The Alpha Attenuation Test: Assessing Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Narcolepsy-Cataplexy

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Alpha Attenuation Test: Assessing Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Narcolepsy-Cataplexy"

Transcription

1 Sleep, 20(4): American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society.j The Alpha Attenuation Test: Assessing Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Narcolepsy-Cataplexy Christi E. D. Alloway, Robert D. Ogilvie and *Colin M. Shapiro Brock University, St. Catharines, and *University of Toronto, The Toronto Hospital, Toronto, Ontario, Canada Summary: Daytime sleep tendency was assessed in 10 drug-free patients with narcolepsy-cataplexy and 10 normals matched for age and gender. Following nocturnal polysomnography, the alpha attenuation test (AAT) and the multiple sleep latency test (MSLT) were administered during five sessions occurring at 2-hour intervals beginning at 0900 and 1000 hours, respectively. For the AAT, participants were polysomnographically recorded for 8 minutes while seated in an illuminated room with their eyes alternately opened and closed. Power spectral analyses of electroencephalograph (EEG) activity at 02-Al (10 second epochs) were calculated using fast Fourier transformations (FFT) within the alpha frequency range (8-12 Hz) to obtain ratios of mean eyes-closed to mean eyes-open alpha power (i.e. the alpha attenuation coefficient, AAC). The narcoleptics were sleepier than the normals as indicated by a significantly smaller mean AAC and a significantly shorter mean latency to stage I on the MSLT. These findings suggest that the AAT may provide a quick and practical objective assessment of the excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) associated with narcolepsy. Key Words: Narcolepsy-Alpha attenuation test-excessive daytime sleepiness-physiological sleep tendency-eeg power spectral analysis. The multiple sleep latency test (MSLT) (1) is the traditional measure for objectively assessing excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) in individuals with narcolepsy. Physiological sleep tendency is assessed by measuring the speed at which individuals fall asleep on multiple occasions while lying with their eyes closed in a darkened room (2)-that is, the speed at which the electroencephalograph (EEG) moves from the alpha activity (8-12 Hz) of relaxed eyes-closed wakefulness to the theta activity (4-8 Hz) of the first stage of sleep. A mean sleep latency of 5 minutes or less on the MSLT is considered to represent a pathological degree of sleepiness and in conjunction with the presence of at least two sleep onset rapid eye movement (REM) periods, is considered diagnostic of narcolepsy (3). However, the validity of the MSLT may be questioned due to the confounding of sleepiness with the learned ability to fall asleep (4). Furthermore, the efficacy of the MSLT is limited by a floor effect (near zero latencies) in clinical populations (5) and the failure of EDS patients to show significant improvement in MSLT latencies following subjectively effective pharmacological treatment (6). In addition, Accepted for publication January Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Christi E. D. Alloway, Psychology Department, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada. 258 the cooperation of the patient in the attempt to fall asleep is essential to the validity of the MSLT, and patients wishing to avoid falling asleep may engage in a variety of behaviors to distort the MSLT results. Furthermore, the MSLT is time-consuming. The MSLT also relies on the presence of a polysomnographer to visually sleep score the EEG concurrently with each nap attempt, introducing a subjective component to an objective measure of sleepiness (7). These shortcomings suggest the need for alternate means of detecting pathological degrees of sleepiness. The technique of EEG power spectral analysis provides a method of quantifying fluctuations in sleepiness and alertness and has the potential to become an alternative to sleep latency testing in the assessment of EDS. Studies (8-9) have demonstrated that as individuals move from alertness toward sleepiness, EEG power in the alpha frequency range decreases when eyes are closed but increases when eyes are open. Based on these findings, the alpha attenuation test (AAT) (10) was developed as a new method of quantifying variations in physiological sleepiness. During the AAT, individuals are asked to repeatedly vary their eyelid position from open to closed while seated in an illuminated room. In a recent validation study, Stampi et al. (7) investigated the ratio of eyes-closed to eyesopen EEG alpha power (referred to as the alpha atten-

2 ALPHA AITENUATION TEST IN NARCOLEPSY 259 ~\., ~' tl- (~ uation coefficient, AAC) to determine its efficacy as an objective measure of physiological sleepiness. Stampi et al. (7) deprived 10 normal sleepers of sleep for 40 hours. Every 2 hours (for a total of 18 sessions) participants were administered the following battery of tests: subjective sleepiness measures and performance tests (starting on the hour), the AAT (at 20 minutes past the hour), and the MSLT (at 40 minutes past the hour) (7). The AAC was found to be sensitive to increasing sleepiness following one night of sleep loss such that participants were significantly less alert (i.e. had lower AACs) on the 2nd day of testing compared to the I st day (7). Furthermore, the AAC correlated significantly with the MSLT in eight out of 10 participants (i.e. r > 0.72 for four participants, r > 0.62 for three participants and r = 0.53 for one participant), and these correlations were higher than the correlations between the MSLT and the subjective sleepiness and performance measures, suggesting that the AAT provides a valid assessment of physiological sleepiness in sleepy normals (7). To our knowledge, the AAT has yet to be evaluated in patients with narcolepsy. The present study is the first to investigate the utility of the AAT in distinguishing narcoleptics from normals [however, preliminary findings have been reported in abstract form (11)]. On the basis of EEG power spectral studies of sleep-deprived normals (7,9) it was predicted that the ratio of eyes-closed to eyes-open alpha power (i.e. the AAC) would be lower in narcoleptics than normal controls and that, compared to controls, narcoleptics would demonstrate greater eyes-open alpha power and less eyes-closed alpha power. Participants METHODS Five female and five male patients diagnosed with narcolepsy-cataplexy and 10 normal sleepers matched for age and gender were studied. Narcoleptics were drug-free at the time of testing. Narcoleptics ranged in age from 29 to 62 years [mean = 44.3 years, standard deviation (SO) = 11.9 years], and controls ranged from 28 to 56 years (mean = 42.7 years, SO = 10.6 years). All participants gave their written informed consent and received an honorarium for their participation in the study. A screening process ensured that narcoleptics met the American Sleep Disorders Association's diagnostic criteria for narcolepsy (3) and that normal controls did indeed report normal sleep habits (i.e. the normals reported that they had no difficulty in falling or remaining asleep at night, typically slept between 6 and 8.5 hours a night, and were alert during the daytime). All narcoleptics complained of daytime sleepiness and reported taking naps daily or several times weekly. Normals reported that they were generally alert during the day. All narcoleptics had a history of cataplexy, six reported experiencing sleep paralysis and hypnagogic hallucinations at least once a month, and two reported incidents of hypnagogic hallucinations that occurred 1-5 times during their lifetime. No normals reported a history of cataplexy, although two normals reported experiencing incidents of sleep paralysis and four reported incidents of hypnagogic hallucinations occurring 1-5 times during their lifetime. All narcoleptics reported multiple nocturnal awakenings. Four normals reported no nocturnal awakenings, and six reported awakening 1-3 times during the night. Participants were also given a sleep diary in which they recorded their sleep and wake patterns for the 7 days preceding testing. A t test showed that narcoleptics and normals did not differ with respect to the selfreported mean duration of nocturnal sleep. The selfreported mean duration of nocturnal sleep ranged from 4.7 to 10.8 hours for the narcoleptics (mean = 7.9 hours, SD = 2.1 hours) and from 6.2 to 8.6 hours for the normal controls (mean = 7.4 hours, SD = 0.9 hours). Symptoms related to depression (which may elicit sleep onset REM periods) were assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory (BOI) (12), which consists of 21 statements related to depression. Participants rank each statement according to the degree to which it is experienced [i.e. from neutral (0) to maximal severity (3)]. BDI scores may range from 0 to 63. A t test showed that narcoleptics and normals did not differ with respect to the number of depression-related symptoms they reported on the BDl. The scores obtained on the BDI for the narcoleptics ranged from 1.0 to 25.0 (mean = 8.6, SO = 7.47) and for the normals ranged from 1.0 to 16.0 (mean = 4.5, SO = 5.0). Prior to (but not during) testing, nine of the 10 narcoleptics were taking medications for their symptoms. Six narcoleptics were taking central nervous system stimulants (five took methylphenidate hydrochloride, one took dexamphetamine sulfate) for their excessive daytime sleepiness, three were taking sleeping pills (immovane) to improve their nocturnal sleep, and two were taking REM sleep suppressants (clomipramine) to reduce their cataplexy. Two of the narcoleptics taking methylphenidate hydrochloride were withdrawn for 2 days prior to testing, and the seven remaining medicated narcoleptics were withdrawn from their medications for 7 days prior to testing. All of the normal participants and nine narcoleptics reported consuming caffeinated beverages. On average, normals consumed 2.5 cups of tea/coffee/pop per day (SO = 1.0). Narcoleptics consumed significantly

