Benefits of napping in healthy adults: impact of nap length, time of day, age, and experience with napping

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Benefits of napping in healthy adults: impact of nap length, time of day, age, and experience with napping"

Transcription

1 J. Sleep Res. (2009) 18, Benefits of napping doi: /j x Benefits of napping in healthy adults: impact of nap length, time of day, age, and experience with napping CATHERINE E. MILNER and KIMBERLY A. COTE Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada Accepted in revised form 20 October 2008; received 16 July 2008 SUMMARY Napping is a cross-cultural phenomenon which occurs across the lifespan. People vary widely in the frequency with which they nap as well as the improvements in alertness and well-being experienced. The systematic study of daytime napping is important to understand the benefits in alertness and performance that may be accrued from napping. This review paper investigates factors that affect the benefits of napping such as duration and temporal placement of the nap. In addition, the influence of subject characteristics such as age and experience with napping is examined. The focus of the review is on benefits for healthy individuals with regular sleep/wake schedules rather than for people with sleep or medical disorders. The goal of the review is to summarize the type of performance improvements that result from napping, critique the existing studies, and make recommendations for future research. keywords age, napping, performance INTRODUCTION Although napping is considered to be a normal daily routine for babies and young children, many people continue to take daytime naps across the lifespan. Moreover, napping is a cross-cultural phenomenon. Estimates on the frequency of napping vary considerably. In a compilation of surveys from a number of countries, the frequency of napping (at least once a week) varied from 36% to 80% (Dinges, 1989). The most recent ÔSleep in AmericaÕ poll indicated that 46% of respondents napped at least twice in the last month, with an average nap duration of approximately 1 h (National Sleep Foundation, 2008). The frequency of napping has consistently been reported to increase with advancing age [e.g., Ohayon and Zulley (1999)]. People choose to take naps for a variety of reasons. While some nap in response to sleep loss (i.e., replacement napping) or in preparation for sleep loss (i.e., prophylactic napping), others simply nap for enjoyment (i.e., appetitive napping) (Broughton and Dinges, 1989). Napping is a practical solution Correspondence: Kimberly A. Cote, MSc, PhD, Associate Professor, Psychology Department, Director, Brock Sleep Research Laboratory, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, L2S 3A1, Canada. Tel.: (ext. 4806, Office), (ext. 3795, Lab); fax: ; kcote@brocku.ca to daytime sleepiness for shiftworkers (Gillberg et al., 1996; Oexman et al., 2002; Rosa, 1993; Takahashi and Arito, 2000) and people with sleep disorders [see Takahashi (2003) for review]. But even for individuals who generally get the sleep they need on a nightly basis, napping may lead to considerable benefits in terms of mood, alertness, and cognitive performance. A number of factors may influence the degree of benefit accrued from a daytime nap. Homeostatic and circadian processes may interact to determine the extent of restoration, e.g., the quality of the prior sleep period, the duration of the nap, the timing of the nap, and the presence of sleep inertia. Other factors such as subject characteristics (e.g., age, gender, individual differences in napping experience, experience with sleep loss, and degree of sleepiness at the time of testing) or test characteristics (e.g., task sensitivity, task difficulty, timing, or instructions) may also influence the benefits of napping. Further, regular nappers may glean more benefits from napping. For example, Evans et al. (1977) categorized nappers and non-nappers based on their frequency of napping and found that only habitual nappers described their naps as restorative in nature. Why do some people continue to seek out a daily nap across their lifetime, while others seem to evade the need altogether? There are likely individual differences in napping behavior that 272 Ó 2009 European Sleep Research Society

2 Benefits of napping in healthy adults 273 are related to both sleep need and the degree of benefit gained (e.g., mood enhancement, memory consolidation). This review paper will examine research on the benefits of napping for healthy adults who have typical sleep wake schedules rather than in those with sleep or circadian rhythm disorders. An overview of the research illustrating the benefits of naps and a description of strategies used to investigate naps is provided. Factors thought to impact the benefits of napping in healthy adults, such as the timing and duration of the nap, sleep inertia, age, and experience with napping are then reviewed in terms of their influence on mood, arousal, and performance. BENEFITS OF NAPPING Benefits of napping for waking performance have been confirmed by many researchers (Table 1). For instance, several studies have provided well-documented evidence of the benefits of naps during total sleep deprivation [e.g., Bonnet (1991); OÕConnor et al. (2004); Song et al. (2002)] and in nightshift workers (Purnell et al., 2002; Sallinen et al., 1998). Other studies have documented the benefits of naps during partial sleep deprivation [e.g., Gillberg (1984); Takahashi and Arito (2000)]. Appetitive napping has also been shown to have benefits for non-sleep-deprived groups (Betrus, 1986). These studies have employed a variety of measures to show that napping leads to subjective and behavioral improvements. Napping also improves mood and subjective levels of sleepiness and fatigue. It is particularly beneficial to performance on tasks, such as addition, logical reasoning, reaction time, and symbol recognition. Naps as a counter-measure to sleepiness A number of studies have investigated benefits of napping in comparison to other counter-measures of sleepiness such as caffeine and stimulant medication. In 1995 Bonnet et al. (1995) compared the effects of a prophylactic nap with those of caffeine, and found that benefits derived from a nap were less variable and lasted longer than those from caffeine. Bonnet and Arand (1994, 2000) and Reyner and Horne (1997) found that the combined treatment of a nap and caffeine was superior to either alone. Hayashi et al. (2003) compared napping, caffeine, bright light, and face-washing as means to combat mid-afternoon sleepiness. They found that the combined Table 1 Overview of key studies documenting the benefits of napping Study Participants Procedure Results Bonnet (1991) Song et al. (2002) OÕConnor et al. (2004) Sallinen et al. (1998) Purnell et al. (2002) Smith et al. (2007) Gillberg (1984) Takahashi and Arito (2000) Betrus (1986) 104 healthy males aged years 8 male medical students aged years 41 healthy individuals (37 male) aged years 14 male industry workers aged years 24 male engineers aged years 9 hospital workers (3 male), mean age 45.7 ± 13.2 years 12 healthy males aged years 12 healthy students (7 male), mean age 22.1 ± 1.6 years 17 young adults (8 male), aged years Between-groups comparison of 2-, 4-, and 8-h naps, with no nap during two nights of sleep deprivation 30-min naps during 40 h of sleep deprivation Between-groups comparison of two 2-h naps, one 2-h nap, and no nap, per 24 h during 3.7 days of sleep deprivation Within-subjects comparison of 30- and 50-min naps, at both 01:00 and 04:00 hours, with no nap Within-subjects comparison of 20-min and no-nap conditions during shift work Within-subjects comparison of 30-min and no-nap conditions during shift work Within-subjects comparison of 1-h naps at 21:00 and 04:30 hours, with no nap after a night restricted to 4 h of sleep Within-subjects comparison of 15-min and no-nap conditions after a night restricted to 4 h of sleep Within-subjects comparison of 10- and 30-min naps, with 30-min bedrest and control-active conditions Naps improved vigilance, addition, logical reasoning, and alertness, only on the first night Naps improved reaction time, but no change in accuracy Dose response trend with neurobehavioral performance improving most for the two 2-h nap group, and least for the no-nap group Naps reduced number of lapses on reaction time task and reduced physiological sleepiness and subjective fatigue Naps improved performance on reaction time and vigilance tasks but no change in subjective sleepiness Naps improved psychomotor speed and subjective sleepiness; these improvements persisted to the end of the shift Naps improved reaction time, reduced sleep latency, and reduced subjective sleepiness Naps improved accuracy but reaction time was unaffected Naps increased vigor, decreased fatigue, decreased confusion, decreased reaction time, and increased number of correct additions

