INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION"

Transcription

1 INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION By JIN HO YOON A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2003

2 Copyright 2003 by Jin Ho Yoon

3 This document is dedicated to my grandfather.

4 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I thank my family and friends who have supported me all this time. iv

5 TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS... iv LIST OF TABLES... vii LIST OF FIGURES... viii ABSTRACT... ix CHAPTER INTRODUCTION... 2 EXPERIMENT...7 Methods...7 Subjects...7 Apparatus...7 Hopper and Keypecking Training...8 FR Shaping...9 Baseline...0 Drugs and Drug Administration Procedure... Acute Dosing (Acute)... Chronic Variable-Dosing with Same Unit Price (SUP)...2 Results...3 Dose-Response Functions...3 Unit Price...5 Discussion EXPERIMENT Method...24 Subjects and Apparatus...24 Chronic Variable-Dosing with Different Unit Price (DUP)...24 Chronic Variable-Dosing with Same Unit Price (SUP2)...24 Results...25 Dose-Response Functions...25 Unit Price...26 Discussion...26 v

6 4 EXPERIMENT Method...32 Subjects nd Apparatus...32 Chronic Fixed-Dosing with Same Unit-Price (FIX)...32 Chronic Fixed-Dosing with Same Unit-Price and Lower Chronic Dose (FIX2) 32 Results...33 Dose-Response Functions...33 Unit Price...34 Discussion GENERAL DISCUSSION...38 LIST OF REFERENCES...4 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH...43 vi

7 LIST OF TABLES Table page. Acute administrations Chronic variable dosing ED50 values...22 vii

8 LIST OF FIGURES Figure page. Dose-response functions Experiment Dose-response functions as a proportion of saline Experiment Unit price Experiment Dose-response functions as a proportion of saline Experiment Dose-response functions as a proportion of saline Experiment Unit price Experiment Dose-response functions as a proportin of saline Experiment Unit price Experiment viii

9 Abstract of Thesis Presented to the Graduate School of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION Chair: Marc N. Branch Major Department: Psychology By Jin Ho Yoon May 2003 Three experiments examined effects of repeated administration of cocaine to pigeons. The pigeons were trained to key peck under a three-component multiple schedule of food presentation according to which either 0, 30, or pecks were required for each delivery of food. In Experiment, time of access to food was correlated with the number of pecks required so that unit price (pecks per second of access to food) was equated. After assessing effects of a range of doses of cocaine administered once per week, drug administration occurred daily before each session, with the dose varying from day to day. Tolerance, the magnitude of which was unrelated to the peck requirement, developed under the repeated-dosing regimen. In Experiment 2, daily drug administration continued using the variable-dose regimen, but the amount of food presented each time was fixed, yielding different unit prices under the three pecking requirements. Subsequently, the conditions of Experiment were reinstated, i.e., unit price was equated. Making unit price different and then the same again had little ix

10 influence on effects of cocaine. In Experiment 3, a fixed dose of cocaine was administered before each session. Under these conditions, tolerance became peckrequirement related. Tolerance was most prevalent under the smaller requirements and less robust or absent when the largest requirement was in effect. Differences in unit price, therefore, were not related to degree of tolerance, but work requirement was. Differences in effects of cocaine across responses requirements, however, were observed only when each session was preceded by the same dose, not when dose varied from session to session. x

11 CHAPTER INTRODUCTION Descriptively, tolerance refers to an attenuation of the initial effects of a drug, following repeated or prolonged exposure, such that increased dose is required to recapture the initial effect (Corfield-Sumner & Stolerman, 978; Wolgin, 989; Hardman, Gilman, & Limbird, 995). The term tolerance may also be used mechanistically, and several mechanisms have been suggested. For example, one suggestion has been that tolerance is mediated through some form of behavioral compensation (Demellweek & Goudie, 983; Wolgin, 989). This line of interpretation has emanated largely from research that follows upon a method introduced by Chen (968). His study described a useful procedure for assessing the potential role of instrumental learning in the development of tolerance, and the procedure suggested a behavioral mechanism of tolerance. Two groups of rats received daily administrations of alcohol. Subjects received reinforcement (i.e., bits of spaghetti) for completing a twoturn circular-maze task. The Before Group received drug prior to each session while the After Group received drug following the session. Disruption in maze running was observed in the Before Group when the rats first received the drug. No effect of receiving repeated administrations of post-session drug was observed in the After Group. Tolerance, as demonstrated by a return to baseline levels of reinforcement following several exposures to alcohol and the testing situation, was observed to the initial disruptive effects of pre-session drug in the Before Group. Once tolerance in the Before Group was observed, the After Group began receiving drug prior to sessions for the first

12 2 time while the Before Group continued to receive drug immediately prior to sessions. Subjects in the After Group showed no tolerance when this regimen began. If the development of tolerance was solely dependent on drug exposure alone, performance in the After Group should have been indistinguishable from that of the Before Group since both groups had received an equal number of administrations of the drug. Instead, performance in the After Group resembled that of the Before Group receiving pre-session drug for the first time. The development of tolerance therefore depended upon the time of drug administration in relation to the behavioral task and not simply drug exposure alone. This type of tolerance has been described as contingent tolerance (Carlton & Wolgin, 97). Differences in performance between the two groups were attributed to differences in behavioral experience. One plausible interpretation of contingent tolerance is that the initial loss of reinforcement due to drug administration promoted the Before Group to engage in some form of behavioral compensation, whereas animals in the After Group experienced no loss of reinforcement during the period of post-session administration and had no opportunity to develop behavior that would compensate for the loss occasioned by pre-session administration. The role of reinforcement loss in the development of tolerance was first suggested in an experiment conducted by Schuster, Dockens, and Woods (966). Three rats were used as subjects. Subjects received administrations of amphetamine immediately prior to exposure to a two-component multiple schedule. One component provided differential reinforcement for low rates of responding (DRL) while the other delivered reinforcement on a fixed-interval (FI) schedule. Effective doses of amphetamine resulted in rate increases in both components for two rats, which resulted in decreased rates of

