The evolution of social philopatry and dispersal in female mammals

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The evolution of social philopatry and dispersal in female mammals"

Transcription

1 Molecular Ecology (2012) 21, doi: /j X x INVITED REVIEW The evolution of social philopatry and dispersal in female mammals T. H. CLUTTON-BROCK and D. LUKAS Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, UK Abstract In most social mammals, some females disperse from their natal group while others remain and breed there throughout their lives but, in a few, females typically disperse after adolescence and few individuals remain and breed in their natal group. These contrasts in philopatry and dispersal have an important consequence on the kinship structure of groups which, in turn, affects forms of social relationships between females. As yet, there is still widespread disagreement over the reasons for the evolution of habitual female dispersal, partly as a result of contrasting definitions of dispersal. This paper reviews variation in the frequency with which females leave their natal group or range (social dispersal) and argues that both the avoidance of local competition for resources and breeding opportunities and the need to find unrelated partners play an important role in contrasts between and within species. Keywords: dispersal, mammals, philopatry, sociality Received 17 March 2011; revision received 17 May 2011; accepted 19 May 2011 Introduction Correspondence: Tim H. Clutton-Brock, Fax: ; thcb@cam.ac.uk Among social mammals, there are marked differences in the kinship structure of female groups: in most species, female group members are philopatric, matrilineal relatives, but in a few species they are typically unrelated immigrants from other groups (Wrangham 1980; Pusey & Packer 1987; Clutton-Brock 2009). These contrasts in kin structure have important consequences for the evolution of social behaviour: where most female group members are close relatives, groups are usually stable and cooperation between females is relatively common while, where females are unrelated, females often move between social groups and cooperative behaviour is seldom highly developed (Sterck 1997; Clutton-Brock 2009). Interspecific differences in the kinship structure of social groups are a consequence of contrasting patterns of female philopatry and dispersal: in some species, most females remain and breed in their natal group throughout their lives and female group members are mostly close relatives while, in others, females usually leave their natal group as adolescents or young adults and either die, join established breeding groups or found new breeding units and female group members are typically unrelated to each other (Lukas et al. 2005). The causes of differences in female philopatry and dispersal have attracted interest for more than 30 years but there is still little agreement as to the importance of different evolutionary mechanisms (Greenwood 1980; Moore & Ali 1984; Pusey 1987; Clutton-Brock 1989; Perrin & Mazalov 1999; Ronce 2007; Clobert et al. 2008). One contributory factor is that the evolution of philopatry and dispersal is relevant to many distinct areas of population biology and has been considered by scientists from different disciplines with contrasting agendas, including population demographers, population geneticists interested in the movement of individuals between local populations or demes (Johnson & Gaines 1990; Clobert et al. 2008) and behavioural ecologists, interested principally in the social mechanisms affecting the tendency of individuals to remain in their natal group or to leave it to search for breeding opportunities elsewhere (Brown 1987; Ekman & Rosander 1992; Kokko & Ekman 2002). As a result, the definitions of philopatry and dispersal used in different studies have varied widely (Reed 1993; Bowler & Benton 2005) confusing comparisons and complicating conclusions. On one hand, population geneticists and demographers

2 PHILOPATRY AND DISPERSAL IN FEMALE MAMMALS 473 commonly use philopatry to refer to the continued presence of individuals within their natal population or deme and dispersal to occur when individuals leave their natal population (see Mayr 1963; Shields 1982; Reed 1993; Laporte & Charlesworth 2002): for example, Shields (1987) defines philopatry as limited dispersal such that the average propagule moves no more than ten home ranges away from its site of origin and regards both sexes as philopatric if neither exceeds this limit. In contrast, behavioural biologists interested in the kinship structure of female groups and the causes of immigration and emigration have commonly defined dispersal as movements out of the natal group or range (Greenwood 1980; Waser & Jones 1983; Clutton-Brock 1989; Waser 1996). Since many individuals that leave their natal group settle within less than ten home range diameters (e.g. Cockburn et al. 1985; Pope 2000; Sutherland et al. 2000; Radespiel et al. 2003) these two approaches commonly produce different results. A second problem is that some studies of dispersal have classified females as philopatric or dispersive on the basis of whether they leave their natal territory or group (Clutton-Brock 1989) while others define sex differences in dispersal as the ratio of mean male dispersal distance to mean female dispersal distance and have classified females on the basis of whether they move further from their natal territory than males (Clarke et al. 1997; Miller et al. 2011). Where classifications are based on sex differences in dispersal distance (Greenwood 1980), species whose females are classified as philopatric commonly include species in which most or all females leave their natal territory to breed but do not move as far as males (as, for example, in many passerine birds) as well as species where females typically breed in their natal group or range. The factors affecting whether individuals leave or remain in their natal range or group are often likely to differ from those affecting the distance they move if they do leave. In many social species, social factors operating within their natal group (including the intensity of aggression directed at them by resident breeding females, their need for alliances with relatives and the availability of unrelated breeding partners) play an important role in determining whether or not females leave their natal group and, if so, which individuals remain and which stay (see below). In contrast, the distances moved by individuals once they have left their natal group are likely to be strongly affected by immediate ecological parameters, including the quality and availability of vacant habitat and breeding partners and there is limited evidence that individuals that have left their natal group continue to discriminate against breeding with relatives (see below). A third problem contributing to ongoing discussions of the evolution of philopatry and dispersal is that empirical data providing reliable and accurate estimates of the frequency of dispersal and the distances moved are scarce. Unless large numbers of individuals are marked it is seldom possible to distinguish between mortality and dispersal while even in studies where all individuals are marked or recognisable, it is frequently difficult to determine whether individuals that disappear have died or dispersed (Waser et al. 1994; Creel & Creel 2002). If the practical difficulties of estimating dispersal frequency are substantial, those of measuring and comparing dispersal distances are far greater. In most vertebrates, individuals are usually difficult to follow once they have left their natal group or range and those that settle close to their group of origin are more likely to be detected than those that move further, so that dispersal distances are often likely to be systematically biased towards shorter distances (van Noordwijk 1995; Koenig et al. 1996; Sharp et al. 2008). Where records of dispersal distance are based on a small proportion of the initial sample (as, for example, in most studies of ringing in birds) biases may be very strong, their magnitude is seldom possible to assess (Koenig et al. 1996) and it may often be unsafe to assume that biases affect both sexes equally and comparisons of mean dispersal distance may conceal important differences in variance (Sharp et al. 2008). For example, in species where one sex commonly remains and breeds in their natal group (and is consequently classified as philopatric), individuals that do disperse are often unable to join established breeding groups and move further than individuals of the sex that disperses more frequently (Pope 2000; Ji et al. 2001; Blundell et al. 2002), so that comparisons of mean dispersal distance mask important differences in distribution. The recent development of extensive genetic sampling to assign parentage represents an important development which has provided new insights into dispersal in a number of rodents (Telfer et al. 2003; Aars et al. 2006; Waser et al. 2006; Waser & Hadfield 2011) as well as in other animals (Christie et al. 2011; Planes & Lemer 2011) but its application to larger, more mobile mammals where it is difficult to catch or sample large numbers of individuals is likely to be limited by logistic problems. Moreover, the application of paternity or other genetic (Prugnolle & de Meeûs 2002; Lawson-Handley & Perrin 2007) or demographic (Abadi et al. 2010) techniques does not solve the problem that, unless the behaviour of recognisable individuals can be monitored, it is usually difficult to gain a reliable understanding of the reasons why they leave their natal groups or of the factors affecting how far they move. Without studies at this level, it is seldom possible to test the assumptions of theoretical models of dispersal and there is a danger that models will be developed that do not reflect the