3 274 C. E. Milner and K. A. Cote treatment of a nap and caffeine was superior in alleviating subjective sleepiness and aiding performance. Batejat and Lagarde (1999) conducted a similar study, comparing the effects of naps, a stimulant (i.e., modafinil), and the combined treatment on performance during sleep deprivation. The authors found that naps did lead to cognitive performance benefits, and that these benefits were further enhanced when modafinil was combined with a nap. Benefits included improved reaction time and enhanced learning on a tracking task. These results are similar to those found in other studies with the stimulant caffeine. Caldwell and Caldwell (1998) used a slightly different approach to compare a nap taken using a non-benzodiazepine hypnotic (i.e., zolpidem tartrate) to placebo nap and no-nap conditions. The nap induced by the drug showed greater attenuation of sleep deprivation-related deficits compared with both no-nap and placebo nap conditions. More specifically, both naps decreased sleepiness as measured by the repeated test of sustained wakefulness and Visual Analog Scales; the naps improved mood as measured with Visual Analog Scales and improved vigor scores; and the naps improved reaction time and accuracy on a flight simulation task. Zolpidem-aided naps were equivalent or superior to placebo naps in all cases. The authors hypothesized that this outcome resulted from differences in the amount of total sleep time, where more sleep was obtained with the drug. More specifically, individuals in the zolpidem-nap condition experienced faster sleep onset, less Stage 1, and more Stage 4 compared with the placebo condition. Predicting benefits from nap EEG A limited number of studies have investigated sleep architecture as a way to predict the type and degree of benefits gained from a nap. In a study using only habitual nappers, Taub et al. (1976) equated postnap benefits with sleep itself and not with a particular stage of sleep or nap duration. Subsequently, however, Taub (1979) reported that sleep architecture co-varied with performance and fatigue measures. Specifically, he found that Stage 4 sleep was associated with increased sleepiness, and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep with decreased sleepiness. More recently, Hayashi et al. (2005) showed that performance and alertness decreased through the postlunch dip when no nap was taken. Naps containing Stage 2 sleep improved performance and alertness. Naps containing Stage 1 but not Stage 2 sleep were successful in improving subjective alertness and reducing fatigue, but participants experienced performance decrements and showed evidence of decreased objective alertness. Tucker et al. (2006) showed that memory is dependent on the sleep architecture of the nap. Specifically, they showed that a nap helps to improve declarative memory, but that a nap and a period of rest were equivalent in terms of procedural memory improvement. They reported that the degree of declarative memory improvement was related to the amount of slow wave sleep (SWS) in the nap. Despite these recent findings, there is a definite lack of studies investigating both how the architecture of a nap differs from nocturnal sleep architecture, and the effect of sleep architecture on postnap measures. Future research would benefit from continuing to include data about the structure of the nap itself. More subtle differences in sleep electroencephalogram (EEG) may be investigated using quantitative EEG measures. Few studies have examined quantitative EEG during a nap in relation to outcome or performance. Dijk et al. (1987) used naps to study how sleep EEG varied according to amount of prior wake time, but they did not use any performance measures in their study. They reported that for a sample of six non-habitual nappers, delta and theta power increased with increasing wake time prior to the nap, indicating deeper sleep followed by more time awake. Sleep onset latency, however, followed a circadian pattern, reaching a minimum in midafternoon. Hirose and Nagasaka (2003) examined quantitative EEG in eight healthy young adults during a 15-min nap opportunity, and found that increasing theta (4 7 Hz), high alpha (11 13 Hz), and low beta (14 20 Hz) power predicted performance improvements following the nap. The authors suggested that this pattern of EEG during the nap was related to the presence of Stage 2 sleep; that is, the presence of Stage 2 would lead nappers to feel that they had obtained sufficient sleep, in contrast to individuals whose naps consisted of only wakefulness and Stage 1. In another study, Schmidt et al. (2006) used a repeated measures design to investigate learningdependent changes in the EEG during a 4-h daytime sleep. They compared declarative memory performance on easy and difficult word-pair learning tasks with a control condition. Results indicated that density of sleep spindles and sigma power during the nap was positively correlated with increases in memory performance from pre- to postnap. These studies suggest that quantitative EEG measures of arousal have real potential to explain the improvements following a nap. It is plausible that the character of the nap may predict how well mood, alertness, and performance will be enhanced after the nap. As with sleep architecture, there is a need to include this finer objective measure of sleep in future studies investigating the benefits of naps. Measuring nap benefits with quantitative EEG techniques Some napping studies have also used quantitative electrophysiological measures following a nap as measures of alertness. For example, Macchi et al. (2002) reported that arousal was increased following a nap, as evidenced by lower theta and alpha power. As well, examining waking event-related potentials (ERPs) following a nap allows investigation of information processing and attention changes resulting from the nap. Using quantitative EEG and ERPs, Takahashi and Arito (1998) reported a positive correlation between delta power in non-rem sleep during 15- and 45-min naps and latency of the P300 ERP component 30 min after awakening. The authors suggested that sleep inertia (i.e., grogginess) due to delta activity during the nap prolonged P300 latency shortly

4 Benefits of napping in healthy adults 275 after the nap, indicating delayed information processing. Three hours after awakening, P300 latency was shortened but this was not related to quantitative EEG measures. Takahashi et al. (1998) used the same ERP paradigm to determine if nap duration (i.e., 0, 15, or 45 min) had an effect on waking P300 latency or amplitude. They found that P300 latency was shortened after the 15-min nap compared with no nap or the 45-min nap. The authors attributed the lack of improvement after the 45-min nap to sleep inertia. P300 amplitude was not affected by nap duration. Takahashi and Arito (2000) next examined P300 latency and amplitude following a nap taken from 12:30 to 12:45 hours after 4 h of nocturnal sleep. The authors found that napping shortened P300 latency but amplitude did not change compared with the no-nap condition. In both studies, attention was enhanced through napping. Song et al. (2002) showed that taking naps during sleep deprivation prevented P300 latency from becoming delayed as much as during sleep deprivation with no nap, therefore preventing delayed information processing. Thus, EEG and ERPs are useful to describe both the quality and depth of nap sleep as well as levels of attention and information processing following a nap. In sum, various studies have confirmed the beneficial effects of naps on measures, such as vigor, alertness, and cognitive performance. These benefits in healthy adults may be influenced by the following factors which are reviewed below: circadian placement of the nap, duration of the nap, sleep inertia, age, and experience with napping. CIRCADIAN PLACEMENT Timing of the nap Investigators have systematically varied the time of day at which a nap is taken. Circadian rhythms Ôreflect 24-h cycles of increases and decreases in a range of biological and physiological functions, including body temperature, heart rate, and hormone secretionõ (Hasher et al., 2002, p. 200). These rhythms are also seen in behavioral indices such as cognitive functioning (Hasher et al., 2002; Higuchi et al., 2000) and mood (Giannotti et al., 2002). Broughton (1989) summarized that there is well-documented evidence for an increase in sleep tendency in the afternoon, making naps most likely at this time of day. Lavie and Weler (1989) demonstrated that sleep efficiency was better, sleep latency shorter, and amount of SWS greater in a nap taken closer to the afternoon circadian dip in alertness (15:00 17:00 hours) compared with a nap taken during the evening forbidden zone for sleep (19:00 21:00 hours). However, Naitoh (1981) cautioned that naps taken at the wrong time could lead to prolonged sleep inertia. As naps are influenced by circadian rhythms (Lockley and Skene, 1997), one research strategy involves varying the time of day a nap takes place during sustained wakefulness. For example, in a study by Dinges et al. (1987), 41 healthy, young adults were assigned to one of five nap groups after 6, 18, 30, 42, or 54 h of wakefulness. The groups were all permitted a 2-h nap during 56 h of sleep deprivation. All groups showed improved reaction time performance following the nap, although subjective sleepiness showed the expected increase as sleep loss increased. Naps taken after 6 or 18 h of wakefulness showed more benefits than those taken after 30, 42, or 54 h of wakefulness. As well, naps taken after fewer hours of wakefulness prevented a mean drop in body temperature. Dinges et al. (1987) concluded that the benefits seen after earlier naps resulted from the fact that naps taken earlier prevented the drop in body temperature seen with extended wakefulness. As naps taken later in the day were associated with greater amounts of wakefulness, a 2-h nap may have been insufficient to alleviate the decrements caused by the long period of wakefulness. Hence, later naps were not sufficient to reduce sleep pressure enough to generate noticeable changes. In a more recent line of research, participants in each of three studies (Hayashi and Hori, 1998; Hayashi et al., 1999a,b) underwent a repeated measures design that consisted of a 20-min nap condition and a no-nap condition at 1-week intervals. As well, in each study, assessments comprised of EEG recordings, Visual Analog Scales for sleepiness and fatigue, performance tests (logical reasoning, alphanumeric detection, addition, and auditory vigilance), and self-rating of performance. Subjective sleepiness, subjective rating of performance, and EEG arousal were improved following a nap in mid-afternoon (14:00 hours; Hayashi et al., 1999b) and preceding the Ôpostlunch dipõ (12:20 hours; Hayashi and Hori, 1998; Hayashi et al., 1999a) but objective performance measures were only improved following the later nap. While Dinges et al. (1987) found that naps following less wakefulness were better, Hayashi et al. (1999a) showed that a later nap was better for performance and alertness. HayashiÕs group (1999a) suggested that because Dinges et al. had 12 h between naps and only 2 h separated the timing of the naps taken in their studies, they could not compare the results. The timing of a nap will have a bearing on the type or amount of benefit observed. Moreover, for sleep-satiated individuals, it may be best to choose a later nap time (following more wake time) to experience maximum benefits, while sleep-deprived individuals are more likely to be extremely fatigued, and thus earlier naps are indicated. Timing of testing Research has also investigated performance at different clock times following a nap placed at a fixed time to determine how long performance benefits last. For example, in one study (Hayashi et al., 1999a), participants each underwent both a nap and no-nap condition at 12:20 hours, and completed a performance assessment battery at multiple testing times throughout the day (every 20 min from 10:00 to 18:00 hours except for 12:20 to 13:00 hours). As reported above, neither reaction time nor accuracy on logical reasoning, mathematics, or auditory vigilance tasks were improved following the nap. Additionally, although a time of day effect was found for

