ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS"

Transcription

1 ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS August 25 APPROVED: Manish Vaidya, Major Professor Sigrid Glenn, Committee Member Richard Smith, Committee Member Richard Smith, Chair of the Department of Behavior Analysis David W. Hartman, Dean of School of Community Service Sandra L. Terrell, Dean of the Robert B. Toulouse School of Graduate Studies

2 Kangas, Brian D. On the effects of extended sample-observing response requirements on adjusted delay in a titrating delay matching-to-sample procedure with pigeons. Master of Science (Behavior Analysis), August 25, 29 pp., 1 table, 7 illustrations, references, 1 titles. A common procedural variation that facilitates the acquisition of conditional discriminations is to increase the time an organism spends in the presence of the sample stimulus by programming extended sample-observing response requirements. Despite their common use, there has been little empirical investigation of the effects of extended sample-observing response requirements. In the current study, four pigeons worked on a titrating delay matching-to-sample procedure in which the delay between sample offset and comparison onset was adjusted as a function of the pigeons accuracy. The number of responses required to produce the comparison array was manipulated across conditions. Results show that all subjects were able to withstand longer delays between sample offset and comparison onset as sample-observing response requirements increased. These data show that the extent of the response requirement in the presence of the sample has systematic effects on conditional discrimination performances and should be considered in the design of experiments.

3 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to acknowledge Manish Vaidya for outstanding advising both inside and outside of the laboratory. His mentoring the last two years on both scientific and philosophical issues has given me a much richer appreciation of what a natural science approach to psychology could mean. I would also like to thank Sigrid Glenn and Richard Smith for sitting on my defense committee and for comments on a previous draft of this paper. Finally, I would like to thank Yusuke Hayashi for technical programming assistance. ii

4 TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS...ii LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES...iv Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION METHOD RESULTS DISCUSSION REFERENCE LIST iii

5 LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES Page Table 1. Number of sessions to reach acquisition Figures 1. Session by session adjusted delay means Summary means of adjusted delays Mean adjusted delay for last 1 sessions Median sample latencies to first sample observing response Median duration between first and last sample response Median running rate of sample observing responses Percent correct as a function of adjusted delay...28 iv

6 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION In a matching-to-sample procedure (MTS), the selection of a stimulus from an array of stimuli (hereafter, comparison stimuli) is reinforced conditionally upon the presence of another stimulus (hereafter, sample stimulus). Correct matching is determined either by some shared property of the sample and comparison stimuli, as in the case of identity or oddity matching (e.g., Weinstein, 1941), or by an arbitrary relation programmed by the experimenter, as in the case of symbolic or arbitrary matching (e.g., Carter & Eckerman, 1975). To further illustrate, a typical identity MTS trial begins with the presentation of a sample stimulus. A single response to the stimulus produces an array of comparison stimuli. Selection of the identical comparison stimulus produces food while selection of the non-identical comparison produces no programmed consequences. Both human and nonhuman animals typically acquire this performance with high accuracy via traditional trial-and-error contingencies. When they do not, arranging extended sample-observing response requirements seems to facilitate highly accurate performances. A report by Berryman, Cumming, and Nevin (1963) was the first published study to employ extended sample-observing response requirements as a procedural variable. The authors were interested in studying delayed matching in pigeons. The first phase of this study analyzed the acquisition of conditional discriminations using simultaneous, zero-delay, and variable delay MTS procedures. In the simultaneous MTS condition, a single response to the sample key illuminated both side comparison keys while the sample key remained on. In the zero-delay MTS condition, a single response to the 1

7 sample key concurrently turned off the sample key and turned on the side comparison keys. In the variable delay MTS condition, a single response to the sample key turned off the sample key and initiated one of several delays (1, 2, 4, 1, or 24-s) to the onset of the side comparison keys. The results indicate that when these conditions were alternated randomly, none of the seven birds acquired the matching performance. In Phase II of this study, each of the seven birds was exposed to the simultaneous condition only, and the programmed contingencies now required five responses to the sample stimulus before the comparison array could be produced. The authors stated that the extended response requirement was introduced to provide greater exposure to the sample stimulus (Berryman, Cumming, & Nevin, 1963, p. 12). The results indicate that all seven subjects quickly acquired the matching performance. However, noting the similar acquisition rates of the three subjects in a previous study on simultaneous matching employing a single sample response requirement (Cumming & Berryman, 1961), the authors concluded that additional exposure to the sample stimulus provided by the center key fixed-ratio requirement did not appear to facilitate acquisition of simultaneous matching (Berryman, Cumming, & Nevin, 1963, p. 14). In Phase III, three of the birds that participated in the previous phases were again exposed to the randomly alternating conditions described in Phase I. The FR 5 response requirement remained in effect. Results indicated high accuracy in the simultaneous condition, and moderate to low accuracy in the zero-and low delay value conditions, respectively. Performance approximated chance when the delay between sample offset and comparison onset was long. These results indicate that increasing the delay increases the difficulty of MTS tasks (lowers the accuracy). However, because 2