3 260 C. E. D. ALLOWAY ET AL. more cups of tea/coffee/pop per day (mean == 5.0, SD == 3.7) [t(18) == 2.13, P = 0.047]. Five normals and three narcoleptics reported consuming alcoholic beverages. On average, normals consumed 1.8 alcoholic drinks per week (SD = 2.7). This did not differ from the average number of alcoholic drinks consumed by narcoleptics (mean = 1.5 drinks weekly, SD = 3.0). None of the normals reported smoking cigarettes or pipes, whereas four narcoleptics smoked an average of 29 cigarettes per day. During testing, these participants were permitted to smoke after each MSLT session. All participants agreed to refrain from alcohol, caffeine, and sleep-related medications during the 24-hours preceding testing. Procedure Participants reported to the sleep lab at 2130 hours for orientation and electrode application. Electrodes were positioned to enable the monitoring of Cz (central) and 02 (right occipital) EEG [referenced to A2 (right mastoid)], submental electromyogram (EMG) and outer canthi electrooculogram (EOG) activity. A 16-channel Neurofax polygraph (Nihon Kohden, Irvine, CA) was used to amplify the polysomnographic recordings. Time constants were set at 0.3 for EEG and EOG recordings and at 0.03 for EMG recordings. High-frequency filters were set at 30 Hz for EEG and EOG and at 70 Hz for EMG. Recording sensitivity was 7 fl V /mm for EEG and EOG and 1-3 fl V /mm for EMG. Polysomnographic data from the polygraph were acquired on paper and on computer using the software program Microcomputer Quantitative Electrophysiology (Imaging Research Inc., under continued development at Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada). Participants retired for the night at 2300 hours and slept undisturbed until 0800 hours. The MSLT was administered according to the guidelines for clinical use outlined in the report from the American Sleep Disorders Association (13). Following the nocturnal polysomnography, nap opportunities were given at 1000, 1200, 1400, 1600, and 1800 hours. At the start of each session of the MSLT, participants were instructed to try to fall asleep while lying quietly with their eyes closed in a darkened room. During the MSLT session, EEG, EOG, and EMG were recorded on paper and computer according to the parameters outlined above and were scored in 30 second epochs using the criteria of Rechtschaffen and Kales (14). Sleep onset was defined as the first three consecutive minutes of stages 1, 2, or REM sleep. Each MSLT session was terminated either 15 minutes following the initial onset of sleep or after 20 minutes in bed with no sleep. A mixed two-factor 2 X 5 (group-by-session) analysis of variance (ANOV A) was performed on the latency to stage 1 sleep with session extracted as the within-subjects factor. The 8-minute version of the AAT was administered at 0900, 1100, 1300, 1500, and 1700 hours. Participants were polysomnographically recorded while seated in an illuminated room within 3 feet of a wall upon which an "X" made of black tape had been placed at eye level. Participants were instructed t~ sit quietly with their eyes open and focus on the black tape on the wall. Following 1 minute of artifact-free recording with eyes open, participants were instructed to sit quietly with their eyes closed for 1 minute. Each eyesopen and eyes-closed session was repeated three more times so that a total of eight I-minute samples of artifact-free EEG were obtained. The MSLT and AAT were scheduled 1 hour apart to reduce the likelihood that participating in one task would influence the other. For the AAT, EEG data were digitized using a sampling rate of Hz with a digitizer sensitivity of 16 bits for ± 2.83 volts. Power spectral analyses of eyes-open and eyes-closed EEG at 02 were calculated using FFT on 10 second epochs within the alpha frequency band (8-12 Hz) using a bin size of 0.1 Hz. The bins were averaged across the 8-12-Hz frequency range to produce an estimate of alpha power in squared microvolts. The ratio of mean eyes-closed to mean eyes-open alpha power (i.e. the AAC) was then calculated. A mixed two-factor 2 X 5 (group-by-session) ANa V A was performed on the AAC, and a mixed three-factor 2 X 2 X 5 (group-by-eyelid-position-by-aat-session) ANOV A was performed on mean alpha power. In both analyses, session was extracted as the within-subjects factor. Subjective sleepiness was assessed using the Stanford sleepiness scale (SSS) (15) and the visual analogue sleepiness scale (VASS) (16). The SSS consists of seven statements describing progressive changes in subjective sleepiness, ranging from" 1. Feeling active and vital; alert; wide awake" to "7. Almost in reverie; sleep onset soon; lost struggle to remain awake". Participants are asked to choose the statement that best represents their present state of sleepiness. The V ASS is a horizontal line approximately 100 mm in length, labeled "Very Alert" on the left and "Very Sleepy" on the right. Participants are asked to draw a vertical mark on the line at the point corresponding to their present state of sleepiness. V ASS scores are obtained by measuring the distance of the mark from the left end of the line (higher scores being associated with more intense feelings of sleepiness). The SSS and V ASS were administered every 1;2 hour during the daytime testing, including immediately prior to and following each MSLT and AAT session. To compare overall differences in subjective sleepiness between

4 ALPHA AITENUATION TEST IN NARCOLEPSY 261 TABLE 1. Raw data for AAT and MSLT Narcoleptics Session I Session 2 Session 3 Session 4 Session 5 Age EO EC SOL EO EC SOL EO EC SOL EO EC SOL EO EC SOL Normals Session 1 Session 2 Session 3 Session 4 Session 5 Age EO EC SOL EO EC SOL EO EC SOL EO EC SOL EO EC SOL a AAT, alpha attenuation test; MSLT, multiple sleep latency test; EO, mean eyes-open alpha power for AAT session in IL y2; EC, mean eyes-closed alpha power for AAT session in IL y2; SOL, latency to stage I for MSLT session in minutes. a R.K. case study (see Discussion). It normals and narcoleptics, mean scores on the SSS and VASS were calculated within each participant, and t tests were used to assess group differences. t tests were also used to assess group differences in SSS and V ASS ratings obtained prior to and following each MSLT and I... Normal-Narcolepticl 3.5, ======= AAT session. The relationship among the subjective sleepiness ratings and the MSLT and AAT was examined using Pearson correlation coefficients. RESULTS Raw data for each AAT and MSLT session are presented in Table 1. 'v r "N :J: 2 N ~ ~ 1.5 '" O+-----~----~ _----~----~r---~ TIME OF DAY (hr) FIG. 1. Mean alpha (8-12 Hz) attenuation coefficient (AAC) on the alpha attenuation test in narcoleptics and normals as a function of time of day. AAT The analysis of the AAC showed no significant group-by-session interaction, but significant main effects of group [F(l,I8) = 4.97, p = 0.039] and session [F(4,72) = 3.28, P = 0.016] were found (see Fig. 1). The ratio of eyes-closed to eyes-open alpha power (AAC) ranged from 0.6 to 3.0 for the narcoleptics, and from 0.9 to 5.9 for the normals. As predicted, collapsing across session, the mean AAC was significantly lower for the narcoieptics (mean = la, SD = 0.7) compared to the normals (mean = 2.5, SD = 104). Collapsing across group, the mean AAC was lowest at the 1500-hours session. The analysis of mean alpha power revealed a significant group by eyelid position interaction [F(l,I8) 6.52, P = 0.020] (see Fig. 2). Eyes-closed alpha