13 3 reinforcement in the DRL component. Decreases in rate across both components were observed for the third subject, which resulted in loss of reinforcement in the FI component. After repeated administrations of mg/kg of drug, tolerance to the initial effects of amphetamine was observed for all subjects only in the components in which an initial loss of reinforcement had been observed. Similar effects have been observed in a wide variety of experiments (see Corfield-Sumner & Stolerman, 978 and Wolgin, 989 for reviews). The view that tolerance to the behavioral effects of drug depends on the initial effect of drug on reinforcement received has been coined as the reinforcementloss hypothesis (Corfield-Sumner & Stolerman, 978). Some qualifications for the development of differential tolerance according to the reinforcement-loss hypothesis were suggested in a study conducted by Smith (986a). Smith investigated the effects of amphetamine on responding in rats on a multiple random-ratio (RR) DRL schedule of reinforcement. Initially, amphetamine caused decreased rates of responding in the RR component and increased rates of responding in the DRL component, resulting in loss of reinforcement in both components. Tolerance during repeated exposure to amphetamine was observed only in the RR component. Tolerance, however, did develop in the DRL component when it was presented alone, and that tolerance subsequently disappeared when the RR component was reintroduced. The results suggest that tolerance in a situation can depend on the context in which that situation appears. Specifically, the author noted that tolerance could be influenced by global reinforcement rates. The loss of reinforcement in the DRL schedule might have been relatively insignificant when compared to the overall rate of reinforcement when the

14 4 RR schedule was present, as opposed to when the DRL schedule was the only one available. An example of differential tolerance that depended on reinforcement-schedule type was demonstrated in research conducted by Hoffman, Branch, and Sizemore (987). Pigeons were placed on a three-component multiple fixed-ratio (FR) schedule of reinforcement. Pigeons were exposed to a multiple FR 5 FR 25 and either FR 25 or 50. Initial exposure to various doses of pre-session cocaine resulted in dose-dependent decreases in rates of responding across all three components. After chronic administration of pre-session cocaine, dose-response functions were reassessed. Generally, more tolerance to the initial rate-decreasing effects of cocaine was observed in the small and medium sized FR components, and little if any tolerance was observed in the large FR component. Other research has also identified parameter-specific tolerance in the context of multiple FR schedules (e.g., Hughes & Branch, 99; Nickel, Alling, Kleiner, & Poling, 993). In these studies, as well as that of Hoffman et al., less tolerance was observed in the largest FR component, which had a lower rate of reinforcement when compared to that of the small and medium FR components. Thus, one possible interpretation is that the development of tolerance is influenced by rate of reinforcement. A view of tolerance based on relative baseline rates of reinforcement, however, cannot explain the results of a follow-up experiment to the Hoffman et al. study conducted by Schama and Branch (989). Pigeons were exposed to a multiple fixedinterval (FI) schedule of food reinforcement. The FI values (i.e., 5s, 30s, and 20s) approximated the average inter-reinforcement times observed in the Hoffman et al.

15 5 experiment. Dose-dependent decreases in rates of responding to the initial administrations of pre-session cocaine were observed for all subjects across the three FI components, a result similar to that seen by Hoffman et al. Unlike the Hoffman et al. study, however, similar levels of tolerance to the initial rate-reducing effects of presession cocaine for a given subject were observed across the different components after chronic administration of pre-session cocaine. That is, tolerance did not depend on the schedule parameter, and therefore not on reinforcement rate. Schama and Branch suggested an alternative possibility. One difference between the Schama and Branch study and the Hoffman et al. experiment was the number of required responses for reinforcement to be delivered. In an FR schedule, the number of responses required for reinforcement delivered is simply the ratio value. In an FI schedule, however, only response is required for reinforcement to be delivered. Schama and Branch s suggestion that response requirement might be important in producing schedule dependent tolerance points to a more thorough analysis of response requirement. One method that has been utilized to quantify the relationship between response requirement and reinforcement is the concept of unit price, a term borrowed from micro-economics (see Bickel, Green, & Vuchinich, 995; DeGrandpre, Bickel, Hughes, Laying, & Badger, 993 for reviews). Unit price refers to the cost-benefit ratio, which can be interpreted as the response effort divided by the reinforcement magnitude. From a unit-price perspective, the various FI components in the Schama and Branch study all had the same unit price. Unit price in the Hoffman et al. experiment, however, was different for each component; since the same amount of reinforcement was delivered regardless of the size of the ratio completed, unit price increased with FR size. A

16 6 possible account of the differences in outcomes of the two studies therefore is that they reflect the effects of differences in unit price. The purpose of the current experiment was to investigate explicitly the relation between unit price and parameter-specific tolerance under FR schedules.

17 CHAPTER 2 EXPERIMENT Methods Subjects Six adult, male White Carneau pigeons maintained at 80% of their free-feeding weights served as subjects. All subjects were both experimentally and drug naïve prior to the start of the experiment. Between experimental sessions, the pigeons were individually housed in a temperature-controlled colony room with a 6:8 hr light/dark cycle. While in the home cages, pigeons had continuous access to vitamin-enriched water and health grit. Apparatus Sessions were conducted in an operant-conditioning chamber for pigeons. The inside of the chamber was 30 cm wide, 36 cm tall, and 35.5 cm deep. The ceiling and walls were painted white except for the work panel, which was brushed aluminum. Chamber illumination was provided by a.-w, 28-VDC lamp (houselight) that was horizontally centered on the work panel and located 2 cm below the chamber ceiling. An aluminum shield below the bulb deflected light towards the ceiling. In addition to the houselight, the work panel was equipped with 3 circular, horizontally aligned response keys and an aperture through which food could be made available. Each response key was 2.5 cm in diameter and located 8.5 cm from the chamber ceiling. Each response key could be transilluminated. Only the center response key was used, and it could be transilluminated yellow, green, or red by.-w, 28-VDC lamps. A static force of 0. N 7

