Handbook Of Relationship Initiation Susan Sprecher, Amy Wenzel, John Harvey

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Handbook Of Relationship Initiation Susan Sprecher, Amy Wenzel, John Harvey"

Transcription

1 This article was downloaded by: On: 16 Nov 2018 Access details: subscription number Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: Registered office: 5 Howick Place, London SW1P 1WG, UK Handbook Of Relationship Initiation Susan Sprecher, Amy Wenzel, John Harvey An Evolutionary Perspective on Mate Choice and Relationship Initiation Publication details David P. Schmitt Published online on: 01 Nov 1997 How to cite :- David P. Schmitt. 01 Nov 1997, An Evolutionary Perspective on Mate Choice and Relationship Initiation from: Handbook Of Relationship, Initiation Routledge Accessed on: 16 Nov PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR DOCUMENT Full terms and conditions of use: This Document PDF may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproductions, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The publisher shall not be liable for an loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

2 3 An Evolutionary Perspective on Mate Choice and Relationship Initiation From an evolutionary perspective, animal mate choice and relationship initiation depend in large part on the natural mating system of a species. Mating systems can vary widely both within and across species, and differences in mating systems fundamentally influence the degree of sexual differentiation and population variability in mate choice and courtship-related behavior (Shuster & Wade, 2003). In humans, there are several indications that we have a monogamous mating system. For example, humans are highly altricial we have prolonged childhoods and rely heavily on extended families throughout our life spans (Alexander & Noonan, 1979). We also appear designed to form romantic pairbonds, having a dedicated neurochemistry of attachment associated with monogamy across mammalian species (Fisher, 1998; Young, 2003). This evidence would suggest humans are designed to choose romantic partners who possess qualities advantageous to a monogamous mating system (e.g., fidelity), and according to sexual selection theory (Darwin, 1871), men and women who displayed cues to qualities such as fidelity would be especially effective at initiating and maintaining romantic relationships. At the same time, however, humans appear to possess evolved design features associated with multimale or multifemale, or promiscuous, mating. For example, humans may possess psychological and physiological adaptations for sperm competition (Baker & Bellis, 1995; Shackelford & LeBlanc, 2001), such as women s adaptive timing of extrapair copulations (i.e., infidelities; Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998; Haselton & Miller, 2006), men s specialized expressions of sexual jealousy (Buss, 2000; Schützwohl, 2006), and the physical structure of the human penis serving as a semen displacement device (Gallup et al., 2003). Among men, casual sex with multiple partners is often viewed as desirable (Oliver & Hyde, 1993; Symons & Ellis, 1989), with most men agreeing to have sex with complete strangers when asked in field experiments (Clark & Hatfield, 1989). Patterns of premarital sex, extramarital sex, and mate poaching by both men and women (i.e., adaptive patterns suggesting these are evolved sexual strategies) have been documented across cultures (Broude & Greene, 1976; Schmitt, Alcalay, Allik, et al., 2004). There is also evidence that humans are designed, at least in part, for polygynous mating. For example, men and women have sexually dimorphic life history traits such as men s tendencies to be more physically aggressive, to die much earlier, and to physically mature much later than women across all known cultures (Archer & Lloyd, 2002; Kaplan & Gangestad, 2005). Such sex differences are usually not seen among truly monogamous species, especially primates (Alexander, Hoogland, Howard, Noonan, & Sherman, 1979). Moreover, across foraging cultures the predominantly polygynous cultures in which humans have spent most of our evolutionary history (Brown, 1991; Frayser, 1985; Pasternak, Ember, & Ember, 1997) there are ethnographically pervasive links among men s status, polygynous marriage, and reproductive success (Low, 2000; 55

3 56 Turke & Betzig, 1985). In contrast, very few cultures (less than 1%) have polyandrous marriage systems (Broude & Greene, 1976). EVOLUTIONARY THEORIES OF MATE CHOICE AND RELATIONSHIP INITIATION Evolutionary psychologists tend to reconcile these seemingly contradictory findings by acknowledging that humans, like many other species, are probably designed and adapted for more than one mating strategy (Barash & Lipton, 2001; Mealey, 2000). Specifically, most evolutionary psychologists view humans as coming equipped with specialized mate choice adaptations for both long-term mating (i.e., marriage and extended pairbonding) and short-term mating (i.e., promiscuity and infidelity; see Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Kenrick, Sadalla, Groth, & Trost, 1990). Not all people try to initiate both types of mating relationships at all times. Instead, humans possess adaptive desires, preferences, and behavioral tactics that are differentially activated depending on whether a long-term or short-term mating strategy is actively being pursued at the time (Gangestad & Simpson, 2000; Schmitt, 2005a; Schmitt et al., 2003; Simpson, Wilson, & Winterheld, 2004). Most evolutionary theories of human mating argue that such a flexible mating design composed of both long-term monogamous adaptations and short-term promiscuous adaptations would have provided important reproductive benefits to humans in our ancestral past, allowing individuals to functionally respond to a wide range of familial, cultural, and ecological contexts (Belsky, 1999; Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Lancaster, 1994; Pedersen, 1991). Evolutionary theories further acknowledge that humans can benefit from shifting between long-term and short-term mating strategies during their life span, when in different stages of romantic relationships, and across the ovulatory cycle (Gangestad, 2001; Klusmann, 2002; Schmitt et al., 2002). Thus, humans have evolved the capacity to initiate a mix of mating relationship types both long-term and short-term depending on fitness-related circumstances. Most evolutionary psychology approaches further postulate that men and women possess design features that cause sex differences within long-term and short-term mating contexts. For example, when men seek short-term mates they appear motivated by adaptive desires for sexual variety desires that lead them to functionally pursue numerous mating partners and to consent to sex relatively quickly compared to women (Clark & Hatfield, 1989; Okami & Shackelford, 2001; Schmitt et al., 2003; Symons & Ellis, 1989). Women s short-term mating motivations appear not to be rooted in the desire for numerous sexual partners and seem focused, instead, on other factors such as obtaining select men who display dominance, intelligence, or creativity (i.e., show high genetic quality; see Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997; Penton-Voak et al., 2003; Regan, Levin, Sprecher, Christopher, & Cate, 2000). As a consequence, evolutionary approaches predict that men s and women s mate choices and relationship initiation tactics will differ in important ways, especially within the context of short-term mating. Most evolutionary theories of human mate choice are based on the assumption that the sexes will differ in some ways, an assumption that can be traced to the logic of parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972). Parental Investment Theory According to parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972), the relative proportion of parental investment the time and energy devoted to the care of individual offspring (at the expense of other offspring) varies across the males and females of different species. In some species, males provide more parental investment than females (e.g., the Mormon cricket). In other species, females possess the heavy-investing burdens (e.g., most mammals; Clutton-Brock, 1991). Sex differences in parental investment burdens are systematically linked to processes of sexual selection (Darwin, 1871) in ways that influence mate choice and relationship initiation. The sex that invests less in offspring

4 AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE ON MATE CHOICE AND RELATIONSHIP INITIATION 57 is intrasexually more competitive, especially over gaining reproductive access to members of the opposite sex, in part because the opposite sex is reluctant to make bad decisions in committing its typically heavier investment. This normally results in the lesser investing sex being reliably more aggressive with his or her own sex, and tending to die earlier, to mature later, and generally to compete for mates with more vigor, than does the heavier investing sex (Alcock, 2001). Furthermore, the lesser investing sex of a species is intersexually less discriminating in mate choice than the heavier investing sex. The lesser investing sex is willing to mate more quickly and at lower cost, and will initiate relationships with more partners than the heavier investing parent (Bateson, 1983). Again, this is largely because members of the heavier investing sex face higher reproductive costs associated with poor mating decisions and also have fewer mating decisions with which to gamble over their reproductive life spans. Much of the evidence in favor of parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972) has come from species where females happen to be the heavy-investing sex (see Clutton-Brock, 1991). In such species, parental investment theory leads to the prediction that sexual selection has been more potent among males. Upon empirical examination, males of these species tend to display more competitiveness with each other over sexual access to heavier investing females, and to exhibit more intrasexual competition through greater aggressiveness, riskier life history strategies, and earlier death than females (Archer & Lloyd, 2002; Trivers, 1985). Lesser investing males are also less discriminate through intersexual mate choice, often seeking multiple partners and requiring less time before initiating sex than females do (see Geary, 1998). Perhaps the most compelling support for parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972), however, has come from sex-role-reversed species. In species where males are the heavy-investing parent, the processes of sexual selection are thought to have been more potent among females. Females of these species vie more ferociously for sexual access to heavy-investing males and require little from males before consenting to sex. Evidence of this form of sexual differentiation has been documented among such sex-role-reversed species as the red-necked phalarope, the Mormon cricket, katydids, dance flies, water bugs, seahorses, and a variety of fish species (Alcock, 2001). Parental investment theory, therefore, is not a theory about males always having more interest in indiscriminate sex than females. Instead, it is a theory about differences in parental investment obligations systematically relating to sex differences in mate choice and relationship initiation. Among humans, many men invest heavily in their children, teaching them social skills, emotionally nurturing them, and investing both resources and prestige in them. Nevertheless, men incur much lower levels of obligatory or minimum parental investment in offspring than women do (Symons, 1979). Women are obligated, for example, to incur the costs of internal fertilization, placentation, and gestation in order to reproduce. The minimum physiological obligations of men are considerably less requiring only the contribution of sperm. Furthermore, all female mammals, including ancestral women, carried the obligatory investments associated with lactation. Lactation can last several years in human foraging environments (Kelly, 1995), years during which it is harder for women than men to reproduce and invest in additional offspring (Blurton Jones, 1986). Finally, across all known cultures human males typically invest less in active parenting efforts than females (Low, 1989; Munroe & Munroe, 1997). This human asymmetry in parental investment should affect mate choice and relationship initiation, with the lesser investing sex (i.e., men) displaying greater intrasexual competitiveness and lower intersexual choosiness in mate preferences. Numerous studies have shown that men exhibit greater physical size and competitive aggression (Archer & Lloyd, 2002), riskier life history strategies (Daly & Wilson, 1988), relatively delayed maturation (Geary, 1998), and earlier death than women do across cultures (Alexander & Noonan, 1979). In addition, men s mate preferences are, as predicted, almost always less choosy or discriminating than women s, especially in the context of short-term mating (Kenrick et al., 1990; Regan et al., 2000). Because men are the lesser investing sex of our species, they also should be more inclined toward initiating low-cost, short-term mating than women. Human sex differences in the desire for short-term sex have been observed in studies of sociosexuality (Jones, 1998; Schmitt, 2005a; Simpson

