Introduction. Overview

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Introduction. Overview"

Transcription

1 Cyclic alternating pattern Liborio Parrino MD ( Dr. Parrino of the University of Parma has no relevant financial relationships to disclose. ) Antonio Culebras MD, editor. ( Dr. Culebras of SUNY Upstate Medical University at Syracuse received an honorarium from Jazz Pharmaceuticals for a speaking engagement.) Originally released February 9, 2014; last updated November 14, 2017; expires November 14, 2020 Introduction Overview Cyclic alternating pattern is an EEG marker of sleep instability that modulates the flexibility of sleep in both physiological conditions and in sleep disorders. Together with sleep duration, sleep intensity, and sleep continuity, cyclic alternating pattern represents a topical pillar of sleep quality. Key points Expressed by the physiological phasic events of NREM sleep, including arousals, cyclic alternating pattern represents the most complex arrangement of sleep microstructure. Cyclic alternating pattern is organized in sequences organized in successive cycles. Each cyclic alternating pattern cycle is composed of a phase A (phasic events) and a phase B (background rhythm). Cyclic alternating pattern involves not only cerebral activities during sleep, but paces, and is reciprocally influenced by ongoing autonomic and motor functions, with activation during phase A and deactivation during phase B. The interaction between cyclic alternating pattern, neurovegetative fluctuations, and motor events determines the pathophysiology of several sleep disorders and the effect of medication and continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) treatment. Recent studies indicate the possibility of scoring cyclic alternating pattern automatically, opening new perspectives for a wider exploitation of this fundamental mechanism of the sleeping brain. Historical note and terminology Sleep staging: the conventional scoring rules. The standardized Rechtschaffen and Kales criteria for sleep staging have constituted the internationally accepted system of sleep scoring for 45 years (Rechtschaffen and Kales 1968). An enormous body of data has been produced and published in the scientific literature using this system. The American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) manual introduced in 2007 revised Rechtschaffen and Kales sleep staging to address digital methodology as well as the scoring of arousals, respiratory events, sleep-related movement disorders, and cardiac abnormalities, with consideration of pediatric and geriatric age groups (Iber et al 2007). In particular, the new visual scoring of sleep expands the number of channels indicated in the Rechtschaffen and Kales rules (single central derivation), allowing a more extensive coverage of scalp areas where significant sleep-wake rhythms and waveforms are better represented. In the AASM system, the distinction between wakefulness (W), non-rem sleep (N), and REM sleep (R) is maintained, but the non-rem stages are reduced to 3: N1 (previous stage 1), N2 (previous stage 2), and N3 (previous stages 3 + 4). Unfortunately, the lumping of stages 3 and 4 in a single stage N3 downsizes the nocturnal development of the process S, characterized by decreasing peaks across the night, according to the decreasing power of the EEG delta band (Borbely 1982). Instead of providing additional information on the power of the EEG signal, the adopted solution is a reductive simplification of the previous sleep stages. Moreover, obliterating the exponential profile of process S, the restorative function of slow-wave sleep, and its direct relationship with waking activities are shadowed. In the Rechtschaffen and Kales rules, EEG arousals were excluded from conventional staging procedures. In 1992, the American Sleep Disorders Association (ASDA; later named the AASM) defined arousals as markers of sleep disruption representing a detrimental and harmful feature for sleep (American Sleep Disorders Association 1992). In the following years, however, a number of studies clarified that spontaneous arousals are natural guests of sleep and undergo a linear increase along the lifespan following the profile of maturation and aging (Boselli et al 1998). Moreover, the

2 spectral composition of arousals and their ultradian distribution throughout the sleep cycles reveal that arousals are endowed within the texture of physiologic sleep under the biological control of REM-on and REM-off mechanisms (Terzano et al 2005). Arousal scoring is now considered a fundamental process in staging classification as well as spindles and K-complexes. In the Rechtschaffen and Kales system, a K-complex was unmistakably a marker of sleep stage 2. According to the AASM manual, if a K-complex is associated with an arousal the epoch is scored as N1. In other words, the Rechtschaffen and Kales approach privileged the role of EEG synchrony in the scoring process, whereas the AASM manual enhances the impact of EEG desynchrony (Parrino et al 2009a). In spite of the differences regarding sleep stages, arousals, and K-complexes, the 2 scoring systems share the same cultural frameworks, ie, rigid epochs of 30 seconds, neglecting evidence that sleep is a continuous function that cannot be restricted to a static sequence of artificial segments. In effect, each 30-sec epoch encompasses several short-time events that carry important information that does not show up in the classical sleep staging reports. These features have been grouped under the general label of sleep phasic events and detailed as K-complexes, sleep spindles, delta bursts, arousals, and K-alpha. The most comprehensive method for their analysis and report is the so-called cyclic alternating pattern (Terzano et al 1985). Because cyclic alternating pattern spans across long periods of NREM sleep, it overcomes the boundaries of rigid epochs and offers a dynamic contribution to the static framework of conventional scoring. Scoring rules and significance of cyclic alternating pattern. Cyclic alternating pattern is a well-defined marker of the physiological cerebral activity occurring under conditions of reduced vigilance (sleep, coma), translating a state of arousal instability and involving muscle, behavioral, and autonomic functions (Terzano et al 2002b; Parrino et al 2006; Parrino et al 2012a). During NREM sleep, cyclic alternating pattern is organized in sequences. A cyclic alternating pattern sequence is composed of a succession of cyclic alternating pattern cycles. The cyclic alternating pattern cycle is composed of a phase A (lumps of sleep phasic events) followed by a phase B (return to EEG background). All cyclic alternating pattern sequences begin with a phase A and end with a phase B. Each phase of cyclic alternating pattern is 2 to 60 s in duration. This cut-off relies on the consideration that the great majority (about 90%) of A phases occurring during sleep are separated by an interval of less than 60 s (Terzano and Parrino 1991). The absence of cyclic alternating pattern for more than 60 s is scored as non-cyclic alternating pattern and coincides with a condition of sustained physiological stability (Terzano et al 1986). An isolated phase A, ie, a phase A separated from another phase A by more than 60 s, is classified as non-cyclic alternating pattern. The reactivity of cyclic alternating pattern. Cyclic alternating pattern and non-cyclic alternating pattern can be consistently manipulated by sensorial inputs. Applying separately the same arousing stimulus during the 2 EEG components of cyclic alternating pattern, phase B is the one that immediately assumes the morphology of the other component, whereas the inverse transformation never occurs when the stimulus is delivered during phase A. This stereotyped reactivity persists throughout the successive phases of cyclic alternating pattern with lack of habituation. In contrast, when the same stimulus is presented during non-cyclic alternating pattern, the EEG responses are generally brief, hypersynchronized (slow-waves), and proceed toward progressive habituation (Terzano et al 1990; Terzano and Parrino 1991). However, a robust or sustained stimulus delivered during non-cyclic alternating pattern induces the immediate appearance of repetitive cyclic alternating pattern cycles that display the same morphology and reactive behavior of spontaneous cyclic alternating pattern sequences. The evoked cyclic alternating pattern sequence may herald a lightening of sleep depth or continue as a damping oscillation before the complete recovery of non-cyclic alternating pattern. General rule. Cyclic alternating pattern cannot be measured without having scored sleep stages in advance; however, it is not limited to epoch fragmentation and spans over long periods of NREM sleep. An A phase is scored within a cyclic alternating pattern sequence only if it precedes and/or follows another phase A in the 2 to 60 s temporal range. At least 2 consecutive cyclic alternating pattern cycles are required to define a cyclic alternating pattern sequence. Consequently, 3 or more consecutive A phases must be identified with each of the first 2 A phases followed by a phase