5 262 C. E. D. ALLOWAY ET AL. I... Normal-Narcolepticl 14r =======~~~ , 12 ", ~.. ~.. ~.. ~.. ~.. '1. -' ~. -':' ~. '~:~""';. ~... ",. ~.".~ ~.: o+-----~----~----_+----_+----~----~ TIME OF DAY (hr) FIG. 2. Mean eyes-open and mean eyes-closed alpha (8-12 Hz) power in f.l V2 on the alpha attenuation test in narcoleptics and normals as a function of time of day. power ranged from 1.1 fj. y2 to 4.9 fj. y2 for the narcoleptics and from 0.9 fj. y2 to 7.8 fj. y2 for the normals. Eyes-open alpha power ranged from 0.9 fj. y2 to 3.1 fj. y2 for the narcoleptics and from 0.7 fj. y2 to 3.3 fj. y2 for the normals. As predicted, during the eyes-closed condition, mean alpha power was significantly lower in the narcoleptics (mean = 2.4 fj. y2, SO = 1.0 fj. y2) than the normals (3.9 fj.y2, SO = 1.8 fj.y2) [t(18) = 2.5, P = 0.022]. However, contrary to predictions, during the eyes-open condition, mean alpha power did not differ between the groups (mean = 1.7 fj. y2, SO = 0.6 fj. y2 for the narcoleptics and mean = 1.7 fj. y2, SO = 0.8 fj.y2 for the normals) [t(18) = 0]. No significant group-by-session, eyelid position-by-session, or groupby-eyelid position-by-session interactions were found for mean alpha power. The main effect of group (i.e. collapsing across eyelid position and session) did not reach significance; however, there was a trend for mean alpha power to be lower in narcoleptics (2.0 fj.y2) compared to normals (2.8 fj.y2) [F(1,18) = 4.01, P = 0.060]. There was a significant main effect for eyelid position [F(1,18) = 19.76, p = 0.001] such that collapsing across group and session, mean eyes-closed alpha power (3.2 fj. y2) was greater than mean eyesopen alpha power (1.7 fj. y2). No session main effect was found for mean alpha power. MSLT The analysis of latency to stage 1 sleep on the MSLT showed no significant group-by-session interaction, but a significant main effect of group was found [F(1,18) = 15.11, P = 0.001] (see Fig. 3). La- C 10 i >- ~ 8 ~,... 6 w Cl ct Iii 4 2 ~ O+-----~----~----_ ~----~ TIME OF DAY (hr) FIG. 3. Mean latency in minutes to stage I sleep on the multiple sleep latency test in narcoleptics and normals as a function of time of day. tency to stage 1 ranged from 0.5 to 15.5 minutes for the narcoleptics and from 1.5 to 20.0 minutes for the normals. Mean latency to stage 1 was significantly shorter for narcoleptics (mean = 3.5 minutes, SO = 3.2 min) than normals (mean = 10.3 minutes, SO = 6.4 minutes). The session main effect did not reach significance [F(4,72) = 2.45, p = 0.054]. Narcoleptics experienced a total of 26 REM-containing naps (i.e. sleep onset REM periods) and 24 non-rem (NREM) sleep naps on the MSLT. Eleven of the narcoleptic REM naps contained stages 1, 2, and REM sleep and 15 contained just stage 1 and REM sleep. Twenty-three of the 24 narcoleptic NREM naps contained both stage 1 and stage 2 sleep. A total of six REM naps occurred at the 1200 and 1400 hours naps, five occurred at the 1000 and 1600 hours naps, and four occurred at the 1800 hours nap. Two narcoleptics had REM sleep on all five naps, five other narcoleptics had two or more REM containing naps, and three had only one nap containing REM sleep. Of these three narcoleptics, one had a sleep onset REM period during the nocturnal sleep. Normals experienced a total of 44 NREM sleep naps and six naps with no sleep onset. Thirty of the 44 normal NREM naps contained stage 1 and stage 2 sleep, whereas 14 contained only stage 1 sleep. One of the normal participants (R.K.) had two naps that contained REM sleep: one at 1400 hours and the other at 1600 hours. AAT and MSLT The relationship between the AAT and the MSLT was investigated by calculating for each participant the fl.,..

6 ALPHA ATTENUATION TEST IN NARCOLEPSY 263 mean AAC and mean latency to stage 1 on the MSLT and then correlating mean AAC and mean MSLT latency for narcoleptics and normals using Pearson correlation coefficients. Mean AAC correlated significantly with mean latency to stage 1 on the MSLT for narcoleptics (r = 0.75, P = 0.006) but not for normals (r = 0.15, P = 0.342). SSS and VASS Overall, mean SSS scores tended to be higher for narcoleptics (mean = 3.2, SO = 1.0) than normals (mean = 2.4, SO = 0.6) [t(18) = 2.09, p = 0.051]. Similarly, mean VASS scores tended to be higher for narcoleptics (mean = 40.7, SO = 19.7) than normals (mean = 25.6, SO = 12.0) [t(18) = 2.06, p = 0.054]. t tests were used to compare narcoleptic and normal subjective sleepiness ratings obtained just prior to and immediately following each AAT and MSLT session. For these analyses, the alpha level was set at 0.01 in order to allow for the fact that more than 20 t tests were computed. As such, p values between 0.01 and 0.05 are reported as indicating trends only. In regard to the pre- and post-aat mean subjective sleepiness ratings, narcoleptics rated themselves as sleepier than normals on the V ASS administered following the AAT session at 1500 hours (narcoleptics: mean = 51.2, SO = 29.9; normals: mean = 25.6, SO = 17.9) [t(18) = 2.32, p = 0.032]. However, no other differences were observed in subjective sleepiness ratings obtained preand post-aat. In regard to the pre- and post-mslt subjective sleepiness ratings, narcoleptics rated themselves as sleepier than normals on the pre-mslt (1000 hours) SSS (narcoleptics: mean = 3.7, SO = 1.8; normals: mean = 2.3, SO = 1.1) [t(18) = 2.15, P = 0.045], the pre-mslt (1200 hours) VASS (narcoleptics: mean = 38.8, SO = 25.9; normals: mean = 16.5, SO = 11.1) [t(18) = 2.50, P = 0.022], the pre-mslt (1400 hours) VASS (narcoleptics: mean = 38.7, SO = 24.2; normals: mean = 19.1, SO = 11.7) [t(18) = 2.31, P = 0.033], the pre-mslt (1600 hours) SSS (narcoleptics: mean = 3.4, SO = 1.3; normals: mean = 2.0, SO = 0.8) [t(18) = 2.94, P = 0.009], the pre MSLT (1600 hours) VASS (narcoleptics: mean = 44.8, SO = 28.4; normals: mean = 19.0, SO = 15.0) [t(18) = 2.54, p = 0.021], the post-mslt (1600 hours) SSS (narcoleptics: mean = 3.9, SO = 1.5; normals: mean = 2.6, SO = 0.8) [t(18) = 2.36, P = 0.030], the post-mslt (1600 hours) VASS (narcoleptics: mean = 52.4, SO = 25.2; normals: mean = 27.7, SO = 14.4) [t(18) = 2.69, P = 0.015], the pre-mslt (1800 hours) SSS (narcoleptics: mean = 3.0, SO = 1.6; normals: mean = 1.8, SO = 0.8) [t(18) = 2.17, P = 0.044], and the post-mslt (1800 hours) SSS (narcoleptics: mean = 3.4, SO = 0.8; normals: mean = 2.5, SO = 0.9) [t(18) = 2.38, P = 0.029]. To examine the relationship between the subjective sleepiness measures and the AAT, mean scores for the SSS and V ASS administered prior to and following each AAT session were first calculated for each participant, then Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated between mean AAC and pre- and post-aat mean SSS and V ASS for narcoleptics and normals (i.e. a total of four correlations were calculated for both narcoleptics and normals). None of these correlations reached significance for narcoleptics. For normals, there was a significant correlation between mean AAC and post-aat mean VASS (r = -0.61, p = 0.032). The relationship of subjective sleepiness measures with the MSLT was investigated in a similar manner. For each participant, mean latency to stage I on the MSLT and pre- and post-mslt mean SSS and V ASS were calculated, then Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated between mean latency and pre- and post-mslt mean SSS and VASS for narcoleptics and normals. None of these correlations reached significance for narcoleptics. For normals there was a significant correlation between mean latency to stage 1 on the MSLT and pre-mslt mean SSS (r = 0.74, p = 0.007). DISCUSSION The present study is the first to demonstrate that the AAT can be used to distinguish a clinical population of excessively sleepy individuals such as narcoleptics from normal controls. As predicted, the ratio of mean eyes-closed to mean eyes-open alpha power (i.e. the AAC) was significantly smaller for narcoleptics than normals, suggesting that increased physiological sleepiness is associated with lower AACs. These findings are consistent with those of Stampi et al. (7) who observed a decrease in AACs in normals throughout 40 hours of sleep deprivation. Studies of experimentally sleep-deprived normals and shiftworkers (9) have demonstrated that, during maximal sleepiness, alpha power is lower during eyes-closed conditions than during eyes-open conditions, and during maximal alertness, alpha power is higher during eyes-closed conditions than during eyes-open conditions. In the present study, it was predicted that narcoleptics would demonstrate lower mean eyes-closed alpha power and higher mean eyes-open alpha power than normals. However, it was found that mean eyes-closed alpha power was significantly reduced in narcoleptics compared to normals; whereas mean eyes-open alpha power did not differ between narcoleptics and normals. It appears that when the eyes were open, the illumination and the task of focusing on a target on the wall