5 58 & Gangestad, 1991), motivations for and prevalence of extramarital mating (Seal, Agostinelli, & Hannett, 1994; Wiederman, 1997), quality and quantity of sexual fantasies (Ellis & Symons, 1990), quality and quantity of pornography consumption (Malamuth, 1996), motivations for and use of prostitution (McGuire & Gruter, 2003), willingness to have sex without commitment (Townsend, 1995), willingness to have sex with strangers (Clark, 1990; Clark & Hatfield, 1989), and the fundamental differences between the short-term mating psychology of gay males and lesbians (Bailey, Gaulin, Agyei, & Gladue, 1994). Clearly, sex differences in parental investment obligations have an influence on men s and women s fundamental mate choices and relationship initiation strategies. Sexual Strategies Theory Buss and Schmitt (1993) expanded on parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972) by proposing sexual strategies theory (SST). According to SST, men and women have evolved a pluralistic repertoire of mating strategies. One strategy within this repertoire is long-term mating. Long-term mating is usually marked by extended courtship, heavy investment, pairbonding, the emotion of love, and the dedication of resources over a long temporal span to the mating relationship and any offspring that ensue. Another strategy within the human mating repertoire is short-term mating, defined as a relatively fleeting sexual encounter such as a brief affair, a hookup, or a one-night stand. Which sexual strategy or mix of strategies an individual pursues is predicted to be contingent on factors such as opportunity, personal mate value, sex ratio in the relevant mating pool, parental influences, regnant cultural norms, and other features of social and personal contexts (see also Gangestad & Simpson, 2000; Schmitt, 2005a, 2005b). EVOLUTION OF SEX DIFFERENCES IN MATE CHOICE AND RELATIONSHIP INITIATION Sex Differences in Long-Term Mating Although SST views both sexes as having long-term and short-term mating strategies within their repertoire, men and women are predicted to differ psychologically in what they desire (i.e., mate choice) and in how they tactically pursue (i.e., initiate) romantic relationships. In long-term mate choice, the sexes are predicted to differ in several respects. Men are hypothesized to possess adaptations that lead them to place a greater mate choice premium during long-term mating on signals of fertility and reproductive value, such as a woman s youth and physical appearance (Buss, 1989; Jones, 1995; Kenrick & Keefe, 1992; Singh, 1993; Symons, 1979). Men also prefer long-term mates who are sexually faithful and are capable of good parenting (see Table 3.1). Women, in contrast, are hypothesized to place a greater premium during long-term mating on a man s status, resources, ambition, and maturity (cues relevant to his ability for long-term provisioning), as well as his kindness, generosity, and emotional openness (cues to his willingness to provide for women and their children) (Buunk, Dijkstra, Kenrick, & Warntjes, 2001; Cashdan, 1993; Ellis, 1992; Feingold, 1992; Townsend & Wasserman, 1998). Conversely, men who display cues to long-term provisioning, and women who display youthfulness, tend to be the ones who are most effective at initiating, enhancing, and preserving monogamous mating relationships (Buss, 1988; Hirsch & Paul, 1996; Landolt, Lalumiere, & Quinsey, 1995; Schmitt, 2002; Tooke & Camire, 1991; Walters & Crawford, 1994). From an evolutionary perspective, the differing qualities that men and women preferentially respond to are thought to help solve the adaptive problems that men and women had to overcome throughout human evolutionary history (Schmitt & Buss, 1996). Of course, in our ancestral past men and women also faced similar problems of mate choice, leading to little or no sex differences in some domains (see Buss & Schmitt, 1993). Numerous survey and meta-analytic studies have confirmed many of the major tenets of SST, including the fact that men and women seeking long-term mates desire different attributes in potential

6 AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE ON MATE CHOICE AND RELATIONSHIP INITIATION 59 partners (e.g., Cunningham, Roberts, Barbee, Druen, & Wu, 1995; Graziano, Jensen-Campbell, Todd, & Finch, 1997; Jensen-Campbell, Graziano, & West, 1995; Kruger, Fisher, & Jobling, 2003; Li, Bailey, Kenrick, & Linsenmeier, 2002; Regan, 1998a, 1998b; Regan & Berscheid, 1997; Urbaniak & Kilmann, 2003). Several investigators have replicated or confirmed SST-related findings using nationally representative, cross-cultural, or multicultural samples (Feingold, 1992; Knodel, Low, Saengtienchai, & Lucas, 1997; Schmitt et al., 2003; Sprecher, Sullivan, & Hatfield, 1994; Walter, 1997). For example, in a recent Internet study of 119,733 men and 98,462 women across 53 nations, Lippa (2007) replicated the classic evolutionary finding of men s greater desires, relative to women, for long-term mates who are physically attractive. Women, in contrast to men, tended to report greater preferences for long-term mates who display cues to the ability and willingness to provide resources (e.g., intelligence, kindness, and dependability; see Lippa, 2007). Other investigators have validated key SST hypotheses concerning sex differences in long-term mate choice using nonsurvey techniques such as studying actual mate attraction, marital choice, spousal conflict, and divorce (Betzig, 1989; Dawson & McIntosh, 2006; Kenrick, Neuberg, Zierk, & Krones, 1994; Salmon & Symons, 2001; Schmitt, Couden, & Baker, 2001; Townsend & Wasserman, 1998; Wiederman, 1993). These experimental, behavioral, and naturalistic methodologies suggest that evolutionary-supportive findings are not merely stereotype artifacts or social desirability biases limited to self-reported mate choice. Kenrick and his colleagues (1994), for example, demonstrated using the contrast effect that experimental exposure to physically attractive women tended to lessen a man s commitment to his current relationship partner. However, exposure to physically attractive men had no effect on women s commitment to their current partners. Conversely, when women were exposed to targets who had high status- and resource-related attributes, this lessened women s (but not men s) commitment to their current romantic partners. Kenrick and others argued that this indirect research method not only confirms self-reported mate preference findings but also further shows that men s and women s evolved mate preferences unconsciously influence men s and women s satisfaction and commitment over the long-term course of relationships (see also Buss & Shackelford, 1997; Little & Mannion, 2006). Another indirect effect of sex-differentiated mating desires can be found in the context of relationship initiation and romantic attraction. According to sexual selection theory (Darwin, 1871), the evolved mate preferences of one sex should have a substantive impact on the effectiveness of attraction tactics used by the opposite sex. If men possess an evolved preference for physical attractiveness, the argument goes, women should be more effective than men at using mate initiation and attraction tactics that manipulate physical attractiveness (e.g., by appearing youthful). Conversely, if women prefer resource-related attributes more than men do, men should be seen as more effective than women at using resource-related tactics of initiation and attraction (e.g., by demonstrating intelligence, kindness, and dependability). Empirical evaluations of this aspect of sexual selection in humans have been supportive. For example, Buss (1988), Tooke and Camire (1991), and Walters and Crawford (1994) all demonstrated that women are judged more effective than men when using appearance-related tactics of initiation and attraction, whereas men are judged more effective than women when using resource-related tactics of romantic initiation and attraction (for a meta-analysis of attraction results, see Schmitt, 2002). Perceived sex differences in physical appearance and resource-related tactic effectiveness have also been documented within more specialized rating contexts of romantic attraction. Buss (1988) found sex differences in effectiveness ratings of appearance and resource-related tactics when used by men and women to both attract and retain a long-term marital partner (see also Bleske-Rechek & Buss, 2001; Flinn, 1985). Schmitt and Buss (1996) documented sex differences in perceived tactic effectiveness across both self-promotion and competitor derogation forms of mate attraction (i.e., when people highlight their own positive qualities and tear down their rivals perceived qualities; see also Greer & Buss, 1994; Walters & Crawford, 1994). Schmitt and Buss (2001) found sex differences in perceived appearance and resource-related mate attraction within the specialized context of obtaining a long-term mating partner who is already in a relationship, what they called the context of mate poaching (see also Bleske & Shackelford, 2001; Schmitt & Shackelford, 2003). Whether