3 B (interval <60 s), and the third phase A followed by a non-cyclic alternating pattern interval of more than 60 s. Cyclic alternating pattern sequence onset must be preceded by non-cyclic alternating pattern (a continuous non-rem sleep EEG pattern for >60 s), with the following 3 exceptions. There is no temporal limitation: (1) before the first cyclic alternating pattern sequence arising in non-rem sleep; (2) after a wake to sleep transition; and (3) after a REM to non- REM sleep transition (Terzano et al 2002b). Cyclic alternating pattern sequences have no upper limits for duration and number of cyclic alternating pattern cycles. In normal young adults, 2.5 min is the approximate mean duration of a cyclic alternating pattern sequence, containing an average of 6 cyclic alternating pattern cycles (Smerieri et al 2007). Stage shifts. Within non-rem sleep, a cyclic alternating pattern sequence is not interrupted by a sleep stage shift if cyclic alternating pattern scoring requirements are satisfied. Consequently, because cyclic alternating pattern sequences can extend across adjacent sleep stages, a cyclic alternating pattern sequence can contain a variety of different phase A and phase B activities. Cyclic alternating pattern in REM sleep. Cyclic alternating pattern sequences commonly precede the transition from non-rem to REM sleep and end just before REM sleep onset. REM sleep is characterized by the lack of EEG synchronization; thus, phase A features in REM sleep consist mainly of desynchronized patterns (fast low-amplitude rhythms), which are separated by a mean interval of 3 to 4 min (Schieber et al 1971). Consequently, under normal circumstances, cyclic alternating pattern does not occur in REM sleep. However, pathologic conditions characterized by repetitive A phases recurring at intervals less than 60 s (eg, periodic REM-related sleep apnea events), can produce cyclic alternating pattern sequences in REM sleep (Terzano et al 1996). Recording techniques and montages. Cyclic alternating pattern is a global EEG phenomenon involving extensive cortical areas. Therefore, A phases should be visible on all or most EEG leads. Bipolar derivations such as Fp1-F3, F3- C3, C3-P3, P3-O1 or Fp2-F4, F4-C4, C4-P4, and P4-O2 guarantee a favorable detection of the phenomenon. A calibration of 50 mv/7 mm with a time constant of 0.1 s and a high-frequency filter in the 30 Hz range is recommended for EEG channels. Monopolar EEG derivations (C3-A2 or C4-A1 and O1-A2 or O2-A1), eye movement channels, and submentalis EMG, currently used for the conventional sleep staging and arousal scoring, are also essential for scoring cyclic alternating pattern. Amplitude limits. Changes in EEG amplitude are crucial for scoring cyclic alternating pattern. Phasic activities initiating a phase A must be a third higher than the background voltage (calculated during the 2 s before onset and 2 s after offset of a phase A). However, in some cases, a phase A can present ambiguous limits due to inconsistent voltage changes. Onset and termination of a phase A are established on the basis of an amplitude/frequency concordance in the majority of EEG leads. The monopolar derivation is mostly indicated when scoring is carried out on a single derivation. All EEG events that do not clearly meet the phase A characteristics cannot be scored as part of phase A. Time limits. The minimal duration of a phase A or a phase B is 2 s. If 2 consecutive A phases are separated by an interval of less than 2 s, they are combined as a single phase A. If they are separated by an interval of 2 s or more, they are scored as independent events. The A phases of cyclic alternating pattern. Subtype classification. Phase A activities can be classified into 3 subtypes. Subtype classification is based on the reciprocal proportion of high-voltage slow waves (EEG synchrony) and low-amplitude fast rhythms (EEG desynchrony) throughout the entire phase A duration. The 3 phase A subtypes are described as follows (Terzano et al 2002b). Subtype A1. EEG synchrony (high-amplitude slow waves) is the predominant activity. If present, EEG desynchrony (low-amplitude fast waves) occupies less than 20% of the entire phase A duration. Subtype A1 specimens include delta bursts, K-complex sequences, vertex sharp transients, and polyphasic bursts with less than 20% of EEG desynchrony. Subtype A2. The EEG activity is a mixture of slow and fast rhythms with 20% to 50% of phase A occupied by EEG desynchrony. Subtype A2 specimens include EEG arousals and polyphasic bursts with more than 20%, but less than 50%, of EEG desynchrony. Subtype A3. The EEG activity is dominated by rapid low-voltage rhythms with more than 50% of phase A occupied by

4 EEG desynchrony. Subtype A3 specimens include K-alpha, EEG arousals, and polyphasic bursts with more than 50% of EEG desynchrony. A movement artifact within a cyclic alternating pattern sequence is also classified as subtype A3. Cyclic alternating pattern sequences include different phase A subtypes. The majority of EEG arousals occurring in non-rem sleep (87%) is inserted within the cyclic alternating pattern sequences and basically coincides with a phase A2 or A3. In particular, 95% of subtype A3 and 62% of subtype A2 meet the AASM criteria for arousals (Parrino et al 2001; Terzano et al 2002a). The broad overlap between arousals and subtypes A2 and A3 is further supported by their similar evolution in relation to age and to their positive correlation with the amount of light NREM sleep and negative correlation with the amount of deep NREM sleep. Spectral composition. Power spectral analysis of cyclic alternating pattern components shows that the different phase A subtypes in non-rem sleep are variants of a continuous 2-fold process: an initial high-voltage slow-wave component, which predisposes the cerebral cortex to a greater readiness and opens the way to the more rapid activity, correlated with strong activating effects (De Carli et al 2004; Ferri et al 2005a). What distinguishes the single event is the buildup and reciprocal distribution of the EEG components. In the A1 phases of cyclic alternating pattern, which exclusively host K-complexes and equivalent slow-wave activities (vertex potentials and delta bursts), the starting delta power increase is maintained and prevails throughout the entire activation process. A balanced representation of slow and fast EEG frequency bands is the main characteristic of A2 phases, whereas rapid EEG activities are the dominant feature of A3 subtypes and arousals. This does not mean that all activating complexes exert equivalent effects on sleep structure and on autonomic functions. A hierarchical activation from the slower EEG patterns (moderate autonomic activation without sleep disruption) to the faster EEG patterns (robust autonomic activation associated with visible sleep fragmentation) has been described in different studies (Guilleminault and Stoohs 1995; Ferri et al 2000; Sforza et al 2000; Halasz et al 2004; Togo et al 2006). If AASM arousal is a sign of transient sleep discontinuity, the finding of phasic EEG delta activities during enhancement of autonomic functions indicates the possibility of physiological activation without sleep disruption (Halasz 1993). In effect, non-visible sleep fragmentation induced by acoustic tones has been associated with increased daytime sleepiness, indicating that the processes of sleep consolidation may be impaired (in this case by sensorial stimulation) without evidence of sleep discontinuity (Martin et al 1997). Description Cyclic alternating pattern and the gating mechanisms of sleep. Despite their EEG differences, slow EEG events (K-complexes and delta bursts) and AASM arousals (fast rhythms) share functional properties and, therefore, may be included in the comprehensive term activating complexes. Such a variety of EEG manifestations relies on specific gates controlling the flow of internal and external inputs. The thalamic-basal forebrain gate is an ultimate step of resistance against arousing impulses. Initially the cortex tries to preserve sleep continuity with reinforcement of its gates that are indicated by the occurrence of K-complexes and delta bursts in sleep EEG. However, when the thalamic gate cannot control the afferent inputs, a cortical change is seen, translated by an alpha mixed or an alpha/beta frequency burst (Hirshkowitz 2002). The initial reaction of the cerebral cortex is a sleep-protective response as the majority of transient rapid activities are preceded by a slow highamplitude EEG burst (Halasz 1993). Slow EEG events during non-rem sleep are an emergent property of corticothalamo-cortical networks. In particular, they originate from the dynamic interplay of 3 cardinal oscillators: the synaptically based cortical oscillator and 2 thalamic oscillators, ie, thalamocortical neurons and nucleus reticularis thalami. The functional implications of this dialogue provide permissive windows for cellular excitability and network plasticity during slow-wave sleep (Crunelli and Hughes 2010). Cyclic alternating pattern sequences reflect the balance between sleep- and wake-promoting systems. Subtype A1 is a natural delta injection fueling the build-up and consolidation of deep NREM stages and defending sleep against perturbations. In contrast, cyclic alternating pattern subtypes A2 and A3 drive the sleeper toward more superficial vigilance states. Therefore, cyclic alternating pattern sequences represent a protective, short-term homeostatic mechanism of non-rem sleep in which the amount of slow-wave activity is buffered and sleep continuity preserved (Halasz 1998). When delta power increases in sleep after sleep deprivation, the cyclic alternating pattern system reacts with a robust decrease of cyclic alternating pattern rate (De Gennaro et al 2002). Supplementation of sleep with slow waves after deprivation counteracts the production of cyclic alternating pattern sequences, probably because the sleep promoting system is under saturation.

