Color Discrimination at Threshold Using Asymmetric Color Matching and Method of

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Color Discrimination at Threshold Using Asymmetric Color Matching and Method of"

Transcription

1 Color Discrimination at Threshold Using Asymmetric Color Matching and Method of Adjustment Discrimination to Test a Six Mechanism Theory of Color Vision Safiya I. Lahlaf Undergraduate Honors Thesis for Honors in Behavioral Neuroscience Departmental Committee: Rhea T. Eskew, Jr. (Advisor) Peter Bex Department of Psychology Northeastern University May 2016

2 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 2 Abstract Human color vision is a vital aspect of our perception of the world. We rely on color perception on a day-to-day basis, ranging from object discrimination and face recognition to the detection and determination of whether food is spoiled. Color vision is caused by the differential activation of three cone photoreceptors, each of which is sensitive to a distinct range of wavelengths. However, we have the ability to detect and discriminate between a variety of colors much finer than the broad and overlapping wavelengths that activate the cones. One theory posits that numerous neural mechanisms are required. Another theory holds that as few as six such mechanisms are sufficient, and a later recombination of the signals from these mechanisms gives rise to the variety of discriminable colors. In the present study, using barely-detectable chromatic stimuli, we conducted color matching and method of adjustment discrimination experiments on human subjects to help us understand the number and the characteristics of these post-receptoral mechanisms. Cluster analysis of the color matching data and examination of the discrimination boundaries revealed six unique color categories, which provides further support for a simple six mechanism model. The six mechanism model does an excellent job of accounting for the psychophysical data and helps resolve a current dispute in color vision research on post-receptoral processing.

3 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 3 Color Discrimination at Threshold Using Asymmetric Color Matching and Method of Adjustment Discrimination to Test a Six Mechanism Theory of Color Vision Introduction Human vision is usually considered to be the most important sense and our primary connection to the outside world. One major component of human vision, the perception of color, greatly enhances our experience of viewing the world around us. It is so fundamental to us that we often errantly believe that the color we perceive is inherently part of every object that we view. However, this is not the case. Color perception is simply an interpretation that the mind gives to the physical stimuli that it receives. How this interpretation is constructed is still actively being studied. The physiological explanation for vision is based upon the action of two types of cells located in the retina of both eyes. The following description of the physiology is based upon Wolfe, Kluender, & Levi (2011). These cells, rods and cones, respond to inputs of light. However, rods and cones are active under different conditions. The rod cells are only active in dim light, or scototopic, conditions. Because there is only one kind of rod photoreceptor, there is no discrimination of the signal based upon color. This explains why the world appears black-andwhite at night. On the other hand, cone photoreceptors are active under brighter light, or phototopic, conditions. Because there are several types of cone photoreceptors, we can discriminate different colors under usual daylight conditions. The basis of color discrimination has to do with the cone cells responses to different wavelengths of light. There are three types of cone cells: long (L), medium (M), and short (S). Each type responds maximally to different wavelengths of light: L cones to 565 nm, M to 535 nm, and S to 440 nm (Figure 1). Because these wavelengths are associated with different colors, the

4 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 4 cones are commonly referred to as the red, green, and blue cones, respectively. However, this categorization is not correct, as the cones respond to wavelengths, not to color. Color perception occurs at higher-order levels of the visual processing system. Cone response is based upon the number and wavelengths of the photons it receives. Therefore, although each cone responds differently to various wavelengths of light, the response is also dependent upon the intensity of the light. The higher the intensity, the more photons. At the level of a single cone receptor, because the response is based upon both the wavelength and the intensity, the level of activation cannot accurately predict the wavelength of light. This is called univariance, and it explains the necessity of having multiple cones in order to interpret different wavelengths of light as different colors. The trichromatic theory of color vision was developed to explain our perception of color. Because we now know about the three different types of cones, it is easy to conjecture that color vision is based upon relationships between the three. However, before the existence of cones was even discovered, the trichromatic theory had been developed. Early experiments carried out by Hermann von Helmholtz built the foundation of this theory. In his experiments, Helmholtz presented subjects with a color composed of one wavelength of light. Simultaneously, he presented a color that was composed of three different wavelengths (Figure 2). A mixture of multiple wavelengths of light that combine to produce a single color is called a metamer. He allowed the subjects to manipulate the intensities of the three wavelengths until the metamer matched the original, single-wavelength light. He found that the task could not be done with a two-wavelength metamer. Further, he discovered that four wavelengths were more than needed. This led to the trichromatic theory of color vision. The second stage of visual processing has to do with how the signals from the three cone

5 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 5 cells interact. Ewald Hering s opponent color theory helps to explain this. He found that we cannot see any reddish-green nor any bluish-yellow colors, suggesting a pattern of interaction between the three cone, or Stage 1, signals. The signals from the L and M cones interact such that subtracting the M cone response from the L cone response (L-M) is perceived as red, while subtracting the L from the M cone response (M-L) is perceived as green. Similarly, blue can be perceived from the difference between the S cones signal and the sum of the L and M cones signals (S-(M+L)), while yellow is perceived from the sum of the M and L cone signals minus the S cone signal ((M+L)-S) (Figure 3). The sum of the M and L cones signals is also known as luminance. Stage 2 is defined as these interactions between Stage 1 signals (Figure 4). These represent the neural pathways that eventually lead to the brain. The cone signal interactions are supported with physiological findings. In the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), there are cells that respond preferentially to the colors described above. In the magnocellular layer, cells respond to luminance; in the parvocellular layer, cells respond to red-green; and in the koniocellular layer, cells respond to blue-yellow information (Figure 5). The cellular responses in the LGN support the color opponent theory, as the cells in each layer have excitatory responses to one end of the spectrum and inhibitory responses to the other end. We need to understand color vision mechanisms in order to understand higher-level visual processing. A color mechanism may be defined as a fixed (relative) combination of cone signals that is correlated with the observer s behavior in psychophysical experiments, (Eskew, 2009). One explanation is known as the cardinal axis theory (Figure 6). This theory is based upon the channels described above, which are viewed to be symmetric and bipolar: the achromatic luminance, the red/green, and the blue/yellow channels (Boynton, 1979). This is a broadly-tuned explanation for color vision.

6 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 6 Support for the cardinal axis thory can be seen in psychophysical data. Subjects intensity thresholds (the intensities where the stimulus is just barely visible) for various wavelengths of light were initially measured. Then, the subjects were adapted either along the red/green channel or the blue/yellow channel. This caused increased thresholds in the adapted channel but had little or no effect on the other channel (Krauskopf, Williams, & Heeley, 1982). These results indicate that the two channels are formed from distinct mechanisms. Further support for the cardinal axis theory with its three cone-opponent signals seemed to solidify the validity of this theory (Buchsbaum & Gottschalk, 1983; Hurvich & Jameson, 1957; Lennie & D'Zmura, 1988; Rubin & Richards, 1988). However, it is now clear that mechanisms are unipolar. This classical model describes six mechanisms, the four chromatic red (R), green (G), blue (B), and yellow (Y) and the two achromatic increments (I) and decrements (D) (Eskew R. T., 2008). Thus, every mechanism is independent of each of the others and formed by the opponent combination of signals from the cone photoreceptors. The broadly-tuned explanation for color vision described in the classical mechanism theory is contested. Some believe that there are many more mechanisms than those designated by the classical model. This higher-order theory is based on the recombination of the classical signals, creating distinct channels for very specific colors in addition to the cardinal (D'Kmura, Lennie, & Krauskopf, 1987; D'Zmura, 1991; Gegenfurtner & Kiper, 1992; Gegenfurtner, Kiper, & Levitt, 1997; Kiper, Fenstemaker, & Gegenfurtner, 1997; Krauskopf, Williams, Mandler, & Brown, 1986; Lennie, Krauskopf, & Sclar, 1990; Webster & Mollon, 1994). Conflicting results have come from studies designed to elucidate the number and form of mechanisms. In one study, noise masks used during detection tasks had a significant effect on threshold for a wide range of colors but had little effect on other colors, supporting the broadly-tuned model with only a few mechanisms (Giulianini

