doi: /brain/awr216 Brain 2011: 134;

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "doi: /brain/awr216 Brain 2011: 134;"

Transcription

1 doi: /brain/awr216 Brain 2011: 134; BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Subtypes of progressive aphasia: application of the international consensus criteria and validation using b-amyloid imaging Cristian E. Leyton, 1,2 Victor L. Villemagne, 3,4,5 Sharon Savage, 1 Kerryn E. Pike, 3,4,6 Kirrie J. Ballard, 7 Olivier Piguet, 1,2 James R. Burrell, 1,2 Christopher C. Rowe 3,5 and John R. Hodges 1,2 1 Neuroscience Research Australia, Randwick, NSW, 2031, Australia 2 The University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, 2052, Australia 3 Department of Nuclear Medicine and Centre for PET, Austin Health, Heidelberg, VIC, 3084, Australia 4 The Mental Health Research Institute, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, 3052, Australia 5 Department of Medicine, Austin Health, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, 3084, Australia 6 School of Psychological Science, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Melbourne, VIC, 3086, Australia 7 Faculty of Health Science, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, 2141, Australia Correspondence to: Prof. John R. Hodges, Neuroscience Research Australia, Barker Street, Randwick, NSW, 2031, Sydney, Australia j.hodges@neura.edu.au Primary progressive aphasia comprises a heterogeneous group of neurodegenerative conditions with diverse clinical profiles and underlying pathological substrates. A major development has been the publication of the recent International Consensus Criteria for the three major s namely: semantic, non-fluent/agrammatic and logopenic. The logopenic is assumed to represent an atypical presentation of Alzheimer pathology although evidence for this is, at present, limited. The semantic and non-fluent/agrammatic s are largely associated with frontotemporal lobar degeneration with TDP-43 and tau pathology, respectively. The applicability of the International Consensus Criteria to an unselected clinical sample is unknown and no agreed clinical evaluation scale on which to derive the diagnosis exists. We assessed 47 consecutive cases of primary progressive aphasic seen over a 3-year period in a specialist centre, using a newly developed progressive aphasia language scale. A subgroup of 30 cases underwent 11 C-labelled Pittsburgh Compound B positron emission tomography imaging, a putative biomarker of Alzheimer s disease that detects b-amyloid accumulation, and they were compared with an age-matched group (n = 10) with typical, predominately amnestic Alzheimer s disease. The application of an algorithm based on four key speech and language variables (motor speech disorders, agrammatism, single-word comprehension and sentence repetition) classified 45 of 47 (96%) of patients and showed high concordance with the gold standard expert clinical diagnosis based on the International Consensus Criteria. The level of neocortical b-amyloid burden varied considerably across aphasic s. Of 13 logopenic patients, 12 (92%) had positive b-amyloid uptake. In contrast, one of nine (11%) semantic and two of eight (25%) non-fluent/agrammatic cases were positive. The distribution of b-amyloid across cortical regions of interest was identical in cases with the logopenic to that of patients with typical Alzheimer s disease although the total load was lower in the aphasic cases. Impairments of sentence repetition and sentence comprehension were positively correlated with neocortical burden of b-amyloid, whereas impaired single-word comprehension showed a negative correlation. The International Received May 30, Revised July 12, Accepted August 1, 2011 ß The Author (2011). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please journals.permissions@oup.com

2 Validation of PPA classification Brain 2011: 134; Consensus Criteria can be applied to the majority of cases with primary progressive aphasic using a simple speech and language assessment scale based upon four key variables. b-amyloid imaging confirms the higher rate of Alzheimer pathology in the logopenic and, in turn, the low rates in the other two s. The study offers insight into the biological basis of clinical manifestations of Alzheimer s disease, which appear topographically independent of b-amyloid load. Keywords: progressive aphasia; Alzheimer s disease; semantic dementia; progressive non-fluent aphasia; 11 C-labelled Pittsburgh compound B Abbreviations: ACE-R = Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination revised; PALS = progressive aphasia language scale; PiB = [ 11 C] Pittsburgh Compound B; PPA = primary progressive aphasia Introduction Relatively isolated aphasia due to neurodegeneration was reported more than a century ago (Pick et al., 1994, 1997), but interest was rekindled following the seminal report of Mesulam (1982, 2001) who coined the term primary progressive aphasia (PPA) to describe patients with relentless language impairment without generalized dementia. Following numerous case reports, it became clear that patients followed two clinical patterns: fluent and nonfluent s (Tyrrell et al., 1990; Snowden et al., 1992; Kertesz et al., 1994, 2003; Hodges and Patterson, 1996; Knibb et al., 2006). Cases with fluent, better known as semantic dementia, present a coherent syndrome with severe anomia and impaired single-word comprehension associated with gradual disintegration of knowledge about words, people and objects, but with preservation of the phonological and syntactic aspects of language (Warrington, 1975; Snowden et al., 1989; Hodges et al., 1992, 2010). Cases with non-fluent/agrammatic, for the sake of convenience hereafter called non-fluent, in contrast, have disturbances of language production (Grossman et al., 1996; Hodges and Patterson, 1996; Knibb et al., 2006; Ash et al., 2009; Wilson et al., 2010) with variable features that include oversimplification of language production and syntactic errors, effortful, halting speech, distorted articulation and prosodic changes (Grossman et al., 1996; Knibb et al., 2006; Ogar et al., 2007; Ash et al., 2009; Rohrer et al., 2010b; Wilson et al., 2010). Efforts to subdivide patients with non-fluent were unsuccessful until the identification of a third clinical, known as logopenic (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004), characterized by word-finding problems with frequent hesitations, marked anomia and difficulty repeating sentences or strings of words, but flawless comprehension of single words and, in contrast to non-fluent, preservation of spoken syntax and motor aspects of speech (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2008; Josephs et al., 2008). Each is associated with a characteristic pattern of brain atrophy that reflects the cognitive system compromised. Patients with semantic dementia show the most consistent pattern, characterized by bilateral, but typically asymmetrical, polar and inferior temporal atrophy (Hodges et al., 1992; Rosen et al., 2002; Schroeter et al., 2008; Rohrer et al., 2009b; Mion et al., 2010). In contrast, patients with non-fluent exhibit a more variable pattern with left-sided perisylvian atrophy, involving the inferior frontal gyrus, anterior portion of the insula and the middle frontal gyrus (Nestor et al., 2003; Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004; Wilson et al., 2010). Data on the pattern of atrophy in logopenic are relatively limited, but it appears to be associated with left inferior parietal and superior temporal atrophy (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2008; Rohrer et al., 2009a). The underlying pathology of PPA s is also diverse. Considered as a whole, the majority of patients with PPA have pathology within the spectrum of frontotemporal lobar degeneration that comprises forms with tau-positive inclusions (Pick s Disease, corticobasal degeneration, familial MAPt gene mutations) and tau-negative cases with deposition of TDP-43, including patients with mutation of the progranulin gene (Kertesz et al., 1994; Hodges and Patterson, 1996; Turner et al., 1996; Forman et al., 2006; Knibb et al., 2006; Cairns et al., 2007; Snowden et al., 2007; Mesulam et al., 2008; Hu et al., 2010). Importantly, however, a significant proportion of cases, up to a quarter in some series, have Alzheimer pathology at post-mortem (Knibb et al., 2006; Mesulam et al., 2008). Clinicopathological studies indicate a degree of convergence between clinical syndromes and underlying pathology. Those with semantic dementia have predominantly frontotemporal lobar degeneration-tdp-43 and rarely frontotemporal lobar degeneration with tau-positive inclusions or Alzheimer pathology (Kertesz et al., 2005; Deramecourt et al., 2010; Hodges et al., 2010). The situation with non-fluent is less clear since most clinicopathological studies pre-date the delineation of logopenic (Kertesz et al., 2005; Josephs et al., 2006; Knibb et al., 2006). Motor speech disorders have been strongly associated with frontotemporal lobar degeneration with taupositive inclusions (Josephs et al., 2006), but some patients with non-fluent have frontotemporal lobar degeneration TDP-43 (Knibb et al., 2006; Snowden et al., 2007). Limited pathological studies of logopenic patients support the concept that this represents an atypical presentation of Alzheimer s disease (Mesulam et al., 2008; Rohrer et al., 2009a). The ability to detect pathological changes in vivo represents a major advance in neurodegenerative diseases and is clearly relevant to PPA. The PET tracer [ 11 C] Pittsburgh Compound B (PiB) has high affinity for fibrilllar b-amyloid peptide, which is one of the hallmarks of Alzheimer pathology (Klunk et al., 2004). Not only is PiB-PET highly sensitive to b-amyloid deposit in vivo, but it also shows a strong anatomical correspondence with the b-amyloid burden in pathologically confirmed cases (Bacskai et al., 2007; Ikonomovic et al., 2008). Studies using PiB-PET in patients with a clinical diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia have shown that