5 Cyclic alternating pattern and oscillation below 1 Hz. Besides cyclic alternating pattern, the other major EEG activity in the frequency range below 1 Hz is the so-called slow oscillation (Amzica and Steriade 1997). This 0.5 to 0.9 Hz EEG rhythm, which characterizes states of reduced tonic arousal, was outlined during anesthesia and NREM sleep in both animals (cats and rats) and human subjects. The slow oscillation is generated in cortical neurons and consists of phases of depolarization characterized by intensive neural firing, followed by long-lasting hyperpolarization. Hence, the 2 phases of the slow oscillation are characterized by opposite neural phenomena: cortical excitation made up of synaptic potentials and cortical inhibition mainly due to disfacilitation in the network. The excitatory component of the slow oscillation is effective in grouping the K-complexes and delta waves, which do not occur in isolation, but are grouped into complex wave sequences. The coalescence of slow rhythms is especially visible during NREM sleep. In particular, high-voltage slow waves rarely appear as isolated features during slow-wave sleep, but in most cases they converge into collectives resulting in the phase A1 subtypes of cyclic alternating pattern. As NREM sleep progresses from stage 1 to stage 4, the differences in morphology and voltage between phase A (clusters of K-complexes and delta bursts) and the successive phase B (sleep stage background) become gradually less evident until the EEG is dominated by the uniform pattern of non-cyclic alternating pattern with the high amplitude slow waves recurring in the frequency range of oscillation below 1 Hz. Neurophysiological investigation has ascertained that the slow cortical oscillation (<1 Hz) is absent at sleep onset but begins to organize in small territories, thereafter recruiting larger ones through coupling mechanisms as sleep deepens. Consolidated slow-wave sleep is characterized by a sustained oscillation below 1 Hz, which reflects a stable non-cyclic alternating pattern condition (Terzano and Parrino 2000; Parrino et al 2012a). Cyclic alternating pattern and the structure of sleep. Sleep architecture is based on the cyclic alternation of 2 major neurophysiological states: NREM and REM sleep. NREM sleep is composed of 3 or 4 stages in which EEG synchrony grows with the increasing depth of sleep. In contrast, EEG desynchrony is the dominant feature of REM sleep. The alternation of NREM and REM sleep constitutes the sleep cycle, and its recurrence during the night determines the classical stepwise profile of sleep macrostructure. The NREM portion of the sleep cycle starts with a slow descending branch sloping from the more superficial to the deeper NREM stages; continues with a central trough that represents the deepest stages of the sleep cycle; and ends with a rapid reverse ascending branch, expressed by the more superficial NREM stages that precede REM sleep. Accordingly, the NREM sleep architecture delineates a continuous pattern of build-up (descending branch), maintenance (trough), and resolution (ascending branch) of EEG synchrony. A detailed investigation has ascertained that the spontaneous EEG fluctuations centered on the 20 to 40 s periodicity of cyclic alternating pattern are involved in the subtle mechanisms that regulate the production and attenuation of slow-wave activities during sleep. There is evidence that the different components of cyclic alternating pattern have a sculpturing effect on the profile of the sleep cycle. Comparing spectral assessment and EEG visual scoring of NREM sleep in normal healthy subjects, the amount of slow rhythmic oscillations (spectral analysis) parallels the number of cyclic alternating pattern cycles (visual detection), with a striking agreement between spectral power gatherings and visually scored A phases (Terzano et al 2005). The regular EEG oscillations that accompany the transition from light sleep to deep stable sleep are basically expressed by the A1 subtypes (Terzano et al 2000). Within the sleep cycle, 90% of the A phases detected in the descending branches and 92% of the A phases detected in the troughs are subtype A1, whereas 64% of the A phases identified in the ascending branches are subtypes A2 (45%) or A3 (19%). These findings indicate that both slow and rapid EEG activating complexes are involved in the structural organization of sleep. The abundance of A1 subtypes in the descending branches and troughs can be the EEG expression of the cerebral mechanisms involved in REM-off activity, whereas the predominance of subtypes A2 and A3 (and arousals) in the ascending branches reflects the REM-on drive (Terzano et al 2005). Therefore, besides their manifold EEG features, activating complexes are also characterized by a non-random distribution across the night, which assumes a clear-cut periodicity during NREM sleep within the framework of cyclic alternating pattern. For this reason, cyclic alternating pattern is considered the main expression of sleep microstructure (Ferri et al 2006). The measures of cyclic alternating pattern: ontogenetic aspects. EEG features are highly sensitive markers of brain development. Accordingly, sleep reflects the physiological changes that accompany the different ages of the lifespan. In particular, slow-wave sleep dominates in the younger decades in contrast to superficial NREM sleep, which increases with the aging process. As well as conventional measures, cyclic alternating pattern parameters undergo dynamic changes across the maturational phases of life. The age-related values of cyclic alternating pattern have been measured and defined in order to establish the physiological ranges of normal sleep (Parrino et al 1998). Cyclic alternating pattern rate. Among the various cyclic alternating pattern parameters, cyclic alternating pattern rate is the most extensively used for clinical purposes. Calculated as the percentage ratio of total cyclic alternating

6 pattern time to non-rem sleep time, cyclic alternating pattern rate is the measure of arousal instability; it can be enhanced when sleep is disturbed by internal or external factors, and its variations correlate with the subjective appreciation of sleep quality with higher values of cyclic alternating pattern rate associated with poorer sleep quality (Terzano et al 1990). In normal sleepers, cyclic alternating pattern rate shows a low intraindividual variability from night-to-night, but undergoes a complex evolution during development (Terzano et al 1986). Cyclic alternating pattern rate is very low in the first months and then shows a gradual increase with a peak in adolescence (Bruni et al 2002; Bruni et al 2005; Lopes et al 2005; Miano et al 2009). The lowest amounts of cyclic alternating pattern rate appear in young adulthood, followed by a linear increase from older adulthood to senescence (Parrino et al 1998). Clinical applications Clinical applications of cyclic alternating pattern. Cyclic alternating pattern and sleep disorders in adults. The cyclic oscillations of cyclic alternating pattern are physiological components of sleep structure in which they act as dynamic segments, pacing the state transition between different NREM sleep stages according to homeostatic and ultradian processes. However, cyclic alternating pattern sequences can occur also in response to external stimuli of different sensorial modality (tactile, thermal, acoustic, painful, etc.). Accordingly, the amount of cyclic alternating pattern increases when sleep is achieved under conditions of noise stimulation or in situations of sleep disruption, such as insomnia, depression, eating disorders, upper airway resistance syndrome, sleep apnea syndrome, periodic limb movements, nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy, primary generalized and focal lesional epilepsy, and Prader-Willi syndrome in adults (Terzano et al 1989; Terzano et al 1990; Terzano et al 1991; Terzano et al 1996; Terzano and Parrino 1992; Parrino et al 1994; Parrino et al 1996; Parrino et al 2000b; Parrino et al 2012b; Zucconi et al 2000; Farina et al 2003; Della Marca et al 2004; Priano et al 2006; Guilleminault et al 2007). It is lowered by sleep-promoting conditions such as narcolepsy, multiple system atrophy, drug administration, CPAP treatment in obstructive sleep apnea, and nighttime recovery sleep after prolonged sleep deprivation (Terzano and Parrino 1992; Parrino et al 1993; Parrino et al 1994; Parrino et al 1997; Parrino et al 2000b; 2005; De Gennaro et al 2002; Ferri et al 2005b; Terzano et al 2006; Vetrugno et al 2007; Svetnik et al 2010; De Paolis et al 2013). Cyclic alternating pattern is not only influenced by sleep disorders, but, in turn, modulates the occurrence and distribution of sleep-related events. In particular, phase A of cyclic alternating pattern triggers and paces the allocation of bruxism, sleepwalking, epileptic events, periodic limb jerks, and rhythmic movements during NREM sleep (Zucconi et al 1995; Parrino et al 1996; Parrino et al 2013; Macaluso et al 1998; Parrino et al 2000a; Manni et al 2004; Guilleminault 2006; Nobili et al 2006; Lavigne et al 2008). In contrast, phase B of cyclic alternating pattern is closely related to the repetitive respiratory events of sleep-disordered breathing, and only the powerful autonomic activation during the following cyclic alternating pattern A phase can restore post-apnea breathing (Terzano et al 1996). Like an alternatively opening (phase A) and closing (phase B) gate, cyclic alternating pattern phases act as periodic permissive windows in NREM sleep, offering a favorable background for phasic and/or repetitive sleep-related manifestations, ie, periodic limb movements, epileptic seizures, sleep apneas. Accordingly, a number of sleep disorders can be classified pathophysiologically on the basis of their relationship with cyclic alternating pattern and non-cyclic alternating pattern. In particular, periodic limb movement disorder, sleep bruxism, and epileptic manifestations can be considered as phase A-related disorders, whereas sleep apneas are a typical expression of a phase B-related disturbance (Terzano and Parrino 1993). The role of cyclic alternating pattern in primary insomnia. Primary insomnia appears to be the exaggeration of a physiological rhythm ordinarily involved in the sleep process. Previous studies have ascertained that acoustic stimuli enhance the physiological amount of cyclic alternating pattern rate and determine poor sleep and daytime dysfunction even without an increase of sleep fragmentation. In this perspective, cyclic alternating pattern operates as a doubleedged sword. Although limited quantities of cyclic alternating pattern mediate physiological effects, larger quantities reflect the brain's difficulties to consolidate and preserve sleep and, therefore, may be associated with detrimental consequences. Because primary insomnia is not supported by any other sleep, medical, psychiatric, or substanceinduced problem, there is no evidence of an organ disorder. In any case, whatever the nature of disturbance, the outcome is the amplification of an otherwise physiological process. Polysomnography investigation based on an extensive sample of Caucasian patients affected by primary insomnia