7 264 C. E. D. ALLOWAY ET AL. may have acted as alerting stimuli for the narcoleptics, enabling the suppression of the latent physiological sleepiness that was observed once they closed their eyes. Thus, the significantly reduced AAC in narcoleptics was mediated almost entirely by the eyesclosed condition of the AAT. The decreased AAC and increased eyes-closed alpha power in narcolepsy mimics the effects of shiftwork and experimentally induced sleep deprivation in normals. However, it must be stressed that in the present study no evidence was found to suggest that the narcoleptics were sleep deprived. Mean nocturnal sleep length did not differ significantly between normals and narcoleptics for either the sleep diary that was completed for 7 days prior testing or the nocturnal polysomnography carried out immediately preceding the daytime testing. Furthermore, the nocturnal polysomnography showed no significant differences between narcoleptics and normals in sleep efficiency or in the percentage of time spent in stages 2, 3, 4, and REM sleep. The only significant differences found between narcoleptics and normal sleepers during the nocturnal polysomnography were that latencies to stage 1 and REM sleep were shorter, and the percentage of time spent in stage 1 was higher for narcoleptics. Thus, there was no evidence to suggest that the reduced AACs observed during seated daytime wakefulness in narcoleptics could be accounted for by the effects of nocturnal-sleep deprivation. In the absence of sleep deprivation in the narcoleptics, some neurophysiological mechanism is producing an elevation in the physiological need for sleep in narcoleptics. The AAC correlated significantly with latency to stage 1 on the MSLT in narcoleptics but not in normals. This result is contrary to the findings of Stampi et al. (7) who demonstrated that the AAC correlated with latency to sleep onset on the MSLT in eight out of 10 sleep-deprived normal participants. However, Stampi et al. (7) measured latency to sleep onset within minutes of measuring eyes-open and eyes-closed alpha power; whereas in the present study, more than 45 minutes separated the two measurements, and neither the normals nor the narcoleptics were experimentally deprived of sleep. We propose that the AAT and MSLT produced relatively independent measures of sleepiness in our nonsleep-deprived normal participants because they were scheduled to begin on alternate hours throughout the day. These tests were not scheduled to run consecutively to avoid both sleepiness priming and sleep inertia effects. It was desirable that participants not become sleepy and fall asleep faster on the MSLT because they had just finished sitting quietly while staring at the wall or closing their eyes and vice versa. Subjective sleepiness ratings obtained just prior to or following four of the AAT sessions did not differ significantly between narcoleptics and normals, but following the AAT session at 1500 hours, narcoleptics tended to rate themselves as sleepier than normals on the V ASS (i.e. p = 0.032). In contrast, although sleepiness ratings obtained just prior to or following the MSLT sessions were more frequently higher for narcoleptics than normals, all but one of these differences failed to reach the 0.01 level of significance. The tendency for subjective sleepiness ratings centered around the MSLT, but not the AAT, to differentiate narcoleptics and normals may have been due in part to the environment in which the subjective sleepiness ratings were obtained. For the AAT, sleepiness ratings were obtained while participants were seated in a chair. By contrast, for the MSLT, sleepiness ratings were obtained while participants were lying down in bed, in anticipation of or just following a nap opportunity. Thus, in situations that promote sleepiness (i.e. lying down for the MSLT vs. sitting in a chair for the AAT), subjective sleepiness ratings tended to be higher for narcoleptics than normals. What is noteworthy is that the AAT successfully differentiated the narcoleptics and normals in the absence of group differences in subjective sleepiness ratings. One of the normal participants in the present study (RK.) presents an interesting case study demonstrating the merit of the AAT in the situation of false-positive MSLT results. RK., age 35 years, is a normal sleeper who reported sleeping an average of 6.9 hours each night during the week before testing and obtained 8.7 hours of sleep the night before testing. RK. reported no need to nap during the day and no history of excessive daytime sleepiness, cataplexy, or sleep paralysis, although he did report having unusual visual or auditory experiences (i.e. hypnagogic hallucinations) one to five times during his lifetime. He was not taking any medications, nor did he demonstrate symptoms of depression (his score was 2 out of 63 on the BDI). RK. experienced two sleep onset REM periods on the MSLT and his mean latency to stage 1 sleep was 3.6 minutes, suggesting that he met the diagnostic criteria for narcolepsy. [The occurrence of sleep onset REM periods in otherwise normal sleepers has been documented by Rosenthal et al. (17) who reported that 15% (i.e. 11 of 73) of their drug-free normal sleepers (asymptomatic for narcolepsy) experienced two or more sleep onset REM periods on the MSLT.] However, RK. subjectively rated himself as alert throughout the day (mean SSS = 1.4, mean VASS = 10.2). Moreover, his AAT results gave no indication of excessive sleepiness. On the contrary, they suggested an above-average degree of physiological alertness. RK. 's mean AAC (3.1) was higher than the average AAC for normals (2.5) as was his mean eyes-closed alpha power (5.9 j.1v2 vs. 3.9 j.1v2). His mean eyes- ~)

8 ALPHA ATTENUATION TEST IN NARCOLEPSY 265,"" open alpha power (1.9 IL V2) was comparable with that obtained in narcoleptics and normals (1.7 IL V2). Thus, the present study has documented the ability of the AAT to confirm the presence of physiological alertness in a normal participant matching the MSLT diagnostic criteria for narcolepsy. The AAT may be instrumental in the clinical assessment of excessive daytime sleepiness. It is quick and simple to administer and is free of the serious limitations associated with the assessment of sleepiness via the MSLT-namely, a floor effect in excessively sleepy populations (5), the confounding of sleepiness with the ability to fall asleep (i.e. "sleepability") (4), and the reliance on the presence of a polysomnographer to sleep score the EEG record "on line" during each nap opportunity (7). Furthermore, the AAT is an ideal measure for assessing sleepiness in field studies or in actual work environments because it is nonintrusive (i.e. it does not necessitate a sleepconducive environment, thereby inducing less sleepiness than the MSLT) (7). Although the MSLT is perhaps the best measure for documenting the occurrence of sleep onset REM periods (18), it clearly has the potential to produce misleading evaluations of physiological sleepiness (due to the sleepability confound and floor effects). The inclusion of the AAT in the clinical assessment of patients with sleep-related complaints appears warranted. The AAT may easily be accommodated into the MSLT paradigm by scheduling these two tests to occur on alternate hours throughout the day as was done in the present study. We recommend that future research investigate the efficacy of the AAT in the evaluation of pharmacological-treatment efficacy for excessive daytime sleepiness. Given that studies of subjectively effective stimulant medications in narcoleptics have failed to demonstrate a reduction in sleepiness on the MSLT (6), it would be advantageous for future studies to investigate the ability of the AAT to detect variations in sleepiness/alertness following clinically effective pharmacological treatment. Future research is also needed to test the ability of the AAT to assess sleepiness in clinical populations other than narcoleptics. For example, it is of interest whether the AAT would identify increased physiological sleepiness in sleep onset insomniacs (who would have trouble falling asleep on the MSLT) and in other patients complaining of excessive daytime sleepiness (e.g. patients with central and obstructive sleep apnea). Some researchers have suggested that the AAT may be limited in its ability to assess sleepiness in highalpha producers. Stampi et al. (19) reported in their study of sleep-deprived normals that in individuals who produced extremely high AACs (labeled "high alpha producers"), the AAC tended not to correlate with latency-to-sleep onset on the MSLT. In the study, the MSLT was administered 5 minutes following the administration of the AAT. Stampi et al. (19) reported that although the high alpha producers demonstrated a high level of alertness on the AAT (i.e. high AAC), this degree of alertness was not related to latency to sleep onset on the MSLT. That is, presumably, these high alpha producers were still able to fall asleep within the 20-minute nap opportunity provided by the MSLT. We propose that these findings may reflect more on the sleepability confound of the MSLT rather than on a specific limitation of the AAT. Heitmann et al. (20), in their study of shiftworkers working a nightshift, reported that the AAC correlated best with subjective sleepiness measures in individuals producing "medium"-level AACs. This suggests that the AAT may be less sensitive to subjective sleepiness in individuals who demonstrate extreme levels of alertness or sleepiness on their AACs. However, in the present study, subjective sleepiness ratings administered prior to and following each AAT correlated with the AAC in just four participants (two narcoleptics and two normals), and no apparent pattern was observed for persons having higher or lower level AACs. We suggest that the AAT is a measure of the underlying or latent physiological sleepiness, whereas subjective sleepiness ratings tend to reflect manifest sleepiness, which is influenced by moment-to-moment fluctuations in alerting stimuli within the environment (2); thus the lack of correlation among these measures in some individuals may not present a serious consequence. To conclude, the present study is the first to demonstrate that normal sleepers can be distinguished from excessively sleepy narcoleptics on the basis of EEG power spectral analysis of alpha power during seated wakefulness. REFERENCES 1. Carskadon MA, Dement We. Sleepiness and sleep state on a 90-minute schedule. Psychophysiology 1977;14: Carskadon MA, Dement WC. The multiple sleep latency test: what does it measure? Sleep 1982;5:S Diagnostic Classification Steering Committee, Thorpy MJ, Chairman. International classification of sleep disorders: diagnostic and coding manual. Rochester, MN: American Sleep Disorders Association, Broughton RJ. Qualitatively different states of sleepiness. In: Broughton RJ, Ogilvie RD, eds. Sleep, arousal and peiformance. Boston: Birkhauser, 1992: Sugarman JL, Walsh JK. Physiological sleep tendency and ability to maintain alertness at night. Sleep 1989;12: Hartse KM. Roth T, Zorick FJ. Daytime sleepiness and daytime wakefulness: the effect of instruction. Sleep 1982;5:SI Stampi C, Stone P, Michimori A. A new quantitative method for assessing sleepiness: the alpha attenuation test. Work & Stress 1995;9: Akerstedt T, Torsvall L, Gillberg M. Sleepiness in shiftwork. A review with emphasis on continuous monitoring of EEG and EOG. Chronobiollnt 1987;4:

9 266 C. E. D. ALLOWAY ET AL. 9. Akerstedt T, Torsvall L, Gillberg M. Sleepiness in laboratory and field experiments. In: Koella Wp, Ruther E, Schulz H, eds. Sleep Stuttgart: Gustav Fischer Verlag, 1985: Stampi C, Stone P, Michimori A. The alpha attenuation test: a new quantitative method for assessing sleepiness and its relationship to the MSLT. Sleep Res 1993;22: Alloway CED, Ogilvie RD, Shapiro CM. The alpha attenuation test: assessing physiological sleepiness in narcolepsy. Sleep Res 1995;24: Beck AT. Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press, Thorpy MJ. Report from the American Sleep Disorders Association: the clinical use of the multiple sleep latency test. Sleep 1992;15: Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques, and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. Los Angeles: Brain Research Institute, Hoddes E, Dement W, Zarcone V. The development and use of the Stanford sleepiness scale (SSS). Psychophysiology 1971;9: Folstein MF, Luria R. Reliability, validity and clinical application of the visual analogue mood scale. Psychol Med 1973;3: Rosenthal L, Bishop C, Helmus T, Roehrs TA, Brouillard L, Roth T. The frequency of multiple sleep onset REM periods among subjects with no EDS. Sleep Res 1995;24: Mitler MM, Van den Hoed J, Carskadon MA, Richardson G, Park R, Guilleminault C, Dement We. REM sleep episodes during the multiple sleep latency test in narcoleptic patients. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1979;46: Stampi C, Michimori A, Aguirre A. Comparison between three versions of the alpha attenuation test for the objective assessment of sleepiness. Sleep Res 1995;24: Heitmann A, Stampi C, Anandan S. The alpha attenuation test: its first application for objective assessment of sleepiness in field studies. Sleep Res 1995;24:100. -I)

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep Sleep, 6(4):319-325 1983 Raven Press, New York Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep T. Roehrs, F. Zorick, J. Sicklesteel, R. Wittig, and T. Roth Sleep Disorders and Research

More information

Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects

Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects Sleep I ():54-60, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 988 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects Edward Stepanski,

More information

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants Sleep, 5:S128-S134 1982 Raven Press, New York Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants T. Roth, T. Roehrs, and F. Zorick Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, Michigan

More information

Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence

Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence Sleep, 5:S165S174 1982 Raven Press, New York Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence F. Zorick, T. Roehrs, G. Koshorek, J. Sicklesteel, *K. Hartse, R. Wittig, and T.

More information

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep Sleep. 19(7):570-575 1996 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep Mats Gillberg, Garan Kecklund, John Axelsson and

More information

Multiple Naps and the Evaluation of Daytime Sleepiness in Patients with Upper Airway Sleep Apnea

Multiple Naps and the Evaluation of Daytime Sleepiness in Patients with Upper Airway Sleep Apnea Sleep. 3(3/4):425-439 1980 Raven Press. New York, Multiple Naps and the Evaluation of Daytime Sleepiness in Patients with Upper Airway Sleep Apnea T. Roth, K. M. Hartse, F. Zorick, and W. Conway Sleep

More information

Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals

Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals Sleep 2(5):449-457, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 989 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals Timothy Roehrs, Victoria Timms, Ardith Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos,

More information

Daytime Sleepiness and Antihistamines

Daytime Sleepiness and Antihistamines Sleep, 7(2): 137-141 1984 Raven Press, New York Daytime Sleepiness and Antihistamines imothy A. Roehrs, Elizabeth I. ietz, Frank J. Zorick, and homas Roth Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford

More information

Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence: Polygraphic and Clinical Data for 100 Patients

Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence: Polygraphic and Clinical Data for 100 Patients Sleep, 4(1):23-37 1981 Raven Press, New York Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence: Polygraphic and Clinical Data for 100 Patients Johanna van den Hoed, Helena Kraemer, Christian Guilleminault, Vincent

More information

Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical

Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical Director, Faulkner EEG and Sleep Testing Center Course

More information

Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value

Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value Sleep 10(6):590-599, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1987 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value Brian Levine, Timothy Roehrs, Edward Stepanski, Frank

More information

EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE

EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE Shih-Bin Yeh 1 and Carlos Hugh Schenck 2,3 1 Department of Neurology

More information

Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): A Standard Measure of Sleepiness *

Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): A Standard Measure of Sleepiness * Sleep 9(4):519-524, Raven Press, New York 1986, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): A Standard Measure of Sleepiness * Chairman: Mary A. Carskadon

More information

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle Sleep 10(1):62-68, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components

More information

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults Sleep 13(5):385-394, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1990 Association of Professional Sleep Societies The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults M.

More information

sleep latencies Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients the tendency to fall asleep

sleep latencies Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients the tendency to fall asleep Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients Sleepiness the tendency to fall asleep Elke De Valck, Raymond Cluydts, Sandra Pirrera Department of Cognitive and Physiological Psychology, Vrije Universiteit

More information

Symptoms of Narcolepsy

Symptoms of Narcolepsy Symptoms of Narcolepsy v Sleep attacks Brief episodes of sleep that occur many times a day May occur without warning or be preceded by drowsiness Patient usually feels refreshed afterwards Refractory period

More information

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association Sleep in patients with a number of sleep disorders and

More information

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: What Does It Measure?

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: What Does It Measure? Sleep,5:S67-S72 1982 Raven Press, New York The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: What Does It Measure? Mary A. Carskadon and William C. Dement Stanford University Sleep Research Center, Stanford, California

More information

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan Sleep 10(4):393-397, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Short Report Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan C. Guilleminault, S. Mondini,

More information

I MSLT=multiple sleep latency test; MWI'=mainteDance of I

I MSLT=multiple sleep latency test; MWI'=mainteDance of I Maintenance of Wakefulness Test and Multiple Sleep Latency Test* Measurement of Different Abilities in Patients With Sleep Disorders Rahul B. Sangal, M.D.;t Larry Thomas;t and Merrill M. Mitler, Ph.D:;f:

More information

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm Sleep, 2(3):299-307 1980 Raven Press, New York The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm L. C. Johnson Naval Health Research Center, San Diego, California Summary: Two studies, using data from fragmented

More information

Automobile Accidents in Patients with Sleep Disorders

Automobile Accidents in Patients with Sleep Disorders Sleep 12(6):487-494, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1989 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Automobile Accidents in Patients with Sleep Disorders Michael S. Aldrich Department of Neurology, University

More information

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs?