3 3032 Brain 2011: 134; C. E. Leyton et al. between a quarter and a third had PiB-positive scans and most had a language presentation (Rabinovici et al., 2007, 2008; Engler et al., 2008). Based on convergent clinical and neuropathological data, it would be predicted that the likelihood of PiB retention would vary considerably across PPA s. To date, only one study of 15 patients with PPA (non-fluent = 6; semantic dementia = 5; logopenic = 4) undergoing a PiB-PET has been reported (Rabinovici et al., 2008). This study found six of their patients to have a PiB-positive (40%) scan, with the following clinical distribution: all four logopenic cases and one from each of the other two categories. These findings are in keeping with the assumption that logopenic represents an atypical presentation of Alzheimer s disease, but the number in each subgroup was very small and it should be noted that this study was completed before the formalization of criteria for the PPA subtypes. To encompass the rapid developments in the field, a broad ranging International Consensus Group recently published recommendations for the diagnosis and classification of PPA (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011). To harmonize terminology, semantic dementia is referred to as semantic of PPA. The guidelines define each according to the presence or absence of core language features, with recommendations for the type of assessment required to classify subjects (Table 1). The applicability of these criteria in a large sample of unselected clinic attendees with PPA is, however, yet to be determined. A key question is the validity of the separation of non-fluent from logopenic based on the assumption that patients with logopenic PPA have predominantly Alzheimer s disease as underlying pathology. The assessment of aphasia is complex and the proposed criteria take a multidimensional approach that may be difficult to apply especially by those not versed in the evaluation of patients with aphasia. An easily applicable bedside clinical scale that is capable of capturing and grading the key language features essential for the classification of PPA is clearly needed. Our primary aim was to validate the new International Consensus Group criteria for PPA using PiB-PET imaging to detect cases with putative Alzheimer pathology and, thereby, to test the hypothesis that patients with logopenic, in contrast to other subtypes, have predominantly Alzheimer pathology. A second aim was to establish the proportion of patients correctly classified by the new criteria. A third aim was to develop a robust and easily applicable assessment scale to be applied systematically to a consecutive series of PPA cases. Materials and methods Participants Consecutive cases with PPA referred to Frontier s specialist Frontotemporal Dementia Clinic at Neuroscience Research Australia in Sydney ( between January 2008 and December 2010 were recruited. Participants were regarded as having PPA if the most prominent clinical feature was slowly progressive aphasia for at least 2 years, without other significant cognitive or behavioural symptoms, and preservation of daily living activities except those attributable to the language impairment (Mesulam, 2001). We excluded: (i) participants with limited English proficiency (proficiency was defined as those who had English as a second language and had lived and worked in an English speaking country for 410 years); (ii) participants with advanced disease who were essentially mute or had Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination revised (ACE-R) (Mioshi et al., 2006) 535; (iii) participants with concomitant motoneuron disease, significant extrapyramidal features, a past history of stroke, epilepsy, alcoholism or significant traumatic brain injury; (iv) participants with abnormalities on MRI brain scan, other than atrophy; or (v) participants without an adequate digital video record. As a result, from a total of 70 cases with PPA, 47 participants were eligible for the study. Healthy control subjects (n = 52) were selected from the volunteer panel of Frontier and matched case by case according to level of Table 1 Summary of international recommendations for the diagnosis and classification of PPA Primary progressive aphasia (i) Most prominent clinical feature is a difficulty with language (word-finding deficits, paraphasias, effortful speech, grammatical and/or comprehension deficits). (ii) Aphasia should be the most prominent deficit at symptom onset and for the initial phases of the disease (other prominent symptoms, such as behavioural, memory or visuospatial impairments should not be present at the onset). (iii) No other conditions that may be better account for the language deficits are present (i.e. non-degenerative or psychiatric conditions). Semantic (i) Poor confrontation naming and impaired single-word comprehension, explained by dissolution of semantic knowledge. (ii) No motor speech disorders or agrammatism. Non-fluent (i) Either agrammatism or motor speech disorders with effortful, halting speech, inconsistent sound errors and distortions. (ii) Spared single-word comprehension and object knowledge. Logopenic (i) Impaired single-word retrieval in spontaneous speech (speech fluency interrupted by word-finding pauses) and confrontational naming; and impaired repetition of sentences and phrases. (ii) Spared single-word comprehension and absence of motor speech disorders.

4 Validation of PPA classification Brain 2011: 134; education (mean: 13.2 years) and age (mean: 68.5 years). All participants underwent a brain MRI and a routine core neuropsychological assessment. The study received approval from the South Eastern Sydney and Illawarra Area Health Service and the University of New South Wales human ethics committees. Language assessment A comprehensive assessment of speech and language skills was performed and video-recorded. The assessment comprised a semi-structured interview followed by a standardized sequence of language tasks required to classify patients into one of the three s of PPA (Table 1) (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011). The assessment began with an introductory conversation of min initiated by the interviewer who introduced a similar range of topics in each case, such as participant s past employment, family, leisure activities and recent holidays. The interviewer took a mainly passive role, encouraging the participant to talk. Video records of each conversation provided a naturalistic speech sample, which was analysed to determine two key features: motor speech disorders and agrammatism. Cardinal features Table 2 Criteria for scoring variables on the progressive aphasia language scale of motor speech disorders included presence of distortions, effortful speech or articulatory groping and loss of normal prosody. Distortions were defined as a lack of accurate articulation, yielding sounds that were not recognizable as spoken English. Changes in prosody were defined as the incorrect placement of stress and natural intonation upon syllables and words within sentences. Features regarded as agrammatism included errors in word arrangement (syntax), inflection (morphology) or omission of grammatical structures resulting in oversimplification of language (Table 2). The informal conversation was followed by several language tasks. Naming was assessed by presentation of animal models and household objects ranging in familiarity (e.g. horse, elephant, crocodile, hippopotamus ; and toothbrush, scissors, tongs, whisk ). Distortions of the target word due to articulation problems were not regarded as errors. Single-word comprehension was assessed in two ways: first, by asking the participant to point to objects (e.g. point to the crocodile ), and secondly, asking to repeat and define multisyllabic words of increasing difficulty (e.g. caterpillar, perimeter, catastrophe, chrysanthemum, etc.). Singleword repetition was evaluated in the repeat and define task described above, and sentence repetition was assessed by repeating plausible Item Assessed features Score 1 (questionable) 2 (definite) 3 (severe) Spontaneous speech Motor speech disorders Distortions, groping or laboured speech Changes in stress, intonation or prosody Level of intelligibility Rarely present Consistent or clearly present Frequently present Subtle and inconsistently present Normal output speed Almost all words are intelligible The message is understandable Consistent or clearly present Output speed is affected Some words are unintelligible The message is understandable Agrammatism Syntax and morphology Largely preserved At least one error in 5 min of spontaneous speech Grammatical complexity Tendency to use simple Consistent and clear structures oversimplification Tasks Naming Single-word repetition Single-word comprehension Sentence repetition Sentence comprehension Word retrieval when an object is presented Repetition of words Identification of words Ability to repeat sentences or strings of words Ability to follow commands of increasing complexity Occasional mistakes on unfamiliar items Occasional or subtle errors, often involving difficult words Occasional mistakes, usually on unfamiliar words (e.g. herbivorous, indigenous) Minimal or occasional mistakes Minimal or occasional mistakes Increased latency in replying, requests for repeating instructions, and self-corrections should be considered Consistent mistakes Clear and consistent errors Consistent mistakes Highly familiar items are often preserved (e.g. dog or elephant) Consistent mistakes Consistent mistakes, often involving complex instructions Almost always present Output speed is deeply reduced Many words are unintelligible. The message is hard to understand Frequent and evident errors Telegraphic language Frequent mistakes, often involving familiar items Frequent errors Frequent mistakes Unable to repeat correctly phrases of more than two words Consistent mistakes involving direct and simple instructions

5 3034 Brain 2011: 134; C. E. Leyton et al. sentences (e.g. the Chinese fan contained a rare emerald ). Finally, sentence comprehension was evaluated by asking the patient to follow instructions with different levels of complexity. Simple instructions included direct two-step commands (e.g. touch the pen and then the scissors ), whereas complex instructions included commands with reversible elements (e.g. pick up the pen after touching the scissors ). Development of the progressive aphasia language scale To develop the scale, a set of eight language features were operationally defined and graded, taking into account the core features specified in the consensus guidelines (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011). According to the magnitude and consistency of impairments, each language feature was scored from absent (0), subtle or questionable (1), mild but definitely present (2) to moderate or severe (3) (Table 2). A pilot version of the scale was then applied to a set of video recordings of patients with PPA (n = 12) who were not included in the main study, by two independent raters who had not been involved in the initial diagnosis of the cases. The total inter-rater reliability (Spearman s r) was 0.93, ranging for each item from 0.63 to The item with the lowest reliability, word-finding problems, was excluded. Based on the results of the inter-rater reliability study and a number of minor modifications to the scoring criteria, a final version of the Progressive Aphasia Language Scale (PALS) with seven items was used in this study (see instructions of the PALS in the online Supplementary Material). Pittsburgh Compound B-positron emission tomography scans Participants were invited to undergo a PET scan at the Austin Hospital, Melbourne. Of the 35 patients asked to undergo a PET scan, 30 agreed to participate. Each participant received 370 MBq 11 C-PiB intravenously over 1 min. A 30-min acquisition in 3D mode starting 40 min after injection of PiB was performed with a Phillips Allegro TM PET camera. A transmission scan was performed for attenuation correction. PET images were reconstructed using a 3D RAMLA algorithm. Images were processed using a preset template of narrow cortical regions of interest placed by an operator who was blind to the participant s clinical status (V.L.V.). PET data were not corrected for partial volume effects. Standardized uptake values for PiB were calculated for all brain regions examined, and standardized uptake value ratios were generated by dividing all regional standardized uptake values by the cerebellar cortex standardized uptake values. Neocortical b-amyloid burden was expressed as the average standardized uptake value ratios of frontal, superior parietal, lateral temporal, lateral occipital, and anterior and posterior cingulate regions. Given the bimodal distribution of PiB-standardized uptake value ratios that is observed in healthy controls, a hierarchical cluster analysis was performed on all elderly healthy-control participants at Austin Health, n = 118, age years, Mini-Mental State Examination (mean SD) that yielded a cut-off for high or low neocortical standardized uptake value ratios of 1.50, consistent with cut-off values used in previous PiB-PET studies (Rowe et al., 2010; Villemagne et al., 2011). To compare positive cases, a sample of patients with typical early-stage Alzheimer s disease, with a predominately amnestic presentation, was identified from the Austin Hospital database and matched according to sex and age (n = 10, age years, Mini-Mental State Examination ). Statistical analysis Non-parametric statistical methods were used to have a consistent approach to non-normal distributed data, such as neocortical PiBstandardized uptake value ratios. Kruskal Wallis followed by pair-wise comparisons with Mann Whitney tests were used for the analysis between PPA s. Spearman rank-order correlations (denoted as r ) were used to evaluate associations between paired numeric measures. Chi-squared tests were used to analyse categorical differences, while the Fisher s exact test was used to analyse differences in the performance of PALS items. The level of significance was adjusted for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni corrections and was set at 0.05, unless otherwise stated. Statistical analyses were undertaken using PASW 17.0 (IBM Corp.). Results Demographic characteristics and general cognition Demographic data on the 47 cases with PPA (14 semantic, 15 non-fluent and 18 logopenic clinically classified) are shown in Table 3. The overall PPA group did not differ from controls in age, education or sex distribution. Similarly, the subgroup of 30 who undertook PiB-PET scans did not differ from the other PPA cases in general demographic features, total time of disease duration or general cognition (ACE-R) (data not shown). Between PPA s, however, logopenic cases had the largest proportion of females and non-fluent cases showed the shortest disease duration. With respect to general cognition, although non-fluent cases showed better performance, all PPA s had lower ACE-R and Mini-Mental State Examination scores than the matched controls (Table 3). On the ACE-R, non-fluent cases performed better than the other PPA s on the language and memory sub-domains, whereas no intergroup differences were found in the remaining sub-domains. Notably, performance was equivalent and well preserved on the visuospatial subscale. Application of the progressive aphasia language scale The performance of clinically classified participants on the language variables assessed by the PALS is shown in Fig. 1. Motor speech disorders were severe in the majority of participants with non-fluent and largely absent in the other groups, with a significant difference between non-fluent and other groups. Although rarely severe, agrammatism was also seen in most participants with non-fluent and displayed the same pattern of intergroup differences as for motor speech disorders. All patients with semantic had moderate or severe anomia and this represented a significantly greater proportion than seen in the other two s. Although a sizeable proportion of participants with logopenic also had severe anomia, this proportion did not differ from the non-fluent group. Single-word repetition was essentially normal in semantic while most participants with