3 474 T. H. CLUTTON-BROCK and D. LUKAS biological processes important in natural populations. For example, some recent models of dispersal have been based on the assumption that local resource competition (Clark 1978) favours the evolution of dispersal from groups that include a high proportion of close kin (Hamilton & May 1977; Perrin & Mazalov 2000) whereas the available evidence suggests that the presence of relatives increases the probability that individuals will remain in their natal group (see below). In this situation, we believe that it is useful to distinguish between questions concerning the reasons why social animals leave their natal group or territory and questions concerning the distances they move after leaving their natal group. This paper reviews our understanding of the evolution of social philopatry and dispersal from the natal group or territory in social mammals. We treat females as philopatric if they remain and breed in their natal range or group and we use natal dispersal to refer to permanent movement out of this area and secondary dispersal to refer to permanent or semi-permanent movements of breeding adults that have previously left their natal group or range. Inevitably, attempts to apply a single definition of dispersal distance face a number of ambiguities (Creel & Creel 2002): for example, in some ungulates, both sexes occupy distinct breeding ranges that are separate from those they use throughout the rest of the year (e.g. Clutton-Brock et al. 1982b; Nelson & Mech 1987). In other cases, individuals of one sex leave their natal group but often settle in part of their natal territory with immigrants of the opposite sex (Creel & Creel 2002). And in some cases, groups sub-divide and occupy part of their original range or parents abandon part of their range to their offspring (Berteaux & Boutin 2000). There is no single definition that is entirely satisfactory in all circumstances and, in such cases, we treat individuals that have left their natal group and associate with unrelated animals of the opposite sex as having dispersed. We initially describe contrasts in female philopatry between and within species (section Variation in female philopatry and dispersal ) before reviewing the benefits of philopatry (section The benefits of philopatry ) and dispersal (section The benefits of dispersal ) and the evolution of species differences in female philopatry (section Species differences in female philopatry ), and of sex differences in dispersal (section The evolution of sex differences in philopatry ). Variation in female philopatry and dispersal Different species of mammals show marked contrasts in the frequency of reproductive philopatry and natal dispersal in females as well as in the incidence of secondary dispersal (Pusey 1987; Nunes 2007). At one extreme, are species, like many cercopithecine primates, where most females have spent their entire lives in their natal group, female immigration into other groups is rare or nonexistent and the formation of new groups is uncommon. In these species, most female group members are matrilineal relatives though, where group size is large and female lifespans are long, groups can include several matrilineal sub-groups whose members often compete with each other (Silk 2007). Since females that leave their natal group are seldom able to join other established breeding groups and the formation of new breeding groups is uncommon, most successful cases of female dispersal involve groups fissioning and occupying a part of their previous range, as in some social rodents, several bats and many baboons and macaques (Altmann et al. 1985; Armitage 1991; Alberts & Altmann 1995; Hoogland 1995; Thierry 2007; Kerth 2008; Armitage et al. 2011). In most of these species, males habitually disperse to join other groups at or soon after adolescence so that male and female group members are seldom close relatives (Pusey & Packer 1987) but, in a few species, including killer whales and pilot whales, both females and males commonly remain in their natal group but females typically mate with members of other social groups (Amos et al. 1993; Baird 2000). In addition, in naked mole-rats, both sexes usually remain in their natal group though some males disperse and breeding between close relatives may be common (Honeycutt et al. 1991a,b; ORiain & Jarvis 1997) though females still show a preference for unrelated males (Clarke & Faulkes 1999). At the other extreme, there are a number of social mammals where most females leave their natal group or territory as adolescents or young adults and, if they survive, breed in other territories or groups so that resident breeding females are almost always immigrants born in different groups or ranges from the one they breed in, and are seldom closely related to each other. They include two distinct groups of species. First, there are species where breeding females occupy individual ranges or territories (like many of the insectivores and smaller carnivores as well as many rodents) or live in mixed-sex pairs or in groups that include a single breeding female (like many nocturnal carnivores and primates) (Gaines & Mcclenaghan 1980; Sandell 1989; van Schaik & Kappeler 2003; Nunes 2007). In these singular breeders, adolescent females may either be evicted from their natal territory by the dominant female or may leave voluntarily. Though females do not move as far as males in some species, they rarely breed in their natal territory, so it is appropriate to regard them as having dispersed. Second, there are a small number of plural breeders where several breeding females live in groups defended by one or

4 PHILOPATRY AND DISPERSAL IN FEMALE MAMMALS 475 more males and almost all females leave their natal breeding group as adolescents or young adults and join other breeding units, so that female group members are typically unrelated. Examples include most social equids, several tropical bats, spider monkeys and woolly spider monkeys, red colobus monkeys, hamadryas baboons and all three of the African great apes (Wrangham 1980; Berger 1987; Pusey 1987; Strier & Ziegler 2000; Korstjens & Schippers 2003; Hammond et al. 2006; di Fiore & Campbell 2007; Stumpf 2007). In many of these species, social bonds between breeding females are weak, groups often disband if the resident male dies or is displaced and secondary dispersal of females is common. Between these extremes lie a wide variety of species where some females breed in their natal groups and others disperse to breed. In some species, many females leave as adolescents or young adults but a proportion remain and breed in their natal group: for example, in meerkats dominant females evict all subordinates above a threshold age, so that groups include one mature breeding female and several younger subordinates (Clutton-Brock et al. 2006). In many of these species, dispersing females are usually prevented from joining established breeding groups by residents so that, unless they displace resident females, dispersers either establish new breeding groups or die: examples include African lions, meerkats, golden lion tamarins, red howler monkeys and black-tailed prairie dogs (Pusey & Packer 1987; Crockett & Pope 1993; Hoogland 1995; SilleroZubiri et al. 1996; Creel & Creel 2002; Clutton-Brock et al. 2006; Vander Waal et al. 2009). In other cases, dispersing females are sometimes able to join established groups so that breeding groups may include a mixture of related and unrelated females, as in some Asian colobine monkeys (Kirkpatrick 2007) and some ungulates where related females forage independently but share overlapping home ranges (Clutton-Brock et al. 1982a). Finally, in many of the more wide ranging semi-migratory or migratory species, including some bats and several of the larger herbivores, bonds between mature females are often weak or absent and adolescent females disperse into local populations after separating from their mothers (Estes 1974; Clutton-Brock et al. 2002; Kerth 2008b). However, if populations are geographically divided or opportunities for dispersal are constrained, related females may still encounter each other more frequently than unrelated females (Pratt & Anderson 1985; Bashaw et al. 2007; Bradley et al. 2007; Kerth 2008a, b). The proximate causes of female dispersal also vary. In some species, adolescent females or young adults are evicted from their natal group by adult females who are frequently close relatives: for example, in meerkats, females only disperse after the dominant female in their group drives them out by force (Clutton-Brock et al. 1998). In others, including many rodents and some primates, dispersing females appear to leave their group voluntarily (Bekoff 1977; Moore & Ali 1984; Wolff 1993; Nunes 2007). Within species, the relative frequency of dispersal commonly increases with group size (Pope 2000; Clutton-Brock et al. 2008; Vander Waal et al. 2009) or with local population density (Matthysen 2005) and is reduced by experimental reductions in population density (Brody & Armitage 1985; Aars & Ims 2000). In many mammals, social dispersal represents a conditional strategy and the probability that individuals will leave their natal group and the timing of leaving are affected by their relative size, condition and development of individuals and, in some cases, by their genotype, too (Bowler & Benton 2005; Nunes 2007; Ronce 2007; Clobert et al. 2009): for example, female meadow voles that are malnourished during early development but are well nourished as adults show a stronger tendency to disperse (Bondrup-Nielsen 1993). In some cases, smaller and more subordinate individuals are more likely to disperse (Hanski et al. 1991; Bowler & Benton 2005) while, in others, larger or heavier individuals are more likely to disperse (Nunes et al. 1998; Clutton-Brock et al. 2002). Rates of female dispersal and, in some cases, dispersal distance, are also commonly associated with variation in hormone levels: for example, experimental increases in androgen levels at the time of birth raise the tendency for female ground squirrels to disperse as adolescents or young adults (Holekamp 1984; Nunes et al. 1999). In addition, the availability of vacant territories may be important. For example, studies of several group-living birds and mammals show that the frequency of dispersal increases with the availability of vacant territories or breeding opportunities outside the group (Dobson 1981; Brody & Armitage 1985; Komdeur et al. 1995; Ekman et al. 2004). Conversely, the probability that individuals will remain in their natal group may be affected by chance of breeding there. For example, as mothers age and their probability of survival falls, the relative benefits of philopatry to their daughters are likely to increase and studies of common lizards show that the offspring of older mothers are less likely to disperse (Ronce et al. 1998). In a number of species where females live in relatively large groups, cohesion declines as group size increases and large groups may fission into two or more new groups (Henzi et al. 1997; Waterman 2002; Lefebvre et al. 2003; Archie et al. 2006). When groups split, maternal kinship commonly affects which group individuals eventually belong to and fissioning often raises coefficients of relatedness between group members (Chepko-Sade et al. 1979a, b; Van Horn

