NICOTINE AND RESPONDING MAINTAINED BY CONDITIONED REINFORCERS: EFFECTS OF TWO NICOTINIC ANTAGONISTS

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "NICOTINE AND RESPONDING MAINTAINED BY CONDITIONED REINFORCERS: EFFECTS OF TWO NICOTINIC ANTAGONISTS"

Transcription

1 NICOTINE AND RESPONDING MAINTAINED BY CONDITIONED REINFORCERS: EFFECTS OF TWO NICOTINIC ANTAGONISTS By JEB JONES A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 9 1

2 9 Jeb Jones 2

3 To my parents without whom I never would have made it this far and to Jeffrey for his endless support 3

4 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I thank my parents for always pushing me and supporting me and encouraging me to work harder and persevere. I thank Jeffrey for his constant support and encouragement. I also thank Jesse Dallery who has been a fantastic and supportive mentor the best advisor I could have asked for. Finally, I would like to thank my colleagues who assisted in conducting the experiment and who reviewed drafts of the current paper: Bethany Raiff, Steve Meredith, Alana Rojewski, Rachel Cassidy, Matthew Weaver, and Matthew Capriotti. 4

5 TABLE OF CONTENTS page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS... 4 LIST OF TABLES... 6 LIST OF FIGURES... 7 ABSTRACT... 8 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION METHOD Subjects Apparatus and Materials Procedure Drug Administration Data Analyses RESULTS Effects of Nicotine Effects of nachr Antagonists Mecamylamine Hexamethonium DISCUSSION LIST OF REFERENCES BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

6 LIST OF TABLES Table page 2-1 Dose order by group Rates (± standard deviation) for each dose combination

7 LIST OF FIGURES Figure page 3-1 Effects of nicotine Group graphs of mecamylamine and hexamethonium Individual graphs of mecamylamine: Observing responses Individual graphs of mecamylamine: Food responses Individual graphs of mecamylamine: Extinction responses

8 Abstract of Thesis Presented to the Graduate School of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science NICOTINE AND RESPONDING MAINTAINED BY CONDITIONED REINFORCERS: EFFECTS OF TWO NICOTINIC ANTAGONISTS Chair: Jesse Dallery Major: Psychology By Jeb Jones December 9 Nicotine has been shown to selectively increase responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers as compared to responding maintained by primary reinforcers. In the current study an observing response procedure was employed in order to test the effects of nicotine and two nicotinic antagonists on responding maintained by conditioned and primary reinforcers and extinction responses. Mecamylamine, a central and peripheral nicotinic antagonist, and hexamethonium, a peripheral nicotinic antagonist, were used in order to localize the effect in the nervous system. Nicotine selectively increased responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers and mecamylamine, but not hexamethonium, attenuated this effect. These results suggest that the reinforcer enhancing effect is mediated exclusively in the central nervous system. 8

9 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Cigarette smoking provides numerous opportunities for establishing conditioned reinforcers from the olfactory, gustatory, and tactile stimulation directly associated with nicotine administration to the environmental contexts and activities associated with smoking over time. Nicotine, the putative unconditioned stimulus, not only plays a role in establishing these stimuli as conditioned reinforcers, it may also enhance responding maintained by them (e.g., Chaudhri, Caggiula, Donny, Booth et al., 6; Chaudhri, Caggiula, Donny, Palmatier et al., 6; Donny et al., 3). Indeed, enhanced responding for conditioned reinforcers following nicotine administration may be partially responsible for the maintenance of cigarette smoking (Caggiula et al., 1; Rose, 6) Understanding why people smoke requires a systematic and thorough analysis of nicotine s effects on responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers (Clarke, 1991). In the current study we examined the effects of nicotine and two nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nachr) antagonists on responses maintained by conditioned reinforcers. Experiments with humans have provided some evidence for the role of conditioned reinforcement in the maintenance of smoking. One method of examining the conditioned reinforcers present in smoking is by using de-nicotinized cigarettes, which provide similar stimuli involved in smoking except for nicotine (Rose, 6). Shahan et al. (1999), for example, found similar break points for subjects responding for nicotinized and de-nicotinized cigarettes on a progressive ratio schedule. Indeed, levels of consumption were similar across both types of cigarettes at each unit price, indicating that subjects worked just as hard to consume both types of cigarettes. 9

10 In a similar study, Donny, Houtsmuller, and Stitzer (7) demonstrated prolonged maintenance of smoking during eleven days in a residential setting in subjects smoking both de-nicotinized and normal cigarettes. Furthermore, those smoking de-nicotinized cigarettes continued to report suppression of craving throughout the study. This finding is consistent with the notion that smoking generates conditioned reinforcers which contribute to the reinforcing efficacy of cigarettes apart from nicotine s primary reinforcing effects. This stipulation is not without its caveats, however. Eleven days of explicit unpairing of nicotine and stimuli associated with smoking ought to be sufficient time to extinguish the purported conditioned reinforcers. Thus, it is possible that some of the stimuli experienced while smoking de-nicotinized cigarettes were actually serving as primary reinforcers, which may partly explain why extinction of the conditioned reinforcers was not observed in the Donny et al. study. Nevertheless, the results of these two studies, and others (e.g., Perkins et al., 1; Shahan, Bickel, Badger, & Giordano, 1), suggest that conditioned reinforcers play a role in the maintenance of smoking. Evidence of the role of conditioned reinforcers in nicotine self-administration (SA) has also been found in the animal laboratory (Caggiula et al., 1). Presentation of nicotine-paired visual stimuli (e.g., combination of cue lights and house lights) results in increased responding for nicotine (Donny et al., 3). Further, it has been shown that the presentation of nicotine-paired stimuli results in increased SA responding throughout extinction (Donny et al., 1999). Rats first acquired nicotine SA on a progressive ratio schedule, and then the SA response was placed on extinction either by pretreatment with mecamylamine or substituting saline for the nicotine. During this 1

11 exctinction phase subjects responded at higher rates when nicotine-paired stimuli were present versus when they were not present. Nicotine also increases responding for the stimuli with which it has been paired, and this has been shown to be a dose-dependent effect, with greater effects at higher doses (Palmatier et al., 8). Palmatier et al. paired a neutral stimulus with nicotine infusions and subsequently made presentation of the paired stimulus contingent on a novel response. Rats that had received higher doses of nicotine emitted the novel response at higher rates than those that received lower doses. One problem in nicotine SA experiments is separating the effects of nicotine SA from the behavioral effects of nicotine. That is, it is difficult to differentiate these effects because nicotine administration results in direct effects on behavior and, additionally, participates in conditioning novel stimuli as reinforcers. Thus, Raiff and Dallery (6) employed an observing response procedure (Wyckoff, 1952) to assess the effects of presession nicotine on responding for primary and conditioned reinforcers and responding during periods of extinction. In an observing response procedure, a rat can respond on either of two levers. One lever (i.e., the food-extinction lever) provides alternating access to a primary reinforcer or extinction. The other lever (i.e., observing lever) provides access to a stimulus which indicates whether the primary reinforcer is available or whether extinction is in effect. The stimulus displayed contingent on responses on the observing lever has either been explicitly paired with primary reinforcer availability (S+; e.g., solid houselight) or extinction (S-; e.g., blinking houselight). 11

