Pentylenetetrazol Produces a State-Dependent Conditioned Place Aversion to Alcohol Withdrawal in Mice

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Pentylenetetrazol Produces a State-Dependent Conditioned Place Aversion to Alcohol Withdrawal in Mice"

Transcription

1 Purdue University Purdue e-pubs Department of Psychological Sciences Faculty Publications Department of Psychological Sciences 2010 Pentylenetetrazol Produces a State-Dependent Conditioned Place Aversion to Alcohol Withdrawal in Mice Julia Chester Purdue University, jcheste@purdue.edu Laran E. Coon Follow this and additional works at: Part of the Psychiatry and Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Chester, Julia and Coon, Laran E., "Pentylenetetrazol Produces a State-Dependent Conditioned Place Aversion to Alcohol Withdrawal in Mice" (2010). Department of Psychological Sciences Faculty Publications. Paper This document has been made available through Purdue e-pubs, a service of the Purdue University Libraries. Please contact epubs@purdue.edu for additional information.

2 Pentylenetetrazol Produces a State-Dependent Conditioned Place Aversion to Alcohol Withdrawal in Mice Julia A. Chester* and Laran E. Coon Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN Running Head: Alcohol withdrawal-induced place aversion *Corresponding author: Julia A. Chester, Ph.D. Purdue University Psychological Sciences 703 Third Street West Lafayette, IN Phone: FAX: jchester@psych.purdue.edu

3 Abstract The purpose of this study was to determine if aversive effects of alcohol withdrawal could be detected in mice using the place conditioning procedure and whether the GABA A receptor antagonist, pentylenetetrazol (PTZ), would increase the aversive effects of alcohol withdrawal and increase the probability of detecting conditioned place aversion. Subjects were alcoholnaïve mice from a specific line selectively bred for low alcohol preference (LAP1; n=91) and were assigned to three groups: alcohol withdrawal, PTZ alone, and PTZ + alcohol withdrawal. On four trials, mice received either a 4.0 g/kg intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of alcohol (alcohol withdrawal, PTZ + alcohol withdrawal groups) or saline (PTZ group) 8 hrs prior to being placed on a distinctive floor texture for a 30-min conditioning session. Five min before these sessions, mice in the PTZ and PTZ + alcohol withdrawal groups received PTZ (5.0 mg/kg; i.p.) and the alcohol withdrawal group received saline. On intervening days mice received two saline injections at the same time points prior to being placed on a different floor texture. Postconditioning floor preference was assessed in two 60-min tests; the first test was drug-free and the second test was state-dependent. Neither alcohol withdrawal nor PTZ produced significant place conditioning. The PTZ + alcohol withdrawal group showed a significant place aversion during the state-dependent test. These data suggest that the combined stimulus properties of PTZ and alcohol withdrawal facilitated the expression of conditioned place aversion to alcohol withdrawal. Key words: aversion; alcohol withdrawal; GABA A ; mice; motivation; pentylenetetrazol; place conditioning; state-dependent 2

4 Introduction The alcohol withdrawal syndrome consists of overt, physical signs and subjective, motivational symptoms that occur when blood alcohol levels are falling and after blood alcohol levels have reached zero mg %. Signs of alcohol withdrawal can range from mild to severe and are similar in both humans and rodents (Kalant, 1977). These signs include tremors, convulsions, increased heat rate, and increased body temperature (Holloway et al., 1993; Majchrowicz, 1975). Symptoms of alcohol withdrawal, which are reportedly aversive based on subjective human descriptions, include irritability, nausea, headache, anxiety, and craving (Swift and Davidson, 1998; Tiffany, 1990). These aversive symptoms of alcohol withdrawal are thought to largely contribute to an individual s propensity to consume alcohol and to their risk for alcoholism (Chester et al., 2002; 2003; Koob, 2003; Wall and Ehlers, 1995). However, discrepancies in the human literature indicate that aversive alcohol withdrawal symptoms may be associated with either increased (McCaul et al., 1991; Newlin and Pretorius, 1990; Span and Earleywine, 1999) or decreased (Wall et al., 2000) subsequent alcohol consumption. Thus, the relationship between alcohol withdrawal and alcohol drinking behavior is not well understood in humans. It is likely that both the type and the severity of the alcohol withdrawal symptom may influence subsequent alcohol drinking behavior. For instance, craving has been associated with a propensity to consume excessive amounts of alcohol (Koob, 2003), whereas headache and nausea have been associated with alcohol avoidance (Wall et al., 2000). Animal models of alcohol withdrawal are advantageous because potentially confounding variables can be better controlled, such as amount of alcohol exposure and individual (e.g., genetic) and environmental factors that are known to influence the expression of the alcohol withdrawal syndrome in humans. Symptoms of alcohol withdrawal in rodents have been difficult to measure because of their subjective nature (see Emmett-Oglesby et al., 1990 for review). However, several models have been used to index withdrawal symptoms such as anxiety and craving in rodents, including pentylenetetrazole (PTZ) drug discrimination (Gauvin et al., 1989), 3

5 acoustic startle responding (e.g., Rassnick et al., 1992), the social interaction test (e.g., Overstreet et al., 2002), the elevated plus maze (e.g., Valdez et al., 2002), and operant selfadministration (e.g., Roberts et al., 2000). Although anxiety and self-administration models have high face validity, it is important to remember that alcohol withdrawal-induced changes in anxiety or self-administration behavior do not necessarily reflect the motivational (rewarding or aversive) effects of the alcohol withdrawal state. The place conditioning procedure has been used as a sensitive measure of both rewarding and aversive motivational effects of drug intoxication and withdrawal (discussed in Bozarth, 1987; Chester and Cunningham, 2002; Cunningham et al., 2000). This procedure involves establishing an association between a neutral environmental conditioning stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned motivational effects of drug intoxication or withdrawal. The motivational effect of this association is determined by examining approach to or avoidance of the drug-paired CS. Greater approach toward and contact with the CS is interpreted as evidence of the rewarding effect of the drug, whereas stronger avoidance of the CS is seen as indicative of the aversive effect of the drug. Advantages and disadvantages of this procedure as a model to study the motivational effects of drugs have recently been reviewed (Cunningham et al., 2006). One valuable aspect of this procedure is that it specifically assesses the learned relationship between environmental stimuli and motivational effects of the drug. Thus, this procedure may serve as a particularly useful model for understanding certain learning and memory processes and the role of environmental cues in influencing craving, relapse, and alcohol-seeking behavior in humans (Cunningham et al., 2000). Another potential advantage, particularly when applied to the study of alcohol withdrawal, is that it may detect motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal after relatively small amounts of alcohol exposure compared to other withdrawal models that often require extended exposure to alcohol before alcohol withdrawal signs are observable. This feature of the place conditioning procedure is useful because initial sensitivity to the aversive motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal following a single or several discrete alcohol 4

6 exposures may be regulated by mechanisms that are different from those that regulate motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal following continuous or chronic alcohol exposure. Place conditioning has often been used in rats as a sensitive measure of aversive motivational effects of withdrawal from both acute (e.g., Azar et al., 2003; Parker et al., 2002) and chronic (e.g. Mucha, 1987; Stinus et al., 1990) opiate exposure, largely because withdrawal from opiates can easily be precipitated using opiate antagonists. There are only two studies in rats that have used the place conditioning procedure to study the motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal. In one study, a place preference was observed (Gauvin et al., 1997) and in another study, a place aversion was observed (Morse et al., 2000). There are no studies in mice that have demonstrated place conditioning to the motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal. The lack of studies showing place conditioning to alcohol withdrawal could be partly due to the lack of an established procedure for precipitating withdrawal from alcohol. Similar to that employed with opiates, such a procedure would produce a discrete period of aversive effects during alcohol withdrawal that could be more easily paired with a neutral CS to produce a conditioned response. The goal of the present study was to determine whether aversive motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal could be detected in mice using the place conditioning procedure. It was hypothesized that the time course of physical and motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal may overlap. Thus, we chose to condition mice at 8 hrs in withdrawal, a time point at which peak physical signs of acute withdrawal, assessed via handling-induced convulsions (HICs), are evident in mice (e.g., Crabbe et al., 1991, 1998; Metten and Crabbe, 1994). Further, we assessed the effect of a GABA A receptor antagonist (PTZ) on alcohol-withdrawal induced place conditioning. PTZ has been shown to enhance the physical signs of alcohol withdrawal (e.g., Chesher and Jackson, 1974; Crabbe et al., 1991; Finn and Crabbe, 1999). Thus, we hypothesized that administration of a subconvulsant dose PTZ would enhance the aversive 5