5 476 T. H. CLUTTON-BROCK and D. LUKAS et al. 2007). In species where paternal kin tend to associate, paternal kinship, too, may influence the membership of sub-groups. For example, studies of baboons, macaques and gorillas show that when troops fission paternal relatives also tend to join the same sub-group (Smith 2000; Widdig et al. 2006; Nsubuga et al. 2008). Variation in kinship between neighbouring groups may also affect the distance moved. For example, in high density populations of African lions, females are less likely to disperse from prides surrounded by large numbers of unrelated females and, when prides fission, daughters settle relatively close to their mother s pride (Vander Waal et al. 2009). The size of dispersing splinter groups also differs between species. Where females occupy separate ranges or territories, individuals usually disperse alone and independent dispersal is also common in species where females are readily accepted into established breeding groups. In contrast, where dispersing females cannot join established breeding groups, several females often disperse together: for example, in meerkats, several females are commonly evicted at the same time and often disperse together (Clutton-Brock et al. 1998). Similarly, in banded mongooses, established breeding females evict multiple younger females in sporadic eviction events and younger females disperse together and either die, join established breeding groups or found new breeding units (Gilchrist 2008; Cant et al. 2010). Where females disperse together, new breeding units often consist of a mixture of descendant and nondescendant relatives, though dominant females may subsequently evict non-descendant relatives so that established groups eventually consist of a dominant female and her descendant kin (Young et al. 2006; Clutton-Brock et al. 2008). In cases where dispersal occurs primarily through group fission, as in many of the baboons and macaques (Okamoto 2004), members of the same matrilineal group join the same sub-group and are seldom evicted, so that many breeding units include a mixture of descendant and non-descendant relatives. The age at which females disperse differs widely between species. In plural breeders where female dispersal usually involves groups fissioning, splinter groups commonly involve old as well as young females and dispersal may occur in females of any age (Okamoto 2004). Where breeding females occupy separate ranges or territories, subordinates usually leave (or are evicted) soon after reaching sexual maturity (Nunes et al. 1997). In contrast, in singular cooperative breeders, females may remain in their natal group as nonbreeders for several years before they leave or are evicted: for example, in meerkats, females reach sexual maturity at a year but are seldom evicted from their natal group until they are at least 2-years old, though few are tolerated after they are 4 years (Clutton-Brock et al. 2008). Different species of cooperative breeders differ in the age at which natal females are evicted and these differences have an important influence on the size of social groups. Finally, the distances moved by dispersing females range from less than a hundred metres in some rodents (Gaines & Mcclenaghan 1980; Jones 1987; Stenseth & Lidicker 1992), through distances of a few hundred metres in the case of the more sedentary primates (Pope 2000) to 20 km or more in some carnivores (Creel & Creel 2002) and much larger distances in some cetaceans (Whitehead & Weilgart 2000). Intraspecific variation in the distance moved can be affected by the kinship structure of local groups. For example, in high density populations of African lions, females are less likely to disperse from prides surrounded by large numbers of unrelated females and, if prides fission, daughters settle relatively close to their mothers (Vander Waal et al. 2009). Interspecific differences in the distances moved reflect variation in relative mobility (Sutherland et al. 2000) as well as differences in the availability of vacant habitat, which is likely to be inversely related to variation in survival. In addition, a substantial number of studies have shown that there are often marked differences in the average distance moved between the sexes and the origin of these differences is still debated (see below). The benefits of philopatry Dispersal from the natal group often has high costs unless dispersing females can join other breeding groups (Berger 1987; Van Vuren & Armitage 1994; Nunes 2007; Ronce 2007; Doligez & Part 2008; Strier 2008). In many species, dispersing individuals are likely to lack detailed knowledge of the distribution of resources accrued by older residents, their feeding efficiency may be impaired and cortisol levels may be increased to a level where immune responses are affected: for example, studies of African elephants feeding in a novel environment show that foraging efficiency increases and foraging time declines as they get to know the area (Pinter-Wollman et al. 2009). Similarly, dispersing meerkats have lower rates of weight gain while foraging than residents, levels of glucocorticoids rise and they suffer higher parasite load (Young & Monfort 2009). As well as affecting survival, the energetic costs of dispersal may delay breeding and reduce reproductive potential (Ronce 2007; Fisher et al. 2009). Individuals that disperse to unfamiliar areas may also be more vulnerable to predators: for example, experimental studies show that dispersing white-footed

6 PHILOPATRY AND DISPERSAL IN FEMALE MAMMALS 477 mice are more susceptible than residents to predation by owls (Metzgar 1967). In addition, they are often likely to be attacked by members of resident groups, sometimes with fatal consequences (Fritts & Mech 1981; Packer & Pusey 1982; Boydston et al. 2001; Creel & Creel 2002). High rates of mortality in dispersers may be particularly common in carnivores, where attacks by residents are likely to be dangerous (Waser 1996): for example, grey wolves making extra-territorial forays die at five times the rate of residents (Messier 1985) while mortality rates in dispersing African wild dogs are 2.7 times higher than those of residents in females and 6.8 times higher in males (Creel & Creel 2002). Several studies have also shown that dispersal is associated with substantial increases in mortality in many other species (Errington 1963; Van Vuren & Armitage 1994): for example, in red howler monkeys, 43 52% of dispersing females are suspected or known to die (Crockett & Pope 1993). As well as suffering the direct costs of moving between groups, dispersing females lose the potential benefits of associating with kin and both empirical results and theoretical models suggest that this may have an important influence on the probability that they will disperse (Lambin et al. 2001; Perrin & Goudet 2001; Silk 2007). Females are often more tolerant of kin than non-kin and associating with kin can have important benefits to breeding success or survival (Silk 2007; Krebs et al. 2007). Some of the clearest evidence comes from studies of rodents where females occupy individual home ranges that overlap those of neighbouring females. For example, in voles, females show a preference for settling close to relatives and individuals with ranges close to kin breed earlier (Pusenius et al. 1998), rear more offspring (Lambin & Yoccoz 1998) and show higher rates of survival in the next breeding season (Lambin & Krebs 1993) than individuals with ranges close to non-kin. In Alpine marmots, infants are more likely to survive their first winter in hibernation groups consisting largely of close relatives than in groups where most individuals are not closely related (Arnold 1990a,b) while the breeding success of dominant females is depressed by the number of unrelated subordinate females in the group but not by the number of daughters present (Hacklander et al. 2003). Similar effects of associating with kin occur in other social mammals: for example, in grey seals, females that breed in areas where individuals are closely related produced larger and faster growing pups than females in areas where relatedness was relatively low (Pomeroy et al. 2000, 2001); in red howler monkeys, females that recruit into their natal group and associate with relatives give birth at earlier ages and have higher breeding success than individuals that disperse and breed in groups consisting largely or exclusively of unrelated females (Crockett & Pope 1993); and, in white-faced capuchins, the length of inter-birth intervals are negatively related to the number of matrilineal kin in their group (Fedigan et al. 2008). Where comparisons of breeding success or survival between philopatric individuals and dispersers are based solely on observational data, there is a danger that local variation in habitat quality generates consistent differences in survival rates in successive generations, leading to correlations between breeding success and association with kin that have no causal basis. However, several rodent studies that have either manipulated the proportion of relatives in local populations or housed animals with kin versus non-kin have shown that the presence of kin exert distinct effects on breeding success or survival: for example, experiments with several voles show that associating with kin increases fecundity and, in some cases, survival, too (Kawata 1990; Sera & Gaines 1994) though it does not always do so (Boonstra & Hogg 1988; Sera & Gaines 1994; Dalton 2000). Similar effects of the presence of kin have also been demonstrated in house mice (Dobson et al. 2000; Rusu & Krackow 2004), where association with kin increases the lifetime breeding success of females (Fig. 1, König 1994). There are probably several different reasons why associating with kin leads to improved fecundity or breeding success. In many rodents, relatives are more tolerant of each other s presence than unrelated females, rates of aggression between relatives are lower than between unrelated females and home range overlap Number of offspring weaned First female Second female Familiar Unfamiliar Familiar Unamiliar/ related unrelated unrelated related Fig. 1 Effects of the presence of kin on the lifetime breeding success of female house mice in artificial groups (data from König 1994). The median number of offspring weaned within a standardized lifespan in four types artificially created social groups of house mice is the highest if individuals are paired with another female which is both familiar and related. While the first female to start breeding (grey columns) only has a lowered reproductive success when paired with an unfamiliar, unrelated female, the second female (black columns) always produces fewer offspring if not paired with a familiar, related female.

7 478 T. H. CLUTTON-BROCK and D. LUKAS between relatives is often greater (Sera & Gaines 1994; Mappes et al. 1995): for example, the reproductive success of female voles in enclosures seeded with unrelated females is negatively correlated with their proximity to neighbours but this disappears when kin are involved (Mappes et al. 1995; Pusenius et al. 1998). Relatives may also be more tolerant of each other s offspring and the risk of female infanticide may decline where neighbours are close relatives (Lambin & Yoccoz 1998; Dalton 2000). In addition, related females may assist each other in defending resources or repelling intruders and younger individuals often benefit from the presence of older relatives (Silk 2007): for example, in wood rats, females whose mothers are present in their group produce more offspring than those whose mothers are absent (Moses & Millar 1994) and similar effects occur in some primates (Fairbanks & Mcguire 1987; Pavelka et al. 2002; Cheney et al. 2004). In some voles and mice, several breeding females often nest together, pool their young and nurse them communally and this practice can improve breeding success and reduce rearing costs to females (Koenig 1994; Hayes 2000): for example, in prairie voles (where communal breeding is common) the presence of additional breeding sisters is associated with reduced weight loss in mothers if food is scarce as well as with improved growth in their pups (Hayes 2000). However, communal breeding is not always associated with improved breeding success: for example, in populations of whitefooted mice, some females form communal breeding groups with relatives where population density is relatively high while others breed alone (Wolff 1994) but, in contrast to prairie voles and house mice, there is no obvious effect of communal breeding on reproductive success (Wolff 1994). The benefits of dispersal The substantial benefits of philopatry suggest that, where females disperse to breed, they must gain large benefits by leaving their natal range or group. Four main categories of benefits have been suggested. First, dispersal may allow subordinate females to escape from groups or ranges where there is intense competition for resources or breeding opportunities with other individuals (Clark 1978; Ekman & Rosander 1992; Perrin & Mazalov 2000; Matthysen 2005; Clobert et al. 2008). In many social species, subordinate females suffer from competition for resources or breeding opportunities with more dominant group members, leading to a reduction in both their fecundity and the survival of their offspring (Clutton-Brock et al. 1982b, 2006; Hoogland 1995): for example, in yellow-bellied marmots, competition between mothers and daughters often delays the age of first reproduction in daughters with costs to the daughter s lifetime fitness (Van Vuren & Armitage 1994). In some populations, the proportion of females that disperse increases with population density, supporting the suggestion that females commonly disperse to avoid competition for limited resources. For example, in several social rodents, the probability that females disperse increases with population density (Lidicker & Patton 1987; Nunes 2007). Increased rates of dispersal are often stimulated by short-term changes in resource abundance (Nunes 2007) and dispersing females frequently move to areas of higher food abundance. For example, in Eurasian red squirrels, dispersing females move to areas where food is relatively abundant (Lurz et al. 1997). In other cases, dispersal rates increase if groups exceed the optimal size for their members. For example, in African lions, female dispersal increases in groups that exceed the habitat-specified optimum (Vander Waal et al. 2009). A number of studies have manipulated the local distribution of resources or competitors and shown that this affects patterns of dispersal: for example in California ground squirrels, supplementation of local food resources increases rates of emigration from un-supplemented to supplemented areas (Dobson 1979) while in pocket gophers (Sullivan et al. 2001), yellow-bellied marmots (Brody & Armitage 1985) and California ground squirrels (Dobson 1981) the experimental removal of individuals or groups has been shown to increase immigration from neighbouring areas. However, dispersal is not associated with competition for resources in all cases: for example, in savannah baboons, females normally remain in their natal group or range, even if there is strong competition for resources (Alberts & Altmann 1995) while, in horses, almost all females emigrate from their natal group, even if population density is relatively low and resources are abundant (Berger 1986). Moreover, in some cases, high population density is associated with reductions in dispersal rather than increases: for example, in some rodents the saturation of local environments is associated with reductions in dispersal and increased retention of daughters (and, in some cases, sons) in the natal range (Gundersen & Andreassen 1998; Lambin et al. 2001) while in brown bears, high population density is associated with reduction of the dispersal distance of females (Støen et al. 2006). In addition, social dispersal may allow females to escape from attempts by dominant females to restrict their ability to conceive, breed and raise young. In some species where females defend individual territories as well as in some socially monogamous species, young adult females become the target of aggression from the resident breeding female (who is often their mother) and are eventually forced to leave (Tilson 1981; Komers