5 276 C. E. Milner and K. A. Cote sleepiness, fatigue, and motivation in this study, changes across the day were due to circadian effects alone and were apparent following both the nap and no-nap conditions. It would be interesting to determine the relative contributions of clock time versus duration of postnap wakefulness in these studies. In summary, research with healthy, young adults, which has focused on the circadian timing of naps has not identified a ÔbestÕ time to nap, although the circadian dip in alertness (i.e., mid-afternoon) is also when sleep propensity is highest. The best time to nap might depend on factors, such as individual sleep need, stability and timing of sleep wake schedule, morningness-eveningness tendencies, quality of sleep during the preceding night, quality of sleep during the nap, or amount of prior wakefulness. The benefits of napping, in general, likely depend on the interaction of these factors. NAP DURATION As well as manipulating the timing of a nap, a number of studies have systematically varied the length of a nap to determine the shortest nap that may be taken to achieve maximum benefits [e.g., Helmus et al. (1997); Lumley et al. (1986); Tietzel and Lack (2002a,b)]. At the same time, these studies aimed to determine the longest nap that may be taken without resulting in substantial sleep inertia. Many of these studies aimed at identifying practical recommendations for napping in the workplace. A series of studies (Brooks and Lack, 2006; Tietzel and Lack, 2001, 2002a,b) has recently focused on identifying the ÔidealÕ nap duration, using healthy, young adults with no history of sleep difficulties as participants. In a first study, Tietzel and Lack (2001) allowed participants to sleep for 10 or 30 min. Benefits were seen following both a 10- and 30-min nap but a delay was needed to see benefits following the longer nap due to sleep inertia. As the longer nap did not prove to be superior, Tietzel and Lack (2001) hypothesized that benefits of napping depended not on the depth or length of sleep, but instead on another process, sleep onset. Here, simply initiating sleep would be sufficient to lead to measurable benefits. This idea was investigated in a subsequent study, where participants limited their sleep on the night prior to the study to 5 h. Tietzel and Lack (2002b) compared a 10-min nap with 30- and 90-s naps to determine if the onset of Stage 1 sleep produced postnap improvements. Thus, in this study, participants were awoken after 30 (i.e., 1 epoch) or 90 (i.e., 3 epochs) seconds of Stage 1 sleep (defined as less than 50% of the epoch characterized by alpha) or after 10 min of sleep from the onset of Stage 1 sleep. The 10-min nap improved subjective and objective alertness, decreased fatigue, increased vigor, and improved performance. No such effects were seen following either length of ultra-short nap, indicating that more than sleep onset is needed to make naps beneficial. These results are consistent with Process S, as described in the two-process model of sleep wake regulation (Borbely, 1982), which predicts that longer sleep is required for restoration and improvements in waking performance and cognition in particular. In a third study, Tietzel and Lack (2002a) used a dose response paradigm to compare 5-, 10-, 20-, and 30-min naps, as well as no nap, to determine if brief naps were as effective as longer naps. They showed that the 10-, 20-, and 30-min naps produced improvements in cognitive performance and alertness, while the 5-min nap and no nap conditions did not. Moreover, the 10-min nap showed immediate benefits, while the 20- and 30-min naps led initially to sleep inertia. Performance was improved 5 and 35 min after the 10-min nap, 95 min after the 20-min nap, and 35 and 155 min after the 30-min nap. Three hours postnap, there were no differences among all five conditions. Thus, benefits following the 10-min nap could be seen immediately after the nap, while a delay following naps of longer durations was needed to allow sleep inertia to dissipate. As well, the benefits following the 10-min nap were superior at all testing times compared with those following either longer nap. Consistent with the two-process model of sleep/wake regulation, it would be expected that longer naps would allow more opportunity for slow wave activity, reducing sleep pressure, thereby producing greater waking benefits. Conversely, increased slow wave activity would also lead to more sleep inertia, thereby delaying benefits. In general, the 10-min nap was more beneficial than longer naps of 20 and 30 min at immediate and delayed testing times, although all three nap lengths were shown to be of some benefit. Brooks and Lack (2006) published a fourth study using a similar paradigm, with the goal of identifying the underlying EEG changes that accompanied restorative nap benefits. Participants slept for 5, 10, 20, or 30 min in the afternoon or participated in a no-nap condition, all following a night restricted to 5 h of sleep. Results indicated that the 5-min nap was equivalent to the no-nap condition, the 10-min nap produced immediate benefits that were maintained, and the longer naps showed benefits emerging later in the test bout. Benefits were attributed to the presence of delta waves in the nap based on visual scoring of the sleep. Utilizing quantitative EEG in future studies would allow a finer measure of delta activity in the nap. Lahl et al. (2008) used a dose response paradigm to investigate the benefit of a daytime nap on memory performance. In an initial study, they reported that naps were superior to a waking condition for memory consolidation, but they were unable to find a relationship between memory performance and any particular aspect of the nap. In a second study, they compared waking with short and long naps and found a dose response trend such that both naps were superior to waking and that the long nap was superior to the short nap. Again, there was no particular aspect of the nap which predicted improvement in memory. Other studies have employed participants experiencing daytime sleepiness. Helmus et al. (1997) investigated the effects of nap duration on daytime sleepiness, using the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT), in narcoleptic, sleep-deprived,

6 Benefits of napping in healthy adults 277 and sleep-satiated participants. Narcoleptic and sleep-satiated participants spent 8 h in bed prior to the study, while sleepdeprived participants spent no time in bed. The authors found that all groups had longer sleep latencies on the MSLT following a 120-min nap, showing that sleepiness was reduced following the longer nap compared with a 15-min nap. No evidence was found to determine if the pathologically sleepy group (i.e., narcoleptics), the experimentally sleep-deprived group, and the sleep-satiated group experienced different benefits of the nap. In a dose response paradigm, Lumley et al. (1986) employed a sleep latency measure to compare 15-, 30-, 60-, and 120-min recovery naps with a no-nap condition, each following a night of total sleep deprivation, in a repeated measures design with 10 healthy young adults (18 to 32 years old). Lumley et al. discovered that alertness increased with nap length, although alertness levels following the two longest nap durations were equivalent. Lumley et al. remarked that the extra 60 min in the 120-min nap was predominately REM sleep. As this extra sleep did not improve performance over and above the 60-min nap, REM sleep during a nap may not improve alertness measured by sleep latency tests. However, REM sleep may benefit individuals in other ways; for example, performance tasks involving learning and memory might show greater benefits following REM-containing naps [e.g., Mednick et al. (2003)]. In general, based on the research outlined above, healthy young adults should ideally nap for approximately 10 to 20 min [e.g., Hayashi and Hori (1998); Hayashi et al. (1999b); Tietzel and Lack (2002a)]. These short naps are ideal for workplace settings where performance immediately upon awakening is normally required. It seems that naps containing REM sleep may not contribute to alertness per se, but may play a role in specific types of memory consolidation gleaned during a nap. SLEEP INERTIA Longer naps (e.g., sleeping for 30 min or longer) produce sleep inertia, making nap benefits obvious only after a delay. Sleep inertia is characterized by a reduction in the ability to think and perform upon awakening due to sleep. Confusion, grogginess, and deficits to cognitive performance may be associated with this transition (Dinges, 1993). Sleep inertia is associated with longer naps as it is typically thought to result from awakening from SWS (Dinges, 1993). For example, Tietzel and Lack (2001) suggested that although a 10-min nap showed no sleep inertia effects, a 30-min nap did show the effect because 7.5 times more SWS occurred in the longer nap. In assessing the influence of sleep inertia, it is important also to consider the timing of the nap. Sleep inertia is worse in the circadian trough than the peak, coinciding with the time when sleep is more likely (Caldwell and Caldwell, 1998; Dinges et al., 1985). It is possible that this phenomenon is associated with the stage of sleep from which the individual awakens (i.e., sleep architecture; Tassi et al., 1992) and or depth of sleep throughout the sleep period (Jewett, 1997). In particular, sleep is deeper at this time (Jewett, 1997), creating more sleep inertia. The amount of wakefulness prior to the nap or the extent of sleep deprivation (Dinges et al., 1987) may also bear on the level of sleep inertia. Dinges (1993) noted that cognitive impairment due to sleep inertia was worse than that due to sleep deprivation, and that impairment due to sleep inertia is smaller for psychomotor and perceptual tasks than for information processing. In a recent experimental investigation of sleep inertia, Ferrara et al. (2000) reported that sleep inertia impairs sensory motor and cognitive tasks, is characterized by confusion and disorientation, worsens when individuals are sleep deprived, impairs accuracy more than speed, and varies in duration and severity with circadian phase. Reports have been inconsistent with respect to the length of sleep inertia following awakening [e.g., Dinges et al. (1981)]. It is important to have an estimate of sleep inertia duration because it must be controlled when testing how performance, arousal, and mood are affected following a nap; that is, these effects must be tested after sleep inertia has dissipated. Ferrara and De Gennaro (2000) found that sleep inertia impaired accuracy more than speed, while Hofer-Tinguely et al. (2005) observed the opposite, that speed was impaired more than accuracy. These differences may be related to the specific tasks measured and or differences in the timing of measurement. A number of factors likely influence the degree to which sleep inertia is experienced, including subject and test characteristics and homeostatic and circadian factors. THE BENEFITS OF NAPPING IN OLDER ADULTS Napping has been shown to benefit young adults. As older adults may be sleepier and have more opportunity to nap due to retirement, it is likely that napping is both more frequent in older adults, and perhaps more beneficial to this group. In a previous review paper, Takahashi (2003) focused on the role of napping in individuals who typically experience sleep loss such as shift workers and those with sleep and medical disorders. While a subset of older adults certainly have sleep disorders and poor health, even healthy older adults who experience agetypical changes to their sleep can benefit from appetitive naps. Research on napping in older adults is quite limited and is typically confined to survey data used to describe the frequency of napping in such a population. For instance, Webb and Swinburne (1971) reported that almost all individuals in a small sample of 19 elderly adults took naps; they suggested that some napping was appetitive. Beh (1994) and Ohayon and Zulley (1999) found that the prevalence of napping increased linearly with age which was also shown with the 2003 ÔSleep in AmericaÕ poll (National Sleep Foundation, 2003). For adults aged 65 and over, Beh reported that nap frequency increased with each 5- year age range, and that a significantly greater proportion of the people aged 75 and above took a daily nap compared with those 70 years old and younger. Ohayon and Zulley similarly found