7 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 7 & Eskew, 1998). A different study found that noise masking had an effect only at very specific colors, supporting a broadly-tuned model but with many color mechanisms (Hansen & Gegenfurtner, 2010). This contention is the basis for the research addressed in this study. This study was designed to help determine the nature of the different color mechanisms. Like in the studies of Guilianini and Eskew (1998) and Hansen and Gegenfurtner (2010), stimuli were restricted to the LM plane of color space, in which only the L and M cones are modulated while the S cone response is kept constant (Figure 7). The experiments were designed to test a six mechanism model (Shepard, Eskew, McCarthy, & Ochandarena, 2015). The mechanism model depends upon the labeled line assumption, which says that stimuli detected by different mechanisms must be discriminable at all intensities because each mechanism is associated with a distinct hue. As will be shown below, this study used three procedures: detection, color matching, and color discrimination. The mechanism model was based off of the results of the detection task. In the color matching test, stimuli detected by different mechanisms were each matched separately to one of the six different color clusters, allowing for a label to be given to each of the six mechanisms. In the discrimination test, color cues were presented in pairs: a standard and a test cue. The cues were unipolar (i.e., a single color). Using the method of adjustment, the subjects changed the color angle of the presented test cue, moving in one direction per trial, until it was distinguished from the standard. Because colors are indeed detected with only six broadlytuned mechanisms, the standard and test colors were indistinguishable until the test color crossed over into detection by a different mechanism. On the other hand, if there were many color mechanisms, the standard and test cues would have been easily distinguishable over very short ranges. The results of these experiments support the six mechanism model of post-receptoral color processing. Therefore, this study adds to the research explaining color vision mechanisms.

8 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 8 Methods Observers Participants in the study had normal color vision, as evaluated by the Farnsworth-Munsell 100 Hue Test (Farnsworth, 1943). Visual acuity was corrected, if necessary, with a trial lens placed in front of their dominant eye while the other eye was patched. All experiments were conducted in a dark room. Stimuli The stimuli were unipolar Gaussian blobs, σ =1, presented against a gray background field with a rapid-start profile sawtooth of 333 ms total duration with energy equivalent to a 200 ms rectangular flash of the same peak (Figure 8a, b). Detection was tested on at least twenty different chromatic angles per noise condition in the LM plane (3-4 blocks of 100 trials at each angle), with 0 referring to an L cone increment, 90 to an M cone increment, 180 to an L cone decrement, and 270 to an M cone decrement. In the no noise condition, 0, 15, 35, 42, 45, 48, 52, 64, 90, 135, 180, 195, 215, 222, 225, 228, 232, 244, 270, and 315 were run. In 42 noise, 0, 15, 35, 41, 42, 43, 45, 48, 52, 90, 135, 180, 195, 215, 221, 222, 223, 225, 228, 232, 270, and 315 were run. In 64 noise, 0, 15, 35, 45, 48, 52, 64, 70, 90, 135, 180, 195, 215, 225, 228, 232, 244, 250, 270, and 315 were run. In some conditions, chromatic masking noise was added to the stimulus field. To accomplish this, all of the stimuli were half-toned along a vertical line on the screen; alternating regions were assigned to the test or to the noise lines in this case, the mean gray field (see Giulianini & Eskew, 1998). Each region was two pixels in height and extended vertically across the entire stimulus (Figure 8c). The test stimulus was created in the gaps between the noise lines. Since it was assumed that chromatic mechanisms have low spatial resolution, the high spatial

9 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 9 frequency components created by this half-toning procedure were not visible in the chromatic only detected tests but were occasionally visible for the tests near the corner of the contour (i.e., ~42-48, ). Stimuli are represented in two color spaces: cone contrast space and MBDKL space (Derrington, Krauskopf, & Lennie, 1984; MacLeod & Boynton, 1979). Cone contrast space is achieved by dividing local cone excitation coordinates by the associated cone space coordinate of the adapting field, giving ΔL/L or ΔM/M (Eskew, McLellan, & Giulianini, 1999). The local cone excitation coordinates are found by subtracting the baseline excitation levels from the test excitation levels (ΔL = Ltest Ladapt). In cone contrast space, the axes delineate 0 /180 and 90 /270. In MBDKL space, the axes are shifted by 45, so they delineate 315 /135 and 45 /225. This shift is because the axes represent L+M and L M and are rescaled to threshold units (Eskew R. T., 2009; Krauskopf, 1999). MBDKL space was used to spread out the corners of the contour, making it easier to view the points in those regions. Apparatus and Software Stimuli were created on a PowerMacintosh and displayed on a SONY GDM-F520 CRT monitor by a standard video card with a 10-bit digital to analog converter (DACs). Spectroradiometric calibration was performed at 8 nm intervals across the entire spectrum and the monitors were then linearized with the Gamma correction lookup tables. Head position was stabilized with a chin and forehead rest, a corrective lens was placed directly in front of the observer s dominant eye if necessary, and an eye-patch was placed over the other eye prior to the adaptation period. Model A model with six detection mechanisms was fit to the data. The colored lines in Figure 9

10 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 10 show an example; each line represents a constant, threshold response of a different color mechanism. The fitting program finds the best combination of weights applied to the L and M cone signals to describe a section of the thresholds as a line. The solid closed contour is the probability sum of the mechanisms, and shows the predicted thresholds based upon all of the mechanisms. Probability summation refers to the concept that if two stimuli are detected by different mechanisms, each mechanism s contributions to the detection process is independent of the other s, but these signals are later combined (Kingdom & Prins, 2016). Detection Procedure A two-alternative forced-choice, adaptive staircase procedure was used to measure detection thresholds. Observers adapted to the gray background field for 60 seconds before each block of 100 trials. In each run, a single test color direction was used. Each trial consisted of two 333 ms intervals signaled by tones and separated by 400 ms. The observer was asked to determine which interval the test stimulus appeared in and received feedback after each response. The stimulus contrast was decreased by 0.1 log units after three consecutive correct responses and increased by the same amount after one incorrect response. Weibull functions were fit to the frequency-of-seeing data for each run using a maximum likelihood method to estimate two parameters of the psychometric function a threshold estimate corresponding to a detection rate of 82% and an estimate of the psychometric slope. After fitting the Weibull functions, thresholds from multiple runs were averaged (~3-5 runs at each color angle); standard errors were calculated using between-run (mostly between-session) variances. Additional runs were added in cases where the coefficient of variation were unusually high. Color Matching Procedure This color matching task was run using two different screens. The stimuli were presented

11 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 11 on the monitor. The color-match selections were made on a MacBook Pro using the Microsoft PowerPoint program. There was a separate PowerPoint file for each noise condition, as trials were conducted with and without noise. Within one noise condition file, there were five slides for each test angle; these slides corresponded with the five blocks per test angle. Each slide was labeled in the notes section with the test angle for each corresponding block. Every test angle for each noise condition was tested a total of five times. The slides were arranged in random order. Each slide consisted of a gray background with a gray circle, outlined in black, of the same hue as the background. The experimenter used the arrow keys on the keyboard to pseudo-randomly select a slide, keeping the test angle hidden from the subjects. If the chosen slide had already been completed, a new slide was selected. The experimenter opened the color selection window for the circle. The color wheel was positioned in the center of the screen (Figure 10). A black cloth with a hole the size of the color wheel was used to block extraneous cues; only the color wheel was visible. After 60 seconds of adaptation, the subjects completed a two-alternative forced-choice detection test, with the contrast fixed at the previously-determined threshold value (the purpose was to check that the stimulus was at threshold, and to give the observers some practice with the particular stimulus). The program was set to display 50 trials, but it could be stopped whenever the subjects were certain of their color choice. Usually only around 30 trials were required, but sometimes more than 50 were needed. In this case, the subjects again adapted for 60 seconds before completing another 50 trials, or however many were necessary. There were no restrictions on the number of trials. The subjects made color selections on the PowerPoint file on the laptop. The subjects could open the laptop screen, which was closed during runs to prevent them from viewing the light, and make selections on the color wheel. The hue and saturation were adjusted, but the scale designating