8 PHILOPATRY AND DISPERSAL IN FEMALE MAMMALS 479 & Brotherton 1997; Nunes 2007). Similarly, in some singular cooperative breeders (including meerkats, African wild dogs and tamarins) older subordinate females become the target of aggression from dominant females and eventually leave the group or are evicted (Dietz & Baker 1993; Clutton-Brock et al. 1998; Creel & Creel 2002; Goldizen 2003). In several of these species, the probability that subordinates will be evicted rises with group size (Clutton-Brock et al. 2008; Cant et al. 2010) and increase at times when dominants or subordinates are attempting to breed (Clutton-Brock et al. 1998b; Ebensperger 1998a,b; Creel & Creel 2002). In other species, younger females appear to leave voluntarily to seek breeding opportunities in other groups (Komers & Brotherton 1997; Brockelman et al. 1998; Goldizen 2003; McGuire et al. 2007). Second, theoretical analyses of dispersal have suggested females may disperse to avoid the indirect costs of competing with kin (Hamilton & May 1977; Gandon 1999; Perrin & Mazalov 2000). However, there is little or no evidence that the presence of maternal kin commonly stimulates voluntary dispersal, after the effects of local density or group size on dispersal frequency have been allowed for (Wolff 1992; Lambin et al. 2001). For example, in root voles, adolescent females exposed either to their mother or to an unrelated breeding female were no more likely to show reproductive suppression or to attempt to disperse in the presence of their mother than in the presence of an unrelated female (Le Galliard et al. 2007). Conversely, in several species, there is evidence that breeding females are more tolerant of closer relatives than of more distant relatives of unrelated individuals and that this is associated with reduced rates of dispersal in close kin. For example, subordinate female meerkats are less likely to be evicted from their natal group by the dominant female if they are close relatives (Clutton-Brock et al. 2010). While in yellow-bellied marmots, the presence of a female s mother reduces the probability that she will disperse (Armitage et al. 2011), probably because mothers provide support that enhances the ability of younger females to compete with other colony members (Bekoff 1977). In addition, in a number of species that rely on predictable, localised resources, including red squirrels (Price & Boutin 1993; Berteaux & Boutin 2000) and Columbian ground squirrels (Harris & Murie 1984), breeding females that have reared offspring alter their ranging behaviour (Fig. 2), leaving their offspring to use part of their previous range (Nunes 2007). Strategies of this kind are probably best interpreted as examples of extended parental care and, as yet, there is no evidence that females treat non-descendant kin in the same way. Percentage of individuals (a) (b) (c) (d) Keep territory n = Share territory n = Bequeath territory n = Juveniles n = Distance moved (m) Fig. 2 Bequeathal in red squirrels (data from Berteaux & Boutin 2000). The distances individual red squirrel females move between breeding seasons. While most adult female red squirrels remain in their territory after a breeding attempt, and either keep it (a) or share it with their daughter (b), some adult females disperse before the next breeding attempt (c). This allows some daughters to stay and inherit the territory (d) even though their mother is still alive. The average diameter of the territory is 42 m for an adult female red squirrel.

9 480 T. H. CLUTTON-BROCK and D. LUKAS Third, females may leave their natal area to reduce the chance that their dependent young will be killed by predators or immigrant males. Where males that have recently immigrated into breeding groups are likely to kill dependent offspring, females with infants often leave groups following recent male take-overs (Hrdy 1977; Sterck & Korstjens 2000; Teichroeb et al. 2009; Zhao et al. 2011) and may even anticipate the risk of take-over and male infanticide by leaving groups which are not defended effectively by the resident male (Steenbeck 2000, Stokes et al. 2003). Finally, females may benefit from dispersal from their natal group because it allows them to gain access to unrelated males and to avoid inbreeding with close relatives (Bengtsson 1978; Greenwood 1980; Pusey 1987; Koenig & Haydock 2004). In most social mammals that live in stable groups, offspring of one sex (usually males) typically disperse to breed in other groups and the incidence of close inbreeding within the group is consequently low (Greenwood 1983; Pusey 1987) but in some species where males have relatively long breeding tenure, female dispersal plays an important role in minimising the chance of breeding with close relatives (Berger 1986; Clutton-Brock 1989; Nunes 2007). Several different kinds of evidence support the suggestion that inbreeding avoidance has played a role in the evolution of female dispersal. There is substantial evidence that, in animals that usually outbreed, mating with close relatives lowers average levels of heterozygosity among offspring and increases the chance that they will be homozygous for deleterious recessive alleles likely to reduce fitness (Morton et al. 1956; Charlesworth & Charlesworth 1987; Keller & Waller 2002). As a result, female fecundity is often reduced when partners are close relatives and so, too, is the viability and breeding success of their offspring (ungulates: Ralls et al. 1979; Slate et al. 2000; Marshall et al. 2002; rodents: Hill 1974; Haigh 1983; Pugh & Tamarin 1988; Keane 1990; Reeve et al. 1990; Krackow & Matuschak 1991; Hoogland 1992; insectivores: Stockley et al. 1993; carnivores: Packer & Pusey 1983; primates: Bulger & Hamilton 1988; Dietz & Baker 1993; Alberts & Altmann 1995; other mammals: Pusey & Wolf 1996; Frankham et al. 2002). The immediate causes of reductions in offspring fitness vary but can include reduced growth and survival (Fig. 3, Jimenez et al. 1994), later age at first breeding, lower fecundity, reduced survival of offspring, shorter lifespans, increased susceptibility to disease or parasites and reduced lifetime breeding success (Fig. 4, Slate et al. 2000) (Acevedo- Whitehouse et al. 2003; Wilson et al. 2004). There is also extensive evidence that females avoid breeding with close relatives. Females whose fathers or brothers are reproductively active in their group when they reach sexual maturity commonly either avoid Cumulative survival probability Weeks after release Non-inbred Inbred Fig. 3 Effects of inbreeding on survival in free-ranging whitefooted mice (data from Jimenez et al. 1994). Survivorship of inbred (open circles and dashed line) and non-inbred (solid diamonds and solid line) mice over 10 weeks after first capture. Non-inbred animals had higher survivorship than inbred animals during all six times, and the combined cumulative effect as shown above is statistically significant. Fig. 4 Effects of standardised heterozygosity on lifetime breeding success in male and female red deer (from Slate et al. 2000). Female Lifetime Breeding Success (LBS) was adjusted for the effects of density and weather in the year of birth. Relationships are significant in both sexes. mating with them, delay breeding or leave the group (Packer 1979; Hoogland 1982, 1995; Armitage 1984; Smale et al. 1997; Nunes 2007): for example, in dwarf mongooses and African wild dogs, adolescent females are more likely to disperse from their natal group if their father is still monopolising access to the group than if he has emigrated or is dead (Rood 1987; McNutt 1996) and, in voles, the presence of unfamiliar males in neighbouring territories stimulates dispersal in females (Mcguire & Getz 1991). In mountain gorillas, females that mature in groups where the only breeding male is their father usually disperse before breeding while those maturing in groups where there is a second breeding male commonly breed at least once in their natal group (Harcourt & Stewart 2007). Experimental studies of rodents confirm that the presence of