7 have demonstrated that cyclic alternating pattern parameters consistently reflect the reduced quality of sleep in insomnia complainers and can substantiate the efficacy of hypnotic medication (Parrino et al 2004). Discriminant analysis indicates that cyclic alternating pattern rate is the most sensitive sleep measure of effective drug treatment, whereas correlation analysis shows that cyclic alternating pattern rate is the polysomnography parameter that better reflects subjective sleep quality (Terzano et al 2003). Similar findings have been confirmed in non-caucasian subjects. In Japanese patients with psychophysiological insomnia, a randomized crossover comparative study with placebo showed that hypnotic treatment (zolpidem) increased sleep stability by significantly improving the overnight cyclic alternating pattern rate as well as subjective sleep quality (Ozone et al 2008). Cyclic alternating pattern parameters are also useful tools to monitor the effects of intermittent hypnotic treatment. In a double-blind study carried out on adults with primary sleep maintenance insomnia lasting longer than 1 month, polysomnography measures and perception of sleep quality were assessed at baseline and during the following 6 consecutive nights of alternating treatment with zolpidem (10 mg) or placebo. Compared to baseline values, cyclic alternating pattern rate, cyclic alternating pattern time, subtype A2, and subtype A1 were significantly reduced with zolpidem treatment and correlated with sleep quality, whereas they did not rebound above baseline with placebo (Parrino et al 2008). An intriguing aspect of insomnia is the established finding that people with this disorder often overestimate the time they take to get to sleep and underestimate the total amount of time they actually sleep. To investigate this mismatching phenomenon, a polysomnography study was carried out in 20 patients with a diagnosis of sleep state misperception or paradoxical insomnia (Parrino et al 2009b). Recruitment of misperceptors without coexisting neurologic, medical, or psychiatric disorders was based on objective total sleep time (TST) of at least 6.5 h, objective sleep latency shorter than 30 min, underestimated difference between objective and subjective TST of at least 120 min, and subjective estimation of sleep latency more than 20% of objective sleep latency. Polysomnography data of misperceptors were compared with those of 20 normal gender- and age-matched subjects (controls). Patients and controls presented nonsignificant differences in the amounts of objective sleep time (464 min vs. 447 min) and objective sleep latency (9 min vs. 8 min). However, compared to controls, misperceptors reported a significantly shorter time of perceived sleep (285 min vs. 461 min; p < ), and a significantly longer duration of perceived sleep latency (51 min vs. 22 min; p < ). Arousal index (32/hour vs. 19/hour; p < ) and total cyclic alternating pattern rate (58% vs. 35%; p < ) were significantly higher in insomniacs. In the sleep period between objective and subjective sleep onset, cyclic alternating pattern rate was 64.4% in misperceptors and 45.1% in controls (p < 0.002). Insomniacs showed significantly higher amounts of cyclic alternating pattern rate in stage 1 (62.7% vs. 37.5%; p < ) and in stage 2 (53.3% vs. 33.2%; p < ), but not in slow-wave sleep. The percentage of subtypes A2, which include both sleep promoting and wake-promoting EEG features, was significantly higher (p < 0.001) in misperceptors (31%) compared to controls (24%). Interestingly, misperceptors reported a limited amount of subjective awakenings (mean: 4) in contrast to objective findings (mean: 11). The mismatch could be explained in part by the high amounts of cyclic alternating pattern between successive awakenings, which were merged together in a single experience. In other words, if sleep between 2 successive awakenings is superficial (expressed by sleep stages 1 and 2), unstable (as reflected by increased amounts of cyclic alternating pattern), and fragmented (increased arousal index), time separating the 2 events is perceived as continuous wake. These findings suggest that difficulty to maintain consolidated sleep is interpreted as wakefulness in misperceptors. Sleep is a dynamic process with a self-regulating character. The nightly recurring sleep process is organized into consecutive cycles in which the sequence of NREM stages and the alternation between NREM and REM sleep show a quite stable tendency and a largely predictable pattern. These constraints produce the macrostructural development of sleep. However, transient EEG changes can interact with the expected development of sleep and ensure adaptation to both internal and external conditions. Cyclic alternating pattern and arousals represent rapid adaptive adjustments of vigilance during sleep. Failure of these compensatory processes conducts to non-restorative sleep. Therefore, assessment of sleep quality relies on a variety of polysomnography measures, including sleep duration (quantified by total sleep time and sleep efficiency), sleep intensity (reflected by stages 3 and 4), sleep continuity (altered by nocturnal awakenings and arousals), and sleep stability (impaired by excessive amounts of cyclic alternating pattern). These polysomnography measures are susceptible to deterioration in varying ways and proportions in accordance to the manifold clinical manifestations of insomnia. In the hierarchy of polysomnography measures, cyclic alternating pattern variables appear to be the most sensitive to any source of internal or external perturbation during sleep. Regardless of the specific characteristics of sleep alteration, insomnia ceases to be an indefinite mental disorder, but emerges as a subjective disturbance supported by measurable neurophysiological changes (Parrino et al 2004).

8 The importance of sleep instability. Objection could be raised on the usefulness of cyclic alternating pattern in routine clinical practice. Besides the consideration that the authors apply cyclic alternating pattern in their daily activities, attention should be focused on the philosophy that lies behind the cyclic alternating pattern scoring methodology. The main assumption of cyclic alternating pattern is that we must consider that during certain periods of the night the arousal level is unstable. The concept of instability is a basic issue of all complex systems and supports the dynamics of biological variability. Within certain ranges, instability warrants flexible and adaptive features to the complex system. In normal sleep, cyclic alternating pattern accompanies the stage transitions maintaining in-phase both the EEG and autonomic functions through regular fluctuations. This means that what we observe on the peripheral sensors (respiratory events, heart rate variations, blood pressure shifts, myoclonic jerks) has a consistent and synchronous expression on the EEG and vice versa. In other words, cyclic alternating pattern allows us to read sleep as a musical score where all the instruments play a coordinated harmony. This means that even without the EEG leads, the detection of an unstable cardiorespiratory pattern is certainly associated with an oscillatory behavior of the cerebral activities. To guarantee survival during prolonged unconsciousness (ie, sleep), a strong interaction among all the biological subsystems is mandatory. Accordingly, life-threatening events, eg, repetitive apneas, enhance the brain's necessity to increase the number of protective arousals determining a metronomic setting of oscillations expressed by prolonged cyclic alternating pattern sequences. Cyclic alternating pattern scoring incorporates all the conventional EEG arousals in NREM sleep and provides additional information supplied by the frontal EEG arousals (subtype A1), which are omitted by the official classification. All this remains unexplored using only the stepwise description of sleep stages. In particular, the new AASM rules have oversimplified the sleep process, merging stages 3 and 4 into stage N3 and overemphasizing the role of single arousals in stage scoring; the new AAMS rules are simply tools offered to accelerate scoring procedures in routine practice. However, it is a common experience that a number of patients remain inadequately diagnosed and treated in spite of an apparently normal sleep structure. A recent study carried out in patients with restless legs syndrome and periodic limb movements during sleep demonstrates that dopamine-agonist medication reduces the amount of leg jerks, but maintains high levels of cyclic alternating pattern with persistence of non-restorative sleep (Ferri et al 2010). Similar controversies happen daily in sleep labs that ascertain maintenance of excessive sleepiness in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome patients even when CPAP restores normal apnea-hypopnea index measures. An exploratory approach using the cyclic alternating pattern framework would probably shed light on these controversial cases. It is known that CPAP titration detached from the concomitant assessment of cyclic alternating pattern can jeopardize the effectiveness of ventilatory treatment (Thomas 2002). The persistence of cyclic alternating pattern and arousals even when respiratory events are controlled by autoadjust equipment indicates an inadequate titration procedure. Finally, the sensitivity of cyclic alternating pattern to medication in insomniac patients could be exploited in the evaluation of new sleep-promoting drugs. So far, hypnotic compounds have based their effectiveness on the paradigm of sleep-onset promotion, sleep-time enhancement, and wake after sleep onset (WASO) reduction. All the available hypnotic agents, especially those belonging to the gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA) family, share these features, which paradoxically offer a flat undifferentiated therapeutic scenario. The new sleep drugs in the pipeline, acting on alternative targets (melatonin, orexin, serotonin), need a new cultural framework that cannot be limited to sleep latency and sleep duration, but also needs to incorporate the issue of sleep and autonomic stability. So far, these objectives have been neglected by most clinical trials for drug approval. Comparing gaboxadol and zolpidem in a model of situational insomnia, cyclic alternating pattern parameters showed a significant independence from other electrophysiological measures (including spectral analysis), disclosed a strong association with subjective sleep quality, and allowed discrimination between the administered drugs. Zolpidem has also been used in a noninsomnia setting. In a recent trial, most patients with idiopathic central sleep apnea experienced a decrease in central apnea/hypopneas with zolpidem. They also had improved sleep continuity, reduced total number of arousals, and decreased subjective daytime sleepiness, without worsening of oxygenation or obstructive events. In obstructive sleep apnea, trazodone increased respiratory effort related arousal threshold in response to hypercapnia and allowed patients to tolerate a higher CO2 level. According to Younes, the percentage of time spent in instability is related to how far removed from stability the system is, relative to how much changes in the stability factors can occur spontaneously during the night. Some of the factors that determine stability change from time to time during sleep. Chief among these is arousal threshold (Younes 2008). A recent study established that trazodone increases the respiratory arousal threshold in obstructive sleep apnea patients with low arousal threshold indicating the potential inclusion of non-gabaergic agents in the management of sleep-related breathing disorders (Eckert et al in press).