7 motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal and thereby increase the strength of conditioned place aversion. Method Subjects. Subjects were alcohol-naïve, adult male mice selectively bred for low alcohol preference (LAP1 line). Mice used in the place conditioning procedures were days old and mice used in the HIC procedure were days old at the start of each experiment. LAP1 mice were used in the current studies because they are readily available in our laboratory and they exhibit robust HICs compared to their counterparts bred for high alcohol preference (HAP) (unpublished data from our laboratory and from the laboratory of Dr. John C. Crabbe at Oregon Health & Science University: P. Metten, N.J. Grahame, J.C. Crabbe, personal communication). This finding is consistent with the well-documented negative correlation between magnitude of alcohol withdrawal signs and alcohol drinking propensity in rodents (Chester et al., 2002; 2003; Metten et al., 1998). Thus, LAP1 mice were used as subjects (as opposed to HAP mice) in an effort to maximize our ability to detect aversive motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal in the place conditioning procedure. The LAP1 mouse line was derived from a foundation stock of outbred HS/Ibg mice (Boulder, Colorado, USA) at the Indiana Alcohol Research Center (IARC) (Grahame et al., 1999). HS/Ibg mice were originally created by an intercross of eight different inbred mouse strains (A, AKR, BALB/c, C3H/2, C57BL, DBA/2, Is/Bi, and RIII) (McClearn et al, 1970). Subjects were generated at Purdue University from 8 original LAP1 breeder pairs (generation 27) obtained from the IARC. The mice used in the present study were 3 rd, 4 th, and 5 th generation offspring from these original breeders maintained with relaxed selection. There were on average 22 breeder pairs used to generate experimental subjects for the current studies and care was taken to ensure heterogeneity in subjects genetic background by avoiding genetic conflicts between breeder pairs at the parental and grandparental levels. 6

8 Mice were weaned at days old and housed in polycarbonate cages (11.5 x 7.5 x 5.0 in) in groups of 2-4 per cage with aspen wood shavings. Ambient temperature in the colony room was maintained at 21±1ºC and mice had free-access to food (Rodent Lab Diet 5001, Purina Mills Inc., St. Louis, MO) and water in the home cage. Experimental procedures were conducted during the light phase of a 12:12 light:dark cycle (lights on at 0700). Experiments were conducted in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and the experimental procedures were approved by the Purdue Animal Care and Use Committee. Apparatus. The apparatus consisted of 8 identical open-top boxes made of Plexiglas (43.2 x 21.6 x 25.4 cm) enclosed in separate ventilated sound- and light-attenuated chambers (76.2 x 50.8 x 20.3 cm). Each box was surrounded by an open field activity frame (SmartFrame Low Density, Lafayette Instrument Co, Lafayette, IN, USA) that contained eight infrared photobeams along the length and four along the width of each frame (internal frame dimensions: 24.1 x 45.7 cm). The floor of each box consisted of interchangeable halves with distinct floor textures. One floor texture (the grid floor) consisted of 4 mm steel rods mounted 3.5 mm apart and the other floor texture (the hole floor) was made up of perforated 16 gauge stainless steel with 6.4 mm holes on 9.5 mm staggered centers. The photobeams were approximately 2 cm above the floor of each box. Locomotor activity and side position (left or right) for each mouse was continuously monitored by a computer program (Hamilton-Kinder MotorMonitor, Model HMM100, Hamilton-Kinder Motor Monitor, San Diego, California, USA). General activity and location of the animal (left or right) within the box is continuously measured by occlusion of the infrared photobeams. This place conditioning apparatus was constructed with slight modifications based on that used in prior work with mice (e.g., Chester and Cunningham, 1998; 1999a, 1999b; Chester et al., 1998; Grahame et al., 2001). 7

9 Drugs. Alcohol was diluted from a 95% (v/v) solution to a concentration of 20% (v/v) with 0.9% saline and administered in a dose of 4.0 g/kg (25.0 ml/kg injection volume). PTZ was dissolved in 0.9% saline and administered in a dose of 5.0 mg/kg (10.0 ml/kg injection volume). Procedures. Handling-induced Convulsions As previously mentioned, we chose to condition mice at 8 hrs in withdrawal because this is the time point at which peak HICs have been demonstrated in mice exposed to an acute dose of 4.0 g/kg alcohol (e.g., Crabbe et al., 1991, 1998; Metten and Crabbe, 1994). Similar findings have previously been found in LAP1 mice (Metten et al., personal communication). Furthermore, we have previously demonstrated that blood alcohol levels in LAP1 mice exposed to an acute dose of 4.0 g/kg alcohol are negligible at 8 hrs in withdrawal (Chester and Barrenha, 2007). However, to confirm the time course of physical withdrawal in LAP1 mice and provide additional support for our choice of withdrawal conditioning time point, we assessed HICs in a group of male LAP1 mice following a single injection of 4.0 g/kg alcohol. HICs were assessed in LAP1 mice (n=10) using a 7-point rating scale modified from Goldstein et al. (1972) and previously described (e.g., Metten and Crabbe, 1994). Each mouse was lifted by the tail and observed for signs of convulsive behavior. If no signs were observed within 2 sec the mouse was gently rotated 180 and observed again. Two baseline HICs scores were taken 20 min apart, the second of which occurred immediately prior to injection of 4.0 g/kg alcohol. Mice were then scored for HICs every hr for 12 hrs and again at 24 hrs post alcohol injection. Place Conditioning The place conditioning procedure involved one 60 min pre-test, eight 30-min conditioning sessions, and two 60-min post-tests. Conditioning sessions were conducted on 8

10 consecutive days except that a 48-hr break separated the first four and the second four sessions. The alcohol withdrawal study was conducted in four replications due to limitations associated with breeding, apparatus number, and conditioning time parameters. An independent control experiment was conducted to verify that evidence for alcohol withdrawal-induced place conditioning was not influenced by potential changes in unconditioned floor preference due to repeated exposure to the apparatus, handling, injections, and floor stimulus cues over the course of the experiment (Cunningham et al., 2003). In this study, mice were exposed to the same experimental procedures but received saline throughout the experiment. Pre-test. Initial preference for the two floor textures used as conditioning stimuli was assessed 24 hrs prior to the start of the first conditioning trial. All mice were placed in the apparatus on a half grid/half hole floor for 60 min. Floor position (left versus right side of the box) was counterbalanced within each conditioning subgroup. Conditioning. Mice were subjected to a differential place conditioning procedure where they were randomly assigned to one of two separate conditioning subgroups within each experimental group. Each conditioning subgroup received exposure to either a grid (G) or hole (H) floor paired with drug treatments (CS+ conditioning sessions) and the other floor type paired with saline (CS- conditioning sessions) for a total of 4 four CS+ and CS- conditioning sessions. All subjects in each group received equal exposure to drug/saline treatments and to both floor textures. The advantage of this differential conditioning procedure is that it provides control over the subjects exposure to the floor types and to drug exposure so that the conditioning subgroups differ only in the specific floor that is paired with drug effects (Cunningham, 1993). Assignment of mice to experimental groups and conditioning subgroups was counterbalanced by litter of origin (genetic background), order of exposure to the CS and drug exposure, and apparatus enclosure. 9

11 For the alcohol withdrawal study, mice were assigned to one of three experimental groups: alcohol withdrawal (n=30), PTZ (n=30), and PTZ + alcohol withdrawal (n=31). On alternating CS+ conditioning days, mice in the alcohol withdrawal and PTZ+ alcohol withdrawal groups received an intraperitoneal (i.p) injection of 4.0 g/kg alcohol and the PTZ group received an equal volume of saline 8 hrs prior to being placed on a homogeneous grid or hole floor for 30 min. Five min before the CS+ conditioning session, mice in the PTZ and PTZ + alcohol withdrawal groups received an i.p. injection of 5.0 mg/kg PTZ (10.0 ml/kg) and the alcohol withdrawal group received an equal volume of saline. The 5-min pretreatment time was chosen based on the fact that PTZ produces effects on behavior within 5 min following i.p. injection (Crabbe et al., 1991; Freund et al., 1987). On intervening CS- conditioning days, all groups received two saline injections, one at 8 hr and one at 5 min, prior to being placed on the alternate floor type. The floors and inside of the box were wiped with a damp sponge between each subject. All mice were handled by the scruff of the neck rather than the tail in order to avoid eliciting HICs during conditioning. No evidence of convulsive behavior was seen during conditioning. For the saline control study, mice (n=16) were randomly assigned to conditioning subgroups but they received saline injections at 8 hr and at 5 min before each conditioning session. Post-tests. The post-tests were conducted in the same manner as the pre-test. The first post-test was conducted in a drug-free state 24 hrs after the last conditioning session. The second post-test was state-dependent, conducted 72 hrs after the first post-test. In the alcohol withdrawal study, a small portion of the mice received their state-dependent test at 96 hrs after the first post-test because one of the replications occurred over a holiday. Prior to the statedependent test, all groups received their same respective drug treatments as that given on CS+ conditioning sessions at 8 hrs and 5 min before the test session. For the control study, two 10