10 PHILOPATRY AND DISPERSAL IN FEMALE MAMMALS 481 closely related males can stimulate female dispersal: for example, meadow voles released in groups into experimental grassland plots are more likely to disperse from their release site if other members of the group are kin than if they are unrelated (Bollinger et al. 1993). Similarly, female white-footed mice whose fathers are still resident in their territory are less likely to become sexually mature before dispersing than those whose mothers are present while those whose mothers are present are no less likely to mature than those lacking both parents (Fig. 5, Wolff 1992) and experimental removal of fathers causes daughters to remain longer in their natal territory while the presence of mothers in the natal territory has similar effects on sons. Finally, habitual female dispersal occurs in species where the reproductive tenure of males exceeds the age of females at first breeding and the rise of close inbreeding is consequently high (see section Species differences in female philopatry ). The relative importance of the different benefits of dispersal are still widely debated (Waser & Jones 1983; Bowler & Benton 2005; Ronce 2007; Clobert et al. 2008). While there is general agreement that females commonly disperse to avoid competition or persecution, there is still disagreement about the importance of inbreeding avoidance as an ultimate cause of female dispersal. Although some scientists argue that it plays an important role (Pusey 1987; Clutton-Brock 1989; Roze & Rousset 2005; Nunes 2007; Szulkin & Sheldon 2008), others are more sceptical (Moore & Ali 1984; Guillaume & Perrin 2006; Clobert et al. 2008), and some theoretical models suggest that inbreeding avoidance is unlikely on its own to account for the evolution of dispersal, though it may modify other mechanisms favouring dispersal (Perrin & Mazalov 1999; Perrin & Goudet 2001). Percentage becoming sexually mature Same-sex parent present Neither parent present Sons Opposite-sex parent present Daughters Both parents present Fig. 5 Effects of the presence of like and unlike-sexed parents on the development of sexual maturity in white-footed mice (data from Wolff 1992). While in the absence of the opposite sex parent juvenile white-footed mice rapidly become sexually mature, they are inhibited in their presence. No son (grey columns) became mature if the mother or both parents were still present in the homerange, and only few females (black columns) became sexually mature in the presence of their father in the homerange. In part, disagreements over the relative importance of inbreeding avoidance in stimulating dispersal reflect variation in definitions of dispersal and philopatry. Many reviews of variation in female philopatry in species that live in stable social groups have concluded that the risk of close inbreeding is a common cause of female dispersal (Pusey 1987; Clutton-Brock 1989) whereas analyses of dispersal distance are often sceptical of the role of inbreeding avoidance and emphasise the importance of ecological parameters (Clobert et al. 2008). Both may well be correct. Evidence that inbreeding avoidance can play an important role in stimulating dispersal from the natal group is strong (Wolff 1992; Szulkin & Sheldon 2008) and the diversity of factors affecting dispersal by females does not (as is sometimes argued) weaken the case that females avoid inbreeding with close relatives and that this can be an important factor constraining female philopatry and promoting dispersal from the natal group (Dobson & Jones 1985; Lambin et al. 2001). However, this need not necessarily suggest that individuals continue to avoid mating with kin after they have left and several studies have found little evidence that dispersers avoid joining close relatives or mating with kin (Keller & Arcese 1998; Daniels & Walters 2000; Szulkin & Sheldon 2008). For example, studies of reindeer found no evidence that females avoided joining the mating groups of related males (Holand et al. 2007) and research on black bears has found no evidence that females avoided mating with any of the males present in the neighborhood (Costello et al. 2008), though studies of some small mammals have found that dispersing females that have left their homerange or group do avoid mating familiar kin (Dewsbury 1988; Ferkin 1990; Pusey & Wolf 1996; Le Galliard et al. 2007). A possible explanation is that these differences are a consequence of interspecific contrasts in mobility: in relatively sedentary species, where individuals live in small, discontinuous populations, the chance that dispersing females will encounter close relatives may be high, generating selection for discrimination against related partners while, in relatively mobile species, individuals that leave their natal group may be unlikely to encounter close relatives after leaving their group so that selection to discriminate against them is weak and the distance moved by dispersing females may depend primarily on the availability of resources, vacant habitat and breeding opportunities (Koenig & Haydock 2004; Clobert et al. 2008). Species differences in female philopatry Arguments concerning the role of inbreeding in stimulating female dispersal have focussed attention on

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 15 Primate sociality: Predators and living in groups Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 Most haplorrine, and

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 15 Primate sociality: Predators and living in groups Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 Most haplorrine, and Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 15 Primate sociality: Predators and living in groups Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 Most haplorrine, and many strepsirrhine, primates are social: they live in

More information

Lecture 9: Primate Behavior - Ecology

Lecture 9: Primate Behavior - Ecology Anthro 101: Human Biological Evolution Lecture 9: Primate Behavior - Ecology Prof. Kenneth Feldmeier feldmekj@lavc.edu Homework 2 Why do primates live in groups? Benefits of group life Costs of group life

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line in evolution is reproductive success reproductive success:

More information

Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens. The following table shows the percentage of male fairy-wrens in various age and social status categories.

Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens. The following table shows the percentage of male fairy-wrens in various age and social status categories. Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens Superb fairy-wrens are small (10g) insectivorous birds found in woodlands and edge habitat throughout eastern Australia. They live in cooperative social groups composed

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons behind the lifestyles of our non-human primate relatives

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons behind the lifestyles of our non-human primate relatives

More information

Male dispersal patterns in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus Part 2: patterns and causes of secondary dispersal

Male dispersal patterns in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus Part 2: patterns and causes of secondary dispersal ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2004, 67, 771e782 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.06.015 Male dispersal patterns in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus Part 2: patterns and causes of secondary dispersal KATHARINE M. JACK*

More information

r = intrinsic rate of natural increase = the instantaneous rate of change in population size (per individual). If r > 0, then increasing

r = intrinsic rate of natural increase = the instantaneous rate of change in population size (per individual). If r > 0, then increasing Zoology 357 - Evolutionary Ecology - First Exam 1. (6 points) List three types of natural selection and show how the population changes over time (graph the initial phenotype frequency distribution and

More information

Cetacean Social & Reproductive Systems

Cetacean Social & Reproductive Systems Cetacean Social & Reproductive Systems Group Living Benefits Reduced predation risk Enhanced detection/ capture of prey Improved reproduction Reduced harassment Larger prey Costs Share food Increased competition

More information

Natal dispersal, interactions among siblings and intrasexual competition

Natal dispersal, interactions among siblings and intrasexual competition Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arl002 Advance Access publication 6 June 2006 Natal dispersal, interactions among siblings and intrasexual competition Jean-Francxois Le Galliard, a G. Gundersen,

More information

Female dispersal in a female-philopatric species, Cebus capucinus

Female dispersal in a female-philopatric species, Cebus capucinus Female dispersal in a female-philopatric species, Cebus capucinus Katharine M. Jack 1,3) & Linda M. Fedigan 2) ( 1 Department of Anthropology, Tulane University, 7041 Freret Street, New Orleans, LA 70118,

More information

Nishant Hasmukh Shah Department of Anthropology Emory University

Nishant Hasmukh Shah Department of Anthropology Emory University COOPERATIVE AN INTEGRATIVE Nishant Hasmukh Shah Department of Anthropology Emory University BREEDING: ApPROACH In cooperative breeding, "members of the social group assist in rearing young that are not

More information

Causes and consequences of sex-biased dispersal in Columbian ground squirrel, Spermophilus columbianus

Causes and consequences of sex-biased dispersal in Columbian ground squirrel, Spermophilus columbianus Causes and consequences of sex-biased dispersal in Columbian ground squirrel, Spermophilus columbianus Peter Neuhaus 1) (Institute of Zoology, University of Neuchâtel; Emile-Argand 11, Case postale 158;

More information

Inbreeding and inbreeding depression of early life traits in a. cooperative mammal

Inbreeding and inbreeding depression of early life traits in a. cooperative mammal Inbreeding and inbreeding depression of early life traits in a cooperative mammal JOHANNA F. NIELSEN, * SINEAD ENGLISH, WILL P. GOODALL-COPESTAKE, * JINLIANG WANG, CRAIG A. WALLING, * ANDREW W. BATEMAN,

More information

Reproducti e skew in birds: models, problems and prospects

Reproducti e skew in birds: models, problems and prospects JOURNAL OF AVIAN BIOLOGY 31: 247 258. Copenhagen 2000 Reproducti e skew in birds: models, problems and prospects Robert D. Magrath and Robert G. Heinsohn, Di ision of Botany and Zoology, Australian National

More information

Natal dispersal and new group formation in capybaras (Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris) in a seasonally flooded savanna of Venezuela

Natal dispersal and new group formation in capybaras (Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris) in a seasonally flooded savanna of Venezuela University of Missouri, St. Louis IRL @ UMSL Dissertations UMSL Graduate Works 12-21-2007 Natal dispersal and new group formation in capybaras (Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris) in a seasonally flooded savanna

More information

MATERNAL DOMINANCE RANK AND AGE AFFECT OFFSPRING SEX RATIO IN PIGTAIL MACAQUES

MATERNAL DOMINANCE RANK AND AGE AFFECT OFFSPRING SEX RATIO IN PIGTAIL MACAQUES Journal of Mammalogy, 83(2):563 568, 2002 MATERNAL DOMINANCE RANK AND AGE AFFECT OFFSPRING SEX RATIO IN PIGTAIL MACAQUES DARIO MAESTRIPIERI* Committee on Evolutionary Biology, The University of Chicago,

More information

The effects of adult removal on dispersal of yearling yellow-bellied marmots

The effects of adult removal on dispersal of yearling yellow-bellied marmots The effects of adult removal on dispersal of yearling yellow-bellied marmots ALISON K. BRODY AND KENNETH B. ARMITAGE' Department of Systematics and Ecology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, U.S.A. 66045

