specific processing. Despite these results, Cantlon et al. s (2006) study found greater activity in the prefrontal cortex in children for numerosity deviants vs. shape deviants in a habituation paradigm. Therefore, this finding rules out the response selection hypothesis because there was no response needed, and brings doubts about the attentional demands hypothesis because there should not be significant differences in attentional demands between the shape deviant and number deviant trials. A second possibility is that prefrontal activation is related to active maintenance (O Reilly & Munakata, 2000). If this were the case, it would make sense that studies that use numerosities show more activity in the prefrontal cortex than studies that use Arabic numerals. Similarly, children would show greater prefrontal activity because they would have more difficulty keeping the information active for enough time to perform the appropriate task. If this hypothesis is true, then there should not be a difference in activation of the prefrontal cortex in a number line task because it is not a task that requires major maintenance demands. Therefore, this study could provide some evidence for or against this hypothesis. Finally, as it was mentioned before, it is possible that the prefrontal activation is more specific to number and is the result of the amount of familiarity that the person has with the modality of the magnitudes, or the specific range of numbers. In this case, the prefrontal cortex would originally be critical for numeric representation and only through extensive learning this process would be automatized and transferred to a posterior cortex (HIPS). Therefore, if the logarithmic-to-linear shift hypothesis is true, adults linear representation should be more entrenched and therefore should produce 8

21 activation only in HIPS, especially with numerals. On the other hand, children would be just starting to learn this linear representation, and therefore their representation for Arabic numerals should not be completely entrenched in HIPS yet. Additionally, children should show more activation of prefrontal cortex than adults, both with numerals and numerosities. Rivera, Reiss, Eckert and Menon (2005) found that there is a developmental shift from prefrontal cortex to the Left IPS while performing arithmetic tasks. The authors found a negative correlation between age and prefrontal cortex activity and a positive correlation between age and Left IPS activity. Moreover, this activity was not related to task difficulty or gray matter density. Perhaps the most interesting aspect of this finding is that other studies have reported increased recruitment of the prefrontal cortex with age for skills such as word reading, response inhibition and working memory (Booth et al., 2003; Kwon, Reiss & Menon, 2002). Thus, consistent with the hypothesis proposed in this paper, these results suggest that the developmental pattern of change in the brain seems to be more related to the automatization of the representation of numbers than to other domain general processes associated to the prefrontal cortex. In conjunction with the results from fmri studies that use children, reviewed above, single cell recording studies bring support to a more numeric-specific role of the prefrontal cortex than just the domain-general process related to difficulty or attentional demands. Recently, Nieder and Merten (2007) extended their findings to numerosities up to 30, and again, in this study the neurons that coded for these numerosities were found in the prefrontal cortex. Similarly, Diester and Nieder (2007) taught monkeys to associate arbitrary shapes (numerals) to numerosities from 1 to 4 and after months of training recorded the activities of neurons in the prefrontal cortex and the IPS. The authors found that the proportion of association neurons that responded to both, sets of numerosities and 9

22 numerals was overwhelmingly higher for the prefrontal cortex in comparison with the IPS. These results support the view that the prefrontal cortex initially plays a major role in number processing and that the association of arbitrary shapes (e.g. Arabic numerals) is originally done there. Thus, these studies suggest that the involvement of the IPS as an abstract representation of numeric magnitude depends on learning and repeated exposure to the associations between the numerals and the magnitudes they represent. Therefore, evidence from single cell recordings appears to give support to a developmental interpretation for a prefrontal to HIPS shift of the abstract numeric magnitude representation. As it was briefly mentioned above, the developmental hypothesis outlined here was originated from results from behavioral experiments in cognitive development research. Therefore to fully understand this hypothesis it is important to summarize some of the behavioral signatures of number representation and number estimation that have been useful to understand the representation of numbers. In particular, studies on the development of number estimation have led to the proposal of a logarithmic to linear shift hypothesis of number representation that provides support for the frontal to parietal shift hypothesis argued in the preceding paragraphs. Behavioral evidence from numerical estimation: Several studies have found that although numerosity is a property of sets of discrete objects, animals and humans represent numerosity as a nonverbal analog magnitude that works in the same way as the representation of continuous quantities like length or brightness. This means that representations of sets follow Fechner s law. This property has been demonstrated by the presence of a size effect and a distance effect in number discrimination tasks (Dehaene, Dehaene-Lamberz & Cohen, 1998; Gallistell, & Gelman, 2000; Moyer & Landauer, 1967; Sekuler & Mierkewitz, 1977; Van Oeffelen & Vos, 1982). The distance effect refers to the fact that it is increasingly more difficult to 10