12 post-tests were given in the same manner as the alcohol withdrawal study but only saline injections were administered. Statistical Analyses. Data were analyzed using analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the significance level set at p<0.05. Between-group factors included Group (alcohol withdrawal, PTZ, PTZ + alcohol withdrawal, saline) or Pretreatment (saline or PTZ) and Conditioning Treatment (saline or alcohol withdrawal). Within-group factors included Floor Type (grid or hole), CS Session Type (CS+ or CS-), Conditioning Session (1-4), Test Phase (pre- versus post-conditioning), and Minute or Hour. Because experimental procedures were identical, place conditioning data from the alcohol withdrawal and the saline control study were analyzed together. For the HICs study, the two baseline scores were averaged and compared to the peak withdrawal magnitude score which was calculated by averaging the highest HIC score and the HIC score taken before and after the highest HIC score (Metten and Crabbe, 1994). Significant main effects and interactions were followed using lower-order one-way ANOVAs and paired samples t-tests, where appropriate (Keppel, 1991). Pearson product moment correlations were conducted to determine the relationship between locomotor activity during conditioning and testing and the magnitude of conditioned place aversion. Handling-Induced Convulsions Results Figure 1 shows the time course of HIC scores in LAP1 mice following a single injection of 4.0 g/kg alcohol. As can be seen in the figure, the peak magnitude of convulsions occurred on average at 8 hrs in withdrawal. One mouse was removed from the analysis because it showed the highest HIC score during the last (24) hr of measurement and thus a peak withdrawal score could not be calculated. Paired samples t-test revealed a significant difference between the average baseline scores and the peak withdrawal magnitude scores (t=-3.3, df=8, p=0.01). 11

13 Insert Figure 1 about here Alcohol Withdrawal Place Conditioning Study Two mice died during the course of the study and were removed from all analyses. Pre-test Overall analysis of the 60-min pretest data (Group x Floor Type x Minute ANOVA) yielded main effects of Floor Type [F(1,103)=12.8, p=0.001] and Minute [F(59,6077)=1.4, p<0.05] and and a Floor Type x Minute interaction [F(59,6077)=1.6, p<0.01]. Follow-up one-way ANOVAs indicated main effects of Floor Type during both the first 30 and last 30 min of the session [Fs>8.0,ps<0.01], due to mice spending on average more time on the grid floor (32.1±0.6 sec/min) versus the hole floor (27.8±0.6 sec/min) during the 60-min pre-test. However, the average amount of time mice spent on their assigned CS+ floor versus CS- floor during the 60- min pre-test was not statistically different (CS+ floor: 29.8±0.6 sec/min; CS- floor 30.2±0.6 sec/min). Mean (±sem) activity counts during the pre-test were 82.6±1.9, 81.1±2.3, 83.6±2.1, and 82.7±2.6 for the alcohol withdrawal, PTZ, PTZ + alcohol withdrawal, and saline group, respectively. One-way ANOVA showed no group differences in activity levels. Conditioning Trial Activity Figure 2 depicts mean (±sem) activity levels during CS+ and CS- conditioning sessions collapsed across conditioning sessions 1-4 in each group. Activity levels in the alcohol withdrawal and PTZ + alcohol withdrawal groups were reduced during CS+ conditioning sessions compared to CS- session activity levels and compared to the PTZ group. Overall analysis of the data (Group x Conditioning Session x CS Session Type) yielded main effects of 12

14 Group [F(3,103)=2.7, p<0.05], Conditioning Session [F(3,309)=6.5, p<0.001], CS Session Type [F(1,103)=36.5, p<0.001], and Group x Conditioning Session [F(9,309)=3.9, p<0.001] and Group x CS Session Type [F(3,103)=11.7, p<0.001]. To explore the Group x CS Session Type interaction, follow up one-way ANOVAs of activity levels on CS+ versus CS- conditioning sessions were conducted within each group (collapsed across the four conditioning sessions). These analyses indicated significantly lower activity levels in the alcohol withdrawal and PTZ + alcohol withdrawal groups on CS+ conditioning sessions compared to CS- conditioning sessions (Fs>58.2, ps<0.001). Activity levels on both trial types were comparable in the saline and PTZ groups. In addition, activity levels on CS+ sessions did not differ between the alcohol withdrawal and PTZ + alcohol withdrawal groups. A three-way ANOVA (Pretreatment x Conditioning Treatment x CS Session Type) indicated main effects of Conditioning Treatment [F(1,103)=6.8, p=0.01; alcohol withdrawal <saline] and CS Session Type [F(1,103)=36.5, p<0.05; CS+<CS-] and a Conditioning Treatment x CS Session Type interaction [F(1,103)=33.3, p<0.001] but no interaction with Pretreatment. Reduced locomotor activity during alcohol withdrawal is a finding consistent with prior reports in rodents (e.g., File et al., 1989; Jung et al., 2000; Kliethermes et al., 2004; Knapp et al., 2005; Onaivi et al., 1989; Rasmussen et al., 2001). Insert Figure 2 about here Post-test 1: Drug-free Evidence for place conditioning was assessed by conducting within-subject comparisons of the amount of time spent on the CS+ floor during the pre-test to the amount of time spent on the CS+ floor during the post-tests. All pre- versus post-test comparisons were conducted on data averaged across the 60-min test session because initial analyses indicated that time spent on the CS+ floor was relatively constant across the 60-min test in all groups. Figure 3 shows mean 13

15 (±sem) difference scores for each group calculated by subtracting the time spent on the CS+ floor during the pre-test from time spent on the CS+ floor during post-test 1 (drug-free). The repeated-measures analysis of time on the CS+ floor [Group x Test Phase ANOVA] indicated a Group x Test Phase interaction [F(2,88)=3.3, p<0.05]. However, follow-up comparisons of Test Phase within each Group indicated no significant effects. Pre- vs. post-test difference scores were also analyzed with a two-way ANOVA (Pretreatment x Conditioning Treatment) to determine whether PTZ pretreatment affected the development of conditioned place aversion to alcohol withdrawal as evidenced during the drug-free test. This analysis showed a main effect of Pretreatment [F(1,103)=4.7, p<0.05; PTZ-treated < saline-treated] but no other significant effects were found. Mean (±sem) activity counts during post-test 1 were 72.7±3.6, 84.1±3.6, 73.6±3.6, and 83.3±4.0 for the alcohol withdrawal, PTZ, PTZ + alcohol withdrawal, and saline group, respectively. Two-way ANOVA (Pretreatment x Conditioning Treatment) indicated a main effect of Conditioning Treatment [F(1,103)=7.5, p<0.01; alcohol withdrawal-treated < saline-treated] but no other effects were found. Pearson correlations between post-test 1 difference scores and average activity levels during CS+ conditioning trials and during post-test 1 were not significant. Insert Figure 3 about here Post-test 2: State-dependent Figure 3 shows mean (±sem) difference scores for each group calculated by subtracting the time spent on the CS+ floor during the pre-test from time spent on the CS+ floor during post-test 2 (state dependent). 14

16 The repeated-measures analysis of time on the CS+ floor [Group x Test Phase ANOVA] indicated a Group x Test Phase interaction [F(3,103)=2.7, p=0.05]. Follow-up comparisons of Test Phase within each group indicated a significant place aversion in the PTZ + alcohol withdrawal group [F(1,30)=4.2, p<0.05; post-test CS+ time < pre-test CS+ time]. As was done with post-test 1 data, pre- vs. post-test difference scores were analyzed with a two-way ANOVA (Pretreatment x Conditioning Treatment) which showed a main effect of Pretreatment [F(1,103) = 5.1, p<0.05; PTZ-treated < saline-treated]. This effect is clearly due to the significant aversion in the PTZ + alcohol withdrawal group (see Figure 3) but there was not enough statistical power to detect a Pretreatment x Conditioning Treatment interaction. Mean (±sem) activity counts during post-test 2 were 44.2±4.2, 73.7±4.2, 50.0±4.1, and 82.3±5.0 for the alcohol withdrawal, PTZ, PTZ + alcohol withdrawal, and saline group, respectively. Two-way ANOVA (Pretreatment x Conditioning Treatment) indicated a main effect of Conditioning Treatment [F(1,103)=46.5, p<0.001; alcohol withdrawal-treated < saline-treated] but no other effects were found. Pearson correlations between post-test 2 difference scores and average activity levels during CS+ conditioning trials and during post-test 2 were not significant. Discussion Alcohol withdrawal produces aversive motivational effects which have been hypothesized to play a primary role in the development of alcoholism (Chester et al., 2002; 2003; Koob, 2003; Wall and Ehlers, 1995). Relatively few animal models have been developed that assess the motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal. The goal of the present study was two-fold: 1) to determine whether a conditioned place aversion to alcohol withdrawal could be detected in mice and 2) whether PTZ administration during alcohol withdrawal would increase the strength of the conditioned place aversion. 15

17 With regard to the first study goal, conditioning during alcohol withdrawal did not produce a significant conditioned place aversion during either the drug-free or state-dependent preference test. Only two prior studies in rats have reported place conditioning to alcohol withdrawal. Gauvin et al. (1997) reported a place preference and Morse et al. (2000) reported a place aversion to alcohol withdrawal. Comparison between these discrepant studies and with the present data is particularly difficult because there is no consistency among the experimental procedures used. For example, Gauvin et al. found a place preference after 8 pairings of the non-preferred environmental stimuli (biased procedure) with alcohol withdrawal whereas Morse et al. found a place aversion after a single pairing of neutral environmental stimuli (unbiased procedure) with alcohol withdrawal. Another major difference between studies was that Gauvin et al. conditioned rats at 18 hrs in withdrawal whereas Morse et al. conditioned rats at 10 hrs in withdrawal. We chose to condition mice at 8 hrs in withdrawal because it is the time point at which peak physical signs of alcohol withdrawal, assessed via HICs, were evident in LAP1 mice following a single injection of 4.0 g/kg alcohol. However, it is possible that we missed the ideal conditioning time point that would have produced a conditioned place aversion. Indeed, there is evidence in rodents that affective or motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal extend past the time that physical signs of withdrawal are evident (e.g., Prediger et al., 2006; Roberts et al., 2000). For example, Prediger et al. (2006) showed that alcohol withdrawal-induced anxiety in mice was greatest between 12 and 18 hrs in withdrawal from a single injection of 4.0 g/kg alcohol. Finally, it should be noted that species differences could account for the discrepant results between the present and prior studies. Alcohol intoxication has been shown to produce place aversion in rats but place preference in mice exposed to identical conditioning parameters (Cunningham et al., 1993). Thus, there may also be relevant species differences in sensitivity to the rewarding or aversive effects of alcohol withdrawal. The overall lack of studies showing place conditioning to alcohol withdrawal, unlike with opiate withdrawal, could partly be due to the fact that alcohol withdrawal is not a discrete stimulus and there is no established procedure 16

