UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Cognitive change in Parkinson's disease Broeders, M. Link to publication

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Cognitive change in Parkinson's disease Broeders, M. Link to publication"

Transcription

1 UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Cognitive change in Parkinson's disease Broeders, M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Broeders, M. (2015). Cognitive change in Parkinson's disease General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam ( Download date: 15 Nov 2018

2 CHAPTER 2 Cognitive change in newlydiagnosed patients with Parkinson s disease: A 5-year follow-up study Mark Broeders, Daan C. Velseboer, Rob de Bie, Johannes D. Speelman, Dino Muslimovic, Bart Post, Rob de Haan, Ben Schmand Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2013). Vol. 19, pp

3 Chapter 2 ABSTRACT Cognitive change is frequently observed in patients with Parkinson s disease (PD). However, the exact profile and extent of cognitive impairments remain unclear due to the clinical heterogeneity of PD and methodological issues in many previous studies. In this study, we aimed to examine the severity, frequency, and profile of cognitive changes in newly diagnosed PD patients over 5 years. At baseline and after 3 and 5 years, a hospital-based sample of PD patients (n=59) and healthy controls (n=40) were given neuropsychological tests covering six cognitive domains. Patients showed greater decline over time than healthy controls on all cognitive domains, except for attention. The profile of decline showed that psychomotor speed and memory were most affected. At the individual level 53% of the patients showed more cognitive decline than controls. Age at onset and memory impairment at baseline predicted cognitive decline. Cognitive functions in PD patients show greater decline in most domains than in healthy elderly over the course of 5 years. Due to selection bias as a result of attrition, the actual degree of decline may be greater than reported here. 20

4 Cognitive change in PD: 5-year follow-up INTRODUCTION Cognitive alterations are common in Parkinson s disease (PD; Litvan et al., 2011; Troster, 2011). Even in the earliest stage, approximately 24 to 36% of PD patients show cognitive impairments (Foltynie, Brayne, Robbins, & Barker, 2004; Muslimovic, Post, Speelman, & Schmand, 2005), while 3 to 8 years after onset, this proportion is 50% to 80% (Aarsland, Bronnick, Larsen, Tysnes, & Alves, 2009; Muslimovic, Post, Speelman, De Haan, & Schmand, 2009; Williams-Gray, Foltynie, Brayne, Robbins, & Barker, 2007). For an overview of longitudinal studies of cognitive changes in PD, see (Muslimovic, Schmand, Speelman, & de Haan, 2007; Williams-Gray et al., 2007). Cognitive changes affect multiple domains (Dubois & Pillon, 1997; Taylor & Saint-Cyr, 1995), but executive dysfunction appears to be most profoundly impaired (Zgaljardic, Borod, Foldi, & Mattis, 2003). It is assumed to result from the primary pathology in PD, that is, dopamine depletion, which disrupts fronto-striatal functioning (Alexander, DeLong, & Strick, 1986; Owen, 2004). Executive deficits in PD typically resemble symptoms seen in patients with frontal lobe damage, that is, problems with working memory (Lewis, Slabosz, Robbins, Barker, & Owen, 2005), deficits in planning, initiating, and monitoring (Morris et al., 1988), impaired inhibition (Gauggel, Rieger, & Feghoff, 2004), and difficulties with set shifting (Cools, Barker, Sahakian, & Robbins, 2001b; Monchi et al., 2004). Patients are impaired on various executive functioning tasks, such as phonemic and semantic fluency, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Stroop interference test, and Trail Making Test part B (for an overview, see Kudlicka, Clare, & Hindle, 2011). Despite a plethora of studies on cognitive impairments in PD, it is still unclear to what extent cognitive functions are affected. While most studies reported impaired executive performance (Zgaljardic et al., 2003), some did not find executive deficits (e.g., Fournet, Moreaud, Roulin, Naegele, & Pellat, 1996). Part of this discrepancy might be explained by dopaminergic medication, which improves some aspects of executive functioning, such as set shifting, but impairs others, such as reversal learning (Cools, Barker, Sahakian, & Robbins, 2001a). Second, although there is ample cross-sectional evidence for executive impairment in PD, and deterioration of executive functions over time has been shown beyond doubt (Kudlicka et al., 2011), executive decline appears to be less prominent than memory decline (Muslimovic et al., 2007). Memory impairments are frequently reported in non- demented PD patients (Stebbins, Gabrieli, Masciari, Monti, & Goetz, 1999). Because this often concerns difficulties with the retrieval of information, memory deficits are seen as secondary to the executive impairments (Dubois & Pillon, 1997; Dujardin, Defebvre, Grunberg, Becquet, & Destee, 2001). However, subtle deficits in recognition memory also exist in PD (Whittington, Podd, & Kan, 2000), which cannot be fully explained by executive dysfunction (Bronnick, Alves, Aarsland, Tysnes, & Larsen, 2011). Thus, memory 21

5 Chapter 2 impairments, in particular impaired delayed recall and recognition, might be more closely related to hippocampal atrophy (Beyer et al., 2012). PD patients show impairments in other domains as well, such as visuospatial functions (Kerai, Bracewell, Hindle, & Leek, 2012) and psychomotor speed (Muslimovic et al., 2009). Notwithstanding the growing number of studies on cognition in PD, the course of decline in newly diagnosed patients still remains unclear. First, most studies were cross-sectional, conducted in convenience samples of prevalent cases. Second, the relatively scarce longitudinal studies have been hindered by methodological issues, such as the lack of control groups or the use of limited neuropsychological test batteries (Muslimovic et al., 2007). Few studies have followed a cohort of newly diagnosed patients over a substantially long period of time (Aarsland, Andersen, Larsen, Lolk, & Kragh-Sorensen, 2003; Williams-Gray et al., 2009). Another problem, which makes it difficult to describe the course of PD, is the heterogeneous nature of the disease (Taylor & Saint-Cyr, 1995). The characteristic pathology is dopamine depletion in the midbrain, but other pathological factors may appear, such as the accumulation of Lewy bodies in the cortex (Braak et al., 2003), and changes in other neurotransmitter systems, such as the cholinergic system (Jellinger, 2001; Jellinger, 2012). To gain better insight into the cognitive changes of PD, we started a study that focused on the course of cognitive decline in a large group of newly diagnosed patients and a control group (Muslimovic et al., 2005; Muslimovic et al., 2009). Both groups were presented with a range of neuropsychological tests at baseline and 3 years later. At baseline, 24% of the patients showed cognitive impairments, most prominently in the executive and memory domains. After 3 years, the percentage with impairments had doubled. The deficits were most prominent in attention and psychomotor speed. The present study further explores cognitive decline with a 5-year follow-up of this sample. Our aims were to (1) determine the pattern and severity of cognitive impairments, (2) describe the frequency of cognitive decline at an individual level, and (3) find prognostic markers for cognitive decline in the individual patient. We also explored the effects of selective attrition. 22

6 Cognitive change in PD: 5-year follow-up METHODS Subjects The current study is part of the larger CARPA project (Comorbidity and Aging in Rehabilitation Patients with sequelae of poliomyelitis, osteoarthritis, and PD: the influence on Activities). Patients were recruited from neurology outpatient clinics. Between 2002 and 2005, 145 newly diagnosed PD patients were referred for inclusion in the study. The inclusion criterion was a diagnosis of idiopathic PD, established at baseline by movement disorder specialists (J.D. or B.P.) according to standard criteria (Gelb, Oliver, & Gilman, 1999), and checked twice during the study (by J.D. or B.P.). Patients were excluded if they were older than 85 years, had a score,24 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975), insufficient knowledge of the Dutch language, or suffered from a somatic illness with a life-expectancy of less than a year. None of the patients had a history of stroke, suffered from psychiatric illness, or any other neurological comorbidity. Eight patients were excluded because their referral diagnosis was incorrect, that is, did not satisfy the standard PD criteria. Two patients did not speak Dutch, one patient appeared to have been diagnosed with PD 2 years earlier, and one patient withdrew consent. Thus, 133 patients were included in the CARPA project. The majority of patients were born in Europe, that is, the Netherlands (N=123), Germany (N=1), Austria (N=1), and Portugal (N=1). Three patients were born in Indonesia, two patients were born in Suriname, one patient was born in Aruba, and one patient was originally from Turkey. Throughout the study, the diagnosis changed in four patients (Lewy body dementia, multiple system atrophy, dystonic tremor, progressive supranuclear palsy). At baseline, 123 patients underwent neuropsychological assessment. In the present 5-year follow-up, 59 (48%) patients participated. See Figure 1 for the reasons of loss-to-followup. Healthy controls were spouses, relatives, or friends of the patients. Controls were excluded if they had impairments in hearing or visual acuity, a history of major psychiatric disorder, head injury with loss of consciousness in excess of 1 hr., cerebrovascular or other central nervous system illness, drug or alcohol abuse, or if they were taking psychoactive medication. At baseline, 70 healthy control subjects participated in the neuropsychological study; 5 years later, 40 (57%) controls agreed to participate (Figure 1). The institutional review boards of the participating hospitals approved the study, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants. All patients used dopaminergic medication. At 5-year follow-up, 13 patients (22%) were on levodopa or dopamine agonist alone, while the remaining 46 patients (78%) were on combination therapies. We converted all medication to a levodopa equivalent dose 23