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs? Sleep, 6(1): 10-\5 1983 Raven Press, New York What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs? Peter Rauri and Elaine Olmstead Dartmouth Medical School, Hanover, New Hampshire, U,S,A, Summary: Subjective

More information

Sleep latency testing as a time course measure of state arousal

Sleep latency testing as a time course measure of state arousal J. Sleep Res. (2005) 14, 387 392 Sleep latency testing as a time course measure of state arousal MICHAEL H. BONNET 1,2,3 and DONNA L. ARAND 2,3 1 Dayton Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, 2

More information

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include:

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include: INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS Introduction Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include: Intrinsic sleep disorders (e.g. narcolepsy, obstructive sleep apnoea/hypopnea

More information

A New Method for Measuring Daytime Sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale

A New Method for Measuring Daytime Sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale Sleep, 14(6):540--545 1991 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society A New Method for Measuring Daytime Sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale Murray W. Johns Sleep Disorders Unit,

More information

Daytime Functioning and Nighttime Sleep Before, During, and After a 146-Hour Tennis Match

Daytime Functioning and Nighttime Sleep Before, During, and After a 146-Hour Tennis Match Sleep 13(6):526-532, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1990 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Daytime Functioning and Nighttime Sleep Before, During, and After a 146-Hour Tennis Match *tjack D. Edinger,

More information

Simplest method: Questionnaires. Retrospective: past week, month, year, lifetime Daily: Sleep diary What kinds of questions would you ask?

Simplest method: Questionnaires. Retrospective: past week, month, year, lifetime Daily: Sleep diary What kinds of questions would you ask? Spencer Dawson Simplest method: Questionnaires Retrospective: past week, month, year, lifetime Daily: Sleep diary What kinds of questions would you ask? Did you nap during the day? Bed time and rise time

More information

ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ

ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ USADA can grant a Therapeutic Use Exemption (TUE) in compliance with the World Anti- Doping Agency International Standard for TUEs. The TUE application process

More information

Periodic Leg Movements in Narcolepsy

Periodic Leg Movements in Narcolepsy In: Nacrolepsy: Symptoms, Causes... ISBN: 978-1-60876-645-1 Editor: Guillermo Santos, et al. 2009 Nova Science Publishers, Inc. Chapter 7 Periodic Leg Movements in Narcolepsy Ahmed Bahammam * Sleep Disorders

More information

Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness

Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness Sleep 20(3):215-223 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness Goran Kecklund Torbjom Akerstedt and Arne Lowden National

More information

Daytime Carryover of Triazolam and Flurazepam in Elderly Insomniacs

Daytime Carryover of Triazolam and Flurazepam in Elderly Insomniacs Sleep, 5(4)361-371 1982 Raven Press, New York Daytime Carryover of Triazolam and Flurazepam in Elderly Insomniacs Mary A. Carskadon, Wesley F. Seidel, *David J. Greenblatt, and William C. Dement Sleep

More information

Behavioral Treatment and Sleep. The Effects of Regularizing Sleep-Wake Schedules on Daytime Sleepiness

Behavioral Treatment and Sleep. The Effects of Regularizing Sleep-Wake Schedules on Daytime Sleepiness Sleep, 19(5):432-441 1996 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Behavioral Treatment and Sleep The Effects of Regularizing Sleep-Wake Schedules on Daytime Sleepiness *Rachel Manber,

More information

Assessment of Sleep Disorders DR HUGH SELSICK

Assessment of Sleep Disorders DR HUGH SELSICK Assessment of Sleep Disorders DR HUGH SELSICK Goals Understand the importance of history taking Be able to take a basic sleep history Be aware the technology used to assess sleep disorders. Understand

More information

T. Roth, Ph.D., T. Roehrs, Ph.D., G. Koshorek, J. Sicldost^, B.A., and

T. Roth, Ph.D., T. Roehrs, Ph.D., G. Koshorek, J. Sicldost^, B.A., and T. Roth, Ph.D., T. Roehrs, Ph.D., G. Koshorek, J. Sicldost^, B.A., and F. Zorick, M.D. Detroit, Mich. The central effects of a newly developed, long-acting H, antihistamine, loratadine (10 and ), were

More information

Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness

Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness Sleep, 18(10):890-894 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness

More information

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms People typically sleep about 8 hours per day, and spend 16 hours awake. Most people sleep over 175,000 hours in their lifetime. The vast amount of time spent sleeping

More information

An 18-Year-Old Woman with Prolonged Eyes Closed Unresponsiveness during Multiple Sleep Latency Testing

An 18-Year-Old Woman with Prolonged Eyes Closed Unresponsiveness during Multiple Sleep Latency Testing pii: jc-00317-16 http://dx.doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.6410 SLEEP MEDICINE PEARLS An 18-Year-Old Woman with Prolonged Eyes Closed Unresponsiveness during Multiple Sleep Latency Testing Romy Hoque, MD 1 ; Victoria

More information

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days Sleep Research Online 5(1): 13-17, 2003 http://www.sro.org/2003/hayashi/13/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 96-214X 2003 WebSciences The Effects of Short Daytime s for Five Consecutive Mitsuo

More information

Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis

Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis J. Sleep Res. (1999) 8, 237 245 REVIEW Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis NANCY JO WESENSTEN, THOMAS J. BALKIN and GREGORY BELENKY Department of Neurobiology and Behavior,

More information

FEP Medical Policy Manual

FEP Medical Policy Manual FEP Medical Policy Manual Effective Date: October 15, 2018 Related Policies: 2.01.18 Diagnosis and Medical Management of Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome Polysomnography for Non-Respiratory Sleep Disorders

More information

Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation

Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation Sleep Research Online 2(4): 101-105, 1999 http://www.sro.org/1999/hodoba/101/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 1999 WebSciences Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation

More information

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle Sleep Sleep is described as a state of unconsciousness or partial consciousness from which a person can be roused by stimulation Period of rest and recovery People spend about a third of their lives sleeping

More information

A Modified Method for Scoring Slow Wave Sleep of Older Subjects

A Modified Method for Scoring Slow Wave Sleep of Older Subjects Sleep, 5(2):195-199 1982 Raven Press, New York A Modified Method for Scoring Slow Wave Sleep of Older Subjects Wilse B. Webb and Lewis M. Dreblow Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville,

More information

H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic Activation During Lucid REM Sleep: A Case Study

H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic Activation During Lucid REM Sleep: A Case Study Sleep 12(4):374-378, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1989 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Short Communication H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic Activation During Lucid REM Sleep: A Case Study

More information

FEP Medical Policy Manual

FEP Medical Policy Manual FEP Medical Policy Manual Effective Date: January 15, 2018 Related Policies: 2.01.18 Diagnosis and Medical Management of Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome Diagnosis and Medical Management of Obstructive

More information

LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART

LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART POLYSOMNOGRAM, SLEEP STAGE SCORING, INTERPRETATION Sleep Computing Committee, Japanese Society of Sleep Research LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART POLYSOMNOGRAM, SLEEP STAGE SCORING,

More information

Treating Insomnia in Primary Care. Judith R. Davidson Ph.D., C. Psych. Kingston Family Health Team

Treating Insomnia in Primary Care. Judith R. Davidson Ph.D., C. Psych. Kingston Family Health Team Treating Insomnia in Primary Care Judith R. Davidson Ph.D., C. Psych. Kingston Family Health Team jdavidson@kfhn.net Disclosure statement Nothing to disclose A ruffled mind makes a restless pillow. ~ Charlotte

More information

Diurnal variations in the waking EEG: comparisons with sleep latencies and subjective alertness

Diurnal variations in the waking EEG: comparisons with sleep latencies and subjective alertness J. Sleep Res. (2000) 9, 243±248 Diurnal variations in the waking EEG: comparisons with sleep latencies and subjective alertness C. LAFRANCE and M. DUMONT Laboratoire de chronobiologie, Hoà pital du SacreÂ

More information

Polysomnography Course Session: Sept 2017

Polysomnography Course Session: Sept 2017 Polysomnography Course Session: Sept 2017 General Information Polysomnography course will be held at SLEEP AND ALERTNESS CLINIC Med-West Medical centre 750 Dundas St. W., Suite 2-259 (Conference Room)

More information

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children QUESTION #1: ARE SLEEP ISSUES IN CHILDREN THE SAME AS IN ADULTS? Distinctive Features

More information

Sleep Across the Life Cycle

Sleep Across the Life Cycle SECTION II Anatomy and Physiology CHAPTER 3 Sleep Across the Life Cycle IOURI KREININ L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S On completion of this chapter, the reader should be able to 1. Describe the elements

More information

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children SLEEP DISORDERS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children Distinctive Features of Pediatric Sleep Daytime sleepiness uncommon

More information

Sleep Questionnaire. If yes, what? If yes, how would you describe it? Please explain? If yes, what times are these?