18 8 registered as a response. Responses on the illuminated center response key resulted in a 30-ms tone. Responses were recorded with a Gerbrands Model C-3 cumulative-response recorder, which provided continuous recording of responses. The cumulative recorder was paused during grain deliveries. Mixed grain was made available via a food aperture that was located below the center key and cm from the chamber floor. The food aperture was 6 cm wide and 5 cm tall. When grain was available, the houselight and keylight were off, and the food aperture was illuminated by a.-w, 28-VDC lamp. Head-in-hopper time was measured with a MED Associates Single Channel I/R Source, Detector, and Control. The device generated an infrared beam across the opening of the food aperature. Entries and exits into and out of the food aperture were detected by breaks in the photobeam. A steel mesh covered the chamber floor, and a ventilation fan, located on the back wall, operated during the entire session. White noise of approximately 95 db was also continuously present in the room that housed the experimental chamber. A half-silvered glass, 2 cm by 24 cm, in the door of the chamber allowed observation of the pigeon. Events were controlled and data collected by a dedicated computer system (Walter & Palya, 984). Hopper and Keypecking Training Pigeons were initially trained to eat from the food hopper. Hopper training was completed in a single session for each subject. A modified autoshaping procedure (Brown & Jenkins, 968) was then implemented in order to establish pecking on the white response key. Sessions were preceded by a 5-min blackout during which all lights in the chamber were turned off and no programmed contingencies were in effect. Sessions began with all lights in the chamber turned off. Access to grain was presented on a fixed-time (FT) -min schedule. Each 3-s hopper presentation was preceded by an 8

19 9 s illumination of the response key by yellow and the houselight. If a peck did not occur on the illuminated keylight, the session was concluded after 30 hopper presentations on the FT -min schedule. All hopper presentations were accompanied by illumination of the hopper aperture and all other chamber lights were extinguished. If a peck occurred on the illuminated response key, 3 s of access to grain was presented immediately. Subsequently, the houselight and keylight were turned on, and food was presented on a fixed-ratio (FR) schedule for 50 food presentations. Pecking on the illuminated response key was not observed for two subjects (i.e., 46 and 435) after 8 sessions of the autoshaping procedure. Therefore, shaping via reinforcement of successive approximations (cf. Ferster, 953) was implemented. Pecking was subsequently observed for both of these subjects after session of shaping. FR Shaping All subsequent sessions continued to be preceded by a 5-min blackout and began with illumination of the houselight and response key. Once responding on the yellow response key reliably occurred on an FR schedule of reinforcement for two consecutive sessions, the response requirement was gradually increased both within and across sessions utilizing the following FR values: 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, 9, 2, 5, 20, 25, 30, 35, 40, 50, 60, 70, 80, 90, and. The FR value was increased after 4 consecutive ratios were completed that all satisfied an inter-response time (IRT) criterion. For ratio values less than 50, all IRTs had to be equal to or less than s. For ratio values greater than or equal to 50, the last 30 responses had to satisfy a -s IRT criterion, and all other responses had to terminate an IRT of 30 s or less. Sessions began with the last FR value that had satisfied the IRT criterion in the previous session. Occasionally, smaller FR values were also included for the first several FR values if erratic responding was observed in the

20 0 previous session. Completion of the FR requirement resulted in 3 s access to grain. Sessions were concluded once either 50 reinforcers were delivered or 40 min had elapsed, whichever came first. Baseline Once responding reliably occurred on the FR schedule, designated the largeratio component, two additional components were introduced at the same time. The small-ratio component, signaled by a green key, was increased from FR 4 to FR 0 after one session. The medium-ratio component, signaled by a red key, was introduced as an FR 5 and subsequently increased to FR 5 then FR 30 across daily sessions. Sessions were conducted 7 days a week at approximately the same time each day. Each session was arranged into 4 blocks where a block consisted of one presentation each of the small- FR, medium-fr, and large-fr components. Components were presented randomly without repetition within a block. Components were separated by a 20-s blackout and were concluded once either the FR requirement had been completed or a time limit had elapsed. The time limits for the small, medium, and large components were min, 2 min, and 6 min, respectively. The timer that controlled access to grain started once a pigeon inserted its head into the hopper, as detected by the photobeam. Head-in-hopper time was recorded to measure obtained unit prices. If a pigeon did not approach the hopper when presented, a limited hold was in force. If the pigeon did not insert its head into the hopper within 0 s of a hopper presentation, the hopper was lowered and the inter-component blackout was initiated, after which the next component ensued. Typical latencies to hopper entry were approximately s for all subjects.

21 Completion of the FR 0 resulted in s access to grain, completion of the FR 30 resulted in 4.5 s access to grain, and completion of the FR resulted in 5 s access to grain. Therefore, the unit price was held constant across components at a value of 6.67 pecks/s of access to grain. Baseline continued until at least 30 sessions had been conducted and rates across components appeared stable as judged by visual inspection of daily response rates and cumulative records (36 to 42 days). Drugs and Drug Administration Procedure Cocaine hydrochloride was dissolved in 0.9% sodium chloride (saline) solution. Drug doses were computed as the salt. Injection volumes were ml/kg. Injections were administered immediately prior to sessions via intramuscular injections into the pectoral muscle. When injections occurred daily, the injection site was alternated between the left and right pectoral muscles in order to minimize any potential bruising. The range of doses that a given subject received during Phase 2 was the same as that administered during acute dose administrations. Acute Dosing (Acute) All subjects initially received the same range of cocaine doses (, 3.0, 5.6, and mg/kg). Subjects 46 and 435 later received a lower range of doses (0.3,, 3.0, and 5.6 mg/kg) based on dose-response curves generated with the original doses. Each dose was administered at least twice under this regimen. Further administrations with some doses were conducted as necessary for individual doses whenever it was deemed necessary to obtain a better estimate of the mean effect. Table shows the number of acute-dose administrations for each dose that each subject received during the Acute Phase. All injections were separated by at least 5 days, and sessions conducted the day before drug-test or vehicle-test sessions provided control values.

22 2 Chronic Variable-Dosing with Same Unit Price (SUP) Pre-session injections were administered immediately prior to every session according to a variable-dosing regimen. Branch, Wilhelm, and Pinkston (2000) and Miller and Branch (2002) demonstrated that a chronic variable-dosing regimen produced similar levels of tolerance to the initial disruptive effects of pre-session cocaine on FR performance when compared to a chronic fixed-dosing regimen, so such a regimen was chosen here. Specifically, beginning with saline, various doses of cocaine were administered in descending order of dose magnitude. That is, on successive days, subjects received pre-session injections of,, 5.6, 3.0, and mg/kg of cocaine in that order, and then the order was repeated. Subjects 46 and 435 received, 5.6, 3.0,, and 0.3 mg/kg of cocaine. Doses were administered in a fixed as opposed to random order so that order effects, should they exist, could better be observed (cf. Sidman, 960). Additionally, Miller and Branch (2002) showed similar degrees of tolerance across three groups of pigeons that were exposed to variable-dosing regimens with different dose orders. Once through the dose-response sequence was defined as a cycle. Thus, a cycle consisted of five sessions. If deviations from the procedure were observed for a particular session (e.g., apparatus failure, incorrect dose administration, etc.), the data from that cycle were neither plotted nor utilized in judging stability. Table 2 shows the number of cycles completed for each subject during Chronic Variable-Dosing. The phase was carried out until at least 5 cycles were conducted, and stable rates across components were observed as judged by visual inspection of plots of response rates for successive dose administrations. The rates for the last 5 successive exposures for each component under each dose were used to judge stability.