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 14 The evolution of social behavior: Altruism and kin selection Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 It was not

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 14 The evolution of social behavior: Altruism and kin selection Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 It was not Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 14 The evolution of social behavior: Altruism and kin selection Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 It was not difficult to understand how selection can affect mating

More information

Inbreeding avoidance under different null models

Inbreeding avoidance under different null models Journal of Animal Ecology 2009, 78, 778 788 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01544.x Inbreeding avoidance under different null models Blackwell Publishing Ltd of random mating in the great tit Marta Szulkin

More information

Effect of act on fitness of recipient

Effect of act on fitness of recipient Kin selection (inclusive fitness) and social behavior These worker ants are sterile, and they differ greatly from their queens and males. Darwin was troubled by social-insect workers, because they have

More information

Polygyny in animals From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Polygyny in animals From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Page 1 of 6 Polygyny in animals From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Polygyny (/pəˈlɪdʒɪniː/; from Neo-Greek πολυγυνία from πολύ- poly- "many", and γυνή gyne "woman" or "wife") [1] is a mating system

More information

RESEARCH ARTICLE The Ties That Bind: Maternal Kin Bias in a Multilevel Primate Society Despite Natal Dispersal by Both Sexes

RESEARCH ARTICLE The Ties That Bind: Maternal Kin Bias in a Multilevel Primate Society Despite Natal Dispersal by Both Sexes American Journal of Primatology RESEARCH ARTICLE The Ties That Bind: Maternal Kin Bias in a Multilevel Primate Society Despite Natal Dispersal by Both Sexes VERONIKA ST ADELE 1 *, MATHEW PINES 2, LARISSA

More information

BLY 122 Lecture Notes (O Brien) Chapter 47 Behavior

BLY 122 Lecture Notes (O Brien) Chapter 47 Behavior BLY 122 Lecture Notes (O Brien) 2005 Chapter 47 Behavior I. Causes of Behavior A. PROXIMAL CAUSATION 1. Describes how actions occur in terms of the neurological, hormonal, and skeletomuscular mechanisms

More information

Sex-Biased Dispersal: A Result of a Sex Difference in Breeding Site Availability

Sex-Biased Dispersal: A Result of a Sex Difference in Breeding Site Availability vol. 171, no. 6 the american naturalist june 008 Sex-Biased Dispersal: A Result of a Sex Difference in Breeding Site Availability Debora Arlt * and Tomas Pärt Department of Ecology, Swedish University

More information

Time allowed: 2 hours Answer ALL questions in Section A, ALL PARTS of the question in Section B and ONE question from Section C.

Time allowed: 2 hours Answer ALL questions in Section A, ALL PARTS of the question in Section B and ONE question from Section C. UNIVERSITY OF EAST ANGLIA School of Biological Sciences Main Series UG Examination 2014-2015 BEHAVIOURAL ECOLOGY BIO-5010B Time allowed: 2 hours Answer ALL questions in Section A, ALL PARTS of the question

More information

The Gene Propagation Game

The Gene Propagation Game The Gene Propagation Game Social Living has Costs and Benefits Competition, Territoriality and Dominance Altruism in the Animal Kingdom Genetic Relatedness and Inclusive Fitness Mating Behavior Depends

More information

An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology

An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology J.R. Krebs FRS Royal Society Research Professor at the Edward Grey Institute of Field Ornithology Department of Zoology University of Oxford and Fellow of Pembroke

More information

4.2 Altruism -'1 I. 44 SECTION2 Evolutionand BehavioralEcology

4.2 Altruism -'1 I. 44 SECTION2 Evolutionand BehavioralEcology 44 SECTION2 Evolutionand BehavioralEcology Figure 4.2 Male Hanuman langur monkeys in India, can act agressively especially toward young which they may killwhen taking over groups of females from other

More information

Lectures 7 & 8 Wednesday, October 12, 2011 & Friday, October 14, 2011

Lectures 7 & 8 Wednesday, October 12, 2011 & Friday, October 14, 2011 Lectures 7 & 8 Wednesday, October 12, 2011 & Friday, October 14, 2011 Recombination Diploid organisms: The first step in sexual reproduction is the production of gametes, each of which has half the chromosomes

More information

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30)

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30) FILM SESSIONS Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30) 1. Talking to Strangers (60 min.) 1 This film surveys some of the general communication signals used by animals from diverse taxa,

More information

Patterns of Reproductive Skew in the Polygynandrous Acorn Woodpecker

Patterns of Reproductive Skew in the Polygynandrous Acorn Woodpecker vol. 162, no. 3 the american naturalist september 2003 Patterns of Reproductive Skew in the Polygynandrous Acorn Woodpecker Joseph Haydock 1,* and Walter D. Koenig 2, 1. Department of Biology, Gonzaga

More information

Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of evolutionary processes leading to speciation (91605)

Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of evolutionary processes leading to speciation (91605) NCEA Level 3 Biology (91605) 2013 page 1 of 6 Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of evolutionary processes leading to speciation (91605) Assessment Criteria Evidence Achievement

More information

GENETIC ADEQUACY of GREATER YELLOWSTONE GRIZZLY BEARS

GENETIC ADEQUACY of GREATER YELLOWSTONE GRIZZLY BEARS GENETIC ADEQUACY of GREATER YELLOWSTONE GRIZZLY BEARS (The Introduction to Endangered Genes of Yellowstone should be read before this section.) Yellowstone grizzly bears, now the southernmost on the continent,

More information

A n t h r o p o l o g y

A n t h r o p o l o g y A n t h r o p o l o g y Appreciating Human Diversity Fifteenth Edition Conrad Phillip Kottak University of Michigan McGraw-Hill 2013 McGraw-Hill Companies. All Rights Reserved. C H A P T E R THE PRIMATES

More information

Animal Social Complexity

Animal Social Complexity Animal Social Complexity Intelligence, Culture, and Individualized Societies EDITED BY Frans B. M. de Waal and Peter L. Tyack HARVARD UNIVERSITY PRESS Cambridge, Massachusetts London, England 2003 CASE

More information

Banded mongooses avoid inbreeding when mating with members of the same natal group.

Banded mongooses avoid inbreeding when mating with members of the same natal group. 1 2 Banded mongooses avoid inbreeding when mating with members of the same natal group. 3 4 5 Authors: Jennifer L Sanderson 1, Jinliang Wang 2, Emma I K Vitikainen 1, Michael A Cant 1, & Hazel J Nichols

More information

INBREEDING IN SOCIAL ANIMAL SYSTEMS. Sam Straus Avilés Lab (Zoology) BIOL 509

INBREEDING IN SOCIAL ANIMAL SYSTEMS. Sam Straus Avilés Lab (Zoology) BIOL 509 INBREEDING IN SOCIAL ANIMAL SYSTEMS Sam Straus Avilés Lab (Zoology) BIOL 509 Overview Why might inbreeding be advantageous? Mathematical models Inbreeding causes and effects Examples of inbred social systems

More information

Paternal kin discrimination in wild baboons

Paternal kin discrimination in wild baboons Paternal kin discrimination in wild baboons Susan C. Alberts Department of Zoology, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA (alberts@duke.edu) Mammals commonly avoid mating with maternal kin, probably as

More information

(A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above

(A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above CLEP Biology - Problem Drill 24: Behavior No. 1 of 10 1. Habituation is a type of. (A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above Having a certain drive

More information

Types of Mating Systems

Types of Mating Systems The Human Mating System Note: this is the stereotypical EP view (e.g., Buss) Types of Mating Systems Random Monogamy Polygyny: variance in male reproductive success > variance in female reproductive success

More information

Animal Behavior 2/21/2017. What is Behavior? Understanding Behavior. Types of Behavior. Types of Behavior

Animal Behavior 2/21/2017. What is Behavior? Understanding Behavior. Types of Behavior. Types of Behavior What is Behavior? Behavior everything an animal does & how it does it response to stimuli in its environment Animal Behavior Why Study Behavior? Evolutionary perspective part of phenotype acted upon by

More information

Determinants of reproductive success in dominant

Determinants of reproductive success in dominant Journal of Animal Ecology 2008, 77, 92 102 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01318.x Determinants of reproductive success in dominant Blackwell Publishing Ltd female meerkats Sarah J. Hodge 1 *, A. Manica

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 17 Mating: Sexual selection Copyright Bruce Owen 2011 Sexual selection: selection that favors traits

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 17 Mating: Sexual selection Copyright Bruce Owen 2011 Sexual selection: selection that favors traits Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 17 Mating: Sexual selection Copyright Bruce Owen 2011 Sexual selection: selection that favors traits that increase male success in mating Sexual selection

More information

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise...

