Effect of short-term prefeeding and body weight on wheel running and responding reinforced by the opportunity to run in a wheel

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Effect of short-term prefeeding and body weight on wheel running and responding reinforced by the opportunity to run in a wheel"

Transcription

1 Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) 1 10 Effect of short-term prefeeding and body weight on wheel running and responding reinforced by the opportunity to run in a wheel Terry W. Belke a,, W. David Pierce b, K. Jensen c a Department of Psychology, Mount Allison University, Sackville, NB, Canada E4L 1C7 b University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alta., Canada c Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany Received 20 August 2003; received in revised form 23 January 2004; accepted 26 January 2004 Abstract A biobehavioural analysis of activity anorexia suggests that the motivation for physical activity is regulated by food supply and body weight. In the present experiment, food allocation was varied within subjects by prefeeding food-deprived rats 0, 5, 10 and 15 g of food before sessions of lever pressing for wheel-running reinforcement. The experiment assessed the effects of prefeeding on rates of wheel running, lever pressing, and postreinforcement pausing. Results showed that prefeeding animals 5 g of food had no effect. Prefeeding 10 g of food reduced lever pressing for wheel running and rates of wheel running without a significant change in body weight; the effect was, however, transitory. Prefeeding 15 g of food increased the animals body weights, resulting in a sustained decrease of wheel running and lever pressing, and an increase in postreinforcement pausing. Overall the results indicate that the motivation for physical activity is regulated by changes in local food supply, but is sustained only when there is a concomitant change in body weight Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Prefeeding; Body weight; Wheel-running reinforcement; Lever press; Rat 1. Introduction Activity anorexia occurs when rats are placed on food restriction and provided with the opportunity to run. The initial effect is that food intake is reduced, body weight declines and wheel running increases. As running escalates, food intake drops off and body weight plummets downward, further augmenting wheel running and suppressing food intake. The result of this cycle is emaciation and, if allowed to continue, the eventual death of the animal (Epling and Pierce, 1991). Corresponding author. Tel.: ; fax: address: tbelke@mta.ca (T.W. Belke). A biobehavioural analysis of activity anorexia involves evolution and natural selection (Pierce, 2001). For organisms faced with sporadic reductions in food supply (e.g., unpredictable, non-seasonal famines or droughts), natural selection would have favoured increased physical activity (see Mrosovsky and Barnes, 1974 for cyclic reductions in food supply, hibernation, and anorexia; also see Mrosovsky and Sherry, 1980 for a review of natural anorexia). The movement from one decimated food patch to an alternative location offering higher net returns is one of the central principles of optimal foraging theory (Pyke et al., 1977). That is, animals that travelled or migrated under conditions of food restriction and loss of body weight would have contacted food, survived and reproduced; other animals that continued to forage in a food depleted /$ see front matter 2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi: /j.beproc

2 2 T.W. Belke et al. / Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) 1 10 location or patch had less reproductive success. Furthermore, natural selection would have favoured anorexia during times of travel induced by severe food shortages. Under famine conditions, there would be a net negative energy balance between foraging for small, difficult to obtain food items and travelling to a more abundant food source. Animals that stopped to search for food along the way often would use up their energy stores and die. Those that kept going rather than foraging more frequently would have contacted a stable and abundant food source increasing their reproductive fitness (see Epling and Pierce, 1991; Pierce and Epling, 1996). Both the laboratory model and the biobehavioural analysis of activity anorexia suggest that motivation for physical activity or travel is sensitive to changes in food supply and body weight. Numerous studies (e.g., Belke, 1996; Collier, 1970; Jakubczak, 1967; Moskowitz, 1959; Price, 1976; Treichler and Hall, 1962) have shown that physical activity level is sensitive to fluctuations in food supply sufficient to induce changes in body weight. For example, Moskowitz (1959) showed a linear