7 278 C. E. Milner and K. A. Cote that 22% of individuals aged 15 to 99 years napped at least twice a week, but this number increased to 53% for those aged just 75 and above. Several studies have investigated the benefits of napping for older adults in particular. Tanaka et al. (2001, 2002) investigated the impact of a nap on the well-being of older adults and found that the combination of a 30-min afternoon nap and moderate intensity exercise in the evening improved sleep quality (i.e., reduced wake after sleep onset, increased sleep efficiency) and reduced subjective daytime sleepiness. The authors also showed that mental health was improved after this intervention. Creighton (1995) demonstrated that some cognitive improvements were apparent when naps were taken by older hospital patients. Participants completed measures of alertness, concentration, strength, coordination, and reaction time. Patients improved on all but reaction time, which is contrary to the napping literature in young adults where response time is typically improved following a nap, e.g., Song et al. (2002). Tanaka and Shirakawa (2004) reported that short naps and moderate exercise produce not only improvements in sleep and well-being but also improved performance on a verbal memory test. Campbell et al. (2005) also confirmed that napping improved performance on a logical reasoning task, two-letter search task, Stroop congruency task, and Wilkinson four-choice reaction time task, in 55 to 85 year olds, with benefits emerging immediately after the nap and throughout the next day. Tamaki et al. (1999, 2000) employed a repeated measures design to assess the benefits of napping in older adults. They demonstrated that with nap and resting conditions 1 week apart, a daytime nap improved performance on a visual detection task and decreased subjective and objective sleepiness and fatigue in older individuals. A recent study by Milner and Cote (2007) investigated whether naps of different lengths benefited younger, middleaged, and older adults in different ways. Given that sleep is typically less restorative with advancing age, they hypothesized that older adults might need a longer nap to accrue the same benefits as younger adults. The authors confirmed that naps are beneficial for mood and cognitive performance but found that older adults are able to garner as much benefit from a nap as their younger counterparts. Interestingly, however, quantitative EEG measures collected during wakefulness suggested that older adults had to put forth more effort to achieve the same results on performance tests. In summary, research examining napping in older adults has shown that they nap more frequently than their younger counterparts, and that napping is beneficial to older adults who nap for both restorative and appetitive reasons. Specifically, research has confirmed that napping leads to improvements in well-being and cognitive performance for older adults. EXPERIENCE WITH NAPPING Very little research has been conducted to investigate individualsõ levels of experience with napping (i.e., whether they habitually nap or not). Most studies such as those by Taub et al. (1976, 1977) and Taub (1979) utilized only habitual nappers simply to ensure that participants slept in the laboratory environment. Anecdotally, it seems that there are individuals who avoid napping and those who must have a daily nap. Adults may choose to nap on a regular basis for many reasons, including perception of sleepiness, opportunity (Cottrell and Hildebrandt Karraker, 2002), and quality and amount of nocturnal sleep (Chan et al., 1989; Cottrell and Hildebrandt Karraker, 2002). A standard definition of habitual napping remains lacking but several operational definitions based on frequency of napping have been put forth (Table 2). An interesting line of research is to examine the structure of the nap in these two groups. Individuals who nap frequently and those who do not nap may differ in their ability to sleep during a daytime nap, the quality of their sleep, and or the benefits derived from napping. Habitual nappers might be predisposed to be good daytime nappers, or they may have learned to become skilled nappers through practice, perhaps being rewarded by increased well-being following the nap. Non-habitual nappers might self-select out of daytime napping because they experience no benefit. They may be unable to fall Table 2 Definitions of habitual and non-habitual nappers Study Lawrence and Shurley (1970) Collected information on frequency of napping, typical nap duration, and typical nap time from 505 male and female undergraduates, aged % of students napped when not working 4.5% of students napped 4 or more days per week; 6.5% napped 3 days per week; 15.6% napped 2 days per week; 24.9% napped 1 day per week; all others had variable napping frequencies Approximately one-third of students napped for 1 h, one-third for 2 3 h, one-third had no consistent nap duration Over half the students napped in the afternoon Taub et al. (1976) Habitual nappers were those who napped in the afternoon one or more times per week, for h, for at least 2 years Evans et al. (1977) Nappers were those who sometimes, usually, or always took naps during the day, and they also found napping to have restorative effects (less sleepy, more satisfied, and more subjective benefit after a 60-min nap than non-nappers) Non-nappers were those who rarely or never napped Spiegel (1981) 61 to 70 year olds Habitual nappers defined as those who took a daily daytime nap (about half the sample) Only habitual nappers awoke refreshed from a nap Bliwise and Swan (2005) Habitual nappers were characterized by older age, higher depression scores, and poorer performance on the trail making test Milner et al. (2006) 70.8% of 137 first-year undergraduates were habitual nappers (napped every day or once or twice a week) 29.2% of respondents were non-habitual nappers (napped at most once or twice a month)

8 Benefits of napping in healthy adults 279 asleep, awaken too often, or sleep lightly; they may also sleep too deeply, thus receiving negative feedback from sleep inertia following the nap. Dinges (1992) compared nap sleep architecture of habitual and non-habitual nappers. Habitual nappers were found to have more Stage 1 and more stage shifts, indicating that nonhabitual nappers had more consolidated sleep. Those who do not regularly nap may therefore experience greater sleep inertia, perhaps explaining why some people choose not to nap. Furthermore, it may be the case that habitual nappers are more sensitive to ultradian rhythms (i.e., biological rhythms with a periodicity shorter than 24 h), which allows them to fall asleep more easily in the daytime (Schulz, 1993). Dinges (1992) also reported that habitual nappers had lower prenap body temperatures on days when they napped compared with days when they did not nap, and lower prenap body temperatures compared with non-habitual nappers on nap days. Although Dinges suggested that this effect reflected the preparatory response of habitual nappers to take a nap, it seems equally likely that habitual nappers take naps in response to this lowered temperature. Differences between habitual and non-habitual nappers may also be due to or reflected in underlying EEG differences. For instance, EEG power values that reflect depth or character of sleep may differ between these groups. In addition, differences in sleep phasic events such as k-complexes and sleep spindles may represent individual differences in sleep-related information processing such as consolidation of newly learned skills. In a study investigating these issues, Milner et al. (2006) compared habitual and non-habitual nappers on a measure of motor procedural learning. Power spectral analysis performed on the EEG during the nap revealed that habitual nappers had more alpha EEG frequencies. The fact that they were able to maintain a tonic level of alertness throughout the nap suggests that habitual nappers may be able to disperse sleep inertia more quickly and thereby perform task in a shorter time frame. Although previous research has confirmed that naps result in benefits to mood, alertness, and cognitive processing in habitual nappers, other studies have documented no performance differences between habitual and non-habitual nappers [e.g., Daiss et al. (1986); Keyes (1989)]. Milner et al., however, demonstrated that motor procedural learning was impaired for non-habitual nappers who took a nap in lab. Postnap performance improvement on the motor procedural task was predicted by the number of sleep spindles and sigma power ( Hz) during the nap; importantly, this finding held true for habitual nappers only. Additional research of this type would help to elucidate the effect of previous napping experience on the benefits that can be gained from a nap. CONCLUSION In conclusion, the existing literature shows that certain variables, such as the timing and duration of a nap, age, and experience with napping are important moderators of the benefits of naps. Information gained from the study of napping impacts our understanding of the functions of sleep itself. As researchers are beginning to employ practical daytime napping paradigms to investigate the role of sleep in learning and memory [e.g., Mednick et al. (2003); Tucker et al. (2006)], it will be important for the validity of these results to consider the influence of timing and duration of the nap, the timing of pre- and postnap testing sessions, and the participant characteristics. There is still a great deal of basic research to be carried out in the area of napping, all of which will better inform the application of prescribed napping in sleepy and applied populations. The following is a research agenda for the investigation of benefits of napping: Identifying the ideal time of day to nap. Assess the time course of benefits following naturalistically longer naps. Investigate factors that contribute to why healthy, sleepsatiated individuals become habitual or non-habitual nappers. Determine additional individual difference variables that influence the benefits derived from a nap. Obtain more specific information on the types of cognitive domains and performance tasks that are improved following napping. Gain more insight into the mechanisms responsible for the robust improvement in mood and affect regulation that consistently follows a nap. Gain a better understanding of the significance of subjective perception of improved alertness and well-being following a nap. Apply more quantitative electrophysiological methods such as EEG and ERPs to understand the neurophysiological basis of nap-related improvements. Investigate the contribution of sleep phasic events such as k-complexes and spindles to the sleep-dependent changes in performance. Use multiple channel EEG ERPs and imaging techniques to localize neural sources driving the immediate benefits in arousal, mood, and performance that can be achieved from such a short bout of sleep. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The Brock University Sleep Research Laboratory is funded by the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) of Canada. REFERENCES Batejat, D. M. and Lagarde, D. P. Naps and modafinil as countermeasures for the effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive perfomance. Aviat. Space Environ. Med., 1999, 70: Beh, H. C. A survey of daytime napping in an elderly Australian population. Aust. J. Psychol., 1994, 46: Betrus, P. A. Afternoon naps: immediate and delayed effects on performance and mood. Dissert. Abstr. Int., 1986, 46: Bliwise, D. L. and Swan, G. E. Habitual napping and performance on the Trail Making test. J. Sleep Res., 2005, 14:

9 280 C. E. Milner and K. A. Cote Bonnet, M. H. The effect of varying prophylactic naps on performance, alertness and mood throughout a 52-hour continuous operation. Sleep, 1991, 14: Bonnet, M. H. and Arand, D. L. The use of prophylactic naps and caffeine to maintain performance during a continuous operation. Ergonomics, 1994, 37: Bonnet, M. H. and Arand, D. L. The effect of naps and caffeine on alertness during sleep loss and nocturnal work periods. In: R. W. Backs and W. Boucsein (Eds) Engineering Psychophysiology. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 2000: Bonnet, M. H., Gomez, S., Wirth, O. and Arand, D. L. The use of caffeine versus prophylactic naps in sustained performance. Sleep, 1995, 18: Borbely, A. A. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum. Neurobiol., 1982, 1: Brooks, A. J. and Lack, L. C. A brief afternoon nap following nocturnal sleep restriction: which nap duration is most recuperative? Sleep, 2006, 29: Broughton, R. J. Chronobiological aspects and models of sleep and napping. In: D. F. Dinges and R. J. Broughton (Eds) Sleep and Alertness: Chronobiological, Behavioural, and Medical Aspects of Napping. Raven Press, New York, NY, 1989: Broughton, R. J. and Dinges, D. F. Napping: a ubiquitous enigma. In: D. F. Dinges and R. J. Broughton (Eds) Sleep and Alertness: Chronobiological, Behavioural, and Medical Aspects of Napping. Raven Press, New York, NY, 1989: 1 7. Caldwell, J. A. and Caldwell, J. L. Comparison of the effects of Zolpidem-induced prophylactic naps to placebo naps and forced rest periods in prolonged work schedules. Sleep, 1998, 21: Campbell, S. S., Murphy, P. J. and Stauble, T. N. Effects of a nap on nighttime sleep and waking function in older subjects. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc., 2005, 53: Chan, O. Y., Phoon, W. H., Gan, S. L. and Ngui, S. J. Sleep-wake patterns and subjective sleep quality of day and night workers: interaction between napping and main sleep episodes. Sleep, 1989, 12: Cottrell, L. and Hildebrandt Karraker, K. Correlates of nap taking in mothers of young infants. J. Sleep Res., 2002, 11: Creighton, C. Effects of afternoon rest on the performance of geriatric patients in a rehabilitation hospital: a pilot study. Am. J. Occup. Ther., 1995, 49: Daiss, S. R., Bertelson, A. D. and Benjamin, L. T. Napping versus resting: effects on performance and mood. Psychophysiology, 1986, 23: Dijk, D. J., Beersma, D. G. M. and Daan, S. EEG power density during nap sleep: reflection of an hourglass measuring the duration of prior wakefulness. J. Biol. Rhythms, 1987, 2: Dinges, D. F. Napping patterns and effects in human adults. In: D. F. Dinges and R. J. Broughton (Eds) Sleep and Alertness: Chronobiological, Behavioural, and Medical Aspects of Napping. Raven Press, New York, NY, 1989: Dinges, D. F. Adult napping and its effects on ability to function. In: C. Stampi (Ed.) Why We Nap: Evolution, Chronobiology, and Functions of Polyphasic and Ultrashort Sleep. Birkhauser, Boston, MA, 1992: Dinges, D. F. Sleep inertia. In: M. A. Carskadon, A. Rechtschaffen, G. Richardson, T. Roth and J. Siegel (Eds) Encyclopedia of Sleep and Dreaming. Maxwell Macmillan Canada, Toronto, ON, 1993: Dinges, D. F., Orne, E. C., Evans, F. J. and Orne, M. T. Performance after naps in sleep-conducive and alerting environments. In: L. C. Johnson, D. I. Tepas, W. P. Colquhoun and M. J. Colligan (Eds) Biological Rhythms, Sleep and Shift Work. SP Medical and Scientific Books, New York, NY, 1981: Dinges, D. F., Orne, M. T. and Orne, E. C. Assessing performance upon abrupt awakening from naps during quasi-continuous operations. Behav. Res. Meth. Ins. C., 1985, 17: Dinges, D. F., Orne, M. T., Whitehouse, W. G. and Orne, E. C. Temporal placement of a nap for alertness: contributions of circadian phase and prior wakefulness. Sleep, 1987, 10: Evans, F. J., Cook, M. R., Cohen, H. D., Orne, E. C. and Orne, M. T. Appetitive and replacement naps: EEG and behavior. Science, 1977, 197: Ferrara, M. and De Gennaro, L. The sleep inertia phenomenon during the sleep-wake transition: theoretical and operational issues. Aviat. Space Environ. Med., 2000, 71: Ferrara, M., De Gennaro, L., Casagrande, M. and Bertini, M. Selective slow-wave sleep deprivation and time-of-night effects on cognitive performance upon awakening. Psychophysiology, 2000, 37: Giannotti, F., Cortesi, F., Sebastiani, T. and Ottaviano, S. Circadian preference, sleep and daytime behavior in adolescence. J. Sleep Res., 2002, 11: Gillberg, M. The effects of two alternative timings of a one-hour nap on early morning performance. Biol. Psychol., 1984, 19: Gillberg, M., Kecklund, G., Axelsson, J. and Akerstedt, T. The effects of a short daytime nap after restricted night sleep. Sleep, 1996, 19: Hasher, L., Chung, C., May, C. P. and Foong, N. Age, time of testing, and proactive interference. Can. J. Exp. Psychol., 2002, 56: Hayashi, M. and Hori, T. The effects of a 20-min nap before postlunch dip. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci., 1998, 52: Hayashi, M., Ito, S. and Hori, T. The effects of a 20-min nap at noon on sleepiness, performance and EEG activity. Int. J. Psychophysiol., 1999a, 32: Hayashi, M., Watanabe, M. and Hori, T. The effects of a 20 min nap in the mid-afternoon on mood, performance and EEG activity. Clin. Neurophysiol., 1999b, 110: Hayashi, M., Masuda, A. and Hori, T. The alerting effects of caffeine, bright light and face washing after a short daytime nap. Clin. Neurophysiol., 2003, 114: Hayashi, M., Motoyoshi, N. and Hori, T. Recuperative power of a short daytime nap with or without Stage 2 sleep. Sleep, 2005, 28: Helmus, T., Rosenthal, L., Bishop, C., Roehrs, T., Syron, M. L. and Roth, T. The alerting effects of short and long naps in narcoleptic, sleep deprived, and alert individuals. Sleep, 1997, 20: Higuchi, S., Liu, Y., Yuasa, T., Maeda, A. and Motohashi, Y. Diurnal variation in the P300 component of human cognitive event-related potential. Chronobiol. Int., 2000, 17: Hirose, A. and Nagasaka, A. The effects of the level of alertness during the rest period on subsequent performance. Int. J. Occup. Saf. Ergon., 2003, 9: Hofer-Tinguely, G., Achermann, P., Landolt, H.-P., Regel, S. J., Retey, J. V., Durr, R., Borbely, A. A. and Gottselig, J. M. Sleep inertia: performance changes after sleep, rest and active waking. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res., 2005, 22: Jewett, M. E. Models of circadian and homeostatic regulation of human performance and alertness. Dissert. Abstr. Int., 1997, 58: Keyes, K. Effects of sleep quality and daytime naps on the mood and functioning of older persons. Dissert. Abstr. Int., 1989, 49: Lahl, O., Wispel, C., Willigens, B. and Pietrowsky, R. An ultra short episode of sleep is sufficient to promote declarative memory performance. J. Sleep Res., 2008, 17: Lavie, P. and Weler, B. Timing of naps: effects on post-nap sleepiness levels. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol., 1989, 72: Lawrence, B. and Shurley, J. Napping habits of a college student population. Psychophysiology, 1970, 7: Lockley, S. W. and Skene, D. J. Relationship between napping and melatonin in the blind. J. Biol. Rhythms, 1997, 12: Lumley, M., Roehrs, T., Zorick, F., Lamphere, J. and Roth, T. The alerting effects of naps in sleep-deprived subjects. Psychophysiology, 1986, 23:

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days Sleep Research Online 5(1): 13-17, 2003 http://www.sro.org/2003/hayashi/13/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 96-214X 2003 WebSciences The Effects of Short Daytime s for Five Consecutive Mitsuo

More information

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep Sleep. 19(7):570-575 1996 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep Mats Gillberg, Garan Kecklund, John Axelsson and

More information

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle Sleep 10(1):62-68, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components

More information

Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People

Sleep and Body Temperature in Morning and Evening People Sleep. 8(4):311-318 1985 Raven Press. New York Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People Jean Foret, *Nathalie Touron, *Odile Benoit, and *Ginette Bouard Laboratoire de Physiologie Neurosensorielle