8 performance in the simultaneous MTS condition was already highly accurate at the onset of Phase III, the relative contributions of simultaneous MTS acquisition training and the FR 5 response requirement cannot be parsed with any certainty. Contrary to the conclusion drawn by Berryman et al. (1963), several systematic within-subject investigations have shown that extended sample-observing responses have an effect (e.g., Sacks, Kamil, and Mack, 1972; Roberts, 1972). For example, Sacks, Kamil, and Mack (1972) trained eight pigeons on a zero-delay MTS task similar to that described by Berryman et al. (1963); however, in this study, four different FR response requirements on the sample key were tested. Four groups of two pigeons experienced sample-observing response requirements of 1, 1, 2, or 4. The subjects in each group performed under a zero-delay MTS procedure until a criterion of 85% correct matching or better was maintained for three consecutive sessions. Results indicate that increasing the FR response requirement increased speed of acquisition; the two birds that experienced an FR 1 response requirement took 13 and 27 sessions to reach the 85% criterion, the two birds that experienced an FR 1 response requirement took 7 and 13 sessions, both birds that experienced the FR 2 response requirement took 11 sessions, and both birds that experienced the FR 4 response requirement took 6 sessions. Results from a condition in which delays were inserted between sample offset and comparison onset indicate that delayed matching performances improved with increased FR sample response requirements; the two birds that experienced the FR 1 requirement were performing at chance after 2 s delays, and both birds that experienced the FR 4 requirement were performing at levels above 75% after 8 s delays. Finally, in a condition in which novel hues replaced 3

9 some of the training hues, pigeons that experienced FR 1 or higher response requirements demonstrated above chance matching during the first transfer session. The authors note that transfer had never been observed in their laboratory; however, previous attempts had always employed an FR 1 response requirement. Roberts (1972) also assessed the effects of sample-observing response requirements on performance in a delayed MTS task. Ten pigeons were first trained using a simultaneous MTS procedure similar to that described by Berryman, et al. (1963), but with an FR 3 sample-observing response requirement. After performances exceeded 85% correct for 5 consecutive sessions, the conditions changed to a delayed MTS procedure in which delays of, 1, 3, or 6 s were initiated after the FR 3 observing response requirement was met. After 15 days of delayed MTS training with the FR 3 response requirement, FR response requirements of 1, 5, or 15 were programmed across sessions. Each subject cycled through the series of FR values six times, each time in a different order. Results from the condition in which FR response requirements were manipulated showed significant increases in accuracy across all delays for all subjects as the FR value increased. Despite the results described above, experimenters seem to have employed various extended sample-observing response requirements on the sample key with little or no stated rationale (e.g., Berryman, Cumming, and Nevin, 1963; Cumming and Berryman, 1965; Nordholm, Moore, and Wenger, 1995; Poling, Temple, and Foster, 1996; Dayer, Baron, Light, and Wenger, 2). For example, Poling, Temple, and Foster (1996) examined the differential outcomes effect (DOE) in chickens using a matching procedure with an FR 5 response requirement on the sample with no stated 4

10 rationale. Although the results indicated that when differential reinforcer magnitudes were programmed, the birds were able to perform accurately after longer delays between sample offset and comparison onset, the extended sample-observing response requirement may have interacted with the effects in unknown ways. For example, the extended observing response requirements may have facilitated performance on the problems with non-differential outcomes such that the overall effect of differential outcomes was understated. Similarly, Dayer, Baron, Light, and Wenger (2), examined the effect of ethanol on working memory and attention in the pigeon using an FR 15 response requirement on the sample, again, with no rationale stated. It remains unclear whether the extended sample-observing response requirement had an effect on the derived dose-response curves. For example, the extended sampleobserving response requirement could have served to reduce the effects of drugs in the delayed MTS task, relative to a preparation requiring only one observing response. In another study in the pharmacology literature, Nordholm, Moore, and Wenger (1995) analyzed the effects of the proposed cognitive enhancing agent Linopirdine on six pigeons and four squirrel monkeys. An FR 15 response requirement was programmed for the pigeons and an FR 2 was programmed for the squirrel monkeys. Not only was the extended sample-observing response requirement not discussed, but the basis for inclusion of between-species, within-experiment procedural differences was also not addressed. Again, without parametric data on the effects of extended sample-observing response requirements, arbitrarily programmed requirements may affect the magnitude of dose-response effects and even perhaps the shape of the derived curve. 5

11 The studies cited above illustrate an important point for current purposes: Extended sample-observing response requirements have been employed in a seemingly unsystematic manner despite some evidence that they may systematically influence performance in MTS preparations. In the studies described above, the ratio values varied between-experiments, within-labs (Wenger and colleagues), and even within-experiment (Nordholm et al., 1995). It may be that researchers have not paid much attention to this variable because there are few parametric data relating performance on conditional discriminations to extended sample-observing response requirements. One reason why parametric analyses of the effects of extended sampleobserving response requirements have not been conducted may involve the use of percent of correct trials as a dependent measure. This measure is problematic because performance may quickly reach near-1% accuracy making the facilitative role of extended sample-observing response requirements difficult to ascertain. Measures like trials to acquisition are better in that the data moves over a wider range than percent of correct trials; however, large differences across subjects may obscure the systematic nature of the control exerted by the independent variable. For example, Berryman et al. (1963) found up to a 4-trial difference in trials to acquisition across birds, and Sacks et al. (1972) found a difference of some 14 sessions. A procedure that avoids some of these limitations is delayed MTS. Systematic variation of delays between sample offset and comparison onset allows an analysis of the effects of extended sample-observing response requirements (e.g., Roberts, 1972) by systematically allowing an assessment of the role of sample observing on accuracy 6

12 on trials with varied delays. However, although delayed matching may show effects of extended observing, after the subject learns the task under a delay, performance will be highly accurate. A better procedure, in these regards, is a titrating delay MTS (TDMTS) procedure (Cumming and Berryman, 1965). In a TDMTS procedure, the delay between sample offset and comparison onset is adjusted as a function of the subject s performance. Typically in TDMTS preparations, correct responses increase the delay between sample offset and comparison onset and incorrect responses decrease the delay between sample offset and comparison onset. By adjusting delay values as a function of subjects performances, this preparation takes individual differences into account and also has the potential to detect systematic effects if they exist. The purpose of the present study was to use a TDMTS procedure to examine parametrically the effects of extended sample-observing response requirements on conditional discrimination performance. Evidence that the extent of sample-observing responses has systematic effects on behavior, paired with the continued use of extended sample-observing requirements in the absence of a thorough understanding of how they may interact with other independent variables, suggests that more research is needed. 7