23 3 Results Dose-Response Functions Figure shows dose-response functions generated from the Acute Phase and Chronic Variable-Dosing. Unit price was equal across components of the multiple schedule in both phases. Figure 2 shows dose-response curves generated from both phases with rate expressed as a proportion of those observed following saline administrations. For both Figure and Figure 2, means for Acute points represent the average of all acute administrations while means for chronic administration were generated from averaging the results of the last five administrations of each dose. Dose-related decreases in response rate were observed for all subjects during the Acute Phase (black filled circles). Overall, visual inspection of the acute dose-response functions showed no consistent differences in the effects of cocaine across components. Response rates for Subjects 435 and 693 showed greater decreases at some doses in the large-ratio component as compared to the decreases in the small-ratio and medium-ratio, but such differences were not seen in other subjects. In contrast, Subject 46 showed some responding at 3.0 mg/kg in the medium-ratio and large-ratio component while no responding was observed in the small-ratio component after this dose. For the remaining pigeons, dose-response functions were similar across components, with the similarity most clearly evident in the normalized data of Figure 2. A comparison of dose-response curves generated from Acute administrations and repeated variable-dosing shows that tolerance developed to the initial rate-decreasing effects of cocaine for all subjects at the smallest dose that lowered rates beyond the control range acutely. This was true in all 3 components of the multiple schedule for all six pigeons (i.e., in 8 of 8 cases). Tolerance was not observed at the largest individual

24 4 dose administered for any subject except for 693. Overall, even though different degrees of tolerance were observed across subjects, similar levels of tolerance were observed across components for each individual subject. That is, tolerance generally was not related systematically to FR value. For example, effects at the largest dose, one that produced similar decreases in all three components, are comparable across all three ratio values. For Subjects 435 and 693 tolerance was somewhat greater in the larger-ratio component, due mainly to the between-component differences in acute effects. For both these subjects, rates returned toward baseline levels at all doses that produced decreases acutely. A quantitative measure based on the dose-response function is the effective dose (ED50) at which responding is suppressed to 50% of Baseline. The ED50 values were calculated by finding the slope of the descending limb of the normalized dose-response function (Figure 2). The descending portion of the dose-response function began at the last point at which rates were at least 90% of those observed during saline sessions and extended through all higher-dose points until rate was zero or to the highest dose presented if no dose eliminated pecking. If none of the points on the normalized doseresponse function were above 90%, all the points were utilized. The second and third sets of columns of Table 3 show the ED50s from acute administrations and chronic variable-dosing. These data confirm the visual impression that drug effects were not related systematically to FR value. For example, after chronic dosing, ED50 values were higher than the Acute ED50 values across subjects and components in 5 out 8 cases (Table 3). The overall picture, therefore, is that the chronic dosing regimen resulted in modest tolerance that was unrelated to FR parameter.

25 5 Unit Price Figure 3 shows median obtained unit-price values from the sessions that generated the data points in Figures and 2. The unit-price for each component was calculated by dividing the ratio requirement by the median head-in-hopper time for all data points during assessment of acute effects. Medians from the last five sessions at each dose were used to summarize data from the repeated-dosing condition. The median instead of average head-in-hopper time values was utilized to minimize the influence of outliers from a few sessions in which the head-in-hopper time was particularly low. Generally, little or no difference was observed between unit-prices calculated from the median vs. average head-in-hopper time values. Absent data points are due either to reinforcers never being obtained in those conditions or to head-in-hopper time values of 0 seconds when the hopper was presented. Note that the y-axis for Subject 46 is expanded for the medium-ratio and large-ratio components to accommodate very high unit prices at the 5.6 mg/kg dose. Overall, under control conditions and following administration of saline, obtained unit-price was very similar for a given subject across components. A modest tendency for price to be slightly higher in the large-ratio component price was observed but obtained values were still close to the programmed value. Cocaine administration produced small increases in unit price across all three components and did so under both acute and chronic-administration conditions. Occasionally, large doses resulted in very high unit prices as a result of reduced head-in-hopper times, but overall we were successful in equating unit price across components.

26 6 Discussion The major outcome of Experiment was that a modest degree of tolerance to cocaine s effects developed, and tolerance was not related systematically to FR parameter value. Thus, the results of Experiment did not systematically replicate the results of previous experiments investigating tolerance to the effects of cocaine under a multiple FR schedule of food reinforcement (Hoffman et al., 987; Hughes & Branch, 99; Nickel et al., 993). In the current experiment, with unit price equated across components, similar degrees of tolerance were produced across different sized FR values. Previous research on the other hand, with unit price uncontrolled, has shown greater degrees of tolerance in the smaller-ratio components when compared to larger-ratio ones. When differences in tolerance development were observed in the present study, the magnitude of tolerance tended to be greater in the large-ratio component, rather than in the smallratio component, exactly the reverse of the pattern evident in the previous literature. The findings, therefore, are consistent with the view that ratio-parameter-dependent tolerance reported in other studies may have been related to the fact that different unit-prices were arranged by the different schedules of reinforcement. Such a conclusion remains tentative, however, because besides differences in unitprice, the current experimental procedure differed in several other ways from the previous studies. The current procedure was most similar to the Hoffman et al. study so relevant comparisons will focus on these two experiments. The current experiment s FR shaping procedure, combined with the equating of unit-prices, produced shorter pauses in the large-ratio component than reported by Hoffman et al. Table 4 shows the average, minimum, maximum, and standard deviation of the pre-ratio pauses obtained from control sessions during the following acute administrations. For all subjects, the pause