23 discriminate between two numbers if the distance between them decreases. Thus, it is harder to discriminate between 5 and 9 than between 5 and 15. The size effect means that even with equal distances, numbers are increasingly difficult to discriminate as the magnitude increases. As a result, it is harder to discriminate between 15 and 19 than it is to discriminate between 5 and 9. These two results have been found robustly in different species and both with non-symbolic numerosities and symbolic numerals (Moyer & Landauer, 1967; Van Oeffelen & Vos, 1982). Size and distance effects have two implications about the representation of numerical value. First, they suggest representations are noisy and inexact with a Gaussian distribution of activations around the representatives ( numerons ) of each numerosity. This leads small differences between numbers to be harder to discriminate than large differences (i.e., distance effects). Second, they suggest that the psychological distance between numbers decreases with magnitude, much like the compressed spacing of numerals on a logarithmic scale. This causes large numbers to be harder to discriminate than small numbers (i.e., size effects). In support of the log-gaussian hypothesis, young children s placement of symbolic numbers on number lines is highly logarithmic, especially for large and unfamiliar numeric ranges (Booth & Siegler, 2006; Opfer & Siegler, 2007; Opfer & Thompson, 2008; Siegler & Opfer, 2003; Siegler, Thompson, & Opfer, in press; Thompson & Opfer, 2008; Thompson & Opfer, in press). For example, when placing numbers on a number line, 2 nd graders estimates increase logarithmically with actual numerical value, whereas their placements of numbers on a number line are highly linear (Siegler & Opfer, 2003). Similarly, a test of people from the Mundurucu tribe an Amazonian tribe that has only a few words to denote numbers and that has very limited contact with Western societies and formal education (Dehaene, Izard, Pica & Spelke (2006) showed that their estimates for a 0-10 number line were logarithmic. 11

24 However, for a group of participants that had contact with formal education, their estimates in this range were linear (Dehaene, Izard, Spelke & Pica, 2008). This change in performance apparently arises from experience, and not just maturation. Evidence to support this claim comes from studies that show a logarithmic-to-linear shift after only providing feedback on a single trial for a number that has a value that differs quite strongly from that represented in a log-gaussian manner (Izard & Dehaene, 2008; Opfer & Siegler, 2007). Moreover, the developmental change in estimation performance does appear to occur at the level of the representation: training that induces linear numerical estimates transfers to other numerical judgment tasks (Opfer & Thompson, 2008), even when it entails a cost in accuracy (Opfer & DeVries, 2008; Thompson & Opfer, 2008). An account that seems to fit the data that has come from the number line task is that, initially, children s representation of numerals is logarithmically compressed, in the same way as the analog non-symbolic representation that we share with other animals. Later, with experience, children will encounter feedback about the inappropriateness of the logarithmic representation and will develop a linear representation on top of the original logarithmic one. However, this transition will only occur as a function of experience. Hence, because the frequency of smaller numbers in text and speech is disproportionally higher compared to larger numbers (Dehaene & Mehler, 1992), the linear representation will form earlier for smaller ranges of numbers. A possible consequence of this hypothesis for the neural representation of numbers is that experience leads the representation to change its location from prefrontal cortex to a more posterior site (i,e, HIPS). Evidence from perceptual learning has shown that the representation of complex conjunctive stimuli can change locus of representation to more basic levels of processing, both within the visual cortex (Mukai et al., 2007) and between the prefrontal cortex and visual cortex (Eriksson, Larsson, Nyberg, 2008). As a result, information changes from being processed serially and with 12

25 effort, to being processed in parallel and automatically. Therefore, for the case of numeric representation, the abstract representation that is needed for processing numeric magnitude regardless of shape, size, modality or context, could very likely be originally in prefrontal cortex, and then gradually shifted to HIPS based on the extensive amount of experience we have with tasks that require the extraction of magnitude. Present study The present study aims to use event-related potentials to provide a better temporal characterization of what happens in the brain during numerical processing, particularly number line estimation. At present, no previous studies have examined brain activity during number line estimation; consequently, it is unknown whether number line estimation relies on brain regions activated by the more commonly used number comparison tasks. For this purpose, I will present a range of Arabic numerals to participants and examine ERPs when hatch marks appear at various locations on a number line. The general expectation is that number line estimation requires access to representations of numerical magnitude, thereby requiring activation of the same cortical areas (in posterior parietal cortex) that are subject to distance and size effects in number comparison tasks. Additionally, this study examines whether numerical magnitudes are represented linearly, logarithmically, both, or neither. To examine representations of numerical magnitude, ERPs will be compared when locations of numbers on the number line appear in the linear (correct) position, the logarithmic position, and in a position midway between the linear and logarithmic position. Specifically, participants were asked to judge when a hatch mark on the number line was placed in the correct position for a given number. Components of event-related potentials to positions of numbers on number lines are potentially quite revealing about representations of numerical magnitude. Generally, 13