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise... 1. Female choice of males that have exaggerated characters has been explained by female choice of males that have better genes. Explain how female choice of higher quality males might lead to exaggerated

More information

SIO Marine Mammal Behavior, and Social Systems: Ma;ng. John Hildebrand, Scripps Inst. Oceanography, UCSD

SIO Marine Mammal Behavior, and Social Systems: Ma;ng. John Hildebrand, Scripps Inst. Oceanography, UCSD SIO 133 - Marine Mammal Behavior, and Social Systems: Ma;ng John Hildebrand, Scripps Inst. Oceanography, UCSD Ma;ng Strategies Individuals behave to maximize reproduc;ve success Male and female reproduc;ve

More information

Cocoon care in the social spider Stegodyphus dumicola (Eresidae)

Cocoon care in the social spider Stegodyphus dumicola (Eresidae) 39 European Arachnology 2000 (S. Toft & N. Scharff eds.), pp. 39-44. Aarhus University Press, Aarhus, 2002. ISBN 87 7934 001 6 (Proceedings of the 19th European Colloquium of Arachnology, Århus 17-22 July

More information

Jurmain et al. Chapter 7. Primate Behavior

Jurmain et al. Chapter 7. Primate Behavior Jurmain et al. Chapter 7 Primate Behavior Primate Studies Primate studies often use an ecological approach to explain variation in primate behavior between different species and within a single species.

More information

The selfish gene. mitochondrium

The selfish gene. mitochondrium The selfish gene selection acts mostly for the benefit of the individual sometimes selection may act for the benefit of relatives rarely, selection acts for the benefit of the group mitochondrium in asexual

More information

Colony structure and parentage in wild colonies of cooperatively breeding Damaraland mole-rats suggest incest

Colony structure and parentage in wild colonies of cooperatively breeding Damaraland mole-rats suggest incest Molecular Ecology (2004) 13, 2371 2379 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2004.02233.x Colony structure and parentage in wild colonies of cooperatively breeding Damaraland mole-rats suggest incest Blackwell Publishing,

More information

Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology

Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology Models of Parent-Offspring Conflict Ethology and Behavioral Ecology A. In this section we will look the nearly universal conflict that will eventually arise in any species where there is some form of parental

More information

Protecting Your Sugar Gliders: Inbreeding Depression and How You Can Avoid It. Inbreeding and Inbreeding Depression

Protecting Your Sugar Gliders: Inbreeding Depression and How You Can Avoid It. Inbreeding and Inbreeding Depression By: Hannah Harris, Oberlin College Written For: Shelly Sterk, Glider Nursery In our culture, the taboo against incest and inbreeding is highly pervasive. Most people have a sense that you shouldn t marry

More information

Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences

Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences Males and females have the same genes. Why do the sexes often look and act so differently? Why is the male often insanely ornamented? (Or simply insane?)

More information

The World of Primates

The World of Primates The World of Primates From mouse lemurs to gorillas, the Primates are an extremely diverse and successful Order of mammals. There is no single feature that makes an animal a primate, but rather a suite

More information

Ecological benefits of cooperation. Life Insurance. Life Insurance. Fortress defense

Ecological benefits of cooperation. Life Insurance. Life Insurance. Fortress defense Ecological benefits of cooperation 1. For cooperation and eusociality to evolve it also is required that ecological conditions lead to a high enough benefit/cost ratio to favor cooperation a. Need B/C

More information

So what is a species?

So what is a species? So what is a species? Evolutionary Forces New Groups Biological species concept defined by Ernst Mayr population whose members can interbreed & produce viable, fertile offspring reproductively compatible

More information

Mechanisms of Evolution

Mechanisms of Evolution Mechanisms of Evolution TEKS 7(F) analyze and evaluate the effects of other evolutionary mechanisms, including genetic drift, gene flow, mutation, and recombination Evolution is. For Darwin (1859): Evolution

More information

Animal Behavior. Hormones and Neurons Organize Behavior

Animal Behavior. Hormones and Neurons Organize Behavior Animal Behavior Hormones and Neurons Organize Behavior Controlling Neural Responses What controls neurons? Ganglia -clusters of neuron cell bodies. Brain-greatest concentration of cell bodies. Praying

More information

Study Guide A. Answer Key. Animal Behavior

Study Guide A. Answer Key. Animal Behavior Animal Behavior Answer Key SECTION 1. ADAPTIVE VALUE OF BEHAVIOR 1. stimulus; behavior 2. Internal; body 3. External; surroundings 4. b, c, a 5. constant; stimuli 6. increase; particular 7. internal; external

More information

Evolution of Mating Systems. Chapter 8

Evolution of Mating Systems. Chapter 8 Evolution of Mating Systems Chapter 8 Mating Systems-Chapter 8 1 Monogamy 2 Polyandry 3 Polygyny And the many combinations within! Why should a male be monogamous? 1 extension of guarding, little chance

More information

Anti-predator behavior: Group defense - Many eyes - Selfish herd - Dilution effects - Tonic immobility. Dispersal:

Anti-predator behavior: Group defense - Many eyes - Selfish herd - Dilution effects - Tonic immobility. Dispersal: Note: These are just the topics that have been covered since Exam 2. The exams are cumulative, so all the material that we have covered in this course can be included in the Final. However, there will

More information

Demographic mechanisms of inbreeding adjustment through extra-pair reproduction

Demographic mechanisms of inbreeding adjustment through extra-pair reproduction Journal of Animal Ecology 2015, 84, 1029 1040 doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12340 Demographic mechanisms of inbreeding adjustment through extra-pair reproduction Jane M. Reid 1 *, A. Bradley Duthie 1, Matthew

More information

Some observations. Some traits are difficult to view as adaptations, because they appear to provide a disadvantage to the organism

Some observations. Some traits are difficult to view as adaptations, because they appear to provide a disadvantage to the organism Some traits are difficult to view as adaptations, because they appear to provide a disadvantage to the organism Darwin asked: Can natural selection explain these differences? Structural traits: cumbersome

More information

AP Biology. What is behavior & Why study it? Animal Behavior. Ethology. What questions do we ask? Innate behavior. Types of behaviors

AP Biology. What is behavior & Why study it? Animal Behavior. Ethology. What questions do we ask? Innate behavior. Types of behaviors Animal Behavior What is behavior & Why study it? Behavior everything an animal does & how it does it response to stimuli in its environment innate = inherited or developmentally fixed learned = develop

More information

How Organisms Evolve Chapters The Theory of Evolution. The Theory of Evolution. Evolution can be traced through the fossil record.

How Organisms Evolve Chapters The Theory of Evolution. The Theory of Evolution. Evolution can be traced through the fossil record. How Organisms Evolve Chapters 14-15 The Theory of Evolution Evolution is the process of change in the inherited traits of a population of organisms from one generation to the next. The inherited traits

More information

Animal Behavior. Types of Communication 4/22/2013

Animal Behavior. Types of Communication 4/22/2013 Animal Behavior A behavior is the nervous system s response to a stimulus and is carried out by the muscular or the hormonal system Behavior is subject to natural selection Behaviors have an impact on

More information

November 4, 2009 Bioe 109 Fall 2009 Lecture 17 The evolution of mating systems. The evolution of sex ratio

November 4, 2009 Bioe 109 Fall 2009 Lecture 17 The evolution of mating systems. The evolution of sex ratio November 4, 2009 Bioe 109 Fall 2009 Lecture 17 The evolution of mating systems The evolution of sex ratio - let us define sex ratio as the proportion of males to females. - in discussing the evolution

More information

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887 The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887 1 Outline causes sexual selection vs. natural selection sexual selection during courtship male-male

More information

Parental Care. External vs. Internal Fertilization. Likelihood of paternal care depends on. Paternal certainty Fertilization mode

Parental Care. External vs. Internal Fertilization. Likelihood of paternal care depends on. Paternal certainty Fertilization mode Parental Care Likelihood of paternal care depends on Who should care? How much care? External vs. Internal Fertilization Paternal certainty Fertilization mode Degree to which male can contribute State

More information

A Sex Allocation Theory for Vertebrates: Combining Local Resource Competition and Condition- Dependent Allocation

A Sex Allocation Theory for Vertebrates: Combining Local Resource Competition and Condition- Dependent Allocation vol. 170, no. 5 the american naturalist november 2007 E-Article A Sex Allocation Theory for Vertebrates: Combining Local Resource Competition and Condition- Dependent Allocation Geoff Wild 1,* and Stuart

More information

Evolutionary Forces. What changes populations?