relationship between physical activity, measured as wheel revolutions per hour, and percentage of body weight from an estimated normal weight. The correlation between body weight and wheel revolutions was 0.99 between 60 and 90% of ad libitum body weight. Collier (1970) reported a 0.79 correlation between log weight loss and log distance run over a range of 5 20% body weight loss. Sclafani and Rendel (1978) found that this linear relationship emerged at different body weight levels in lean, normal, and obese rats and that the rate of increase in physical activity was lowest in obese rats and highest in lean animals. While a relationship between body weight loss and activity level is well established, other research suggests that physical activity level may also be sensitive to changes in food supply that are of a shorter term. For example, Kanarek and Collier (1983) showed that running varied with meal frequency. Rats were provided access to running wheels and fed one meal of 60 min, two 30 min meals or four meals of 15 min. Results showed that the greater the meal frequency the more food the animals consumed; more food consumption raised body weight and decreased wheel running over days. Because body weight changed with meal frequency, the changes in wheel running cannot be attributed uniquely to variation in food supply and its allocation. Boer (1989, also reported in Pierce and Epling, 1996), however, indicated that wheel running is sensitive to fluctuations in 4-day food supply. Based on a procedure to induce voluntary prolonged running (Russell et al., 1987), five 6-week-old rats were given the opportunity to run on a wheel with an attached side cage. Over 10 days the animals were fed a fixed-restricted amount of food (15 or 18 g) until wheel running stabilised around the criterion of 6000 revolutions of a 1.1 m wheel (6600 m in a day). On day 10, some animals were fed 15 g of food and others received 18 g, depending on their level of wheel running (above or below 6000 revolutions). Over the next 4 days, the rats were maintained on 15 or 18 g of food; following this period, the daily amount of food was triturated up or down relative to the initial fixed amount, and each time the new food supply was maintained over 4 days. Each titration was followed by a return to the initial fixed amount of food. For example, one rat received the food values of 15, 14, 15, 16, 15, 13, 15, 17, 15, 12, 15, 18, 15, 11, 15, 19, 15, 10, 15 and 20 g, for consecutive 4-day periods. Results showed that the level of wheel running tracked the changes in food supply increasing when food supply dropped and decreasing when the amount of food jumped. Generally, the larger the change in food supply the greater the adjustment in wheel running (up or down). Boer found that changes in wheel running were sensitive to adjustment in food supply but were not associated with local changes in body weight over the 4-day periods. Body weight and wheel running were, however, inversely correlated over the course of the experiment, confirming the findings of previous studies. In addition to experiments on fluctuations in 4-day food supply and meal frequency, there are studies of the immediate effects of a prior meal (prefeeding) on physical activity. In one study, Taylor (1973) investigated the regulation of running speed in a straight alley by short-term food supply over successive days. Experimental conditions involved no prefeeding, prefeeding a nutritive substance (N) and prefeeding a non-nutritive substance (NN). In his first experiment, the N group showed a lower rate of increase in running speed over successive days compared to the other two groups. Under the prefeeding condition

3 T.W. Belke et al. / Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) (free access to food for 30 min), the N group gained weight and ate considerably more than animals in the NN condition. In a second experiment designed to equate the groups on body weight, the N and NN groups were given 8 g of their respective substances 30 min prior to a session. Under these conditions, body weight remained constant, and the groups did not differ in increases in running speed over successive days. Taylor s results suggest that in the absence of a change in body weight, short-term adjustments in food supply do not affect running. Note, however, that the amount of food and non-nutritive substance was held constant at 8 g rather than varied making it questionable to rule out short-term food supply as a controlling variable of physical activity. The present study addresses the motivation to run and its regulation by short-term food supply and body weight. In the current experiment, rats were fed chow prior to sessions of lever pressing maintained by the opportunity to run in a wheel. For each session, animals pressed a lever on tandem fixed ratio (FR) 1 variable interval (VI) schedules for wheel-running reinforcement. In contrast to previous studies, we show the effects of prefeeding on lever pressing for wheel running a measure of motivation to run independent of the physical activity itself. 