26 the greatest P3 amplitudes are elicited by targets that most violate subjects expectations. Thus, a higher P3 response to numbers marked in non-linear positions would indicate that the number was expected to appear in the linear position, whereas a higher P3 response to numbers marked in linear positions would indicate the number was expected to appear in a non-linear position. The N1 component of ERPs to positions of numbers on the number line is similarly revealing. Generally, the greatest N1 amplitudes are elicited when targets match the subject s orientation of attention. Thus, higher N1 responses to numbers marked in the non-linear position would indicate the numbers were expected to appear in the non-linear positions, whereas a higher N1 response to numbers marked in the linear position would indicate that they were expected to appear in the linear position. Previous behavioral studies have suggested that linear and logarithmic representations can co-exist and compete for use. Results from number line estimation tasks show that individual children know and use both linear and logarithmic representations of numerical magnitude; that with age and experience, children rely increasingly on correct, linear representations rather than intuitive, logarithmic ones; and that the same integers can elicit either a logarithmic or linear pattern of magnitude estimates, depending on the size of the numeric interval tested (Opfer & DeVries, 2008; Opfer & Siegler, 2007; Opfer & Thompson, 2008; Siegler & Opfer, 2003). Similar use of logarithmic representations have also been found among adult Amazonian indigene (Deahene et al., 2008; Pica et al., 2004), and logarithmic representations of numeric magnitude are also implied by adults numeric comparisons when pressured by time or economic competition (Moyer & Landauer, 1967; Furlong & Opfer, 2009; though see Cantlon et al., 2009, for an alternative interpretation). Thus, the design of our study allowed us to examine whether logarithmic representations also co-exist with linear representations among Western, college-educated students. Finally, this study can 14

27 provide information regarding the spatial topography of numerical representation. In particular, we were interested in exploring the possible roles that prefrontal cortex and HIPS play during number line estimation. 15

28 Method Participants Participants (N = 21, mean age = 20.5, 8 female) were recruited from an introductory psychology class and were awarded course credit for their participation in the experiment. Nineteen participants were right handed, and all had normal or corrected to normal vision. Task Participants were presented with a line flanked by a 0 and a 1000, an Arabic numeral, and were asked to determine if a hatch mark on the number line corresponded or not to the given numeral. For each of the numerals, the hatch mark appeared in three different positions. On the linear trials, the position of the hatch mark across the number line corresponded exactly to its linear position (e.g. 100 would appear at 10% of the line); on the logarithmic trials, the position of the mark was where log (n) would be (e.g. 100 would appear at 66% of the line); and in the log-linear trials, the position of the mark was half-way between the linear position and the logarithmic position. Stimuli Each problem presented a blank number line with a width of 255 pixels, labeled with 0 on the left end and 1000 on the right end. The numbers presented appeared on the top of the screen 192 pixels over the line (half point between the top of the screen and the number line). The numbers tested were 5, 78, 150, 606, 725 and 938. These numerosities were selected because they sample the whole length of the line and also maximize the discriminability between the linear and logarithmic representations. All 16

29 stimuli were presented in a dark and sound attenuated room using DirecRT (Jarvis, 2006). Procedure and design: Participants were instructed to identify if the position of the hatch mark on a number line corresponded to the numeral presented by pressing one key ( Q ) if the position of the hatch mark was correct, and by pressing another ( P ) if the position of the hatch mark was incorrect (keys were counterbalanced between participants). Additionally, they were instructed to answer as quickly and accurate as possible. Finally, participants were instructed to hold as still as possible, to avoid blinking, and to try to limit their blinks to the periods between trials (i.e. after responding). At the beginning of each trial, the number line with the marked end points appeared and a fixation was placed where the target numerals were going to be shown for a period of 1 second. Next, the stimulus (i.e. the numeral) replaced the fixation for another 1-second interval. After this period, the hatch mark was placed either in the linear, logarithmic or log-linear position. Once the hatch mark was in place, participants had to decide if the mark was correct or incorrect and press the appropriate key. A 2000 ms intertrial stimulus interval (ISI) was used (See Figure 1). Participants were tested on three different sets of trials and the design of the study was all within subjects. Thus, on each block, participants encountered each of the six numerals compared to three possible hatch mark positions (linear, logarithmic, loglinear). The experiment consisted of 16 blocks and presentation of the trials was randomized within each block. ERP recording procedure: After attaining informed consent from participants, a NuAmps quick cap with 32 Ag/AgCl electrodes (Compumedics Neuroscan, El Paso, TX, USA) was placed on their heads to record their brain activity (see Figure 2). Linked ears served as reference during 17