12 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 12 color value was kept hidden and remained at 100 for all trials. The subjects could resume the forced-choice detection task and return to the color matching window to refine their selection. After they were satisfied, the experimenter used the subjects color selection to fill in the circle on the slide. The subjects then confirmed the choice a final time by looking at the color selection circle. The stimulus size on the presentation monitor and the color selection circle size were the same. Also, the gray background on the presentation monitor was measured using a colorimeter to be the same as the background on the PowerPoint slides. After each block was completed, the HSV values were measured. The HSV values of the five blocks for each color angle were averaged together. These average colors were measured using an X-Rite spectrophotometer and converted to CIE (u,v) color space (Pauli, 1976). Discrimination Procedure In each trial of the method of adjustment discrimination experiment, the observers were presented with two stimuli (a standard color angle along with a test color angle), both of which were fixed at their individual thresholds (as determined by the model). Observers were asked to adjust the color angle of the second stimulus (the test) until it appeared to be different from the standard; the software kept the second stimulus at threshold (according to the model). The subject could adjust the test angle in increments of one degree to find the angle that was different in appearance from the standard (Figure 9). The adjustments were made in both the clockwise and counterclockwise directions, separately, with five blocks in each direction per run and five runs per color angle done in total. Since the stimuli were equal in strength, they were only discriminated on the basis of hue of the stimulus. In the discrimination task, subjects were adapted for 60 seconds before every run. Each run consisted of five blocks. Each stimulus was presented at the predicted threshold values, determined

13 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 13 from the model fit. Standards were set at 0, 45, 90, 135, 180, 225, 270, and 315. For each standard angle, subjects moved clockwise around the contour for one run and counterclockwise for the other. Therefore, a total of 16 runs were conducted per condition. Each run was repeated a total of five times, with repetitions conducted on separate days to account for daily differences in discrimination. A key pad was used to allow the subjects to adjust the test stimulus. After hitting enter on the key pad, two stimuli flashed on the screen for 200 ms each, with 400 ms between each presentation. A tone sounded simultaneously with each stimulus presentation. The first stimulus was the standard color angle, and the second was the test. Subjects had to determine whether or not the two stimuli were discriminable. At the first presentation, the test was the same angle as the standard. Subjects then used the keypad to change the color angle of the test stimulus. Pressing 1 moved the test one degree clockwise, while pressing 3 moved it one degree counterclockwise; pressing 2 presented the two stimuli again, with the test at the same angle it was at the previous presentation. Hitting 4 and 6 moved the test angle clockwise and counterclockwise, respectively, by increments of 5 degrees, while 7 and 9 moved the test angle clockwise and counterclockwise, respectively, by increments of 10 degrees. There were no time limits or other restrictions, although subjects generally took about five minutes per run. Hitting the period button caused the block to end and indicated that the standard and test stimuli were discriminable. After each block, the angles of the standard and of the discriminable test were recorded. The average of all twenty-five blocks per boundary (five runs at five blocks per run) was calculated, as well as the standard deviation. Results were plotted on the subject s detection contour. Subjects conducted the method of adjustment discrimination task in several different conditions, including no noise and at least two other noise conditions.

14 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 14 Results Threshold averages calculated from the detection data were analyzed and fit with mechanisms. The model used probability summation and fit the threshold detection data across all noise conditions simultaneously. It used the Test Energy vs. Noise Power Relationship and fit the data to k linear mechanisms (Shepard, Eskew, McCarthy, & Ochandarena, 2015). Six, eight, and sixteen mechanisms were tested, with the fit not improving after more than six mechanisms. For six mechanisms, the R 2 value was 98%. The detection threshold averages are shown in Figure 11, which has three columns: the detection data plotted in cone contrast space, the threshold versus color angle plot, and a regression line with the y-axis denoting the predicted values and the x-axis denoting the observed thresholds. The first row shows the data for the no noise condition, the second for the 42 noise condition, and the third for the 64 noise condition (Figure 11a-c). The threshold versus color angle plot includes error bars. In the no noise condition, the data fits closely to the contour (Figure 11a, left) and to the threshold versus color angle plot (Figure 11a, middle). The fit with the regression line is very good (Figure 11a, right). In the 42 noise condition (Figure 11b, left), the data also fits closely with the contour. Additionally, the contour is stretched in the direction of the noise, indicating that the thresholds of points near the noise chromaticity are increased much more than points along the flanks of the contour. This is demonstrated in the sensitivity plot, which has much higher values overall than the no noise plot, but especially along the noise angles (compare Figure 11a and b, middle). The sensitivity plot also reveals several points that do not lie on the contour, within error: the 41 and 43 degree points lie below the contour peak, while the 222 point extends above it. The data fits

15 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 15 well with the line in the regression plot (Figure 11b, right). For the 64 noise, the data fits well with the contour (Figure 11c, left). The contour is not as stretched as the 42 plot in cone contrast space (compare Figure 11b and c, left panels). However, as the threshold plot indicates, the thresholds are much higher overall in the 64 noise condition than in the no noise condition, but lower than in the 42 condition (Figure 11, middle panels). The 15, 52, 64, and 135 points lie slightly above the contour line, while the 15 and 250 points lie slightly below. However, the contour fits the rest of the data well. The data fits well with the line in the regression plot (Figure 11c, right). Each noise condition was analyzed independently and the contours with the linear mechanism lines plotted onto two different planes: cone contrast space and MBDKL space (see Stimuli). These two plots of the same data and model are shown in the left and right panels of Figure 12, respectively. For the no noise condition, six mechanisms were fit to the data (Figure 12a). The threshold points fit the mechanisms very well. None of the mechanisms are parallel to another. The same six mechanisms were fit to the 42 noise data, but the sensitivities were changed (Figure 12b). However, two of the mechanisms, indicated by the blue and dotted lines in the figure, were pushed out. Therefore, only four of the mechanisms were necessary to fit these thresholds. The mechanisms fit the data well. In the 64 condition, the same six mechanisms, with altered sensitivities, were fit to the data (Figure 12c). In this case, the mechanism indicated by the green line was pushed out, leaving the remaining five mechanisms to contribute to the contour. The data fit well with the model mechanisms The color matching data was also analyzed. As an example, the average color match for each color angle in the no noise condition was placed over the corresponding data point in the mechanism plot in cone contrast space (Figure 13). These circular symbols indicate the color

16 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 16 appearance of each test and correspond to the coloring of the mechanism lines that were fit to the thresholds in the detection experiment. These average color matches were also plotted onto CIE (u,v) color space (Figure 14a-c). A K-means Euclidean cluster analysis was conducted, in which individual points were grouped into clusters formed from the points closer to themselves than to other points on the plane. Therefore, the plane was divided into regions, where every point within a particular cell was closer to that cell s centroid than to any other centroid. Within a cluster, the points are not significantly different than each other, so the variability between angles is the same as the variability between tests of a single angle in that cluster. However, between clusters, the points are significantly different from each other. The points in each cluster were colored with a representative hue for the label given to that specific cluster. The white point on each plot represents the color coordinates of the gray background that the stimuli were presented over. The number of clusters of the color matches generally corresponds to the number of mechanisms that describe the detection data in a particular condition. In the no noise condition, there were six clusters; in the 42 noise condition, there were four; and in the 64 noise condition, there were six (Figure 14a-c). The labels given to the clusters were reddish, orangish, yellowish, greenish, bluish, and purplish. In other words, the color appearance of the test stimuli maps very well onto the predictions of the model fit to the forced-choice detection thresholds. For the method of adjustment test, the regions of indiscriminability, both above and below each standard, were indicated with arcs that followed the subjects contours (Figure 15a, b). Each arc was colored according to the color label given to that specific mechanism. If two standards fell within the boundaries of the same mechanism, they were colored the same but differentiated based upon the style of the line. Both the no noise and the 64 noise condition were tested.