27 7 was short under the smaller two ratios and somewhat longer in the largest ratio. Although Hoffman et al. did not report overall average session pause values, they did provide average pause values obtained from within-session blocks. Pause values they obtained in the FR 5 and FR 25 components were comparable to those observed in the FR 0 and FR 30 components of the current study. The average of the within-session average pause values from the large-ratio component, however, were s and s for the two pigeons that experienced FR 25 and 7.76 s in the subject whose large-ratio was an FR 50. These values are substantially larger than values shown for the FR in Table 5. It may be, therefore, that the differential tolerance reported by Hoffman et al. was due, at least in part, to the fact that the large-ratio component controlled behavior was characterized by relatively long pauses. Whether these differences in pause length in the large-ratio component were due to use of equal unit prices in the present study or to the FR training procedure used here remains to be investigated. Longer pauses result in lower reinforcement rate under ratio schedules, so it is possible that an important difference between the current study and that of Hoffman et al. is the difference in reinforcement rates. From a reinforcement-rate perspective, the shorter large-ratio pauses in the current experiment made the rate of access to food between components more similar, which may have contributed to the comparable levels of tolerance. Table 5 shows average rates of food hopper presentations during control sessions for the current study and those from Hoffman et al. Rates of food presentation for the Hoffman et al. study were calculated by first determining the average of the average rates of responding across blocks then these rates of responding and the FR value for a particular component were used to calculate the average rate of food delivery. The

28 8 left 3 columns show the rates of reinforcer deliveries. Differences across components were larger in the Hoffman et al. study. The differences in baseline responding across components may also have contributed to the modest differences observed in the acute effects of cocaine between the current experiment and the Hoffman et al. study. The dose-response functions generated from acute administrations of cocaine in the Hoffman et al. experiment showed greater dose-related decreases in the larger-ratio components than the smaller-ratio components. Acute dose-response curves generated from the current study were generally similar across components. An additional difference between the procedure of Experiment and that in the Hoffman et al. study was the chronic dosing regimen. Hoffman et al. used a regimen in which the same dose of cocaine was administered prior to every session. In the current study, a variable-dosing regimen was used in which different doses of drug were delivered prior to each session. The potential role of chronic variable-dosing in producing the current results was explored in greater detail in Experiment 3. The results of the current experiment showed similar degrees of tolerance across different sized ratio-values. In an attempt to elucidate potential factors that might have been responsible for the current results, Experiment 2 began with a shift to different unitprice.

29 9 Table. Acute administrations. Number of administrations of each dose during the Acute Phase for each subject. Dose Subject Saline Table 2. Chronic variable dosing. Number of cycles completed for each subject during the chronic dosing phases of Experiment and 2. Phase Subject Exp Equal Unit Price Exp 2 Different Unit Price Exp 2 Equal Unit Price

30 Small Medium Large ACUTE SUP 0 C S C S C S C S C S C S RESPONSES/MIN C S C S C S C S C S C S C S C S C S C S C S C S MG/KG COCAINE Figure. Pecks/minute as a function of dose of cocaine. Data for each subject are presented horizontally while components are presented vertically. Black filled circles represent mean values under conditions of acute administration. Grey filled circles represent mean values for the last five administrations under conditions of chronic variable dosing where access to food was correlated with ratio size (same unit price) and a the chronic dose changed for every session. Points above C show means from control sessions immediately preceding injection sessions. Points above S are means from sessions preceded by saline injections. Vertical bars through control values represent 99% confidence intervals.

31 46 2 Small Medium Large C C C PROPORTION OF SALINE VALUE C C C C C C C ACUTE SUP C C C C C C C C MG/KG COCAINE Figure 2. Dose-response functions as a proportion of values observed during saline administrations for sessions represented in Fig. during acute administrations (black filled circles) and chronic variable dosing administrations with equal unit price (grey filled circles). The vertical bar above C represents 99% confidence intervals as a proportion of saline values. All other details are as in Fig.

32 Table 3. ED50 values. Obtained ED50 values from Experiments, 2, and 3. Phase Exp Acute Exp Chronic Exp 2 Unequal Unit Price Exp 2 Equal Unit Price Exp 3 Subject S M L S M L S M L S M L S M L Mean

33 Small Medium Large C S C S C S UNIT PRICE (PECKS/S OF FOOD ACCESS) C S C S C S C S C S C S C S ACUTE SUP C S C S C S C S C S C S C S C S MG/KG COCAINE Figure 3. Unit price (average component ratio value divided by the average component head-in-hopper time) as a function of dose of cocaine for sessions represented in Fig.. Values in which head-in-hopper time was zero or the ratio requirement was not completed were not used. All other details are as in Fig..

34 CHAPTER 3 EXPERIMENT 2 Method Subjects and Apparatus Subjects and apparatus used in Experiment remained the same in Experiment 2. Chronic Variable-Dosing with Different Unit Price (DUP) Experiment 2 began immediately on completion of Experiment. Its first condition was similar to the variable-dosing condition in Experiment except that completion of each FR requirement resulted in 4.5 s access to grain, regardless of ratio size. Thus, a different unit price was arranged for each component. The same daily variable-dosing regimen utilized in Experiment was continued during Experiment 2. The phase was carried out until at least the same number of cycles completed in the variable-dosing condition of Experiment had been conducted and stable rates were observed as judged by visual inspection of the last 5 exposures for each component under each dose. Chronic Variable-Dosing with Same Unit Price (SUP2) The second condition of Experiment 2 was a direct replication of the variabledosing condition of Experiment and continued until at least 5 cycles were conducted and stable rates were observed as judged by visual inspection. Table 2 shows the number of cycles completed for each subject in the two conditions of Experiment 2. 24

35 25 Results Dose-Response Functions Figure 4 shows dose-response functions generated from the condition with unequal unit prices (white squares) and the subsequent return to equal unit prices (open circles) with rate presented as a proportion of those following saline administrations. Also plotted for comparison are dose-response functions from the acute administrations from Experiment (black circles). When, in the context of daily variable dosing, unit price was made unequal across components, no large changes were observed. The modest tolerance, however that had been evident in Experiment was no longer present in Subjects 405 and 693. During the unequal unit-price condition ED50 values were ordered such that they were the smallest in the large-ratio component and greatest in the small-ratio component for 5 out of 6 subjects as seen in Table 3. Overall, across all components ED50 values obtained with unequal unit price were greater in than those seen with equal unit prices 3 out 8 components across subjects, but in several cases just barely so. When unit-price was equated across components again in the second condition of Experiment 2, few substantial changes were observed, but in 3 of 8 components, the ED50 decreased. Thus, Experiment 2 revealed a modest trend in which tolerance was slightly greater under conditions of unequal unit price. No consistent systematic relations between tolerance and ratio values, however, were observed. Recall that the second phase of Experiment 2 was a direct replication of the second part of Experiment. Figure 5 shows dose-response functions from the variable-dosing condition of Experiment and the identical condition in Experiment 2. Dose-response functions observed during Experiment were generally recaptured during Experiment 2,