18 for precipitating withdrawal from alcohol. This situation, and the cumbersome nature of the place conditioning procedure, makes it challenging to identify effective conditioning parameters that will capture the aversive effects of alcohol withdrawal and maximize the salience of alcohol withdrawal as an unconditioned stimulus. This is the first study to report a place aversion to alcohol withdrawal in mice using a drug (PTZ) to precipitate or increase the aversive effects of alcohol withdrawal. The PTZ + alcohol withdrawal group showed a significant place aversion during the state-dependent test. These data suggest that the combined stimulus properties of PTZ and alcohol withdrawal facilitated the expression of conditioned place aversion to alcohol withdrawal. Further, this finding suggests that PTZ can reveal sub-threshold aversive motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal that are present at 8 hrs in withdrawal, a time point when peak physical signs of alcohol withdrawal are evident in mice (See Figure 1; Crabbe et al., 1991, 1998; Metten and Crabbe, 1994). However, additional studies are needed to determine whether this effect is dependent on the presence of PTZ during conditioning trials or whether acute administration of PTZ during the expression test is sufficient to reveal a conditioned place aversion to alcohol withdrawal. It should also be noted that the magnitude of conditioned place aversion seen in the PTZ+ alcohol withdrawal group was small and more work is needed to explore whether other experimental parameters, such as a different PTZ dose or conditioning time point, may produce a greater effect. The lack of preference or aversion behavior in the PTZ alone and saline-treated groups indicates that the observed effect in the PTZ + alcohol withdrawal group is not due to rewarding or aversive effects of PTZ itself or to changes in unconditioned preference for the conditioning stimuli during repeated conditioning and testing procedures. PTZ is a GABA A receptor antagonist and is frequently used as a chemoconvulsant to precipitate the physical signs of alcohol withdrawal and assess alcohol dependence-related phenomena (e.g., Cagetti et al., 2004; Chesher and Jackson, 1974; Crabbe et al., 1991; Finn and Crabbe, 1999; Ripley et al., 2002). However, little is known about the effects of PTZ on the 17

19 motivational or affective symptoms of alcohol withdrawal. Prior studies indicate that the discriminative stimulus effects of PTZ are similar to alcohol withdrawal (Gauvin et al., 1989, 1993; Lal et al., 1988), particularly in male rats (Jung et al., 1999). Thus, the current findings could be explained via an additive effect where combined subthreshold aversive effects of PTZ and alcohol withdrawal produce a salient enough unconditioned stimulus to support the development of a conditioned place aversion. One potential mechanism for this effect is that PTZ increases anxiety during alcohol withdrawal. Both PTZ (see Jung et al., 2002 for review) and withdrawal from high doses of alcohol produces anxiety-related behavior in both rats and mice (e.g., Overstreet et al., 2002; Valdez et al., 2002; Verleye et al. 2009; Zhang et al., 2007) and drugs that enhance activity at the GABA A receptor can reverse the anxiogenic effects of alcohol withdrawal (e.g., Knapp et al., 2005; Roberto et al., 2008; Verleye et al. 2009; Watson et al., 1997). However, Gauvin et al. (1991) showed that PTZ can also produce a conditioned place preference in rats. This result led to the suggestion that PTZ may have some rewarding motivational effects in addition to its anxiogenic effects and that anxiety induced with low to moderate doses of PTZ may serve to enhance learning in the place conditioning task, similar to that reported in other classical conditioning procedures (e.g., Taylor, 1951). It should also be noted that an alternative interpretation of the current results is that PTZ facilitates learning about stimulus effects of the alcohol withdrawal state independent of its influence on the motivational aspects alcohol withdrawal. GABA A receptor antagonists are known to enhance memoryrelated processes in different types of learning tasks (see reviews by Chapouthier and Venault, 2002; Izquierdo and Medina, 1991). The significant conditioned place aversion to alcohol withdrawal in the PTZ + alcohol withdrawal group during the second post-conditioning preference test suggests that the expression of the aversion is influenced by a state-dependent memory retrieval process. Statedependent retrieval occurs when the recall of a previously learned response is facilitated by, or entirely dependent upon, the physiological or affective state under which the organism 18

20 originally learned the response (see review by Overton, 1991). In the drug place conditioning literature, a wide range of findings have been reported in rodents, with many studies indicating that state-dependent processes do not influence the expression of conditioned drug responses (e.g., substance P: Elliott, 1988; alcohol: Gremel and Cunningham, 2007; morphine: Mucha and Iverson, 1984; amphetamine: Reicher and Holman, 1977; diazepam: Spyraki et al., 1985) while others have shown that administration of the training drug prior to testing enhances (heroin: Bozarth, 1987; alcohol: Cunningham, 1979; cholecystokinin: Swerdlow et al., 1983) or reveals (morphine: Bespalov et al., 1999; WAY161503: Mosher et al., 2006; lithium chloride, naloxone, FG1742: Oberling et al., 1993) a conditioned place preference or aversion. To our knowledge, this is the first report indicating state-dependent effects on the expression of place aversion to withdrawal from a drug, in this case, alcohol. In the two prior reports of place conditioning to alcohol withdrawal in rats (Gauvin et al., 1997; Morse et al. 2000), the authors did not address the question of whether conditioned behavior was influenced by a state-dependent mechanism. A point worth mentioning is that reduced locomotor activity during alcohol withdrawal may have somehow influenced the acquisition of conditioned place aversion or the expression of conditioned place aversion during the state-dependent test. However, this possibility seems unlikely because both alcohol withdrawal groups showed reduced locomotor activity during conditioning trials and during the state-dependent test but only the PTZ + alcohol withdrawal group displayed a conditioned place aversion. Consistent with prior studies of conditioned place preference (e.g., Chester and Cunningham, 1999; Cunningham, 1995; Gremel and Cunningham, 2007), we found no correlation between activity levels on CS+ conditioning trials and place aversion magnitude during the state-dependent test. In addition, although some previous studies have indicated a positive correlation between magnitude of place preference and test activity levels (e.g., Gremel and Cunningham, 2007; Neisewander et al., 1990), we found no correlation between place aversion magnitude and activity levels during the statedependent test. 19

21 In summary, PTZ administration during alcohol withdrawal conditioning trials produced a state-dependent conditioned place aversion. This finding perhaps reflects a phenomenon whereby PTZ increases or precipitates the aversive effects of alcohol withdrawal and provides further support for a role of the GABA A receptor in modulating the neurochemical and behavioral effects of alcohol withdrawal (e.g., Cagetti et al., 2003; Devaud et al., 2003; Follesa et al., 2006; Knapp et al., 2005; Morrow et al., 1991). GABA A receptor antagonists such as PTZ may be useful to increase the sensitivity of the place conditioning procedure as a measure of the aversive effects of alcohol withdrawal in rodents. A model such as this is a valuable tool to identify and characterize genetic and neurochemical mechanisms that mediate sensitivity to aversive motivational effects of alcohol withdrawal that, in turn, may influence alcohol drinking behavior. Future studies using this technique could eventually lead to the discovery of novel behavioral and pharmacological treatments for alcoholism. 20

22 Acknowledgements This research was supported by Purdue University and NIAAA grant AA Special thanks to Dr. Nicholas J. Grahame for providing breeders for the LAP1 mice. We appreciate the technical assistance of Mr. Phillip M. Bohn and Mr. Gustavo D. Barrenha. We also thank Dr. Christopher L. Cunningham for insightful discussions of the data and Mr. Matthew S. Powers for careful reading of the manuscript. 21

23 References Azar MR, Jones BC, Schulteis G. Conditioned place aversion is a highly sensitive index of acute opioid dependence and withdrawal. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003; 170: Bespalov AY, Tokarz ME, Bowen SE, Balster RL, Beardsley PM. Effects of test conditions on the outcome of place conditioning with morphine and naltrexone in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999; 141: Bozarth MA. Neuroanatomical boundaries of the reward-relevant opiate-receptor field in the ventral tegmental area as mapped by the conditioned place preference method in rats. Brain Res 1987; 414: Cagetti E, Baicy KJ, Olsen RW. Topiramate attenuates withdrawal signs after chronic intermittent ethanol in rats. Neuroreport 2004; 15: Cagetti E, Liang J, Spigelman I, Olsen RW. Withdrawal from chronic intermittent ethanol treatment changes subunit composition, reduces synaptic function, and decreases behavioral responses to positive allosteric modulators of GABA A receptors. Mol Pharmacol 2003; 63: Chapouthier G, Venault P. GABA A receptor complex and memory processes. Curr Top Med Chem 2002; 2: Chesher GB, Jackson DM. The effect of withdrawal from cannabis on pentylenetetrazol convulsive threshold in mice. Psychopharmacologia 1974; 40:

24 Chester JA, Barrenha GD. Acoustic startle at baseline and during acute alcohol withdrawal in replicate mouse lines selectively bred for high or low alcohol preference. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2007, 31: Chester JA, Cunningham CL. Modulation of corticosterone does not affect the acquisition or expression of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference in DBA/2J mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1998; 59: Chester JA, Cunningham CL. Baclofen alters ethanol-stimulated activity but not conditioned place preference or taste aversion in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1999; 63: Chester JA, Blose AM, Froehlich JC. Further evidence of an inverse genetic relationship between innate differences in alcohol preference and alcohol withdrawal magnitude in multiple selectively bred rat lines. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2003; 27: Chester JA, Cunningham CL. Baclofen alters ethanol-stimulated activity but not conditioned place preference or taste aversion in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1999a; 63: Chester JA, Cunningham CL. GABA A receptors modulate ethanol-induced conditioned place preference and taste aversion in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999b; 144: Chester JA, Cunningham CL. GABA A receptor modulation of the rewarding and aversive effects of ethanol. Alcohol 2002; 26: Chester JA, Price CS, Froehlich JC. Inverse genetic association between alcohol preference and severity of alcohol withdrawal in two sets of rat lines selected for the same phenotype. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2002; 26: Chester JA, Risinger FO, Cunningham CL. Ethanol reward and aversion in mice bred for sensitivity to ethanol withdrawal. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1998; 22:

25 Crabbe JC. Provisional mapping of quantitative trait loci for chronic ethanol withdrawal severity in BXD recombinant inbred mice. J Psychopharmacol Exp Ther 1998; 286: Crabbe JC, Merrill CD, Belknap JK. Effects of convulsants on handling-induced convulsions in mice selected for ethanol withdrawal severity. Brain Res 1991; 550:1-6. Cunningham CL. Flavor and location aversions produced by ethanol. Behav Neural Biol 1979; 27: Cunningham CL. Pavlovian drug conditioning. In: van Haaren F, editor. Methods in behavioral pharmacology. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers; p Cunningham CL. Localization of genes influencing ethanol-induced conditioned place preference and locomotor activity in BXD recombinant inbred mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995; 120: Cunningham CL, Ferree NK, Howard MA. Apparatus bias and place conditioning with ethanol in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003; 170: Cunningham CL, Fidler TL, Hill KG. Animal models of alcohol's motivational effects. Alcohol Res Health 2000; 24: Cunningham CL, Gremel CM, Groblewski PA. Drug-induced conditioned place preference and aversion in mice. Nat Protoc 2006; 1: Cunningham CL, Niehus JS, Noble D. Species difference in sensitivity to ethanol's hedonic effects. Alcohol 1993; 10: Devaud LL, Alele P, Ritu C. Sex differences in the central nervous system actions of ethanol. Crit Rev Neurobiol 2003; 15:

26 Elliott PJ. Place aversion induced by the substance P analogue, dimethyl-c7, is not state dependent: implication of substance P in aversion. Exp Brain Res 1988; 73: Emmett-Oglesby MW, Mathis DA, Moon RT, Lal H. Animal models of drug withdrawal symptoms. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1990; 101: File SE, Baldwin HA, Hitchcott PK. Flumazenil but not nitrendipine reverses the increased anxiety during ethanol withdrawal in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1989; 98: Finn DA, Crabbe JC. Chronic ethanol differentially alters susceptibility to chemically induced convulsions in withdrawal seizure-prone and -resistant mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1999; 288: Follesa P, Biggio F, Talani G, Murru L, Serra M, Sanna E, et al. Neurosteroids, GABA A receptors, and ethanol dependence. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006; 186: Freund RK, Marley RJ, Wehner JM. Differential sensitivity to bicuculline in three inbred mouse strains. Brain Res Bull 1987; 18: Gauvin DV, Briscoe RJ, Baird TJ, Vallett M, Holloway FA. The paradoxical hedonic valence of acute ethanol withdrawal (hangover) states in rats: place and taste conditioning. Alcohol 1997; 14: Gauvin DV, Dormer KN, Holloway FA. Pentylenetetrazole can induce a conditioned place preference. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1991; 40: Gauvin DV, Goulden KL, Holloway FA. State-dependent stimulus control: cueing attributes of ethanol "hangover" in rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1993; 17:

27 Gauvin DV, Harland RD, Criado JR, Michaelis RC, Holloway FA. The discriminative stimulus properties of ethanol and acute ethanol withdrawal states in rats. Drug Alcohol Depend 1989; 24: Goldstein DB. Relationship of alcohol dose to intensity of withdrawal signs in mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1972; 180: Grahame NJ, Chester JA, Rodd-Henricks K, Li TK, Lumeng L. Alcohol place preference conditioning in high- and low-alcohol preferring selected lines of mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2001; 68: Grahame NJ, Li TK, Lumeng L. Selective breeding for high and low alcohol preference in mice. Behav Genet 1999; 29: Gremel CM, Cunningham CL. Role of test activity in ethanol-induced disruption of place preference expression in mice. Psychopharmacology 2007; 191: Holloway FA, Miller JM, King DA, Bedingfield JB. Delayed ethanol effects on physiological and behavioral indices in the rat. Alcohol 1993; 10: Izquierdo I, Medina JH. GABAA receptor modulation of memory: the role of endogenous benzodiazepines. Trends Pharmacol Sci 1991; 12: Jung ME, Lal H, Gatch MB. The discriminative stimulus effects of pentylenetetrazol as a model of anxiety: recent developments. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2002; 26: Jung ME, Wallis CJ, Gatch MB, Lal H. Sex differences in the pentylenetetrazol-like stimulus induced by ethanol withdrawal. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1999; 291:

28 Jung ME, Wallis CJ, Gatch MB, Lal H. Abecarnil and alprazolam reverse anxiety-like behaviors induced by ethanol withdrawal. Alcohol 2000; 21: Kalant H. Alcohol withdrawal syndromes in the human: comparison with animal models. Adv Exp Med Biol 1977; 85B: Keppel G. Design and Analysis: A researcher s handbook. 3 rd ed. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall Inc; Kliethermes CL, Cronise K, Crabbe JC. Anxiety-like behavior in mice in two apparatuses during withdrawal from chronic ethanol vapor inhalation. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2004; 28: Knapp DJ, Overstreet DH, Breese GR. Modulation of ethanol withdrawal-induced anxiety-like behavior during later withdrawals by treatment of early withdrawals with benzodiazepine/gamma-aminobutyric acid ligands. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2005; 29: Koob GF. Alcoholism: allostasis and beyond. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2003; 27: Lal H, Harris CM, Benjamin D, Springfield AC, Bhadra S, Emmett-Oglesby MW. Characterization of a pentylenetetrazol-like interoceptive stimulus produced by ethanol withdrawal. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1988; 247: Majchrowicz E. Induction of physical dependence upon ethanol and the associated behavioral changes in rats. Psychopharmacologia 1975; 43: McCaul ME, Turkkan JS, Svikis DS, Bigelow GE. Alcohol and secobarbital effects as a function of familial alcoholism: extended intoxication and increased withdrawal effects. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1991; 15:

29 McClearn GE, Wilson JR, Meredith W. The use of isogenic and heterogenic mouse stocks in behavioral research. In: Lindsey G, Thiessen DD, editors. Contributions to Behavior-Genetic Analysis: The Mouse as a Prototype. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; p Metten P, Crabbe JC. Common genetic determinants of severity of acute withdrawal from ethanol, pentobarbital and diazepam in inbred mice. Behav Pharmacol 1994; 5: Metten P, Phillips TJ, Crabbe JC, Tarantino LM, McClearn GE, Plomin R, et al. High genetic susceptibility to ethanol withdrawal predicts low ethanol consumption. Mamm Genome 1998; 9: Morrow AL, Montpied P, Paul SM. GABA A receptor function and expression following chronic ethanol and barbiturate administration. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1991; 625: Morse AC, Schulteis G, Holloway FA, Koob GF. Conditioned place aversion to the "hangover" phase of acute ethanol administration in the rat. Alcohol 2000; 22: Mosher TM, Smith JG, Greenshaw AJ. Aversive stimulus properties of the 5-HT2C receptor agonist WAY in rats. Neuropharmacology 2006; 51: Mucha RF. Is the motivational effect of opiate withdrawal reflected by common somatic indices of precipitated withdrawal? A place conditioning study in the rat. Brain Res 1987; 418: Mucha RF, Iversen SD. Reinforcing properties of morphine and naloxone revealed by conditioned place preferences: a procedural examination. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1984; 82: Neisewander JL, Pierce RC, Bardo MT. Naloxone enhances the expression of morphineinduced conditioned place preference. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1990; 100:

Drug and Alcohol Dependence and Withdrawal Models

Drug and Alcohol Dependence and Withdrawal Models Drug and Alcohol Dependence and Withdrawal Models John C. Crabbe, PhD Department of Behavioral Neuroscience Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR Drug and Alcohol Dependence and Withdrawal

More information

Protective Effect of Mentat (BR-16A) A Herbal Preparation, on Alcohol Abstinence-Induced Anxiety and Convulsions

Protective Effect of Mentat (BR-16A) A Herbal Preparation, on Alcohol Abstinence-Induced Anxiety and Convulsions [Indian Journal of Experimental Biology (1993): (31), 435] Protective Effect of Mentat (BR-16A) A Herbal Preparation, on Alcohol Abstinence-Induced Anxiety and Convulsions Kulkarni, S.K. and Anita Verma,

More information

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION INTRODUCTION Epidemiologic reports indicate that mood disorders in children and adolescents are quite common, with up to 70% of depressed children and adolescents experiencing a recurrence within 5 years

More information

LOW DOSE OF ETHANOL INDUCES CONDITIONED PLACE PREFERENCE IN RATS AFTER REPEATED EXPOSURES TO ETHANOL OR SALINE INJECTIONS

LOW DOSE OF ETHANOL INDUCES CONDITIONED PLACE PREFERENCE IN RATS AFTER REPEATED EXPOSURES TO ETHANOL OR SALINE INJECTIONS Alcohol < Alcoholism Vol. 31, No. 6, pp. 547-553, 1996 LOW DOSE OF ETHANOL INDUCES CONDITIONED PLACE PREFERENCE IN RATS AFTER REPEATED EXPOSURES TO ETHANOL OR SALINE INJECTIONS PRZEMYSLAW BIENKOWSKI 12,

More information

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON OBJECT RECOGNITION. Impairing Effects of Alcohol on Object Recognition. A Senior Honors Thesis

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON OBJECT RECOGNITION. Impairing Effects of Alcohol on Object Recognition. A Senior Honors Thesis Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON OBJECT RECOGNITION Alcohol on Object Recognition 1 Impairing Effects of Alcohol on Object Recognition A Senior Honors Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the

More information

Animal models of ethanol and nicotine interactions

Animal models of ethanol and nicotine interactions Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Presentations 2006: Alcohol and Tobacco Dependence: from Bench to Bedside 2006 Animal models of ethanol and nicotine interactions Allen C.

More information

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Alcohol Clin Exp Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 April 01.

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Alcohol Clin Exp Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2016 April 01. Relative Fluid Novelty Differentially Alters the Time Course of Limited-Access Ethanol and Water Intake in Selectively Bred High Alcohol Preferring Mice David N. Linsenbardt, PhD and Stephen L. Boehm II,

More information

Reference place conditioning procedure with cocaine: Increased sensitivity for measuring associatively motivated choice behavior in rats

Reference place conditioning procedure with cocaine: Increased sensitivity for measuring associatively motivated choice behavior in rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 2010 Reference place conditioning procedure with

More information

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1992. 20 (3). 233-239 The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats JAMES S. MILLER and JOYCE A.

More information

Motivational Effects of Ethanol in DARPP-32 Knock-Out Mice

Motivational Effects of Ethanol in DARPP-32 Knock-Out Mice The Journal of Neuroscience, January 1, 2001, 21(1):340 348 Motivational Effects of Ethanol in DARPP-32 Knock-Out Mice Fred O. Risinger, 1 Pierre A. Freeman, 1 Paul Greengard, 2 and Allen A. Fienberg 2,3

More information

Contribution of the Opioid System to Alcohol Aversion and Alcohol Drinking Behavior 1

Contribution of the Opioid System to Alcohol Aversion and Alcohol Drinking Behavior 1 0022-3565/98/2871-0284$03.00/0 THE JOURNAL OF PHARMACOLOGY AND EXPERIMENTAL THERAPEUTICS Vol. 287, No. 1 Copyright 1998 by The American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics Printed in

More information

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT ACTA NEUROBIOL: EXP. 1988, 48: 117-121 Short communication LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT W. Jeffrey WILSON and Jennifer C. HALL Department of Psychological

More information

NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 2002 VOL. 27, NO American College of Neuropsychopharmacology

NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 2002 VOL. 27, NO American College of Neuropsychopharmacology Conditioned Locomotion Is Not Correlated with Behavioral Sensitization to Cocaine: An Intra-Laboratory Multi-Sample Analysis Gregory Hotsenpiller, Ph.D., and Marina E. Wolf, Ph.D. Pre-clinical and clinical

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Alcohol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 December 01.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Alcohol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 December 01. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Alcohol. 2014 December ; 48(8): 765 772. doi:10.1016/j.alcohol.2014.06.009. The effect of prior alcohol consumption on the ataxic

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

Behavioral Pharmacology

Behavioral Pharmacology Psych 181: Dr. Anagnostaras Lecture 4 Behavioral Pharmacology Behavioral Pharmacology Behavioral Pharmacology The study of the relationship between the physiological actions of drugs and their effects

More information

Seizure Sensitivity and GABAergic Modulation of Ethanol Sensitivity in Selectively Bred FAST and SLOW Mouse Lines 1

Seizure Sensitivity and GABAergic Modulation of Ethanol Sensitivity in Selectively Bred FAST and SLOW Mouse Lines 1 0022-3565/98/2872-0606$03.00/0 THE JOURNAL OF PHARMACOLOGY AND EXPERIMENTAL THERAPEUTICS Vol. 287, No. 2 Copyright 1998 by The American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics Printed in

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

"One-trial tolerance" to the anxiolytic actions of benzodiazepines in the elevated plus-maze, or the development of a phobic state?

One-trial tolerance to the anxiolytic actions of benzodiazepines in the elevated plus-maze, or the development of a phobic state? Psychopharmacology (1993) 11:24-244 Psychopharmacology Springer-Verlag 1993 "One-trial tolerance" to the anxiolytic actions of benzodiazepines in the elevated plus-maze, or the development of a phobic

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Ultra-low-dose naltrexone suppresses rewarding effects of opiates and aversive effects of opiate withdrawal in rats

Ultra-low-dose naltrexone suppresses rewarding effects of opiates and aversive effects of opiate withdrawal in rats Psychopharmacology (2005) 181: 576 581 DOI 10.1007/s00213-005-0022-7 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Mary C. Olmstead. Lindsay H. Burns Ultra-low-dose naltrexone suppresses rewarding effects of opiates and aversive

More information

Effect of Withania somnifera Dunal in ethanol-induced anxiolysis and withdrawal anxiety in rats

Effect of Withania somnifera Dunal in ethanol-induced anxiolysis and withdrawal anxiety in rats Indian Journal of Experimental Biology Vol. 46, June 2008, pp. 470-475 Effect of Withania somnifera Dunal in ethanol-induced anxiolysis and withdrawal anxiety in rats Girdhari Lal Gupta & Avtar Chand Rana

More information

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet University of Amsterdam Supplementary Figures and Legends Day 1 Day 2 Day 3

More information

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University Undergraduate Honors Theses 5-2014 Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? Tarra L. Baker Follow

More information

Repeated Cycles of Binge-Like Ethanol Drinking in C57BL/6J Mice Augments Subsequent Voluntary Ethanol Intake But Not Other Dependence-Like Phenotypes

Repeated Cycles of Binge-Like Ethanol Drinking in C57BL/6J Mice Augments Subsequent Voluntary Ethanol Intake But Not Other Dependence-Like Phenotypes Repeated Cycles of Binge-Like Ethanol Drinking in C57BL/6J Mice Augments Subsequent Voluntary Ethanol Intake But Not Other Dependence-Like Phenotypes Benjamin Reid Cox A thesis submitted to the faculty

More information

DIFFERENT EFFECTS OF STRESS ON ALCOHOL DRINKING BEHAVIOUR IN MALE AND FEMALE MICE SELECTIVELY BRED FOR HIGH ALCOHOL PREFERENCE

DIFFERENT EFFECTS OF STRESS ON ALCOHOL DRINKING BEHAVIOUR IN MALE AND FEMALE MICE SELECTIVELY BRED FOR HIGH ALCOHOL PREFERENCE Alcohol & Alcoholism Vol. 41, No. 1, pp. 44 53, 2006 Advance Access publication 18 November 2005 doi:10.1093/alcalc/agh242 DIFFERENT EFFECTS OF STRESS ON ALCOHOL DRINKING BEHAVIOUR IN MALE AND FEMALE MICE

More information

Journal of Chemical and Pharmaceutical Research

Journal of Chemical and Pharmaceutical Research Available online www.jocpr.com Journal of Chemical and Pharmaceutical Research ISSN No: 0975-7384 CODEN(USA): JCPRC5 J. Chem. Pharm. Res., 2011, 3(5):468-472 Study on the anticonvulsant activity of Pentazocine

More information

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys PII S0091-3057(99)00058-1 Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, Vol. 64, No. 2, pp. 367 371, 1999 1999 Elsevier Science Inc. Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0091-3057/99/$ see front matter Effects

More information

Effects of Repeated Alcohol Deprivations on Operant Ethanol Self-Administration by Alcohol-Preferring (P) Rats

Effects of Repeated Alcohol Deprivations on Operant Ethanol Self-Administration by Alcohol-Preferring (P) Rats (3) 28, 1614 1621 & 3 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0893-133X/03 $25.00 www.neuropsychopharmacology.org Effects of Repeated Alcohol Deprivations on Operant Ethanol Self-Administration by

More information

Figure1. Trials. (ml/5min) (licks/3min) (ml/5min) Fischer. Lewis. Mean Intake Mean Saccharin Intake. Saline Cocaine.