Evolutionary Forces. What changes populations? Evolutionary Forces What changes populations? Forces of evolutionary change Natural selection traits that improve survival or reproduction accumulate in the population ADAPTIVE change Genetic drift frequency

More information

Mammalogy Lecture 16 Conservation Genetics (with a side emphasis on Marine Mammals)

Mammalogy Lecture 16 Conservation Genetics (with a side emphasis on Marine Mammals) Mammalogy Lecture 16 Conservation Genetics (with a side emphasis on Marine Mammals) Initial definition: Allele An allele is a viable DNA coding that occupies a given locus (position) on a chromosome (plural

More information

Juvenile dispersal in relation to adult densities

Juvenile dispersal in relation to adult densities Acta Theriologica 41 (2): 177-186, 1996. PL ISSN 0001-7051 Juvenile dispersal in relation to adult densities in wood mice Apodemus sylvaticus Susanne PLESNER JENSEN* Plesner Jensen S. 1996. Juvenile dispersal

More information

Mating systems and parental investment. Mating systems. Resource distribution. Polygyny. Pattern of matings in a population. Antithesis = promiscuity

Mating systems and parental investment. Mating systems. Resource distribution. Polygyny. Pattern of matings in a population. Antithesis = promiscuity 1 Mating systems and parental investment Mating systems Pattern of matings in a population green anole Antithesis = promiscuity Polygyny Scramble: no attempts to defend females, resources horseshoe crabs

More information

Section 8.1 Studying inheritance

Section 8.1 Studying inheritance Section 8.1 Studying inheritance Genotype and phenotype Genotype is the genetic constitution of an organism that describes all the alleles that an organism contains The genotype sets the limits to which

More information

Environmental Potential for Polygamy. Polygamy Favored. Do females always benefit from being mates of polygynous males? Resource Defense Polygyny

Environmental Potential for Polygamy. Polygamy Favored. Do females always benefit from being mates of polygynous males? Resource Defense Polygyny 1 Polygamy Favored Biparental care not mandatory Food super-abundant Resource abundance varies spatially & temporally Environmental Potential for Polygamy Distribution of resources or mates Spatial & temporal

More information

Biology 352, Spring 2018 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam Part 1 (short answer worth 21 % of grade)

Biology 352, Spring 2018 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam Part 1 (short answer worth 21 % of grade) Part 1 (short answer worth 21 % of grade) 1-1) Match the following terms used in behavioral ecology with the appropriate topics of behavioral study listed to the right (you may apply more than one topic

More information

Reprinted from: Evolution - Essays in honour of John Maynard Smith

Reprinted from: Evolution - Essays in honour of John Maynard Smith Reprinted from: Evolution - Essays in honour of John Maynard Smith pp. -186 Edited by J. J. Greenwood and M. Slatkin Cambridge University Press 1985 Printed in Great Britain at the University Press, Cambridge

More information

1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts)

1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts) Zoology 357 - Evolutionary Ecology - First Exam 1. (6 pts) a. Can all characteristics of organisms be explained by natural selection? Explain your answer in a sentence (3 pts) b. Name two non-selectionist

More information

UNIT 9. PARENTAL CARE AND MATING SYSTEMS

UNIT 9. PARENTAL CARE AND MATING SYSTEMS UNIT 9. PARENTAL CARE AND MATING SYSTEMS SOURCES (for powerpoint format: http://wfsc.tamu.edu/jpackard/behavior/wfsc622/powerpoints.zip) required: Chapter 9 in Krebs & Davies (1993:208-243) remedial: "Rearing

More information

Bi412/512 Animal Behavior, Exam 1 Practice Page 1

Bi412/512 Animal Behavior, Exam 1 Practice Page 1 Bi412/512 Animal Behavior, Exam 1 Practice Page 1 1. Suppose you observe that song sparrows sing more at dawn than at other times of the day. Hmm, maybe singing early in the morning communicates to female

More information

Sex-biased dispersal: a review of the theory

Sex-biased dispersal: a review of the theory Biol. Rev. (2018), pp. 000 000. 1 doi: 10.1111/brv.12475 Sex-biased dispersal: a review of the theory Xiang-Yi Li and Hanna Kokko Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University

More information

In some social animals, a single female in

In some social animals, a single female in Reproductive skew, concessions and limited control In some social animals, a single female in each group produces most or all of the young, and subordinate females rarely breed until they attain a dominant

More information

Last Lecture. Cont. Being Social, Chapter 3 Evolution of Human Behavior, Chapter 14

Last Lecture. Cont. Being Social, Chapter 3 Evolution of Human Behavior, Chapter 14 Last Lecture Cont. Being Social, Chapter 3 Evolution of Human Behavior, Chapter 14 Is there a reason to be social? What are the benefits of sociality? 1. Cooperation, both benefit 2. Postponed cooperation

More information

University of Groningen. Local adaptation or dispersal? Burger, Claudia

University of Groningen. Local adaptation or dispersal? Burger, Claudia University of Groningen Local adaptation or dispersal? Burger, Claudia IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the

More information

Microevolution: The Forces of Evolutionary Change Part 2. Lecture 23

Microevolution: The Forces of Evolutionary Change Part 2. Lecture 23 Microevolution: The Forces of Evolutionary Change Part 2 Lecture 23 Outline Conditions that cause evolutionary change Natural vs artificial selection Nonrandom mating and sexual selection The role of chance

More information

Energetics of cooperative breeding in meerkats Suricata Suricatta

Energetics of cooperative breeding in meerkats Suricata Suricatta International Congress Series 1275 (2004) 367 374 www.ics-elsevier.com Energetics of cooperative breeding in meerkats Suricata Suricatta M. Scantlebury a, *, T.H. Clutton-Brock b, J.R. Speakman c,d a Mammal

More information

EnSt/Bio 295 Exam II This test is worth 100 points; you have approximately 50 minutes. Allocate your time accordingly.

EnSt/Bio 295 Exam II This test is worth 100 points; you have approximately 50 minutes. Allocate your time accordingly. Name: 1 NAME: EnSt/Bio 295 Exam II This test is worth 100 points; you have approximately 50 minutes. Allocate your time accordingly. 1) Describe the following concepts in a few sentences (2 points each)

More information

AP Biology. What is behavior? Behavioral Ecology. Why study behavior? What questions can we ask? Evolutionary perspective.

AP Biology. What is behavior? Behavioral Ecology. Why study behavior? What questions can we ask? Evolutionary perspective. Behavioral Ecology What is behavior? Behavior everything an animal does & how it does it response to stimuli in its environment innate inherited, instinctive automatic & consistent learned ability to learn

More information

Writing Assignment 1

Writing Assignment 1 Writing Assignment 1 Writing Assignment #1 Due Wednesday October 15th at the beginning of lecture To read: A Tephritid Fly Mimics the Territorial Displays of its Jumping Spider Predators Erick Greene;

More information

Elephant behaviour and conservation: social relationships, the effects of poaching, and genetic tools for management

Elephant behaviour and conservation: social relationships, the effects of poaching, and genetic tools for management Molecular Ecology (2012) 21, 765 778 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05237.x INVITED REVIEW Elephant behaviour and conservation: social relationships, the effects of poaching, and genetic tools for management

More information

Chapter 21.2 Mechanisms of Evolutionary Change

Chapter 21.2 Mechanisms of Evolutionary Change Beak depth of Beak depth Colonie High AP Biology Chapter 21.2 Mechanisms of Evolutionary Change Populations Evolve! Natural selection acts on individuals differential survival survival of the fittest differential

More information

Physiology of hibernation,

Physiology of hibernation, Physiology of hibernation, aestivation and diapause. BIOS 0903A; Gr. A BAT This is a specialized type of adipose tissue often reddish brown BAT receive a rich supply of blood vessels and are well innervated

More information

SIO Marine Mammal Behavior, and Social Systems: Ma;ng. John Hildebrand, Scripps Inst. Oceanography, UCSD

SIO Marine Mammal Behavior, and Social Systems: Ma;ng. John Hildebrand, Scripps Inst. Oceanography, UCSD SIO 133 - Marine Mammal Behavior, and Social Systems: Ma;ng John Hildebrand, Scripps Inst. Oceanography, UCSD Ma;ng Strategies Individuals behave to maximize reproduc;ve success Male and female reproduc;ve

More information

SEXUAL SELECTION IN WILD BABOONS: FROM MATING OPPORTUNITIES TO

SEXUAL SELECTION IN WILD BABOONS: FROM MATING OPPORTUNITIES TO 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 SEXUAL SELECTION IN WILD BABOONS: FROM MATING OPPORTUNITIES TO PATERNITY SUCCESS SUSAN C. ALBERTS 1,2, JASON C. BUCHAN 1*, JEANNE

More information

Tayassu tajacu (Collared Peccary or Quenk)

Tayassu tajacu (Collared Peccary or Quenk) Tayassu tajacu (Collared Peccary or Quenk) Family: Tayassuidae (Peccaries) Order: Artiodactyla (Even-toed Ungulates) Class: Mammalia (Mammals) Fig 1. Collared peccary, Tayassu tajacu. [http://a-z-animals.com/animals/collared-peccary/,

More information

Biology 352, Spring 2017 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam

Biology 352, Spring 2017 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam Biology 352, Spring 2017 Exam Number KEY Exam Instructions: 1) Write your NAME here: 2) Write your exam number (given above, right) at the top of the other pages. 3) Look over the exam to verify that you

More information

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection Intrasexual Natural Selection Sexual Selection Survival Mating Success Parental Care Intrasexual

More information

Female nursing partner choice in a population of wild house mice (Mus musculus domesticus)

Female nursing partner choice in a population of wild house mice (Mus musculus domesticus) Harrison et al. Frontiers in Zoology (2018) 15:4 https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-018-0251-3 RESEARCH Open Access Female nursing partner choice in a population of wild house mice (Mus musculus domesticus)

More information

POST-ALLOGROOMING REDUCTIONS IN SELF-DIRECTED BEHAVIOUR ARE AFFECTED BY ROLE AND STATUS IN THE GREEN WOODHOOPOE

POST-ALLOGROOMING REDUCTIONS IN SELF-DIRECTED BEHAVIOUR ARE AFFECTED BY ROLE AND STATUS IN THE GREEN WOODHOOPOE 2 3 4 5 6 POST-ALLOGROOMING REDUCTIONS IN SELF-DIRECTED BEHAVIOUR ARE AFFECTED BY ROLE AND STATUS IN THE GREEN WOODHOOPOE Andrew N. Radford Electronic Supplementary Material 8 9 0 2 3 4 5 6 8 9 Study Species

More information