9 Automatic analysis of cyclic alternating pattern. There is consolidated evidence that cyclic alternating pattern parameters provide more detailed information and are significantly more sensitive than conventional sleep measures. However, the visual scoring of cyclic alternating pattern is time-consuming, and this can compromise an extensive utilization of the method. In other words, only the availability of an adequate system for the automatic detection of cyclic alternating pattern can really make it an easily exploitable tool. The problem of automatic cyclic alternating pattern recognition has been addressed in several studies, with interesting results (Barcaro et al 2004; Ferri et al 2005c; Largo et al 2005). However, most proposed classification methods required the preliminary measurement of sleep macrostructure, making it necessary for the human scorer to visually classify the sleep profile. Moreover, they relied on the extraction of spectral parameters from the EEG signal to compute descriptors on time-windows and on the application of machine-learning algorithms for classifying each window as belonging to a phase A of cyclic alternating pattern or not. A preliminary study investigated a set of descriptors computed on 1-second-long windows with different techniques, and the information content of each descriptor was evaluated by means of ROC curves (Mariani et al 2011). Those descriptors were used in a subsequent work training 4 different classifiers for the classification of cyclic alternating pattern A phases, where the linear discriminant resulted to be the most accurate, with an average accuracy equal to 83% (Mariani et al 2012). However, due to the intrinsic characteristics of cyclic alternating pattern phases A, which consist of transient, non-stationary phenomena, it is likely that the signal will vary its characteristics over time, and it is not convenient to define a fixed window length. A dataset of 16 polysomnographic recordings from healthy subjects was employed, and the EEG traces first underwent an automatic isolation of NREM sleep portions by means of an Artificial Neural Network and then a segmentation process based on the Spectral Error Measure. The information content of the descriptors was evaluated by means of ROC curves and compared with that of descriptors obtained without the use of segmentation. Finally, the descriptors were used to train a discriminant function for the automatic classification of cyclic alternating pattern phases A. With respect to previous scoring methods, a significant improvement in terms of both information content carried by the descriptors and accuracy of the classification was obtained. Average cyclic alternating pattern rate obtained with automatic scoring was 44.83±12.41 vs ±10.13 measured with visual analysis. The correlation coefficient between the 2 series of cyclic alternating pattern rates was 0.73, and the P-value for the null hypothesis that the 2 populations had the same distribution was 0.62 (Mariani et al 2013). Although the sensitivity was still not very high (69.55±6.6), the specificity (90.49±2.8) and the accuracy (87.19±2.48) had a significant increase with respect to the previous study, indicating that EEG segmentation proves to be a useful step in the computation of descriptors for cyclic alternating pattern scoring (Mariani et al 2012). Conclusion. A certain amount of information remains uncovered if we limit our assessment to macrostructural events. As a marker of sleep instability, cyclic alternating pattern rate may be enhanced by a number of internal or external factors. Cyclic alternating pattern rate is a highly sensitive, poorly specific index as it cannot reveal the nature of perturbation. However, it certainly indicates that 1 or more factors are interfering with the processes of sleep consolidation and quantifies the magnitude of perturbation. Specificity of the disturbance factor is mainly supplied by the phase A subtype, which is differently modified by sleep disorders and medication. In contrast, the presence of noncyclic alternating pattern is closely related to a global condition of stability when all the subsystems that control and influence sleep mechanisms have achieved a reciprocally balanced interaction. From the cyclic alternating pattern/non-cyclic alternating pattern perspective, analysis of sleep microstructure is not limited to the finding of a single event (eg, an isolated arousal), but to the identification of a pattern (presence or absence of a cyclic alternating pattern sequence) that translates a physiological state involving cerebral activities, autonomic functions, and behavioral features. In other words, what happens upstairs (brain) is reflected at the lower levels (autonomic and muscle parameters) and vice versa. Once we attribute an activation relevance to all cyclic alternating pattern A phases (including K-complexes and delta bursts), we overcome the barren contrast between visible and non-visible arousals, which for years has frozen the possibility of understanding the flexible capacity of the brain to use different EEG features in different neurophysiological situations. Conventional EEG arousals are the peak of an iceberg composed of other more subtle, but equally powerful, activating events. References cited American Sleep Disorders Association. EEG arousals: scoring rules and examples: a preliminary report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association. Sleep 1992;15(2): Amzica F, Steriade M. The K-complex: its slow (<1-Hz) rhythmicity and relation to delta waves. Neurology

10 1997;49(4): PMID Barcaro U, Bonanni E, Maestri M, Murri L, Parrino L, Terzano MG. A general automatic method for the analysis of NREM sleep microstructure. Sleep Med 2004;5(6): PMID Borbely A. A two-process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol 1982;1(3): PMID Boselli M, Parrino L, Smerieri A, Terzano MG. Effect of age on EEG arousals in normal sleep. Sleep 1998;21(4): PMID Bruni O, Ferri R, Miano S, et al. Sleep cyclic alternating pattern in normal school-age children. Clin Neurophysiol 2002;113(11): PMID Bruni O, Ferri R, Miano S, et al. Sleep cyclic alternating pattern in normal preschool-aged children. Sleep 2005;28(2): PMID Crunelli V, Hughes SW. The slow (<1 Hz) rhythm of non-rem sleep: a dialogue between three cardinal oscillators. Nat Neurosci 2010;13(1):9-17. PMID De Carli F, Nobili L, Beelke M, et al. Quantitative analysis of sleep EEG microstructure in the time-frequency domain. Brain Res Bull 2004;63(5): PMID De Gennaro L, Ferrara M, Spadini V, Curcio G, Cristiani R, Bertini M. The cyclic alternating pattern decreases as a consequence of total sleep deprivation and correlates with EEG arousals. Neuropsychobiology 2002;45(2):95-8. PMID Della Marca G, Farina B, Mennuni GF, et al. Microstructure of sleep in eating disorders: preliminary results. Eat Weight Disord 2004;9(1): PMID De Paolis F, Colizzi E, Milioli G, et al. Effects of antiepileptic treatment on sleep and seizures in nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy. Sleep Med 2013;14(7): PMID Eckert DJ, Malhotra A, Wellman A, White DP. Trazodone increases the respiratory arousal threshold in obstructive sleep apnea patients with a low arousal threshold. Sleep. In press. Farina B, Della Marca G, Grochocinski VJ, et al. Microstructure of sleep in depressed patients according to the cyclic alternating pattern. J Affect Disord 2003;77(3): PMID Ferri R, Bruni O, Miano S, et al. The time structure of the cyclic alternating pattern during sleep. Sleep 2006;29(5): PMID Ferri R, Bruni O, Miano S, Smerieri A, Spruyt K, Terzano MG. Inter-rater reliability of sleep cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) scoring and validation of a new computer-assisted CAP scoring method. Clin Neurophysiol 2005c;116(3): PMID Ferri R, Bruni O, Miano S, Terzano MG. Topographic mapping of the spectral components of the cyclic alternating pattern (CAP). Sleep Med 2005a;6(1): PMID Ferri R, Manconi M, Arico D, et al. Acute dopamine-agonist treatment in restless legs syndrome: effects on sleep architecture and NREM sleep instability. Sleep 2010;33(6): PMID Ferri R, Miano S, Bruni O, et al. NREM sleep alterations in narcolepsy/cataplexy. Clin Neurophysiol 2005b;116(11): PMID Ferri R, Parrino L, Smerieri A, et al. Cyclic alternating pattern and spectral analysis of heart rate variability during normal sleep. J Sleep Res 2000;9(1):13-8. PMID Guilleminault C. Hypersynchronous slow delta, cyclic alternating pattern and sleepwalking. Sleep 2006;29(1):14-5. PMID

Atlas, rules, and recording techniques for the scoring of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) in human sleep

Atlas, rules, and recording techniques for the scoring of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) in human sleep Sleep Medicine 3 (2002) 187 199 Consensus Report Atlas, rules, and recording techniques for the scoring of cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) in human sleep Mario Giovanni Terzano a, *, Liborio Parrino a,

More information

The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events

The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events Summary of Updates in Version 2.1 July 1, 2014 The American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) is committed to ensuring that The AASM Manual

More information

Sleep stages. Awake Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3 Stage 4 Rapid eye movement sleep (REM) Slow wave sleep (NREM)

Sleep stages. Awake Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3 Stage 4 Rapid eye movement sleep (REM) Slow wave sleep (NREM) Sleep stages Awake Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3 Stage 4 Rapid eye movement sleep (REM) Slow wave sleep (NREM) EEG waves EEG Electrode Placement Classifying EEG brain waves Frequency: the number of oscillations/waves

More information

states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY

states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY introduction all of us are aware of the many different states of brain activity, including sleep, wakefulness, extreme

More information

EEG Electrode Placement

EEG Electrode Placement EEG Electrode Placement Classifying EEG brain waves Frequency: the number of oscillations/waves per second, measured in Hertz (Hz) reflects the firing rate of neurons alpha, beta, theta, delta Amplitude:

More information

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association Sleep in patients with a number of sleep disorders and

More information

LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART

LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART POLYSOMNOGRAM, SLEEP STAGE SCORING, INTERPRETATION Sleep Computing Committee, Japanese Society of Sleep Research LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART POLYSOMNOGRAM, SLEEP STAGE SCORING,

More information

Sleep Medicine. Maintenance of Certification Examination Blueprint. Purpose of the exam

Sleep Medicine. Maintenance of Certification Examination Blueprint. Purpose of the exam Sleep Medicine Maintenance of Certification Examination Blueprint Purpose of the exam The exam is designed to evaluate the knowledge, diagnostic reasoning, and clinical judgment skills expected of the

More information

EEG and some applications (seizures and sleep)

EEG and some applications (seizures and sleep) EEG and some applications (seizures and sleep) EEG: stands for electroencephalography and is a graphed representation of the electrical activity of the brain. EEG is the recording of electrical activity

More information

EEG workshop. Epileptiform abnormalities. Definitions. Dr. Suthida Yenjun

EEG workshop. Epileptiform abnormalities. Definitions. Dr. Suthida Yenjun EEG workshop Epileptiform abnormalities Paroxysmal EEG activities ( focal or generalized) are often termed epileptiform activities EEG hallmark of epilepsy Dr. Suthida Yenjun Epileptiform abnormalities

More information

Similarities between deep slow wave sleep and absence epilepsy

Similarities between deep slow wave sleep and absence epilepsy Similarities between deep slow wave sleep and absence epilepsy A.M.L. COENEN NICI, DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF NIJMEGEN P.O. BOX 9104 6500 HE NIJMEGEN THE NETHERLANDS Prologue Deep slow wave

More information

Beyond the Basics in EEG Interpretation: Throughout the Life Stages

Beyond the Basics in EEG Interpretation: Throughout the Life Stages Beyond the Basics in EEG Interpretation: Throughout the Life Stages Steve S. Chung, MD, FAAN Chairman, Neuroscience Institute Director, Epilepsy Program Banner University Medical Center University of Arizona