6 Validation of PPA classification Brain 2011: 134; Table 3 Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants Controls (n = 52) non-fluent and a sizeable proportion of logopenic cases produced repetition errors. The difference between non-fluent and logopenic did not reach statistical significance. Single-word comprehension was invariably poor in semantic participants with no definite impairment in the other two groups. Sentence repetition deficits were universal in non-fluent and logopenic s, showing a significant difference between these and the semantic group. Those with logopenic were severely impaired, typically omitted words and could only produce the first two or three words, while patients with non-fluent showed less marked deficits characterized by distortions and some omissions. Finally, no intergroup differences were found in the performance of sentence comprehension. Classification of primary progressive aphasia s: comparison of the clinical gold standard and the PALS PPA (n = 47) The gold standard clinical diagnosis was based on the consensus of two experienced clinicians (J.R.H. and J.R.B. or K.J.B.), following the clinical assessment and using the results of neuropsychological and language tests. This gold standard was compared with a diagnosis based purely on the results of a simple short speech and language evaluation, the PALS, with the aim of being able to replace a lengthy expert-dependent diagnosis with one based on the PALS. We developed an algorithm capable of classifying the participants using empirical data. Based on the results detailed above, three components of the PALS were excluded. Naming was excluded as all three groups contained members with moderate to severe impairment. Single-word repetition deficits, although present in all non-fluent cases, were also P-value PPA Semantic (n = 14) Non-fluent (n = 15) Logopenic (n = 18) P-value Demographic* Sex, female (%) NS 14 a b Age, years 69 (52 81) 66 (48 84) NS 64 (53 73) 67 (48 84) 67 (54 79) NS Education, years 13 (8 20) 13 (7 22) NS 12 (9 19) 12 (7 22) 13 (9 19) NS Disease duration, years 4 (0 11) 5 (2 11) c 3 (0 8) b 4 (1 11) General cognition* ACE-R (/100) 95 (88 100) 65 (41 95) (43 79) c 73 (41 95) b 63 (44 78) Attention (/18) 18 (16 18) 15 (11 18) (13 18) 16 (12 18) 15 (11 18) NS Memory (/26) 24 (20 26) 14 (1 26) (7 19) c 18 (1 26) a,b 13 (4 23) c Fluency (/14) 12 (8 14) 5 (0 12) (0 10) 5 (0 12) 6 (2 10) NS Language (/26) 25 (22 26) 16 (7 26) (7 20) a,c 20 (13 26) a,b 16 (8 21) b,c Visuospatial (/16) 16 (12 16) 14 (7 16) (9 16) 14 (7 16) 14 (10 16) NS Mini-Mental State Examination (/30) 29 (27 30) 23 (16 29) (19 29) 24 (17 29) 22 (16 26) NS *Except for Sex, all values are expressed as mean (range). a Significant difference from logopenic cases. b Significant difference from semantic cases. c Significant difference from non-fluent cases. NS = not significant. common in a large proportion of logopenic cases. Sentence comprehension performance did not vary significantly across the s. The four features retained were, therefore: motor speech disorders, agrammatism, single-word comprehension and sentence repetition. For the purposes of classification, scores were dichotomized as present (definite or severe) or absent (questionable or absent). Since several core features may be present simultaneously in one case, a hierarchical approach was adopted and a number of models contrasted. To determine the classifier level, a correlational analysis of the four items was carried out (Supplementary Table 1). Single-word comprehension was the only item that showed a significant negative correlation with the other items and was therefore the first classifier. The second classifying feature was the presence of either agrammatism or motor speech disorders that were strongly correlated (Spearman s r = 0.68; P ) but could be present independently. The third feature, sentence repetition, showed the most diverse pattern of correlations. By far the best algorithm using the four PALS features is shown in Fig. 2. This four-feature solution consistently classified a high proportion of cases and had a good concordance with the clinical diagnosis. In 44 of 47 cases (94%), there was concordance between the expert clinical diagnosis and the PALS-based algorithm. As shown in Table 4, the semantic and logopenic groups had a 100 and 94% concordance, respectively, between approaches, whereas the non-fluent group had the lowest level, with 87% concordance. The discrepant cases included one clinically diagnosed logopenic case classified as non-fluent by the PALS-based algorithm and two clinically diagnosed non-fluent cases who had mild or questionable deficits on PALS items and consequently could not be classified.

7 3036 Brain 2011: 134; C. E. Leyton et al. Figure 1 Score distribution of the PALS items across the clinically defined PPA s. NFV = non-fluent ; LV = logopenic ; SV = semantic. Pittsburgh Compound B retention status Thirty patients (nine semantic, eight non-fluent and 13 logopenic ) underwent a PiB-PET scan. Overall, half of them showed significant neocortical PiB retention (Fig. 3). Of the 15 with PiB retention, 12 presented with the logopenic of PPA. As predicted, the semantic and non-fluent groups showed the lowest proportion of positive cases: one of nine semantic (11%) and two of eight non-fluent cases (25%). In contrast, 12 of 13 logopenic (92%) cases had positive scans. Accordingly, the semantic and non-fluent groups had a neocortical PiB-standardized uptake value ratios level [mean (range)] of 1.19 ( ) and 1.28 ( ), respectively, both significantly lower than the logopenic group, 2.02 ( ), Kruskal Wallis test, P The only unexpected negative logopenic case had four neocortical right-sided regions that exceeded the PiB-standardized uptake value ratios cut-off of 1.50: orbito and ventrolateral frontal, lateral temporal and occipital (Fig. 4). On the

8 Validation of PPA classification Brain 2011: 134; Total PPA cases (n=47) Present (n=14) Single Word Comprehension Impaired Absent (n=33) Motor Speech Disorder or Agrammatism Absent (n=14) Present (n=0) other hand, the unexpected positive semantic case and the two non-fluent cases had mean neocortical PiB-standardized uptake value ratios of 1.79, 1.87 and 1.61, respectively, which were lower than the mean neocortical PiB-standardized uptake value ratios of logopenic cases (Fig. 3). With regards to the PALS-based classification, of the three discrepant cases, two underwent PiB scans. One unclassifiable case had a negative scan, but one logopenic case diagnosed by the algorithm as non-fluent had a positive scan. As a result, three of eight Absent (n=2) Motor Speech Disorder or Agrammatism Absent (n=19) Sentence repetition Impaired Present (n=17) Present (n=14) 14 SV cases 0 unclassifiable 2 unclassifiable 17 LV cases 14 NFV cases cases cases Figure 2 Algorithm for diagnosing PPA cases based on four key clinical features. Present represents a rating of definite or severe on PALS. NFV = non-fluent ; LV = logopenic ; SV = semantic. Table 4 Comparison of diagnostic approaches Clinical based Algorithm based Semantic Nonfluent Logopenic Unclassifiable Semantic Non-fluent Logopenic Total Total PALS-based non-fluent cases, 11 of 12 PALS-based logopenic cases and one of nine semantic cases had PiB-positive scans. Comparison between logopenic and typical Alzheimer s disease cases Although logopenic had significantly lower PiBstandardized uptake value ratio levels than Alzheimer s disease cases in all cortical regions, except the primary visual and the lateral occipital cortices (Table 5), no difference in the pattern of PiB retention was found between both groups (Fig. 4). PALS, global and regional neocortical PiB-standardized uptake value ratio correlations Neocortical PiB-standardized uptake value ratios did not correlate with age, disease evolution or educational level in any of the PPA groups. Mini-Mental State Examination (r = 0.43, P = 0.017), but not ACE-R, was significantly associated with neocortical PiB-standardized uptake value ratios (Supplementary Table 2). To explore potential correlations between language performances and cortical b-amyloid accumulation, PALS items were entered in a correlation matrix with both regional and global neocortical

9 3038 Brain 2011: 134; C. E. Leyton et al. PiB-standardized uptake value ratio values. Three items correlated significantly with global PiB-standardized uptake value ratios: sentence repetition and sentence comprehension showed positive correlations (r = 0.64, P and r = 0.45, P = 0.03, respectively), while single-word comprehension showed a negative correlation (r = 0.47, P = 0.009) (Supplementary Table 3). The only item that showed a regional correlation was single-word repetition that was positively correlated with the left mesial temporal region (r = 0.38, P = 0.047). No other items correlated with PiB retention region. Figure 3 Neocortical PiB-standardized uptake value ratios (SUVR) for each PPA (n = 30) and the Alzheimer s disease (AD) sample (n = 10). The red dashed line indicates the PiB positivity s threshold. Horizontal lines represent the mean SUVRs for each group. *P ; **P NFV = non-fluent ; LV = logopenic ; SV = semantic. Discussion We found that 45 of 47 consecutive cases with PPA could be classified into one of the three s recently defined by the International Consensus Group (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011) offering the first objective support for these criteria. Moreover, the algorithm-based approach in which key neurolinguistic features were systematically assessed using a simple standardized speech and language assessment, the PALS, was highly consistent with the gold standard expert clinical classification. Four variables emerged as critical for accurate classification. PiB-PET imaging validated the hypothesis that the logopenic of PPA represents an atypical presentation of Alzheimer s disease, which has clear implications for therapy. Application of the PALS and primary progressive aphasia classification Although each PPA is associated with a spectrum of impaired or preserved linguistic abilities, which have been fully documented in a large number of neuropsychological studies Figure 4 Representative examples of PiB-PET scanning. (A) PiB-PET image of a negative scan. (B) Image of the logopenic case who had a positive scan. Arrows indicate the cortical regions with a PiB-standardized uptake values ratios higher than (C and D) Images show positive scans of logopenic and typical Alzheimer s disease cases, respectively. SUVR = standardized uptake value ratios.