13 CHAPTER 2 METHOD Subjects Four naïve White Carneux pigeons, obtained from Double T Farms, Glenwood, Iowa, were maintained at approximately 8% of their free-feeding weights by postsession feeding as needed. The animals were housed in individual cages, in a temperature- and humidity-controlled vivarium, with exposure to a 12-hour light/dark cycle (lights on at 7: a.m.). Water and grit were available continuously in the birds home cages. Apparatus The experiment was conducted in a standard operant chamber measuring 3 cm high, 8 cm long, and 3 cm deep. The chamber was sound- and light-attenuating with an exhaust fan to provide ventilation and masking noise. One side wall (the intelligence panel) contained a houselight, three horizontally arrayed response keys (2.5 cm in diameter and 1 cm apart from edge to edge) and a 5 cm square opening for access to a hopper (Model #ENV-25M) filled with mixed grain located 2 cm above the floor and centered below the center key. The center key was horizontally centered on the intelligence panel cm above the floor. The two side keys were located 1 cm to the left and right of the center key. Each key could be transilluminated with a variety of colors and geometric forms using Industrial Electronics in-line projectors (IEEE Model #ENV-13M [11-1-OK21-182]). Scheduling of experimental events and data collection were controlled via computer using MED-PC software (Ver. 4., Med Associates, St. Albans, VT). 8

14 Procedure A parametric assessment of the effects of extended sample-observing response requirements (across conditions) on delays between sample offset and comparison onset that adjusted as a function of the subject s performance was conducted. Prior to the experiment, each pigeon was trained to eat from the food hopper and then hand shaped to peck each of the three keys (illuminated white) via differential reinforcement of successive approximations. After the pigeon pecked each of the keys reliably when lit, acquisition training began. Acquisition Training All four pigeons were trained on the MTS task using a simultaneous MTS procedure. Discrete trials began with the illumination of the houselight and the center (sample) key with either a red or yellow hue. A single response to the sample key illuminated the two side (comparison) keys with matching and non-matching hues. A single peck to the side key illuminated with the same color as the sample key (i.e. the correct match) turned off the houselight, the sample key, and both comparison keys, and raised the food hopper for 2 s followed by a 1-s intertrial-interval (ITI). A single peck to the non-matching comparison key (i.e. the incorrect response) turned off all lights in the chamber and initiated a 12-s ITI. The 1-s ITI (plus 2-s hopper access) following a correct match, and 12-s ITI following an incorrect match ensured equivalent ITIs following a correct or incorrect match. A two-color, two-comparison MTS procedure yields 4 possible configurations (RRY, YRR, YYR, RYY). The computer arranged the presentation of these 9

15 configurations on each trial in a quasi-random order. Specifically, each of the four configurations was presented before any configuration could be repeated (i.e. random selection without replacement). All subjects were exposed to daily sessions consisting of 72 trials. After 1 consecutive sessions with 85% or greater accuracy, each subject was exposed to a successive, or zero-delay, MTS procedure. In this condition, a single peck to the center key turned off the sample and simultaneously illuminated both side keys. The consequences for pecking the matching or non-matching key remained the same as before. After 1 consecutive sessions with 85% or greater accuracy, each pigeon was exposed to the TDMTS procedure. Titrating Delay Matching-to-Sample The TDMTS procedure was identical to the zero-delay procedure described above with the exception that the delay between sample stimulus offset and comparison stimuli onset was adjusted as a function of the pigeon s accuracy on immediately preceding trials. Specifically, every 2 consecutive correct matches increased the delay by 1 s, and every incorrect match decreased the delay by 1 s (regardless of trial type). The programmed contingencies of this titrating procedure will eventually hold accuracy around 67% as the delay between sample offset and comparison onset titrates. The first condition began as a zero-delay; thereafter, each daily session began with the delay value from the end of the previous session. Each subject was exposed to an ascending series of sample-observing response requirements (FR 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16) across conditions. For example, in the FR 16 1

16 condition, the 16 th peck on the center key turned off the sample key and initiated the delay interval to comparison onset. After a minimum of 2 sessions, conditions were changed when the mean delay values from the last 1 sessions were all within ± 25% of the mean of the 1-session means. Because more variability in performance was likely to occur at higher delay values, a percentage criterion was used to accommodate greater variability at higher adjusted delays. An absolute number would have made the criterion more stringent as a function of higher adjusted delays. Each subject was exposed to higher sample-observing response requirements until the mean delay value from the last 1 sessions was smaller than or equal to the value obtained in the previous FR value. 11