12 The observing response procedure has been used to test for conditioned reinforcement (e.g., Dinsmoor, 1985), and offers advantages over other procedures (e.g., the conditioning-a-new-response procedure) in that it allows testing for conditioned reinforcement over long durations because the conditioned reinforcer does not undergo extinction, as in conditioning a new response procedures. The observing response procedure allowed Raiff & Dallery to demonstrate that nicotine increases responding for conditioned reinforcers but not responding for primary reinforcers or responding during periods of extinction. Note that in the observing response procedure, a period of extinction simply means that food reinforcers are not available, it does not mean that the S+ is undergoing extinction. The S+ is always associated with the availability of food. The current study expanded on the results of Raiff and Dallery in order to determine the effects of mecamylamine and hexamethonium, two nachr antagonists, on responding for conditioned reinforcers. Mecamylamine and hexamethonium have different sites of action in the nervous system. Mecamylamine is a centrally and peripherally active nicotinic antagonist whereas hexamethonium is only active peripherally (Goodman, Gilman, Brunton, Lazo, & Parker, 6). Thus, differential effects of these two antagonists on responding for conditioned reinforcers can help to delineate the neurophysiological locus of nicotine s effects on such responses. Mecamylamine, along with other nachr antagonists (e.g., dihydro-β-erythroidine) has been used to demonstrate that many of nicotine s behavioral effects are centrally mediated. Liu et al. (7) demonstrated that central nicotinic antagonists result in decreased responding for a moderately reinforcing stimulus. Further, nicotine blocks the 12

13 reinstatement of responding for nicotine-paired stimuli following extinction (Liu et al., 6). Nicotinic antagonists also result in decreased SA. DeNoble & Mele (6) found that presession injections of mecamylamine resulted in decreased nicotine SA in rats, whereas hexamethonium did not have an effect. Pre-cigarette administration of mecamylamine in humans results in self-reported increases in craving and negative affect (McClernon & Rose, 5) and reduced satisfaction of smoking and oral stimulation (Rose et al., 1994). The effect of nicotinic antagonists on nicotine-induced increases in responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers has not been examined using the observing response procedure. In the current study we employed the observing response procedure and administered mecamylamine and hexamethonium in conjunction with.3 mg/kg nicotine. First, we replicated previous studies by demonstrating that nicotine induced a selective increase in responding for conditioned reinforcers, but not for primary reinforcers or extinction responses (Donny et al., 3; Raiff & Dallery, 6, 8). Second, we tested the effects of mecamylamine and hexamethonium on these three responses. If the increase in responding for conditioned reinforcers is centrally mediated then mecamylamine, but not hexamethonium, should result in a decrease in responding for conditioned reinforcers. 13

14 CHAPTER 2 METHOD Subjects Subjects were eight experimentally naive male Long-Evans rats (Harlan; Indianapolis, IN). The rats were approximately 15 days old at the beginning of the experiment. They were individually housed in hanging polycarbonate cages with bedding, in a room that was temperature and humidity controlled. Subjects had free access to water and were maintained at approximately 85% of their 15 day old ad libitum weights, via post-session feeding (Lab Diet Rodent Diet, Formula 51; PMI Nutrition International, LLC; Brentwood, MO). The colony room was on a 12:12 hr light dark cycle (lights on from 8am-8 pm). Apparatus and Materials Four Med Associates extra tall operant conditioning chambers (Model ENV 7; 3.48 cm L x cm W x cm H) were used to conduct experimental sessions. Chambers were contained in large sound attenuating boxes equipped with fans for ventilation and to mask experimental noise. Each intelligence panel contained a food receptacle (5 cm x 5 cm x 3 cm) that was equidistant between two levers (requiring approximately.31 N force), each of which measured 4.5 cm x 2 cm and were located 22 cm from the chamber ceiling. Seven cm above each lever were three light-emitting diodes (LED; red, yellow, green;.8 cm in diameter,.7 cm apart from each other). On the wall parallel to the intelligence panel was a house light (28 volt), centered left to right and 1.5 cm from the ceiling. Purified Rodent Tablets (45 mg sucrose food pellets; TestDiet, Richmand, IN) were located outside of the chamber, but inside the sound 14

15 attenuating box. Experimental events and data collection took place on a computer in the same room, using Med-PC software and hardware (MED Associates). Drugs. Nicotine ([-]-Nicotine Hydrogen Tartrate Salt; Sigma, St. Louis, MO) was dissolved in potassium phosphate for all subcutaneous injections. Doses of.3 mg/kg were injected immediately prior to session. Nicotine vehicle injections consisted of potassium phosphate in isolation. Mecamylamine and hexamethonium were dissolved in saline. Doses of 1 and 5 mg/kg of mecamylamine hydrochloride and doses of 1 and 5 mg/kg of hexamethonium bromide (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) were injected subcutaneously 2 min before nicotine injections (Araki et al., 4; Ise, Narita, Nagase, & Suzuki, ; Olausson, Jentsch, & Taylor, 4a; Palmatier, Peterson, Wilkinson, & Bevins, 4). Mecamylamine and hexamethonium vehicle injections consisted of saline in isolation (1mL/kg). Procedure All sessions were conducted on separate days, seven days per week, at approximately the same time each day. Lever training. Research assistants used 45 mg sucrose pellets to reinforce successive approximations of the rats pressing the right and left levers. These initial lever training sessions ended after 3 min or after 2 responses on each lever. One response resulted in a food pellet, with the exception that after three consecutive responses on the same lever food could only be earned by pressing the other lever. Lever training continued for up to three additional sessions, as needed. Once lever pressing was acquired, subjects continued to earn food for pressing levers but were required to strictly alternate between the two levers such that the second consecutive response on one lever did not result in food. These alternation sessions lasted for a 15

16 maximum of 3 min or until 3 responses had been made on each lever, on seven separate days. The houselight was continuously illuminated during all lever training sessions. Discrimination training. After subjects were trained to press both levers, the Discrimination Training condition began. During the last minute of the blackout period, all three LEDs above each lever were illuminated to signal the beginning of the session. The LEDs turned off after 1 min and the houselight was simultaneously illuminated, either blinking or continuous (the first component type was determined randomly at the beginning of each session). This LED signal was in effect for the remainder of the experiment. Components alternated between a continuously illuminated houselight (S+) which signaled periods when food was available for pressing the left, food-extinction, lever (i.e., food components) and a blinking houselight (S-;.2 sec light-dark alternation) which signaled periods when food was not available for pressing the foodextinction lever (i.e., extinction components). Initially, a variable-interval (VI) 15 sec schedule was in effect on the food-extinction lever when the houselight was continuously illuminated. After seven sessions, this value was increased to a VI 2 sec schedule of food delivery. VI distributions were based on the Fleshler-Hoffman distribution (Fleshler & Hoffman, 1962) and were composed of 15 interval values. Components lasted an average of 6 sec (rectangular distribution ranging from 1 to 11 sec); however, if the extinction component was scheduled to change to a food component, it would not change until 5 sec elapsed without a response on either lever (i.e., differential reinforcement of other behavior [DRO] 5 sec). The DRO procedure was implemented to prevent adventitious pairings between responding during extinction 16