10 Validation of PPA classification Brain 2011: 134; Table 5 Comparison of regional PiB-standardized uptake value ratios between logopenic and Alzheimer s disease cases Cortical region Logopenic (n = 13) Alzheimer s disease (n = 10) P-value Frontal lobe Dorsolateral Right 1.92 ( ) 2.55 ( ) Left 1.87 ( ) 2.49 ( ) Ventrolateral Right 2.10 ( ) 2.74 ( ) Left 2.10 ( ) 2.70 ( ) Orbitofrontal Right 2.07 ( ) 2.75 ( ) Left 2.08 ( ) 2.69 ( ) Gyrus recto Right 2.02 ( ) 2.88 ( ) Left 2.05 ( ) 2.84 ( ) Cingulate cortex Anterior cingulate Right 2.03 ( ) 2.72 ( ) Left 2.05 ( ) 2.69 ( ) Posterior cingulate Right 2.25 ( ) 2.82 ( ) Left 2.28 ( ) 2.86 ( ) Temporal lobe Lateral temporal Right 2.11 ( ) 2.60 ( ) Left 1.98 ( ) 2.63 ( ) Mesial temporal Right 1.41 ( ) 1.79 ( ) Left 1.41 ( ) 1.81 ( ) Parietal lobe Superior parietal Right 1.95 ( ) 2.42 ( ) Left 1.87 ( ) 2.48 ( ) Occipital lobe Lateral occipital Right 1.80 ( ) 2.07 ( ) Left 1.91 ( ) 1.99 ( ) Visual cortex Right 1.81 ( ) 2.00 ( ) Left 1.86 ( ) 1.94 ( ) Neocortex Right 2.03 ( ) 2.58 ( ) Left 2.02 ( ) 2.57 ( ) Whole neocortex 2.02 ( ) 2.58 ( ) All values expressed as mean (range). (Hodges and Patterson, 1996; Knibb et al., 2006; Mesulam et al., 2009; Rohrer et al., 2010b; Sapolsky et al., 2010), we found that the distinction can be made on relatively simple grounds by considering four language variables: single-word comprehension, motor speech disorder, agrammatism and impaired sentence repetition. These findings have implications for the international consensus criteria for PPA s. Participants with semantic showed the most homogeneous pattern characterized by impaired word comprehension, but preservation of speech articulation, grammatical language production and sentence repetition. This is in contrast to non-fluent and logopenic participants who demonstrated preservation of single-word comprehension. Although the proposed criteria include non-verbal-based semantic tasks, such as object knowledge and reading/spelling tasks, all semantic participants, who had undergone a full neuropsychological assessment, could be correctly classified applying the PALS-based algorithm. Prior work has established that even patients with very early stage semantic (semantic dementia) show impairment on non-verbal-based tests of semantic knowledge (Adlam et al., 2006), but the demonstration of such impairment is not necessary for clinical classification and may be redundant in the criteria. The cases with non-fluent presented either with motor speech disorders, expressive agrammatism or both. The distinction between non-fluent and logopenic s is, however, demanding not only because of the overlapping deficits in these s, but also because the core deficits of non-fluent may be subtle and inconsistent. Motor speech disorders are caused by disturbance in the planning, programming or execution of movements for speech and assessment usually requires a complete battery of speech motor tasks to elicit characteristic disturbances in articulation and/or prosody (Duffy, 2005, pp ; Ogar et al., 2007; Rohrer et al., 2010a). Nevertheless, our simple clinical scale was largely concordant with the more thorough evaluation. The two clinically diagnosed non-fluent cases who were misclassified by the PALS had mild speech deficits apparent to an expert that the scale could not capture. Of the three PPA s, logopenic was identified most recently (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2008; Rogalski et al., 2011) but has emerged as a readily recognized syndrome if appropriate clinical assessments are performed. Such cases may be thought initially to have the non-fluent because of their frequent pauses that disrupt the flow of conversation and the generation of phonological errors, but logopenic cases lack motor speech disorders or agrammatism and, instead, show a clear reduction in sentence repetition that is often striking with omission of words (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2008). In our group, a few logopenic participants showed questionable motor speech disorders or agrammatism that can lead to confusion. For instance, articulatory errors, which represent motor planning or programming errors, can distort spatial or temporal aspects of a target phoneme to such a degree that it sounds like a different phoneme. In these cases, the articulatory errors can be mistaken for phonemic paraphasias that represent linguistically based errors in phonemic selection (Ziegler, 2008). Moreover, logopenic cases often present false starts that resemble articulatory groping (Wilson et al., 2010). Even so, using strictly the PALS, no single logopenic case had definite motor speech disorders. In contrast to semantic cases in which single-word repetition was largely preserved, all non-fluent cases and a sizeable proportion of logopenic cases had single-word repetition defects. The nature of these defects, however, differed in each case. Whereas patients with non-fluent displayed errors of articulation and syllabic fragmentation, logopenic cases showed

11 3040 Brain 2011: 134; C. E. Leyton et al. phonological errors that indicate a breakdown in phonological integration (Canter et al., 1985; Croot, 2002; Ogar et al., 2007). Impaired sentence repetition was also present in both non-fluent and logopenic cases, but in contrast to single-word repetition, it was the only item that showed definite or severe impairment in all clinically defined logopenic participants and occurred without motor speech disorders or agrammatism. In addition, rather than defining logopenic by the absence of motor speech disorders or agrammatism, impaired sentence repetition positively classified cases with logopenic. In non-fluent cases, sentence repetition errors are mainly due to articulatory defects that also affect single-word processing and are associated with atrophy in the inferior gyrus and dorsolateral regions of the left frontal lobe and the anterior insular region (Nestor et al., 2003; Josephs et al., 2006; Ogar et al., 2007; Rohrer et al., 20010a). Sentence repetition errors in logopenic reflect a breakdown in the phonological processing that also affects the repetition of single words. Yet, the gross impairment in sentence repetition is almost certainly secondary to a reduction of verbal short-term memory resources. These findings are in keeping with the proposed anatomical locus of pathology in logopenic, the left temporal parietal junction, area 21 (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2008; Mesulam et al., 2009; Rohrer et al., 2009a), a region shown to be critically involved both in phonological processing and verbal short-term memory (Hickok and Poeppel, 2007). The relative importance of phonological and verbal short-term memory deficits in logopenic requires future exploration. Anomia was present across PPA s and, therefore, was not helpful in the classification of patients, despite its inclusion in the International Consensus Criteria. The naming performance was, however, quantitatively and qualitatively different in each PPA. Those with semantic are typically extremely anomic and show a marked familiarity effect. Their errors are typically semantic coordinates, superordinate substitutions or don t know responses (Hodges et al., 1992; Rogers et al., 2004). Cases with logopenic are also markedly anomic and may produce phonological errors or don t know responses (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2008). On the other hand, those with non-fluent, despite their profound disruption of conversational speech, show the best performance and their errors mirror spontaneous speech with articulatory distortions (Rohrer et al., 2010b). The qualitative analysis of naming errors may differentiate PPA subtypes (Rohrer et al., 2008), but a fine-grained analysis is beyond the scope of routine outpatient language assessment. Finally, sentence comprehension was also disappointing in its classificatory ability. Patients with non-fluent and logopenic were equally mildly impaired but for arguably different reasons. Those with non-fluent have a true syntactic deficit, which has been demonstrated using tasks that do not put high demands on working memory load (Weintraub et al., 2009). Patients with logopenic are also impaired on clinical tests of sentence comprehension that require subjects to process complex sentences. Their deficits almost certainly reflect impairment in attentional resources and phonological short-term memory (Amici et al., 2007; Gorno-Tempini et al., 2008). Pittsburgh Compound B retention status To date, only one PiB imaging study of logopenic cases exists (Rabinovici et al., 2008). This study involved four participants with logopenic, all of whom showed abnormal PiB retention. Our series of cases represent the largest to date and confirm the strong association with PiB retention, suggesting, therefore, underlying Alzheimer pathology in logopenic. Twelve out of 13 logopenic cases were positive for PiB, with an average standardized uptake value ratio significantly higher than the other s. The only negative case had a neocortical PiB-standardized uptake value ratio of 1.41 (Fig. 2), just under the cut-off, but had several cortical regions exceeding the standardized uptake value ratio threshold for positivity. Given that neocortical PiB-standardized uptake value ratios represents overall b-amyloid deposition across the brain, these positive regions were diluted by the others. Even so, a neocortical PiB-standardized uptake value ratio of 1.41 already indicates a substantial b-amyloid deposition in the brain. This degree of PiB retention likely represents an early stage in the disease process, particularly knowing that asymptomatic elderly individuals with positive scans present high retention in these regions (Becker et al., 2011) and the b-amyloid burden increases with time, especially in the earlier stages of Alzheimer s disease (Villemagne et al., 2011). Follow-up studies are needed to confirm this hypothesis. One semantic and two non-fluent cases had unexpectedly positive PiB-PET scans. The explanation and clinical relevance of these findings are not clear although two main possibilities exist. First, positive PiB-PET scans may be a coincidental finding, taking into account that up to a third of the cognitively normal elderly population displays positive PiB-PET scans (Pike et al., 2007; Aizenstein et al., 2008; Mosconi et al., 2010; Rowe et al., 2010). PiB positivity, however, may represent a preclinical phase of Alzheimer s disease since b-amyloid deposition is associated with brain atrophy even in asymptomatic cases (Becker et al., 2011; Oh et al., 2011). In the same way, several longitudinal follow-up studies have demonstrated an increased risk of cognitive decline in PiB-positive healthy volunteers (Scheinin et al., 2009; Resnick et al., 2010; Villemagne et al., 2011). Furthermore, b-amyloid deposition increases with age paralleling the rise in prevalence of Alzheimer s disease, but with the increase in PiB occurring 15 years before Alzheimer s disease onset (Rowe et al., 2010). It is possible, therefore, that these unexpected positive cases have coincidental mild Alzheimer pathology that is not the cause of their progressive aphasia. A second, more likely, explanation is that Alzheimer s disease can manifest as a range of aphasic syndromes. Although most strongly associated with logopenic, a minority of cases with semantic have advanced Alzheimer pathology at post-mortem: in the most recent update of the Cambridge series, 3 of 24 semantic cases coming to post-mortem had Alzheimer pathology (Hodges et al., 2010). Similarly, cases with well-documented non-fluent can also have Alzheimer pathology (Greene et al., 1996; Harasty et al., 2001; Knibb et al., 2006; Mesulam et al., 2008; Hodges et al., 2010). The pattern of cortical b-amyloid deposition in patients with logopenic was comparable with typical cases with Alzheimer s disease although logopenic had a lower