More information

Sleep Medicine Reviews

Sleep Medicine Reviews Sleep Medicine Reviews 14 (2010) 249 258 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Sleep Medicine Reviews journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/smrv CLINICAL REVIEW Naps, cognition and performance

More information

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants Sleep, 5:S128-S134 1982 Raven Press, New York Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants T. Roth, T. Roehrs, and F. Zorick Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, Michigan

More information

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep Sleep, 6(4):319-325 1983 Raven Press, New York Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep T. Roehrs, F. Zorick, J. Sicklesteel, R. Wittig, and T. Roth Sleep Disorders and Research

More information

Daytime Sleepiness and Antihistamines

Daytime Sleepiness and Antihistamines Sleep, 7(2): 137-141 1984 Raven Press, New York Daytime Sleepiness and Antihistamines imothy A. Roehrs, Elizabeth I. ietz, Frank J. Zorick, and homas Roth Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford

More information

Sleep and Human Performance

Sleep and Human Performance Sleep and Human Performance Kevin Walker MD Scott Schauss PA-C Intermountain Sleep Disorders Clinic Disclosures: Objectives How sleep extension and restriction impact performance To Nap or Not to Nap Circadian

More information

Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours

Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours Halszka OGINSKA Department of Ergonomics, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland Abstract:

More information

SLEEP, ADOLESCENCE AND SCHOOL Overview of problems and solutions

SLEEP, ADOLESCENCE AND SCHOOL Overview of problems and solutions SLEEP, ADOLESCENCE AND SCHOOL Overview of problems and solutions Professor Greg Murray, FAPS Dr Suzanne Warner Today s talk Why do we sleep? What s wrong with adolescent sleep? How can adolescents improve

More information

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults Sleep 13(5):385-394, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1990 Association of Professional Sleep Societies The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults M.

More information

COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE

COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE J. Snel 1, M.B. de Ruiter 1, J. Ruijter 1 and M.M. Lorist 2 1 Department of Psychonomics, Faculty of Psychology, University of Amsterdam 2 Faculty of Psychology, Rijks

More information

Physiology & Behavior

Physiology & Behavior Physiology & Behavior 107 (2012) 40 44 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Physiology & Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/phb The effect of nap frequency on daytime sleep

More information

Managing Sleep and Fatigue in Today s Healthcare Environment Tricks of the Trade

Managing Sleep and Fatigue in Today s Healthcare Environment Tricks of the Trade Managing Sleep and Fatigue in Today s Healthcare Environment Tricks of the Trade 92 nd Annual Meeting of the American Association of Thoracic Surgery Scott Shappell, Ph.D. Clemson University How tired

More information

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm Sleep, 2(3):299-307 1980 Raven Press, New York The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm L. C. Johnson Naval Health Research Center, San Diego, California Summary: Two studies, using data from fragmented

More information

Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance

Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance Clin Sports Med 24 (2005) 237 249 Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance Hans P.A. Van Dongen, PhD*, David F. Dinges, PhD Division of Sleep and Chronobiology, Department of Psychiatry, University

More information

Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance. Gregory Belenky, M.D. Sleep and Performance Research Center

Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance. Gregory Belenky, M.D. Sleep and Performance Research Center Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance Gregory Belenky, M.D. The Earth at Night: The Problem of 24/7 Operations The 24-Hour Sleep/Wake Cycle Waking 0000 Slow Wave 1800 0600 REM 1200 Sleep-Related Factors Affecting

More information

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children SLEEP DISORDERS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children Distinctive Features of Pediatric Sleep Daytime sleepiness uncommon

More information

Fatigue and Circadian Rhythms

Fatigue and Circadian Rhythms 16.400/453J Human Factors Engineering Fatigue and Circadian Rhythms Caroline Lowenthal Lecture 19 1 16.400/453 Outline Situations where fatigue is a factor Effects of fatigue Sleep Components Circadian

More information

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11:

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11: Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11: 876-881. CLINICAL PEARL Managing the Effects of Shift Work in Medicine Holger Link, MD, and Robert Sack,

More information

The Alpha Attenuation Test: Assessing Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Narcolepsy-Cataplexy

The Alpha Attenuation Test: Assessing Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Narcolepsy-Cataplexy Sleep, 20(4):258-266 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society.j The Alpha Attenuation Test: Assessing Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Narcolepsy-Cataplexy Christi E. D. Alloway,

More information

Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation

Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation Sleep Research Online 2(4): 101-105, 1999 http://www.sro.org/1999/hodoba/101/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 1999 WebSciences Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation

More information

The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration

The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration Sara C. Mednick 1, Ken Nakayama 1, Jose L. Cantero 2, Mercedes Atienza 2, Alicia A. Levin 2, Neha Pathak 2 and Robert Stickgold 2 1 Department

More information

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children QUESTION #1: ARE SLEEP ISSUES IN CHILDREN THE SAME AS IN ADULTS? Distinctive Features

More information

Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value

Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value Sleep 10(6):590-599, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1987 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value Brian Levine, Timothy Roehrs, Edward Stepanski, Frank

More information

Why are we so sleepy?

Why are we so sleepy? Why are we so sleepy? Focus Fall 2017 Conference 37 th Annual Michael Aiello Memorial Respiratory Care & Sleep Medicine Poughkeepsie, NY Dr. Steven A Thau MD Director, Pulmonary and Sleep Medicine Phelps

More information

Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed. NAPNAP National Conference 2018

Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed. NAPNAP National Conference 2018 Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed NAPNAP National Conference 2018 (Honaker & Meltzer, 2016; Keyes, Maslowsky, Hamilton & Schulenberg, 2015) Chronically disrupted sleep

More information

Overview. Surviving shift work. What is the circadian rhythm? Components of a Generic Biological Timing System 31/10/2017

Overview. Surviving shift work. What is the circadian rhythm? Components of a Generic Biological Timing System 31/10/2017 Overview Surviving shift work Dr Claire M. Ellender Respiratory and Sleep Physician Princess Alexandra Hospital Conflicts nil relevant Circadian rhythm Impacts of shift work on health Case example Circadian

More information

Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals

Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals Sleep 2(5):449-457, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 989 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals Timothy Roehrs, Victoria Timms, Ardith Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos,

More information

IN ITS ORIGINAL FORM, the Sleep/Wake Predictor

IN ITS ORIGINAL FORM, the Sleep/Wake Predictor Commentary on the Three-Process of Alertness and Broader ing Issues Jaques Reifman and Philippa Gander REIFMAN J, GANDER P. Commentary on the three-process model of alertness and broader modeling issues.

More information

COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY

COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY Jeklin, A., Aguirre, A., Guttkuhn, R., Davis, W. Circadian Technologies Inc., Boston, United States Introduction Petrochemical

More information

This brief animation illustrates the EEG patterns of the different stages of sleep, including NREM and REM sleep.

This brief animation illustrates the EEG patterns of the different stages of sleep, including NREM and REM sleep. Brain wave frequency and amplitude This brief animation illustrates the EEG patterns of the different stages of sleep, including NREM and REM sleep. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=u WYwMnMMEoU&feature=related

More information

Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation

Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation Sleep. 18( I): 1--6 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation Validation of the Sand C Components of the Three-Process Model of Alertness Regulation

More information

Defense Technical Information Center Compilation Part Notice

Defense Technical Information Center Compilation Part Notice UNCLASSIFIED Defense Technical Information Center Compilation Part Notice ADP010467 TITLE: Sleep Inertia and On-Call Readiness DISTRIBUTION: Approved for public release, distribution unlimited This paper

More information

Why Do We Sleep At Night?

Why Do We Sleep At Night? 4 Why Do We Sleep At Night? Now that you know what actually happens across the sleep period, let s look at what determines when we sleep. There are two biological determiners of sleep, sleep pressure and

More information

Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence

Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence Sleep, 5:S165S174 1982 Raven Press, New York Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence F. Zorick, T. Roehrs, G. Koshorek, J. Sicklesteel, *K. Hartse, R. Wittig, and T.