7 Chapter 2 Figure 1. Overview of patients (left) and controls (right) who participated in the present study and those who were lost to neuropsychological follow-up. (LED; (Esselink et al., 2004). Six patients (10%) used benzodiazepines, two (4%) antidepressants, and one (2%) antipsychotics (quetiapine). Procedures Approximately 1 month before neuropsychological testing, a trained nurse practitioner collected detailed information about the disease course, symptoms, medical history, medication use, and response to pharmacotherapy using a semi-structured interview. The nurse also administered the MMSE and scales related to disease severity and functional status (described below). A clinical neuropsychologist (D.M. or M.B.) administered cognitive tests, either in the hospital or at the participant s home. Patients were tested in their optimal motor state, and we tried to keep distractions to a minimum. Tests were presented in a fixed order (see Appendix A) with short breaks every half hour. The duration of the testing session lasted between 2 and 3 hrs. This was not sufficient for some patients to complete all tests; in such cases, the last three or four tests were omitted. Instruments We evaluated six cognitive domains: psychomotor speed, attention, language, memory, executive functioning, and visuospatial abilities (see Appendix A). 24

8 Cognitive change in PD: 5-year follow-up Stage of disease was rated with the Hoehn & Yahr scale (Hoehn & Yahr, 1967). The motor section of the Unified Parkinson s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS; Fahn & Elton, 1987) was used to assess severity of motor dysfunction. Functional status was determined with the Schwab and England scale (SE; Schwab & England, 1969), Barthel s Activities of Daily Living Index (BADL; Collin, Wade, Davies, & Horne, 1988), and the Functional Independence Measure (FIM; van der Putten, Hobart, Freeman, & Thompson, 1999). Higher scores indicate a more advanced stage of PD (Hoehn & Yahr scale), more severe extrapyramidal symptoms (UPDRS), and better functional status (SE, BADL, and FIM). Mood and anxiety were measured with the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; Zigmond & Snaith, 1983), and the Montgomery-Ǻsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS; Montgomery & Asberg, 1979). Higher scores indicate possible presence of mood or anxiety disorders. Data Imputation In the PD group, 8.9% neuropsychological data points were missing, and 2.8% in the control group. For the PD group, most missing data were from Wechsler Memory Scale- III (WMS-III) facial recognition (38%), Tower of London (33%), Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) Similarities (45%), and Groningen Intelligence Test (GIT) spatial task (37%). These were tests administered at the end of the session. Without these tests, 1.3% data points were missing. However, due to the importance of these tests, we chose to include them in the analyses. Additional analyses (not described here) showed that patients who performed all tests were younger, had a lower age of disease onset, and less axial impairments than patients who did not complete the full neuropsychological examination. No other differences in disease characteristics were observed between the two groups. For tests that a subject attempted but was unable to perform, we replaced missing scores with the lowest score obtained within that group. For tests that could not be completed due to fatigue, lack of motivation or time constraints, missing values were replaced with the groups mean score. We also explored imputation by estimates of missing scores with regression analyses. This method gave comparable results (data not shown; available on request). Statistical Analysis Continuous variables were visually inspected and statistically tested by Kolmogorov Smirnov test for normality of the distribution. Demographic and clinical variables were analyzed with t tests, w2-tests, or Mann-Whitney tests, where appropriate. All p-values <.05 were considered statistically significant unless stated otherwise. Because of the explorative nature of our analyses, we did not correct for multiple comparisons (other than the correction inherent to multivariate analyses; see below) (Rothman, 1990). 25

9 Chapter 2 Pattern and Severity of Cognitive Impairments First, to examine group differences at baseline, multivariate analyses of covariance (MANCOVAs) were performed for each cognitive domain with baseline neuropsychological test scores as independent variables, group (patients vs. controls) as factor, and age, sex, and education as covariates. In case of significant main effects in a cognitive domain, subsequent analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were performed for each test separately. Next, longitudinal data were analyzed using Linear Mixed Models (LMMs) with maximum likelihood estimation to examine cognitive decline. In these models, we accounted for the correlations between repeated measurements. For each neuropsychological test measure, we performed a LMM with age, sex, and education as covariates, group (patient vs. controls) and time as factors, and raw test scores at the three time points as dependent variables. First, we tested whether group differences existed, that is, main effect of group. Thereafter, we added time 3 group to the model to examine interaction effects between time and group. We aimed to study cognitive functions as purely as possible by controlling for motor slowness in analyses of tests with speeded motor components. Therefore, we calculated a composite measure for motor processing speed by averaging standard scores on the Trail Making Test part A and Stroop CWT word reading. These are the two subtests with the strongest motor components, while they do not heavily tax cognitive functions. Instead, they depend on highly automatic processes that require few cognitive resources and are quite robust against brain diseases. Frequency of Individual Cognitive Decline Patients exhibiting cognitive decline were identified by multivariate normative comparison (MNC; Huizenga, Smeding, Grasman, & Schmand, 2007). This method is more sensitive than the conventional clinical approach, which uses normative scores in a univariate way. In neuropsychological evaluations a patient usually completes several tests, the results of which are then compared to normative samples. However, these conventional univariate comparisons have a considerable type I error risk, and are insensitive to patterns in deviation from the norm. The MNC method, based on Hotelling s T2 statistic, compares the pattern of change over the follow-up period of each individual patient to the pattern of change in the control group. This method has an increased power compared to Bonferroni corrected univariate comparisons in groups of this size and with at least moderate correlations between tests. For this MNC method, we used raw change scores for each neuropsychological test measure, that is, 5- year follow-up score baseline score. 26

10 Cognitive change in PD: 5-year follow-up Prognostic Factors of Decline To identify prognostic factors of decline, we compared demographic characteristics, baseline clinical variables, and baseline neuropsychological test scores of the patients with cognitive decline as identified by MNC to those of patients without decline. Separate scores were created from the UPDRS total motor score to assess levodopa responsive motor symptoms (i.e., tremor and bradykinesia; Levy A score) and symptoms that are unresponsive or partially responsive to levodopa (i.e., postural reflexes and speech; Levy B score) (Levy et al., 2000). Next, we entered all variables associated with cognitive decline at p.15 in a hierarchical logistic regression analysis (forward stepwise). For ease of interpretation, and because not all variables were normally distributed, we dichotomized continuous variables. Demographic and clinical baseline variables were entered in the first block. In a second block, we added the baseline neuropsychological test scores to the model. Goodness of model fit was assessed with Hosmer and Lemeshow test. Effect sizes were expressed as odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence interval. PDD Patients were diagnosed with PD dementia (PDD) according to MDS criteria (Dubois et al., 2007; Emre et al., 2007). We diagnosed a patient with PDD if the PD diagnosis was made before the onset of dementia, the patient had a MMSE score <26, cognitive deficits that were severe enough to interfere with daily living, and showed impairments on at least two of the following test items: MMSE serial sevens, MMSE pentagon copying, MMSE 3-word recall, and the clock drawing test. Cognitive impairment severe enough to interfere with daily life was defined as a score 5 on at least one of the FIM items 14 to 18. These items examine to what extent cognitive dysfunction affects functional independence in everyday life; they cover language comprehension and expression, social interaction, problem solving, and memory. Loss to Follow-up Two sets of secondary analyses were conducted to examine attrition in detail. First, we compared baseline clinical characteristics and test performance of patients who were lost to follow-up with those of patients who continued to participate. Next, we compared partial information on neurological and cognitive status at 5-year follow-up from patients lost to neuropsychological follow-up with data from the patients who were still fully participating. 27

11 Chapter 2 RESULTS Demographic and Clinical Variables Patients had 1 year less education, higher HADS and MADRS scores, and lower MMSE scores than controls (Table 1). The follow-up interval was slightly longer for controls (66 months) than for patients (62 months). At 5-year follow-up, 22 patients suffered from on off fluctuations (median duration in off: less than 25% of the day). Pattern and Severity of Cognitive Impairments At baseline, MANCOVAs showed significant differences in performance for each cognitive domain, except visuospatial skills, between patients and controls, that is, Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of PD patients (N = 59) and healthy controls (N = 40). Mean (SD) Baseline 3-year follow-up 5-year follow-up Variable PD HCS PD HCS PD HCS Age 62.5 (9.5) 61.4 (6.8) 65.4 (9.4) 64.5 (6.7) 67.7 (9.5) 66.9 (6.6) Gender (M/F) 32/27 22/18 Education (yrs) 11.6 (2.4) * 12.6 (2.0) DART-IQ 99.9 (20.7) (15.1) MMSE 27.9 (1.8) * 28.9 (1.1) 27.9 (2.0) 28.5 (1.1) 27.5 (2.6) *** 29.1 (1.0) Disease duration (m) 17.5 (7.4) 54.3 (7.7) 79.3 (7.8) Side of onset Right (n) 24 Left (n) 24 Symmetrical (n) 3 Right > Left (n) 6 Left < Right (n) 2 L-dopa (mg) (144.3) (259.6) (234.2) UPDRS motor section 16.0 (7.4) 23.3 (9.0) 25.9 (11.2) Tremor 2.4 (2.1) 2.0 (2.4) 1.8 (2.2) Bradykinesia 6.0 (3.1) 9.1 (3.7) 9.8 (3.8) Axial impairments 1.4 (1.9) 2.5 (2.3) 3.2 (3.4) Levy A 12.3 (5.2) 17.2 (6.6) 13.9 (5.2) Levy B 1.9 (2.2) 3.4 (2.7) 4.3 (3.9) Hoehn and Yahr stage 1.7 (0.7) 2.5 (0.5) 2.6 (0.7) Median MADRS 5.2 (6.8) *** 1.2 (2.8) HADS 10.5 (6.9) * 7.5 (5.0) 10.3 (6.2) ** 6.5 (4.0) 10.5 (7.0) *** 6.2 (4.1) Depression 4.7 (3.6) * 3.0 (2.7) 4.8 (3.2) ** 2.6 (2.2) 5.1 (3.7) *** 2.7 (2.1) SE-ADL (%) 90.7 (5.8) 86.1 (9.7) 82.9 (16.4) BADL 19.8 (0.6) 19.4 (1.3) 18.8 (3.6) FIM (3.6) (8.7) (18.0) * <.05 compared to controls at baseline. ** <.05 compared to controls at 3-year follow-up. *** <.05 compared to controls at 5-year follow-up. PD= patients with Parkinson s disease, HCS=healthy control subjects, DART-IQ=Dutch Adult Reading Test, MMSE=Mini Mental State Examination, MADRS=Montgomery and Asberg Depression Rating Scale, HADS=Hospital Anxiety and Depression Rating Scale, UPDRS=Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale, Levy A=Motor impairments that are responsive to levodopa, Levy B=Motor symptoms that are partially responsive or unresponsive to levodopa, SE-ADL=Schwab and England Activities of Daily Living, BADL=Barthel Activities of Daily Living Index, FIM=Functional Independence Measure 28