17 CHAPTER 3 RESULTS All pigeons learned to eat from the hopper and to peck lit keys reliably within 2 half-hour sessions. Table 1 presents the number of sessions in each of the two preliminary training conditions. Subjects 66, 659, 16, and 57 met the criterion after 33, 45, 27, and 31 sessions in the simultaneous and 27, 1, 28, and 4 sessions in the successive MTS training conditions, respectively. Figure 1 presents the mean adjusted delay values produced by 66, 659, 16, and 57 during daily sessions. Data from every session are included in these graphs. For Subject 66, the stable adjusted delay with an FR 1 response requirement was around.8 s. After the stability criterion was met, the FR 2 response requirement was implemented. There was a several session lag before increased variability was observed in adjusted delay values. A gradual increase was eventually observed, with a high peak midway through the condition. Between-session variability decreased in subsequent sessions, and delays eventually reached steady state. Delays in the FR 4 condition again exhibited day-to-day variability for roughly 2 days, but eventually reached stability at a value higher than the previous condition. Increased variability with a clear upward trend was observed in the FR 8 condition before the stability criterion was met. The highest variability observed for this subject was seen during the FR 16 response requirement condition. Following an initial ascending trend, a downward trend was observed, with day-to-day adjusted delay variation often exceeding 1 s. Eventually, decreasing levels of variability, and then steady state performance was observed. Adjusted delays decreased to near-zero levels almost immediately after the 12

18 return to the FR 1 sample-observing response requirement condition. This was followed by daily variability in adjusted delays before stability was reached. Like Subject 66, Subject 659 maintained the adjusted delay at low values during the FR 1 and FR 2 conditions. During the FR 4 condition, the adjusted delay values were slightly but consistently higher than the previous conditions, with variability remaining low. The variability in adjusted delays however, increased at the onset of the FR 8 and FR 16 sample-observing response requirements. A wave-like pattern was observed before adjusted delay values became stable during the FR 8 condition. More dramatic day-to-day variability was observed during the FR 16 condition before stability was observed. Subject 16 also maintained the adjusted delay at low values in the FR 1 condition. These low values persisted several sessions into the FR 2 condition, but an upward trend was eventually observed. This trend was initially highly variable, but eventually reached steady state at a value approximately 3 s higher than in the previous condition. The FR 4 condition was marked by a decrease in daily adjusted delay values for several sessions before an increasing trend that eventually leveled off at steady state. Some of the highest adjusted delay values were observed during the beginning of the FR 8 condition. However, values decreased within 15 sessions to levels similar to but slightly higher than in the previous condition. Subject 57 maintained consistently low adjusted delay values during the FR 1, 2, and 4 sample-observing response requirement conditions. At the onset of the FR 8 condition, higher delay values with increased daily variability were observed. Adjusted delays reached a peak of almost 2 s after about 3 sessions in the condition, but 13

19 steady state was eventually reached at lower values. High day-to-day variability was observed in the FR 16 condition, with the subject ultimately exhibiting the highest adjusted delays observed in this experiment. Figure 2 presents the summary means (bars) and the inter-quartile range (error bars) for the last 1 sessions of each condition for all 4 subjects. The sessions included are those that met the stability criteria described above. For 66, the delay between sample offset and comparison onset settled around.8 s under the FR1 observing response requirement, followed by an increase to almost 5-s delay under the FR 2 observing response requirement. A smaller increase to over 6 s was observed under the FR 4 response requirement, followed by a mean adjusted delay of 8.5 s in the FR 8 observing response requirement. Steady state summary data from the FR 16 condition showed a decrease in the mean adjusted delay (7.77 s). In accord with the rules for cessation of the ascending series, the FR1 condition was reinstated to assess the extent to which sample-observing response requirements alone were responsible for the observed increases in the mean adjusted delay values. As shown in Figure 1, the bird s adjusted delay value was closer to but lower than the FR2 condition and clearly higher than the first exposure to FR1 than the value obtained under the first exposure to this condition at the beginning of the experiment. For Subject 659 the delay between sample offset and comparison onset settled around.8 s under the FR 1 sample-observing response requirement. This was followed by a slightly higher mean adjusted delay (.9 s) during the FR 2 condition. An increase to around 2.3 s was observed in the FR 4 condition. A further increase to almost a 7 s adjusted delay was observed during the FR 8 sample-observing response 14

20 requirement, and the FR 16 condition produced the highest adjusted delays, averaging almost 18 s across the last 1 sessions. As with the previous two subjects, Subject 16 showed low measures of mean adjusted delay (.8 s) in the FR 1 condition. A 4.24 s adjusted delay was observed the FR 2 condition. A 5.37 s mean adjusted delay was observed under the FR 4 condition, and a slight increase to 5.74 s was observed in the FR 8 condition. Subject 57 s mean adjusted delay in the FR 1 condition was slightly lower than the previous 3 subjects, settling around.5 s. Mean adjusted delay in the FR 2 condition was only about.2 s higher, settling around.7 s. FR 4 showed a slightly higher mean adjusted delay of 1 s. Subsequently, an increase to almost 5 s was observed in the FR 8 condition. A very large increase in adjusted delay values was observed in the FR 16 condition, ultimately reaching over 38 s, a value that was over 8 times greater than in the previous condition. Figure 3 presents the individual session means (filled circles) taken from sessions that contributed to the means shown in Figure 2 (i.e., the last 1 sessions in each condition). The error bars indicate each session s inter-quartile range. Open triangles represent data from Subject 66 s FR 1 condition replication. This figure shows that variability in adjusted delay values increased as the sample-observing response requirements increased, suggesting that the choice to assess stability using relative rather than absolute terms was appropriate. Figure 4 presents the mean sample latencies (time between sample onset, and the first peck to the sample) for all 4 subjects when performance was stable. For Subject 66, sample latencies of about 1 s were observed during the FR 1, 2, and 4 15