7 60 researchers ask people directly, observe their real-life behavior, or subtly look for indirect effects, the pervasive range of sex differences in long-term mating psychology supports the evolutionary perspective on mate choice and relationship initiation. Sex Differences in Short-Term Mating According to SST, both sexes are hypothesized to pursue short-term mateships in certain contexts, but for different reproductive reasons that reflect sex-specific adaptive problems (Buss & Schmitt, 1993). For women, the asymmetry in obligatory parental investment (Symons, 1979; Trivers, 1972) leaves them little to gain in reproductive output by engaging in indiscriminate, short-term sex with high numbers of partners. Women can reap evolutionary benefits from short-term mating (Greiling & Buss, 2000; Hrdy, 1981). However, women s psychology of short-term mate choice appears to center on obtaining men of high genetic quality rather than numerous men in high-volume quantity (Banfield & McCabe, 2001; Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998; Li & Kenrick, 2006; Smith, 1984). For men, the potential reproductive benefits from short-term mating with numerous partners can be profound. A man can produce as many as 100 offspring by mating with 100 women over the course of a year, whereas a man who is monogamous will tend to have only one child with his partner during that same time period. In evolutionary currencies, this represents a strong selective pressure and a potent adaptive problem for men s short-term mating strategy to center on obtaining large numbers of partners (Schmitt et al., 2003). Obviously, 100 instances of only onetime mating would rarely produce precisely 100 offspring. However, a man mating with 100 women over the course of a year particularly repeated matings when the women are nearing ovulation and are especially interested in short-term mating (Gangestad, 2001) would likely have significantly more offspring than a woman mating repeatedly with 100 interested men over the course of a year. According to SST, three of the specific design features of men s short-term mating psychology are that (a) men possess a greater desire than women do for a variety of sexual partners, (b) men require less time to elapse than women do before consenting to sexual intercourse, and (c) men tend to more actively seek short-term mateships than women do (Buss & Schmitt, 1993). This suite of hypothesized sex differences has been well supported empirically. For example, Schmitt and his colleagues (2003) documented these fundamental sex differences across 10 major regions of the world. When people from North America were asked, Ideally, how many different sexual partners would you like to have in the next month? over 23% of men, but only 3% of women, indicated that they would like more than one sexual partner in the next month. This finding confirmed that many men, and few women, desire sexual variety in the form of multiple sexual partners over short time intervals. Similar degrees of sexual differentiation were found in South America (35.0% versus 6.1%), Western Europe (22.6% versus 5.5%), Eastern Europe (31.7% versus 7.1%), Southern Europe (31.0% versus 6.0%), the Middle East (33.1% versus 5.9%), Africa (18.2% versus 4.2%), Oceania (25.3% versus 5.8%), South and Southeast Asia (32.4% versus 6.4%), and East Asia (17.9% versus 2.6%). These sex differences also persisted across a variety of demographic statuses, including age, socioeconomic status, and sexual orientation. Moreover, when men and women who reported actively pursuing a short-term mating strategy were asked whether they wanted more than one partner in the next month, over 50% of men, but less than 20% of women, expressed desires for multiple sexual partners (Schmitt et al., 2003). This finding supports the key SST hypothesis that men s short-term mating strategy is very different from women s and is based, in part, on obtaining large numbers of sexual partners. Other findings from the cross-cultural study by Schmitt and his colleagues (2003) documented that men universally agree to have sex after less time has elapsed than women do, and that men from all world regions expend more effort on seeking brief sexual relationships than women do. For example, across all cultures nearly 25% of married men, but only 10% of married women, reported that they are actively seeking short-term, extramarital relationships (see also Wiederman, 1997). These culturally universal findings support the view that men evolved to seek large numbers of sex partners when they pursue a short-term mating strategy. Some women also pursue short-term mates. However, when women seek short-term mates they are more selective and tend to seek out men who

8 AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE ON MATE CHOICE AND RELATIONSHIP INITIATION 61 are physically attractive, are intelligent, and otherwise possess high-quality genes (Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997, 2003). EVOLUTION OF INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MATE CHOICE AND RELATIONSHIP INITIATION The previous section addressed the evolutionary psychology of how men and women choose and initiate short-term and long-term mating relationships. Another important question is when and why an individual man or woman would choose to pursue a long-term mateship versus a shortterm mateship. Several theories have suggested that personal circumstances including stage of life, personal characteristics, and physical attributes play an adaptive role in shaping or evoking people s strategic mating choices (Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Gangestad & Simpson, 2000). Among the more important sex-specific features that affect mating strategies are men s overall mate value and women s ovulatory status. Mating Differences within Men According to SST (Buss & Schmitt, 1993), men possess a menu of alternative mating strategies that they can follow. Whether a man chooses to pursue a short-term or long-term mating strategy (or both) may depend, in part, on his status and prestige. In foraging cultures, men with higher status and prestige tend to possess multiple wives (Betzig, 1986; Borgerhoff Mulder, 1987, 1990; Cronk, 1991; Heath & Hadley, 1998), and in so doing polygynous men are able to satisfy aspects of both their long-term pairbonding desires and short-term numerous partner desires. In most modern cultures, men with high status are unable to legally marry more than one woman. However, high-status men are more likely to successfully pursue extramarital affairs and to practice de facto or effective polygyny in the form of serial divorce and remarriage compared to others (Brown & Hotra, 1988; Buss, 2000; Fisher, 1992). Given an equal sex ratio of men and women in a given culture, this results in other men namely, those with low status and prestige being limited to monogamy in the form of one wife. Some low-status men are left with no wives at all, and may choose to resort to coercive, low-investment mating strategies (Thornhill & Palmer, 2000). Consequently, important sources of individual variation in men s mate choice and relationship initiation tactics are status and prestige. Whether a man follows a more short-term- or long-term-oriented mating strategy depends on other factors as well, many of which relate to the man s overall value in the mating marketplace (Gangestad & Simpson, 2000). A man s mate value is determined, in part, by his status and prestige. It is also affected by his current resource holdings, long-term ambition, intelligence, interpersonal dominance, social popularity, sense of humor, reputation for kindness, maturity, height, strength, and athleticism (Chagnon, 1988; Ellis, 1992; Miller, 2000; Nettle, 2002; Pierce, 1996). Most studies of men in modern cultures find that, when they are able to do so as a result of high mate value, men choose to engage in multiple mating relationships. For example, Lalumiere, Seto, and Quinsey (1995) designed a scale to measure overall mating opportunities. The scale, similar to overall mate value, included items such as Relative to my peer group, I can get dates with ease. They found that men with higher mate value tended to have sex at an earlier age, to have a larger number of sexual partners, and to follow a more promiscuous mating strategy overall (see also James, 2003; Landolt et al., 1995). Another potential indicator of mate value is the social barometer of self-esteem (Kirkpatrick, Waugh, Valencia, & Webster, 2002). Similar to the results with mating opportunities, men who score higher on self-esteem scales tend to choose and to successfully engage in more short-term mating relationships (Baumeister & Tice, 2001; Walsh, 1991). Indeed, in a recent cross-cultural study by Schmitt (2005b), this revealing trend was evident across several world regions. The same relationship was usually not evident, and was often reversed, among women in modern nations (see also Mikach

9 62 & Bailey, 1999). That is, women with high self-esteem were more likely to pursue monogamous, longterm mating strategies. These findings would seem to support parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972), in that when mate value is high and people are given a choice, men prefer short-term mating (sometimes in addition to long-term mating), whereas women strategically prefer a single monogamous mateship. An important determinant of individual mate choice, therefore, is overall mate value in the mating marketplace, with men of high mate value and women of low mate value more likely to pursue short-term mating strategies (see Table 3.1). According to strategic pluralism theory (Gangestad & Simpson, 2000), men should also be more likely to engage in short-term mating when they exhibit the physical characteristics most preferred by women who desire a short-term mate, especially those traits indicative of high genetic quality. Higher facial symmetry, for example, is indicative of low genetic mutation load in men, and women adaptively prefer facial symmetry when pursing short-term mates (Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997). This is because one of the key benefits women can reap from short-term mating is to gain access to highquality genes that they might not be able to secure from a long-term partner (Gangestad, 2001). TABLE 3.1 Fundamental Features of Long-Term and Short-Term Mating Strategies in Men and Women Men s Long-Term Mating Strategy Key mate choice adaptations Prefer cues to youth and fertility, prefer sexual fidelity, and prefer good parenting skills Effective relationship initiation tactics Demonstrate ability and willingness to invest, and demonstrate emotional commitment Associated personal characteristics Low mate value, feminine and asymmetrical facial features, and low testosterone Eliciting cultural and familial factors High sex ratio (more men than women), and secure parent child attachment Women s Long-Term Mating Strategy Key mate choice adaptations Prefer attributes that indicate ability and willingness to invest in self and offspring Effective relationship initiation tactics Provide cues to youth and fertility, suggest sexual fidelity, and suggest good parenting skills Associated personal characteristics High mate value, high self-esteem, and luteal phase of ovulatory cycle Eliciting cultural and familial factors High sex ratio, and secure parent child attachment Men s Short-Term Mating Strategy Key mate choice adaptations Prefer large number of partners, prefer easy sexual access, and minimize commitment Effective relationship initiation tactics Provide immediate resources, demonstrate intelligence, and feign long-term interests Associated personal characteristics High mate value, masculine and symmetrical facial features, and high testosterone Eliciting cultural and familial factors Low sex ratio (more women than men), and insecuredismissing parent child attachment Women s Short-Term Mating Strategy Key mate choice adaptations Prefer immediate resources, and prefer genetic quality (intelligence, masculinity, and symmetry) Effective relationship initiation tactics Provide easy sexual access, and limit future commitment Associated personal characteristics Low mate value, low self-esteem, and late follicular phase of ovulatory cycle Eliciting cultural and familial factors Low sex ratio, and insecure-fearful and insecurepreoccupied parent child attachment