12 Cognitive change in PD: 5-year follow-up psychomotor speed (p<.001); attention (p=.001); language (p=.043); memory (p<.001); executive function (p=.001); visuospatial skills (p=.090) (Table 2). Note that the language domain consisted of one test and therefore we only performed an ANCOVA. Additional ANCOVAs showed that the groups significantly differed from each other on all tests, except for Digit Span forward, Visual Association Test, Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (MWCST) perseverations, Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) letter fluency, GIT matrices, and Clock Drawing Test. The vast majority of controls obtained normal scores, that is, between -2 and +2 SD from the age, sex, and education corrected mean. Seven controls scored below -2 SD on one or two tests. LMMs showed that patients obtained lower scores than controls on all tests, except for Digit Span forward (Table 2). Significant interaction effects of group and time were seen on all tests, except for tests of attention, and Trail Making Test A and animal fluency. On most tests, patients obtained lower scores over time while scores in the control group were relatively stable, showing that cognition in PD declines faster than in normal healthy aging. When we added the motor composite score as an extra covariate, only Stroop CWT color naming lost its statistical significance, whereas the interaction effect for animal fluency became significant. The profile of test scores is visualized in Figure 2a. Patients who showed cognitive decline (n=31) were compared to patients showing no cognitive decline (n=28), with respect to demographic and clinical characteristics at baseline (Table 3). Significant differences (with p 0.15) were found on age, years of education, age at onset of PD, UPDRS motor score, bradykinesia, axial symptoms, Levy B, Hoehn & Yahr scale, and LED. Patients with cognitive decline had lower test scores than cognitively stable patients on Digit Symbol test (p=.01), Stroop CWT color naming (p=.09) and interference test (p=.03), Trail Making Test part B (p=.03), Auditory Verbal Learning Test immediate (p=.04) and delayed recall (p=.06), Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test stories immediate (p<.01) and delayed recall (p<.01), Visual Association Test (p=.02), MWCST categories (p=.03), COWAT letter fluency (p=.03), and Similarities (p=.09). Age and age at onset of PD were highly correlated. Therefore, only age at onset was used as a predictor variable. Logistic regression analyses showed that the model significantly improved at each block (block 1: p<.03, block 2: p=.01). In the first block, age of onset (OR=4.71; 95% CI= ; p=.01) and Hoehn & Yahr score (OR=3.37; 95% CI= ; p=.04) were significant predictors of cognitive decline. In the second block where the psychological test variables were entered, Hoehn & Yahr score lost its significance. Age of onset (OR=4.34; 95% CI= ; p=.024) and Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test immediate recall (OR=7.78; 95% CI= ; p=.002) contributed significantly to the 29

13 Chapter 2 prediction of decline. The Hosmer and Lemeshow test showed the model fitted the data (χ 2 =1.605; p=.90). a) b) Figure 2. Standardized test scores in Figure 2a are z-scores obtained by dividing the difference between the subject s raw test score and the mean baseline score of the control group by the standard deviation of scores in the control group, i.e. (raw test score minus mean score of control group) / SD of scores of the control group. Standardized change scores in Figure 2b are z-scores obtained by dividing the difference between the subject s change score and the mean change score of the control group by the standard deviation of change scores in the control group, i.e. (change score minus mean change score of control group) / SD of change scores of the control group. Negative z-scores indicate decreased performance compared to baseline performance of controls (Figure 2a) or faster decline than the control group (Figure 2b). PD=Parkinson s disease, HCS=Healthy control subjects, RAVLT=Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, RBMT= Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test, WMS=Wechsler Memory Scale, MWCST= Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, COWAT= Controlled Oral Word Association Test, WAIS=Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, GIT= Groningen Intelligence Test, JOLO=Judgment of Line Orientation. 30

14 Cognitive change in PD: 5-year follow-up Table 2. Neuropsychological test findings (raw scores) of 59 PD patients and 40 healthy controls (HCS) at baseline, three and five year follow-up. Mean (SD). Baseline 3-Year follow-up 5-Year follow-up Group Group* Time Group*Time Measure PD HCS PD HCS PD HCS p-value p-value p-value Psychomotor speed Digit Symbol test 40.8 (11.3) 53.5 (10.0) 36.7 (13.7) 52.8 (10.9) 36.3 (14.5) 50.9 (9.8) < < < Trail Making Test A 46.1 (23.9) 35.6 (10.6) 47.9 (23.3) 35.5 (13.1) 64.6 (59.4) 30.9 (8.8) < Stroop test (word reading) 47.8 (9.9) 42.7 (7.6) 50.8 (12.5) 41.6 (5.8) 58.0 (21.2) 42.1 (5.7) < < Stroop test (color naming) 63.0 (14.6) 56.1 (10.0) 65.0 (15.2) 55.6 (8.9) 74.0 (29.1) 53.9 (7.7) < Attention Frequency of Cognitive Decline Next, we examined decline at the individual level with MNC using raw change scores. By this statistical criterion, 31 patients [53% of 59; 95% confidence interval (CI) ] showed significantly more decline than controls (at p<.05, one-sided). Removing tests that were influenced by motor processing speed did not alter this result. The pattern of change is depicted in Figure 2b. Digit span forward 12.1 (2.9) 12.2 (3.5) 12.2 (3.2) 12.4 (3.3) 11.7 (2.8) 13.2 (3.1) Digit span backward 8.2 (2.5) 9.9 (2.9) 8.6 (3.5) 9.7 (2.8) 7.8 (2.6) 9.5 (2.8) Trail Making Test B 107 (46) 75 (21) 134 (108) 78 (28) 164 (145) 81 (30) < Stroop interference test 121 (55) 95 (24) 124 (69) 97 (20) 129 (53) 98 (22) Language Boston Naming Test 26.3 (3.0) 27.7 (2.0) 25.7 (3.1) 27.5 (2.2) 52.4 (5.1) 55.0 (3.6) Memory RAVLT trial (9.6) 50.7 (7.7) 40.2 (10.4) 48.7 (8.5) 38.1 (12.5) 48.1 (8.7) < RAVLT delayed recall 8.3 (2.7) 11.0 (2.7) 7.8 (3.0) 10.4 (2.6) 6.9 (3.8) 10.1 (3.2) < RAVLT recognition 28.2 (1.7) 29.0 (1.8) 27.8 (2.6) 29.0 (1.5) 26.7 (2.8) 29.0 (1.8) < < RBMT immediate recall 15.1 (5.8) 19.6 (4.4) 14.8 (6.5) 20.7 (5.6) 14.5 (5.2) 18.9 (4.7) < RBMT delayed recall 11.4 (5.5) 15.8 (4.5) 11.1 (6.9) 17.5 (5.5) 10.9 (5.5) 15.6 (5.2) < WMS-III Faces immediate 31.8 (4.4) 35.4 (3.7) 34.6 (4.4) 36.7 (3.9) 34.1 (3.3) 36.6 (3.7) < < WMS-III Faces delayed 36.0 (3.6) 38.3 (3.4) 36.8 (4.2) 39.0 (3.9) 35.1 (2.8) 35.7 (4.0) < Visual Association Test 11.6 (0.9) 11.9 (0.6) 11.6 (1.1) 11.8 (0.5) 11.0 (2.7) 11.9 (0.5) Executive functions MWCST, no categories 4.2 (1.6) 5.1 (1.3) 4.4 (1.7) 5.3 (1.3) 3.9 (2.0) 5.6 (0.9) < MWCST, no perseverations 4.9 (3.8) 3.5 (5.4) 4.8 (4.3) 2.4 (2.3) 8.1 (9.3) 2.0 (2.8) Animal fluency 19.9 (5.5) 22.7 (4.6) 21.3 (6.0) 24.3 (4.2) 19.7 (5.9) 23.8 (3.3) < Supermarket fluency 20.4 (5.6) 22.6 (3.9) 21.7 (5.5) 23.6 (3.6) 20.2 (7.4) 25.0 (5.0) < COWAT letter fluency 30.8 (10.3) 35.9 (9.7) 30.5 (11.7) 38.5 (10.9) 31.5 (11.8) 38.1 (10.8) Tower of London test 6.1 (1.8) 7.6 (1.9) 5.6 (2.3) 7.2 (2.0) 5.9 (1.7) 7.5 (1.8) < WAIS-III Similarities 21.3 (5.9) 25.7 (3.5) 20.6 (6.5) 25.3 (4.5) 20.2 (4.9) 24.8 (4.5) < < Visuospatial/constructive skills JOLO 24.4 (4.1) 26.6 (3.5) 23.8 (4.7) 27.2 (3.1) 22.6 (4.4) 25.9 (3.5) < GIT spatial task 10.2 (3.1) 11.8 (3.2) 10.2 (3.2) 11.6 (3.8) 10.7 (2.9) 12.4 (3.5) < Clock Drawing Test 12.7 (1.3) 13.1 (1.2) 12.4 (1.6) 13.1 (1.0) 10.7 (2.6) 12.4 (1.3) < Prognostic Factors of Cognitive Decline Linear Mixed Model with UPDRS item 31 (bodybradykinesia) as covariate, in addition to age, sex, and years of education. PD=Parkinson s disease, HCS=Healthy control subjects, RAVLT=Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, RBMT= Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test, WMS=Wechsler Memory Scale, MWCST=Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, COWAT= Controlled Oral Word Association Test, WAIS=Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, GIT= Groningen Intelligence Test, JOLO=Judgment of Line Orientation. 31