2. Method 2.1. Subjects Eight male Wistar rats selected from an initial group of 21 rats based on running rates served as experimental subjects. Rats were approximately 75 days old, and between 317 and 360 g in body weight, at the beginning of the experiment (i.e., training). The animals were maintained at approximately 80% of their initial free-feeding body weight throughout the experiment Apparatus The apparatus consisted of four running wheels (three Med Associates ENV 041, one Wahmann) located in soundproof shells. Each wheel was 35.5 cm in diameter. A retractable lever (Med Associates ENV-112) was mounted directly at the opening of each wheel (7 cm 9 cm). The lever extended 1.8 cm into the wheel chamber and when extended was located 8.5 cm above the floor of the wheel chamber. A solenoid-operated brake was attached to the base of each wheel. When the solenoid was operated, a rubber tip attached to a metal shaft contacted the outer rim of the wheel and caused the wheel to stop. A microswitch attached to the wheel frame recorded wheel revolutions. A 24 V dc light was mounted on each side of the frame of the wheel, 20 cm above the base, to illuminate the interior of the wheel and the area of the lever. The design of the apparatus is similar to that portrayed in Fig. 1 in Pierce et al. (1986). Control of experimental events and the recording of data were handled by a Digital PDP 8A computer in an adjacent room Procedure Training and baseline Initially 21 rats were given free access to a running wheel for 30 min each day for 15 days and the number of wheel revolutions was recorded. From this group of 21 rats, 14 were selected as subjects based on the number of revolutions (>500 revolutions/30 min). These 14 subjects were shaped to press a lever in a standard operant conditioning chamber. Each lever press produced a 0.1 ml of 10% sucrose solution. When the rats reliably pressed the lever, the schedule of reinforcement was shifted from a continuous reinforcement schedule (CRF) to a VI 10 s schedule. Throughout the period of lever training (i.e., 15 days), subjects continued to run in wheels for 30 min sessions just prior to sessions in the operant chamber. When lever pressing for sucrose solution appeared to be established, the procedure was discontinued. At this time, the retractable lever in the wheel chamber was made operative and a single lever press produced the opportunity to run in the wheel for 60 s. A session consisted of 50 opportunities to run for 60 s. Each session was divided into five components with 10 reinforcements in each component. Between each component there was a 2 min timeout. During this period, the wheel could not turn (brake activated), the lever retracted, and the lights at the side of the wheel were extinguished. The rats were trained to respond on five VI schedules for wheel running through the following steps. A CRF schedule was in effect in each component for

4 4 T.W. Belke et al. / Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) days; next, the schedule in each component was changed to a variable-ratio 5-response schedule (VR 5). After 4 days, the schedule in each component was changed to a VI schedule. With the change from a response to time-based reinforcement, the operation of the schedule was modified so that the reinforcement interval did not start timing until the first response was made (i.e., tandem FR 1 VI schedules). Thus, after the termination of reinforcement, a new interval was selected, but did not begin to time out until the first press of the lever. This modification was necessary due to the markedly longer postreinforcement pauses (PRP) that tend to follow the termination of wheel-running reinforcers. Without this modification, the reinforcement interval would most likely time out during the PRP and convert a short duration interval schedule into a continuous reinforcement schedule. The initial sequence of VI schedules for the five components was VI 15 s, VI 7.5 s, VI 5 s, VI 7.5 s and VI 15 s. Over the following 15 days, VI 30 s and VI 60 s schedules were introduced to arrive at the final sequence of interval schedules in the baseline condition: VI 60 s, VI 15 s, VI 5 s, VI 7.5 s, and VI 30 s. The programmed interreinforcement intervals for the VI schedules approximated an exponential distribution (Fleshler and Hoffman, 1962). Lever presses and time were recorded for each interval. Wheel revolutions were counted for each obtained opportunity to run. Rats were exposed