21 conditions. An increase of about 1 s was observed during the FR 8 condition. A large increase in mean latency variability was observed during the FR 16 condition. The FR 1 condition replication (open triangles) showed slightly higher mean latencies, with a marked increase in between-session variability relative to the first FR 1 condition. Mean sample latencies for Subject 659 were about 1 s for the FR 1, 2, 4 and 8 response requirement conditions. However, during the FR 16 condition, considerably higher mean latencies were observed, ranging from roughly 1-23 s. Mean sample latencies for Subject 16 were consistently between.5 and 1.5 s during the FR 1, 2, 4, and 8 response requirement conditions. Near 1 s mean sample latencies were observed for Subject 57 under the FR 1, 2, 4, and 8 response requirements. However, a large increase in both sample latency and session-to-session variability was observed during the FR 16 condition. Figure 5 presents the median elapsed time (filled circles) between the first and last response to the sample stimulus in the conditions with an extended sampleobserving response requirement for all 4 subjects when performance was stable. Median values were chosen to provide a more representative description. This was necessary because there were a few trials, especially in the higher FR values (8 and 16), where the subject paused for an unusually long time during the ratio. Error bars indicate the inter-quartile range. As with the summary measures of adjusted delay values (Figures 2 and 3), duration between the first and last sample response increased as a function of the FR response requirement. This is not, of course, an unexpected outcome; higher FR ratios typically take longer to complete. However, increases in 16

22 durations are proportionately greater across conditions than the increase in FR sampleobserving response requirements. Figure 6 shows the median running rate for all 4 subjects when performance was stable. A running rate describes the rate of responding between the first and last sample-observing response during their programmed response requirement. The figure suggests that for all 4 subjects, response rate decreased as a function of the FR response requirement. In other words, the birds were pecking slower when FR values were higher. Figure 7 shows the percent of correct trials as a function of adjusted delay values for each condition. Although many of the curves overlap, close inspection of the figure suggests that the smaller FR response requirements (i.e., FR 1, 2, and 4) produced steeper slopes relative to the higher response requirements (i.e., FR 8, and 16). The smaller FR response requirements have fewer data points (shorter curves) because the subject adjusted the delay less and thus were exposed to fewer delay values. 17

23 CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION The results of this study suggest that extended sample-observing response requirements affect adjusted delay values in a titrating MTS procedure. Figures 1, 2, and 3 indicate that, across all four subjects, as the sample-observing response requirement increased, adjusted delay between sample offset and comparison onset also increased. Although the magnitude of this effect varied between subjects, the overall function, which may be described as curvilinear, appears to be similar. The current data suggest that these effects are due to the sample-observing response requirements and not to extended practice. First, although the data from the last 1 sessions in each condition showed clear effects of the sample-observing response requirements, some low values were observed in almost every condition. If extended practice accounted for the obtained results, then a consistent rise in the adjusted delay values would be expected as the subject s performance becomes more and more accurate. Second, a replication of the FR1 condition with one subject showed that the mean adjusted delay value during the second exposure was higher than the original exposure but lower than the adjusted delay value from FR2 suggesting that the sample observing response requirements were important. The data presented in Figures 4, 5, and 6 however, suggest that the time spent in the presence of the sample may have been, at least in part, responsible for the increased adjusted delays. These data show that the time the birds spent in the presence of the sample stimulus was longer than the time necessary to emit the required sample-observing responses. Figure 4 shows that the subjects paused before 18

24 making the first observing response during higher response requirements. Figure 5 shows that durations to complete larger response requirements were proportionally longer than the programmed increases in those requirements. The longer durations are accounted for by slower running rates on higher response requirements, as shown in Figure 6. Moreover, a direct comparison of the elapsed time between first and last response with obtained titrated delays shows that the adjusted delays across response requirements seem to track changes in duration measures better than changes in the sample-observing response requirement. This suggests that the time spent in the presence of the sample may have been a more important determinant of adjusted delays than the actual number of responses required. Future research should examine whether extended sample-observing responses, additional time spent in the presence of the sample, or a combination of the two, is responsible for the increased adjusted delay values. This could be examined by programming a yoked response-initiated FI for the sample-observing response requirement. For example, the mean duration of responding on an FR 8 sample-observing response requirement could serve as the value of a response-initiated FI programmed on the sample key. Data on the number of pecks on the sample key together with the adjusted delay values might help elucidate the relative contributions of response requirements versus time spent in the presence of the sample. Whether response requirements or the time spent in the presence of the sample stimulus is directly responsible for improved MTS performances, extended sample-observing response requirements are likely to remain a very useful procedure to ensure that the organism attends to the sample stimulus for at least the amount of time it takes to complete the requirement. Without requiring observing responses that are 19

25 directed to the sample, subjects may engage in other behavior (e.g., preening, looking away), limiting contact with the sample stimulus. Figure 7 shows that the likelihood of accurate choices decreases as a function of the delay between sample offset and comparison onset. This figure further shows that extended sample-observing response requirements appear to attenuate that effect. Although there is considerable overlap in the curves, this display clearly shows the effects of the response requirement and, interestingly, weakening of the effect as a function of increased delay between sample offset and comparison onset. In conclusion, the results from this study provide a strong rationale for employing caution while designing MTS experiments using extended sample-observing response requirements. These requirements may have a greater effect on the dependent variable then simply providing greater exposure to the sample stimulus. For example, Nordholm and colleagues (1995) investigated the effects of the proposed cognitive enhancing agent Linopirdine with six pigeons using an FR 15 response requirement and four squirrel monkeys using an FR 2 response requirement. Results of the current study suggest that the outcomes of the prior investigation may have revealed only a fragment of the total possible effects of the drug on conditional discrimination performance. Latency to respond, duration to complete the ratio, response running rate, and adjusted delay values may be more significantly affected by extended sampleobserving response requirements than laboratory lore suggests. 2