Sleep Questionnaire. If yes, what? If yes, how would you describe it? Please explain? If yes, what times are these? THE ADRENAL THYROID REVOLUTION Professional Mastermind Aviva Romm MD Sleep Questionnaire Sleep is important for musculoskeletal healing and for healthy immune function, mood, cognitive and brain function,

More information

Awake. Rapid Eye Movement (REM) or dreaming sleep. Normally, we go through Stages 2 to 5 a few times every night, before waking up in the morning.

Awake. Rapid Eye Movement (REM) or dreaming sleep. Normally, we go through Stages 2 to 5 a few times every night, before waking up in the morning. Narcolepsy Need-to-Know Guide Childhood Narcolepsy This guide provides information for parents or carers of children and young people with narcolepsy. It aims to promote a clear understanding of the condition

More information

Stephanie Mazza, Jean-Louis Pepin, Chrystele Deschaux, Bernadette Naegele, and Patrick Levy

Stephanie Mazza, Jean-Louis Pepin, Chrystele Deschaux, Bernadette Naegele, and Patrick Levy Analysis of Error Profiles Occurring during the OSLER Test A Sensitive Mean of Detecting Fluctuations in Vigilance in Patients with Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome Stephanie Mazza, Jean-Louis Pepin, Chrystele

More information

The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events

The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events Summary of Updates in Version 2.1 July 1, 2014 The American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) is committed to ensuring that The AASM Manual

More information

UNDERSTANDING NARCOLEPSY

UNDERSTANDING NARCOLEPSY PATIENT GUIDE This brochure is designed to help you understand the symptoms of narcolepsy, so that you can discuss any symptoms you may be having with your doctor. UNDERSTANDING NARCOLEPSY Patient Counseling

More information

Sleep habits and their consequences: a survey. Umar A. Khan, Sara N. Pasha, Sarah K. Khokhar, Asim A. Rizvi

Sleep habits and their consequences: a survey. Umar A. Khan, Sara N. Pasha, Sarah K. Khokhar, Asim A. Rizvi 1 Original Article Sleep habits and their consequences: a survey Umar A. Khan, Sara N. Pasha, Sarah K. Khokhar, Asim A. Rizvi Department of Medicine, Shifa International Hospital and Shifa College of Medicine,

More information

Corporate Medical Policy

Corporate Medical Policy Corporate Medical Policy Polysomnography for Non Respiratory Sleep Disorders File Name: Origination: Last CAP Review: Next CAP Review: Last Review: polysomnography_for_non_respiratory_sleep_disorders 10/2015

More information

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night?

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night? Sleep Research Online 2(1): 15-19, 1999 http://www.sro.org/1999/ferrara/15/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 1999 WebSciences Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound:

More information

Daytime Sleepiness in Patients With Congestive Heart Failure and Cheyne-Stokes Respiration*

Daytime Sleepiness in Patients With Congestive Heart Failure and Cheyne-Stokes Respiration* Daytime Sleepiness in Patients With Congestive Heart Failure and Cheyne-Stokes Respiration* Patrick Hanly, MBBCh, FCCP; and Naheed Zuberi-Khokhar, MD, BSc Study objective: To determine whether patients

More information

Basics of Polysomnography. Chitra Lal, MD, FCCP, FAASM Assistant professor of Medicine, Pulmonary, Critical Care and Sleep, MUSC, Charleston, SC

Basics of Polysomnography. Chitra Lal, MD, FCCP, FAASM Assistant professor of Medicine, Pulmonary, Critical Care and Sleep, MUSC, Charleston, SC Basics of Polysomnography Chitra Lal, MD, FCCP, FAASM Assistant professor of Medicine, Pulmonary, Critical Care and Sleep, MUSC, Charleston, SC Basics of Polysomnography Continuous and simultaneous recording

More information

Statistical Features of Hypnagogic EEG Measured by a New Scoring System

Statistical Features of Hypnagogic EEG Measured by a New Scoring System Sleep, 19(9):731-738 1996 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Statistical Features of Hypnagogic EEG Measured by a New Scoring System Hideki Tanaka, Mitsuo Hayashi and Tadao

More information

EFFECTS OF BENZODIAZEPINES ON SLEEP AND WAKEFULNESS

EFFECTS OF BENZODIAZEPINES ON SLEEP AND WAKEFULNESS Br. J. clin. Pharmac. (1981), 11, 31S-3S EFFECTS OF BENZODIAZEPINES ON SLEEP AND WAKEFULNESS Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, Michigan The differential effects of short

More information

Relationship of Autonomic Nervous System Activity to Daytime Sleepiness and Prior Sleep

Relationship of Autonomic Nervous System Activity to Daytime Sleepiness and Prior Sleep Sleep 12(3):239-245, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1989 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Relationship of Autonomic Nervous System Activity to Daytime Sleepiness and Prior Sleep Mark R. Pressman

More information

Simulator Performance vs. Neurophysiologic Monitoring: Which is More Relevant to Assess Driving Impairment?

Simulator Performance vs. Neurophysiologic Monitoring: Which is More Relevant to Assess Driving Impairment? University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2005 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 28th, 12:00 AM Simulator Performance vs. Neurophysiologic Monitoring: Which is More Relevant

More information

Polysomnography (PSG) (Sleep Studies), Sleep Center

Polysomnography (PSG) (Sleep Studies), Sleep Center Policy Number: 1036 Policy History Approve Date: 07/09/2015 Effective Date: 07/09/2015 Preauthorization All Plans Benefit plans vary in coverage and some plans may not provide coverage for certain service(s)

More information

The value of sleep deprivation as a diagnostic tool in adult sleepwalkers

The value of sleep deprivation as a diagnostic tool in adult sleepwalkers CME The value of sleep deprivation as a diagnostic tool in adult sleepwalkers Steve Joncas, BSc; Antonio Zadra, PhD; Jean Paquet, PhD; and Jacques Montplaisir, MD, PhD, CRCPc Abstract Objective: Adult

More information

Nocturnal sleep, daytime sleepiness, and napping among women with significant emotional behavioral premenstrual symptoms

Nocturnal sleep, daytime sleepiness, and napping among women with significant emotional behavioral premenstrual symptoms J. Sleep Res. (2007) 16, 262 268 Nocturnal sleep, daytime sleepiness, and napping among women with significant emotional behavioral premenstrual symptoms LYNNE J. LAMARCHE 1, HELEN S. DRIVER 2, SABRINA

More information

Treatment of Chronic Insomnia by Restriction of Time in Bed

Treatment of Chronic Insomnia by Restriction of Time in Bed Sleep 10(1):45-56, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Treatment of Chronic Insomnia by Restriction of Time in Bed Arthur 1. Spielman, Paul Saskin, and Michael J. Thorpy

More information

The Effects of Caffeine on Simulated Night-Shift Work and Subsequent Daytime Sleep

The Effects of Caffeine on Simulated Night-Shift Work and Subsequent Daytime Sleep Sleep. 18(1):22-29 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society The Effects of Caffeine on Simulated Night-Shift Work and Subsequent Daytime Sleep Mark J. Muehlbach and James K.

More information

Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation

Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation Sleep. 18( I): 1--6 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation Validation of the Sand C Components of the Three-Process Model of Alertness Regulation

More information

Pharmacology and Sleep

Pharmacology and Sleep Sleep. 16(4):31-35 1993 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Pharmacology and Sleep Sedative Effects and Plasma Concentrations Following Single Doses of Triazolam, Diphenhydramine,

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects ORIGINAL ARTICLES Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects Maria Josè Esposito, Ms.Sc.,Vincenzo Natale, M.D., Ph.D., Miranda Occhionero, M.D., Ph.D., and PierCarla Cicogna, Ph.D.