30 recording. Before the beginning of the experiment, impedances were held below 40 kω 1. The electroencephalogram (EEG) was amplified with an A/D conversion rate of 1000 and a gain of 250mV. Finally, a recording low-pass filter of 300Hz was used. Before analysis of the data, the raw EEG data were processed offline using BESA (Version 5.2). Raw data were re-referenced to an average of all electrodes to get a better approximation to the true voltage average over the whole head (Dien, 1998). Additionally, a digital 0.1 to 30Hz bandpass filter was used to eliminate noise from nonneural origin such as skin potentials, muscle contraction and electrical devices (Holinger et al., 2000; Luck, 2005). Also, using an artifact correction algorithm based on spatial components method (Berg & Scherg, 1994), the data were corrected to reduce ocular artifacts and blinks. After the data were corrected, an artifact rejection procedure (tailored to each individual) was conducted. In any case, trials that had ± 100µV, amplitude fluctuations from one point to the next greater than 11µV, or signal lower than.01 µv, were rejected. After artifact rejection, 7 participants who had less than 85% of the trials accepted were removed from further analyses. After artifact rejection, ERP epochs (-200ms to 1000ms) were created for the three trial types (i.e. linear, logarithmic and log-linear), for number size (i.e. small numbers and large numbers), and for hatch mark number size (i.e. hatch marks that corresponded to small numbers and large numbers). These epochs were selected to look at possible differences between linear and logarithmic representations of numbers, and to find the components relevant to the size effect, respectively. 1 Although the impedance threshold for accepting a participant was 40 kω, in reality most of the electrodes achieved impedances of 10 to 15 kω. 18

31 Results Behavioral results Number comparison is typically characterized by effects of distance and size on speed and accuracy of judgments (Moyer & Landauer, 1967). In this study we obtained similar results for judgments of number line placements (see Table 1). Consistent with distance effects, log-linear trials, which were closer to the correct (linear) placements than logarithmic ones, required more time to solve and resulted in the lowest accuracy rates. Consistent with size effects, judging the location of large numbers (i.e. larger than 500) on the number line required more time than judging the location of small numbers (i.e. smaller than 500), with accuracy also being lower for placement of large numbers compared to small numbers. Finally, there was evidence of interactive effects of size and trial type, with larger effects of trial type for small numbers (ω 2 =.54) than for large numbers (ω 2 =.32) on reaction times. This interaction is interesting because it suggests that representations of small numeric magnitudes are more strongly linear and nonlogarithmic than representations of large numeric magnitudes, leading to less discriminability between trial types for the large magnitudes. A potential problem with accuracy measures, such as those reported above is that they can fail to detect systematic response biases. To address this issue, d analyses were conducted for each number presented to assess the discriminability of the linear, logarithmic, and log-linear number line placements. Because performance of participants was near ceiling, hits and false alarm rates were corrected. Specifically, hit 19

32 rates were constructed by the formula (hits + 1)/(total trials + 2), and false alarm rates were constructed by the formula (false alarms + 1)/(total trials + 2). As predicted by the size effect, discriminability between the linear, and the logarithmic and log-linear trials declined with numeric size (see Figures 3-4). This result was confirmed by a one-way repeated measures ANOVA (F(5,100) = 36.83, p <.001, ω 2 = 0.59). An alternative explanation of this result is that it is due to the distance between the linear and logarithmic trials not being constant throughout the whole range of numbers (See Figure 3). Thus, it is possible that the reason why discrimination decreases for the numbers 725 and 938 is because the distances between the linear and logarithmic trials decrease too. To test this alternative hypothesis, I performed a planned comparison between two numbers that differ in size but that have the same distance between the linear and logarithmic hatch mark positions (5 and 725). As predicted by the size effect, even though the distance between the linear and logarithmic trials is equal for these two numbers, discriminability was significantly smaller for 725 (d = 2.25, SD = 0.78) than for 5(d = 3.04, SD = 0.56; p <.001). Electrophysiological results One of the main goals of this study was to understand the temporal characterization of the number line estimation task. Behavioral results showed that participants judged linear trials as correct whereas they judged logarithmic and loglinear trials as incorrect. Also, consistent with the distance effect, judgments for the loglinear trials were less accurate and took more time. Finally, consistent with the size effect, participants judgments for larger numerals were less accurate and took more time than their judgments for smaller numerals. However, behavioral measures only allow us to look at the end result of the processing. On the other hand, ERP measures allow a continuous inspection of processing in a millisecond-by-millisecond basis. Therefore, to gain insight into this pattern of results the use of EEG waveforms evoked 20

33 by the presentation of the hatch marks in the number line estimation task can be very useful. Two components that could be relevant for understanding these behavioral results are the N1 and P3 components. The N1 component has been found to be present in number identification (Dehaene, 1996) and therefore, could help explain the identification of linear trials as correct. On the other hand, the P3 component is related to surprise (Luck, 2005) and therefore could be involved in the rejection of logarithmic and log-linear trials. To understand the temporal characterization of the number line estimation task, average waveforms were computed for the three experimental trials (i.e. linear, logarithmic, log-linear). Additionally, these waveforms were averaged into four different electrode sites with the purpose of reducing the experiment wise error caused by computing multiple statistical comparisons. Because in previous studies there were no significant effects in the midline, these electrodes were not included in the analyses. The frontal left (FL) electrode site was computed by averaging the electrodes FP1, F3, F7, FC3, and FC7. The frontal right (FR) electrode site was computed by averaging the electrodes FP2, F4, F8, FC4, FC8. The parietal left (PL) electrode site was computed by averaging the electrodes CP3, TP7, P3, P7. The parietal right (PR) electrode site was computed by averaging the electrodes CP4, TP8, P4, P8 (See Figure 2). A visual inspection of these waveforms showed an effect of trial type at the N1, P3 and P1 peaks (see Figures 5-6). To calculate the N1 peak, mean amplitudes were computed over the ms time window. To calculate the P3 peak, mean amplitudes were computed for the ms time window. Although in previous experiments P2p and P3 components were discriminated, in the present study, there seems to be a strong overlap between these two peaks. Therefore, the time window mentioned above corresponds to the average of both P2p and P3 peaks. Finally, because there were no theoretical reasons to expect an effect at the P1 component, and because 21