17 components and subsequent transitions to food components. Aside from the DRO contingency, responses on the right, observing, lever did not have any additional programmed consequences during Discrimination Training. Performance during the Discrimination Training phase was evaluated using the discrimination index (DI) equal to the rate of responding in the S+ divided by the combined rate of responding in the S+ and S-. Discrimination Training lasted a minimum of 65 days and until all but one subject displayed a DI of.75 or higher. Observing response procedure. The DRO contingency was discontinued when the observing response procedure began. At the beginning of the session, the computer randomly determined whether a VI 2 sec food or extinction component would be in effect; however, the stimulus corresponding to the selected component was only shown contingent on a response to the right, observing lever. Initially, only one response on the observing lever was required to illuminate the schedule correlated stimulus for 1 sec (i.e., fixed-ratio [FR] 1). If a component was scheduled to end during the 1 sec stimulus presentation, the component continued until the stimulus turned off. Immediately after the stimulus turned off the schedule changed. For example, if after 5 sec of S+ stimulus presentation the food component was scheduled to switch to extinction, the S+ and food schedule would remain in effect for the final 5 sec of the stimulus presentation and the component immediately changed to extinction when the stimulus turned off. For the first five sessions of the observing procedure, the first five observing responses resulted in the S+ stimulus being presented. If the extinction component was in effect when one of the first five responses occurred, the component switched to VI 2 sec food (Shahan, 17

18 2). Otherwise, components alternated every 6 sec on average as described for Discrimination Training. The presence or absence of schedule-correlated stimuli did not affect the probability of food deliveries during VI components. In other words, if the VI 2 food timer elapsed, the next response on the food-extinction lever during the VI component resulted in a food delivery, regardless of whether the schedule-correlated stimuli were on or off. Furthermore, the presence of schedule-correlated stimuli during the VI component did not ensure that the next response on the food-extinction lever would result in food this only occurred if the VI timer had elapsed. If the VI timer elapsed, but a food-extinction response did not occur during the VI component, the arranged food delivery was saved for the next VI component. After 11 sessions under the conditions described above, the VI 2 sec food schedule was increased to a VI 3 sec food schedule. Ten sessions later the observing response requirement was increased from FR1 to VI 5 sec. Thus, the terminal parameters of the observing response procedure consisted of a VI 3 sec food schedule alternating with extinction approximately every 6 sec, and stimuli were presented for 1 sec on a VI 5 sec schedule. All sessions from this point forward were arranged according to these terminal parameters and were 3 min in duration. Drug Administration Establishing increases with nicotine. Drug administration began after a minimum of 65 sessions on the observing response procedure. The first 14 drug sessions were the antagonist vehicle (i.e., saline) 2 minutes presession and.3 mg/kg nicotine immediately presession. These doses were administered in order to demonstrate that nicotine reliably and selectively increased rates of the observing 18

19 response. Injections were administered every other day and occasionally every fourth day. Sessions were always conducted during the intervening days. Antagonist administration. Drug administration continued to occur every other day. As stated above injections of antagonist occurred 2 minutes prior to session and injections of nicotine occurred immediately prior to the start of the session. The subjects were randomly divided into two groups and received a counterbalanced order of antagonist drug. Group 1 received mecamylamine first and Group 2 received hexamethonium first. The order of doses is shown in Table 2-1. The antagonists were given in descending order of dose (i.e., 5. then 1. mg/kg). The complete series of probe injections was administered two times. Data Analyses Paired samples t-tests were conducted to evaluate the effects of nicotine on observing, food, and extinction responses as compared to vehicle. Repeated measures analysis of variance, with Huynh-Feldt correction, were performed to evaluate whether there were significant differences across the different dose combinations for each type of response (i.e., observing, food, and extinction), as well as to test for effects based on order of administration of the antagonists. Bonferroni post-hoc analyses were performed to make direct comparisons of the effects of each dose on each response type. Alpha was set to.5 for all analyses. 19

20 Table 2-1. Dose order by group Group 1 Group 2 2 min presession Immediately presession 2 min presession Immediately presession Saline.3 Nicotine Saline.3 Nicotine Saline KPO 4 Saline KPO 4 Mecamylamine 5..3 Nicotine Hexamethonium 5..3 Nicotine Mecamylamine 1..3 Nicotine Hexamethonium 1..3 Nicotine Hexamethonium 5..3 Nicotine Mecamylamine 5..3 Nicotine Hexamethonium 1..3 Nicotine Mecamylamine 1..3 Nicotine Mecamylamine 5. KPO 4 Hexamethonium 5. KPO 4 Mecamylamine 1. KPO 4 Hexamethonium 1. KPO 4 Hexamethonium 5. KPO 4 Mecamylamine 5. KPO 4 Hexamethonium 1. KPO 4 Mecamylamine 1. KPO 4 Order of probe doses presented to both groups. Probes were administered every other day. This order of doses was presented twice. 2

21 CHAPTER 3 RESULTS Rates of responding for each of the three response types (observing, food, and extinction) are presented in Table 3-1 for each dose combination. All statistical analyses below were calculated using the percentage of vehicle responding for the relevant conditions. Effects of Nicotine Prior to administering the nachr antagonists, we examined the effects of nicotine alone on observing, food, and extinction responses. Figure 3-1 shows observing, food, and extinction responses plotted as percent of vehicle following vehicle and nicotine injections. Paired samples t-tests showed that nicotine selectively increased observing responses (t(15) = , p <.1), but not food or extinction responses compared to vehicle. Effects of nachr Antagonists There were no main effects of order of antagonist administration on observing or food responses. There was, however, a main effect of order on extinction responses, F(1, 14) = , p <.1. Extinction responding was consistently lower in the group that received mecamylamine first. Mecamylamine Figure 3-2 (left panels) shows the aggregated effects of both doses of mecamylamine on observing, food, and extinction responses when administered with vehicle and with nicotine. Mecamylamine resulted in a statistically significant reduction of observing responses when administered with vehicle, F(2, 3) = 23.61, p <.1, and nicotine, F(2, 3) = 36.5, p <.1. Mecamylamine also resulted in statistically 21

22 significant reductions in food responses when administered with vehicle, F(2, 3) = 2.51, p <.1, and nicotine, F(2, 3) = 19.59, p <.1. Mecamylamine did not have a statistically significant effect on extinction responses. Graphs of the individual subject data are presented for observing, food, and extinction responses in Figures 3-3, 3-4, and 3-5, respectively. Individual data were generally representative of the observed group effects. Post hoc analyses revealed that only the 5. mg/kg dose of mecamylamine, and not the 1. mg/kg dose, reduced observing and food responses when administered with vehicle, suggesting a general rate reducing effect of this higher dose. When administered with nicotine, 5. mg/kg mecamylamine reduced both observing and food responses. However, only the 1. mg/kg dose resulted in a reduction in nicotineinduced increases of observing responses. This lower dose had no effect on observing or food responses when administered with vehicle, nor did it have an effect on food responses when paired with nicotine. Effects of mecamylamine on both observing and food responses were dose dependent. Hexamethonium Figure 3-2 (right panels) shows the effects of both doses of hexamethonium on observing, food, and extinction responses when administered with vehicle and with nicotine. Hexamethonium did not result in statistically significant changes in any of the three different response types. Individual data were generally representative of the observed group effects. 22

23 Table 3-1. Rates (± standard deviation) for each dose combination Nicotine - Immediately presession Vehicle (KPO 4 ).3 Nicotine Vehicle (KPO 4 ).3 Nicotine Vehicle (KPO 4 ).3 Nicotine Vehicle (Saline) Mecamylamine 5. Nicotinic Antagonist 2 min presession Mecamylamine 1. Observing Responses Hexamethonium 5. Hexamethonium Food Responses Extinction Responses

24 Observing Responses Food Responses EXT Responses Percent of Vehicle Saline-KPO4 Saline-.3mg/kg Nicotine Saline-KPO4 Saline-.3mg/kg Nicotine Saline-KPO4 Saline-.3mg/kg Nicotine Figure 3-1. Effects of nicotine versus vehicle on observing (first panel), food (second panel), and extinction (third panel) responses. Error bars represent standard deviations. 24