to the series of reinforcement schedules, described as the final sequence in the previous paragraph, within the same session to obtain response and reinforcement rates on each schedule. The purpose was to fit Herrnstein s (1970, 1974) hyperbolic matching law equation to these rates in order to generate estimates of the parameters, k and Re, that are used to assess changes in reinforcement efficacy (Heyman and Monaghan, 1987). The form of the hyperbolic matching law equation is B 1 = kr 1 (1) R 1 + Re Response rates are measured over a series of reinforcement schedules and Eq. (1) is fit to the obtained reinforcement rates (R 1 ) and response rates (B 1 ). In Eq. (1), k is defined as the asymptotic rate of responding while Re is the rate of reinforcement associated with one half the asymptotic level of responding. Heyman and Monaghan (1987) suggested that k and Re can be interpreted as an indices of motor and motivational components of a reinforced response. Wilkinson s (1961) method of estimating the parameters of a hyperbolic function was used to generate k and Re values. Belke (2000), however, showed that the parameter estimates vary with the order of the schedules when wheel running is used as reinforcement and suggested that changes in efficacy of wheel running as a reinforcer within the session affect the estimated parameters. Consequently, k and Re estimates generated using wheel-running as a reinforcer should interpreted with caution. As a result, the effects of prefeeding in the present article were principally based on session level data Prefeeding conditions One hour prior to the start of the session, animals were given 5, 10, or 15 g of Purina TM rat chow. Each prefeeding condition remained in effect for five consecutive days. After each condition, the rats were returned to baseline feeding for 6 days and then exposed to the next prefeeding value. Half the subjects received the prefeeding conditions in an order of 5, 10, and 15 g while the other half received the order 10, 5, and 15 g. The 15 g prefeeding condition was arranged last because, unlike the 5 and 10 g conditions, 15 g of food was more than average daily intake required to maintain 80% free-feeding body weight. For the 5 and 10 g conditions, after each session, the rats were fed the difference between the amount of food consumed at prefeeding and the amount of food necessary to maintain 80% free-feeding body weight. For example, if an animal required 11 g of food to maintain 80% body weight and ate 5 g of food at prefeeding, then after the session, the rat received 6 g of chow. In this way, body weight over a 24 h period was maintained at 80% of free-feeding level in the 5 and 10 g prefeeding conditions. It was not possible to hold body weight at 80% for the 15 g condition because the ration exceeded the requisite amount; consequently, the 15 g condition always occurred last. All animals were weighed prior to prefeeding, before the commencement of a session, and at the termination of each session. This procedure allowed measures of body weight before prefeeding, weight

5 T.W. Belke et al. / Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) gained from prefeeding, and weight lost from wheel running and metabolism during the session. 3. Results Wheel-running rates for and within each prefeed condition are presented in Table 1 and Fig. 1a, respectively. Table 1 shows wheel-running rates averaged over the 5 days of each prefeed condition and, in the case of the 0 g condition, the 5 days before prefeeding manipulations began. These data suggest that prefeeding affected subsequent wheel running when the amount fed was greater than 5 g. A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a significant effect of prefeeding on wheel-running rate over sessions, F (3,21) = 44.66, P = Post hoc Dunnett s t-tests showed that wheel-running rates were significantly reduced in the 10 and 15 g conditions compared with the 0 g baseline, t (21) = 4.77, P = 0.001, and t (21) = 10.04, P = 0.001, respectively. Fig. 1a shows the average wheel-running rates across successive days of each prefeeding condition. While wheel-running rates collapsed across days showed that running was decreased by 10 and 15 g, wheel-running rates within each condition showed that the effect on running differed. In the 10 g prefeeding condition, wheel-running rates were reduced on the first day but recovered toward baseline levels over successive days. For the 10 g condition, wheel-running Fig. 1. (a) Mean wheel-running rates (revolutions/min), (b) local lever-pressing rates (presses/min), (c) median postreinforcement pauses (s), and (d) body weight (g) prior to prefeeding across the five successive days of the 0, 5, 10, and 15 g prefeed conditions. Standard errors are provided for means.