34 there could be a high overlap between the P1 and N1 components (Luck, 2005), the P1 peak was not included in further analyses. The visual inspection of these waveforms is consistent with the main hypothesis from the study (see Figures 5-7). First linear trials generated a greater N1 peak than both the logarithmic and log-linear trials, especially in parietal electrode sites. Moreover, at frontal electrode sites, the logarithmic trials generated a greater P3 peak than the loglinear trials, and in turn, the log-linear trials generated a greater P3 peak than the linear trials. These effects suggest that even before the behavioral response is effectuated, there is a strong recognition of the linear placements of numbers followed by a signal of surprise related to the logarithmic and log-linear placements of numbers. To test for these effects statistically, a 3-way (trial type: linear, logarithmic, loglinear x electrode site: FL, FR, PL, PR x component: N1, P3) repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on the mean amplitudes calculated for the N1 and P3 time windows. All reported p-values are Greenhouse-Geisser corrected for violations of sphericity assumptions. Results indicated a significant component x electrode interaction, (F(3,39) = 8.37, p <.001, η 2 = 0.39). This effect is largely due to a larger N1 component in parietal sites compared to frontal sites. Furthermore, as expected, a trial type x electrode x component interaction was significant (F(6,78) = 3.85, p =.033, η 2 = 0.23). A further exploration of this interaction determined that it was due to significant simple main effects of trial type at the N1 component for the PL (F(2,26) = 8.07, p =.003, ω 2 = 0.25) and PR (F(2,26) = 14.81, p <.001, ω 2 = 0.40) electrode sites, and to significant simple main effects of trial type at the P3 component for the FR (F(2,26) = 4.69, p =.048, ω 2 = 0.16) and PL (F(2,26) = 18.25, p <.001, ω 2 = 0.45) electrode sites. For the simple main effects at the N1 component (see Figure5), pairwise comparisons revealed that at the PL electrode site, there was a significant difference in mean amplitude between the linear trials (M = -0.11, SD = 0.81) and both the logarithm 22

Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures

Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this

More information

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Submitted to Current Biology Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Anna Plodowski, Rachel Swainson, Georgina M. Jackson, Chris Rorden and Stephen R. Jackson School of Psychology

More information

Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli

Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli Behav Res (2011) 43:981 986 DOI 10.3758/s13428-011-0097-5 Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli Titia Gebuis & Bert Reynvoet Published online: 22 April 2011 # Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2011 Abstract Studies

More information

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source

More information

How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled?

How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 21, 72 (7), 1839-1853 doi:1758/app.72.7.1839 How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled? MIDORI TOKITA

More information

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential Title of Thesis Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential 2014 September Yang Weiping The Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology (Doctor s Course)

More information

Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans Jessica F. Cantlon and Elizabeth M. Brannon

Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans Jessica F. Cantlon and Elizabeth M. Brannon PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans Jessica F. Cantlon and Elizabeth M. Brannon Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Mental Representation of Number in Different Numerical Forms

Mental Representation of Number in Different Numerical Forms Current Biology, Vol. 13, 2045 2050, December 2, 2003, 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2003.11.023 Mental Representation of Number in Different Numerical Forms Anna Plodowski,

More information

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction Proceedings of the First International Conference on Complex Medical Engineering (CME2005) May 15-18, 2005, Takamatsu, Japan (Organized Session No. 20). Paper No. 150, pp. 715-719. Event-Related Potentials

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG Outline of Talk Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials Shafali Spurling Jeste Assistant Professor in Psychiatry and Neurology UCLA Center for Autism Research and Treatment Basic definitions and

More information

Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel

Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel 1 This is an in press article to appear in Psychological Science, summer 2006 Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel Justin Halberda, Johns Hopkins University Sean F. Sires,

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task Psychophysiology, 44 (2007), 913 917. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2007 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00561.x BRIEF REPORT Reward prediction

More information

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention University of New Mexico UNM Digital Repository Psychology ETDs Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2-9-2010 Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention David B.