25 Observing Responses Observing Responses Percent of Vehicle Responding 4 3 Food Responses Extinction Responses 4 3 Food Responses Extinction Responses Saline - KPO4 5. Mec - KPO4 1. Mec - KPO4 Saline -.3 Nic 5. Mec -.3 Nic 1. Mec -.3 Nic Saline - KPO4 5. Hex - KPO4 1. Hex - KPO4 Saline -.3 Nic 5. Hex -.3 Nic 1. Hex -.3 Nic Figure 3-2. Effects of mecamylamine (left panels) and hexamethonium (right panels) on observing (top panels), food (middle panels), and extinction responses (bottom panels) as percent of vehicle responding. Bars represent group means and closed circles are individual data points. 25

26 Observing Responses Percent of Vehicle Responding Sal - KPO4 5. Mec - KPO4 1. Mec - KPO4 Sal - Nic 5. Mec - Nic 1. Mec - Nic Sal - KPO4 5. Mec - KPO4 1. Mec - KPO4 Sal - Nic 5. Mec - Nic 1. Mec - Nic Figure 3-3. Effects of both doses of mecamylamine on observing responses for each subject plotted as percent of vehicle responding. Error bars represent the range. 26

27 Food Responses Percent of Vehicle Responding Sal - KPO4 5. Mec - KPO4 1. Mec - KPO4 Sal - Nic 5. Mec - Nic 1. Mec - Nic Sal - KPO4 5. Mec - KPO4 1. Mec - KPO4 Sal - Nic 5. Mec - Nic 1. Mec - Nic Figure 3-4. Effects of both doses of mecamylamine on food responses for each subject plotted as percent of vehicle responding. Error bars represent the range. 27

28 Extinction Responses Percent of Vehicle Responding Sal - KPO4 5. Mec - KPO4 1. Mec - KPO4 Sal - Nic 5. Mec - Nic 1. Mec - Nic Sal - KPO4 5. Mec - KPO4 1. Mec - KPO4 Sal - Nic 5. Mec - Nic 1. Mec - Nic Figure 3-5. Effects of both doses of mecamylamine on extinction responses for each subject plotted as percent of vehicle responding. Error bars represent the range. 28

29 CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION The results of the current study add to the growing corpus of data suggesting that nicotine has a selective, enhancing effect on responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers. We demonstrated 1) that nicotine selectively increased responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers, and 2) that mecamylamine, but not hexamethonium, antagonized this effect. Because mecamylamine readily crosses the blood-brain barrier and hexamethonium does not, these results suggest that nicotine s effect on responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers is mediated via nachrs in the central nervous system. Further, by using the observing response procedure we were able to conduct a rigorous, extended test of conditioned reinforcement. This procedure also allowed us to compare responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers to responding maintained by primary reinforcers and responding in extinction. It has previously been demonstrated that the S+ is responsible for maintaining responding for the observing stimulus (Dinsmoor, Browne, & Lawrence, 1972). When presented in isolation, Dinsmoor et al. found that only the S+, and not the S-, maintained levels of responding similar to when the two stimuli were both available. Thus, it is not information per se that maintains responding on the observing lever, rather it is the conditioned reinforcer aspects of the stimulus that has in the past been paired with primary reinforcer delivery. Indeed responding is higher when only an S+ is available as compared to when an S+ and S- are available, as in the current study. It has been suggested that the increases in responding observed following nicotine administration are due to a general behavioral activation as opposed to a 29

30 reinforcer-enhancing effect (Frenk & Dar, 4). However, it should be noted that the behavioral activation explanation does not account for the selectivity of response increases in the current study and others (e.g., Raiff & Dallery, 8). That is, a general behavioral activation should increase responding across the board; however we demonstrated that nicotine selectively increases responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers, but not responding maintained by primary reinforcers or responses under extinction. The effects engendered by nicotine might be characterized as rate-dependent, particularly the food responses. In a comparison of rates following administration of vehicle versus nicotine, Pearson s r for observing, food, and extinction responses was -.65, -.84, and -.69, respectively. Thus, particularly for food responses the effects appear to be correlated with the rate of vehicle responding. It should be noted, however, that rate-dependency is merely descriptive of the relationship between vehicle and drug responding and does not account for why the relationship exists (Poling & Byrne, ). Specifically, it does not account for the disparity in increases across response types nor for the effects of the nachr anagonists. Nicotinic interaction with nachrs results in wide ranging effects on nicotine SA and other responses influenced by nicotine across species. Mecamylamine attenuates nicotine SA (DeNoble & Mele, 6; Donny et al., 1999) or increases preference for higher doses of nicotine (Glick, Visker, & Maisonnneuve, 1996) in rats. Mecamylamine also blocks reinstatement of nicotine SA brought on by nicotine-paired stimuli (Liu et al., 6; Liu, Caggiula et al., 7). It has also been noted that the β2 subunit of nachrs plays a vital role in the reinforcing properties of nicotine (Wannacott, Sidhpura, & 3

31 Balfour, 5). Because it is known to be active on these nachr subtypes (Papke, Sanberg, & Shytle, 1), some of mecamylamine s behavioral effects might likely be mediated through β2 receptor subtypes. In humans, contrary to its effect on rats, mecamylamine results in increased nicotine SA, whether via smoking (Rose & Corrigall, 1997) or intravenous injection (Rose, Behm, Westman, & Bates, 3). Rose and Corrigall suggest that the disparate effects between rats and humans might be due to the fact that human study participants are nicotine dependent but rats are not. Thus humans respond more for nicotine following mecamylamine administration in order to overcome the nachr antagonizing effect of the drug. Even still, mecamylamine does result in reduced smoking satisfaction (Rose et al., 1994). Nicotine also effects behavior in ways which may be indirectly related to SA. It has been shown to have a reinforcer-enhancing effect in rats (Chaudhri, Caggiula, Donny, Palmatier et al., 6; Olausson et al., 4a; Olausson, Jentsch, & Taylor, 4b; Raiff & Dallery, 8). Olausson et al. demonstrated that nicotine selectively increases responding for conditioned reinforcers and that mecamylamine, a nachr antagonist, offsets this effect. Nicotine did not, however, increase responding for other stimuli suggesting that the increase in responding was dependent on the conditioned reinforcing aspects of the stimulus and was not due to a general, undifferentiated increase in responding. Additionally, this effect is present whether nicotine delivery is response-dependent or response-independent (Chaudhri, Caggiula, Donny, Booth et al., 6). 31

32 Activation of nachrs in the central nervous system results in widespread neural effects involving many neurotransmitter systems including dopamine, glutamate, and γ- aminobutyric acid. Indeed, complex interactions over time result in different effects on dopaminergic systems in acute versus chronic dosing (Laviolette & van der Kooy, 4). For this reason it would be beneficial to replicate the current study under a chronic dosing regimen to determine if the results are disparate with the current data. However, it is important to note that Laviolette and van der Kooy report that mesolimbic dopamine signaling might serve to signal stimuli which predict reward and that this effect might be similar in both acute and chronic dosing. Activation of dopaminergic tracts in the nucleus accumbens is also widely implicated in as one factor influencing reward, and thus abuse liability, of drugs of abuse (Di Chiara et al., 4). Indeed, nicotine elicits a similar increase in dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens as cocaine and amphetamine, albeit to a lesser extent. Furthermore, it has been noted that the two subunits of the nucleus accumbens, the core and the shell, have different behavioral effects (Balfour, 4). It is beyond the scope of the current study to tease apart the effects of these antagonists on accumbal dopamine expression, particularly due to mecamylamine s lack of specificity. However, it would be beneficial for future studies to examine the role of more specific nicotinic antagonists on sensitivity to conditioned reinforcers, as well as to nicotine in acquisition and maintenance of nicotine self-administration. Balfour (4) has also argued for a specific role of extracellular dopamine in the medial shell of the nucleus accumbens. His hypothesis is that behavior that is accompanied by an increase in extracellular dopamine is reinforced. Thus, any and all 32