Figure1. Trials. (ml/5min) (licks/3min) (ml/5min) Fischer. Lewis. Mean Intake Mean Saccharin Intake. Saline Cocaine. The classical notion of taste aversion learning originated with radiation sickness as documented in the classical work of John Garcia and his colleagues (see highlight by Garcia). In Garcia s original

More information

Evaluation of Anxiolytic Effect of XXX in Guinea Pig Maternal Separation Test.

Evaluation of Anxiolytic Effect of XXX in Guinea Pig Maternal Separation Test. Evaluation of Anxiolytic Effect of XXX in Guinea Pig Maternal Separation Test. DATE This study was conducted under terms of a Services Agreement between NeuroDetective Inc. and CLIENT, entitled TITLE OF

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Xue Y-X, Deng J-H, Chen Y-Y, et al. Effect of selective inhibition of reactivated nicotineassociated memories with propranolol on nicotine craving. JAMA Psychiatry. Published

More information

Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste

Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (4), 379-390 Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste MICHELLE SYMONDS and GEOFFREY HALL University of York, York, England

More information

Constitutively Active Mu Opioid Receptors Mediate the Enhanced Conditioned Aversive Effect of Naloxone in Morphine-Dependent Mice

Constitutively Active Mu Opioid Receptors Mediate the Enhanced Conditioned Aversive Effect of Naloxone in Morphine-Dependent Mice (2006) 31, 171 177 & 2006 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0893-133X/06 $30.00 www.neuropsychopharmacology.org Constitutively Active Mu Opioid Receptors Mediate the Enhanced Conditioned Aversive

More information

Effects of contextual or olfactory cues previously paired with morphine withdrawal on behavior and pain sensitivity in the rat

Effects of contextual or olfactory cues previously paired with morphine withdrawal on behavior and pain sensitivity in the rat Psychopharmacology (2001) 156:381 387 DOI 10.1007/s002130100743 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Gavan P. McNally Huda Akil Effects of contextual or olfactory cues previously paired with morphine withdrawal on behavior

More information

Supplementary Methods

Supplementary Methods 1 Supplementary Methods Social Preference Test Subjects Seventy-four Long-Evans, male rats served as subjects (S-rats) in the foodpreference test, with 40 assigned to the CXT-Same (CXT-S) Condition and

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5983/1288/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Induction of Fear Extinction with Hippocampal-Infralimbic BDNF Jamie Peters, Laura M. Dieppa-Perea, Loyda M. Melendez,

More information

Running head: ENVIRONMENTAL ENRICHMENT AND ALCOHOL ADDICTION 1

Running head: ENVIRONMENTAL ENRICHMENT AND ALCOHOL ADDICTION 1 Running head: ENVIRONMENTAL ENRICHMENT AND ALCOHOL ADDICTION 1 Environmental Enrichment and Reinstatement of Alcohol Addiction in Mice Julie Rutter This thesis is submitted in partial fulfillment of the

More information

Repeated stress exposure causes strain-dependent shifts in the behavioral economics of cocaine in rats

Repeated stress exposure causes strain-dependent shifts in the behavioral economics of cocaine in rats bs_bs_banneraddiction Biology PRECLINICAL STUDY doi:10.1111/adb.12123 Repeated stress exposure causes strain-dependent shifts in the behavioral economics of cocaine in rats Peter A. Groblewski 1, Chad

More information

Discrete versus cumulative dosing in dose response discrimination studies

Discrete versus cumulative dosing in dose response discrimination studies Ž. European Journal of Pharmacology 326 1997 113 118 Discrete versus cumulative dosing in dose response discrimination studies Martin D. Schechter Department of Pharmacology, Northeastern Ohio UniÕersities

More information

The Effects of Benzodiazepines on Ingestive Behavior. Hannah Dinnen & Ivy Far

The Effects of Benzodiazepines on Ingestive Behavior. Hannah Dinnen & Ivy Far The Effects of Benzodiazepines on Ingestive Behavior Hannah Dinnen & Ivy Far Submitted as partial fulfillment of the senior research thesis requirement of the psychology major at Wofford College 1 Abstract

More information

Prevention of Development of Tolerance and Dependence to Opiate in Mice by BR-16A (Mentat) A Herbal Psychotropic Preparation

Prevention of Development of Tolerance and Dependence to Opiate in Mice by BR-16A (Mentat) A Herbal Psychotropic Preparation [Indian Journal of Experimental Biology (1992): (30), 885] Prevention of Development of Tolerance and Dependence to Opiate in Mice by BR-16A (Mentat) A Herbal Psychotropic Preparation Kulkarni, S.K. and

More information

Pharmacological Profile of BR-16A (Mentat)

Pharmacological Profile of BR-16A (Mentat) [Probe (1995): (XXXIV), 2, 124-139] Pharmacological Profile of BR-16A (Mentat) Kulkarni, S.K. and Verma, A. Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Panjab University, Chandigarh, India. ABSTRACT The present

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories Liz Miller Molly McGinnis Lindsey Richardson A research thesis submitted in partial completion of PSY451 senior research thesis, at Wofford

More information

Second-order conditioning during a compound extinction treatment

Second-order conditioning during a compound extinction treatment Learning and Motivation 38 (2007) 172 192 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Second-order conditioning during a compound extinction treatment Oskar Pineño a, *, Jessica M. Zilski a, Todd R. Schachtman b a Dpto.

More information

Effects of Alprazolam and Fluoxetine on Morphine Sensitization in Mice

Effects of Alprazolam and Fluoxetine on Morphine Sensitization in Mice Physiol. Res. 51: 417-423, 2002 Effects of Alprazolam and Fluoxetine on Sensitization in Mice M. VOTAVA, M. KRŠIAK, V. MORAVEC Department of Pharmacology, Third Faculty of Medicine, Charles University,

More information

Chelsea Kasten, PhD. Postdoctoral Fellow LSU Health Sciences Center New Orleans

Chelsea Kasten, PhD. Postdoctoral Fellow LSU Health Sciences Center New Orleans Chelsea Kasten, PhD Postdoctoral Fellow LSU Health Sciences Center New Orleans Department of Cell Biology & Anatomy 1901 Perdido St, Rm 6103 New Orleans, LA 70130 Phone: (724)263-2693 Email: ckaste@lsuhsc.edu

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

Key words: apomorphine; dopamine; prepulse inhibition; schizophrenia; startle; strain

Key words: apomorphine; dopamine; prepulse inhibition; schizophrenia; startle; strain The Journal of Neuroscience, June 1, 2000, 20(11):4325 4336 Toward Understanding the Biology of a Complex Phenotype: Rat Strain and Substrain Differences in the Sensorimotor Gating- Disruptive Effects

More information

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats

Taste quality and extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of September 1999 Taste quality and extinction of

More information

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Psicológica (2004), 25, 45-65. Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Matías López*, Raúl Cantora*, and Luis Aguado** 1 * Universidad de

More information

Recent Advances in Energy, Environment, Biology and Ecology

Recent Advances in Energy, Environment, Biology and Ecology Acute and long-term effects elicited by psychoactive drugs on 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats: development of a new experimental tool for the study of drug-mediated reward NICOLA SIMOLA Department

More information

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning Chapter 6: Learning 1 Learning 1. In perception we studied that we are responsive to stimuli in the external world. Although some of these stimulus-response associations are innate many are learnt. 2.

More information

Perceptual Learning in Flavor Aversion: Evidence for Learned Changes in Stimulus Effectiveness

Perceptual Learning in Flavor Aversion: Evidence for Learned Changes in Stimulus Effectiveness Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2003, Vol. 29, No. 1, 39 48 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.29.1.39

More information

ATTENUATION OF STRESS-INDUCED BEHAVIORAL DEFICITS BY AZADIRACHTA INDICA (NEEM): ROLE OF SEROTONIN

ATTENUATION OF STRESS-INDUCED BEHAVIORAL DEFICITS BY AZADIRACHTA INDICA (NEEM): ROLE OF SEROTONIN Pak. J. Bot., 38(1): 131-138, 26. ATTENUATION OF STRESS-INDUCED BEHAVIORAL DEFICITS BY AZADIRACHTA INDICA (NEEM): ROLE OF SEROTONIN NOREEN SAMAD, TAHIRA PARVEEN, SAIDA HAIDER AND DARAKHSHAN JABEEN HALEEM

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

Efficacy and the discriminative stimulus effects of negative GABA A modulators, or inverse

Efficacy and the discriminative stimulus effects of negative GABA A modulators, or inverse JPET This Fast article Forward. has not been Published copyedited and on formatted. May 16, The 26 final as version DOI:1.1124/jpet.16.13168 may differ from this version. Efficacy and the discriminative

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Pregnanolone effects on the blood pressure of stress-induced hypertension in rats

Pregnanolone effects on the blood pressure of stress-induced hypertension in rats Acta Physiologica Sinica August June 25 25 2004 2004 56 56 (3) (4) 269-274 471-475 http//www.actaps.com.cn 471 * 130021 (pregnanolone ) (stress-induced hypertension SIH) (0.24 mg/kg) (angiotensin Ang )

More information

Standard Operating Procedure

Standard Operating Procedure 1.0 Purpose: 1.1 An acoustic startle model of sensorimotor gating, in which a weak acoustic stimulus (the pre-pulse) is used to decrease the reflex response (startle) produced by a second, more intense,

More information

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit Denniston, J. C., Savastano, H. I., Blaisdell, A. P., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Cue competition as a retrieval deficit. Learning and Motivation, 34(1): 1-31. (Feb 2003) Published by Elsevier (ISSN: 1095-9122).