26 Table 1 Number of Sessions to Reach Acquisition Training Criteria Subject Simultaneous MTS Successive MTS

27 2 FR 1 FR 2 FR 4 FR 8 FR 16 FR 1R FR 1 FR 2 FR 4 FR 8 FR Adjusted Delay (sec) FR 1 FR 2 FR 4 FR FR 1 FR 2 FR 4 FR 8 FR Sessions Figure 1. Session by session adjusted delay means. 22

28 FR1 FR1R FR2 FR4 FR8 FR Adjusted Delay (sec) FR1 FR2 FR4 FR8 FR FR1 FR2 FR4 FR8 FR FR1 FR2 FR4 FR8 FR16 Condition Figure 2. Summary means of adjusted delays. 23

29 Adjusted Delay (sec) FR1 FR2 FR4 FR8 FR16 57 Condition Figure 3. Mean adjusted delay for last 1 sessions. 24

30 Seconds FR1 FR2 FR4 FR8 FR16 57 Condition Figure 4. Median sample latencies to first sample observing response. 25

31 Seconds FR2 FR4 FR8 FR16 57 Condition Figure 5. Median duration between first and last sample response. 26

32 Seconds FR2 FR4 FR8 FR16 57 Condition Figure 6. Median running rate of sample observing responses. 27

33 1% 8% 6% 4% 2% FR1 FR2 FR4 FR8 FR16 FR1R % % 8% 6% Percent Correct 4% 2% % % 659 8% 6% 4% 2% % % 8% 6% 4% 2% % Delay Value 57 Figure 7. Percent correct as a function of adjusted delay. 28

34 REFERENCES Berryman, R., Cumming, W. W., & Nevin, J. A. (1963). Acquisition of delayed matching in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, Carter, D. E., & Eckerman, D. A. (1975). Symbolic matching by pigeons: Rate of learning complex discriminations predicted by simple discriminations. Science, 187, Cumming, W. W., & Berryman, R. (1961). Some data on matching behavior in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, Cumming, W. W., & Berryman, R. (1965). The complex discriminated operant: Studies of matching-to-sample and related problems. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization (pp ). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Dayer, C. A., Baron, S., Light, K. E., & Wenger, G. R. (2). Effects of ethanol on working memory and attention in pigeons. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 293, Nordholm, A. F., Moore, E., & Wenger, G. R. (1995). Linopirdine does not improve matching performance in the titrating matching-to-sample paradigm. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 52, Poling, A., Temple, W., & Foster, T. M. (1996). The differential outcomes effect: A demonstration in domestic chickens responding under a titrating-delay-matchingto-sample procedure. Behavioural Processes, 36, Roberts, W. A. (1972). Short-term memory in the pigeon: Effects of repetition and spacing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, Sacks, R. A., Kamil, A. C., & Mack, R. (1972). The effects of fixed-ratio sample requirements on matching to sample in the pigeon. Psychonomic Science, 26, Weinstein, B. (1941). Matching-from-sample by rhesus monkeys and children. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31,

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December 2008 APPROVED:

More information

AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS. Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS. Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December 2011 APPROVED:

More information

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A.

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

EFFECTS OF NICOTINE ADMINISTRATION ON PERFORMANCE UNDER TWO PROCEDURES DESIGNED TO TEST SHORT-TERM REMEMBERING

EFFECTS OF NICOTINE ADMINISTRATION ON PERFORMANCE UNDER TWO PROCEDURES DESIGNED TO TEST SHORT-TERM REMEMBERING EFFECTS OF NICOTINE ADMINISTRATION ON PERFORMANCE UNDER TWO PROCEDURES DESIGNED TO TEST SHORT-TERM REMEMBERING By BRIAN D. KANGAS A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

DOES STIMULUS COMPLEXITY AFFECT ACQUISITION OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS AND THE EMERGENCE OF DERIVED RELATIONS? Tiffani L. Martin, B.A., B.S.

DOES STIMULUS COMPLEXITY AFFECT ACQUISITION OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS AND THE EMERGENCE OF DERIVED RELATIONS? Tiffani L. Martin, B.A., B.S. DOES STIMULUS COMPLEXITY AFFECT ACQUISITION OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS AND THE EMERGENCE OF DERIVED RELATIONS? Tiffani L. Martin, B.A., B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Summer 2005 Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli William

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 8-2012 Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Meredith

More information

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 74-84 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.74 Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure PATRICK VAN ROOYEN AND ANGELO

More information

Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching

Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (2), 189 196 Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching THOMAS R. ZENTALL and LOU M. SHERBURNE University of Kentucky, Lexington,

More information

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(4),621-629 Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives THOMAS R. ZENTALL University ofkentucky, Lexington,

More information

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, 152 162 2329-8456/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000053 Memory Systems

More information

EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION METHOD ON ACQUISITION SPEED OF AN IDENTITY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASK

EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION METHOD ON ACQUISITION SPEED OF AN IDENTITY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASK EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION METHOD ON ACQUISITION SPEED OF AN IDENTITY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASK A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Stanislaus In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

More information

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez The Psychological Record, 28, 58, 57 528 Some Effects of Magnitude of Reinforcement on Persistence of Responding Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman The University of Minnesota Angel Jimenez The University

More information

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING JOURNL OF THE EXPERMENTL NLYSS OF BEHVOR 1979, 32, 121-127 NUMBER (JULY) CQUSTON OF STMULUS CONTROL WHLE NTRODUCNG NEW STMUL N FDNG LNNY FELDS THE COLLEGE OF STTEN SLND fter establishing a discrimination