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study Armita Faghani Jadidi, Raheleh Davoodi, Mohammad Hassan Moradi Department of Biomedical Engineering Amirkabir University

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2008, 8 (2), 195-210 doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.2.195 The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies MIKA

More information

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences from the N170 adaptation effect Xiao-hua Cao 1, Chao Li 1, Carl M Gaspar 2, Bei Jiang 1 1. Department of Psychology,Zhejiang Normal

More information

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln 1 DATA MANAGEMENT Backups Storage Identification Analyses 2 Data Analysis Pre-processing Statistical Analysis

More information

Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults

Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 95, pp. 7836 7841, June 1998 Psychology Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults ELISE TEMPLE AND MICHAEL I. POSNER* Department of Psychology,

More information

Auditory Dominance: Overshadowing or Response Competition?

Auditory Dominance: Overshadowing or Response Competition? Auditory Dominance: Overshadowing or Response Competition? Christopher W. Robinson (robinson.777@osu.edu) Center for Cognitive Science The Ohio State University 208F Ohio Stadium East, 1961 Tuttle Park

More information

Supplementary material to Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures

Supplementary material to Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures Supplementary material to Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures I. Method Stanislas Dehaene, Véronique Izard, Elizabeth Spelke and Pierre Pica

More information

ABSTRACT 1. INTRODUCTION 2. ARTIFACT REJECTION ON RAW DATA

ABSTRACT 1. INTRODUCTION 2. ARTIFACT REJECTION ON RAW DATA AUTOMATIC ARTIFACT REJECTION FOR EEG DATA USING HIGH-ORDER STATISTICS AND INDEPENDENT COMPONENT ANALYSIS A. Delorme, S. Makeig, T. Sejnowski CNL, Salk Institute 11 N. Torrey Pines Road La Jolla, CA 917,

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

Chronometric Studies of Numerical Cognition in Five-month-old Infants. Justin N. Wood & Elizabeth S. Spelke Harvard University

Chronometric Studies of Numerical Cognition in Five-month-old Infants. Justin N. Wood & Elizabeth S. Spelke Harvard University Chronometric Studies of Numerical Cognition in Five-month-old Infants Justin N. Wood & Elizabeth S. Spelke Harvard University Address correspondence to: Justin N. Wood Laboratory for Developmental Studies

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Journal of Experimental Psychology: General The Interplay Between Nonsymbolic Number and Its Continuous Visual Properties Titia Gebuis and Bert Reynvoet Online First Publication, November 14, 2011. doi:

More information

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Cognitive Brain Research 18 (2004) 255 272 Research report Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Audrey Duarte a, *, Charan Ranganath

More information

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials Chinese Science Bulletin 2007 Science in China Press Springer-Verlag Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials SONG Yan 1, PENG

More information

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition RESEARCH ARTICLE A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition Sameer Sethi 1 *, Dr. Simon Rigoulot 2, Dr. Marc D. Pell 3 1 Faculty of Science, McGill University,

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information ten Oever and Sack 10.1073/pnas.1517519112 SI Materials and Methods Experiment 1. Participants. A total of 20 participants (9 male; age range 18 32 y; mean age 25 y) participated

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Goals for the lecture Be able to: Conceptually understand some of the analysis and jargon encountered in a typical (sensory)

More information

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Fundamentals of Psychophysics Fundamentals of Psychophysics John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3045! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Visual neuroscience physiology stimulus How do we see the world? neuroimaging

More information

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 50 (2010) 329 339 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Switching associations between facial identity and emotional expression:

More information

Comparing the neural distance effect derived from the non symbolic comparison and the same different task

Comparing the neural distance effect derived from the non symbolic comparison and the same different task HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 14 February 2013 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00028 Comparing the neural distance effect derived from the non symbolic comparison and the same different

More information

Adults number-line estimation strategies: Evidence from eye movements

Adults number-line estimation strategies: Evidence from eye movements Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:557 563 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0081-1 Adults number-line estimation strategies: Evidence from eye movements Jessica L. Sullivan & Barbara J. Juhasz & Timothy J. Slattery & Hilary

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131 ! Chapter 5 Summary and Conclusions! 131 Chapter 5!!!! Summary of the main findings The present thesis investigated the sensory representation of natural sounds in the human auditory cortex. Specifically,

More information

Attentional Blink Paradigm

Attentional Blink Paradigm Attentional Blink Paradigm ATTENTIONAL BLINK 83 ms stimulus onset asychrony between all stimuli B T D A 3 N P Z F R K M R N Lag 3 Target 1 Target 2 After detection of a target in a rapid stream of visual

More information

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J. The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem By Andrew Laguna, S.J. Outline I. Introduction II. The Visual System III. What is the Binding Problem? IV. Possible Theoretical Solutions

More information

The effect of endpoint knowledge on dot enumeration

The effect of endpoint knowledge on dot enumeration UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones 8-2011 The effect of endpoint knowledge on dot enumeration Alex Michael Moore University of Nevada, Las Vegas Follow this and additional works

More information

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired

More information

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2009) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Child Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp Children s

More information

Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks

Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks Psychon Bull Rev (211) 18:331 338 DOI 1.3758/s13423-11-59-z Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks Dale J. Cohen & Daryn Blanc-Goldhammer Published online: 11 February 211 # Psychonomic

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry International Journal of Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities Volume 5 Article 3 June 2013 Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry Whitney R. Arthur Central Washington

More information

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves SICE Annual Conference 27 Sept. 17-2, 27, Kagawa University, Japan Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves Seiji Nishifuji 1, Kentaro Fujisaki 1 and Shogo Tanaka 1 1

More information

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight? How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? What happens to visual-devoted brain structure in individuals who are born deprived of sight?