More information

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology INSOMNIAS INSOMNIAS General criteria for insomnia A. Repeated difficulty with sleep initiation, duration, consolidation or quality. B. Adequate sleep opportunity, persistent sleep difficulty and associated

More information

NATIONAL COMPETENCY SKILL STANDARDS FOR PERFORMING POLYSOMNOGRAPHY/SLEEP TECHNOLOGY

NATIONAL COMPETENCY SKILL STANDARDS FOR PERFORMING POLYSOMNOGRAPHY/SLEEP TECHNOLOGY NATIONAL COMPETENCY SKILL STANDARDS FOR PERFORMING POLYSOMNOGRAPHY/SLEEP TECHNOLOGY Polysomnography/Sleep Technology providers practice in accordance with the facility policy and procedure manual which

More information

Do You Get Enough Sleep?

Do You Get Enough Sleep? LP 3A sleep deprivation 1 Do You Get Enough Sleep? Many college students do not get enough sleep. In a survey of more than 200,000 first year students, more than 80% say that stayed up all night at least

More information

YOU REALLY NEED TO SLEEP: Several methods to improve your sleep

YOU REALLY NEED TO SLEEP: Several methods to improve your sleep YOU REALLY NEED TO SLEEP: Several methods to improve your sleep Sleep is essential to our well-being. When humans fail to get good sleep over a period of time, numerous problems can occur. CAN T SLEEP!!

More information

Sleep Onset. Nighttime Drop in Body Temperature: A Physiological Trigger for Sleep Onset?

Sleep Onset. Nighttime Drop in Body Temperature: A Physiological Trigger for Sleep Onset? Sleep, 20(7):505-511 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep Onset Nighttime Drop in Body Temperature: A Physiological Trigger for Sleep Onset? Patricia J. Murphy and

More information

Blood Pressure and Heart Rate During Continuous Experimental Sleep Fragmentation in Healthy Adults

Blood Pressure and Heart Rate During Continuous Experimental Sleep Fragmentation in Healthy Adults Blood Pressure and heart rate during sleep fragmentation Blood Pressure and Heart Rate During Continuous Experimental Sleep Fragmentation in Healthy Adults Melinda J. Carrington, PhD 1,2 ; John Trinder,

More information

Sleep and Ageing. Siobhan Banks PhD. Body and Brain at Work, Centre for Sleep Research University of South Australia

Sleep and Ageing. Siobhan Banks PhD. Body and Brain at Work, Centre for Sleep Research University of South Australia Sleep and Ageing Siobhan Banks PhD Body and Brain at Work, Centre for Sleep Research University of South Australia Health and Active Ageing, 22 nd September 2015 Sleep and Aging How does sleep change as

More information

Sleep and Sleep Hygiene in an Occupational Health & Safety Context

Sleep and Sleep Hygiene in an Occupational Health & Safety Context Sleep and Sleep Hygiene in an Occupational Health & Safety Context Glenn Legault Ph.D. Center for Research in Occupational Safety and Health, Laurentian University Nov. 12, 2014 Overview: Sleep what is

More information

Table of Contents RECOGNIZE SCREEN REFER. This brochure can help you: Narcolepsy Overview Narcolepsy Symptoms... 5

Table of Contents RECOGNIZE SCREEN REFER. This brochure can help you: Narcolepsy Overview Narcolepsy Symptoms... 5 CLINICIAN GUIDE 1 This brochure can help you: RECOGNIZE possible manifestations of excessive daytime sleepiness, the cardinal symptom of narcolepsy 1-3 SCREEN all patients with manifestations of excessive

More information

MWT PROTOCOL PURPOSE POLICY

MWT PROTOCOL PURPOSE POLICY PURPOSE A standard MWT protocol that is consistent with AASM practice parameters promotes consistency, allows comparisons between tests, and ensures accurate interpretations that are consistent with published

More information

Index SLEEP MEDICINE CLINICS. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Cerebrospinal fluid analysis, for Kleine-Levin syndrome,

Index SLEEP MEDICINE CLINICS. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Cerebrospinal fluid analysis, for Kleine-Levin syndrome, 165 SLEEP MEDICINE CLINICS Index Sleep Med Clin 1 (2006) 165 170 Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. A Academic performance, effects of sleepiness in children on, 112 Accidents,

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. SOREMs in Sleep Clinic Patients: Association with Sleepiness, Alertness and Fatigue

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. SOREMs in Sleep Clinic Patients: Association with Sleepiness, Alertness and Fatigue ORIGINAL ARTICLES SOREMs in Sleep Clinic Patients: Association with Sleepiness, Alertness and Fatigue Chris Y. Kim, Alan Ong, MD, FRCP(C), D.ABPN, D.ABSM, Sharon A. Chung, PhD, Colin M. Shapiro, MBBCh,

More information

THE SLEEP DIAGNOSIS LIST (SDL): A QUESTIONNAIRE FOR SCREENING GENERAL SLEEP DISORDERS

THE SLEEP DIAGNOSIS LIST (SDL): A QUESTIONNAIRE FOR SCREENING GENERAL SLEEP DISORDERS THE SLEEP DIAGNOSIS LIST (SDL): A QUESTIONNAIRE FOR SCREENING GENERAL SLEEP DISORDERS Sweere Y 1, Kerkhof GA 2, De Weerd AW 1, Kamphuisen HAC 1, Kemp B 1 & Schimsheimer RJ 1 1 Centre for Sleep and Wake

More information

The Consequences of a Week of Insomnia II: Patients with Insomnia

The Consequences of a Week of Insomnia II: Patients with Insomnia THE INSOMNIAS The Consequences of a Week of Insomnia II: Patients with Insomnia Michael H. Bonnet and Donna L. Arand Dayton Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Wright State University, and Kettering

More information

Waking Quantitative Electroencephalogram and Auditory Event-Related Potentials Following Experimentally Induced Sleep Fragmentation

Waking Quantitative Electroencephalogram and Auditory Event-Related Potentials Following Experimentally Induced Sleep Fragmentation SLEEP, SLEEP RESTRICTION AND PERFORMANCE Waking Quantitative Electroencephalogram and Auditory Event-Related Potentials Following Experimentally Induced Sleep Fragmentation Kimberly A. Cote, PhD; Catherine

More information

Daytime Sleepiness: An Epidemiological Study of Young Adults

Daytime Sleepiness: An Epidemiological Study of Young Adults .. Daytime : An Epidemiological Study of Young Adults.19-. M Naomi Breslau, PhD, Thomas Roth, PhD, Leon Rosenthal, MD, and Patricia Andreski, MA Introduction The connection between insufficient sleep and

More information

Narcolepsy. Jon R. Doud, MD Pulmonary Physician Midwest Center for Sleep Disorders Aurora, IL

Narcolepsy. Jon R. Doud, MD Pulmonary Physician Midwest Center for Sleep Disorders Aurora, IL Narcolepsy Jon R. Doud, MD Pulmonary Physician Midwest Center for Sleep Disorders Aurora, IL Narcolepsy A central nervous system disorder that is an important cause of persistent sleepiness. The second

More information

Diagnosis and treatment of sleep disorders

Diagnosis and treatment of sleep disorders Diagnosis and treatment of sleep disorders Normal human sleep Sleep cycle occurs about every 90 minutes, approximately 4-6 cycles occur per major sleep episode NREM (70-80%) slow wave sleep heart rate,

More information

Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison With Young Adults

Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison With Young Adults Sleep 20(4):294-300 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep In Normal Subjects Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison

More information

Home Sleep Testing Questionnaire

Home Sleep Testing Questionnaire Home Sleep Testing Questionnaire Patient Name: DOB: / / Gender: Male Female Study Date: / / Marital Status: Married Cohabitate Single Divorced Widow/Widower Email: Phone: Height: Weight: Neck Size: What

More information

Procedures in the Sleep Laboratory

Procedures in the Sleep Laboratory AAST Technologist Fundamentals Date: May 7, 2017 Focus Conference Location: Orlando, Florida Workshop Procedures in the Sleep Laboratory Laree Fordyce, RST, RPSGT, CCRP Conflict of Interest Disclosures

More information

Arousal detection in sleep

Arousal detection in sleep Arousal detection in sleep FW BES, H KUYKENS AND A KUMAR MEDCARE AUTOMATION, OTTHO HELDRINGSTRAAT 27 1066XT AMSTERDAM, THE NETHERLANDS Introduction Arousals are part of normal sleep. They become pathological

More information

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. A Accidents, risk of, with insufficient sleep, 318 Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS), comorbid with narcolepsy, 298 299 Actigraphy, in

More information