10 AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE ON MATE CHOICE AND RELATIONSHIP INITIATION 63 Evidence that physically attractive men adaptively respond to women s desires and become more promiscuous comes from other sources, as well. For example, men who possess broad and muscular shoulders, a physical attribute preferred by short-term-oriented women (Frederick, Haselton, Buchanan, & Gallup, 2003), tend toward short-term mating as reflected in an earlier age of first intercourse, more sexual partners, and more extrapair copulations (Hughes & Gallup, 2003). In numerous studies, Gangestad and his colleagues have shown that women who seek short-term mates place special importance on the physical attractiveness of their partners, and that physically attractive men are more likely to pursue short-term mating strategies (Gangestad & Cousins, 2001; Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997; Thornhill & Gangestad, 1994, 1999). Some research suggests that genetic and hormonal predispositions may affect men s mate choice and relationship initiation strategies (Bailey, Kirk, Zhu, Dunne, & Martin, 2000). Much of this research focuses on the moderating effects of testosterone (Dabbs & Dabbs, 2000). For example, married men, compared to their same-age single peers, tend to have lower levels of testosterone (Burnham et al., 2003), though this is not true among married men who are also interested in concurrent extrapair copulations or short-term mateships (McIntyre et al., 2006). Men who are expectant fathers and hope to have children only with their current partner have relatively low testosterone (Gray, Kahlenberg, Barrett, Lipson, & Ellison, 2002), whereas men possessing high testosterone tend to have more sexual partners, to start having sex earlier, to have higher sperm counts, to be more interested in sex, and to divorce more frequently, and are more likely to have affairs (Alexander & Sherwin, 1991; Manning, 2002; Mazur & Booth, 1998; Udry & Campbell, 1994). The root cause of this mate choice variability may lie in early testosterone exposure and its effects on the activation of men s short-term mating psychology. Exposure to high testosterone levels in utero causes increased masculinization of the human brain and increased testosterone in adulthood (Manning; Ridley, 2003). If men s brains are programmed for greater short-term mating in general (Symons, 1979; Trivers, 1972), this would lead to the hypothesis that those who are exposed to higher testosterone levels in utero would be more likely to develop short-term mating strategies in adulthood. In women, though, other factors appear to adaptively influence mating strategy choice. Mating Differences within Women Women s desires for engaging in sexual intercourse tend to vary across their ovulatory cycles. On average, women s desires for sex peak during the late follicular phase, just before ovulation, when the odds of becoming pregnant would be maximized (Regan, 1996). It was once thought that this shift in sexual desire evolved because it increased the probability of having conceptive intercourse in our monogamous female ancestors. However, several studies have now documented that women s short-term desires for men with high-quality genes actually peak in the highly fertile days just before ovulation (Gangestad, 2001; Gangestad, Garver-Apgar, & Simpson, 2007; Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997; Haselton & Miller, 2006). For example, women who are interested in short-term mating tend to prefer men who are high in dominance and masculinity (Buss & Schmitt, 1993), as indicated by testosterone-related attributes such as prominent brows, large chins, and other features of facial masculinity (Mueller & Mazur, 1997; Penton-Voak & Chen, 2004; Perrett et al., 1998). Short-term-oriented women may prefer these attributes because facial markers of testosterone are honest indicators of immunocompetence quality in men (Gangestad & Thornhill, 2003). During the late follicular phase, women s preferences for men with masculine faces conspicuously increase (Johnston, Hagel, Franklin, Fink, & Grammer, 2001; Penton-Voak et al., 2003), as do their preferences for masculine voices (Puts, 2006), precisely as though women are shifting their mating psychology to follow a more short-term-oriented strategy around ovulation. A similar ovulatory shift can be seen in women s preference for symmetrical faces. Women who generally pursue a short-term mating strategy express strong preferences for male faces that are symmetrical, perhaps because facial symmetry is indicative of low mutation load (Gangestad & Thornhill, 1997). During the late follicular phase, women s preference for symmetrical faces increases

11 64 even further (Gangestad & Cousins, 2001), again as though they have shifted their psychology to that of a short-term mating strategist. It has also been shown that women who are nearing ovulation find the pheromonal smell of symmetrical men more appealing than when women are less fertile (Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998; Rikowski & Grammer, 1999), that women who mate with more symmetrical men have more frequent and intense orgasms (Thornhill, Gangestad, & Comer, 1995), and that men with attractive faces have qualitatively better health (Shackelford & Larsen, 1999) and semen characteristics (Soler et al., 2003). Finally, women appear to dress more provocatively when nearing ovulation (Grammer, Renninger, & Fischer, 2004), though women near ovulation also reduce risky behaviors associated with being raped, especially if they are not taking contraception (Bröder & Hohmann, 2003). Overall, there is compelling evidence that women s mating strategies shift at the within-person level from a long-term mating psychology to a more short-term-oriented mating psychology, precisely when they are the most fertile. It is possible that these shifts reflect women seeking highquality genes from extrapair copulations while maintaining a long-term relationship with a heavily investing partner (Gangestad, 2001; Haselton & Miller, 2006). Additional individual differences and personal situations may be linked to adaptive variability in women s mate choices and relationship initiation strategies. For example, short-term mating strategies are more likely to occur during adolescence, when one s partner is of low mate value, when one desires to get rid of a mate, and after divorce all situations where short-term mating may serve adaptive functions (Cashdan, 1996; Greiling & Buss, 2000). In some cases, short-term mating seems to emerge as an adaptive reaction to early developmental experiences within the family (Michalski & Shackelford, 2002). For example, short-term mating strategies are more likely to occur among women growing up in father-absent homes (Moffit, Caspi, Belsky, & Silva, 1992; Quinlan, 2003), especially in homes where a stepfather is present (Ellis & Garber, 2000). In these cases, the absence of a father and presence of a stepfather may indicate to young women that mating-age men are unreliable. In such environments, short-term mating may serve as the more viable mating strategy choice once in adulthood (see also Belsky, 1999). Finally, some have argued that frequency-dependent or other forms of selection have resulted in different heritable tendencies toward long-term versus short-term mating (Gangestad & Simpson, 1990). There is behavioral genetic evidence that age at first intercourse, lifetime number of sex partners, and sociosexuality a general trait that varies from restricted long-term mating to unrestricted short-term mating are somewhat heritable (Bailey et al., 2000; Rowe, 2002). However, most findings suggest that heritability in mate choice and mating strategy is stronger in men than in women (Dunne et al., 1997). EVOLUTION OF CULTURAL DIFFERENCES IN MATE CHOICE AND RELATIONSHIP INITIATION Sex Ratios and Human Mating In addition to sex and individual differences in mating strategies, mate choices and relationship initiation behaviors appear to vary in evolutionary-relevant ways across cultures (Frayser, 1985; Kelly, 1995; Pasternak et al., 1997). Pedersen (1991) has speculated that the relative number of men versus women in a given culture should influence mating behavior. Operational sex ratio can be defined as the relative balance of marriage-age men versus marriage-age women in the local mating pool (Secord, 1983). Sex ratios are considered high when the number of men significantly outsizes the number of women in a local culture. Sex ratios are considered low when there are relatively more women than men in the mating market. In most cultures women tend to slightly outnumber men, largely because of men s polygynous tendency to have a higher mortality rate (Daly & Wilson, 1988). Nevertheless, significant variation often exists in sex ratios across cultures, and within cultures when viewed over historical time (Grant, 1998; Guttentag & Secord, 1983).

12 AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE ON MATE CHOICE AND RELATIONSHIP INITIATION 65 Pedersen (1991) argued that a combination of sexual selection theory (Darwin, 1871) and parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972) leads to a series of predictions concerning the effects of sex ratios on human mating strategies. According to sexual selection theory, when males desire a particular attribute in potential mating partners, females of that species tend to respond by competing in the expression and provision of that desired attribute. Among humans, when sex ratios are especially low and there are many more women than men, men should become an especially scarce resource that women compete for with even more intensity than normal (see also Guttentag & Secord, 1983). When combined with the parental investment notion described earlier in which men tend to desire short-term mating (Trivers, 1972), this leads to the hypothesis that humans in cultures with lower sex ratios (i.e., more women than men) should possess more short-term-oriented mating strategies. Conversely, when sex ratios are high and men greatly outnumber women, men must enter into more intense competition for the limited number of potential female partners. Women s preferences for long-term monogamous relationships become the key desires that must be responded to if men are to remain competitive in the courtship marketplace. Using data from sex ratio fluctuations over time within the United States, Pedersen (1991) marshaled a compelling case for a causal link between sex ratios and human mating strategies (see also Guttentag & Secord, 1983). For example, high sex ratio fluctuations have been historically associated with increases in monogamy, as evidenced by lower divorce rates and men s greater willingness to invest in their children. Low sex ratios have been historically associated with indexes of short-term mating, such as an increase in divorce rates and a reduction in what he termed female sexual coyness. In a recent cross-cultural study (Schmitt, 2005a), national sex ratios were correlated with direct measures of basic human mating strategies across 48 nations in an attempt to test Pedersen s theory. As expected, cultures with more men than women tended toward long-term mating, whereas cultures with more women than men tended toward short-term mating (see also Barber, 2000). Attachment and Human Mating Several combinations of life history theory (Low, 1998) and attachment theory (Bowlby, 1982) have suggested that certain critical experiences during childhood play a role in the development of human mating strategies (Belsky, 1999). Perhaps the most prominent of these theories is a life span model developed by Belsky, Steinberg, and Draper (1991). According to this model, early social experiences adaptively channel children down one of two reproductive pathways. Children who are socially exposed to high levels of stress especially insensitive or inconsistent parenting, harsh physical environments, and economic hardship tend to develop insecure attachment styles. These children also tend to physically mature earlier than those children who are exposed to less stress. According to Belsky and his colleagues (1991), attachment insecurity and early physical maturity subsequently lead to the evolutionary-adaptive development of what is called an opportunistic reproductive strategy in adulthood (i.e., short-term mating). In cultures with unpredictable social environments, it is therefore argued, children adaptively respond to stressful cues by developing the more viable strategy of short-term mating. Conversely, those children exposed to lower levels of stress and less environmental hardship tend to be more emotionally secure and to physically mature later. These children are thought to develop a more investing reproductive strategy in adulthood (i.e., long-term mating) that pays evolutionary dividends in low-stress environments. Although the causal mechanisms that influence strategic mating are most prominently located within the family, this model also suggests that certain aspects of culture may be related to mating strategy variation (see also Belsky, 1999). A closely related theory has been proposed by Chisholm (1996). Chisholm argued that local mortality rates presumably related to high stress and inadequate resources act as cues that facultatively shift human mating strategies in evolutionary-adaptive ways. In cultures with high mortality rates and unpredictable resources, the optimal mating strategy is to reproduce early and often, a strategy related to insecure attachment, short-term temporal orientations, and promiscuous mating strategies. In cultures that are physically safe and have abundant resources, mortality rates are lower