17 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 17 Discussion The purpose of this study was to help resolve the current contention in color vision research regarding the nature of higher-order mechanisms that lead to color perception. One theory claims that there are numerous broadly-tuned mechanisms, even at an early post-receptoral stage in visual processing (Hansen & Gegenfurtner, 2010). Our theory, based on previous research, supports not more than six broadly-tuned mechanisms that mediate the L and M cone signals (Giulianini & Eskew, 1998). The results of this research support the six mechanism theory explaining postreceptoral color processing. The detection task was designed to allow us to create a threshold contour, which was then used to create a computer model for the visual mechanisms. As the results indicate, six mechanisms were sufficient to fit the data across all noise conditions and observers (Figure 11a-c). Withinsubject, the slopes of each mechanism line were constrained to be the same across noise conditions, maintaining consistency in terms of the described mechanisms (Figure 12a-c). We tested six, eight, and sixteen mechanisms, but the fit did not improve significantly after six mechanisms, supporting the theory that only six mechanisms are sufficient to account for the detection data in the LMmodulated color space used for these experiments. In some noise conditions, one or two mechanisms were not necessary to fit the data and were pushed out of the contour. This can be explained by the effects of the noise masking, which increases threshold levels in general and alters absolute mechanism sensitivity (leaving relative sensitivity to different LM angles unchanged, so the slopes of the mechanism threshold lines is always the same). Certain mechanisms require more stimulation than others in order to be activated these are the ones that get pushed out, while the other mechanisms compensate. Nevertheless, all six mechanisms were required to fit the data over all the noise conditions in aggregate. The distinct stretch of the 42 noise contour demonstrates the

18 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 18 presence of selective masking, which is accounted for by the six mechanism model. The high goodness of fit, with an R 2 of 98%, and the ability of the model to account for selective masking, supplies the initial evidence for the validity of the model. The color matching test was designed to further test the model and to allow us to associate a color with each of the six defined mechanisms. Subjective examination of the color matches when plotted with the mechanism lines over the contour suggests that each mechanism is associated with a corresponding color (Figure 13). A K-means Euclidean cluster analysis of the color matches, displayed in CIE (u,v) color space, revealed distinct groups (Figure 14a-c). Within each cluster, the individual angles should not be discriminable; the differences in color coordinates only represent variance, no different than the variance in coordinates of each individual angle s five test blocks. However, any two points that lie in different clusters should be discriminable. Interestingly, the red, orange, and yellow clusters were very close to the white point, indicating that they were much less saturated than the green, blue, and purple clusters. The number of clusters can be used to test the validity of the six mechanism model, because the number of clusters should correspond to the number of mechanisms that contribute to each noise condition. The no noise condition had six clusters, which corresponds to the number of mechanisms defined by the model (Figure 14a). The 42 noise condition had four clusters, which also corresponds to the number of contributing mechanisms in the model of that noise condition (Figure 14b). The 64 noise condition was more complicated; it had six clusters, but the associated model only allowed for five to contribute (Figure 14c). Interestingly, the sixth cluster is made up of only one test angle, the 64 stimulus. This matches the angle of the noise. Perhaps the stimulus presented as a more highly saturated region of the noise, prompting the subject to assume that the color of the test matched the color of the noise, allowing for a relatively accurate color

19 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 19 match despite the presence of adapting noise. This would explain the additional cluster, which actually only comprised of a single point. Each cluster is clearly associated with a specific color designation. Interestingly, there are points that shift in apparent hue when viewed under different noise conditions. For example, a 45 stimulus appears to be yellow, green, or orange when viewed under the no noise, 42 noise, and 64 noise conditions, respectively. This indicates that color is not an inherent property of the physical world, nor is it even directly associated with specific wavelengths of light. Rather, color is associated with the mechanism that is activated. In other words, each mechanism has an associated color. Activation of a mechanism leads to a perception of the corresponding color. If a specific mechanism is pushed out due to the presence of noise, then the other mechanisms compensate. Whichever mechanism detects a specific stimulus dictates the perceived color of that stimulus. This supports the labeled-line model of color vision, with each mechanism associated with a specific color. The third experiment was the method of adjustment discrimination task. This test was designed to allow us to quickly determine the boundaries of discrimination between colors (Figure 15a, b). Because the stimuli were presented at the threshold levels predicted by the detection model, they were difficult to see. At such a low level, discrimination would rely on differential activation of the mechanisms. If one stimulus activated the same mechanism as another stimulus, they would not be discriminable. The results of the test appear to agree with this assumption, providing further evidence in support of the six mechanism model. The no noise condition discrimination data supports the six mechanism theory. Conversely, at first look, there would appear to be only four boundaries (Figure 15a). However, the 0 standard appears to lie directly on the boundary between the red and orange mechanisms. Therefore, the

20 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 20 upper region actually encompasses the orange mechanism, while the lower region, like the regions of the 270 and 315 standards, encompasses the red mechanism. The 45 standard range denotes the boundaries of the yellow mechanism. The yellow and green boundaries almost directly meet, indicating the point of separation between the two mechanisms. The apparent boundary between the green and blue mechanism appears to be very noisy, as the 90, 135, and 180 upper boundaries do not quite end in the same location. There is also a sizeable gap between these green mechanisms and the purple mechanism, denoted by the region measured with the 225 standard. The model indicates that the blue mechanism is located in this region. Because it encompasses a short range of the contour, there was no standard tested within its boundaries, so its presence can be inferred from the gaps between the green and purple mechanisms. Overall, this data seems to corroborate with the six mechanism theory, but more data is needed. The method of adjustment discrimination data for the 64 noise condition also appears to support the six mechanism theory. The rough boundaries delineate five mechanisms (Figure 15b). The 315 and 0 standards lie in the orange mechanism. However, because 270 appears to be located in the corner of two mechanisms, the upper limit of the 270 standard also appears to be within the boundaries of the orange mechanism. The 45 standard is in the yellow mechanism. There is a slight gap between the yellow and blue mechanism boundaries. The 90, 135, and 180 standards are in the blue mechanism. The 225 standard appears to be in the corner of the blue and purple mechanisms, so the lower region is within the boundaries of the blue mechanism as well. The upper range appears to be within the purple mechanism. There is another gap, this one between the purple and red mechanisms. The lower range of the 270 standard appears to fall within the red mechanism. Overall, five rough mechanisms are outlined here, although this data is quite noisy. More follow-up is necessary. However, the 64 noise condition discrimination data appears to the

21 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 21 match the model prediction, lending further support to the six mechanism model. There are several potential reservations to this study. First, the model is based off of a highthreshold model of detection, which has been generally discarded in favor of signal detection theory (Wickens, 2002). Contrary to signal detection theory, the high-threshold model holds that errors are due to guessing, not due to the effect of noise (Burmester & Wallis, 2012). However, in the case of these experiments, the high-threshold model is a reasonable approximation. This is because we use the two-alternative forced-choice method for the collection of detection data. Another possible issue is that data from only one subject are demonstrated here. However, detection models and color matching data from two other subjects show similar results to the data presented here. Also, preliminary method of adjustment data from another subject also follow the same patterns exhibited here. Therefore, the data in this paper is representative. The compatibility between the discrimination data and the mechanism model lends further support to the six mechanism theory, which is based upon a labeled-line assumption. The labeledline theory derives from Müller s Law of Specific Nerve Energies, which is based upon the assumption that the strength of two stimuli that lie on the same mechanism can be adjusted such that the two stimuli are indiscriminable, and that any two stimuli that lie on difference mechanisms are as discriminable as they are detectable (Boring, 1942; Eskew, Newton, & Giulianini, 2001; Graham, 1989; Watson & Robson, 1981). In these experiments, discrimination only occurred when the stimuli were detected by different mechanisms, lending evidence to the labeled-line theory of color vision. Each mechanism is distinct from the others, and activation of one mechanism corresponds to the perception of a specific color. However, activation of the same mechanism with threshold-level stimuli, even if the stimuli are at different angles, leads to a perception that the two stimuli are the same color, making them indiscriminable. Thus, these two experiments both support

22 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 22 a labeled-line theory of color vision. The results of this study establish the validity of the six mechanism model calculated from detection data. The color matching experiment demonstrated that there are six different perceived colors when stimuli are presented at threshold. This confirmed the number of mechanisms and provided a label designation for each. The method of adjustment discrimination experiment demonstrated that stimuli that lie on different mechanisms are discriminable, but stimuli that lie on the same mechanism are indiscriminable. Also the boundaries delineated by the experiment correspond to the model predictions, giving further evidence for the existence of six mechanisms. Therefore, both the color matching and the method of adjustment discrimination experiments support the detection-derived six mechanism model explaining post-receptoral processing that contributes to human color vision. Moving forward, we will conduct more experiments to test the validity of the six mechanism model. For example, we will perform another discrimination test where subjects are presented with two different stimuli a standard and a test and will have to indicate the interval during which the standard was presented. This will be a two-alternative forced-choice test. This test will be performed within and across mechanism boundaries. Two stimuli that lie on the same mechanism should not be discriminable when presented at threshold levels, so the discrimination rate should be at chance level, or 50%. However, two stimuli that are detected by different mechanisms should be discriminated at the same rate as they are detected at threshold level, or 82%. Stimuli that lie near a boundary and are potentially detected by two mechanisms are discriminated at intermediate levels. Using this additional experiment, we will further demonstrate the validity of our six mechanism model. The results of this study support the six mechanism theory explaining intermediate

23 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 23 processing in human color vision. The model fit the detection data well and accurately accounted for selective masking. Furthermore, the model accurately predicted the results of two different experiments, a color matching task and a method of adjustment discrimination test. The color matching task confirmed the number of mechanisms and provided a color label for each. The method of adjustment test corroborated the number and boundaries of the proposed mechanisms. Overall, these results, in line with labeled-line theory, support a six mechanism model of color vision at the post-receptoral stage.