More information

Effects of d-amphetamine and Scopolamine on Activity Before and After Shock in Three Mouse Strains'

Effects of d-amphetamine and Scopolamine on Activity Before and After Shock in Three Mouse Strains' Effects of d-amphetamine and Scopolamine on Activity Before and After Shock in Three Mouse Strains' By: Hymie Anisman, Douglas Wahlsten, and Larry Kokkinidis Anisman, H., Wahlsten, D., and Kokkinidis,

More information

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(2),199-203 Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning PETER C. HOLLAND and DESWELL T. FORBES University ofpittsburgh, Pittsburgh,

More information

Complex Trait Genetics in Animal Models. Will Valdar Oxford University

Complex Trait Genetics in Animal Models. Will Valdar Oxford University Complex Trait Genetics in Animal Models Will Valdar Oxford University Mapping Genes for Quantitative Traits in Outbred Mice Will Valdar Oxford University What s so great about mice? Share ~99% of genes

More information

C81ADD Psychology of Addiction. Alcohol. Ethyl alcohol (ethanol) School of Psychology. Tobias Bast.

C81ADD Psychology of Addiction. Alcohol. Ethyl alcohol (ethanol) School of Psychology. Tobias Bast. C81ADD Psychology of Addiction Alcohol Ethyl alcohol (ethanol) Tobias Bast School of Psychology tobias.bast@nottingham.ac.uk 1 Selected aspects of the psychopharmacology of alcohol (ethanol) Primary neuropharmacological

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Understanding Addiction: Why Can t Those Affected Just Say No?

Understanding Addiction: Why Can t Those Affected Just Say No? Understanding Addiction: Why Can t Those Affected Just Say No? 1 The Stigma of Addiction There continues to be a stigma surrounding addiction even among health care workers. Consider the negative opinions

More information

AN EXPERIMENTAL MODEL OF ALCOHOL-INDUCED ANXIETY AND DEPRESSIVE BEHAVIOUR IN RATS

AN EXPERIMENTAL MODEL OF ALCOHOL-INDUCED ANXIETY AND DEPRESSIVE BEHAVIOUR IN RATS ORIGINAL ARTICLES AN EXPERIMENTAL MODEL OF ALCOHOL-INDUCED ANXIETY AND DEPRESSIVE BEHAVIOUR IN RATS Stefka Valcheva-Kuzmanova 1, Miroslav Eftimov 1, Krasimir Kuzmanov 2 ABSTRACT 1 Department of Preclinical

More information

Blocked and test-stimulus exposure effects in perceptual learning re-examined

Blocked and test-stimulus exposure effects in perceptual learning re-examined Behavioural Processes xxx (2004) xxx xxx Blocked and test-stimulus exposure effects in perceptual learning re-examined M del Carmen Sanjuan a,, Gumersinda Alonso b, James Byron Nelson b a Universidad del

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor.

from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor. Learning & Behavior 2010, 38 (1), 68-79 doi:10.3758/lb.38.1.68 Protection from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor BRIDGET L. MCCONNELL AND RAR LPH R. MILLER State University of New York, Binghamton,

More information

The relationship between anxiety and depression in animal models: a study using the forced swimming test and elevated plus-maze

The relationship between anxiety and depression in animal models: a study using the forced swimming test and elevated plus-maze Brazilian Correlation Journal between of Medical plus-maze and and Biological forced Research swimming (1999) behaviors 32: 1121-1126 ISSN 0100-879X Short Communication 1121 The relationship between anxiety

More information

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1965, Vol. 59, No. 1, 13-17 AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 GORDON BOWER, RONALD STARR, AND LEAH LAZAROVITZ Stanford

More information

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus Behavioral Neuroscience 1995. Vol. 109, No. 2, 366-370 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/$3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With

More information

Evaluation of rat ultrasonic vocalizations as predictors of the conditioned aversive effects of drugs

Evaluation of rat ultrasonic vocalizations as predictors of the conditioned aversive effects of drugs Psychopharmacology (2001) 155:35 42 DOI 10.1007/s002130100685 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Jeffrey Burgdorf Brian Knutson Jaak Panksepp Toni S. Shippenberg Evaluation of rat ultrasonic vocalizations as predictors

More information

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING Pergamon Neuroscience Vol. 83, No. 3, pp. 837 841, 1998 Copyright 1998 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved PII: S0306-4522(97)00457-0 0306 4522/98 $19.00+0.00

More information

A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors

A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors Learning & Behavior 29, 37 (4), 35-31 doi:1.3758/lb.37.4.35 A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors DOMINIC M. DWYER Cardiff University,

More information

Conditioned taste aversion produced by the oral ingestion of ethanol in the rat

Conditioned taste aversion produced by the oral ingestion of ethanol in the rat Physiological Psychology 1975, Vol. 3 (4),3]7.321 Conditioned taste aversion produced by the oral ingestion of ethanol in the rat MICHAEL J. ECKARDT University of Oregon Health Sciences Center, Portland,

More information

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear Historical views that thought and emotion were processed separately in the brain Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: So far, fear is the best understood What is fear? Dictionary: A feeling of agitation and

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts By Erin Marie Fleming

More information

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery L132 NT MJA/cla Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 60-73 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.60 Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery Gonzalo P. Urcelay University of Cambridge, Cambridge,

More information

Interaction effects of ethanol and pyrazole in laboratory rodents

Interaction effects of ethanol and pyrazole in laboratory rodents Br. J. Pharmac. (1971), 43, 67-73. Interaction effects of ethanol and pyrazole in laboratory rodents K. BLUM, I. GELLER AND J. E. WALLACE* Department of Experimental Pharmacology, Southwest Foundation

More information

Novel Compounds for Treatment of Alcohol Addiction and Anxiety

Novel Compounds for Treatment of Alcohol Addiction and Anxiety Novel Compounds for Treatment of Alcohol Addiction and Anxiety (OTT ID 1147) Inventors: James Cook, Ph.D.; Michael Van Linn, Ph.D.; Wenyuan Yin, Ph.D. Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry UW-Milwaukee

More information

Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching

Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (2), 189 196 Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching THOMAS R. ZENTALL and LOU M. SHERBURNE University of Kentucky, Lexington,

More information

Alcohol Antagonists 1

Alcohol Antagonists 1 Alcohol Antagonists 1 What is an alcohol antagonist? An alcohol antagonist is a drug that specifically blocks the effects of alcohol. If you take an alcohol antagonist and then drink a bunch of alcohol,

More information

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition Denniston, James C., Robert P. Cole, and Ralph R. Miller. (1998). "The role of temporal relationships in the transfer of conditioned inhibition." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Food restriction: enhancing effects on drug reward and striatal cell signaling

Food restriction: enhancing effects on drug reward and striatal cell signaling Food restriction: enhancing effects on drug reward and striatal cell signaling K.D. Carr Departments of Psychiatry & Pharmacology NYU School of Medicine Common Neural Substrates for Incentive-Motivating

More information

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Hippocampal Pyramidal Neuron of Mice and Rats Figure 1 Open Field Apparatus Open Field Behavior

More information

Main Questions. Why study addiction? Substance Use Disorders, Part 1 Alecia Schweinsburg, MA Abnromal Psychology, Fall Substance Use Disorders

Main Questions. Why study addiction? Substance Use Disorders, Part 1 Alecia Schweinsburg, MA Abnromal Psychology, Fall Substance Use Disorders Substance Use Disorders Main Questions Why study addiction? What is addiction? Why do people become addicted? What do alcohol and drugs do? How do we treat substance use disorders? Why study addiction?

More information

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M.

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M. Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making Johanna M. Jarcho 1,2 Elliot T. Berkman 3 Matthew D. Lieberman 3 1 Department of Psychiatry

More information

The effects of environmental enrichment on nicotine sensitization in a rodent model of schizophrenia

The effects of environmental enrichment on nicotine sensitization in a rodent model of schizophrenia East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University Undergraduate Honors Theses 5-2014 The effects of environmental enrichment on nicotine sensitization in a rodent model

More information

Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place

Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place Preference Stephanie Willis, Jonnique Adjmul, Shabaaz Sandhu, Antoniette M. Maldonado-Devincci, Cheryl Kirsten ABSTRACT The present

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information