More information

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, & JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR1 1982, 38, 71-85 NUMBER I (JULY) MEMORY FOR RECENT BEHAVIOR IN THE PIGEON STEPHEN P. KRAMER CORRECTIONS DIVISION, DRAPER, UTAH Variations of the symbolic

More information

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER UNIVERSITY OF IOWA 1969, 12, 451-461 NUMBER

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired

More information

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Learning and Motivation 39 (2008) 71 94 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Luís Oliveira,

More information

Testing pigeon memory in a change detection task

Testing pigeon memory in a change detection task Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21, 17 (2), 243-249 doi:1.3758/pbr.17.2.243 Testing pigeon memory in a change detection task ANTHONY A. WRIGHT University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston, Texas JEFFREY

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented 1 Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample A Thesis Presented by Kimberly M. Walter The Department of Counseling and Applied

More information

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS The Psychological Record, 2000, 50, 429-442 LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS LUIS A. PEREZ-GONZALEZ, JOSEPH E. SPRADLIN, and KATHRYN J. SAUNDERS University of Oviedo, Spain

More information

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 348 370 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Chris Hope and Angelo Santi Wilfrid

More information

GENERALIZED IDENTITY MATCHING IN THE PIGEON: ROLE OF EXTENDED OBSERVING- AND CHOICE-RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS

GENERALIZED IDENTITY MATCHING IN THE PIGEON: ROLE OF EXTENDED OBSERVING- AND CHOICE-RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS REVISTA MEXICANA DE ANÁLISIS DE LA CONDUCTA MEXICAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 215 NÚMERO 1 (JUNIO) VOL. 41, 53-67 NUMBER 1 (JUNE) GENERALIZED IDENTITY MATCHING IN THE PIGEON: ROLE OF EXTENDED OBSERVING-

More information

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1998, 70, 165 183 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement Learning & Behavior 2005, 33 (3), 309-323 Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement GLENN S. BROWN and K. GEOFFREY WHITE University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand In two experiments, pigeons responding

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 89 (2012) 212 218 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes j o ur nal homep age : www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Providing a reinforcement history

More information

"Same/different" symbol use by pigeons

Same/different symbol use by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1983, 11 (3),349 355 "Same/different" symbol use by pigeons CHARLES A, EDWARDS, JOYCE A. JAGIELO, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University ofkentucky, Lexington, Kentucky Pigeons learned

More information

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION By JIN HO YOON A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY

More information

UNIVERSITY OF IOWA AND SOUTHERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY AT CARBONDALE

UNIVERSITY OF IOWA AND SOUTHERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY AT CARBONDALE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2002, 78, 365 373 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) BRIEF PRESENTATIONS ARE SUFFICIENT FOR PIGEONS TO DISCRIMINATE ARRAYS OF SAME AND DIFFERENT STIMULI EDWARD A. WASSERMAN,

More information

Instrumental Conditioning I

Instrumental Conditioning I Instrumental Conditioning I Basic Procedures and Processes Instrumental or Operant Conditioning? These terms both refer to learned changes in behavior that occur as a result of the consequences of the

More information

Coding of feature and no-feature events by pigeons performing a delayed conditional discrimination

Coding of feature and no-feature events by pigeons performing a delayed conditional discrimination Animal Learning & Behavior 1993, 21(2), 92-1 Coding of feature and no-feature events by pigeons performing a delayed conditional discrimination LOU M. SHERBURNE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky,

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition A Thesis Presented By Robert Mark Grant In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science

More information

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented 1 Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present A Thesis Presented by Lindsey M. Ryan The Department of Counseling and Applied

More information

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand).

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). http://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/ Research Commons at the University of Waikato Copyright Statement: The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). The thesis

More information

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a Effects of Food Deprivation on Memory Performance. Jacob. L. Kerewaro Abstract The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)

More information

Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed Through Partial Versus Total Reversals of Many-to-One Conditional and Simple Discriminations

Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed Through Partial Versus Total Reversals of Many-to-One Conditional and Simple Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psycholo Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 2, 194-201 Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/43.00 Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed

More information

Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness

Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2013 Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness Kerin A. Weingarten University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Follow this

More information

CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION AND STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE: EFFECTS OF SUPPRESSING DERIVED SYMMETRICAL RESPONSES ON THE EMERGENCE OF TRANSITIVITY

CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION AND STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE: EFFECTS OF SUPPRESSING DERIVED SYMMETRICAL RESPONSES ON THE EMERGENCE OF TRANSITIVITY CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION AND STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE: EFFECTS OF SUPPRESSING DERIVED SYMMETRICAL RESPONSES ON THE EMERGENCE OF TRANSITIVITY Aaron A. Jones, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER

More information

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 5, NUMBF- 4 OCITOBER, 1 962 THE EFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES OF PRIMARY AND CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT JOAN G. STEVENSON1 AND T. W. REESE MOUNT HOLYOKE

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 93 102 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (3), 264-270 Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pullman, Washington 99163 Five pigeons

More information

Transfer Across Delayed Discriminations: II. Differences in the Substitutability of Initial Versus Test Stimuli

Transfer Across Delayed Discriminations: II. Differences in the Substitutability of Initial Versus Test Stimuli Joulmal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/98/$3.00 1998, VoL 24, No. 1, 47-59 Transfer Across Delayed Discriminations:

More information

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques C O N F I D E N T I A L Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques DATE This study was conducted under the terms of a Materials Transfer and Services Agreement between NeuroDetective International and

More information

An inquiry of mediating events in the solution of the matching-to-sample problem.