36 26 except for Subjects 46 and 693. Subject 46 showed increased tolerance in the mediumratio and large-ratio components in the second exposure to the condition, whereas Subject 693 showed increased sensitization across components. The general tendency for the effects to be similar is validated by examination of the fourth column of Table 3, which shows that in 0 cases, the ED50 increased and in 8 it decreased. Unit Price Figure 6 shows median obtained unit-price values from acute assessments and the two conditions where unit price was equated. Programmed unit-prices were the same for all data points represented by circles and for points in the medium-ratio component when unit prices were different. Programmed unit-price values were different only in the small-ratio and large-ratio in the first condition of Experiment 2 (open squares). Subjects showed similar obtained unit-price values wherever programmed unit-price values were the same. That is, obtained unit prices were different only when they were programmed to be different. Overall, smaller doses of cocaine left obtained unit price unchanged. Larger doses often increased it. Again, unit price was generally controlled as predicted. Discussion The purpose of Experiment 2 was to investigate the effects of altering unit-price during a regimen of daily variable dosing. Overall, when the same amount of food was made available regardless of ratio size, the current results generally failed to replicate the findings observed in the Hoffman et al. (987) experiment. That is, pronounced FRvalue specific tolerance did not develop. In addition to the differences between the Hoffman et al. study and the present one noted in the discussion of Experiment. Another potential explanation for this failure to replicate may be that tolerance, once obtained is difficult to eliminate. That is, repeated

37 27 exposure to cocaine in Experiment, where unit prices were equated, resulted in tolerance in all three components of the multiple schedule, and when unit prices were made different the tolerance that had developed in the FR component carried over to Experiment 2, making FR-value-specific tolerance harder to detect. To the degree that tolerance represents some form of learned compensatory response (Wolgin, 989), one would not necessarily expect the recovered responding in the large-ratio component that had developed in Experiment to disappear with a decrease in reinforcer duration. Although tolerance in the large-ratio component may have been initially driven by the larger reinforcer magnitude in Experiment, responding may well have been maintained with reduced food-duration time as long as that food-duration time still functioned as a reinforcer, which it obviously did. A moderate increase in overall levels of tolerance was observed when unit price was not equal across components when compared to the second conditions of Experiment and Experiment 2, where unit price was equated. One potential contributing factor may have been differences in absolute amount of available reinforcement between conditions. When unit price was equated across components food was available for 84 s in each session. When unit price varied across components in Experiment 2, the total foodaccess time was 54 s. The globally smaller amount of reinforcement may have increased the reinforcing efficacy of food presentations when unit prices varied in Experiment 2 and resulted in moderately greater levels of tolerance. A potential reason for the failure to see schedule-parameter-dependent tolerance when unit priced varied in Experiment 2 was the use of the variable-dosing regimen to assess effects of repeated cocaine exposure. The Hoffman et al. study involved a

38 28 chronic-fixed dosing regimen (i.e., the same dose was given daily). The decision to use a variable-dosing regimen in the present study was based on reports by Branch et al. (2000) and Miller and Branch (2002), who demonstrated that comparable degrees of tolerance were produced by chronic variable-dosing and fixed-dosing regimens. Both of those studies, however, examined an FR 20 schedule of reinforcement. Differences in tolerance with different dosing regimens for larger ratio values, or larger ratio values in the context of smaller ratio values, have yet to be examined. In the variable-dosing regimen we used, administrations of small doses of drug and saline were intermixed with larger ones, and it may have made recovery of responding in the large-ratio component more likely. In an attempt to assess this possibility, Experiment 3 examined effects of changing the dosing routine to a chronic fixed-dosing regimen.

39 46 29 Small Medium Large 435 C C C C C C PROPORTION OF SALINE VALUE C C C C ACUTE SUP2 DUP C C C C C C C C MG/KG COCAINE Figure 4. Pecks/min as a proportion of values observed during saline administrations for acute administrations (black filled circles), chronic variable dosing with unequal unit price (white open squares), and chronic variable dosing with equal unit price (white open circles). During chronic variable dosing with unequal unit price, access to food was the same regardless of ratio size. All other details are as in Fig. 2.

40 46 30 Small Medium Large C C C PROPORTION OF SALINE VALUE C C C C C C C SUP SUP2 C C C C C C C C MG/KG COCAINE Figure 5. Dose-response functions from chronic variable dosing administrations with equal unit price during Experiment (grey filled circles) and Experiment 2 (white filled circles). All other details are as in Fig. 2.

41 Small Medium Large C S C S C S C S C S C S UNIT PRICE (PECKS/S ACCESS OF FOOD) C S C S C S ACUTE SUP SUP2 DUP C S C S C S F FIX C S C S C S C S C S C S MG/KG COCAINE Figure 6. Unit price as a function of dose of cocaine for sessions represented in Fig. 4 and 5 are shown. All other details are as in Fig..

42 CHAPTER 4 EXPERIMENT 3 Method Subjects and Apparatus Subjects and apparatus used in Experiment and 2 remained the same in Experiment 3. Chronic Fixed-Dosing with Same Unit-Price (FIX) Experiment 3 began immediately on completion of Experiment 2. The only change was that subjects were administered 5.6 mg/kg of cocaine immediately prior to every session. Unit price remained equal across components. After at least 50 sessions were conducted and stability in daily response rates were observed, occasional sessions were preceded by different doses of cocaine or saline, with these probe administrations spaced by at least 5 sessions. Each dose was administered at least twice. Data from sessions preceding substitute doses served as representative sessions for 5.6 mg/kg of cocaine. Chronic Fixed-Dosing with Same Unit-Price and Lower Chronic Dose (FIX2) Only Subjects 405 and 693 participated in this condition. This second condition of Experiment 3 was conducted in a manner similar to the first condition, except that subjects received daily administrations of 3.0 mg/kg, rather than 5.6 mg/kg, of cocaine immediately prior to every session. The shift to the smaller dose was made because previous studies have shown that daily administrations of a relatively large dose of chronic cocaine may reduce or eliminate tolerance (Bowen, Faller, & Kallman, 993; 32