24 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 24 References Boring, E. G. (1942). Sensation and Perception in The History of Experimental Psychology. New York: Irvington. Boynton, R. K. (1979). Human Color Vision (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. Buchsbaum, G., & Gottschalk, A. (1983). Trichromacy, opponent colours coding and optimum colour information transmission in the retina. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 220(1218), Burmester, A., & Wallis, G. (2012). Contrasting predictions of low- and high-threshold models for the detection of changing visual features. Perception, 41(5), doi: /p7176 Dartnall, H. J., Bowmaker, J. K., & Mollon, J. D. (1983). Human Visual Pigments: Microspectrophotometric Results from the Eyes of Seven Persons. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 220(1218), doi: /rspb Derrington, A. M., Krauskopf, J., & Lennie, P. (1984). Chromatic Mechanisms in Lateral Geniculate Nucleus of Macaque. The Journal of Physiology, 357, doi: /jphysiol.1984.sp D'Kmura, M., Lennie, P., & Krauskopf, J. (1987). Hue selectivity revealed by heterochromatic noise masking. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 28, 92. D'Zmura, M. (1991). Color in visual research. Vision Research, 31(6), doi: / (91)90203-h Eskew, R. T. (2008). Chromatic Detection and Discrimination. In A. I. Basbaum, A. Kaneko, G. M. Shepherd, G. Westheimer, T. D. Albright, & R. Masland (Eds.), The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference (Vols. II, Vision II, pp ). San Diego: Academic Press.

25 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 25 Eskew, R. T. (2009). Higher order color mechanisms: A critical review. Vision Research, 49, doi: /j.visres Eskew, R. T., McLellan, J. S., & Giulianini, F. (1999). Chromatic detection and discrimination. In K. Gegenfurtner, & L. T. Sharpe (Eds.), Color vision: from genes to perception (pp ). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Eskew, R. T., Newton, J. R., & Giulianini, F. (2001). Chromatic detection and discrimination analyzed by a Bayesian classifier. Vision Research, 41(7), doi: /s (00) Farnsworth, D. (1943). The Farnsworth-Munsell 100-Hue and Dichotomous Tests for Color Vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 33(10), doi: /josa Gegenfurtner, K. R., & Kiper, D. C. (1992). Contrast detection in luminance and chromatic noise. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 9(11), doi: /josaa Gegenfurtner, K. R., Kiper, D. C., & Levitt, J. B. (1997). Functional properties of neurons in macaque area V3. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, Giulianini, F., & Eskew, R. T. (1998). Chromatic masking in the (ΔL/L, ΔM/M) plane of conecontrast space reveals only two detection mechanisms. Vision Research, 38(24), doi: /s (98) Goldstein, E. B. (2010). Sensation and Perception (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. Graham, N. V. (1989). Visual pattern analyzers. New York: Oxford University Press. Hansen, T., & Gegenfurtner, K. (2010). Uncovering multiple higher order chromatic mechanisms

26 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 26 in cone contrast space. Journal of Vision, 10(7), 388. doi: / Hurvich, L. M., & Jameson, D. (1957). An opponent-process theory of color vision. Psychological Review, 64(6), Kingdom, F. A., & Prins, N. (2016). Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction (2nd ed.). Waltham, MA: Academic Press. Kiper, D. C., Fenstemaker, S. B., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (1997). Chromatic properties of neurons in macaque area V2. Visual Neuroscience, 14, Krauskopf, J. (1999). Higher order color mechanisms. In K. R. Gegenfurtner, & I. T. Sharpe (Eds.), Color vision: From genes to perception (pp ). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Krauskopf, J., Williams, D. R., & Heeley, D. W. (1982). Cardinal Directions of Color Space. Vision Research, 22, doi: /82/ Krauskopf, J., Williams, D. R., Mandler, M. B., & Brown, A. M. (1986). Higher Order Color Mechanisms. Vision Research, 26(1), doi: Lennie, P., & D'Zmura, M. (1988). Mechanisms of color vision. Critical Reviews in Neurobiology, 3(4), Lennie, P., Krauskopf, J., & Sclar, G. (1990). Chromatic Mechanisms on Striate Cortex of Macaque. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10(2), MacLeod, D. I., & Boynton, R. M. (1979). Chromaticity diagram showing cone excitation by stimuli of equal luminance. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 69, Pauli, H. (1976). Proposed extension of the CIE recommendation on Uniform color spaces, color difference equations, and metric color terms. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 66(8), doi: /josa

27 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 27 Rubin, J. M., & Richards, W. A. (1988). Color Vision: Representing Material Categories. In W. A. Richards (Ed.), Natural Computation (pp ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Shepard, T., Eskew, R. T., McCarthy, C., & Ochandarena, N. (2015). Selective noise masking of L and M cone stimuli: unipolar tests reveal theoretically significant asymmetries. Journal of Vision, 15(12), 260. doi: / Stockman, A., & Brainard, D. H. (2010). Color Vision Mechanisms. In M. Bass, C. DeCusatis, J. Enoch, V. Lakshminarayanan, G. Li, C. Macdonald,... E. van Stryland (Eds.), The Optical Society of America Handbook of Optics (3rd ed., Vol. III: Vision and Optics, pp ). New York: McGraw Hill. Watson, A. B., & Robson, J. G. (1981). Discrimination at threshold: labelled detectors in human vision. Vision Research, 21, Webster, M. A., & Mollon, J. D. (1994). The influence of contrast adaptation on color appearance. Vision Research, 34(15), doi: / (94) Wickens, T. D. (2002). Elementary Signal Detection Theory. New York: Oxford University Press. Wolfe, J. M., Kluender, K. R., & Levi, D. M. (2011). Sensation and Perception (3rd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc. Young, T. (1802). The Bakerian Lecture: On the Theory of Light and Colours. Philosophical Transactions: The Royal Society London, 92, doi: /rstl

28 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 28 Appendix A Figures Figure 1. Absorption spectra of the three cone photoreceptors (Dartnall, Bowmaker, & Mollon, 1983; Goldstein, 2010). Figure 2. Helmholtz s presentation of two different colors. The top portion is composed of a single wavelength, while the bottom portion is a metamer, or mixture of wavelengths (Young, 1802).

29 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 29 Figure 3. The interactions between the cone signals in color opponent theory. Three channels are created: a blue-yellow channel created from the interaction between S, L, and M cone signals; a red-green channel formed from the difference between the L and M cone signals, and a luminance channel based on the sum of the L and M cone signals (Stockman & Brainard, 2010). Figure 4. Spectral sensitivities. Stage 1 represents the three cone receptors, while Stage 2 demonstrates the interactions between the Stage 1 signals (Stockman & Brainard, 2010).

30 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 30 Figure 5. Left: the lateral geniculate nucleus. Right: color-sensitive cells in the LGN respond preferentially to stimuli in the red-green and blueyellow color directions (Derrington, Krauskopf, & Lennie, 1984). Figure 6. Cardinal axis theory of color vision. Modified from Eskew (2009).

31 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 31 Figure 7. LM color plane. A B C Figure 8. (A) Stimulus presentation. (B) Top: Sample stimuli at suprathreshold levels. Bottom: Sample stimuli at pseudo-threshold levels. (C) Sample noise masking pattern.

32 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 32 M L M L Figure 9. Color discrimination task. Black circle: standard. Black crosses: indiscriminable test cues. Gray crosses: discriminable test cues. Red circles: measured detection thresholds. Colored lines: modelled linear mechanisms. Top: test angles adjusted in the counterclockwise direction. Bottom: test angles adjusted in the clockwise direction. Figure 10. HSV color wheel.