15 Chapter 2 Table 3. Demographic and clinical variables at baseline for patients who were cognitively stable (N=28) and those who showed cognitive decline after five years (N=31). Stable Decline Variable M SD M SD p Value Age Gender (m/f) 16/15 16/ Education (years) DART-IQ Age at onset of PD Duration of PD (months) UPDRS motor section Tremor Bradykinesia Axial symptoms Levy A Levy B Hoehn and Yahr scale MMSE HADS Depression Anxiety LED (mg/day) SE-ADL (%) Barthel Activities of Daily Living * DART-IQ=Dutch Adult Reading Test, UPDRS=Unified Parkinson s Disease Rating Scale, Levy A=Motor impairments that are responsive to levodopa, Levy B=Motor symptoms that are partially responsive or unresponsive to levodopa, MMSE=Mini-Mental State Examination, HADS=Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, LED=Levodopa equivalent dose, SE-ADL=Schwab and England Activities of Daily Living. PDD Diagnoses Of the 59 patients who completed all three assessments, 2 patients had PDD at the 3- year follow-up, and another 3 patients at the 5-year follow-up. Thus, after 5 years, 5 of 59 (8.5%) had PDD. Of the 133 patients who were included in the CARPA study, 20 patients (15.0%) had developed PDD. Loss to Follow-up Due to the relatively large number of patients lost to follow-up, the true percentage of patients showing cognitive decline is larger than 53%. We compared patients lost to neuropsychological follow-up (n=64) with patients who fully participated in the current follow-up (n=59). Patients who were lost to follow-up were older, had higher age at disease onset, more axial impairments, and scored higher on the Hoehn & Yahr scale (Table 4). Thus, patients who were lost to follow-up had greater disease severity at baseline. Of the 64 patients who were lost to neuropsychological follow-up, 14 patients developed PDD. Since this confirmed that attrition led to an underestimation of cognitive decline, we conducted a second set of analyses. We compared the clinical data we were able to obtain from 34 patients, who were lost to the present neuropsychological followup but had neurological and functional assessments, with data obtained from patients 32

16 Cognitive change in PD: 5-year follow-up who continued to fully participate (n=59; Table 5). Patients who were lost had lower MMSE scores, higher Hoehn & Yahr scores, showed more axial symptoms and more motor symptoms unresponsive or partially responsive to levodopa, had more symptoms of anxiety and depression, and scored lower on the SE-ADL and BADL scales. Again, these results indicate greater disability in patients who were lost to follow-up. Due to loss to follow-up, reliable data on cognitive decline 5 years after diagnosis are lacking in 50 patients (64 patients lost to follow-up minus 14 patients with PDD). If we assume as a conservative estimate the same percentage of decline in this subgroup as in those who continued to participate (i.e., 53%), then approximately 27 patients in this group should have declined. The total number of patients, who show cognitive decline, including PDD, should then be estimated at 73 of 123, or 59%. Table 4. Demographic and clinical baseline data of PD patients who were lost to neuropsychological follow-up (N = 64) and patients who fully participated in the 5-year follow-up (N = 59); mean (SD). Variable PD patients lost to PD patients in 5-year p-value follow-up follow-up Age (yrs) 69.4 (10.3) 62.5 (9.5) Age of onset (yrs) 67.7 (10.4) 61.0 (9.5) Disease duration (m) 20.2 (12.9) 17.5 (7.4) L-dopa (mg) (138.2) (144.3) MMSE 27.3 (2.1) 27.9 (1.8) UPDRS motor section 18.1 (8.3) 16.0 (7.4) Tremor 2.4 (1.7) 2.4 (2.1) Bradykinesia 6.5 (3.3) 6.0 (3.1) Axial impairments 2.2 (2.1) 1.4 (1.9) Levy A 13.3 (5.7) 12.3 (5.2) Levy B 2.6 (2.5) 1.8 (2.2) Hoehn and Yahr stage 1.9 (0.7) 1.7 (0.7) Median HADS 10.1 (7.8) 10.5 (6.9) Anxiety 5.2 (4.3) 5.8 (4.0) Depression 4.9 (3.9) 4.7 (3.6) SE-ADL (%) 89.7 (7.6) 90.7 (5.8) BADL 19.4 (1.4) 19.8 (0.6) MMSE=Mini Mental State Examination, HADS=Hospital Anxiety and Depression Rating Scale, UPDRS=Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale, Levy A=Motor impairments that are responsive to levodopa, Levy B=Motor symptoms that are partially responsive or unresponsive to levodopa HADS=Hospital Anxiety and Depression Rating Scale, SE-ADL=Schwab and England Activities of Daily Living, BADL=Barthel Activities of Daily Living Index, = <.05 compared to PD patients participating in the 5 year follow-up. 33

17 Chapter 2 Table 5. Clinical data at 5 year follow-up of PD patients who were lost to neuropsychological follow-up and patients who fully participated in the 5 year follow-up; mean (SD). Variable N* PD patients lost to PD patients in 5-year p-value follow-up follow-up L-dopa (mg) (250.8) (234.2) MMSE (4.9) (2.6) UPDRS motor section (10.9) (11.2) Tremor (1.3) (1.2) Bradykinesia (4.8) (3.8) Axial impairments (3.7) (3.4) Levy A (5.6) (5.2) Levy B (4.3) (3.9) Hoehn and Yahr stage (0.8) (0.7) Median HADS (3.0) (7.0) Anxiety (1.7) (4.0) Depression (2.2) (3.7) SE-ADL (%) (23.0) (16.4) BADL (4.5) (3.6) MMSE=Mini Mental State Examination, HADS=Hospital Anxiety and Depression Rating Scale, UPDRS=Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale, Levy A=Motor impairments that are responsive to levodopa, Levy B=Motor symptoms that are partially responsive or unresponsive to levodopa HADS=Hospital Anxiety and Depression Rating Scale, SE-ADL=Schwab and England Activities of Daily Living, BADL=Barthel Activities of Daily Living Index, <.05 compared to PD patients participating in the 5 year follow-up, *Varies due to missing values. 34

18 Cognitive change in PD: 5-year follow-up DISCUSSION Our aim was to examine the pattern and severity of cognitive impairments, the frequency of individual cognitive decline, and prognostic markers of cognitive decline in a hospital-based cohort of newly diagnosed PD patients. Overall, patients performed worse than controls on most neuropsychological tests. Impairments were already observed at baseline in most cognitive domains, and patients declined faster than controls in all domains except attention (Table 2). Fifty-three percent of the patients who continued to participate showed more cognitive decline than the 5th percentile of control subjects over the course of 5 years, including five patients (8.5%) with PDD. In total, 15% of the original patient sample had become demented. Attrition led to selection bias; therefore, the actual percentage of patients with cognitive decline is probably even greater. We estimate it at 59%, including PDD. Higher age at onset and weak memory performance at baseline predicted cognitive decline. These results, that is, impaired performance in patients on the majority of neuropsychological tests, are in line with our previous analyses and with other studies (Muslimovic et al., 2005; Muslimovic et al., 2009; Williams-Gray et al., 2007). PD patients not only obtain lower baseline scores on neuropsychological testing, but also their cognitive performance declines faster over time than in normal healthy aging. Some neuropsychological tests have strong motor components such as Trail Making Test part B and verbal fluency tests. Correcting for motor processing speed, however, did not greatly alter the results. Thus, after 5 years, patients showed cognitive impairments irrespective of motor processing speed. The pattern of cognitive decline (Figure 2) is largely similar to our previous 3- year follow-up report with substantial decline in the psychomotor domain. The decline in attention and executive functioning is more restricted to certain tests, that is, Digit Span, and the Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (categories and perseverations). The large score drop on the Trail Making Test part B must be due to motor slowing, since the group by time interaction effect was not significant after correcting for motor speed (Table 2). Decline over the course of 5 years was slightly smaller, but evident in the visuospatial domain. However, this was largely restricted to clock drawing. In the memory domain we observed moderate decline. Figure 2 even shows a slight increase in performance for WMS-III Faces. However, this is due to decreased performance in the control group, while performance in patients was relatively stable. The decline in memory and visuospatial skills, albeit moderate, could be related to the neuropathological staging of PD. In clinically manifest PD, the essential neuropathology involves the nigrostriatal tracts and the mesocortical pathways (Jellinger, 2001). Disruption in both pathways has been related to executive dysfunctioning. In later stages, pathology is more widespread, also affecting posterior cortical areas, which correlates with the risk of developing dementia (Braak, Rub, Jansen 35