More information

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14 CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines Assignment 5: Neural mechanisms of object-based attention Due: Friday, April 14 This Assignment is a guided reading of the 2014 paper, Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Chapter 1 Introduction. Welcome to Our World Thresholds and the Dawn of Psychophysics Sensory Neuroscience and the Biology of Perception

Chapter 1 Introduction. Welcome to Our World Thresholds and the Dawn of Psychophysics Sensory Neuroscience and the Biology of Perception 1 Introduction Chapter 1 Introduction Welcome to Our World Thresholds and the Dawn of Psychophysics Sensory Neuroscience and the Biology of Perception Introduction What do we mean by Sensation & Perception?

More information

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems Experimental Design Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems Overview Design of functional neuroimaging studies Categorical designs Factorial designs Parametric

More information

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey 3.6.QH Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey Versnel, Huib; Zwiers, Marcel; Van Opstal, John Department of Biophysics University of Nijmegen Geert Grooteplein 655

More information

Provided by the author(s) and NUI Galway in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite the published version when available. Title Prefrontal cortex and the generation of oscillatory visual persistence

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Forsyth et al. 10.1073/pnas.1509262112 SI Methods Inclusion Criteria. Participants were eligible for the study if they were between 18 and 30 y of age; were comfortable reading in

More information

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences 2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences Stanislas Dehaene Chair of Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture n 5 Bayesian Decision-Making Lecture material translated

More information

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Materials Supplementary Materials 1. Material and Methods: Our stimuli set comprised 24 exemplars for each of the five visual categories presented in the study: faces, houses, tools, strings and false-fonts. Examples

More information

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Running Head: Visual Working Memory and the CDA Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Nick Berggren and

More information

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society

Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society Online Publication Date: 15 th July 2012 Publisher: Asian Economic and Social Society Event-Related Potentials Reveal Perceptual Simulation of Color in Language Comprehension Aitao Lu (Center for Studies

More information

Categorical Perception

Categorical Perception Categorical Perception Discrimination for some speech contrasts is poor within phonetic categories and good between categories. Unusual, not found for most perceptual contrasts. Influenced by task, expectations,

More information

Practicalities of EEG Measurement and Experiment Design YATING LIU

Practicalities of EEG Measurement and Experiment Design YATING LIU Practicalities of EEG Measurement and Experiment Design YATING LIU 2014.02.04 Content Designing Experiments: Discuss & Pilot Event Markers Intra- and Intertrial Timing How Many Trials You Will Need How

More information

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Elizabeth M. Brannon*, Courtney J. Wusthoff #, C.R. Gallistel^, and John Gibbon @ * Center for Cognitive Neuroscience

More information

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Hillyard(1973) Dichotic Listening Task N1 component enhanced for attended stimuli Supports early selection Effects of Voluntary

More information

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background:

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background: This presentation was given during the Dianalund Summer School on EEG and Epilepsy, July 24, 2012. The main purpose of this introductory talk is to show the possibilities of improved seizure onset analysis

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 Supporting Information Variances and biases of absolute distributions were larger in the 2-line

More information

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search Steven Phillips (steve@ni.aist.go.jp) Neuroscience Research Institute (AIST), Tsukuba Central 2, 1-1-1 Umezono, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8568

More information

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 B87 Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized Wim Gevers*,

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Comparing Data Sets: Implicit Summaries of the Statistical Properties of Number Sets

Comparing Data Sets: Implicit Summaries of the Statistical Properties of Number Sets Cognitive Science 39 (2015) 156 170 Copyright 2014 Cognitive Science Society, Inc. All rights reserved. ISSN: 0364-0213 print / 1551-6709 online DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12141 Comparing Data Sets: Implicit Summaries

More information

Discovering Processing Stages by combining EEG with Hidden Markov Models

Discovering Processing Stages by combining EEG with Hidden Markov Models Discovering Processing Stages by combining EEG with Hidden Markov Models Jelmer P. Borst (jelmer@cmu.edu) John R. Anderson (ja+@cmu.edu) Dept. of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University Abstract A new method

More information

Spatial Intuition in Elementary Arithmetic: A Neurocomputational Account

Spatial Intuition in Elementary Arithmetic: A Neurocomputational Account Spatial Intuition in Elementary Arithmetic: A Neurocomputational Account Qi Chen 1,2 *, Tom Verguts 2 1 Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Albert Einstein Healthcare Network, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania,