43 33 Stafford & Branch, 996; Branch et al. 2000). Pigeons 405 and 693 showed little or no tolerance when 5.6 mg/kg was given daily. The shift to a smaller dose was therefore made to see if 5.6 mg/kg was too large a dose to produce tolerance. After at least 50 sessions had occurred and stability in rates was observed, dose-response functions were assessed. Data from sessions preceding probe doses were used to characterize the effects of 3.0 mg/kg of cocaine. Results Dose-Response Functions Figure 7 shows dose-response functions generated during daily administrations of 5.6 mg/kg (white triangles) and 3.0 mg/kg (white diamonds), where subjects received daily administrations of the same dose of cocaine. The original acute dose-response functions are also represented by black-filled circles for comparison. Subjects 405 and 693, as they had in Experiment 2, exhibited little or no tolerance in any component, either during repeated exposure to 5.6 mg/kg or to 3.0 mg/kg. The remaining four pigeons, however, did show tolerance, and its magnitude was related to the FR value. The differences are obvious for Subjects 46, 435, and 654, where tolerance was much more pronounced at the lower ratios and less so at the largest ratio. These changes are confirmed in the ED50s shown in Table 3. Following the transition from variable dosing at the end of Experiment 2 to fixed dosing in Experiment 3, the ED50s for the small ratio increased substantially, whereas the ED50s for the large ratio either decreased (46 & 654) or changed little (435). Subject 46 also revealed a similar FR-parameter dependent effect. Throughout all the chronic-dosing regimens of Experiments, 2, and 3, this subject had higher ED50s in the larger ratios, but when the shift was made from variable dosing to fixed dosing, the ED50 in the large ratio was decreased by half (see Table 3),

TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND MARC N.

TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND MARC N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2008, 90, 207 218 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2005, 83, 281 296 NUMBER 3(MAY) MORPHINE TOLERANCE AS A FUNCTION OF RATIO SCHEDULE: RESPONSE REQUIREMENT OR UNIT PRICE? CHRISTINE E. HUGHES, STACEY C. SIGMON,

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-009 The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Wesley Paul Thomas Utah

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee The Psychological Record, 1997,47,145-166 THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pigeons served

More information

Behavioral Tolerance under Variable Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement

Behavioral Tolerance under Variable Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 6-27 Behavioral Tolerance under Variable Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement Barry Eshkol Adelman Western Michigan University

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2015, 103, 269 287 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT. Lee Davis Thomas

EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT. Lee Davis Thomas EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT Lee Davis Thomas A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington for Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

More information

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1998, 70, 165 183 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Behavioural Processes 66 (2004) 213 233 Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Raymond C. Pitts, Stacy

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 89 (2012) 212 218 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes j o ur nal homep age : www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Providing a reinforcement history

More information

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER UNIVERSITY OF IOWA 1969, 12, 451-461 NUMBER

More information

Fixed-Ratio Size as a Determinant of the Development of Tolerance to Morphine

Fixed-Ratio Size as a Determinant of the Development of Tolerance to Morphine Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1992 Fixed-Ratio Size as a Determinant of the Development of Tolerance to Morphine Mark J. Nickel Western Michigan University

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (4),380-384 Copyright 1978 by The Psychonornic Society, nc. Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information BRUCE A. WALD Utah State University, Logan,

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

d-amphetamine S EFFECTS ON BEHAVIOR PUNISHED BY TIME-OUT FROM POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT Emily E. Guido

d-amphetamine S EFFECTS ON BEHAVIOR PUNISHED BY TIME-OUT FROM POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT Emily E. Guido d-amphetamine S EFFECTS ON BEHAVIOR PUNISHED BY TIME-OUT FROM POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT Emily E. Guido A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

More information

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 217, 17, 123 135 NUMBER 1 (JANUARY) SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 1

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 261-268 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) CONCURRENT SCHEULES OF PRIMARY AN CONITIONE REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' ONAL W. ZIMMERMAN CARLETON UNIVERSITY Rats responded

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A.

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December

More information

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a Effects of Food Deprivation on Memory Performance. Jacob. L. Kerewaro Abstract The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)

More information

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, & JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR1 1982, 38, 71-85 NUMBER I (JULY) MEMORY FOR RECENT BEHAVIOR IN THE PIGEON STEPHEN P. KRAMER CORRECTIONS DIVISION, DRAPER, UTAH Variations of the symbolic

More information

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1974 Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer Richard H. Weiss Western

More information

REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ATTENUATES APOMORPHINE S EFFECTS ON OPERANT PECKING JONATHAN W. PINKSTON AND R. J. LAMB

REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ATTENUATES APOMORPHINE S EFFECTS ON OPERANT PECKING JONATHAN W. PINKSTON AND R. J. LAMB JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2012, 98, 273 282 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ATTENUATES APOMORPHINE S EFFECTS ON OPERANT PECKING JONATHAN W. PINKSTON AND R. J. LAMB UNIVERSITY

More information

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 5, NUMBF- 4 OCITOBER, 1 962 THE EFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES OF PRIMARY AND CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT JOAN G. STEVENSON1 AND T. W. REESE MOUNT HOLYOKE

More information

SERIAL CONDITIONING AS A FUNCTION OF STIMULUS, RESPONSE, AND TEMPORAL DEPENDENCIES

SERIAL CONDITIONING AS A FUNCTION OF STIMULUS, RESPONSE, AND TEMPORAL DEPENDENCIES University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of January 199 SERIAL CONDITIONING AS A FUNCTION OF

More information

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand).

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). http://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/ Research Commons at the University of Waikato Copyright Statement: The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). The thesis

More information

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior.