33 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 33 A B SIL C SIL Figure 11. Left column: Detection data with contour in cone contrast space. Middle column: Threshold versus color angle plot for detection data. Right column: Regression line (y-axis: predicted threshold, x-axis: observed threshold). (A) No noise condition. (B) 42 noise condition. (C) 64 noise condition. SIL

34 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 34 A A B SIL B C SIL C Figure 12. Left column: Detection data with contour and mechanisms in cone contrast space. Right column: MBDKL space. (A) No noise condition. (B) 42 noise condition. (C) 64 noise condition. SIL

35 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 35 Figure 13. Color matches on the no noise contour. Mechanism lines are colored according to the label given by the observer Circles: color matches superimposed onto detection threshold points. Left: cone contrast space. Right: MBDKL space. SIL

36 COLOR DISCRIMINATION AT THRESHOLD 36 A v WP SIL u B v WP SIL u C v WP SIL Figure 14. Color matching points in CIE (u,v) color space. Each colored point represents a stimulus angle. The white point represents the white point of the monitor. (A) No noise condition. (B) 42 noise condition. (C) 64 noise condition. u

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Javier R. Movellan Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0515 movellan@inc.ucsd.edu Thomas

More information

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function VISION Vol. 20, No. 2, 67 71, 2008 Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function Keizo SHINOMORI Department of Information Systems Engineering, Kochi University of Technology

More information

Seeing Color. Muller (1896) The Psychophysical Axioms. Brindley (1960) Psychophysical Linking Hypotheses

Seeing Color. Muller (1896) The Psychophysical Axioms. Brindley (1960) Psychophysical Linking Hypotheses Muller (1896) The Psychophysical Axioms The ground of every state of consciousness is a material process, a psychophysical process so-called, to whose occurrence the state of consciousness is joined To

More information

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I Monocular Sensory Processes of Vision: Color Vision Mechanisms of Color Processing . Neural Mechanisms of Color Processing A. Parallel processing - M- & P- pathways B. Second

More information

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited by law: any public performance or display,

More information

Human colour perception and its adaptation

Human colour perception and its adaptation Network: Computation in Neural Systems 7 (1996) 587 634. Printed in the UK Human colour perception and its adaptation Michael A Webster Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, NV 89557-0062,

More information

Color scaling of discs and natural objects at different luminance levels

Color scaling of discs and natural objects at different luminance levels Visual Neuroscience ~2006!, 23, 603 610. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2006 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238006 $16.00 DOI: 10.10170S0952523806233121 Color scaling of discs and natural objects at different

More information

The color of night: Surface color categorization by color defective observers under dim illuminations

The color of night: Surface color categorization by color defective observers under dim illuminations Visual Neuroscience ~2008!, 25, 475 480. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2008 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238008 $25.00 doi:10.10170s0952523808080486 The color of night: Surface color categorization

More information

Image Processing in the Human Visual System, a Quick Overview

Image Processing in the Human Visual System, a Quick Overview Image Processing in the Human Visual System, a Quick Overview By Orazio Gallo, April 24th, 2008 The Visual System Our most advanced perception system: The optic nerve has 106 fibers, more than all the

More information

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!)

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!) Introduction to Physiological Psychology Vision ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ /~ksweeney/psy260.html Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy (the part that s visible to us!)

More information

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 49 (2009) 2686 2704 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Higher order color mechanisms: A critical review Rhea T. Eskew

More information

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1 Announcements exam 1 this Thursday! review session: Wednesday, 5:00-6:30pm, Meliora 203 Bryce s office hours: Wednesday, 3:30-5:30pm, Gleason https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zdw7pvgz0um M Cells M cells

More information

SENSES: VISION. Chapter 5: Sensation AP Psychology Fall 2014

SENSES: VISION. Chapter 5: Sensation AP Psychology Fall 2014 SENSES: VISION Chapter 5: Sensation AP Psychology Fall 2014 Sensation versus Perception Top-Down Processing (Perception) Cerebral cortex/ Association Areas Expectations Experiences Memories Schemas Anticipation

More information

Higher order color mechanisms: Evidence from noise-masking experiments in cone contrast space

Higher order color mechanisms: Evidence from noise-masking experiments in cone contrast space Journal of Vision (2013) 13(1):26, 1 21 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/1/26 1 Higher order color mechanisms: Evidence from noise-masking experiments in cone contrast space Thorsten Hansen Karl

More information

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision Biological Bases of Behavior 6: Vision Sensory Systems The brain detects events in the external environment and directs the contractions of the muscles Afferent neurons carry sensory messages to brain

More information

Interactions between chromatic adaptation and contrast adaptation in color appearance

Interactions between chromatic adaptation and contrast adaptation in color appearance Vision Research 40 (2000) 3801 3816 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Interactions between chromatic adaptation and contrast adaptation in color appearance Michael A. Webster a, *, J. Anthony Wilson b a Department

More information

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Fundamentals of Psychophysics Fundamentals of Psychophysics John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3045! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Visual neuroscience physiology stimulus How do we see the world? neuroimaging

More information

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the Some transformations of color information from lateral geniculate nucleus to striate cortex Russell L. De Valois*, Nicolas P. Cottaris, Sylvia D. Elfar*, Luke E. Mahon, and J. Anthony Wilson* *Psychology

More information

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye: Problems confronting the visual system: Visual Physiology image contains a huge amount of information which must be processed quickly. image is dim, blurry and distorted. Light levels vary enormously.

More information

LEA Color Vision Testing

LEA Color Vision Testing To The Tester Quantitative measurement of color vision is an important diagnostic test used to define the degree of hereditary color vision defects found in screening with pseudoisochromatic tests and

More information

Mr. Silimperi Council Rock High School South Chapter 5 Sensation Sensation II

Mr. Silimperi Council Rock High School South Chapter 5 Sensation Sensation II Mr. Silimperi Council Rock High School South AP Psychology Name: Date: Chapter 5 Sensation Sensation II Psychophysics study of the relationship between physical characteristics of stimuli and our psychological

More information

The Perceptual Experience

The Perceptual Experience Dikran J. Martin Introduction to Psychology Name: Date: Lecture Series: Chapter 5 Sensation and Perception Pages: 35 TEXT: Lefton, Lester A. and Brannon, Linda (2003). PSYCHOLOGY. (Eighth Edition.) Needham

More information

Vision Research. Very-long-term and short-term chromatic adaptation: Are their influences cumulative? Suzanne C. Belmore a, Steven K.

Vision Research. Very-long-term and short-term chromatic adaptation: Are their influences cumulative? Suzanne C. Belmore a, Steven K. Vision Research 51 (2011) 362 366 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Very-long-term and short-term chromatic adaptation: Are their

More information

Vision Seeing is in the mind

Vision Seeing is in the mind 1 Vision Seeing is in the mind Stimulus: Light 2 Light Characteristics 1. Wavelength (hue) 2. Intensity (brightness) 3. Saturation (purity) 3 4 Hue (color): dimension of color determined by wavelength

More information

Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color tuning properties

Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color tuning properties Visual Neuroscience (2004), 21, 275 282. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2004 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238004 $16.00 DOI: 10.10170S0952523804213426 Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color

More information

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2 Name: ID # ID: A Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2 Multiple Choice (38 questions, 1 point each) Identify the letter of the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

More information

Postreceptoral chromatic-adaptation mechanisms in the red green and blue yellow systems using simple reaction times

Postreceptoral chromatic-adaptation mechanisms in the red green and blue yellow systems using simple reaction times J. M. Medina and J. A. Díaz Vol. 23, No. 5/May 2006/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 993 Postreceptoral chromatic-adaptation mechanisms in the red green and blue yellow systems using simple reaction times José M. Medina

More information

What do we perceive?