33 behavior occurring in conjunction with nicotine-induced increases in extracellular nicotine should increase in rate. However, as the current study indicates, it is not all behavior that is increased, but rather only behavior maintained by specific types of consequences, namely conditioned reinforcers. The current study suffered from a few limitations. In order to make comparisons across subjects, each subject received the same dose of nicotine (.3 mg/kg). Although this dose has frequently been the most behaviorally active in previous experiments in our lab, testing for the dose of nicotine that resulted in the greatest increases in responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers might have resulted in clearer effects of the two different antagonists. Further, there were only two tests of each antagonist dose. More replications would have been desirable. Future studies should conduct more extend investigations of these drugs as well as more specific nachr antagonists (e.g., dihydro-β-erythroidine [DHBE] or varenicline). Even still, the results of the current study strongly implicate central, but not peripheral, nachrs in the mediation of nicotine s effects on responding maintained by conditioned reinforcers. 33

34 LIST OF REFERENCES Araki, H., Kawakami, K.-y., Jin, C., Suemaru, K., Kitamura, Y., Nagata, M., et al. (4). Nicotine attenuates place aversion induced by naloxone in single-dose, morphinetreated rats. Psychopharmacology, 171(4), Balfour, D. J. (4). The neurobiology of tobacco dependence: A preclinical perspective on the role of the dopamine projections to the nucleus. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 6(6), Caggiula, A. R., Donny, E. C., White, A. R., Chaudhri, N., Booth, S., Gharib, M. A., et al. (1). Cue dependency of nicotine self-administration and smoking. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 7(4), Chaudhri, N., Caggiula, A. R., Donny, E. C., Booth, S., Gharib, M., Craven, L., et al. (6). Operant responding for conditioned and unconditioned reinforcers in rats is differentially enhanced by the primary reinforcing and reinforcement-enhancing effects of nicotine. Psychopharmacology, 189(1), Chaudhri, N., Caggiula, A. R., Donny, E. C., Palmatier, M. I., Liu, X., & Sved, A. F. (6). Complex interactions between nicotine and nonpharmacological stimuli reveal multiple roles for nicotine in reinforcement. Psychopharmacology, 184(3), Clarke, P. B. (1991). Nicotinic receptor blockade therapy and smoking cessation. British Journal of Addiction, 86(5), DeNoble, V. J., & Mele, P. C. (6). Intravenous nicotine self-administration in rats: effects of mecamylamine, hexamethonium, and naloxone. Psychopharmacology, 184(3-4), Di Chiara, G., Bassareo, V., Fenu, S., De Luca, M. A., Spina, L., Cadoni, C., et al. (4). Dopamine and drug addiction: the nucleus accumbens shell connection. Neuropharmacology, 47(Suppl1), Dinsmoor, J. A. (1985). The role of observing and attention in establishing stimulus control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43(3), Dinsmoor, J. A., Browne, M. P., & Lawrence, C. E. (1972). A test of the negative discriminative stimulus as a reinforcer of observing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18(1), Donny, E. C., Caggiula, A. R., Mielke, M. M., Booth, S., Gharib, M., Hoffman, A., et al. (1999). Nicotine self-administration in rats on a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology, 147(2),

35 Donny, E. C., Chaudhri, N., Caggiula, A. R., Evans-Martin, F. F., Booth, S., Gharib, M. A., et al. (3). Operant responding for a visual reinforcer in rats is enhanced by noncontingent nicotine: implications for nicotine self-administration and reinforcement. Psychopharmacology, 169(1), Donny, E. C., Houtsmuller, E., & Stitzer, M. L. (7). Smoking in the absence of nicotine: Behavioral, subjective and physiological effects over 11 days. Addiction, 12(2), Fleshler, M., & Hoffman, H. S. (1962). A progression for generating variable interval schedules. J. exp. Anal. Behav., 5, Frenk, H., & Dar, R. (4). Reward potentiation or behavioral activation? A comment on Donny et al. Psychopharmacology, 171(4), Glick, S. D., Visker, K. E., & Maisonnneuve, I. M. (1996). An oral self-administration model of nicotine preference in rats: Effects of mecamylamine. Psychopharmacology, 128(4), Goodman, L. S., Gilman, A., Brunton, L. L., Lazo, J. S., & Parker, K. L. (6). Goodmand & Gilman's The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics (11th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Ise, Y., Narita, M., Nagase, H., & Suzuki, T. (). Modulation of opioidergic system on mecamylamine-precipitated nicotine-withdrawal aversion in rats. Psychopharmacology, 151(1), Kumar, R., Reavill, C., & Stolerman, I. P. (1987). Nicotine cue in rats: effects of central administration of ganglion-blocking drugs. British Journal of Pharmacology, 9(1), Laviolette, S. R., & van der Kooy, D. (4). The neurobiology of nicotine addiction: bridging the gap from molecules to behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci, 5(1), Liu, X., Caggiula, A. R., Yee, S. K., Nobuta, H., Poland, R. E., & Pechnick, R. N. (6). Reinstatement of nicotine-seeking behavior by drug-associated stimuli after extinction in rats. Psychopharmacology, 184(3), Liu, X., Caggiula, A. R., Yee, S. K., Nobuta, H., Sved, A. F., Pechnick, R. N., et al. (7). Mecamylamine Attenuates Cue-Induced Reinstatement of Nicotine- Seeking Behavior in Rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(3), Liu, X., Palmatier, M. I., Caggiula, A. R., Bonny, E. C., & Sved, A. F. (7). Reinforcement enhancing effect of nicotine and its attenuation by nicotinic antagonists in rats. Psychopharmacology, 194(4), McClernon, F. J., & Rose, J. E. (5). Mecamylamine moderates cue-induced emotional responses in smokers. Addictive Behaviors, 3(4),

36 Olausson, P., Jentsch, J. D., & Taylor, J. R. (4a). Nicotine enhances responding with conditioned reinforcement. Psychopharmacology, 171(2), Olausson, P., Jentsch, J. D., & Taylor, J. R. (4b). Repeated nicotine exposure enhances responding with conditioned reinforcement. Psychopharmacology, 173(1), Palmatier, M. I., Coddington, S. B., Liu, X., Donny, E. C., Caggiula, A. R., & Sved, A. F. (8). The motivation to obtain nicotine-conditioned reinforcers depends on nicotine dose. Neuropharmacology, 55(8), Palmatier, M. I., Peterson, J. L., Wilkinson, J. L., & Bevins, R. A. (4). Nicotine serves as a feature-positive modulator of Pavlovian appetitive conditioning in rats. Behavioural Pharmacology, 15(3), Papke, R. L., Sanberg, P. R., & Shytle, R. D. (1). Analysis of mecamylamine stereoisomers on human nicotinic receptor subtypes. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 297(2), Perkins, K. A., Gerlach, D., Vender, J., Grobe, J., Meeker, J., & Hutchison, S. (1). Sex differences in the subjective and reinforcing effects of visual and olfactory cigarette smoke stimuli. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 3(2), Poling, A., & Byrne, T. (Eds.). (). Introduction to Behavioral Pharmacology. Reno, Nevada: Context Press. Raiff, B. R., & Dallery, J. (6). Effects of Acute and Chronic Nicotine on Responses Maintained by Primary and Conditioned Reinforcers in Rats. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 14(3), Raiff, B. R., & Dallery, J. (8). The generality of nicotine as a reinforcer enhancer in rats: effects on responding maintained by primary and conditioned reinforcers and resistance to extinction. Psychopharmacology, 21(2), Rose, J. E. (6). Nicotine and nonnicotine factors in cigarette addiction. Psychopharmacology, 184(3-4), Rose, J. E., Behm, F. M., Westman, E. C., & Bates, J. E. (3). Mecamylamine acutely increases human intravenous nicotine self-administration. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 76(2), Rose, J. E., Behm, F. M., Westman, E. C., Levin, E. D., Stein, R. M., Lane, J. D., et al. (1994). Combined effects of nicotine and mecamylamine in attenuating smoking satisfaction. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 2(4), Rose, J. E., & Corrigall, W. A. (1997). Nicotine self-administration in animals and humans: Similarities and differences. Psychopharmacology, 13(1),