12 Validation of PPA classification Brain 2011: 134; b-amyloid burden. This finding raises fundamental questions concerning the relation between b-amyloid deposition, neuronal dysfunction and clinical symptomatology. Multiple studies have shown that this relation is not direct or robust, suggesting that other, as yet unknown, factors are involved in the development of neurodegeneration (Jack et al., 2009; Scheinin et al., 2009; Becker et al., 2011; Chetelat et al., 2010). The assessment of other biomarkers in vivo, such as tau, might help elucidate this issue (Fodero-Tavoletti et al., 2011). Three PALS items correlated with the degree of PiB retention and were, therefore, predictive of b-amyloid deposition. Sentence repetition and sentence comprehension were positively correlated with the b-amyloid cortical burden. Both tasks depend heavily on a common resource: working memory. As discussed earlier, impairments in working memory represent a pivotal deficit in logopenic and have been also described in typical cases with Alzheimer s disease (Peters et al., 2009). Given the strong association between the syndrome of logopenic and PiB status, it is not surprising that these two language hallmarks were also related to PiB retention. On the other hand, single-word comprehension, which was almost exclusively present in semantic patients, drove the association in the opposite direction, highlighting the importance of semantic impairment in the classification of PPA cases. Limitations Although we have demonstrated the validity of the proposed criteria, using a biomarker, certain caveats need to be considered. The stringent selection of participants may have yielded mostly mild or moderate cases who tended to display circumscribed language impairments, making their classification easier. The hierarchical approach, together with the quantification of impairments, however, may have overcome this issue since a few contrasting features could discriminate cases with mixed characteristics. Another issue is the application of our results in routine clinical practice. Although the PALS-based algorithm was applied in a research setting, the scale was simple, with clear instructions and scoring. We demonstrated a reasonable inter-rater reliability and feel that the scale should be usable and reliable by a range of clinicians. Further work, including the provision of illustrative video clips available online, is planned. Finally, although PiB-PET scanning is a putative biomarker of Alzheimer pathology, the diagnosis ultimately requires histopathological confirmation. The inclusion of other biomarkers, such as structural imaging or CSF protein measures may further contribute to the diagnosis in vivo. Even so, multiple clinicopathological series have consistently characterized semantic as a unique clinicopathological entity (Hodges and Patterson, 2007). Our results support this fact: semantic cases showed the highest level of concordance between diagnostic approaches and, as was predicted, the lowest likelihood of a positive PiB scan. In contrast, the differentiation of logopenic from non-fluent has proven to be more elusive, in part, due to the intrinsic difficulties in assessing key features. As a result, PiB-PET scanning may be most useful in non-semantic cases to differentiate logopenic (PiB positive) from non-fluent cases (PiB negative). Conclusion The recommendations for the diagnosis and classification of PPA of the International Consensus Group are easily applicable and appear to show a high clinical pathological correlation, particularly in logopenic. Moreover, the proposed PALS-based algorithm offers a simple bedside assessment that can readily classify PPA cases based just on four speech and language variables. Our findings raise questions regarding the need to include some features, such as anomia, word repetition, sentence comprehension and reading, since patients can be reliably classified using four key features. Acknowledgements The authors thank the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study for providing PiB image data on the patients with Alzheimer s disease. Funding National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia Project Grant (# , in part); CONICYT scholarship, Chilean Government (C.E.L.); Australian Research Council Federation Fellowship (#FF to J.R.H.). Supplementary material Supplementary material is available at Brain online. References Adlam AL, Patterson K, Rogers TT, Nestor PJ, Salmond CH, Acosta- Cabronero J, et al. Semantic dementia and fluent primary progressive aphasia: two sides of the same coin? Brain 2006; 129: Aizenstein HJ, Nebes RD, Saxton JA, Price JC, Mathis CA, Tsopelas ND, et al. Frequent amyloid deposition without significant cognitive impairment among the elderly. Arch Neurol 2008; 65: Amici S, Brambati SM, Wilkins DP, Ogar J, Dronkers NL, Miller BL, et al. Anatomical correlates of sentence comprehension and verbal working memory in neurodegenerative disease. J Neurosci 2007; 27: Ash S, Moore P, Vesely L, Gunawardena D, McMillan C, Anderson C, et al. Non-Fluent speech in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. J Neurolinguistics 2009; 22: Bacskai BJ, Frosch MP, Freeman SH, Raymond SB, Augustinack JC, Johnson KA, et al. Molecular imaging with Pittsburgh Compound B confirmed at autopsy: a case report. Arch Neurol 2007; 64: Becker JA, Hedden T, Carmasin J, Maye J, Rentz DM, Putcha D, et al. Amyloid-beta associated cortical thinning in clinically normal elderly. Ann Neurol 2011; 69: Cairns NJ, Bigio EH, Mackenzie IR, Neumann M, Lee VM, Hatanpaa KJ, et al. Neuropathologic diagnostic and nosologic criteria for frontotemporal lobar degeneration: consensus of the Consortium for Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration. Acta Neuropathol 2007; 114: Canter GJ, Trost JE, Burns MS. Contrasting speech patterns in apraxia of speech and phonemic paraphasia. Brain Lang 1985; 24:

Frontotemporal Dementia: Towards better diagnosis. Frontotemporal Dementia. John Hodges, NeuRA & University of New South Wales, Sydney.

Frontotemporal Dementia: Towards better diagnosis. Frontotemporal Dementia. John Hodges, NeuRA & University of New South Wales, Sydney. I.1 I.2 II.1 II.2 II.3 II.4 II.5 II.6 III.1 III.2 III.3 III.4 III.5 III.6 III.7 III.8 III.9 III.10 III.11 III.12 IV.1 IV.2 IV.3 IV.4 IV.5 Frontotemporal Dementia: Towards better diagnosis Frontotemporal

More information

Piano playing skills in a patient with frontotemporal dementia: A longitudinal case study

Piano playing skills in a patient with frontotemporal dementia: A longitudinal case study International Symposium on Performance Science ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 The Author 2009, Published by the AEC All rights reserved Piano playing skills in a patient with frontotemporal dementia: A longitudinal

More information

FTD basics! Etienne de Villers-Sidani, MD!

FTD basics! Etienne de Villers-Sidani, MD! FTD basics! Etienne de Villers-Sidani, MD! Frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) comprises 3 clinical syndromes! Frontotemporal dementia (behavioral variant FTD)! Semantic dementia (temporal variant

More information

DISCLOSURES. Objectives. THE EPIDEMIC of 21 st Century. Clinical Assessment of Cognition: New & Emerging Tools for Diagnosing Dementia NONE TO REPORT

DISCLOSURES. Objectives. THE EPIDEMIC of 21 st Century. Clinical Assessment of Cognition: New & Emerging Tools for Diagnosing Dementia NONE TO REPORT Clinical Assessment of Cognition: New & Emerging Tools for Diagnosing Dementia DISCLOSURES NONE TO REPORT Freddi Segal Gidan, PA, PhD USC Keck School of Medicine Rancho/USC California Alzheimers Disease

More information

FRONTOTEMPORAL DEGENERATION: OVERVIEW, TRENDS AND DEVELOPMENTS

FRONTOTEMPORAL DEGENERATION: OVERVIEW, TRENDS AND DEVELOPMENTS FRONTOTEMPORAL DEGENERATION: OVERVIEW, TRENDS AND DEVELOPMENTS Norman L. Foster, M.D. Director, Center for Alzheimer s Care, Imaging and Research Chief, Division of Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurology

More information

I do not have any disclosures

I do not have any disclosures Alzheimer s Disease: Update on Research, Treatment & Care Clinicopathological Classifications of FTD and Related Disorders Keith A. Josephs, MST, MD, MS Associate Professor & Consultant of Neurology Mayo

More information

Brain Advance Access published February 25, doi: /brain/awu024 Brain 2014: Page 1 of 17 1

Brain Advance Access published February 25, doi: /brain/awu024 Brain 2014: Page 1 of 17 1 Brain Advance Access published February 25, 2014 doi:10.1093/brain/awu024 Brain 2014: Page 1 of 17 1 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Asymmetry and heterogeneity of Alzheimer s and frontotemporal pathology

More information

Form D1: Clinician Diagnosis

Form D1: Clinician Diagnosis Initial Visit Packet Form D: Clinician Diagnosis NACC Uniform Data Set (UDS) ADC name: Subject ID: Form date: / / Visit #: Examiner s initials: INSTRUCTIONS: This form is to be completed by the clinician.