19 Chapter 2 Steur, Del Tredici, & de Vos, 2005). Memory decline and visuospatial impairment may be mediated by these posterior disease processes (Lee et al., 2010; Williams-Gray et al., 2007). An alternative explanation is that decline in memory and visuospatial skills is due to executive components in visuospatial and memory tests (Beyer et al., 2012; Dubois & Pillon, 1997; Emre, 2007). Neuropsychological tests never exclusively tax a single cognitive domain. The Trail Making Test part B and Stroop interference test, for instance, are frequently seen as measures of executive functioning, but also as tests of attention, while both require a speeded psychomotor response. Thus, the division of cognition into domains, and the allocation of tests to a particular domain, is artificial to a certain extent. Our final aim was to identify predictors of cognitive decline. Previous studies reported various predictors, such as age, male sex, hallucinations, axial impairment, impaired semantic fluency or pentagon copying, and executive dysfunction (Aarsland et al., 2003; Hobson & Meara, 2004; Levy et al., 2002; Muslimovic et al., 2009; Uc et al., 2009; Williams-Gray et al., 2009). In our study, only age at onset and weak memory performance at baseline were independent prognostic factors of cognitive decline. Age at onset of PD has previously been reported as a predictor of cognitive decline(muslimovic et al., 2009). Memory impairment as a predictor for cognitive decline, however, is a less common finding. In 2004, Hobson and Meara reported that more profound memory impairment at baseline increases the risk of dementia (Hobson & Meara, 2004). Due to the high age (i.e., mean age of 78) in their cohort, they ascribed this to the development of Alzheimer s disease. However, patients in our cohort were relatively young. Furthermore, Alzheimer s disease pathology in PD is usually rather mild. It has, therefore, been suggested that cognitive decline is more likely attributable to cumulative effects of PD pathology (Braak et al., 2005). The fact that memory impairments at baseline also predict further cognitive decline, and the actual decline in memory that we found, may thus reflect the involvement of other brain areas besides the nigrostriatal system and mesocortical pathways. This notion is in line with another finding in our study, that is, the finding that the primary determinants of disability in PD are motor symptoms not responsive to levodopa and cognitive dysfunction at the time of diagnosis (Velseboer et al., 2013). Although memory impairment in PD most often concerns the retrieval of information, in the current study we also saw decline on a recognition test and visual association test. These tests reflect more posterior, medial temporal lobe dysfunction. This supports the recently postulated dual syndrome hypothesis of PD: (1) a syndrome characterized by frontal-striatal dysfunction and (2) a syndrome with distinctive early visuospatial deficits that are indicative of the involvement of temporal and other posterior cortical areas (Kehagia, Barker, & Robbins, 2012). Patients with more profound executive 36

20 Cognitive change in PD: 5-year follow-up impairment at baseline, suggestive of fronto-striatal dysfunction, generally progress slower than those with more profound memory and visuospatial impairments. Despite the fact that a memory test predicted cognitive decline, we see a moderate decline over time in the memory domain itself. This could be due to the psychometric properties of the neuropsychological tests: it is possible that the reliability of neuropsychological tests is not uniform across the score distribution. Scores in the midst of the distribution, thus in the normal scoring ranges, might be measured more reliably and sensitively than scores in the lower ranges of the distribution. Therefore, decline in the range that patients already score lower than healthy controls, may be more difficult to measure. A second possible explanation for the discrepancy between memory impairment as a prognostic factor and the modest decline in memory functioning, relates to the unpredictable pattern of disease progression. The progression of cognitive decline might be unequally phased. For example, early decline in a cognitive domain may be followed by a relatively stable period without decline. This could be related to the characteristics of disease progression itself, but also by other factors, such as dopaminergic medication. An increase in medication may elevate dopamine levels in the midbrain which could positively affect cognition. These explanations are admittedly speculative. One of the main problems in follow-up studies is attrition, because this may lead to selection bias. Selection bias is often left implicit, so that the true extent of cognitive decline remains unclear. Fortunately, we had some information about cognitive status and physical functioning of patients who refused to participate in the neuropsychological follow-up. These data corroborated that patients who were lost to follow-up were older, had an older age of onset, and had more severe motor symptoms and more cognitive deficits than the patients who were still participating. Thus, the true extent of cognitive decline in PD is greater than what is shown by our neuropsychological test results. This is also supported by the percentage of patients with PDD after 5 years, which was 8.5% in the neuropsychological part of the study, but twice as high in the total sample. This study has several strengths. First, at baseline we studied a relatively large sample of newly diagnosed PD patients. This allowed us to monitor disease progression and cognitive decline in detail from the moment of diagnosis. Second, we used a sensitive statistical method (MNC) to detect cognitive change at the individual level. Third, by extending the follow-up duration to 5 years, the power of our analyses was improved. A fourth strength is the large battery of neuropsychological tests. Fifth, the diagnosis of PD was validated twice, at baseline and at 5-year follow-up. There are also caveats. First, our patient sample was recruited in hospitals. One may argue, therefore, that its representativeness for the PD population at large is not clear. However, the clinical characteristics of our sample are not very different from 37

21 Chapter 2 those in population-based studies (Aarsland et al., 2003; Elgh et al., 2009; Foltynie et al., 2004). Moreover, in the Netherlands virtually all PD patients are sooner or later referred to a neurologist. Second, our study has a high attrition rate. This is not just an issue in the current study, but selection bias should be taken into account in all studies that investigate (cognitive) changes in PD. If patients who are lost to follow-up (or who refuse to participate in cross-sectional studies) are more severely ill, then it is likely that the true extent of cognitive decline is greater than what has been shown until now in PD research. The high attrition rate was partly due to the length of the neuropsychological evaluation, which some patients found too burdensome. Third, another drawback of using a large neuropsychological test battery was that not all patients could complete all tests. We imputed missing values so we could analyze these tests and would not lose power in our analyses, but this might affect the results. Therefore, we repeated the analyses without tests that had a large number of missing values (data not shown), but this did not change our conclusions. In conclusion, 5 years after the diagnosis of PD, approximately two-thirds of the patients show cognitive decline. The greatest decline was observed in the areas of psychomotor speed and memory. Patients with a higher age at onset and memory impairment in an early disease stage are most at risk for further cognitive decline. Acknowledgements The authors thank the reviewers for their valuable comments which helped us to improve this manuscript. We thank the nurse practitioners for their help in data collection. Also, we thank the Medical Center Alkmaar, Meander Medical Center Amersfoort, Slotervaart Hospital Amsterdam, St. Lucas-Andreas Hospital Amsterdam, OLVG Amsterdam, and Academic Medical Center Amsterdam for their help with patient inclusion and for facilitating the neurological and neuropsychological examinations. And most of all, we thank the patients and healthy controls for their cooperation. Study funding This work was supported by the Prinses Beatrix fonds [grant PGO to BS] and the Michael J. Fox Foundation (grant to BS). It is part of the CARPA project, sponsored by ZonMW. Rob M.A. de Bie received an unrestricted fellowship grant from Medtronic (Minneapolis, MN). All other authors report no conflicts of interest. 38

Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B.

Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Post, B. (2009). Clinimetrics,

More information

Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B.

Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Post, B. (2009). Clinimetrics,

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Brain training improves recovery after stroke but waiting list improves equally: A multicenter randomized controlled trial of a computer-based cognitive flexibility

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Vascular factors in dementia and apathy Eurelings, Lisa. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Vascular factors in dementia and apathy Eurelings, Lisa. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Vascular factors in dementia and apathy Eurelings, Lisa Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Eurelings, L. S. M. (2016). Vascular factors in

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Cognitive change in Parkinson's disease Broeders, M. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Cognitive change in Parkinson's disease Broeders, M. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Cognitive change in Parkinson's disease Broeders, M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Broeders, M. (2015). Cognitive change in Parkinson's

More information

Cognitive-Motor Interference in Persons with Parkinson Disease

Cognitive-Motor Interference in Persons with Parkinson Disease Cognitive-Motor Interference in Persons with Parkinson Disease Tara L. McIsaac, PhD, PT Associate Professor of Physical Therapy A.T. Still University Arizona School of Health Sciences October 11, 2014

More information

Nature, prevalence and clinical significance. Barcelona, Spain

Nature, prevalence and clinical significance. Barcelona, Spain Nature, prevalence and clinical significance Jaime Kulisevsky Barcelona, Spain 1 Non motor (neuropsychiatric) symptoms are an integral part of Parkinson s s disease (PD) Affective disorders And are associated

More information

Neuropsychological Evaluation of

Neuropsychological Evaluation of Neuropsychological Evaluation of Alzheimer s Disease Joanne M. Hamilton, Ph.D. Shiley-Marcos Alzheimer s Disease Research Center Department of Neurosciences University of California, San Diego Establish

More information

Citation for published version (APA): van Munster, B. C. (2009). Pathophysiological studies in delirium : a focus on genetics.

Citation for published version (APA): van Munster, B. C. (2009). Pathophysiological studies in delirium : a focus on genetics. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Pathophysiological studies in delirium : a focus on genetics van Munster, B.C. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): van Munster, B. C. (2009).

More information

Gezinskenmerken: De constructie van de Vragenlijst Gezinskenmerken (VGK) Klijn, W.J.L.

Gezinskenmerken: De constructie van de Vragenlijst Gezinskenmerken (VGK) Klijn, W.J.L. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Gezinskenmerken: De constructie van de Vragenlijst Gezinskenmerken (VGK) Klijn, W.J.L. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Klijn, W. J. L. (2013).

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Den Bosch: Boxpress

Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Den Bosch: Boxpress UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Parigger, E.M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language

More information

Original Articles. Calne, resting tremor. Mortimer, Pirozzolo, Hansch, & Webster, postural disturbance III

Original Articles. Calne, resting tremor. Mortimer, Pirozzolo, Hansch, & Webster, postural disturbance III 2004 97-106 Original Articles 1 2 3 1 1 2 3 47 22 III I II muscular rigidity postural disturbance resting tremor bradykinesia Calne, 2001 Mortimer, Pirozzolo, Hansch, & Webster, 1982 Tel: 02-23627076 E-mail:

More information

Dementia in Parkinson s disease:

Dementia in Parkinson s disease: Dementia in Parkinson s disease: A 20 year Prospective Neuropsychological Study Sydney Multicentre Study Associate Professor Wayne GJ Reid PhD FAPS 149 newly diagnosed community living Parkinson s Disease

More information

Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B.

Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Post, B. (2009). Clinimetrics,

More information

NO LOWER COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING IN OLDER ADULTS WITH ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER

NO LOWER COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING IN OLDER ADULTS WITH ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER CHAPTER 6 NO LOWER COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING IN OLDER ADULTS WITH ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER INT PSYCHOGERIATR, 2015, 27(9): 1467 1476 DOI: 10.1017/S1041610215000010 73 NO LOWER COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING

More information

Advances in Abdominal Aortic Aneurysm Care - Towards personalized, centralized and endovascular care van Beek, S.C.

Advances in Abdominal Aortic Aneurysm Care - Towards personalized, centralized and endovascular care van Beek, S.C. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Advances in Abdominal Aortic Aneurysm Care - Towards personalized, centralized and endovascular care van Beek, S.C. Link to publication Citation for published version

More information

University of Groningen. Visual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease Meppelink, Anne Marthe

University of Groningen. Visual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease Meppelink, Anne Marthe University of Groningen Visual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease Meppelink, Anne Marthe IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from

More information

Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B.

Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Clinimetrics, clinical profile and prognosis in early Parkinson s disease Post, B. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Post, B. (2009). Clinimetrics,

More information

Iron and vitamin D deficiency in children living in Western-Europe Akkermans, M.D.

Iron and vitamin D deficiency in children living in Western-Europe Akkermans, M.D. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Iron and vitamin D deficiency in children living in Western-Europe Akkermans, M.D. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Akkermans, M. D. (2017).

More information

AGED SPECIFIC ASSESSMENT TOOLS. Anna Ciotta Senior Clinical Neuropsychologist Peninsula Mental Health Services

AGED SPECIFIC ASSESSMENT TOOLS. Anna Ciotta Senior Clinical Neuropsychologist Peninsula Mental Health Services AGED SPECIFIC ASSESSMENT TOOLS Anna Ciotta Senior Clinical Neuropsychologist Peninsula Mental Health Services Issues in assessing the Elderly Association between biological, psychological, social and cultural

More information

University of Alberta

University of Alberta University of Alberta Longitudinal performance of Neuropsychological Assessments in Parkinson s Disease. by Taim Abdullah Muayqil A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial

More information

The Five-Point Test: Reliability, Validity and Normative Data for Children and Adults

The Five-Point Test: Reliability, Validity and Normative Data for Children and Adults The Five-Point Test: Reliability, Validity and Normative Data for Children and Adults Lara Tucha 1 *, Steffen Aschenbrenner 2, Janneke Koerts 1, Klaus W. Lange 3 1 Department of Clinical and Developmental

More information

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE 5.1 GENERAL BACKGROUND Neuropsychological assessment plays a crucial role in the assessment of cognitive decline in older age. In India, there

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Schilt, T. (2009). Thinking of ecstasy : neuropsychological aspects of ecstasy use

Citation for published version (APA): Schilt, T. (2009). Thinking of ecstasy : neuropsychological aspects of ecstasy use UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Thinking of ecstasy : neuropsychological aspects of ecstasy use Schilt, T. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Schilt, T. (2009). Thinking of

More information

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist M------- P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION Name: Date of Birth: Date of Evaluation: 05-28-2015 Tests Administered: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Genetic basis of hypertrophic cardiomyopathy Bos, J.M. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Genetic basis of hypertrophic cardiomyopathy Bos, J.M. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Genetic basis of hypertrophic cardiomyopathy Bos, J.M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Bos, J. M. (2010). Genetic basis of hypertrophic

More information

Cognitive sequelae of Parkinson s disease : nature, course, risk factors and functional impact Muslimovi, D.

Cognitive sequelae of Parkinson s disease : nature, course, risk factors and functional impact Muslimovi, D. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Cognitive sequelae of Parkinson s disease : nature, course, risk factors and functional impact Muslimovi, D. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA):

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Schilt, T. (2009). Thinking of ecstasy : neuropsychological aspects of ecstasy use

Citation for published version (APA): Schilt, T. (2009). Thinking of ecstasy : neuropsychological aspects of ecstasy use UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Thinking of ecstasy : neuropsychological aspects of ecstasy use Schilt, T. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Schilt, T. (2009). Thinking of

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Improving aspects of palliative care for children Jagt, C.T. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Improving aspects of palliative care for children Jagt, C.T. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Improving aspects of palliative care for children Jagt, C.T. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Jagt, C. T. (2017). Improving aspects of palliative

More information

Clinical Study A New Approach for the Quantitative Evaluation of the Clock Drawing Test: Preliminary Results on Subjects with Parkinson s Disease

Clinical Study A New Approach for the Quantitative Evaluation of the Clock Drawing Test: Preliminary Results on Subjects with Parkinson s Disease Neurology Research International Volume 2010, Article ID 283890, 6 pages doi:10.1155/2010/283890 Clinical Study A New Approach for the Quantitative Evaluation of the Clock Drawing Test: Preliminary Results

More information

Anxiety disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: A clinical and health care economic perspective van Steensel, F.J.A.

Anxiety disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: A clinical and health care economic perspective van Steensel, F.J.A. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Anxiety disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: A clinical and health care economic perspective van Steensel, F.J.A. Link to publication Citation for

More information

Process of a neuropsychological assessment

Process of a neuropsychological assessment Test selection Process of a neuropsychological assessment Gather information Review of information provided by referrer and if possible review of medical records Interview with client and his/her relative

More information

Prediction of toxicity in concurrent chemoradiation for non-small cell lung cancer Uijterlinde, W.I.

Prediction of toxicity in concurrent chemoradiation for non-small cell lung cancer Uijterlinde, W.I. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Prediction of toxicity in concurrent chemoradiation for non-small cell lung cancer Uijterlinde, W.I. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Uijterlinde,

More information

Anterior and Posterior Types of Neuropsychological Deficits in Parkinson s Disease: A Subgroup Classification of CognitiveOutcome

Anterior and Posterior Types of Neuropsychological Deficits in Parkinson s Disease: A Subgroup Classification of CognitiveOutcome Undergraduate Review Volume 10 Article 26 2014 Anterior and Posterior Types of Neuropsychological Deficits in Parkinson s Disease: A Subgroup Classification of CognitiveOutcome Megan Risi Follow this and

More information

Citation for published version (APA): van der Put, C. E. (2011). Risk and needs assessment for juvenile delinquents

Citation for published version (APA): van der Put, C. E. (2011). Risk and needs assessment for juvenile delinquents UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Risk and needs assessment for juvenile delinquents van der Put, C.E. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): van der Put, C. E. (2011). Risk and

More information

Characterizing scaphoid nonunion deformity using 2-D and 3-D imaging techniques ten Berg, P.W.L.

Characterizing scaphoid nonunion deformity using 2-D and 3-D imaging techniques ten Berg, P.W.L. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Characterizing scaphoid nonunion deformity using 2-D and 3-D imaging techniques ten Berg, P.W.L. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): ten Berg,

More information

Identifying and evaluating patterns of prescription opioid use and associated risks in Ontario, Canada Gomes, T.

Identifying and evaluating patterns of prescription opioid use and associated risks in Ontario, Canada Gomes, T. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Identifying and evaluating patterns of prescription opioid use and associated risks in Ontario, Canada Gomes, T. Link to publication Citation for published version

More information

The Relation Between Cognition and Motor Dysfunction in Drug-Naive Newly Diagnosed Patients with Parkinson s Disease

The Relation Between Cognition and Motor Dysfunction in Drug-Naive Newly Diagnosed Patients with Parkinson s Disease RESEARCH ARTICLE The Relation Between Cognition and Motor Dysfunction in Drug-Naive Newly Diagnosed Patients with Parkinson s Disease Magdalena Eriksson Domellöf, MSc, 1,2 * Eva Elgh, PhD, 2 and Lars Forsgren,

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) The artificial pancreas Kropff, J. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) The artificial pancreas Kropff, J. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) The artificial pancreas Kropff, J. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Kropff, J. (2017). The artificial pancreas: From logic to life General

More information

Different PD-MCI criteria and risk of dementia in Parkinson s disease: 4-year longitudinal study

Different PD-MCI criteria and risk of dementia in Parkinson s disease: 4-year longitudinal study www.nature.com/npjparkd All rights reserved 2373-8057/16 ARTICLE OPEN Different PD-MCI criteria and risk of dementia in Parkinson s disease: 4-year longitudinal study Kyla-Louise Wood 1,2, Daniel J Myall

More information

DEMENTIA, THE BRAIN AND HOW IT WORKS AND WHY YOU MATTER

DEMENTIA, THE BRAIN AND HOW IT WORKS AND WHY YOU MATTER OVERCOMING THE CHALLENGES OF MANAGING CHRONIC DISEASES IN PERSONS WITH DEMENTIA DEMENTIA, THE BRAIN AND HOW IT WORKS AND WHY YOU MATTER LEARNING OBJECTIVES Be familiar with the diagnostic criteria for

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Von Eije, K. J. (2009). RNAi based gene therapy for HIV-1, from bench to bedside

Citation for published version (APA): Von Eije, K. J. (2009). RNAi based gene therapy for HIV-1, from bench to bedside UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) RNAi based gene therapy for HIV-1, from bench to bedside Von Eije, K.J. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Von Eije, K. J. (2009). RNAi based

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Standaarden voor kerndoelen basisonderwijs : de ontwikkeling van standaarden voor kerndoelen basisonderwijs op basis van resultaten uit peilingsonderzoek van der

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Lammers-van der Holst, H. M. (2016). Individual differences in shift work tolerance

Citation for published version (APA): Lammers-van der Holst, H. M. (2016). Individual differences in shift work tolerance UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Individual differences in shift work tolerance Lammers-van der Holst, H.M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Lammers-van der Holst, H. M.