More information

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms People typically sleep about 8 hours per day, and spend 16 hours awake. Most people sleep over 175,000 hours in their lifetime. The vast amount of time spent sleeping

More information

Stage at Awakening, Sleep Inertia and Performance

Stage at Awakening, Sleep Inertia and Performance Sleep Research Online 5(3): 89-97, 2003 http://www.sro.org/2003/cavallero/89/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 2003 WebSciences Stage at Awakening, Sleep Inertia and Performance Corrado

More information

Symptoms of Narcolepsy

Symptoms of Narcolepsy Symptoms of Narcolepsy v Sleep attacks Brief episodes of sleep that occur many times a day May occur without warning or be preceded by drowsiness Patient usually feels refreshed afterwards Refractory period

More information

Arousal and Vigilance: Do They Differ? Study in a Sleep Inertia Paradigm

Arousal and Vigilance: Do They Differ? Study in a Sleep Inertia Paradigm Sleep Research Online 5(3): 83-87, 2003 http://www.sro.org/2003/tassi/83/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 096-24X 2003 WebSciences Arousal and Vigilance: Do They Differ? Study in a Sleep Inertia

More information

Sleep and Dreaming. Sleep Deprivation Trivia

Sleep and Dreaming. Sleep Deprivation Trivia Sleep and Dreaming Sleep Deprivation Trivia Peter Tripp stayed awake for 201 hours in 1959. Guinness Book of Records record is 18 days, 21 hours, 40 minutes. Sleep deprivation implicated in Three Mile

More information

Stage REM. Stage 3/4. Stage 2. Sleep 101. NREM vs. REM. Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Is Needed To: 9/24/2013

Stage REM. Stage 3/4. Stage 2. Sleep 101. NREM vs. REM. Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Is Needed To: 9/24/2013 The Power of Sleep: Supporting Healthy Sleep in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders REM Stage 1 TERRY KATZ, PHD UNIVERSITY OF COLORADO SCHOOL OF MEDICINE JFK PARTNERS CHILD DEVELOPMENT UNIT, CHILDREN

More information

Understanding Sleep Regulatory Processes to Improve Waking Performance

Understanding Sleep Regulatory Processes to Improve Waking Performance Understanding Sleep Regulatory Processes to Improve Waking Performance 1 st :envihab Symposium Cologne, Germany May 23, 2011 Daniel Aeschbach Division of Sleep Medicine Brigham and Women s Hospital Harvard

More information

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle Sleep Sleep is described as a state of unconsciousness or partial consciousness from which a person can be roused by stimulation Period of rest and recovery People spend about a third of their lives sleeping

More information

Key FM scientific principles

Key FM scientific principles Key FM scientific principles Philippa Gander Research Professor, Director Fatigue Management Approaches Symposium 5-6 April 2016, Montréal, Canada Fatigue a physiological state of reduced mental or physical

More information

Shift Work: An Occupational Health and Safety Hazard. Sandra Buxton, BA (Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Master of Philosophy

Shift Work: An Occupational Health and Safety Hazard. Sandra Buxton, BA (Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Master of Philosophy Shift Work: An Occupational Health and Safety Hazard Sandra Buxton, BA (Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Master of Philosophy of Murdoch University 2003 ii I declare that this thesis is

More information

Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis

Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis J. Sleep Res. (1999) 8, 237 245 REVIEW Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis NANCY JO WESENSTEN, THOMAS J. BALKIN and GREGORY BELENKY Department of Neurobiology and Behavior,

More information

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night?

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night? Sleep Research Online 2(1): 15-19, 1999 http://www.sro.org/1999/ferrara/15/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 1999 WebSciences Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound:

More information

The Hidden Dangers of Fatigue

The Hidden Dangers of Fatigue The Hidden Dangers of Fatigue Janette Edmonds BSc(Hons) MSc CErgHF FIEHF CMIOSH Director / Principal Consultant Ergonomist www.keilcentre.co.uk janette@keilcentre.co.uk 07967 164145 v1.0 0215 The Keil

More information

Sleep, circadian rhythms, and cognitive function. Rebecca M. C. Spencer, PhD Associate Professor of Psychological & Brain Sciences

Sleep, circadian rhythms, and cognitive function. Rebecca M. C. Spencer, PhD Associate Professor of Psychological & Brain Sciences Sleep, circadian rhythms, and cognitive function Rebecca M. C. Spencer, PhD Associate Professor of Psychological & Brain Sciences Current funding: Disclosures University of Massachusetts National Institutes

More information

Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems.

Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems. COURSES ARTICLE - THERAPYTOOLS.US Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems. Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems.

More information

Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness

Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness Sleep 20(3):215-223 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness Goran Kecklund Torbjom Akerstedt and Arne Lowden National

More information

Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes

Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY Copyright 1989 by The Society for Psychophysiological Research, Inc. Vol. 26, No. 5 Printed in U.S.A. Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes

More information

EFFECTS OF NIGHTTIME NAPS ON BODY TEMPERATURE CHANGES, SLEEP PATTERNS, AND SELF-EVALUATION

EFFECTS OF NIGHTTIME NAPS ON BODY TEMPERATURE CHANGES, SLEEP PATTERNS, AND SELF-EVALUATION J. Human Ergol., 10:173-184, 1981 EFFECTS OF NIGHTTIME NAPS ON BODY TEMPERATURE CHANGES, SLEEP PATTERNS, AND SELF-EVALUATION OF SLEEP Kazuya MATSUMOTO Department of Hygiene, Kyorin University School of

More information

Shift Work, Sleep, Health, Safety, and Solutions. Prof Philippa Gander PhD, FRSNZ Sleep/Wake Research Centre Massey University

Shift Work, Sleep, Health, Safety, and Solutions. Prof Philippa Gander PhD, FRSNZ Sleep/Wake Research Centre Massey University Shift Work, Sleep, Health, Safety, and Solutions Prof Philippa Gander PhD, FRSNZ Sleep/Wake Research Centre Massey University Defining shift work Shift work, sleep, health, and safety Shift work and fatigue

More information

Improving Your Sleep Course. Session 1 Understanding Sleep and Assessing Your Difficulties

Improving Your Sleep Course. Session 1 Understanding Sleep and Assessing Your Difficulties Improving Your Sleep Course Session 1 Understanding Sleep and Assessing Your Difficulties Course Information Session Details Sessions Session 1 Session 2 Session 3 Session 4 Optional Review Session 5 Session

More information

A Short Nap and Natural Bright Light Exposure Improve Positive Mood Status

A Short Nap and Natural Bright Light Exposure Improve Positive Mood Status Industrial Health 2007, 45, 301 308 Original Article A Short Nap and Natural Bright Light Exposure Improve Positive Mood Status Kosuke KAIDA*, Masaya TAKAHASHI and Yasumasa OTSUKA National Institute of

More information

P08 Reversible loss of consciousness. E365 Aviation Human Factors

P08 Reversible loss of consciousness. E365 Aviation Human Factors P08 Reversible loss of consciousness E365 Aviation Human Factors Need to sleep Sleep is a natural state of rest for the body and mind that involves the reversible loss of consciousness. You sleep to not

More information

sleep latencies Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients the tendency to fall asleep

sleep latencies Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients the tendency to fall asleep Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients Sleepiness the tendency to fall asleep Elke De Valck, Raymond Cluydts, Sandra Pirrera Department of Cognitive and Physiological Psychology, Vrije Universiteit

More information

Dr Alex Bartle. Director Sleep Well Clinic

Dr Alex Bartle. Director Sleep Well Clinic Dr Alex Bartle Director Sleep Well Clinic 1 Fatigue in the Workforce The structure of sleep Fatigue and sleep Consequences of fatigue Management of Shiftwork Conclusion Sleep Architecture REM NREM Rapid

More information

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology INSOMNIAS INSOMNIAS General criteria for insomnia A. Repeated difficulty with sleep initiation, duration, consolidation or quality. B. Adequate sleep opportunity, persistent sleep difficulty and associated

More information

Diurnal variations in the waking EEG: comparisons with sleep latencies and subjective alertness

Diurnal variations in the waking EEG: comparisons with sleep latencies and subjective alertness J. Sleep Res. (2000) 9, 243±248 Diurnal variations in the waking EEG: comparisons with sleep latencies and subjective alertness C. LAFRANCE and M. DUMONT Laboratoire de chronobiologie, Hoà pital du SacreÂ

More information

The Effects of Caffeine on Simulated Night-Shift Work and Subsequent Daytime Sleep

The Effects of Caffeine on Simulated Night-Shift Work and Subsequent Daytime Sleep Sleep. 18(1):22-29 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society The Effects of Caffeine on Simulated Night-Shift Work and Subsequent Daytime Sleep Mark J. Muehlbach and James K.

More information

Napping and its Effect on Short Term Memory 1

Napping and its Effect on Short Term Memory 1 Poster Presented at the April (2011) 39th Annual Western Pennsylvania Undergraduate Psychology Conference. New Wilmington, PA. Napping and its Effect on Short Term Memory 1 Abbie Leverich Robert Morris

More information

Rodney Heller LC, CLEP Senior Lighting Designer & Lighting Evangelist Energy Performance Lighting Cottage Grove, WI

Rodney Heller LC, CLEP Senior Lighting Designer & Lighting Evangelist Energy Performance Lighting Cottage Grove, WI Rodney Heller LC, CLEP Senior Lighting Designer & Lighting Evangelist Energy Performance Lighting Cottage Grove, WI Not promoting any product or manufacturer Based on existing body of research & my experience

More information

The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia

The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia Chapter e39 The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia Leon Lack and Helen Wright Department of Psychology, Flinders University, Adelaide, South Australia PROTOCOL NAME The use of bright light

More information

Adolescents and Sleep

Adolescents and Sleep Adolescents and Sleep Sarah Spinks is an independent director and producer. She was with the Canadian Broadcasting Corporation for seventeen years where her documentaries won many awards. Spinks' last

More information

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AG Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AG Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology Biological Psychology Unit Two AG Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology Consciousness Consciousness is your awareness of how and why you react to your surroundings. During this lesson, you may realize

More information

Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison With Young Adults

Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison With Young Adults Sleep 20(4):294-300 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep In Normal Subjects Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison

More information

Chapter 5. Variations in Consciousness 8 th Edition

Chapter 5. Variations in Consciousness 8 th Edition Chapter 5 Variations in Consciousness 8 th Edition Consciousness: Personal Awareness Awareness of Internal and External Stimuli Levels of awareness James stream of consciousness Freud unconscious Sleep/dreaming