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR TECHNIQUES FOR ESTABLISHING SCHEDULES WITH WHEEL RUNNING AS REINFORCEMENT IN RATS IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA 1993, 60, 219-238 NUMBER 1

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING JOURNL OF THE EXPERMENTL NLYSS OF BEHVOR 1979, 32, 121-127 NUMBER (JULY) CQUSTON OF STMULUS CONTROL WHLE NTRODUCNG NEW STMUL N FDNG LNNY FELDS THE COLLEGE OF STTEN SLND fter establishing a discrimination

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Learning and Motivation 39 (2008) 71 94 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Luís Oliveira,

More information

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December 2008 APPROVED:

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

AUTOSHAPING OF KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS

AUTOSHAPING OF KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 521-531 NUMBER 4 (JULY) AUTOSHAPING OF KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS WITH NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT' HOWARD RACHLIN HARVARD UNIVERSITY Pigeons exposed to

More information

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 3-2016 Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Andrew R. Craig Utah State University

More information

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 74-84 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.74 Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure PATRICK VAN ROOYEN AND ANGELO

More information

EFFECTS OF ECONOMY TYPE, REINFORCER MAGNITUDE, AND NICOTINE ON THE ESSENTIAL VALUE OF FOOD IN RATS

EFFECTS OF ECONOMY TYPE, REINFORCER MAGNITUDE, AND NICOTINE ON THE ESSENTIAL VALUE OF FOOD IN RATS EFFECTS OF ECONOMY TYPE, REINFORCER MAGNITUDE, AND NICOTINE ON THE ESSENTIAL VALUE OF FOOD IN RATS By RACHEL CASSIDY A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 93 102 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND

More information

AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS. Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS. Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December 2011 APPROVED:

More information

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 12-1991 Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio

More information

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez The Psychological Record, 28, 58, 57 528 Some Effects of Magnitude of Reinforcement on Persistence of Responding Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman The University of Minnesota Angel Jimenez The University

More information

Instrumental Conditioning I

Instrumental Conditioning I Instrumental Conditioning I Basic Procedures and Processes Instrumental or Operant Conditioning? These terms both refer to learned changes in behavior that occur as a result of the consequences of the

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE By ANTHONY L. DEFULIO A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Learn Behav. 2009 November ; 37(4): 357 364. doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.357. Behavioral Momentum and Relapse of Extinguished Operant Responding

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys PII S0091-3057(99)00058-1 Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, Vol. 64, No. 2, pp. 367 371, 1999 1999 Elsevier Science Inc. Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0091-3057/99/$ see front matter Effects

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 348 370 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Chris Hope and Angelo Santi Wilfrid

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 8-2012 Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Meredith

More information

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction PUNISHMENT A ND RECO VER Y D URING FIXED-RA TIO PERFORMA NCE' NATHAN H. AZRIN2 ANNA STATE HOSPITAL When a reinforcement is delivered according to a fixed-ratio schedule, it has been found that responding

More information

Schedules of Reinforcement

Schedules of Reinforcement Schedules of Reinforcement MACE, PRATT, ZANGRILLO & STEEGE (2011) FISHER, PIAZZA & ROANE CH 4 Rules that describe how will be reinforced are 1. Every response gets SR+ ( ) vs where each response gets 0

More information

THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL

THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2012, 97, 85 100 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem

Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem Animal Learning & Behavior 1981,9 (4),581-586 Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem DAVID R. THOMAS, ALAN R. McKELVIE, MICHAEL RANNEY, and THOMAS B. MOYE University ofcolorado,

More information

Cyclic Responding by Pigeons on the Peak Timing Procedure

Cyclic Responding by Pigeons on the Peak Timing Procedure Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996. Vol. 22, No, 4. 447 460 Copyright [996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. (X)97-7403/96/$3.00 Cyclic Responding by Pigeons

More information

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1987, 47, 57-72 CHOICE BETWEEN RELIABLE AND UNRELIABLE OUTCOMES: MIXED PERCENTAGE-REINFORCEMENT IN CONCURRENT CHAINS MARCIA L. SPETCH AND ROGER DUNN DALHOUSIE

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Demand under Fixed-Ratio Schedules with Domestic Hens. Alan Poling 2

Reinforcer Magnitude and Demand under Fixed-Ratio Schedules with Domestic Hens. Alan Poling 2 Reinforcer Magnitude and Demand under Fixed-Ratio Schedules with Domestic Hens Amber A. Grant 1, T. Mary Foster 1, William Temple 1, Surrey Jackson, 1 Jennifer Kinloch 1 and Alan Poling 2 Affiliation of

More information

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT'

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT' D. ALAN STUBBS AND PHILIP J. SILVERMAN UNIVERSITY OF MAINE, ORONO AND WORCESTER

More information

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL'

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1973, 2, 393-43 NUMBER 3 (NOVEMBER) PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' M. C. DAVISON AND W. TEMPLE UNIVERSITY OF AUCKLAND AND

More information

The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance

The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1972 The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance Deborah Ann Cory Western

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement Learning & Behavior 2005, 33 (3), 309-323 Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement GLENN S. BROWN and K. GEOFFREY WHITE University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand In two experiments, pigeons responding

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Summer 2005 Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli William

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l Journal oj Experimental Psychology 12, Vol. 63, No. 3, 289-293 REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l MAURICE HERSHENSON 2 Brooklyn College In measuring reaction time (RT) to simultaneously

More information

CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY

CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY By ANDREW LAWRENCE SAMAHA A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia AM. ZOOI.OCIST, 12:507-512 (1972). Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia PAUL DOWNEY AND BEHRUS JAHAN-PARWAR Biology Department, Clark University, Worcester, Massachusetts 01610 and Worcester Foundation

More information

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (3), 264-270 Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pullman, Washington 99163 Five pigeons

More information

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 009, 9, 367 377 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) WITHIN-SUBJECT REVERSIBILITY OF DISCRIMINATIVE FUNCTION IN THE COMPOSITE-STIMULUS CONTROL OF BEHAVIOR STANLEY J. WEISS,

More information

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(4),621-629 Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives THOMAS R. ZENTALL University ofkentucky, Lexington,

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching

Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (2), 189 196 Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching THOMAS R. ZENTALL and LOU M. SHERBURNE University of Kentucky, Lexington,

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (1986) 38B, 81-95 Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon P. J. Durlach and N. J. Mackintosh Department of Experimental Psychology, University

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 11, 79-91 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2010) 79 Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats Rogelio Escobar National Autonomous University of Mexico

More information

Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed Through Partial Versus Total Reversals of Many-to-One Conditional and Simple Discriminations

Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed Through Partial Versus Total Reversals of Many-to-One Conditional and Simple Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psycholo Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 2, 194-201 Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/43.00 Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed

More information

Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data

Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 2004, 36 (4), 661 669 Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data PAULO GUILHARDI and RUSSELL M.

More information

Learning and Motivation

Learning and Motivation Learning and Motivation 4 (29) 178 18 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Learning and Motivation journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Context blocking in rat autoshaping: Sign-tracking

More information