More information

Perspectives on Frontotemporal Dementia and Primary Progressive Aphasia

Perspectives on Frontotemporal Dementia and Primary Progressive Aphasia Perspectives on Frontotemporal Dementia and Primary Progressive Aphasia Bradley F. Boeve, M.D. Division of Behavioral Neurology Department of Neurology Mayo Clinic Rochester, Minnesota Alzheimer s Disease

More information

Screening for Cognitive Dysfunction in Corticobasal Syndrome: Utility of Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination

Screening for Cognitive Dysfunction in Corticobasal Syndrome: Utility of Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination Original Research Article DOI: 10.1159/000327169 Accepted: March 8, 2011 Published online: April 8, 2011 Screening for Cognitive Dysfunction in Corticobasal Syndrome: Utility of Addenbrooke s Cognitive

More information

Dementia Update. October 1, 2013 Dylan Wint, M.D. Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health Las Vegas, Nevada

Dementia Update. October 1, 2013 Dylan Wint, M.D. Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health Las Vegas, Nevada Dementia Update October 1, 2013 Dylan Wint, M.D. Cleveland Clinic Lou Ruvo Center for Brain Health Las Vegas, Nevada Outline New concepts in Alzheimer disease Biomarkers and in vivo diagnosis Future trends

More information

Presenter Disclosure Information. I have no financial relationships to disclose:

Presenter Disclosure Information. I have no financial relationships to disclose: Sandra Weintraub, Ph.D. Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer s Disease Center Northwestern University, Feinberg School of Medicine Chicago, Illinois http://www.brain.northwestern.edu/dementia/ppa/index.html

More information

Dementia Past, Present and Future

Dementia Past, Present and Future Dementia Past, Present and Future Morris Freedman MD, FRCPC Division of Neurology Baycrest and University of Toronto Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest CNSF 2015 Objectives By the end of this presentation,

More information

Dementia and Aphasia-AD, FTD & aphasia

Dementia and Aphasia-AD, FTD & aphasia 特別講演 Dementia and Aphasia-AD, FTD & aphasia Shunichiro Shinagawa, Bruce L. Miller Key words : Alzheimer s disease, frontotemporal dementia, primary progressive aphasia Alzheimer s disease(ad)and frontotemporal

More information

doi: /brain/awt266 Brain 2013: 136;

doi: /brain/awt266 Brain 2013: 136; doi:10.1093/brain/awt266 Brain 2013: 136; 3474 3488 3474 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Deciphering logopenic primary progressive aphasia: a clinical, imaging and biomarker investigation Marc Teichmann,

More information

The frontotemporal dementia spectrum what the general physician needs to know Dr Jonathan Rohrer

The frontotemporal dementia spectrum what the general physician needs to know Dr Jonathan Rohrer The frontotemporal dementia spectrum what the general physician needs to know Dr Jonathan Rohrer MRC Clinician Scientist Honorary Consultant Neurologist Dementia Research Centre, UCL Institute of Neurology

More information

LANGUAGE AND PATHOLOGY IN FRONTOTEMPORAL DEGENERATION

LANGUAGE AND PATHOLOGY IN FRONTOTEMPORAL DEGENERATION LANGUAGE AND PATHOLOGY IN FRONTOTEMPORAL DEGENERATION Murray Grossman University of Pennsylvania Support from NIH (AG17586, AG15116, NS44266, NS35867, AG32953, AG38490), IARPA, ALS Association, and the

More information

Dementia Update. Daniel Drubach, M.D. Division of Behavioral Neurology Department of Neurology Mayo Clinic Rochester, Minnesota

Dementia Update. Daniel Drubach, M.D. Division of Behavioral Neurology Department of Neurology Mayo Clinic Rochester, Minnesota Dementia Update Daniel Drubach, M.D. Division of Behavioral Neurology Department of Neurology Mayo Clinic Rochester, Minnesota Nothing to disclose Dementia Progressive deterioration in mental function

More information

School of Animal Biology. Jade Cartwright

School of Animal Biology. Jade Cartwright School of Animal Biology Primary Progressive Aphasia: The Potential for Change Jade Cartwright This thesis is presented for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy of University of Western Australia September

More information

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE 5.1 GENERAL BACKGROUND Neuropsychological assessment plays a crucial role in the assessment of cognitive decline in older age. In India, there

More information

Objectives. Objectives continued: 3/24/2012. Copyright Do not distribute or replicate without permission 1

Objectives. Objectives continued: 3/24/2012. Copyright Do not distribute or replicate without permission 1 Frontotemporal Degeneration and Primary Progressive Aphasia Caregiver and Professional Education Conference Diana R. Kerwin, MD Assistant Professor of Medicine-Geriatrics Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer

More information

Clinical phenotypes in autopsy-confirmed Pick disease

Clinical phenotypes in autopsy-confirmed Pick disease Clinical phenotypes in autopsy-confirmed Pick disease O. Piguet, PhD G.M. Halliday, PhD W.G.J. Reid, PhD B. Casey, PhD R. Carman, MPhil Y. Huang, PhD J.H. Xuereb, MD J.R. Hodges, FRCP J.J. Kril, PhD Address

More information

DEMENTIA 101: WHAT IS HAPPENING IN THE BRAIN? Philip L. Rambo, PhD

DEMENTIA 101: WHAT IS HAPPENING IN THE BRAIN? Philip L. Rambo, PhD DEMENTIA 101: WHAT IS HAPPENING IN THE BRAIN? Philip L. Rambo, PhD OBJECTIVES Terminology/Dementia Basics Most Common Types Defining features Neuro-anatomical/pathological underpinnings Neuro-cognitive

More information

Research on Primary Progressive Aphasia: What It Is Teaching Us About Brain Function and Progression

Research on Primary Progressive Aphasia: What It Is Teaching Us About Brain Function and Progression Research on Primary Progressive Aphasia: What It Is Teaching Us About Brain Function and Progression Sandra Weintraub, PhD Clinical Core Leader and Professor Northwestern Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer

More information

Neuroimaging in the Differential Diagnosis of Primary Progressive Aphasia Illustrative Case Series in the Light of New Diagnostic Criteria

Neuroimaging in the Differential Diagnosis of Primary Progressive Aphasia Illustrative Case Series in the Light of New Diagnostic Criteria Signature: Pol J Radiol, 2014; 79: 251-258 DOI: 10.12659/PJR.890320 CASE REPORT Received: 2014.01.09 Accepted: 2014.03.18 Published: 2014.08.08 Authors Contribution: A Study Design B Data Collection C

More information

Ruolo dei biomarcatori come criterio di supporto nella diagnostica delle demenze ad esordio precoce

Ruolo dei biomarcatori come criterio di supporto nella diagnostica delle demenze ad esordio precoce Ruolo dei biomarcatori come criterio di supporto nella diagnostica delle demenze ad esordio precoce ALESSANDRO MARTORANA UOC NEUROLOGIA-CENTRO ALZHEIMER POLICLINICO TOR VERGATA-UNIVERSITÀ DI ROMA TOR VERGATA

More information

INTERNATIONAL RECOMMENDATIONS FOR THE DIAGNOSIS OF PRIMARY PROGRESSIVE APHASIA AND ITS VARIANTS

INTERNATIONAL RECOMMENDATIONS FOR THE DIAGNOSIS OF PRIMARY PROGRESSIVE APHASIA AND ITS VARIANTS Gorno-Tempini 1 INTERNATIONAL RECOMMENDATIONS FOR THE DIAGNOSIS OF PRIMARY PROGRESSIVE APHASIA AND ITS VARIANTS M. L. Gorno-Tempini, MD, PhD 1,2, A. E. Hillis, MD 3, S. Weintraub, PhD 4, A. Kertesz, MD

More information

Primary progressive aphasia A tale of two syndromes and the rest

Primary progressive aphasia A tale of two syndromes and the rest Primary progressive aphasia A tale of two syndromes and the rest S.A. Sajjadi, MRCP K. Patterson, FMedSci R.J. Arnold P.C. Watson, PhD P.J. Nestor, FRACP, PhD Correspondence & reprint requests to Dr. Nestor:

More information

Disorders of language and speech. Samuel Komoly MD PhD DHAS Professor and Chairman Department of Neurology

Disorders of language and speech. Samuel Komoly MD PhD DHAS Professor and Chairman Department of Neurology Disorders of language and speech Samuel Komoly MD PhD DHAS Professor and Chairman Department of Neurology http://neurology.pote.hu major categories disorders of language and speech cortical types aphasias

More information

Brain & Language 127 (2013) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Brain & Language

Brain & Language 127 (2013) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Brain & Language Brain & Language 127 (2013) 121 126 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Brain & Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&l Patterns of longitudinal brain atrophy in the logopenic

More information

Do not copy or distribute without permission. S. Weintraub, CNADC, NUFSM, 2009

Do not copy or distribute without permission. S. Weintraub, CNADC, NUFSM, 2009 Sandra Weintraub, Ph.D. Clinical Core Director, Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer s Disease Center Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine Chicago, Illinois Dementia: a condition caused by

More information

The Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle Flagship Study of Ageing an example of Australian research on Alzheimer s disease

The Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle Flagship Study of Ageing an example of Australian research on Alzheimer s disease The Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle Flagship Study of Ageing an example of Australian research on Alzheimer s disease AIBL: Two site collaborative study Study is conducted at two sites: Perth

More information

MOTOR FUNCTION AND BEHAVIOUR ACROSS THE ALS-FTD SPECTRUM

MOTOR FUNCTION AND BEHAVIOUR ACROSS THE ALS-FTD SPECTRUM MOTOR FUNCTION AND BEHAVIOUR ACROSS THE ALS-FTD SPECTRUM Dinuksha De Silva, 1, 2 Sharpley Hsieh 4, Jashelle Caga, 4 Felicity V.C. Leslie, 1,2,3 Matthew C. Kiernan, 1, 4 John R. Hodges, 1, 2, 3 Eneida Mioshi,

More information

Comments to this discussion are invited on the Alzforum Webinar page. Who Should Use the New Diagnostic Guidelines? The Debate Continues

Comments to this discussion are invited on the Alzforum Webinar page. Who Should Use the New Diagnostic Guidelines? The Debate Continues Comments to this discussion are invited on the Alzforum Webinar page. Who Should Use the New Diagnostic s? The Debate Continues Ever since new criteria came out for a research diagnosis of prodromal/preclinical

More information

Role of TDP-43 in Non-Alzheimer s and Alzheimer s Neurodegenerative Diseases

Role of TDP-43 in Non-Alzheimer s and Alzheimer s Neurodegenerative Diseases Role of TDP-43 in Non-Alzheimer s and Alzheimer s Neurodegenerative Diseases Keith A. Josephs, MD, MST, MSc Professor of Neurology 13th Annual Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) Symposium: Alzheimer and Non-Alzheimer

More information

Diagnosis before NIA AA The impact of FDG PET in. Diagnosis after NIA AA Neuropathology and PET image 2015/10/16

Diagnosis before NIA AA The impact of FDG PET in. Diagnosis after NIA AA Neuropathology and PET image 2015/10/16 The impact of FDG PET in degenerative dementia diagnosis Jung Lung, Hsu MD, Ph.D (Utrecht) Section of dementia and cognitive impairment Department of Neurology Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taipei

More information

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI)

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) October 19, 2018 Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) Yonas E. Geda, MD, MSc Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry Consultant, Departments of Psychiatry & Psychology, and Neurology Mayo Clinic College of Medicine

More information

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications. Dr. Peter J. Fiester November 14, 2012 Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications. Briefly discuss a few examples

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Devenney E, Bartley L, Hoon C, et al. Progression in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a longitudinal study. JAMA Neurol. Published online October 26, 2015. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.2061.