What do we perceive? THE VISUAL SYSTEM Aditi Majumder What do we perceive? Example: Switch off the light in room What we perceive Not only the property of the scene But also that of the visual system Our perception is filtered

More information

A MULTI-STAGE COLOR MODEL REVISITED: IMPLICATIONS FOR A GENE THERAPY CURE FOR RED-GREEN COLORBLINDNESS

A MULTI-STAGE COLOR MODEL REVISITED: IMPLICATIONS FOR A GENE THERAPY CURE FOR RED-GREEN COLORBLINDNESS Abstract for Chapter A MULTI-STAGE COLOR MODEL REVISITED: IMPLICATIONS FOR A GENE THERAPY CURE FOR RED-GREEN COLORBLINDNESS Katherine Mancuso 1, Matthew C. Mauck 2, James A. Kuchenbecker 1, Maureen Neitz

More information

Adaptation and perceptual norms

Adaptation and perceptual norms Adaptation and perceptual norms Michael A. Webster*, Maiko Yasuda, Sara Haber, Deanne Leonard, Nicole Ballardini Department of Psychology / 296, University of Nevada-Reno, Reno NV 89557, USA ABSTRACT We

More information

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 05 Why do you need bright light to read? Rods and cones Photoreceptors are not evenly distributed across the retina 1 Rods and cones Cones are most dense in

More information

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity S. B. Stevenson *, L. K. Cormack Vision Research 40, 2881-2884. (2000) * University of Houston College of Optometry, Houston TX 77204

More information

= add definition here. Definition Slide

= add definition here. Definition Slide = add definition here Definition Slide Definition Slides Sensation = the process by which our sensory receptors and nervous system receive and represent stimulus energies from our environment. Perception

More information

Transitions between color categories mapped with a reverse Stroop task

Transitions between color categories mapped with a reverse Stroop task Visual Neuroscience ~2006!, 23, 453 460. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2006 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238006 $16.00 DOI: 10.10170S0952523806233388 Transitions between color categories mapped with

More information

Color Science and Spectrum Inversion: A Reply to Nida-Rümelin

Color Science and Spectrum Inversion: A Reply to Nida-Rümelin Consciousness and Cognition 8, 566 570 (1999) Article ID ccog.1999.0418, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Color Science and Spectrum Inversion: A Reply to Nida-Rümelin Peter W. Ross Department

More information

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus Perceived color of light is determined by 3 characteristics (properties of electromagnetic energy): 1. Hue: the spectrum (wavelength) of light (color)

More information

Surface Color Perception under Different Illuminants and Surface Collections

Surface Color Perception under Different Illuminants and Surface Collections Surface Color Perception under Different Illuminants and Surface Collections Inaugural-Dissertation zur Erlangung der Doktorwürde der Philosophischen Fakultät II (Psychologie, Pädagogik und Sportwissenschaft)

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect. Read each question carefully and answer it completely. Multiple-choice questions are worth one point each, other questions

More information

Response latencies to chromatic. and achromatic visual stimuli

Response latencies to chromatic. and achromatic visual stimuli Response latencies to chromatic and achromatic visual stimuli Adam Kane School of Psychology University of Adelaide 2014 Thesis submitted for the degree of Doctorate of Philosophy 1 Acknowledgements I

More information

Spectrograms (revisited)

Spectrograms (revisited) Spectrograms (revisited) We begin the lecture by reviewing the units of spectrograms, which I had only glossed over when I covered spectrograms at the end of lecture 19. We then relate the blocks of a

More information

Introduction to Physiological Psychology

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Introduction to Physiological Psychology Vision ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ksweeney/psy260.html This class n Sensation vs. Perception n How light is translated into what we see n Structure

More information

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013 Definition Slides Sensation = the process by which our sensory receptors and nervous system receive and represent stimulus energies from our environment. Perception = the process of organizing and interpreting

More information

Chromatic discrimination: differential contributions from two adapting fields

Chromatic discrimination: differential contributions from two adapting fields D. Cao and Y. H. Lu Vol. 29, No. 2 / February 2012 / J. Opt. Soc. Am. A A1 Chromatic discrimination: differential contributions from two adapting fields Dingcai Cao* and Yolanda H. Lu Department of Ophthalmology

More information

2/3/17. Visual System I. I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex

2/3/17. Visual System I. I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex 1 Visual System I I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex 2 1 2/3/17 Window of the Soul 3 Information Flow: From Photoreceptors

More information

Psychophysical Chromatic Mechanisms in Macaque Monkey

Psychophysical Chromatic Mechanisms in Macaque Monkey 15216 The Journal of Neuroscience, October 24, 2012 32(43):15216 15226 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Psychophysical Chromatic Mechanisms in Macaque Monkey Cleo M. Stoughton, 1 Rosa Lafer-Sousa, 1 Galina

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action Categorization and Discrimination Animals respond to stimuli in ways that suggest they form categories. Pigeons

More information

Sensory Systems Vision, Audition, Somatosensation, Gustation, & Olfaction

Sensory Systems Vision, Audition, Somatosensation, Gustation, & Olfaction Sensory Systems Vision, Audition, Somatosensation, Gustation, & Olfaction Sarah L. Chollar University of California, Riverside sarah.chollar@gmail.com Sensory Systems How the brain allows us to see, hear,

More information

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3 eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3 retinal specialization fovea: highest density of photoreceptors, aimed at where you are looking -> highest acuity optic disk: cell-free area, where

More information

Construction of the Visual Image

Construction of the Visual Image Construction of the Visual Image Anne L. van de Ven 8 Sept 2003 BioE 492/592 Sensory Neuroengineering Lecture 3 Visual Perception Light Photoreceptors Interneurons Visual Processing Ganglion Neurons Optic

More information

COMP 3020: Human-Computer Interaction I

COMP 3020: Human-Computer Interaction I reddit.com 1 2 COMP 3020: Human-Computer Interaction I Fall 2017 Prototype Lifetime James Young, with acknowledgements to Anthony Tang, Andrea Bunt, Pourang Irani, Julie Kientz, Saul Greenberg, Ehud Sharlin,

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect. absolute threshold. Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information.

The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect. absolute threshold. Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information. absolute threshold The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect accommodation Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information. acuity Sharp perception or vision audition

More information

Senses are transducers. Change one form of energy into another Light, sound, pressure, etc. into What?

Senses are transducers. Change one form of energy into another Light, sound, pressure, etc. into What? 1 Vision 2 TRANSDUCTION Senses are transducers Change one form of energy into another Light, sound, pressure, etc. into What? Action potentials! Sensory codes Frequency code encodes information about intensity

More information

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AD Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AD Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology Biological Psychology Unit Two AD Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology Sensory and Perception Though each sense works a little differently to do this, psychologists have developed principles to describe

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* Perception & Psychophysics 1973. Vol. 13. So. 2.356-360 Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* ANGEL VASSILEV+ Institu te of Physiology. Bulgarian Academy of Sciences.

More information

Stable Receptive Field Structure of Color Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex under Adapting and Non-adapting Conditions

Stable Receptive Field Structure of Color Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex under Adapting and Non-adapting Conditions Stable Receptive Field Structure of Color Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex under Adapting and Non-adapting Conditions Bevil R. Conway, Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston MA 02115.

More information

COLOUR CONSTANCY: A SIMULATION BY ARTIFICIAL NEURAL NETS

COLOUR CONSTANCY: A SIMULATION BY ARTIFICIAL NEURAL NETS OLOUR ONSTANY: A SIMULATION BY ARTIFIIAL NEURAL NETS enrikas Vaitkevicius and Rytis Stanikunas Faculty of Psychology, Vilnius University, Didlaukio 47, 257 Vilnius, Lithuania e-mail: henrikas.vaitkevicius@ff.vu.lt

More information

7. Sharp perception or vision 8. The process of transferring genetic material from one cell to another by a plasmid or bacteriophage

7. Sharp perception or vision 8. The process of transferring genetic material from one cell to another by a plasmid or bacteriophage 1. A particular shade of a given color 2. How many wave peaks pass a certain point per given time 3. Process in which the sense organs' receptor cells are stimulated and relay initial information to higher

More information

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus Perceived color of light is determined by 3 characteristics (properties of electromagnetic energy): 1. : the spectrum (wavelength) of light (color) 2.