More information

Evolution of cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson s disease cohort

Evolution of cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson s disease cohort doi:10.1093/brain/awm111 Brain (2007), 130,1787^1798 Evolution of cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson s disease cohort C. H. Williams-Gray, 1 T. Foltynie, 1 C. E. G. Brayne, 2 T. W. Robbins

More information

Chronic Effects of Dopaminergic Replacement on Cognitive Function in Parkinson s Disease: A Two-Year Follow-Up Study of Previously Untreated Patients

Chronic Effects of Dopaminergic Replacement on Cognitive Function in Parkinson s Disease: A Two-Year Follow-Up Study of Previously Untreated Patients Movement Disorders Vol. 15, No. 4, 2000, pp. 613 626 2000 Movement Disorder Society Chronic Effects of Dopaminergic Replacement on Cognitive Function in Parkinson s Disease: A Two-Year Follow-Up Study

More information

Method. NeuRA Schizophrenia and bipolar disorder April 2016

Method. NeuRA Schizophrenia and bipolar disorder April 2016 Introduction Schizophrenia is characterised by positive, negative and disorganised symptoms. Positive symptoms refer to experiences additional to what would be considered normal experience, such as hallucinations

More information

Neuropsychology and Parkinson s Disease. Erin Holker, Ph.D., ABPP Neuropsychology Laboratory

Neuropsychology and Parkinson s Disease. Erin Holker, Ph.D., ABPP Neuropsychology Laboratory Neuropsychology and Parkinson s Disease Erin Holker, Ph.D., ABPP Neuropsychology Laboratory Parkinson s disease and cognition Parkinson s disease is not just a disorder of movement The same circuits associated

More information

Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives

Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives B. Arts 1 *, N. Jabben 1, L. Krabbendam 1 and J. van Os 1,2 1 Department of Psychiatry

More information

Use a diagnostic neuropsychology HOW TO DO IT PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY

Use a diagnostic neuropsychology HOW TO DO IT PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY 170 PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY HOW TO DO IT Pract Neurol: first published as 10.1046/j.1474-7766.2003.08148.x on 1 June 2003. Downloaded from http://pn.bmj.com/ Use a diagnostic neuropsychology on 16 October

More information

The ABCs of Dementia Diagnosis

The ABCs of Dementia Diagnosis The ABCs of Dementia Diagnosis Dr. Robin Heinrichs, Ph.D., ABPP Board Certified Clinical Neuropsychologist Associate Professor, Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences Director of Neuropsychology Training What

More information

Physical performance and cognition in older adults with and without dementia Blankevoort, Gerwin

Physical performance and cognition in older adults with and without dementia Blankevoort, Gerwin University of Groningen Physical performance and cognition in older adults with and without dementia Blankevoort, Gerwin IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's

More information

21/05/2018. Today s webinar will answer. Presented by: Valorie O Keefe Consultant Psychologist

21/05/2018. Today s webinar will answer. Presented by: Valorie O Keefe Consultant Psychologist Today s webinar will answer. 1. What is the RBANS, and how is the updated version different than the original version? 2. What are the neurocognitive areas assessed by the RBANS and what scores are available?

More information

Research Article Assessment of Early Cognitive Impairment in Patients with Clinically Isolated Syndromes and Multiple Sclerosis

Research Article Assessment of Early Cognitive Impairment in Patients with Clinically Isolated Syndromes and Multiple Sclerosis Behavioural Neurology, Article ID 637694, 5 pages http://dx.doi.org/1.1155/214/637694 Research Article Assessment of Early Cognitive Impairment in Patients with Clinically Isolated Syndromes and Multiple

More information

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly Hindawi Publishing Corporation Depression Research and Treatment Volume 2011, Article ID 396958, 6 pages doi:10.1155/2011/396958 Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Superinfection with drug-resistant HIV is rare and does not contribute substantially to therapy failure in a large European cohort Bartha, I.; Assel, M.; Sloot, P.M.A.;

More information

Working memory in medicated patients with Parkinson's disease: the central executive seems to work

Working memory in medicated patients with Parkinson's disease: the central executive seems to work J7ournal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1996;60:313-317 Working memory in medicated patients with Parkinson's disease: the central executive seems to work Nathalie Fournet, Olivier Moreaud,

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) An electronic nose in respiratory disease Dragonieri, S. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) An electronic nose in respiratory disease Dragonieri, S. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) An electronic nose in respiratory disease Dragonieri, S. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Dragonieri, S. (2012). An electronic nose in respiratory

More information

Dexamethasone and Long-Term Outcome in Adults with Bacterial Meningitis

Dexamethasone and Long-Term Outcome in Adults with Bacterial Meningitis Dexamethasone and Long-Term Outcome in Adults with Bacterial Meningitis Martijn Weisfelt, MD, 1 Martine Hoogman, MSc, 1 Diederik van de Beek, PhD, 1 Jan de Gans, PhD, 1 Wouter A. Dreschler, PhD, 2 and

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL DOMAIN-SPECIFIC COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT IN PATIENTS WITH COPD AND CONTROL SUBJECTS

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL DOMAIN-SPECIFIC COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT IN PATIENTS WITH COPD AND CONTROL SUBJECTS SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL DOMAIN-SPECIFIC COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT IN PATIENTS WITH COPD AND CONTROL SUBJECTS Fiona A.H.M. Cleutjens, Frits M.E. Franssen, Martijn A. Spruit, Lowie E.G.W. Vanfleteren, Candy Gijsen,

More information

APPENDIX A TASK DEVELOPMENT AND NORMATIVE DATA

APPENDIX A TASK DEVELOPMENT AND NORMATIVE DATA APPENDIX A TASK DEVELOPMENT AND NORMATIVE DATA The normative sample included 641 HIV-1 seronegative gay men drawn from the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study (MACS). Subjects received a test battery consisting

More information

UDS version 3 Summary of major changes to UDS form packets

UDS version 3 Summary of major changes to UDS form packets UDS version 3 Summary of major changes to UDS form packets from version 2 to VERSION 3 february 18 final Form A1: Subject demographics Updated question on principal referral source to add additional options

More information

DSM-5 MAJOR AND MILD NEUROCOGNITIVE DISORDERS (PAGE 602)

DSM-5 MAJOR AND MILD NEUROCOGNITIVE DISORDERS (PAGE 602) SUPPLEMENT 2 RELEVANT EXTRACTS FROM DSM-5 The following summarizes the neurocognitive disorders in DSM-5. For the complete DSM-5 see Diagnostic and Statistical Manualof Mental Disorders, 5th edn. 2013,

More information

AMORE (Ablative surgery, MOulage technique brachytherapy and REconstruction) for childhood head and neck rhabdomyosarcoma Buwalda, J.

AMORE (Ablative surgery, MOulage technique brachytherapy and REconstruction) for childhood head and neck rhabdomyosarcoma Buwalda, J. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) AMORE (Ablative surgery, MOulage technique brachytherapy and REconstruction) for childhood head and neck rhabdomyosarcoma Buwalda, J. Link to publication Citation

More information

Table 7.2B: Summary of Select Screening Tools for Assessment of Vascular Cognitive Impairment in Stroke Patients

Table 7.2B: Summary of Select Screening Tools for Assessment of Vascular Cognitive Impairment in Stroke Patients Table 7.2B: Summary of Select Screening Tools for Assessment of Vascular Cognitive Impairment in Stroke Patients Recommended First Line Screening and s Montreal Cognitive (MoCA) The MoCA is available for

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Bartels, S. A. L. (2013). Laparoscopic colorectal surgery: beyond the short-term effects

Citation for published version (APA): Bartels, S. A. L. (2013). Laparoscopic colorectal surgery: beyond the short-term effects UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Laparoscopic colorectal surgery: beyond the short-term effects Bartels, S.A.L. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Bartels, S. A. L. (2013).

More information

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Grant L. Iverson, Ph.D, Professor Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation Harvard Medical School & Red Sox

More information

Study of Verbal Working Memory in Patients with Parkinson s Disease

Study of Verbal Working Memory in Patients with Parkinson s Disease Study of Verbal Working Memory in Patients with Parkinson s Disease Gilbert, Belleville, Bherer, & Chouinard, 2005 presented by Jonathan Reinharth May 2, 2005 Parkinson s Disease Symptoms: resting tremors,

More information

Frontotemporal dementia and neuropsychology: the value of missing values

Frontotemporal dementia and neuropsychology: the value of missing values 726 Department of Neurology H2 222, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, PO Box 22700, NL-1100 DD Amsterdam, The Netherlands H M M Smeding Department of Neurology H660, University Hospital

More information

Use of the Pill Questionnaire to detect cognitive deficits and assess their impact on daily life in patients with Parkinson s disease

Use of the Pill Questionnaire to detect cognitive deficits and assess their impact on daily life in patients with Parkinson s disease Neurology Asia 2013; 18(4) : 369 375 Use of the Pill Questionnaire to detect cognitive deficits and assess their impact on daily life in patients with Parkinson s disease 1 Ji Seon Kim MD, 2 Jong-Min Kim

More information

Apathy, but Not Depression, Reflects Inefficient Cognitive Strategies in Parkinson s Disease

Apathy, but Not Depression, Reflects Inefficient Cognitive Strategies in Parkinson s Disease Apathy, but Not Depression, Reflects Inefficient Cognitive Strategies in Parkinson s Disease Sara Varanese 1,2 *, Bernardo Perfetti 2,3, Maria Felice Ghilardi 3, Alessandro Di Rocco 1 1 Division of Movement

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Wijkerslooth de Weerdesteyn, T. R. (2013). Population screening for colorectal cancer by colonoscopy