More information

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above.

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above. Section: Chapter 14: Multiple Choice 1. Alex the parrot: pp.529-530 shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all

More information

Objectives. RAIN Difficult Diagnosis 2014: A 75 year old woman with falls. Case History: First visit. Case History: First Visit

Objectives. RAIN Difficult Diagnosis 2014: A 75 year old woman with falls. Case History: First visit. Case History: First Visit Objectives RAIN Difficult Diagnosis 2014: A 75 year old woman with falls Alexandra Nelson MD, PhD UCSF Memory and Aging Center/Gladstone Institute of Neurological Disease Recognize important clinical features

More information

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) DSM-5 (Criteria and Major Changes for SLP-Related Conditions) Individuals meeting the criteria will be given a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder with three levels of severity based on degree of support

More information

Sandra Weintraub, Ph.D. Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer s Disease Center Northwestern University, Feinberg School of Medicine Chicago, Illinois

Sandra Weintraub, Ph.D. Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer s Disease Center Northwestern University, Feinberg School of Medicine Chicago, Illinois Sandra Weintraub, Ph.D. Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer s Disease Center Northwestern University, Feinberg School of Medicine Chicago, Illinois Presenter Disclosure Information Employee of: Author of:

More information

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Author's response to reviews Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Authors: Julia Miro (juliamirollado@gmail.com) Pablo Ripollès (pablo.ripolles.vidal@gmail.com)

More information

U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland

U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland AWARD NUMBER: W81XWH-14-1-0418 TITLE: Tau and Beta-Amyloid Deposition, Micro hemorrhage and Brain Function after Traumatic Brain Injury in War Veterans PRINCIPAL INVESTIGATOR: Christopher Rowe CONTRACTING

More information

Imaging of Alzheimer s Disease: State of the Art

Imaging of Alzheimer s Disease: State of the Art July 2015 Imaging of Alzheimer s Disease: State of the Art Neir Eshel, Harvard Medical School Year IV Outline Our patient Definition of dementia Alzheimer s disease Epidemiology Diagnosis Stages of progression

More information

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch. The Frontal Lobes Readings: KW Ch. 16 Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions E.L. Highly organized college professor Became disorganized, showed little emotion, and began to miss deadlines Scores on intelligence

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neurology. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 September 2.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neurology. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 September 2. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Neurology. 2008 May 27; 70(22): 2036 2045. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000303816.25065.bc. Longitudinal decline in autopsy-defined frontotemporal

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language

Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language Cognitive Neuroscience Cortical Hemispheres Attention Language Based on: Chapter 18 and 19, Breedlove, Watson, Rosenzweig, 6e/7e. Cerebral Cortex Brain s most complex area with billions of neurons and

More information

NEUROGENIC COMMUNICATIVE DISORDERS KAREN RIEDEL, PH.D. CCC-SLP CLINICAL ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR RUSK REHABILITATION SCHOOL OF MEDICINE, NYULMC

NEUROGENIC COMMUNICATIVE DISORDERS KAREN RIEDEL, PH.D. CCC-SLP CLINICAL ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR RUSK REHABILITATION SCHOOL OF MEDICINE, NYULMC Speech-Language Pathology Department NEUROGENIC COMMUNICATIVE DISORDERS KAREN RIEDEL, PH.D. CCC-SLP CLINICAL ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR RUSK REHABILITATION SCHOOL OF MEDICINE, NYULMC March 28, 2017 Neurogenic

More information

Clinicopathologic and genetic aspects of hippocampal sclerosis. Dennis W. Dickson, MD Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, Florida USA

Clinicopathologic and genetic aspects of hippocampal sclerosis. Dennis W. Dickson, MD Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, Florida USA Clinicopathologic and genetic aspects of hippocampal sclerosis Dennis W. Dickson, MD Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, Florida USA The hippocampus in health & disease A major structure of the medial temporal

More information

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu www.cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ksweeney/psy260.html n Learning and Memory n Human Communication n Emotion 1 What is memory? n Working Memory:

More information

Neuro degenerative PET image from FDG, amyloid to Tau

Neuro degenerative PET image from FDG, amyloid to Tau Neuro degenerative PET image from FDG, amyloid to Tau Kun Ju Lin ( ) MD, Ph.D Department of Nuclear Medicine and Molecular Imaging Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital ( ) Department of Medical Imaging

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

! slow, progressive, permanent loss of neurologic function.

! slow, progressive, permanent loss of neurologic function. UBC ! slow, progressive, permanent loss of neurologic function.! cause unknown.! sporadic, familial or inherited.! degeneration of specific brain region! clinical syndrome.! pathology: abnormal accumulation

More information

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation. REVIEW OF FRONTAL LOBE STRUCTURES Main organization of frontal cortex: 1. Motor area (precentral gyrus). 2. Premotor & supplementary motor areas (immediately anterior to motor area). Includes premotor,

More information

birds were used. Every two weeks during treatment, all thirty-two items were probed. A final probe was collected six to seven weeks post-treatment.

birds were used. Every two weeks during treatment, all thirty-two items were probed. A final probe was collected six to seven weeks post-treatment. The Complexity Account of Treatment Efficacy (CATE) predicts that selective training of complex targets will produce generalization to less complex targets that are operationally defined as possessing

More information

On the right side? A longitudinal study of left- versus right-lateralized semantic dementia

On the right side? A longitudinal study of left- versus right-lateralized semantic dementia doi:10.1093/brain/awv387 BRAIN 2016: 139; 986 998 986 On the right side? A longitudinal study of left- versus right-lateralized semantic dementia Fiona Kumfor, 1,2,3 Ramon Landin-Romero, 1,3 Emma Devenney,

More information

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1 Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1 Please use the following citation when referencing this work: McGill, R. J. (2013). Test review: Children s Psychological Processing Scale (CPPS). Journal of Psychoeducational

More information

Round table: Moderator; Fereshteh Sedaghat, MD, PhD Brain Mapping in Dementias and Non-invasive Neurostimulation

Round table: Moderator; Fereshteh Sedaghat, MD, PhD Brain Mapping in Dementias and Non-invasive Neurostimulation Round table: Moderator; Fereshteh Sedaghat, MD, PhD Brain Mapping in Dementias and Non-invasive Neurostimulation 1. Reflection of Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and Dementias by Molecular Imaging, PET

More information

Assessing Functional Neural Connectivity as an Indicator of Cognitive Performance *

Assessing Functional Neural Connectivity as an Indicator of Cognitive Performance * Assessing Functional Neural Connectivity as an Indicator of Cognitive Performance * Brian S. Helfer 1, James R. Williamson 1, Benjamin A. Miller 1, Joseph Perricone 1, Thomas F. Quatieri 1 MIT Lincoln

More information

Regulatory Challenges across Dementia Subtypes European View

Regulatory Challenges across Dementia Subtypes European View Regulatory Challenges across Dementia Subtypes European View Population definition including Early disease at risk Endpoints in POC studies Endpoints in pivotal trials 1 Disclaimer No CoI The opinions

More information

Cognition and Anatomy in Three Variants of Primary Progressive Aphasia

Cognition and Anatomy in Three Variants of Primary Progressive Aphasia Cognition and Anatomy in Three Variants of Primary Progressive Aphasia Maria Luisa Gorno-Tempini, MD, PhD, 1 Nina F. Dronkers, PhD, 2,3 Katherine P. Rankin, PhD, 1 Jennifer M. Ogar, MS, 2 La Phengrasamy,

More information

Yin-Hui Siow MD, FRCPC Director of Nuclear Medicine Southlake Regional Health Centre

Yin-Hui Siow MD, FRCPC Director of Nuclear Medicine Southlake Regional Health Centre Yin-Hui Siow MD, FRCPC Director of Nuclear Medicine Southlake Regional Health Centre Today Introduction to CT Introduction to MRI Introduction to nuclear medicine Imaging the dementias The Brain ~ 1.5

More information

Alzheimer s disease dementia: a neuropsychological approach

Alzheimer s disease dementia: a neuropsychological approach Alzheimer s disease dementia: a neuropsychological approach Dr. Roberta Biundo, PhD Neuropsychology Coordinator at Parkinson s disease and movement disorders unit of San Camillo rehabilitation hospital

More information

Biomarkers: Translating Research into Clinical Practice

Biomarkers: Translating Research into Clinical Practice Biomarkers: Translating Research into Clinical Practice AFTD Education Conference San Diego, April 2015 Nadine Tatton, PhD Scientific Director, AFTD HelpLine 866-5507-7222 u info@theaftd.org u www.theaftd.org

More information

Memory impairment at initial clinical presentation in posterior cortical atrophy

Memory impairment at initial clinical presentation in posterior cortical atrophy Memory impairment at initial clinical presentation in posterior cortical atrophy Samrah Ahmed a, Ian Baker b, Masud Husain a,d, Sian Thompson c, Christopher Kipps e, Michael Hornberger f, John R. Hodges

More information

Clinicopathological and imaging correlates of progressive aphasia and apraxia of speech

Clinicopathological and imaging correlates of progressive aphasia and apraxia of speech doi:10.1093/brain/awl078 Brain (2006), 129, 1385 1398 Clinicopathological and imaging correlates of progressive aphasia and apraxia of speech Keith A. Josephs, 1,2 Joseph R. Duffy, 3 Edyth A. Strand, 3

More information

DSM-5 MAJOR AND MILD NEUROCOGNITIVE DISORDERS (PAGE 602)

DSM-5 MAJOR AND MILD NEUROCOGNITIVE DISORDERS (PAGE 602) SUPPLEMENT 2 RELEVANT EXTRACTS FROM DSM-5 The following summarizes the neurocognitive disorders in DSM-5. For the complete DSM-5 see Diagnostic and Statistical Manualof Mental Disorders, 5th edn. 2013,