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Vision. The Eye External View. The Eye in Cross-Section

Vision. The Eye External View. The Eye in Cross-Section Vision The Eye External View cornea pupil iris The Eye in Cross-Section Light enters via cornea, Is focused by cornea and lens, Forming image on retina, Which contains photoreceptors. 1 The Retina Photoreceptors

More information

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception 1 Virginia R. de Sa Department of Cognitive Science UCSD Lecture 7: Color (Chapter 6) Course Information 2 Class web page: http://cogsci.ucsd.edu/ desa/101a/index.html

More information

Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity

Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity J. A. Richardson and I. Davies Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford GU2 5XH, Surrey, United

More information

Processing in the probed-sinewave paradigm is likely retinal

Processing in the probed-sinewave paradigm is likely retinal Visual Neuroscience (2001), 18, 1003 1010. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2001 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238001 $12.50 DOI: 10.1017.S0952523801186177 Processing in the probed-sinewave paradigm is

More information

Variations in normal color vision. V. Simulations of adaptation to natural color environments

Variations in normal color vision. V. Simulations of adaptation to natural color environments Visual Neuroscience (29), 26, 1 of 13. Printed in the USA. Copyright Ó 29 Cambridge University Press 952-5238/9 $25. doi:1.117/s952523888942 Variations in normal color vision. V. Simulations of adaptation

More information

Neuroscience - Problem Drill 13: The Eye and Visual Processing

Neuroscience - Problem Drill 13: The Eye and Visual Processing Neuroscience - Problem Drill 13: The Eye and Visual Processing Question No. 1 of 10 needed, (3) Pick the answer, and (4) Review the core concept tutorial as needed. 1. Which of the following statements

More information

Lesson 5 Sensation, Perception, Memory, and The Conscious Mind

Lesson 5 Sensation, Perception, Memory, and The Conscious Mind Lesson 5 Sensation, Perception, Memory, and The Conscious Mind Introduction: Connecting Your Learning The beginning of Bloom's lecture concludes his discussion of language development in humans and non-humans

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination Timothy N. Rubin (trubin@uci.edu) Michael D. Lee (mdlee@uci.edu) Charles F. Chubb (cchubb@uci.edu) Department of Cognitive

More information

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features Dikran J. Martin Psychology 110 Name: Date: Lecture Series: Chapter 3 Sensation and Perception: Pages: 31 Making Contact with the World around Us TEXT: Baron, Robert A. (2001). Psychology (Fifth Edition).

More information

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System Lecturer: Jitendra Malik Scribe: Ryan White (Slide: layout of the brain) Facts about the brain:

More information

Noise masking of S-cone increments and decrements

Noise masking of S-cone increments and decrements Journal of Vision (2014) 14(13):8, 1 17 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/14/13/8 1 Noise masking of S-cone increments and decrements Quanhong Wang* Psychology Department, Northeastern University,

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Sensation and Perception. A. Sensation: awareness of simple characteristics B. Perception: making complex interpretations

Sensation and Perception. A. Sensation: awareness of simple characteristics B. Perception: making complex interpretations I. Overview Sensation and Perception A. Sensation: awareness of simple characteristics B. Perception: making complex interpretations C. Top-Down vs Bottom-up Processing D. Psychophysics -- thresholds 1.

More information

The Role of Color and Attention in Fast Natural Scene Recognition

The Role of Color and Attention in Fast Natural Scene Recognition Color and Fast Scene Recognition 1 The Role of Color and Attention in Fast Natural Scene Recognition Angela Chapman Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems Boston University 677 Beacon St. Boston, MA

More information

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) What happens beyond the retina? What happens in Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)- 90% flow Visual cortex Information Flow Superior colliculus 10% flow Slide 2 Information

More information

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired

More information

Opponent theory PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 18. Trichromatic theory

Opponent theory PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 18. Trichromatic theory PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 18 Reach that last 1%. Trichromatic theory Different colors are represented as a pattern across the three basic colors Nicely predicted the existence of the three cone types

More information

Effect of Colorimetric Attributes on Perceived Blackness of Materials

Effect of Colorimetric Attributes on Perceived Blackness of Materials Effect of Colorimetric Attributes on Perceived Blackness of Materials Reid Clonts, Renzo Shamey, David Hinks, Polymer & Color Chemistry Program, TECS Department, North Carolina State University, Raleigh,

More information

Wavelength discrimination at detection threshold

Wavelength discrimination at detection threshold K. T. Mullen and J. J. Kulikowski Vol. 7, No. 4/April 199/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 733 Wavelength discrimination at detection threshold K. T. Mullen Physiological Laboratory, Downing Street, Cambridge CB3 3EG,

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 Supporting Information Variances and biases of absolute distributions were larger in the 2-line

More information

Scotopic hue percepts in natural scenes

Scotopic hue percepts in natural scenes Journal of Vision (2013) 13(13):15, 1 13 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/13/15 1 Scotopic hue percepts in natural scenes Sarah L. Elliott Department of Psychology, Roosevelt University, IL, USA

More information

OPTO Physiology Of Vision II

OPTO Physiology Of Vision II Lecture 8 Relative Luminous Efficiency The sensitivity of the eye to different wavelengths in an equal energy spectrum is known as the Relative Luminous Efficiency (V λ ) function. At photopic levels of

More information

Contrast discrimination with sinusoidal gratings of different spatial frequency

Contrast discrimination with sinusoidal gratings of different spatial frequency Bird et al. Vol. 19, No. 7/July 2002/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1267 Contrast discrimination with sinusoidal gratings of different spatial frequency C. M. Bird Department of Neuropsychology, The National Hospital,

More information

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1996, 3 (2), 222 226 Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli ROBERT M. NOSOFSKY Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana and THOMAS J. PALMERI Vanderbilt University,

More information

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Current Biology, in press Supplemental Information: Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Grigorios Sotiropoulos, Aaron R. Seitz, and Peggy Seriès Supplemental Data Detailed

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Visual discrimination and adaptation using non-linear unsupervised learning

Visual discrimination and adaptation using non-linear unsupervised learning Visual discrimination and adaptation using non-linear unsupervised learning Sandra Jiménez, Valero Laparra and Jesús Malo Image Processing Lab, Universitat de València, Spain; http://isp.uv.es ABSTRACT

More information

Outline 2/19/2013. Please see me after class: Sarah Pagliero Ryan Paul Demetrius Prowell-Reed Ashley Rehm Giovanni Reynel Patricia Rochin

Outline 2/19/2013. Please see me after class: Sarah Pagliero Ryan Paul Demetrius Prowell-Reed Ashley Rehm Giovanni Reynel Patricia Rochin Outline 2/19/2013 PSYC 120 General Psychology Spring 2013 Lecture 8: Sensation and Perception 1 Dr. Bart Moore bamoore@napavalley.edu Office hours Tuesdays 11:00-1:00 How we sense and perceive the world

More information

The Color Between Two Others

The Color Between Two Others The Color Between Two Others Ethan D. Montag Munsell Color Science Laboratory, Chester F. Carlson Center for Imaging Science Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, New York Abstract A psychophysical

More information

Diagram the perceptual principles of hue, saturation, and brightness and the role they play in our perception of color.

Diagram the perceptual principles of hue, saturation, and brightness and the role they play in our perception of color. Color Perception 6 LEARNING OBJECTIVES 6.1 Examine the relation of the wavelength of light to perceived color. 6.2 6.3 Diagram the perceptual principles of hue, saturation, and brightness and the role

More information

1.4 MECHANISMS OF COLOR VISION. Trichhromatic Theory. Hering s Opponent-Colors Theory

1.4 MECHANISMS OF COLOR VISION. Trichhromatic Theory. Hering s Opponent-Colors Theory 17 exceedingly difficult to explain the function of single cortical cells in simple terms. In fact, the function of a single cell might not have meaning since the representation of various perceptions

More information

Chapter 5 Test Review. Try the practice questions in the Study Guide and on line

Chapter 5 Test Review. Try the practice questions in the Study Guide and on line Chapter 5 Test Review Try the practice questions in the Study Guide and on line Printing game plan Put six slides on a page Select pure black and white as the printing option Okay, now wade into the answers>>>>

More information

The cone inputs to the unique-hue mechanisms

The cone inputs to the unique-hue mechanisms Vision Research 45 (2005) 3210 3223 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres The cone inputs to the unique-hue mechanisms Sophie M. Wuerger a, *, Philip Atkinson b, Simon Cropper c a Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience,

More information

The Perception of Static Colored Noise: Detection and Masking Described by CIE94

The Perception of Static Colored Noise: Detection and Masking Described by CIE94 The Perception of Static Colored Noise: Detection and Masking Described by CIE94 Marcel P. Lucassen, 1 * Piet Bijl, 1 Jolanda Roelofsen 2 1 TNO Human Factors, Soesterberg, The Netherlands 2 Cognitive Psychology,

More information

Main Study: Summer Methods. Design

Main Study: Summer Methods. Design Main Study: Summer 2000 Methods Design The experimental design is within-subject each participant experiences five different trials for each of the ten levels of Display Condition and for each of the three

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect, so keep your eyes foveated on your own exam. I tend to think of a point as a minute, so be sure to spend the appropriate amount

More information

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES Varinthira Duangudom and David V Anderson School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Georgia Institute of Technology Atlanta, GA 30332

More information