37 Shahan, T. A. (2). The observing-response procedure: A novel method to study drug-associated conditioned reinforcement. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 1(1), 3-9. Shahan, T. A., Bickel, W. K., Badger, G. J., & Giordano, L. A. (1). Sensitivity of nicotine-containing and de-nicotinized cigarette consumption to alternative nondrug reinforcement: A behavioral economic analysis. Behavioural Pharmacology, 12(4), Shahan, T. A., Bickel, W. K., Madden, G. J., & Badger, G. J. (1999). Comparing the reinforcing efficacy of nicotine containing and de-nicotinized cigarettes: a behavioral economic analysis. Psychopharmacology, 147(2), Wannacott, S., Sidhpura, N., & Balfour, D. J. (5). Nicotine: from molecular mechanisms to behaviour. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 5(1), Wyckoff, L. B., Jr. (1952). The role of observing responses in discrimination learning. Part I. Psychological Review, 59(6),

38 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH Jeb Jones received his Bachelor of Science with High Honor in applied psychology from the Georgia Institute of Technology in May of 5. Subsequently he worked at the Marcus Institute, a research and treatment facility for children and adolescents with developmental disabilities and severe behavior problems, eventually being promoted to serve as a primary therapist and assist in the development of treatment and research protocols. He was accepted for admission to the behavior analysis program in the Department of Psychology at the University of Florida for the fall semester of 7 to study under the leadership of Dr. Jesse Dallery. There he conducted the current experiment as well as assisted in numerous other experiments with animals and humans. He earned his Master of Science degree from the University of Florida in the fall of 9. 38

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University Undergraduate Honors Theses 5-2014 Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? Tarra L. Baker Follow

More information

QUANTIFYING NICOTINE S VALUE ENHANCEMENT EFFECT USING A BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC APPROACH

QUANTIFYING NICOTINE S VALUE ENHANCEMENT EFFECT USING A BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC APPROACH QUANTIFYING NICOTINE S VALUE ENHANCEMENT EFFECT USING A BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC APPROACH By RACHEL CASSIDY A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information

Nicotine Enhances Operant Responding for Qualitatively Distinct Reinforcers Under Maintenance and Extinction Conditions

Nicotine Enhances Operant Responding for Qualitatively Distinct Reinforcers Under Maintenance and Extinction Conditions University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 2013 Nicotine Enhances Operant Responding for Qualitatively

More information

A quantitative analysis of the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine using reinforcer demand

A quantitative analysis of the reward-enhancing effects of nicotine using reinforcer demand University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 2012 A quantitative analysis of the reward-enhancing

More information

NICOTINE ENHANCES THE EXPRESSION OF A CONDITIONED PLACE PREFERENCE IN ADULT MALE RATS. Nana Marfo. Submitted to the Undergraduate Faculty of

NICOTINE ENHANCES THE EXPRESSION OF A CONDITIONED PLACE PREFERENCE IN ADULT MALE RATS. Nana Marfo. Submitted to the Undergraduate Faculty of NICOTINE ENHANCES THE EXPRESSION OF A CONDITIONED PLACE PREFERENCE IN ADULT MALE RATS by Nana Marfo Submitted to the Undergraduate Faculty of Kenneth P. Dietrich School of Arts and Sciences in partial

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

EFFECTS OF NICOTINE ON RESPONDING MAINTAINED BY ENVIRONMENTAL STIMULI IN RATS

EFFECTS OF NICOTINE ON RESPONDING MAINTAINED BY ENVIRONMENTAL STIMULI IN RATS EFFECTS OF NICOTINE ON RESPONDING MAINTAINED BY ENVIRONMENTAL STIMULI IN RATS By BETHANY R. RAIFF A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

EFFECTS OF ECONOMY TYPE, REINFORCER MAGNITUDE, AND NICOTINE ON THE ESSENTIAL VALUE OF FOOD IN RATS

EFFECTS OF ECONOMY TYPE, REINFORCER MAGNITUDE, AND NICOTINE ON THE ESSENTIAL VALUE OF FOOD IN RATS EFFECTS OF ECONOMY TYPE, REINFORCER MAGNITUDE, AND NICOTINE ON THE ESSENTIAL VALUE OF FOOD IN RATS By RACHEL CASSIDY A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information

The psychobiology of nicotine dependence

The psychobiology of nicotine dependence Eur Respir Rev 2008; 17: 110, 172 181 DOI: 10.1183/09059180.00011001 CopyrightßERSJ Ltd 2008 The psychobiology of nicotine dependence D.J.K. Balfour ABSTRACT: There is abundant evidence to show that nicotine

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION By JIN HO YOON A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Xue Y-X, Deng J-H, Chen Y-Y, et al. Effect of selective inhibition of reactivated nicotineassociated memories with propranolol on nicotine craving. JAMA Psychiatry. Published

More information

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION INTRODUCTION Epidemiologic reports indicate that mood disorders in children and adolescents are quite common, with up to 70% of depressed children and adolescents experiencing a recurrence within 5 years

More information

COMPLEX INTERACTIONS BETWEEN NICOTINE AND NONPHARMACOLOGICAL STIMULI REVEAL A NOVEL ROLE FOR NICOTINE IN REINFORCEMENT.

COMPLEX INTERACTIONS BETWEEN NICOTINE AND NONPHARMACOLOGICAL STIMULI REVEAL A NOVEL ROLE FOR NICOTINE IN REINFORCEMENT. COMPLEX INTERACTIONS BETWEEN NICOTINE AND NONPHARMACOLOGICAL STIMULI REVEAL A NOVEL ROLE FOR NICOTINE IN REINFORCEMENT by Nadia Chaudhri B.A. in Biological Foundations of Behavior, Franklin & Marshall

More information

Addiction Biology PRECLINICAL STUDY

Addiction Biology PRECLINICAL STUDY Addiction Biology PRECLINICAL STUDY doi:1.1111/j.1369-16.212.436.x Nicotine increases sucrose self-administration and seeking in rats Jeffrey W. Grimm, Christine Ratliff, Kindsey North, Jesse Barnes &

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

HYPERACTIVITY AND SENSITIZATION IN DIFFERENTIALLY REARED RATS ROSEMARY HA. B.S., Kansas State University, 2005 A THESIS MASTER OF SCIENCE