More information

Aging often includes changes to vision, hearing, taste, smell, skin, hair, weight & changes to Brain:

Aging often includes changes to vision, hearing, taste, smell, skin, hair, weight & changes to Brain: Aging & Cognition Ladan Ghazi Saidi, Ph.D., Assistant Professor, Department of Communication Disorders, COE, University of Nebraska at Kearney 2018 Nebraska Speech-Language-Hearing Association Fall Convention

More information

The added value of the IWG-2 diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer s disease

The added value of the IWG-2 diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer s disease The added value of the IWG-2 diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer s disease Miami, January 2016 Bruno Dubois Head of the Dementia Research Center (IMMA) Director of INSERM Research Unit (ICM) Salpêtrière

More information

Neuroimaging methods vs. lesion studies FOCUSING ON LANGUAGE

Neuroimaging methods vs. lesion studies FOCUSING ON LANGUAGE Neuroimaging methods vs. lesion studies FOCUSING ON LANGUAGE Pioneers in lesion studies Their postmortem examination provided the basis for the linkage of the left hemisphere with language C. Wernicke

More information

Development of methodology for conducting clinical trials in frontotemporal lobar degeneration

Development of methodology for conducting clinical trials in frontotemporal lobar degeneration doi:10.1093/brain/awn234 Brain (2008), 131,2957^2968 Development of methodology for conducting clinical trials in frontotemporal lobar degeneration David S. Knopman, 1 Joel H. Kramer, 2 Bradley F. Boeve,

More information

Moving Targets: An Update on Diagnosing Dementia in the Clinic

Moving Targets: An Update on Diagnosing Dementia in the Clinic Moving Targets: An Update on Diagnosing Dementia in the Clinic Eric McDade DO Department of Neurology School of Medicine Alzheimer Disease Research Center Disclosures No relevant financial disclosures

More information

HHS Public Access Author manuscript JAMA Neurol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 April 30.

HHS Public Access Author manuscript JAMA Neurol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 April 30. HHS Public Access Author manuscript Published in final edited form as: JAMA Neurol. 2018 March 01; 75(3): 342 352. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.4309. Rates of Amyloid Imaging Positivity in Patients With

More information

Confronting the Clinical Challenges of Frontotemporal Dementia

Confronting the Clinical Challenges of Frontotemporal Dementia Confronting the Clinical Challenges of Frontotemporal Dementia A look at FTD s symptoms, pathophysiology, subtypes, as well as the latest from imaging studies. By Zac Haughn, Senior Associate Editor Ask

More information

Pacific Assessment of Confabulation. Confabulation has been defined as unintentional verbal distortions or misinterpretations

Pacific Assessment of Confabulation. Confabulation has been defined as unintentional verbal distortions or misinterpretations Abstract This paper presents data on the Pacific Assessment of Confabulation (PAC), which was designed to accomplish two goals. First, the PAC is intended to quantify confabulation. Confabulation has been

More information

Can group singing help long term aphasia?

Can group singing help long term aphasia? PRIORITY BRIEFING The purpose of this briefing paper is to aid Stakeholders in prioritising topics to be taken further by PenCLAHRC as the basis for a specific evaluation or implementation research project.

More information

Neural basis of conceptual knowledge: insights from noninvasive

Neural basis of conceptual knowledge: insights from noninvasive Neural basis of conceptual knowledge: insights from noninvasive brain stimulation JSPS Fellowship 2013 Bridge Fellowship 2015 Gorana Pobric School of Psychological Sciences University of Manchester Neuroscience

More information

Psy /16 Human Communication. By Joseline

Psy /16 Human Communication. By Joseline Psy-302 11/16 Human Communication By Joseline Lateralization Left Hemisphere dominance in speech production in 95% of right handed and 70% of left handed people Left -> Timing, Sequence of events Right

More information

En Attendant Centiloid

En Attendant Centiloid Edith Cowan University Research Online ECU Publications Post 2013 2014 En Attendant Centiloid Victor L. Villemagne Vincent Doré Paul Yates Belinda Brown Edith Cowan University, belinda.brown@ecu.edu.au

More information

Cerebral Cortex Structure, Function, Dysfunction Reading Ch 10 Waxman Dental Neuroanatomy Lecture. Suzanne Stensaas, Ph.D.

Cerebral Cortex Structure, Function, Dysfunction Reading Ch 10 Waxman Dental Neuroanatomy Lecture. Suzanne Stensaas, Ph.D. Cerebral Cortex Structure, Function, Dysfunction Reading Ch 10 Waxman Dental Neuroanatomy Lecture Suzanne Stensaas, Ph.D. March 7, 2012 Anatomy Review Lobes and layers Brodmann s areas Vascular Supply

More information

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION INTRODUCTION Research on the Deaf Brain is beginning to provide a new evidence base for policy and practice in relation to intervention with deaf children. This talk outlines the multi-channel nature of

More information

EARLY ONSET FRONTOTERMPORAL DEMENTIA AND ALZHEIMERS DISEASE: DIAGNOSIS, TREATMENT AND CARE

EARLY ONSET FRONTOTERMPORAL DEMENTIA AND ALZHEIMERS DISEASE: DIAGNOSIS, TREATMENT AND CARE EARLY ONSET FRONTOTERMPORAL DEMENTIA AND ALZHEIMERS DISEASE: DIAGNOSIS, TREATMENT AND CARE John Rudge, BA Hons This thesis is presented as partial requirement for the degree of Doctor of Psychology at

More information

Update on functional brain imaging in Movement Disorders

Update on functional brain imaging in Movement Disorders Update on functional brain imaging in Movement Disorders Mario Masellis, MSc, MD, FRCPC, PhD Assistant Professor & Clinician-Scientist Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre University of Toronto 53 rd CNSF

More information

Dementia. Stephen S. Flitman, MD Medical Director 21st Century Neurology

Dementia. Stephen S. Flitman, MD Medical Director 21st Century Neurology Dementia Stephen S. Flitman, MD Medical Director 21st Century Neurology www.neurozone.org Dementia is a syndrome Progressive memory loss, plus Progressive loss of one or more cognitive functions: Language

More information

MILD HYPERTHYROIDISM CAUSES A PSEUDO-ALZHEIMER S PATTERN OF CEREBRAL METABOLISM WHICH IS REVERSIBLE WITH CEREBRAL PERFUSION STIMULANTS

MILD HYPERTHYROIDISM CAUSES A PSEUDO-ALZHEIMER S PATTERN OF CEREBRAL METABOLISM WHICH IS REVERSIBLE WITH CEREBRAL PERFUSION STIMULANTS MILD HYPERTHYROIDISM CAUSES A PSEUDO-ALZHEIMER S PATTERN OF CEREBRAL METABOLISM WHICH IS REVERSIBLE WITH CEREBRAL PERFUSION STIMULANTS Harold Thomas Pretorius, MD, PhD, and Nichole Richards Abstract Objective:

More information

Supplementary Online Content 2

Supplementary Online Content 2 Supplementary Online Content 2 Bieleninik Ł, Geretsegger M, Mössler K, et al; TIME-A Study Team. Effects of improvisational music therapy vs enhanced standard care on symptom severity among children with

More information

Dr Fiona Kumfor University of Sydney

Dr Fiona Kumfor University of Sydney Social Cognition in Dementia: Informing Diagnosis, Prognosis and Management Dr Fiona Kumfor University of Sydney CHANGES IN DEMENTIA Memory Social cognition Language ELEMENTS OF SOCIAL COGNITION Face processing

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information

Frontotemporal dementia:

Frontotemporal dementia: Frontotemporal dementia: Where we ve been What s on the horizon Howard Rosen, M.D. UCSF Department of Neurology Memory and Aging Center www.memory.ucsf.edu None Disclosures Overview FTD, What is it? Origins

More information

Neurodegenerative diseases that degrade regions of the brain will eventually become

Neurodegenerative diseases that degrade regions of the brain will eventually become Maren Johnson CD 730 Research Paper What is the underlying neurological explanation for overeating in Frontotemporal Dementia? Does the change in overeating affect swallowing? Neurodegenerative diseases

More information

21/05/2018. Today s webinar will answer. Presented by: Valorie O Keefe Consultant Psychologist

21/05/2018. Today s webinar will answer. Presented by: Valorie O Keefe Consultant Psychologist Today s webinar will answer. 1. What is the RBANS, and how is the updated version different than the original version? 2. What are the neurocognitive areas assessed by the RBANS and what scores are available?

More information

Differential diagnosis of Frontotemporal Dementia FTLD using visual rating scales

Differential diagnosis of Frontotemporal Dementia FTLD using visual rating scales Differential diagnosis of Frontotemporal Dementia FTLD using visual rating scales Poster No.: C-0491 Congress: ECR 2016 Type: Scientific Exhibit Authors: S. Manouvelou 1, G. ANYFANTAKIS 2, V. Koutoulidis

More information

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni VISUAL AGNOSIA -damage to the extrastriate visual regions (occipital, parietal and temporal lobes) disrupts recognition of complex visual stimuli

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Semin Neurol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 14.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Semin Neurol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 14. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Semin Neurol. 2013 September ; 33(4): 386 416. doi:10.1055/s-0033-1359312. Neuroimaging Biomarkers of Neurodegenerative Diseases and

More information

Differential Longitudinal Decline on the Mini-Mental State Examination in Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration and Alzheimer Disease

Differential Longitudinal Decline on the Mini-Mental State Examination in Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration and Alzheimer Disease ORIGINAL ARTICLE Differential Longitudinal Decline on the Mini-Mental State Examination in Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration and Alzheimer Disease Kay See Tan, MS,* David J. Libon, PhD,w Katya Rascovsky,

More information

Prof Tim Anderson. Neurologist University of Otago Christchurch

Prof Tim Anderson. Neurologist University of Otago Christchurch Prof Tim Anderson Neurologist University of Otago Christchurch Tim Anderson Christchurch Insidious cognitive loss From subjective memory complaints (SMC) to dementia Case 1. AR. 64 yrs Male GP referral

More information

Clinical Genetics & Dementia

Clinical Genetics & Dementia Clinical Genetics & Dementia Dr Nayana Lahiri Consultant in Clinical Genetics & Honorary Senior Lecturer Nayana.lahiri@nhs.net Aims of the Session To appreciate the potential utility of family history

More information