13 66 and the optimal strategy is to invest heavily in fewer numbers of offspring. In safer environments, therefore, one should pursue a long-term strategy associated with more monogamous mating. Collectively, the Belsky et al. (1991) and Chisholm (1996) theories can be referred to as a developmental-attachment theory of human mating strategies. Numerous studies have provided support for developmental-attachment theory (Barber, 2003; Belsky, 1999; Ellis & Garber, 2000; Moffit et al., 1992; Quinlan, 2003). In a recent attempt to test developmental-attachment theory, Schmitt and his colleagues (Schmitt, Alcalay, Allensworth, et al., 2004) measured the romantic attachment styles of over 17,000 people from 56 nations. They related insecure attachment styles to various indexes of familial stress, economic resources, mortality, and fertility. They found overwhelming support for developmental-attachment theory. For example, nations with higher fertility rates, higher mortality rates, higher levels of stress (e.g., poor health and education), and lower levels of resources tended to have higher levels of insecure romantic attachment. Schmitt (2005a) also found that short-term mating was related to insecure attachment across cultures. As expected, the dismissing form of insecure attachment was linked to short-term mating in men, and fearful or preoccupied forms of insecure attachment were linked to short-term mating in women. These findings support the view that stressful environments cause increases in insecure romantic attachment, increases presumably linked to short-term mating strategies (see also Kirkpatrick, 1998). LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS Evolutionary psychology is but one perspective from which to view the special psychology of human romance, and relying solely on the perspective presented here would be a mistake. For example, religion has been shown to have a strong influence on mate choice and relationship initiation (Pasternak et al., 1997; Reynolds & Tanner, 1983), particularly among women (Baumeister & Twenge, 2002). The same appears true for political ideology, education level, and other sociopolitical facets of the modern human condition (Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994; Pratto, 1996). None of these factors have been fully integrated into the current review. Future research on human mate choice and relationship initiation should attempt to integrate evolutionary perspectives with other theories and viewpoints on human sexuality, particularly social role theories (e.g., see Kenrick, 2006; Kenrick, Trost, & Sundie, 2004; Schmitt, 2005a). It can be tempting to contrast evolutionary and social role theories as either-or explanations of human mate choice and relationship initiation. However, an increasing number of investigators have focused on integrating these perspectives into coherent accounts of how biology and culture interact to produce the patterns of human sexuality we see across sexes, individuals, and cultures (Gangestad, Haselton, & Buss, 2006; Lippa, 2007). The current chapter, in which mate choice and relationship initiation were viewed as resulting from a collection of evolved psychological adaptations, focused primarily on the evolutionary perspective and, as a result, may appear quite limited. Still, any comprehensive theory of mate choice and relationship initiation must first take into account the most fundamental evolutionary questions: As a species, what is our natural mating system, and how does our resulting evolved psychology influence modern human sexuality? Based on the evidence reviewed here, humans appear to possess psychological adaptations related to several mating systems, including monogamy, polygyny, and promiscuity. Our pluralistic human mating repertoire may be fundamentally organized in terms of basic long-term and short-term mating psychologies. The activation and pursuit of these mating psychologies including concomitant patterns of mate choice and relationship initiation differ in adaptive ways across sex, individual circumstance, and cultural context. The sexes differ significantly in their adaptations for short-term mate choice. Men s short-term mating strategy is based primarily on obtaining large numbers of partners, being quick to consent to sex, and more actively seeking brief sexual encounters. Women s short-term strategy seems more heavily rooted in obtaining partners of high genetic quality, including men who possess masculine

Intersexual Competition

Intersexual Competition (in press, October 2012). In P. Whelan & A. Bolin (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Human Sexuality. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Blackwell. Intersexual Competition Valerie G. Starratt, Nova Southeastern University, valerie.starratt@nova.edu

More information

It s Not All about Her: Men s Mate Value and Mate Retention. Emily J. Miner. Florida Atlantic University. Valerie G. Starratt

It s Not All about Her: Men s Mate Value and Mate Retention. Emily J. Miner. Florida Atlantic University. Valerie G. Starratt Mate Value and Mate Retention - 1 - Running Head: MATE VALUE AND MATE RETENTION [in press, March 2009, Personality and Individual Differences] It s Not All about Her: Men s Mate Value and Mate Retention

More information

Empirical testing of evolutionary hypotheses has used to test many theories both directly and indirectly. Why do empirical testing?

Empirical testing of evolutionary hypotheses has used to test many theories both directly and indirectly. Why do empirical testing? Empirical testing of evolutionary hypotheses has used to test many theories both directly and indirectly. Why do empirical testing? In fact, the general theory of evolution has been accepted as more or

More information

Adaptive Individual Differences Revisited

Adaptive Individual Differences Revisited Adaptive Individual Differences Revisited David M. Buss University of Texas, Austin ABSTRACT Humans possess a complex array of evolved psychological mechanisms, only a subset of which is activated at any

More information

Sex Ratio and Mate Preferences: A Cross-Cultural Investigation. Emily A. Stone and Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University. David M.

Sex Ratio and Mate Preferences: A Cross-Cultural Investigation. Emily A. Stone and Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University. David M. Sex ratio p. 1 Running Head: SEX RATIO AND MATE PREFERENCES [in press, European Journal of Social Psychology, May 4, 2006] Sex Ratio and Mate Preferences: A Cross-Cultural Investigation Emily A. Stone

More information

Test Bank. Chapter 2. Abrams, Sexuality and Its Disorders SAGE Publishing, 2017

Test Bank. Chapter 2. Abrams, Sexuality and Its Disorders SAGE Publishing, 2017 Test Bank Chapter 2 1. Is the male female dichotomy an appropriate mode of defining gender? Why, or why not? Ans: The male female dichotomy is strongly embodied in most of cultures, religions, and even

More information

Do Women Pretend Orgasm to Retain a Mate?

Do Women Pretend Orgasm to Retain a Mate? Do Women Pretend Orgasm to Retain a Mate? Farnaz Kaighobadi 1,3, Todd K. Shackelford 2, and Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford 2 1, 3 HIV Center for Clinical and Behavioral Studies, Columbia University and

More information

Running head: MATE RETENTION STRATEGIES p. 1. Solving the Problem of Partner Infidelity:

Running head: MATE RETENTION STRATEGIES p. 1. Solving the Problem of Partner Infidelity: Running head: MATE RETENTION STRATEGIES p. 1 [in press, Personality and Individual Differences, February 2015] Solving the Problem of Partner Infidelity: Individual Mate Retention, Coalitional Mate Retention,

More information

The Basic Cognition of Jealousy: An Evolutionary Perspective. Jon K. Maner. Florida State University. Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

The Basic Cognition of Jealousy: An Evolutionary Perspective. Jon K. Maner. Florida State University. Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University Evolution, cognition 1 RUNNING HEAD: JEALOUSY AND COGNITION The Basic Cognition of Jealousy: An Evolutionary Perspective Jon K. Maner Florida State University Todd K. Shackelford Florida Atlantic University

More information

Sex ratio and mate preferences: A cross-cultural investigation

Sex ratio and mate preferences: A cross-cultural investigation European Journal of Social Psychology Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 37, 288 296 (2007) Published online 18 August 2006 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.357 Sex ratio and mate

More information

Mate Value of Romantic Partners Predicts Men s Partner-Directed Verbal Insults. Emily J. Miner and Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

Mate Value of Romantic Partners Predicts Men s Partner-Directed Verbal Insults. Emily J. Miner and Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University Mate Value and Insults - 1 - Running Head: MATE VALUE AND INSULTS [in press, Personality and Individual Differences, September 2008] Mate Value of Romantic Partners Predicts Men s Partner-Directed Verbal

More information

PERCEPTIONS OF PARTNER FEMININITY PREDICT INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MEN S SENSITIVITY TO FACIAL CUES OF MALE DOMINANCE

PERCEPTIONS OF PARTNER FEMININITY PREDICT INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MEN S SENSITIVITY TO FACIAL CUES OF MALE DOMINANCE Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 2011, 69-82 DOI: 10.1556/JEP.9.2011.10.1 PERCEPTIONS OF PARTNER FEMININITY PREDICT INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MEN S SENSITIVITY TO FACIAL CUES OF MALE DOMINANCE CHRISTOPHER

More information

Individual Differences

Individual Differences Individual Differences 1.How do life history theory, costly signaling theory, balancing selection, and contingent shifts according to the environment predict individual differences? Give an example of

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This article was originally published in the International Encyclopedia of the Social