HYPERACTIVITY AND SENSITIZATION IN DIFFERENTIALLY REARED RATS ROSEMARY HA. B.S., Kansas State University, 2005 A THESIS MASTER OF SCIENCE EFFECTS OF MECAMYLAMINE ON NICOTINE-INDUCED CONDITIONED HYPERACTIVITY AND SENSITIZATION IN DIFFERENTIALLY REARED RATS by ROSEMARY HA B.S., Kansas State University, 2005 A THESIS Submitted in partial fulfillment

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Examining the Effects of Reinforcement Context on Relapse of Observing

Examining the Effects of Reinforcement Context on Relapse of Observing Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2011 Examining the Effects of Reinforcement Context on Relapse of Observing Eric A. Thrailkill Utah State

More information

Recent Advances in Energy, Environment, Biology and Ecology

Recent Advances in Energy, Environment, Biology and Ecology Acute and long-term effects elicited by psychoactive drugs on 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats: development of a new experimental tool for the study of drug-mediated reward NICOLA SIMOLA Department

More information

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson, H Djafari Marbini, AJ Toner, TW Robbins, BJ Everitt Departments of

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

A Quantitative Analysis of the Value-Enhancing Effects of Nicotine, Bupropion, and Varenicline in Male and Female Rats

A Quantitative Analysis of the Value-Enhancing Effects of Nicotine, Bupropion, and Varenicline in Male and Female Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Theses, Dissertations, and Student Research: Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of Fall 12-4-2014 A Quantitative

More information

EFFECTS OF BUPROPION ON NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION AND FOOD- MAINTAINED RESPONDING IN RATS. Dustin J. Stairs

EFFECTS OF BUPROPION ON NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION AND FOOD- MAINTAINED RESPONDING IN RATS. Dustin J. Stairs EFFECTS OF BUPROPION ON NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION AND FOOD- MAINTAINED RESPONDING IN RATS Dustin J. Stairs A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina at Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment

More information

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 11, 79-91 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2010) 79 Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats Rogelio Escobar National Autonomous University of Mexico

More information

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 42, No. 4, 401 414 2329-8456/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000112 Contextual

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN DELAY DISCOUNTING AND NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION IN RATS. Maggie M. Sweitzer. B.A., University of Akron, 2005

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN DELAY DISCOUNTING AND NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION IN RATS. Maggie M. Sweitzer. B.A., University of Akron, 2005 INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN DELAY DISCOUNTING AND NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION IN RATS by Maggie M. Sweitzer B.A., University of Akron, 2005 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Arts and Sciences in partial

More information

Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK),

Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK), Supplemental Material: Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK), weighing between 280 300 g at the beginning of the experiment, were housed singly in holding rooms

More information

by popular demand: Addiction II

by popular demand: Addiction II by popular demand: Addiction II PSY/NEU338: Animal learning and decision making: Psychological, computational and neural perspectives drug addiction huge and diverse field of research (many different drugs)

More information

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-009 The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Wesley Paul Thomas Utah

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D.

DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D. DAVID N. KEARNS, PH.D. Psychology Department American University Washington, DC 20016 Phone: 202-885-1725 Email: kearns@american.edu EDUCATION 2005 American University Washington, DC Ph.D., Psychology

More information

The interoceptive Pavlovian stimulus effects of caffeine

The interoceptive Pavlovian stimulus effects of caffeine University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of April 2007 The interoceptive Pavlovian stimulus

More information

Serotonin System May Have Potential as a Target for Cocaine Medications

Serotonin System May Have Potential as a Target for Cocaine Medications NIDA - Publications - NIDA Notes - Vol. 21, No. 3 - Research Findings of 4 http://www.drugabuse.gov/nida_notes/nnvol21n3/serotonin.html 9/26/2011 3:45 PM NIDA NEWS NIDA Home > Publications > NIDA Notes

More information

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 00, 79, 6 NUMBER (MARCH) RESPONDING FOR SUCROSE AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: EFFECTS OF SUCROSE CONCENTRATION AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCER DURATION

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

Extending the role of associative learning processes in nicotine addiction.

Extending the role of associative learning processes in nicotine addiction. University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of September 2004 Extending the role of associative

More information

The Neurobiology of Addiction

The Neurobiology of Addiction The Neurobiology of Addiction Jodi Gilman, Ph.D. Center for Addiction Medicine Massachusetts General Hospital Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry Harvard Medical School What is Addiction? commonly

More information

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE By ANTHONY L. DEFULIO A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE

More information

Management of Tobacco Dependence. Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur

Management of Tobacco Dependence. Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur Management of Tobacco Dependence Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur Difficult to identify any other condition that presents such a mix of lethality, prevalence,

More information

CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY

CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY By ANDREW LAWRENCE SAMAHA A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez The Psychological Record, 28, 58, 57 528 Some Effects of Magnitude of Reinforcement on Persistence of Responding Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman The University of Minnesota Angel Jimenez The University

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

DIFFERENTIATING THE PRIMARY REINFORCING AND REINFORCEMENT-ENHANCING EFFECTS OF VARENICLINE IN A TWO-LEVER, RAT SELF-ADMINISTRATION PARADIGM

DIFFERENTIATING THE PRIMARY REINFORCING AND REINFORCEMENT-ENHANCING EFFECTS OF VARENICLINE IN A TWO-LEVER, RAT SELF-ADMINISTRATION PARADIGM DIFFERENTIATING THE PRIMARY REINFORCING AND REINFORCEMENT-ENHANCING EFFECTS OF VARENICLINE IN A TWO-LEVER, RAT SELF-ADMINISTRATION PARADIGM by Melissa Levin BS, University of Pittsburgh, 2009 Submitted

More information

Dopaminergic Signaling Mediates the Motivational Response Underlying the Opponent Process to Chronic but Not Acute Nicotine

Dopaminergic Signaling Mediates the Motivational Response Underlying the Opponent Process to Chronic but Not Acute Nicotine (21) 35, 943 954 & 21 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 893-133X/1 $32. www.neuropsychopharmacology.org Dopaminergic Signaling Mediates the Motivational Response Underlying the Opponent Process

More information

Behavioral and neuropharmacological characterization of nicotine as a conditional stimulus

Behavioral and neuropharmacological characterization of nicotine as a conditional stimulus University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of April 2007 Behavioral and neuropharmacological

More information

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition A Thesis Presented By Robert Mark Grant In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science

More information

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys PII S0091-3057(99)00058-1 Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, Vol. 64, No. 2, pp. 367 371, 1999 1999 Elsevier Science Inc. Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0091-3057/99/$ see front matter Effects

More information

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism A Thesis Presented by Melissa A. Ezold The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial fulfillment

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Repeated stress exposure causes strain-dependent shifts in the behavioral economics of cocaine in rats

Repeated stress exposure causes strain-dependent shifts in the behavioral economics of cocaine in rats bs_bs_banneraddiction Biology PRECLINICAL STUDY doi:10.1111/adb.12123 Repeated stress exposure causes strain-dependent shifts in the behavioral economics of cocaine in rats Peter A. Groblewski 1, Chad

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (4),380-384 Copyright 1978 by The Psychonornic Society, nc. Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information BRUCE A. WALD Utah State University, Logan,

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques C O N F I D E N T I A L Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques DATE This study was conducted under the terms of a Materials Transfer and Services Agreement between NeuroDetective International and

More information

ASSOCIATED WITH NALORPHINE IN

ASSOCIATED WITH NALORPHINE IN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION BY A STIMULUS ASSOCIATED WITH NALORPHINE IN MORPHINE-DEPENDENT MONKEYS1 STEVEN R. GOLDBERG AND CHARLES R. SCHUSTER UNIVERSITY OF