Citation for published version (APA): Wijkerslooth de Weerdesteyn, T. R. (2013). Population screening for colorectal cancer by colonoscopy UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Population screening for colorectal cancer by colonoscopy de Wijkerslooth, T.R. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Wijkerslooth de Weerdesteyn,

More information

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 917 924 Abstract Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition Alison J. Donnell a, Neil Pliskin a, James Holdnack

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Marfan syndrome: Getting to the root of the problem Franken, Romy. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Marfan syndrome: Getting to the root of the problem Franken, Romy. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Marfan syndrome: Getting to the root of the problem Franken, Romy Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Franken, R. (2016). Marfan syndrome: Getting

More information

Table 2B: Summary of Select Screening and Initial Assessment Tools for Vascular Cognitive Impairment in Stroke Patients (Updated 2014)

Table 2B: Summary of Select Screening and Initial Assessment Tools for Vascular Cognitive Impairment in Stroke Patients (Updated 2014) Table 2B: Summary of Select Screening and Initial s for Vascular Cognitive Impairment in Stroke Patients (Updated 2014) Recommended First Line Screening and s Montreal Cognitive (MoCA) The MoCA is available

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Cognitive impairment evaluated with Vascular Cognitive Impairment Harmonization Standards in a multicenter prospective stroke cohort in Korea Supplemental Methods Participants From

More information

CSF Aβ1-42 predicts cognitive impairment in de novo PD patients

CSF Aβ1-42 predicts cognitive impairment in de novo PD patients CSF Aβ1-42 predicts cognitive impairment in de novo PD patients Mark Terrelonge MPH *1, Karen Marder MD MPH 1, Daniel Weintraub MD 2, Roy Alcalay MD MS 1 1 Columbia University Department of Neurology 2

More information

Learning objectives 6/20/2018

Learning objectives 6/20/2018 Cognitive impairment of patients with chronic migraine, in a neuropsychological assessment, does not depend on the use of topiramate or comorbidities Ferreira KS, MD, PhD Professor, Neurology Clinic, Medicine

More information

The neuropsychological profiles of mild Alzheimer s disease and questionable dementia as compared to age-related cognitive decline

The neuropsychological profiles of mild Alzheimer s disease and questionable dementia as compared to age-related cognitive decline Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2003), 9, 720 732. Copyright 2003 INS. Published by Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. DOI: 10.10170S1355617703950053 The neuropsychological

More information

Moving the brain: Neuroimaging motivational changes of deep brain stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder Figee, M.

Moving the brain: Neuroimaging motivational changes of deep brain stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder Figee, M. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Moving the brain: Neuroimaging motivational changes of deep brain stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder Figee, M. Link to publication Citation for published

More information

COGNITIVE CHANGES AFTER DEEP BRAIN STIMULATION SURGERY FOR PARKINSON S DISEASE

COGNITIVE CHANGES AFTER DEEP BRAIN STIMULATION SURGERY FOR PARKINSON S DISEASE COGNITIVE CHANGES AFTER DEEP BRAIN STIMULATION SURGERY FOR PARKINSON S DISEASE By LAURA BETH ZAHODNE A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE

More information

Overview. Case #1 4/20/2012. Neuropsychological assessment of older adults: what, when and why?

Overview. Case #1 4/20/2012. Neuropsychological assessment of older adults: what, when and why? Neuropsychological assessment of older adults: what, when and why? Benjamin Mast, Ph.D. Associate Professor & Vice Chair, Psychological & Brain Sciences Associate Clinical Professor, Family & Geriatric

More information

Psychological factors that influence fall risk: implications for prevention

Psychological factors that influence fall risk: implications for prevention Psychological factors that influence fall risk: implications for prevention Kaarin J. Anstey Professor & Director, Ageing Research Unit, Centre for Mental Health Research Psychological perspective on Injury

More information

Can Tango Help Improve Quality of Life for Patients with Parkinson s Disease?

Can Tango Help Improve Quality of Life for Patients with Parkinson s Disease? Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine DigitalCommons@PCOM PCOM Physician Assistant Studies Student Scholarship Student Dissertations, Theses and Papers 2018 Can Tango Help Improve Quality of Life

More information

Use of the comprehensive geriatric assessment to improve patient-centred care in complex patient populations Parlevliet, J.L.

Use of the comprehensive geriatric assessment to improve patient-centred care in complex patient populations Parlevliet, J.L. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Use of the comprehensive geriatric assessment to improve patient-centred care in complex patient populations Parlevliet, J.L. Link to publication Citation for published

More information

Non Alzheimer Dementias

Non Alzheimer Dementias Non Alzheimer Dementias Randolph B Schiffer Department of Neuropsychiatry and Behavioral Science Texas Tech University Health Sciences Center 9/11/2007 Statement of Financial Disclosure Randolph B Schiffer,,

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Marfan syndrome: Getting to the root of the problem Franken, Romy. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Marfan syndrome: Getting to the root of the problem Franken, Romy. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Marfan syndrome: Getting to the root of the problem Franken, Romy Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Franken, R. (2016). Marfan syndrome: Getting

More information

Plenary Session 2 Psychometric Assessment. Ralph H B Benedict, PhD, ABPP-CN Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry SUNY Buffalo

Plenary Session 2 Psychometric Assessment. Ralph H B Benedict, PhD, ABPP-CN Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry SUNY Buffalo Plenary Session 2 Psychometric Assessment Ralph H B Benedict, PhD, ABPP-CN Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry SUNY Buffalo Reliability Validity Group Discrimination, Sensitivity Validity Association

More information

WHAT IS DEMENTIA? An acquired syndrome of decline in memory and other cognitive functions sufficient to affect daily life in an alert patient

WHAT IS DEMENTIA? An acquired syndrome of decline in memory and other cognitive functions sufficient to affect daily life in an alert patient DEMENTIA WHAT IS DEMENTIA? An acquired syndrome of decline in memory and other cognitive functions sufficient to affect daily life in an alert patient Progressive and disabling Not an inherent aspect of

More information

Enzyme replacement therapy in Fabry disease, towards individualized treatment Arends, M.

Enzyme replacement therapy in Fabry disease, towards individualized treatment Arends, M. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Enzyme replacement therapy in Fabry disease, towards individualized treatment Arends, M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Arends, M. (2017).

More information

The Person: Dementia Basics

The Person: Dementia Basics The Person: Dementia Basics Objectives 1. Discuss how expected age related changes in the brain might affect an individual's cognition and functioning 2. Discuss how changes in the brain due to Alzheimer

More information

Citation for published version (APA): van Munster, B. C. (2009). Pathophysiological studies in delirium : a focus on genetics

Citation for published version (APA): van Munster, B. C. (2009). Pathophysiological studies in delirium : a focus on genetics UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Pathophysiological studies in delirium : a focus on genetics van Munster, B.C. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): van Munster, B. C. (2009).

More information

THE ROLE OF ACTIVITIES OF DAILY LIVING IN THE MCI SYNDROME

THE ROLE OF ACTIVITIES OF DAILY LIVING IN THE MCI SYNDROME PERNECZKY 15/06/06 14:35 Page 1 THE ROLE OF ACTIVITIES OF DAILY LIVING IN THE MCI SYNDROME R. PERNECZKY, A. KURZ Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Technical University of Munich, Germany. Correspondence

More information

AD Prevention Trials: An Industry Perspective

AD Prevention Trials: An Industry Perspective AD Prevention Trials: An Industry Perspective Robert A. Lasser, MD, MBA Global Development Lead, Product Development Group Medical Director, Neurodegenerative Disorders F. Hoffmann - La Roche Ltd Regulatory

More information

Fatigue in early Parkinson s disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study

Fatigue in early Parkinson s disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study ORIGINAL ARTICLE Fatigue in early Parkinson s disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study S. O. Ongre a, J. P. Larsen b, O. B. Tysnes c and K. Herlofson a a Department of Neurology, Sorlandet Hospital, Arendal;

More information

Diagnostic research in perspective: examples of retrieval, synthesis and analysis Bachmann, L.M.

Diagnostic research in perspective: examples of retrieval, synthesis and analysis Bachmann, L.M. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Diagnostic research in perspective: examples of retrieval, synthesis and analysis Bachmann, L.M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Bachmann,

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Oderkerk, A. E. (1999). De preliminaire fase van het rechtsvergelijkend onderzoek Nijmegen: Ars Aequi Libri

Citation for published version (APA): Oderkerk, A. E. (1999). De preliminaire fase van het rechtsvergelijkend onderzoek Nijmegen: Ars Aequi Libri UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) De preliminaire fase van het rechtsvergelijkend onderzoek Oderkerk, A.E. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Oderkerk, A. E. (1999). De preliminaire

More information

What APS Workers Need to Know about Frontotemporal, Lewy Body and Vascular Dementias

What APS Workers Need to Know about Frontotemporal, Lewy Body and Vascular Dementias What APS Workers Need to Know about Frontotemporal, Lewy Body and Vascular Dementias Presenter: Kim Bailey, MS Gerontology, Program & Education Specialist, Alzheimer s Orange County 1 1 Facts About Our

More information

MOS SF-36 health survey (Ware &

MOS SF-36 health survey (Ware & Biomedical and Physical Functioning General Health Height Weight Waist Circumfernece % Body Fat Tanita scale BMI Self-rated Health - overall clinical protocol Self-rated Health - rel. to age clinical protocol

More information

What if it s not Alzheimer s? Update on Lewy body dementia and frontotemporal dementia

What if it s not Alzheimer s? Update on Lewy body dementia and frontotemporal dementia What if it s not Alzheimer s? Update on Lewy body dementia and frontotemporal dementia Dementia: broad term for any acquired brain condition impairing mental function such that ADLs are impaired. Includes:

More information