More information

FEP Medical Policy Manual

FEP Medical Policy Manual FEP Medical Policy Manual Effective Date: January 15, 2018 Related Policies: 2.01.18 Diagnosis and Medical Management of Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome Diagnosis and Medical Management of Obstructive

More information

FEP Medical Policy Manual

FEP Medical Policy Manual FEP Medical Policy Manual Effective Date: October 15, 2018 Related Policies: 2.01.18 Diagnosis and Medical Management of Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome Polysomnography for Non-Respiratory Sleep Disorders

More information

National Sleep Disorders Research Plan

National Sleep Disorders Research Plan Research Plan Home Foreword Preface Introduction Executive Summary Contents Contact Us National Sleep Disorders Research Plan Return to Table of Contents SECTION 5 - SLEEP DISORDERS SLEEP-DISORDERED BREATHING

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR In Physiology Today What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may

More information

Polysomnography Course Session: Sept 2017

Polysomnography Course Session: Sept 2017 Polysomnography Course Session: Sept 2017 General Information Polysomnography course will be held at SLEEP AND ALERTNESS CLINIC Med-West Medical centre 750 Dundas St. W., Suite 2-259 (Conference Room)

More information

Physiology of Normal Sleep: From Young to Old

Physiology of Normal Sleep: From Young to Old Physiology of Normal Sleep: From Young to Old V. Mohan Kumar Sree Chitra Tirunal Institute for Medical Sciences and Technology, Thiruvananthapuram 1 What is sleep? As per behavioral criteria: Reduced motor

More information

Basics of Polysomnography. Chitra Lal, MD, FCCP, FAASM Assistant professor of Medicine, Pulmonary, Critical Care and Sleep, MUSC, Charleston, SC

Basics of Polysomnography. Chitra Lal, MD, FCCP, FAASM Assistant professor of Medicine, Pulmonary, Critical Care and Sleep, MUSC, Charleston, SC Basics of Polysomnography Chitra Lal, MD, FCCP, FAASM Assistant professor of Medicine, Pulmonary, Critical Care and Sleep, MUSC, Charleston, SC Basics of Polysomnography Continuous and simultaneous recording

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may have as many as 200,000

More information

NORAH Sleep Study External Comment Mathias Basner, MD, PhD, MSc

NORAH Sleep Study External Comment Mathias Basner, MD, PhD, MSc NORAH Sleep Study External Comment Mathias Basner, MD, PhD, MSc University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine Page 1 > Mathias Basner Disclaimer The University of Pennsylvania and the German Aerospace

More information

Index SLEEP MEDICINE CLINICS. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Cerebrospinal fluid analysis, for Kleine-Levin syndrome,

Index SLEEP MEDICINE CLINICS. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Cerebrospinal fluid analysis, for Kleine-Levin syndrome, 165 SLEEP MEDICINE CLINICS Index Sleep Med Clin 1 (2006) 165 170 Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. A Academic performance, effects of sleepiness in children on, 112 Accidents,

More information

Neurophysiology & EEG

Neurophysiology & EEG Neurophysiology & EEG PG4 Core Curriculum Ian A. Cook, M.D. Associate Director, Laboratory of Brain, Behavior, & Pharmacology UCLA Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences Semel Institute for

More information

A Scream in the Night. ARTP Conference 2010 Dr Christopher Kosky

A Scream in the Night. ARTP Conference 2010 Dr Christopher Kosky A Scream in the Night ARTP Conference 2010 Dr Christopher Kosky Parasomnia Slow Wave Sleep Arousal Disorder REM Sleep Behaviour Disorder Nocturnal Epilepsy Catathrenia Slow Wave Sleep Arousal Disorders

More information

Sleep Medicine Maintenance of Certification Examination Blueprint

Sleep Medicine Maintenance of Certification Examination Blueprint Sleep Medicine Maintenance of Certification Examination Blueprint Purpose of the exam The exam is designed to evaluate the knowledge, diagnostic reasoning, and clinical judgment skills expected of the

More information

Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming. Elaine M. Hull

Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming. Elaine M. Hull Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming Elaine M. Hull Rhythms of Waking and Sleeping Animals generate 24 hour cycles of wakefulness and sleep. Some animals generate endogenous circannual rhythms (yearly

More information

Modules 7. Consciousness and Attention. sleep/hypnosis 1

Modules 7. Consciousness and Attention. sleep/hypnosis 1 Modules 7 Consciousness and Attention sleep/hypnosis 1 Consciousness Our awareness of ourselves and our environments. sleep/hypnosis 2 Dual Processing Our perceptual neural pathways have two routes. The

More information

Disorders of Sleep: An Overview 305 Leon Ting and Atul Malhotra

Disorders of Sleep: An Overview 305 Leon Ting and Atul Malhotra SLEEP MEDICINE Preface Robert D. Ballard and Teofilo L. Lee-Chiong Jr xiii Disorders of Sleep: An Overview 305 Leon Ting and Atul Malhotra Only recently has the medical profession focused on the importance

More information

Sleep Disorders. Sleep. Circadian Rhythms

Sleep Disorders. Sleep. Circadian Rhythms Sleep Disorders Sleep The Sleep Wakefulness Cycle: Circadian Rhythms Internally generated patterns of bodily functions that vary over a ~24-hour period Function even in the absence of normal cues 2 Circadian

More information

linkedin.com/in/lizziehillsleeptechservices 1

linkedin.com/in/lizziehillsleeptechservices  1 BSS2015 Hands-On Tech Breakfast SCORING SLEEP USING AASM GUIDELINES: A BRIEF INTRODUCTION Lizzie Hill BSc RPSGT EST Specialist Respiratory Clinical Physiologist, Royal Hospital for Sick Children, Edinburgh

More information

MOVEMENT RULES. Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program)

MOVEMENT RULES. Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program) MOVEMENT RULES Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program) 1. Scoring Periodic Limb Movement in Sleep (PLMS) A. The following rules define

More information

Diagnosis and treatment of sleep disorders

Diagnosis and treatment of sleep disorders Diagnosis and treatment of sleep disorders Normal human sleep Sleep cycle occurs about every 90 minutes, approximately 4-6 cycles occur per major sleep episode NREM (70-80%) slow wave sleep heart rate,

More information

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms People typically sleep about 8 hours per day, and spend 16 hours awake. Most people sleep over 175,000 hours in their lifetime. The vast amount of time spent sleeping

More information

Introduction to EEG del Campo. Introduction to EEG. J.C. Martin del Campo, MD, FRCP University Health Network Toronto, Canada

Introduction to EEG del Campo. Introduction to EEG. J.C. Martin del Campo, MD, FRCP University Health Network Toronto, Canada Introduction to EEG J.C. Martin, MD, FRCP University Health Network Toronto, Canada What is EEG? A graphic representation of the difference in voltage between two different cerebral locations plotted over

More information

Characterization of Sleep Spindles

Characterization of Sleep Spindles Characterization of Sleep Spindles Simon Freedman Illinois Institute of Technology and W.M. Keck Center for Neurophysics, UCLA (Dated: September 5, 2011) Local Field Potential (LFP) measurements from sleep

More information

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. A Actigraphy, 475, 485, 496 Adolescents, sleep disorders in, 576 578 Adults, sleep disorders in, 578 580 Advanced sleep phase disorder, 482 Age,

More information

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves SICE Annual Conference 27 Sept. 17-2, 27, Kagawa University, Japan Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves Seiji Nishifuji 1, Kentaro Fujisaki 1 and Shogo Tanaka 1 1

More information

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. A Accidents, risk of, with insufficient sleep, 318 Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS), comorbid with narcolepsy, 298 299 Actigraphy, in

More information

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle Sleep 10(1):62-68, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 7: Network models Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis II 6 Single neuron

More information

Circadian rhythm and Sleep. Radwan Banimustafa MD

Circadian rhythm and Sleep. Radwan Banimustafa MD Circadian rhythm and Sleep Radwan Banimustafa MD Homeostasis Maintenance of equilibrium by active regulation of internal states: Cardiovascular function (blood pressure, heart rate) Body temperature Food

More information

Normal sleep mechanisms & why do we sleep?

Normal sleep mechanisms & why do we sleep? 4 rd Congress of the European Academy of Neurology Lisbon, Portugal, June 16-19, 2018 Teaching Course 18 Basics of sleep medicine - Level 1 Normal sleep mechanisms & why do we sleep? Rolf Fronczek Leiden,

More information

Do non-benzodiazepine-hypnotics prove a valuable alternative to benzodiazepines for the treatment of insomnia?

Do non-benzodiazepine-hypnotics prove a valuable alternative to benzodiazepines for the treatment of insomnia? Do non-benzodiazepine-hypnotics prove a valuable alternative to benzodiazepines for the treatment of insomnia? A. KNUISTINGH NEVEN, DEPARTMENT OF GENERAL PRACTICE, LEIDEN UNIVERSITY MEDICAL CENTER Introduction

More information

Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms. Reading: BCP Chapter 19

Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms. Reading: BCP Chapter 19 Sleep-Wake Cycle I Brain Rhythms Reading: BCP Chapter 19 Brain Rhythms and Sleep Earth has a rhythmic environment. For example, day and night cycle back and forth, tides ebb and flow and temperature varies

More information

Neuroscience of Consciousness I

Neuroscience of Consciousness I 1 C83MAB: Mind and Brain Neuroscience of Consciousness I Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 What is consciousness? 3 Consciousness State of consciousness - Being awake/alert/attentive/responsive

More information

Simplest method: Questionnaires. Retrospective: past week, month, year, lifetime Daily: Sleep diary What kinds of questions would you ask?