More information

Update on Medications for Tobacco Cessation

Update on Medications for Tobacco Cessation Update on Medications for Tobacco Cessation Marc Fishman MD Johns Hopkins University Dept of Psychiatry Maryland Treatment Centers Baltimore MD MDQuit Best Practices Conference Jan 2013 Nicotine Addiction

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Learn Behav. 2009 November ; 37(4): 357 364. doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.357. Behavioral Momentum and Relapse of Extinguished Operant Responding

More information

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING JOURNL OF THE EXPERMENTL NLYSS OF BEHVOR 1979, 32, 121-127 NUMBER (JULY) CQUSTON OF STMULUS CONTROL WHLE NTRODUCNG NEW STMUL N FDNG LNNY FELDS THE COLLEGE OF STTEN SLND fter establishing a discrimination

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuropharmacology. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 September 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuropharmacology. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 September 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Neuropharmacology. 2011 September ; 61(4): 687 698. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.05.012. Intravenous nicotine self-administration

More information

Behavioral Pharmacology

Behavioral Pharmacology Psych 181: Dr. Anagnostaras Lecture 4 Behavioral Pharmacology Behavioral Pharmacology Behavioral Pharmacology The study of the relationship between the physiological actions of drugs and their effects

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 8-2012 Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Meredith

More information

Actigraphy-based sleep parameters during the reinstatement of methamphetamine self-administration in rhesus monkeys.

Actigraphy-based sleep parameters during the reinstatement of methamphetamine self-administration in rhesus monkeys. Actigraphy-based sleep parameters during the reinstatement of methamphetamine self-administration in rhesus monkeys. Laís F. Berro, Emory University Monica L. Andersen, Universidade Federal de São Paulo

More information

CHAPTER 27: BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT OF DRUG ADDICTION

CHAPTER 27: BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT OF DRUG ADDICTION CHAPTER 27: BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT OF DRUG ADDICTION Fisher ch.27 Drug addiction is a very prevalent issue in the current United States society. The substance abuse treatments that are available

More information

Interoceptive conditioning in rats: Effects of using a single training dose or a set of 5 different doses of nicotine

Interoceptive conditioning in rats: Effects of using a single training dose or a set of 5 different doses of nicotine University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 2013 Interoceptive conditioning in rats: Effects

More information

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience Dopamine and Learning Michael Dorris Associate Professor of Physiology & Neuroscience Studies dorrism@biomed.queensu.ca http://brain.phgy.queensu.ca/dorrislab/ NSCI 324 Systems

More information

Do smokers self-administer pure nicotine? A review of the evidence

Do smokers self-administer pure nicotine? A review of the evidence Psychopharmacology (2004) 173: 18 26 DOI 10.1007/s00213-004-1781-2 REVIEW Reuven Dar. Hanan Frenk Do smokers self-administer pure nicotine? A review of the evidence Received: 24 July 2003 / Accepted: 7

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5983/1288/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Induction of Fear Extinction with Hippocampal-Infralimbic BDNF Jamie Peters, Laura M. Dieppa-Perea, Loyda M. Melendez,

More information

A HISTORY OF HIGH DOSE NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION INCREASES THE RATE OF SELF-ADMINISTRATION AT LOW NICOTINE DOSES. Tracy T.

A HISTORY OF HIGH DOSE NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION INCREASES THE RATE OF SELF-ADMINISTRATION AT LOW NICOTINE DOSES. Tracy T. A HISTORY OF HIGH DOSE NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION INCREASES THE RATE OF SELF-ADMINISTRATION AT LOW NICOTINE DOSES by Tracy T. Smith B.S., College of Charleston, 2008 M.A., University of North Carolina

More information

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, 2017 The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain David Reyher, MSW, CAADC Behavioral Health Program Director Alvernia University Defining Addiction

More information

NICOTINE DELIVERY-ISSUES AND TECHNOLOGY. Jed E. Rose, Ph.D. Center for Nicotine and Smoking Cessation Research Duke University Medical Center

NICOTINE DELIVERY-ISSUES AND TECHNOLOGY. Jed E. Rose, Ph.D. Center for Nicotine and Smoking Cessation Research Duke University Medical Center NICOTINE DELIVERY-ISSUES AND TECHNOLOGY Jed E. Rose, Ph.D. Center for Nicotine and Smoking Cessation Research Duke University Medical Center 1. To what degree is habit maintenance reinforced by the nicotine

More information

The Incentive Amplifying Effects of Nicotine Are Reduced by Selective and Non-Selective Dopamine Antagonists in Rats

The Incentive Amplifying Effects of Nicotine Are Reduced by Selective and Non-Selective Dopamine Antagonists in Rats East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University ETSU Faculty Works Faculty Works 11-1-2014 The Incentive Amplifying Effects of Nicotine Are Reduced by Selective and Non-Selective

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2015, 103, 269 287 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG

More information

If you give any person a prescription of something like Valium and have them take it on

If you give any person a prescription of something like Valium and have them take it on As always I am happy to do this presentation, which is my favorite topic in addiction medicine. I am an internist, and I have done healthcare for the homeless in Springfield as well as been the medical

More information

VASILE HEFCO 1*, LUCIAN HRITCU 1, ADRIAN TIRON 1, ANDREEA-IOANA HEFCO 1

VASILE HEFCO 1*, LUCIAN HRITCU 1, ADRIAN TIRON 1, ANDREEA-IOANA HEFCO 1 THE EFFECTS OF NICOTINIC TREATMENT ON MEMORY AND LEARNING IMPAIRMENT INDUCED BY BLOCKADE OF MUSCARINIC ACETYLCHOLINE RECEPTORS ON PERFORMANCE IN RADIAL ARM-MAZE TASK IN RATS VASILE HEFCO, LUCIAN HRITCU,

More information

Any use of the contents of this presentation is authorized, on the condition of informing the author about it and quoting the source.

Any use of the contents of this presentation is authorized, on the condition of informing the author about it and quoting the source. Any use of the contents of this presentation is authorized, on the condition of informing the author about it and quoting the source. Toute utilisation du contenu de cette présentation est autorisée, à

More information

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A. Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing Social Consequences as Reinforcers A Thesis Presented by Hilary A. Gibson The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational

More information

NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION IN ADOLESCENT MALE AND FEMALE RATS. Rachel L. Schassburger. B.S. in Neuroscience, Bucknell University, 2009

NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION IN ADOLESCENT MALE AND FEMALE RATS. Rachel L. Schassburger. B.S. in Neuroscience, Bucknell University, 2009 NICOTINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION IN ADOLESCENT MALE AND FEMALE RATS by Rachel L. Schassburger B.S. in Neuroscience, Bucknell University, 2009 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Kenneth P. Dietrich School

More information

The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties

The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties Paul J. Meyer*, Elizabeth S. Cogan, Terry E. Robinson Department of Psychology, University

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

Facilitation by drug states does not depend on acquired excitatory strength

Facilitation by drug states does not depend on acquired excitatory strength University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of January 2007 Facilitation by drug states does not

More information

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010 Notes Textbook used Week 1-3: Bouton, M.E. (2016). Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis. 2nd Ed. Sinauer Week 4-6: Rieger, E. (Ed.) (2014) Abnormal Psychology:

More information

Still at the Choice-Point

Still at the Choice-Point Still at the Choice-Point Action Selection and Initiation in Instrumental Conditioning BERNARD W. BALLEINE AND SEAN B. OSTLUND Department of Psychology and the Brain Research Institute, University of California,

More information

TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND MARC N.

TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND MARC N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2008, 90, 207 218 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND

More information