More information

Sexual behavior and jealousy: An evolutionary perspective

Sexual behavior and jealousy: An evolutionary perspective Romanian Journal of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy and Hypnosis Volume 5, Issue 1-2, January June 2018 Theoretical Paper Sexual behavior and jealousy: An evolutionary perspective Irina Grigorovici Titu Maiorescu

More information

Short-Term Sexual Strategies

Short-Term Sexual Strategies Short-Term Sexual Strategies Theories of Men s Short-Term Mating Primary reproductive benefit: Direct increase in number of offspring Competition (usually Males) Sperm are cheap RS with # of matings Potential

More information

Mate Attraction, Retention, and Expulsion. Emily J. Miner and Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

Mate Attraction, Retention, and Expulsion. Emily J. Miner and Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University Mate Attraction, Retention, Expulsion - 1 - Running Head: MATE ATTRACTION, RETENTION, EXPULSION [in press, November 2009, Psichothema] Mate Attraction, Retention, and Expulsion Emily J. Miner and Todd

More information

To Poach or Not to Poach? Men are more Willing to Short-term Poach Mated Women who are more Attractive than their Mates

To Poach or Not to Poach? Men are more Willing to Short-term Poach Mated Women who are more Attractive than their Mates PROFESSIONAL ARTICLE EvoS Journal: The Journal of the Evolutionary Studies Consortium? Men are more Willing to Short-term Poach Mated Women who are more Attractive than their Mates Moran, J. B. 1, Kuhle,

More information

This article was published in an Elsevier journal. The attached copy is furnished to the author for non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the author s institution, sharing

More information

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection Intrasexual Natural Selection Sexual Selection Survival Mating Success Parental Care Intrasexual

More information

An Evolutionary Psychological Perspective on Infidelity. Alastair P.C. Davies, Todd K. Shackelford, and Aaron T. Goetz. Florida Atlantic University

An Evolutionary Psychological Perspective on Infidelity. Alastair P.C. Davies, Todd K. Shackelford, and Aaron T. Goetz. Florida Atlantic University [in press, June, 2006, in: In P. R. Peluso (Ed.), In Love s debris: A practitioner s guide to addressing infidelity in couples therapy (Routledge series on Family Therapy). New York: Routledge.] An Evolutionary

More information

Types of Mating Systems

Types of Mating Systems The Human Mating System Note: this is the stereotypical EP view (e.g., Buss) Types of Mating Systems Random Monogamy Polygyny: variance in male reproductive success > variance in female reproductive success

More information

8162_Ch04_Geher_LEA 2/15/07 1:07 PM Page 103 III. Strategic Flexibility in Mating Intelligence

8162_Ch04_Geher_LEA 2/15/07 1:07 PM Page 103 III. Strategic Flexibility in Mating Intelligence 8162_Ch04_Geher_LEA 2/15/07 1:07 PM Page 103 III Strategic Flexibility in Mating Intelligence 8162_Ch04_Geher_LEA 2/15/07 1:07 PM Page 104 8162_Ch04_Geher_LEA 2/15/07 1:07 PM Page 105 Chapter 4 Intelligent

More information

The Influence of Women s Self-Esteem on Mating Decision Making

The Influence of Women s Self-Esteem on Mating Decision Making University of Dayton ecommons Honors Theses University Honors Program 4-2017 The Influence of Women s Self-Esteem on Mating Decision Making Kathleen D. Golterman Follow this and additional works at: https://ecommons.udayton.edu/uhp_theses

More information

Suschinsky, Kelly D. University of Lethbridge Research Repository Evolutionary Psychology

Suschinsky, Kelly D. University of Lethbridge Research Repository   Evolutionary Psychology University of Lethbridge Research Repository OPUS Faculty Research and Publications http://opus.uleth.ca Lalumière, Martin 2007 The view from the cuckold: A review of S. M. Platek and T. K. Shackelford

More information

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin AQA A Level Psychology Topic Companion Relationships Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin Page 2 AQA A LEVEL Psychology topic COMPANION: RELATIONSHIPS Contents Topic Evolutionary Explanations 3 Attraction: Self-Disclosure

More information

[In press, Personality and Individual Differences, February 2008] Not all Men are Sexually Coercive:

[In press, Personality and Individual Differences, February 2008] Not all Men are Sexually Coercive: Infidelity, desirability, and coercion - 1 - Running Head: INFIDELITY, DESIRABILITY, AND COERCION [In press, Personality and Individual Differences, February 2008] Not all Men are Sexually Coercive: A

More information

Human Mating Behavior: An Evolutionary Perspective

Human Mating Behavior: An Evolutionary Perspective Hot Science - Cool Talk # 109 Human Mating Behavior: An Evolutionary Perspective Dr. David M. Buss October 27, 2017 Produced by and for Hot Science - Cool Talks by the Environmental Science Institute.

More information

Universal Sex Differences in the Desire for Sexual Variety: Tests From 52 Nations, 6 Continents, and 13 Islands

Universal Sex Differences in the Desire for Sexual Variety: Tests From 52 Nations, 6 Continents, and 13 Islands Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 85, No. 1, 85 104 0022-3514/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.85 Universal Sex

More information

The Mere Presence of Opposite-Sex Others on Judgments of Sexual and Romantic Desirability: Opposite Effects for Men and Women

The Mere Presence of Opposite-Sex Others on Judgments of Sexual and Romantic Desirability: Opposite Effects for Men and Women The Mere Presence of Opposite-Sex Others on Judgments of Sexual and Romantic Desirability: Opposite Effects for Men and Women Sarah E. Hill David M. Buss University of Texas at Austin Men s and women s

More information

The Relationship between Objective Sperm Competition Risk and Men s Copulatory Interest is. Moderated by Partner s Time Spent with Other Men

The Relationship between Objective Sperm Competition Risk and Men s Copulatory Interest is. Moderated by Partner s Time Spent with Other Men Running head: SPERM COMPETITION 1 [in press, Human Nature, February 2013] The Relationship between Objective Sperm Competition Risk and Men s Copulatory Interest is Moderated by Partner s Time Spent with

More information

Preferences for symmetry in faces change across the menstrual cycle

Preferences for symmetry in faces change across the menstrual cycle Biological Psychology 76 (2007) 209 216 www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Preferences for symmetry in faces change across the menstrual cycle Anthony C. Little a,b, *, Benedict C. Jones c, D. Michael Burt

More information

Self-Defense and Female-Perpetrated Violence. Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford

Self-Defense and Female-Perpetrated Violence. Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford Self-Defense and Female-Perpetrated Violence Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford Synonyms: retaliation, self-defense, physical aggression, male sexual jealousy, intimate partner violence Definition:

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Personality and Individual Differences 47 (2009) 191 196 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Estimated hormones

More information

Absence Makes the Adaptations Grow Fonder: Proportion of Time Apart from Partner, Male Sexual Psychology, and Sperm Competition in Humans

Absence Makes the Adaptations Grow Fonder: Proportion of Time Apart from Partner, Male Sexual Psychology, and Sperm Competition in Humans Sperm competition in humans p. 1 Running Head: SPERM COMPETITION IN HUMANS [in press, Journal of Comparative Psychology, November 2006] Absence Makes the Adaptations Grow Fonder: Proportion of Time Apart

More information

Chapter 11 Psychological Adaptation and Human Fertility Patterns: Some Evidence of Human Mating Strategies as Evoked Sexual Culture

Chapter 11 Psychological Adaptation and Human Fertility Patterns: Some Evidence of Human Mating Strategies as Evoked Sexual Culture Chapter 11 Psychological Adaptation and Human Fertility Patterns: Some Evidence of Human Mating Strategies as Evoked Sexual Culture 1 2 3 4 David P. Schmitt 5 Abstract Several features of human fertility,

More information

The affective shift hypothesis: The functions of emotional changes following sexual intercourse

The affective shift hypothesis: The functions of emotional changes following sexual intercourse Personal Relationships, 8 (2001), 357-369. Printed in the United States of America. Copyright 0 2001 ISSPR. 1350-4126/01 $9.50 The affective shift hypothesis: The functions of emotional changes following

More information

Evolution and Sex Differences in Preferences for Short-Term Mates: Results from a Policy Capturing Study

Evolution and Sex Differences in Preferences for Short-Term Mates: Results from a Policy Capturing Study Evolution and Sex Differences in Preferences for Short-Term Mates: Results from a Policy Capturing Study Michael W. Wiederman Department of Psychological Science, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana

More information

Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in humans

Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in humans 6 Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in humans AARON T. GOETZ AND TODD K. SHACKELFORD Florida Atlantic University Identifying sperm competition Sexual selection is the mechanism that favors

More information

Mate Preferences and Their Behavioral Manifestations

Mate Preferences and Their Behavioral Manifestations Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2019. 70:23.1 23.34 The Annual Review of Psychology is online at psych.annualreviews.org https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103408 Copyright c 2019 by Annual Reviews. All

More information

Functionality, Parsimony, Discovery, Avoiding Hamartia: How Evolutionary Perspectives are Changing Psychology

Functionality, Parsimony, Discovery, Avoiding Hamartia: How Evolutionary Perspectives are Changing Psychology UNDERGRADUATE CONTRIBUTION EvoS Journal: The Journal of the Evolutionary Studies Consortium Functionality, Parsimony, Discovery, Avoiding Hamartia: How Evolutionary Perspectives are Changing Psychology

More information

Last Lecture. Cont. Being Social, Chapter 3 Evolution of Human Behavior, Chapter 14

Last Lecture. Cont. Being Social, Chapter 3 Evolution of Human Behavior, Chapter 14 Last Lecture Cont. Being Social, Chapter 3 Evolution of Human Behavior, Chapter 14 Is there a reason to be social? What are the benefits of sociality? 1. Cooperation, both benefit 2. Postponed cooperation

More information

B.C. Jones a, *, A.C. Little b, L. Boothroyd c, L.M. DeBruine c, D.R. Feinberg c, M.J. Law Smith c, R.E. Cornwell c, F.R. Moore c, D.I.