More information

Caffeine Eliminates Psychomotor Vigilance Deficits from Sleep Inertia

Caffeine Eliminates Psychomotor Vigilance Deficits from Sleep Inertia PHARMACOLOGY Caffeine Eliminates Psychomotor Vigilance Deficits from Sleep Inertia Hans P.A. Van Dongen PhD, 1 Nicholas J. Price, 1 Janet M. Mullington PhD, 2 Martin P. Szuba MD, 1 Shiv C. Kapoor PhD,

More information

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch Dr Alex Bartle Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch 11:00-11:55 WS #113: Circadian Sleep Disorders 12:05-13:00 WS #125: Circadian Sleep Disorders (Repeated) Overview The Structure of Sleep

More information

THE PREVALENCE of shiftwork has substantially

THE PREVALENCE of shiftwork has substantially A Model to Predict Work-Related Fatigue Based on Hours of Work Gregory D. Roach, Adam Fletcher, and Drew Dawson ROACH GD, FLETCHER A, DAWSON D. A model to predict workrelated fatigue based on hours of

More information

Sleep Science: better sleep for you and your patients CHUNBAI ZHANG, MD MPH UW MEDICINE VALLEY MEDICAL CENTER

Sleep Science: better sleep for you and your patients CHUNBAI ZHANG, MD MPH UW MEDICINE VALLEY MEDICAL CENTER Sleep Science: better sleep for you and your patients CHUNBAI ZHANG, MD MPH UW MEDICINE VALLEY MEDICAL CENTER Disclosure: Financial - none Non-Financial - none Selected Topics Sleep epidemiology Sleep

More information

Sleep and Dreams UNIT 5- RG 5A

Sleep and Dreams UNIT 5- RG 5A Sleep and Dreams UNIT 5- RG 5A Goals for today Can you Discuss the circadian rhythm, what it is and how it effects us. Identify and explain each of the 5 stages of sleep. As well as the typical waves of

More information

Evidence for a Circadian Distribution of Eye Movement Density During REM Sleep in Humans

Evidence for a Circadian Distribution of Eye Movement Density During REM Sleep in Humans Sleep Research Online 4(2): 59-66, 2001 http://www.sro.org/2001/witzenhausen/59/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214 2001 WebSciences Evidence for a Circadian Distribution of Eye Movement

More information

Introduction. What is Shiftwork. Normal Human Rhythm. What are the Health Effects of Shiftwork? Blue Light

Introduction. What is Shiftwork. Normal Human Rhythm. What are the Health Effects of Shiftwork? Blue Light Shiftwork Health Effects and Solutions James Miuccio, MSc, CIH, CRSP Occupational Hygienist February 28, Introduction What is Shiftwork Normal Human Rhythm What are the Health Effects of Shiftwork? Blue

More information

Cumulative Sleepiness, Mood Disturbance, and Psychomotor Vigilance Performance Decrements During a Week of Sleep Restricted to 4-5 Hours per Night

Cumulative Sleepiness, Mood Disturbance, and Psychomotor Vigilance Performance Decrements During a Week of Sleep Restricted to 4-5 Hours per Night Sleep, 20(4):267-277 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Cumulative Sleepiness, Mood Disturbance, and Psychomotor Vigilance Performance Decrements During a Week of Sleep

More information

Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services

Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services www.lakeshoresupport.com Humans have biological rhythms, known as Circadian Rhythms (CR) CR refers to cyclical

More information

HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS. Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann Acacia Aguirre Martin Moore-Ede

HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS. Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann Acacia Aguirre Martin Moore-Ede Proceedings of the 23 Winter Simulation Conference S. Chick, P. J. Sánchez, D. Ferrin, and D. J. Morrice, eds. HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann

More information

NIGHT FIT AT SEAWAY HEAVY LIFTING. Improving the quality of sleep, health and safety at the Oleg Strashnov KM HUMAN FACTORS ENGINEERING

NIGHT FIT AT SEAWAY HEAVY LIFTING. Improving the quality of sleep, health and safety at the Oleg Strashnov KM HUMAN FACTORS ENGINEERING NIGHT FIT AT SEAWAY HEAVY LIFTING Improving the quality of sleep, health and safety at the Oleg Strashnov KM HUMAN FACTORS ENGINEERING Cornelis Vermuydenstraat 63 1018 RN, Amsterdam The Netherlands info@km-humanfactors.com

More information

Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects

Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects Sleep I ():54-60, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 988 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects Edward Stepanski,

More information

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include:

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include: INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS Introduction Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include: Intrinsic sleep disorders (e.g. narcolepsy, obstructive sleep apnoea/hypopnea

More information

Teenagers: Sleep Patterns and School Performance

Teenagers: Sleep Patterns and School Performance The National Healthy Sleep Awareness Project involves a partnership between the American Academy of Sleep Medicine, Center for Disease Control and Sleep Research Society. The long term goal of the project

More information

EEG Electrode Placement

EEG Electrode Placement EEG Electrode Placement Classifying EEG brain waves Frequency: the number of oscillations/waves per second, measured in Hertz (Hz) reflects the firing rate of neurons alpha, beta, theta, delta Amplitude:

More information

Circadian Rhythms in Children and Adolescents

Circadian Rhythms in Children and Adolescents Circadian Rhythms in Children and Adolescents Sarah Morsbach Honaker, Ph.D., CBSM Assistant Professor of Pediatrics IU School of Medicine Society for Behavioral Sleep Medicine Practice and Consultation

More information

Sleep Across the Life Cycle

Sleep Across the Life Cycle SECTION II Anatomy and Physiology CHAPTER 3 Sleep Across the Life Cycle IOURI KREININ L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S On completion of this chapter, the reader should be able to 1. Describe the elements

More information

Unit 3 Psychology ~ 2013

Unit 3 Psychology ~ 2013 Unit 3 Psychology ~ 2013 Sleep is described as regularly occurring altered state of consciousness that typically occurs spontaneously and is characterised by a loss of conscious awareness Sleep occurs

More information

Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH Professor of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Pediatrics Temple University Philadelphia, PA

Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH Professor of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Pediatrics Temple University Philadelphia, PA 37 th Annual Meeting Society of Behavioral Medicine The Impact of School Start Time Change on Adolescents Sleep, Health, Safety, and School Functioning Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH Professor of Epidemiology,

More information

Sleep & Brain Health

Sleep & Brain Health The Neuroscience School Sleep & Brain Health 2017 The Neuroscience School Sleep plays a vital role in good health and well-being throughout our life. Getting enough quality sleep at the right times can

More information

SLEEP PROBLEMS DURING ADOLESCENCE: LINKS

SLEEP PROBLEMS DURING ADOLESCENCE: LINKS In: The Dance of Sleeping and Eating Among Adolescents ISBN: 978-1-61209-710-7 Editors: Yael Latzer and Orna Tzischinsky 2012 Nova Science Publishers, Inc. Chapter 9 SLEEP PROBLEMS DURING ADOLESCENCE:

More information

Commercial Vehicle Drivers Hours of Service Module 1 Overview

Commercial Vehicle Drivers Hours of Service Module 1 Overview Module 1 Overview June 23, 2008 Things to think about What if there were no rules limiting how many hours a driver could drive a commercial vehicle? What would happen to the commercial vehicle driver?

More information

Exclusion criteria and outlier detection

Exclusion criteria and outlier detection 1 Exclusion criteria and outlier detection 1 2 Supplementary Fig. 1 31 subjects complied with the inclusion criteria as tested during the familiarization session. The upper part of the figure (ovals) indicates

More information

Week 4 Psychology. Theory of mind is an individual s understanding that they and others think, feel, perceive, and have private experiences.

Week 4 Psychology. Theory of mind is an individual s understanding that they and others think, feel, perceive, and have private experiences. Week 4 Psychology Before we explore the concept in detail, let us understand Theory of Mind as well as what Consciousness and Stream of Consciousness are. Theory of mind is an individual s understanding

More information

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association Sleep in patients with a number of sleep disorders and

More information

MODULE 7 SLEEP. By Dr David Dominic

MODULE 7 SLEEP. By Dr David Dominic MODULE 7 SLEEP By Dr David Dominic CONTENTS WHAT IS SLEEP? STAGES OF SLEEP WHY DO WE SLEEP? SLEEP ARCHITECTURE - THE RIGHT MIX OF SLEEP THE SLEEP-WAKE CYCLE SLEEP DEBT PRYMD SLEEP KEY TAKEAWAYS 3 4 6 7

More information

Students will be able to determine what stage of sleep someone is in by analyzing their EEG.

Students will be able to determine what stage of sleep someone is in by analyzing their EEG. Outline 2Lesson Unit1.2 OVERVIEW Rationale: This lesson is intended to engage students with the concept of the neural circuit. The lesson and unit as a whole use sleep, a behavior everyone is familiar

More information

Sleep Deprivation, Fatigue and Effects on Performance The Science and Its Implications for Resident Duty Hours

Sleep Deprivation, Fatigue and Effects on Performance The Science and Its Implications for Resident Duty Hours Sleep Deprivation, Fatigue and Effects on Performance The Science and Its Implications for Resident Duty Hours David F. Dinges, Ph.D. University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine ACGME Annual Educational

More information