Simplest method: Questionnaires. Retrospective: past week, month, year, lifetime Daily: Sleep diary What kinds of questions would you ask? Spencer Dawson Simplest method: Questionnaires Retrospective: past week, month, year, lifetime Daily: Sleep diary What kinds of questions would you ask? Did you nap during the day? Bed time and rise time

More information

SLEEP APNEA IN THE ELDERLY SLEEP THAT KNITS UP THE RAVELED SLEEVE OF CARE

SLEEP APNEA IN THE ELDERLY SLEEP THAT KNITS UP THE RAVELED SLEEVE OF CARE SLEEP APNEA IN THE ELDERLY SLEEP THAT KNITS UP THE RAVELED SLEEVE OF CARE OBJECTIVES 1. TO DESCRIBE THE NORMAL AGE RELATED CHANGES TO SLEEP 2. TO DESCRIBE THE SPECTRUM OF SLEEP-DISORDERED BREATHING. 3.

More information

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology INSOMNIAS INSOMNIAS General criteria for insomnia A. Repeated difficulty with sleep initiation, duration, consolidation or quality. B. Adequate sleep opportunity, persistent sleep difficulty and associated

More information

EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122

EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122 EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122 Zoltán Lelkes Electroencenphalography Hans Berger pen time amplifier electrodes 1 The waves of the EEG gamma > 30 Hz beta: 13-30 Hz Mental activity:

More information

CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL

CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL 116 CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL 6.1 INTRODUCTION Electrical impulses generated by nerve firings in the brain pass through the head and represent the electroencephalogram (EEG). Electrical

More information

Arousal detection in sleep

Arousal detection in sleep Arousal detection in sleep FW BES, H KUYKENS AND A KUMAR MEDCARE AUTOMATION, OTTHO HELDRINGSTRAAT 27 1066XT AMSTERDAM, THE NETHERLANDS Introduction Arousals are part of normal sleep. They become pathological

More information

Chapter 5. Variations in Consciousness 8 th Edition

Chapter 5. Variations in Consciousness 8 th Edition Chapter 5 Variations in Consciousness 8 th Edition Consciousness: Personal Awareness Awareness of Internal and External Stimuli Levels of awareness James stream of consciousness Freud unconscious Sleep/dreaming

More information

Bursting dynamics in the brain. Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST

Bursting dynamics in the brain. Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST Bursting dynamics in the brain Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST Tonic and phasic activity A neuron is said to exhibit a tonic activity when it fires a series of single action potentials

More information

MODEL-BASED QUANTIFICATION OF THE TIME- VARYING MICROSTRUCTURE OF SLEEP EEG SPINDLES: POSSIBILITY FOR EEG-BASED DEMENTIA BIOMARKERS

MODEL-BASED QUANTIFICATION OF THE TIME- VARYING MICROSTRUCTURE OF SLEEP EEG SPINDLES: POSSIBILITY FOR EEG-BASED DEMENTIA BIOMARKERS MODEL-BASED QUANTIFICATION OF THE TIME- VARYING MICROSTRUCTURE OF SLEEP EEG SPINDLES: POSSIBILITY FOR EEG-BASED DEMENTIA BIOMARKERS P.Y. Ktonas*, S. Golemati*, P. Xanthopoulos, V. Sakkalis, M. D. Ortigueira,

More information

Periodic Leg Movements in Narcolepsy

Periodic Leg Movements in Narcolepsy In: Nacrolepsy: Symptoms, Causes... ISBN: 978-1-60876-645-1 Editor: Guillermo Santos, et al. 2009 Nova Science Publishers, Inc. Chapter 7 Periodic Leg Movements in Narcolepsy Ahmed Bahammam * Sleep Disorders

More information

Exclusion criteria and outlier detection

Exclusion criteria and outlier detection 1 Exclusion criteria and outlier detection 1 2 Supplementary Fig. 1 31 subjects complied with the inclusion criteria as tested during the familiarization session. The upper part of the figure (ovals) indicates

More information

SLEEP STAGING AND AROUSAL. Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program)

SLEEP STAGING AND AROUSAL. Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program) SLEEP STAGING AND AROUSAL Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program) Scoring of Sleep Stages in Adults A. Stages of Sleep Stage W Stage

More information

SOMNAMBULISM: CLINICAL ASPECTS AND PATHOPHYSIOLOGICAL HYPOTHESES. Zadra, A., Desautels, A., Petit, D., Montplaisir, J. (2013) The Lancet Neurology

SOMNAMBULISM: CLINICAL ASPECTS AND PATHOPHYSIOLOGICAL HYPOTHESES. Zadra, A., Desautels, A., Petit, D., Montplaisir, J. (2013) The Lancet Neurology 11.11.2015 SOMNAMBULISM: CLINICAL ASPECTS AND PATHOPHYSIOLOGICAL HYPOTHESES Zadra, A., Desautels, A., Petit, D., Montplaisir, J. (2013) The Lancet Neurology Marion Charmillot Sleep, Cognition and Health

More information

A TECH S TOOLKIT FOR THE PEDIATRIC SLEEP LAB

A TECH S TOOLKIT FOR THE PEDIATRIC SLEEP LAB A TECH S TOOLKIT FOR THE PEDIATRIC SLEEP LAB Craig Canapari, MD craig.canapari@gmail.com drcraigcanapari.com: Updated syllabus will be here along with link to visual presentation. Twitter: DrCanapari INTRODUCTION

More information

ICNIRP 7th International NIR Workshop Edinburgh, United Kingdom, 9-11 May 2012

ICNIRP 7th International NIR Workshop Edinburgh, United Kingdom, 9-11 May 2012 RADIOFREQUENCY EFFECTS ON THE HUMAN ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAM: ITS RELEVANCE FOR HEALTH AND HOW DO WE EXPLAIN THIS PHENOMENON BLANKA POPHOF FEDERAL OFFICE FOR RADIATION PROTECTION OUTLINE History Cognition

More information

Normal brain rhythms and the transition to epileptic activity

Normal brain rhythms and the transition to epileptic activity School on Modelling, Automation and Control of Physiological variables at the Faculty of Science, University of Porto 2-3 May, 2007 Topics on Biomedical Systems Modelling: transition to epileptic activity

More information

The secrets of conventional EEG

The secrets of conventional EEG The secrets of conventional EEG The spike/sharp wave activity o Electro-clinical characteristics of Spike/Sharp wave The polymorphic delta activity o Electro-clinical characteristics of Polymorphic delta

More information

Night-to-night variability of apnea indices

Night-to-night variability of apnea indices Night-to-night variability of apnea indices M.M.R. VERHELST, R.J. SCHIMSHEIMER, C. KLUFT, A.W. DE WEERD CENTRE FOR SLEEP AND WAKE DISORDERS, MCH, WESTEINDE HOSPITAL, THE HAGUE In our centre, the diagnosis

More information

Sleep Across the Life Cycle

Sleep Across the Life Cycle SECTION II Anatomy and Physiology CHAPTER 3 Sleep Across the Life Cycle IOURI KREININ L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S On completion of this chapter, the reader should be able to 1. Describe the elements

More information

States of Consciousness

States of Consciousness States of Consciousness Sleep, Dreams, and Body Rhythms Introduction Consciousness Awareness of oneself and one s environment Body Rhythms Biological Rhythms Periodic physiological fluctuations Can affect

More information

Coding for Sleep Disorders Jennifer Rose V. Molano, MD

Coding for Sleep Disorders Jennifer Rose V. Molano, MD Practice Coding for Sleep Disorders Jennifer Rose V. Molano, MD Accurate coding is an important function of neurologic practice. This section of is part of an ongoing series that presents helpful coding

More information

Classification of Pre-Stimulus EEG of K-complexes using Competitive Learning Networks

Classification of Pre-Stimulus EEG of K-complexes using Competitive Learning Networks Classification of Pre-Stimulus EEG of K-complexes using Competitive Learning Networks Martin Golz 1, David Sommer 1, Thomas Lembcke 2, Brigitte Kurella 2 1 Fachhochschule Schmalkalden, Germany 2 Wilhelm-Griesinger-Krankenhaus,

More information

Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed. NAPNAP National Conference 2018

Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed. NAPNAP National Conference 2018 Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed NAPNAP National Conference 2018 (Honaker & Meltzer, 2016; Keyes, Maslowsky, Hamilton & Schulenberg, 2015) Chronically disrupted sleep

More information

Computational & Systems Neuroscience Symposium

Computational & Systems Neuroscience Symposium Keynote Speaker: Mikhail Rabinovich Biocircuits Institute University of California, San Diego Sequential information coding in the brain: binding, chunking and episodic memory dynamics Sequential information

More information

Practical 3 Nervous System Physiology 2 nd year English Module. Dept. of Physiology, Carol Davila University of Medicine and Pharmacy

Practical 3 Nervous System Physiology 2 nd year English Module. Dept. of Physiology, Carol Davila University of Medicine and Pharmacy Electroencephalography l h (EEG) Practical 3 Nervous System Physiology 2 nd year English Module Dept. of Physiology, Carol Davila University of Medicine and Pharmacy What is EEG EEG noninvasively records