B.C. Jones a, *, A.C. Little b, L. Boothroyd c, L.M. DeBruine c, D.R. Feinberg c, M.J. Law Smith c, R.E. Cornwell c, F.R. Moore c, D.I. Hormones and Behavior 48 (2005) 283 290 www.elsevier.com/locate/yhbeh Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle

More information

Personality and Individual Differences

Personality and Individual Differences Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 509 513 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid The association

More information

Mate retention, semen displacement, and sperm competition in humans

Mate retention, semen displacement, and sperm competition in humans 9 Mate retention, semen displacement, and sperm competition in humans AARON T. GOETZ AND TODD K. SHACKELFORD Florida Atlantic University Introduction Competition between males to fertilize a female s eggs

More information

8/20/2012. This talk is designed to provide:

8/20/2012. This talk is designed to provide: Primer on Evolutionary Psychology: Foundational Ideas, Exemplary Research, and Criticisms April Bleske-Rechek Associate Professor of Psychology University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire This talk is designed

More information

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection Natural Selection Survival Mating Success Parental Care Intrasexual Selection: Weapons that are used

More information

Changes in Women s Mate Preferences Across the Ovulatory Cycle

Changes in Women s Mate Preferences Across the Ovulatory Cycle Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 92, No. 1, 151 163 0022-3514/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.151 Changes in Women

More information

Attached or Unattached : With Whom do Men and Women Prefer to Mate, and Why?

Attached or Unattached : With Whom do Men and Women Prefer to Mate, and Why? Psychological Topics 15 (2006), 2, 297-314 Original Scientific Aricle UDC 159.015.7.072 159.922.1.072 177.6 Attached or Unattached : With Whom do Men and Women Prefer to Mate, and Why? Alastair P.C. Davies,

More information

Sex differences (and similarities) in jealousy The moderating influence of infidelity experience and sexual orientation of the infidelity

Sex differences (and similarities) in jealousy The moderating influence of infidelity experience and sexual orientation of the infidelity Evolution and Human Behavior 24 (2003) 17 23 Sex differences (and similarities) in jealousy The moderating influence of infidelity experience and sexual orientation of the infidelity Brad J. Sagarin a,

More information

Morbid Jealousy from an Evolutionary Psychological Perspective JUDITH A. EASTON. University of Texas at Austin LUCAS D. SCHIPPER TODD K.

Morbid Jealousy from an Evolutionary Psychological Perspective JUDITH A. EASTON. University of Texas at Austin LUCAS D. SCHIPPER TODD K. Morbid Jealousy 1 Running head: MORBID JEALOUSY [in press, Evolution and Human Behavior, May, 2007] Morbid Jealousy from an Evolutionary Psychological Perspective JUDITH A. EASTON University of Texas at

More information

Running Head: INSULTS AND MATE RETENTION

Running Head: INSULTS AND MATE RETENTION Insults and mate retention 1 Running Head: INSULTS AND MATE RETENTION [in press, Personality and Individual Differences, November 2006] Why Do Men Insult Their Intimate Partners? William F. McKibbin, Aaron

More information

Women s Oral Sex Behaviors and Risk of Partner Infidelity. Michael N. Pham, Todd K. Shackelford, and Yael Sela. Oakland University

Women s Oral Sex Behaviors and Risk of Partner Infidelity. Michael N. Pham, Todd K. Shackelford, and Yael Sela. Oakland University [in press, Personality and Individual Differences, April 2013] Women s Oral Sex Behaviors and Risk of Partner Infidelity Michael N. Pham, Todd K. Shackelford, and Yael Sela Oakland University Correspondence:

More information

Uncorrected proofs for Geary, D. C. (2006). Coevolution of paternal investment and cuckoldry in humans. In T. K.Shackelford & S. Platek (Eds.

Uncorrected proofs for Geary, D. C. (2006). Coevolution of paternal investment and cuckoldry in humans. In T. K.Shackelford & S. Platek (Eds. Uncorrected proofs for Geary, D. C. (2006). Coevolution of paternal investment and cuckoldry in humans. In T. K.Shackelford & S. Platek (Eds.), Female infidelity and paternal uncertainty (pp.14-34). New

More information

Is Cunnilingus-Assisted Orgasm a Male Sperm-Retention Strategy?

Is Cunnilingus-Assisted Orgasm a Male Sperm-Retention Strategy? Evolutionary Psychology www.epjournal.net 2013. 11(2): 405-414 Original Article Is Cunnilingus-Assisted Orgasm a Male Sperm-Retention Strategy? Michael N. Pham, Department of Psychology, Oakland University,

More information

Sperm Competition in Humans

Sperm Competition in Humans Sperm Competition in Humans Todd K. Shackelford Editors Nicholas Pound Sperm Competition in Humans Classic and Contemporary Readings Todd K. Shackelford Department of Psychology Florida Atlantic University

More information

Distress about mating rivals

Distress about mating rivals Personal Relationships, 7 (2000), 235-243. Printed in the United States of America. Copyright 0 2000 ISSPR. 1350-4126/99 $9.50 Distress about mating rivals DAVID M. BUSS," TODD K. SHACKELFORD? JAE CHOE,c

More information

Desires in Human Mating

Desires in Human Mating Desires in Human Mating DAVID M. BUSS Department of Psychology,University of Texas, Austin, Austin, Texas 78712, USA ABSTRACT: This chapter traces theoretical and empirical progress in the study of human

More information

VARIATION IN REPRODUCTIVE STRATEGIES INFLUENCES POST-COITAL EXPERIENCES WITH PARTNERS

VARIATION IN REPRODUCTIVE STRATEGIES INFLUENCES POST-COITAL EXPERIENCES WITH PARTNERS Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology www.jsecjournal.com org - 2010, 4(4), 254-264. Proceedings of the 4 th Annual Meeting of the NorthEastern Evolutionary Psychology Society Original

More information

Violence from a Woman s Kin. Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford

Violence from a Woman s Kin. Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford Violence from a Woman s Kin Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford Synonyms: daughter-guarding, honor killing, parent-offspring conflict, homicide Definition: Genetic kin benefit from monitoring and defending

More information

Aggressive mating strategies in young adolescent girls. Kelly Sichel

Aggressive mating strategies in young adolescent girls. Kelly Sichel Aggressive mating strategies in young adolescent girls Kelly Sichel Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy under the Executive Committee of The Graduate

More information

Personality and Individual Differences

Personality and Individual Differences Personality and Individual Differences 50 (2011) 253 258 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Meet the parents:

More information

THE INFLUENCE OF MALE FACIAL ATTRACTIVENESS ON WOMEN S RECEPTIVITY

THE INFLUENCE OF MALE FACIAL ATTRACTIVENESS ON WOMEN S RECEPTIVITY Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology www.jsecjournal.com, 2009-3 (1): 49-61. Original Article THE INFLUENCE OF MALE FACIAL ATTRACTIVENESS ON WOMEN S RECEPTIVITY Maryanne Fisher. Department

More information

Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo

Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo This article was downloaded by: [Costanza Scaffidi Abbate] On: 29 July 2013, At: 06:31 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Methodology A. Converging Evidence

Methodology A. Converging Evidence Methodology A. Converging Evidence Different Species Males vs. Females Variations within Species Single Individual across Context True Experimental Methods Vs. Quasi-Experimental Research Manipulation

More information

Mate Guarding and Frequent In-Pair Copulation in Humans

Mate Guarding and Frequent In-Pair Copulation in Humans Anti-Cuckoldry Tactics 239 Mate Guarding and Frequent In-Pair Copulation in Humans Concurrent or Compensatory Anti-Cuckoldry Tactics? Todd K. Shackelford, Aaron T. Goetz, Faith E. Guta Florida Atlantic

More information

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise...

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise... 1. Female choice of males that have exaggerated characters has been explained by female choice of males that have better genes. Explain how female choice of higher quality males might lead to exaggerated

More information

Predicting violence against women from men s mate-retention behaviors

Predicting violence against women from men s mate-retention behaviors 4 Predicting violence against women from men s mate-retention behaviors TODD K. SHACKELFORD AND AARON T. GOETZ Florida Atlantic University Introduction Male sexual jealousy is a frequently cited cause

More information

To link to this article:

To link to this article: This article was downloaded by: [University of Kiel] On: 24 October 2014, At: 17:27 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

To link to this article:

To link to this article: This article was downloaded by: [University of Notre Dame] On: 12 February 2015, At: 14:40 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

Status, prestige, and social dominance

Status, prestige, and social dominance Status, prestige, and social dominance Does status have fitness benefits? Status-seeking and costly signalling Sex differences in competition and dominance Hormones, competition, and status Status and

More information

Socioeconomic Development and Shifts in Mate Preferences

Socioeconomic Development and Shifts in Mate Preferences Evolutionary Psychology www.epjournal.net 2008. 6(3): 447-455 Original Article Socioeconomic Development and Shifts in Mate Preferences Emily A. Stone, Department of Anthropology, University of Utah, Salt

More information

I only have eyes for you: Ovulation redirects attention (but not memory) to attractive men

I only have eyes for you: Ovulation redirects attention (but not memory) to attractive men I only have eyes for you: Ovulation redirects attention (but not memory) to attractive men The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story