More information

Neurophysiologically Driven Image Triage: A Pilot Study

Neurophysiologically Driven Image Triage: A Pilot Study Neurophysiologically Driven Image Triage: A Pilot Study Santosh Mathan Honeywell Laboratories 3660 Technology Dr Minneapolis, MN 55418 USA santosh.mathan@honeywell.com Stephen Whitlow Honeywell Laboratories

More information

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Alexa Morcom Edinburgh SPM course 2017 Thanks to Rik Henson, Thomas Wolbers, Jody Culham, and the SPM authors for slides Overview Categorical designs Factorial designs

More information

Accepted Manuscript. Title: Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers: an ERPs study

Accepted Manuscript. Title: Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers: an ERPs study Accepted Manuscript Title: Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers: an ERPs study Authors: Elena Salillas, Radouane El Yagoubi, Carlo Semenza PII: S0010-9452(07)00083-4

More information

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals Takako Mitsudo *1, Yoshitaka Nakajima 2, Gerard B. Remijn 3, Hiroshige Takeichi 4, Yoshinobu Goto

More information

PAPER The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift

PAPER The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift Developmental Science 14:1 (2011), pp 125 135 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00962.x PAPER The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift Hilary C. Barth and Annie M.

More information

Two-digit Number Comparison:

Two-digit Number Comparison: Two-digit Number Comparison: Digit Similarity Effect on Reaction Time in Two-digit Number Comparison by Hyunmo Pang A thesis submitted to the Department of Psychology in conformity with requirements for

More information

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest Experimental Design Kate Watkins Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford With thanks to: Heidi Johansen-Berg Joe Devlin Outline Choices for experimental paradigm Subtraction / hierarchical

More information

This presentation is the intellectual property of the author. Contact them for permission to reprint and/or distribute.

This presentation is the intellectual property of the author. Contact them for permission to reprint and/or distribute. Modified Combinatorial Nomenclature Montage, Review, and Analysis of High Density EEG Terrence D. Lagerlund, M.D., Ph.D. CP1208045-16 Disclosure Relevant financial relationships None Off-label/investigational

More information

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings J. Besle, P. Lakatos, C.A. Schevon, R.R. Goodman, G.M. McKhann, A. Mehta, R.G. Emerson, C.E.

More information

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Nick Berggren and Martin Eimer Abstract During the retention of

More information

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts Main Goals of this Paper: Professor JeeLoo Liu 1. To present an account of phenomenal

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Distinct Cerebral Pathways for Object Identity and Number in Human Infants

Distinct Cerebral Pathways for Object Identity and Number in Human Infants Distinct Cerebral Pathways for Object Identity and Number in Human Infants Véronique Izard 1,2,3,4*, Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz 1,2,3,5, Stanislas Dehaene 1,2,3,6 PLoS BIOLOGY 1 INSERM, U562, Cognitive

More information

Quick Guide - eabr with Eclipse

Quick Guide - eabr with Eclipse What is eabr? Quick Guide - eabr with Eclipse An electrical Auditory Brainstem Response (eabr) is a measurement of the ABR using an electrical stimulus. Instead of a traditional acoustic stimulus the cochlear

More information

EEG History. Where and why is EEG used? 8/2/2010

EEG History. Where and why is EEG used? 8/2/2010 EEG History Hans Berger 1873-1941 Edgar Douglas Adrian, an English physician, was one of the first scientists to record a single nerve fiber potential Although Adrian is credited with the discovery of

More information

Attention and Scene Perception

Attention and Scene Perception Theories of attention Techniques for studying scene perception Physiological basis of attention Attention and single cells Disorders of attention Scene recognition attention any of a large set of selection

More information

Parallels in Stimulus-Driven Oscillatory Brain Responses to Numerosity Changes in Adults and Seven-Month-Old Infants

Parallels in Stimulus-Driven Oscillatory Brain Responses to Numerosity Changes in Adults and Seven-Month-Old Infants DEVELOPMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 36(6), 651 667 Copyright 2011 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 8756-5641 print / 1532-6942 online DOI: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549883 Parallels in Stimulus-Driven Oscillatory

More information

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA 1 Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA CONTENTS I Personal and clinical data II Conclusion. III Recommendations for therapy IV Report. 1. Procedures of EEG recording and analysis 2. Search for

More information

Carnegie Mellon University Annual Progress Report: 2011 Formula Grant

Carnegie Mellon University Annual Progress Report: 2011 Formula Grant Carnegie Mellon University Annual Progress Report: 2011 Formula Grant Reporting Period January 1, 2012 June 30, 2012 Formula Grant Overview The Carnegie Mellon University received $943,032 in formula funds

More information

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Clayton Hickey 1,2, Vincent Di Lollo 2, and John J. McDonald 2 Abstract & Attentional selection of a target presented among

More information

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception Overview of Questions What is it that makes sounds high pitched or low pitched? How do sound vibrations inside the ear lead to the perception

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 10: Brain-Computer Interfaces Ilya Kuzovkin So Far Stimulus So Far So Far Stimulus What are the neuroimaging techniques you know about? Stimulus So Far

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information