An inquiry of mediating events in the solution of the matching-to-sample problem. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1971 An inquiry of mediating events in the solution of the matching-to-sample problem. Richard Paul Mack

More information

Olfactory Matching-To-Sample In Rats Using a Novel Apparatus. Rhiannon D. Thomas

Olfactory Matching-To-Sample In Rats Using a Novel Apparatus. Rhiannon D. Thomas Olfactory Matching-To-Sample In Rats Using a Novel Apparatus Rhiannon D. Thomas A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

More information

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons by Thomas A. Daniel A thesis proposal submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Auburn University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree

More information

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2015, 103, 269 287 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG

More information

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL'

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1973, 2, 393-43 NUMBER 3 (NOVEMBER) PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' M. C. DAVISON AND W. TEMPLE UNIVERSITY OF AUCKLAND AND

More information

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (1986) 38B, 81-95 Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon P. J. Durlach and N. J. Mackintosh Department of Experimental Psychology, University

More information

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 217, 17, 123 135 NUMBER 1 (JANUARY) SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 1

More information

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 12-1991 Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio

More information

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism A Thesis Presented by Melissa A. Ezold The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial fulfillment

More information

TRANSFER OF MA TCHING- TO-FIGURE SAMPLES

TRANSFER OF MA TCHING- TO-FIGURE SAMPLES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR TRANSFER OF MA TCHING- TO-FIGURE SAMPLES IN THE PIGEON RICHARD PISACRETA, EDWARD REDWOOD, AND KEVIN WIrT FERRIS STATE COLLEGE 1984P 42, 223-237 NUMBER 2

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Hannorah O. Thurman

Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Hannorah O. Thurman Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism A Thesis Presented by Hannorah O. Thurman The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial fulfillment of the requirements

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

Masaki Ishizuka, Tetsumi Moriyama. Tokiwa University, Mito, Japan

Masaki Ishizuka, Tetsumi Moriyama. Tokiwa University, Mito, Japan Psychology Research, September 2018, Vol. 8, No. 9, 411-434 doi:10.17265/2159-5542/2018.09.001 D DAVID PUBLISHING Symmetry Formation by Undergraduates in Successive Matching: A Re-evaluation of Urcuioli

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1974 Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer Richard H. Weiss Western

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2005, 83, 281 296 NUMBER 3(MAY) MORPHINE TOLERANCE AS A FUNCTION OF RATIO SCHEDULE: RESPONSE REQUIREMENT OR UNIT PRICE? CHRISTINE E. HUGHES, STACEY C. SIGMON,

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2008 Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional

More information

Abstract-Concept Learning Carryover Effects From the Initial Training Set in Pigeons (Columba livia)

Abstract-Concept Learning Carryover Effects From the Initial Training Set in Pigeons (Columba livia) Journal of Comparative Psychology 29 American Psychological Association 29, Vol. 123, No. 1, 79 89 735-36/9/$12. DOI: 1.137/a13126 Abstract-Concept Learning Carryover Effects From the Initial Training

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR LICK-SHOCK CONTINGENCIES IN THE RATT1 VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY 1972, 17, 119-125 NUMBER I (JANUARY) Hungry rats were allowed to lick an 8%

More information

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee The Psychological Record, 1997,47,145-166 THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pigeons served

More information

by instructional cues

by instructional cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1985 13 (4) 383-391 Control of pigeons' matching and mismatching performance by instructional cues CHARLES A. EDWARDS Dalhousie University Halifax Nova Scotia Canada and JAMES

More information

THE EFFECT OF DISPLAY TIMING ON CHANGE BLINDNESS IN PIGEONS (COLUMBA LIVIA) WALTER T. HERBRANSON AND EVA T. DAVIS

THE EFFECT OF DISPLAY TIMING ON CHANGE BLINDNESS IN PIGEONS (COLUMBA LIVIA) WALTER T. HERBRANSON AND EVA T. DAVIS JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2016, 105, 85 99 511 NUMBER 1 (JANUARY) THE EFFECT OF DISPLAY TIMING ON CHANGE BLINDNESS IN PIGEONS (COLUMBA LIVIA) WALTER T. HERBRANSON AND EVA T. DAVIS

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves

A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment

More information

A Search for Derived Stimulus Relations in Rats

A Search for Derived Stimulus Relations in Rats A Search for Derived Stimulus Relations in Rats MARK GALIZIO UNC WILMINGTON TXABA HOUSTON TX MARCH, 2018 These projects were supported in part by NIH grant DA29252 WHERE DO NOVEL STIMULUS RELATIONS COME

More information

Stimulus Control Exerted by Remember Cues Over Processing in Pigeons Short-Term Memory

Stimulus Control Exerted by Remember Cues Over Processing in Pigeons Short-Term Memory Wilfrid Laurier University Scholars Commons @ Laurier Theses and Dissertations (Comprehensive) 1982 Stimulus Control Exerted by Remember Cues Over Processing in Pigeons Short-Term Memory Patricia E. Hernandez

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action Categorization and Discrimination Animals respond to stimuli in ways that suggest they form categories. Pigeons

More information

THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL

THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2012, 97, 85 100 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

More information

Codes and coding processes in pigeon short-term memory

Codes and coding processes in pigeon short-term memory Animal Learning& Behavior 1980,8(3),341.351 Codes and coding processes in pigeon short-term memory H. L. ROITBLAT Columbia University, New York, New York 10027 The delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS) task

More information

EVALUATIONS OF DELAYED REINFORCEMENT IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES

EVALUATIONS OF DELAYED REINFORCEMENT IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES EVALUATIONS OF DELAYED REINFORCEMENT IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES By JOLENE RACHEL SY A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information