More information

Evolutionary Psychology

Evolutionary Psychology Evolutionary Psychology Mating & Reproduction 1 I. Sex as evolved/selected strategy A. Why sex works B. Why monogamy works Hints: 1.See sex as phenomenon 2. Distal vs. proximal causality 3. Consider why

More information

Why Sex? Mating. Disadvantages of Sex. Advantages of Sex. What Would We Expect? Sex Differences in Parental Investment

Why Sex? Mating. Disadvantages of Sex. Advantages of Sex. What Would We Expect? Sex Differences in Parental Investment Mating Cost of males Red Queen hypothesis Reproductive value Waist-hip ratio (WHR) Body mass index (BMI) Parental investment (PI) Why Sex? Most genes have a 5% chance of being replicated in sexually produced

More information

The environment s influence on mate preferences

The environment s influence on mate preferences Sexualities, Evolution and Gender, August 2005; 7(2): 115 134 The environment s influence on mate preferences R. MATTHEW MONTOYA University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, USA Abstract Two studies tested

More information

Evolutionary Psychology (Psych 459) Midterm Exam Winter 2010

Evolutionary Psychology (Psych 459) Midterm Exam Winter 2010 Evolutionary Psychology (Psych 459) Midterm Exam Winter 2010 A. Seminar papers Answer any 6 of the following 7 questions 1. Both Silverman et al 2007 and New et al 2007 tested a hypothesis about sex differences

More information

Sexual peak: Socially shared cognitions about desire, frequency, and satisfaction in men and women

Sexual peak: Socially shared cognitions about desire, frequency, and satisfaction in men and women Personal Relationships, 9 (2002), 287 299. Printed in the United States of America. Copyright # 2002 ISSPR. 1350-4126/02 Sexual peak: Socially shared cognitions about desire, frequency, and satisfaction

More information

NANCY FUGATE WOODS a a University of Washington

NANCY FUGATE WOODS a a University of Washington This article was downloaded by: [ ] On: 30 June 2011, At: 09:44 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer

More information

Chapter 9-Sexuality-Psy222

Chapter 9-Sexuality-Psy222 Chapter 9-Sexuality-Psy222 Chapter 9-Sexuality There are few components of relationships that have seen as much evolution and change as that of sexuality Our attitudes about sex have become increasingly

More information

Cooperation between a man and a woman is virtually a. Sex, Lies, and Strategic Interference: The Psychology of Deception Between the Sexes

Cooperation between a man and a woman is virtually a. Sex, Lies, and Strategic Interference: The Psychology of Deception Between the Sexes 10.1177/0146167204271303 PERSONALITY Haselton et al. / AND SEX, SOCIAL LIES, AND PSYCHOLOGY INTERFERENCE BULLETIN Sex, Lies, and Strategic Interference: The Psychology of Deception Between the Sexes Martie

More information

Human mate poaching can be defined as a special

Human mate poaching can be defined as a special 10.1177/0146167203253471 PERSONALITY Schmitt, Shackelford AND / SOCIAL MATE POACHING PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN ARTICLE Nifty Ways to Leave Your Lover: The Tactics People Use to Entice and Disguise the Process

More information

Sexual Conflict in Humans: Evolutionary Consequences of Asymmetric Parental Investment and Paternity Uncertainty

Sexual Conflict in Humans: Evolutionary Consequences of Asymmetric Parental Investment and Paternity Uncertainty Sexual Conflict in Humans p.1 [in press, Animal Biology, February 2009] Sexual Conflict in Humans: Evolutionary Consequences of Asymmetric Parental Investment and Paternity Uncertainty Aaron T. Goetz 1

More information

The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction

The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction I. Introduction to Behavioral Ecology Behavioral ecologists test hypotheses about the adaptive function of a behavior Behavioral Ecology Behavioral ecologists

More information

Women s Life History Attributes are Associated with Preferences in Mating Relationships

Women s Life History Attributes are Associated with Preferences in Mating Relationships Evolutionary Psychology www.epjournal.net 2008. 6(2): 289-302 Original Article Women s Life History Attributes are Associated with Preferences in Mating Relationships Daniel J. Kruger, School of Public

More information

PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions

PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions Session 3 SEXUAL MOTIVES: THE NATURE & SEXUAL PRACTICES Lecturer: Mrs. Jennifer Sasu - Mensah, Psychology Department Contact Information: jpeprah@ug.edu.gh College of Education

More information

Types of behaviors that are elicited in response to simple stimuli

Types of behaviors that are elicited in response to simple stimuli Lecture 19: Animal Behavior I. Background A. Animal behavior reflects and arises from biological properties 1. Exhibited behavior defends on the physiological systems and processes unique to a given organism

More information

Hypothesized Fitness Indicators and Mating Success A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY OF THE STATE UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK AT NEW PALTZ

Hypothesized Fitness Indicators and Mating Success A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY OF THE STATE UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK AT NEW PALTZ Hypothesized Fitness Indicators and Success A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY OF THE STATE UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK AT NEW PALTZ IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE

More information

HOOKUPS TO ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS: SEXUAL BEHAVIORS IN VARIOUS PARTNERSHIPS JENNIFER A. SHUKUSKY. A thesis submitted to the. Graduate School-Camden

HOOKUPS TO ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS: SEXUAL BEHAVIORS IN VARIOUS PARTNERSHIPS JENNIFER A. SHUKUSKY. A thesis submitted to the. Graduate School-Camden HOOKUPS TO ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIPS: SEXUAL BEHAVIORS IN VARIOUS PARTNERSHIPS by JENNIFER A. SHUKUSKY A thesis submitted to the Graduate School-Camden Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey in partial

More information

Personality and Individual Differences

Personality and Individual Differences Personality and Individual Differences 47 (2009) 476 480 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Waist hip ratio

More information

Status, prestige, and social dominance

Status, prestige, and social dominance Status, prestige, and social dominance Does status have fitness benefits? Status-seeking and costly signalling Sex differences in competition and dominance Hormones, competition, and status Status and

More information

Sexual selection. Intrasexual selection mating success determined by within-sex interactions e.g., male-male combat

Sexual selection. Intrasexual selection mating success determined by within-sex interactions e.g., male-male combat Sexual dimorphism Sexual selection Is sexual selection different from natural selection? Darwin saw them as distinct - only sexual selection could produce traits that compromise survival The basic principle

More information

Male Financial Consumption is Associated with Higher Mating Intentions and Mating Success

Male Financial Consumption is Associated with Higher Mating Intentions and Mating Success Evolutionary Psychology www.epjournal.net 2008. 6(4): 603-612 Original Article Male Financial Consumption is Associated with Higher Mating Intentions and Mating Success Daniel J. Kruger, School of Public

More information

Supplemental Material. Romance, Risk, and Replication: Nisha Hickin, Amanda J. F. Tamman, and Lara M. C. Puhlmann

Supplemental Material. Romance, Risk, and Replication: Nisha Hickin, Amanda J. F. Tamman, and Lara M. C. Puhlmann Supplemental Material Romance, Risk, and Replication: Can Consumer Choices and Risk-Taking be Primed by Mating Motives? David R. Shanks, Miguel A. Vadillo, Benjamin Riedel, Ashley Clymo, Sinita Govind,

More information

SEX-DIFFERENCES IN PREFERENCE FOR LOOKING AT THE FACE OR BODY IN SHORT-TERM AND LONG-TERM MATING CONTEXTS

SEX-DIFFERENCES IN PREFERENCE FOR LOOKING AT THE FACE OR BODY IN SHORT-TERM AND LONG-TERM MATING CONTEXTS Evolution, Mind and Behaviour 13(2015), 1-17 DOI: 10.1556/2050.2015.0003 SEX-DIFFERENCES IN PREFERENCE FOR LOOKING AT THE FACE OR BODY IN SHORT-TERM AND LONG-TERM MATING CONTEXTS DANIELLE L. WAGSTAFF 1

More information

Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece. Published online: 13 Nov 2014.

Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece. Published online: 13 Nov 2014. This article was downloaded by: [Dimitrios Pnevmatikos] On: 14 November 2014, At: 22:15 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2002, 16 (2), BRIEF REPORT. Forgiveness or breakup: Sex differences in responses to a partner s infidelity

COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2002, 16 (2), BRIEF REPORT. Forgiveness or breakup: Sex differences in responses to a partner s infidelity COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2002, 16 (2), 299 307 BRIEF REPORT Forgiveness or breakup: Sex differences in responses to a partner s infidelity Todd K. Shackelford Florida Atlantic University, Davie, USA David

More information

Personality and Self-Esteem in Newlyweds. Todd K. Shackelford. Oakland University. Richard L. Michalski. Hollins University

Personality and Self-Esteem in Newlyweds. Todd K. Shackelford. Oakland University. Richard L. Michalski. Hollins University Running head: PERSONALITY AND SELF-ESTEEM [in press, Personality and Individual Differences, July 2011] Personality and Self-Esteem in Newlyweds Todd K. Shackelford Oakland University Richard L. Michalski

More information

Female Aggression and Evolutionary Theory

Female Aggression and Evolutionary Theory Page 1 of 8 Home Current Issue Editorial Board Instructions for Authors Contact Lethbridge Undergraduate Research Journal ISSN 1718-8482 Disclaimer: The work represented here is entirely the creation of

More information

Evolutionary Psychology. by Elizabeth Anderson

Evolutionary Psychology. by Elizabeth Anderson Evolutionary Psychology by Elizabeth Anderson Questions! What, according to Evolutionary Psychologists, do humans look for in a mate?! How might rape be explained according to Evolutionary Psychology?

More information