More information

Sleep and Dreams UNIT 5- RG 5A

Sleep and Dreams UNIT 5- RG 5A Sleep and Dreams UNIT 5- RG 5A Goals for today Can you Discuss the circadian rhythm, what it is and how it effects us. Identify and explain each of the 5 stages of sleep. As well as the typical waves of

More information

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep Sleep, 6(4):319-325 1983 Raven Press, New York Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep T. Roehrs, F. Zorick, J. Sicklesteel, R. Wittig, and T. Roth Sleep Disorders and Research

More information

Polysomnography (PSG) (Sleep Studies), Sleep Center

Polysomnography (PSG) (Sleep Studies), Sleep Center Policy Number: 1036 Policy History Approve Date: 07/09/2015 Effective Date: 07/09/2015 Preauthorization All Plans Benefit plans vary in coverage and some plans may not provide coverage for certain service(s)

More information

EEG in Medical Practice

EEG in Medical Practice EEG in Medical Practice Dr. Md. Mahmudur Rahman Siddiqui MBBS, FCPS, FACP, FCCP Associate Professor, Dept. of Medicine Anwer Khan Modern Medical College What is the EEG? The brain normally produces tiny

More information

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition Chair: Jason Samaha, University of Wisconsin-Madison Co-Chair: Ali Mazaheri, University of Birmingham

More information

Insomnia. Learning Objectives. Disclosure 6/7/11. Research funding: NIH, Respironics, Embla Consulting: Elsevier

Insomnia. Learning Objectives. Disclosure 6/7/11. Research funding: NIH, Respironics, Embla Consulting: Elsevier Insomnia Teofilo Lee-Chiong MD Professor of Medicine National Jewish Health University of Colorado Denver School of Medicine Learning Objectives Learn about the causes of transient and chronic Learn how

More information

SLEEP DISORDERS IN HUNTINGTON S DISEASE. Gary L. Dunbar, Ph.D.

SLEEP DISORDERS IN HUNTINGTON S DISEASE. Gary L. Dunbar, Ph.D. SLEEP DISORDERS IN HUNTINGTON S DISEASE Gary L. Dunbar, Ph.D. Executive Director, Field Neurosciences Institute Co-Director, Program in Neuroscience Central Michigan University Pre-Talk Test 1. Which type

More information

Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems.

Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems. COURSES ARTICLE - THERAPYTOOLS.US Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems. Individual Planning: A Treatment Plan Overview for Individuals Sleep Disorder Problems.

More information

Correlation Dimension versus Fractal Exponent During Sleep Onset

Correlation Dimension versus Fractal Exponent During Sleep Onset Correlation Dimension versus Fractal Exponent During Sleep Onset K. Šušmáková Institute of Measurement Science, Slovak Academy of Sciences Dúbravská cesta 9, 84 19 Bratislava, Slovak Republic E-mail: umersusm@savba.sk

More information

Biomarkers in Schizophrenia

Biomarkers in Schizophrenia Biomarkers in Schizophrenia David A. Lewis, MD Translational Neuroscience Program Department of Psychiatry NIMH Conte Center for the Neuroscience of Mental Disorders University of Pittsburgh Disease Process

More information

The Role of Mitral Cells in State Dependent Olfactory Responses. Trygve Bakken & Gunnar Poplawski

The Role of Mitral Cells in State Dependent Olfactory Responses. Trygve Bakken & Gunnar Poplawski The Role of Mitral Cells in State Dependent Olfactory Responses Trygve akken & Gunnar Poplawski GGN 260 Neurodynamics Winter 2008 bstract Many behavioral studies have shown a reduced responsiveness to

More information

ARMA Modelling of Sleep Spindles

ARMA Modelling of Sleep Spindles ARMA Modelling of Sleep Spindles João Caldas da Costa, Manuel Duarte Ortigueira 2, and Arnaldo Batista 2 Department of Systems and Informatics, EST, IPS, Setubal, Portugal 2 UNINOVA and Department of Electrical

More information

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background:

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background: This presentation was given during the Dianalund Summer School on EEG and Epilepsy, July 24, 2012. The main purpose of this introductory talk is to show the possibilities of improved seizure onset analysis

More information

AASM guidelines, when available. Does this mean if our medical director chooses for us to use an alternative rule that our accreditation is at risk?

AASM guidelines, when available. Does this mean if our medical director chooses for us to use an alternative rule that our accreditation is at risk? GENERAL G.1. I see that the STANDARDS FOR ACCREDITATION state that we are to use the recommended AASM guidelines, when available. Does this mean if our medical director chooses for us to use an alternative

More information

Effects of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms on Epilepsy

Effects of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms on Epilepsy Effects of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms on Epilepsy Milena Pavlova, M.D. Medical Director, Faulkner Neurophysiology Laboratory Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women s Hospital Harvard Medical School

More information

Outline 3/5/2013. Practice Question. Practice question. PSYC 120 General Psychology. Spring 2013 Lecture 11: States of consciousness

Outline 3/5/2013. Practice Question. Practice question. PSYC 120 General Psychology. Spring 2013 Lecture 11: States of consciousness Outline 3/5/2013 PSYC 120 General Psychology Spring 2013 Lecture 11: States of consciousness The Nature of Consciousness Sleep and Dreams Psychoactive Drugs Hypnosis Meditation Dr. Bart Moore bamoore@napavalley.edu

More information

Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG

Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG Educational Symposium, MEG UK 2014, Nottingham, Jan 8th 2014 Krish Singh CUBRIC, School of Psychology Cardiff University What are we trying to achieve? Bridge the gap from

More information

Normal EEG of wakeful resting adults of years of age. Alpha rhythm. Alpha rhythm. Alpha rhythm. Normal EEG of the wakeful adult at rest

Normal EEG of wakeful resting adults of years of age. Alpha rhythm. Alpha rhythm. Alpha rhythm. Normal EEG of the wakeful adult at rest Normal EEG of wakeful resting adults of 20-60 years of age Suthida Yenjun, M.D. Normal EEG of the wakeful adult at rest Alpha rhythm Beta rhythm Mu rhythm Vertex sharp transients Intermittent posterior

More information

Sleep is that golden chain that ties health and our bodies together. Thomas Dekker, English dramatist ( ).

Sleep is that golden chain that ties health and our bodies together. Thomas Dekker, English dramatist ( ). Sleep Sleep is that golden chain that ties health and our bodies together. Thomas Dekker, English dramatist (1572-1632). Without adequate sleep people become irritable, have lowered resistance to illness,

More information

RESTLESS SLEEP IN CHILDREN. Lourdes DelRosso, M.D. MS Associate Professor of Pediatrics AASM, Scoring Manual Editorial Board

RESTLESS SLEEP IN CHILDREN. Lourdes DelRosso, M.D. MS Associate Professor of Pediatrics AASM, Scoring Manual Editorial Board RESTLESS SLEEP IN CHILDREN Lourdes DelRosso, M.D. MS Associate Professor of Pediatrics AASM, Scoring Manual Editorial Board To identify the clinical characteristics of children who present with restless

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

Combining EEG with Heart Rate Training for Brain / Body Optimization. Combining EEG with Heart Rate Training. For Brain / Body Optimization

Combining EEG with Heart Rate Training for Brain / Body Optimization. Combining EEG with Heart Rate Training. For Brain / Body Optimization Combining EEG with Heart Rate Training For Brain / Body Optimization Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. March 13, 2009 DRAFT There is a growing interest in combining different biofeedback modalities, in particular

More information

Chapter 6. Consciousness

Chapter 6. Consciousness Consciousness Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne What s It For? The Value of Consciousness Setting Priorities for Mental Functioning Sleeping and Dreaming Altering Awareness: Psychoactive Drugs

More information

Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data

Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data Recognition of Sleep Dependent Memory Consolidation with Multi-modal Sensor Data The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation

More information

EEG in the ICU: Part I

EEG in the ICU: Part I EEG in the ICU: Part I Teneille E. Gofton July 2012 Objectives To outline the importance of EEG monitoring in the ICU To briefly review the neurophysiological basis of EEG To introduce formal EEG and subhairline

More information

CONTROL OF MOVEMENT BY THE BRAIN A. PRIMARY MOTOR CORTEX:

CONTROL OF MOVEMENT BY THE BRAIN A. PRIMARY MOTOR CORTEX: CONTROL OF MOVEMENT BY THE BRAIN A. PRIMARY MOTOR CORTEX: - responsible for - like somatosensory cortex, primary motor cortex show (motor homunculus) - amount of cortex devoted to different parts of body

More information

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan Sleep 10(4):393-397, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Short Report Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan C. Guilleminault, S. Mondini,

More information

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children SLEEP DISORDERS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children Distinctive Features of Pediatric Sleep Daytime sleepiness uncommon

More information

A. PRIMARY MOTOR CORTEX: - responsible for - like somatosensory cortex, primary motor cortex show (motor homunculus) - amount of cortex devoted to

A. PRIMARY MOTOR CORTEX: - responsible for - like somatosensory cortex, primary motor cortex show (motor homunculus) - amount of cortex devoted to CONTROL OF MOVEMENT BY THE BRAIN A. PRIMARY MOTOR CORTEX: - responsible for - like somatosensory cortex, primary motor cortex show (motor homunculus) - amount of cortex devoted to different parts of body

More information

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night?

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night? Sleep Research Online 2(1): 15-19, 1999 http://www.sro.org/1999/ferrara/15/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 1999 WebSciences Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound:

More information