6 6 T.W. Belke et al. / Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) 1 10 Table 1 Mean running rates (revolutions/min), lever-pressing rates (lever presses/min), median postreinforcement pause (s) and body weight (g) per session for each rat for the 0, 5, 10 and 15 g prefeed conditions Rat 0g 5g 10g 15g Revolutions/min A (0.5) 42.7 (1.6) 39.8 (1.8) 27.3 (0.8) A (0.9) 40.9 (0.5) 35.5 (0.7) 30.9 (0.7) A (0.6) 34.0 (0.8) 32.5 (1.0) 26.2 (0.3) A (0.5) 42.8 (0.7) 37.7 (1.5) 37.7 (0.9) A (0.9) 35.1 (0.8) 29.8 (0.5) 25.0 (0.6) A (1.0) 35.0 (1.2) 28.4 (2.0) 23.5 (1.4) A (0.9) 36.6 (0.8) 34.1 (1.3) 28.6 (0.8) A (0.8) 38.4 (0.9) 32.3 (0.8) 29.4 (0.7) Mean 38.4 (1.4) 38.2 (1.3) 33.7 (1.4) 28.6 (1.6) Lever presses/min A (1.0) 31.9 (2.1) 28.8 (2.2) 23.1 (0.5) A (1.8) 38.9 (1.1) 39.7 (2.0) 29.4 (1.6) A (1.3) 24.0 (1.5) 31.4 (2.3) 24.7 (0.8) A (0.5) 30.2 (0.6) 28.1 (0.5) 25.7 (0.7) A (0.6) 35.8 (0.7) 29.1 (0.5) 26.6 (1.3) A (1.2) 38.3 (2.3) 27.0 (2.0) 19.2 (1.1) A (0.6) 37.1 (0.8) 36.7 (0.9) 27.3 (1.1) A (2.5) 53.1 (0.7) 39.4 (2.4) 35.1 (2.6) Mean 35.2 (2.0) 36.2 (3.0) 32.5 (1.9) 26.4 (1.6) Median postreinforcement pause (s) A (28.8) 30.9 (30.4) 33.8 (40.9) 37.1 (41.4) A (29.2) 22.7 (24.1) 23.6 (26.4) 40.3 (65.1) A (11.9) 12.9 (11.6) 14.8 (11.9) 15.8 (14.5) A (18.8) 14.9 (28.3) 18.6 (37.3) 15.8 (42.3) A (15.9) 23.0 (25.5) 30.1 (31.2) 35.2 (47.2) A (24.8) 24.4 (32.8) 29.9 (40.1) 29.0 (49.7) A (24.9) 24.0 (43.2) 32.3 (44.4) 39.2 (77.6) A (54.8) 44.4 (42.9) 45.1 (59.5) 76.7 (66.4) Mean 24.5 (3.9) 24.6 (3.5) 28.5 (3.4) 36.1 (6.8) Body weight (g) A (1.8) (0.9) (1.0) (2.7) A (0.9) (0.2) (0.6) (3.2) A (0.8) (0.4) (2.0) (2.1) A (0.6) (1.2) (0.4) (1.3) A (0.6) (1.5) (0.4) (2.6) A (1.1) (0.7) (0.5) (4.0) A (0.7) (0.7) (0.4) (2.9) A (1.0) (0.7) (0.5) (4.2) Mean (3.9) (3.5) (3.7) (3.7) Standard errors are provided for each mean and interquartile ranges are provided for each median. rates differed across days, F (4,28) = 5.58, P = 0.002, and a linear contrast confirmed that wheel running rates increased across days, F (1,28) = 11.09, P = For the 15 g condition, wheel-running rates were reduced and did not change across days, F (4,28) = 0.69, ns. For the baseline condition, across days, wheel-running rates did not differ, F (4,28) = 0.33, ns. For the 5 g prefeeding condition, running rates also did not differ across days, F (4,28) = 1.81, ns, and did not differ from baseline rates. Lever-pressing rates for and within each prefeed condition are presented in Table 1 and Fig. 1b, respectively. Like wheel-running rates, lever-pressing rates were also affected by prefeeding, F (3,21) = 10.92, P = 0.001; however, relative to 0 g, rates were only lower in the 15 g condition, t (21) = 4.67, P = Lever-pressing rates within prefeeding conditions showed the same pattern as wheel-running rates. Lever-pressing rates did not differ across days in the 0g, F (4,28) = 0.60, ns, or 5 g conditions, F (4,28) = 0.88, ns. For the 10 g condition, lever-pressing rates were reduced on the first day and recovered toward baseline over subsequent days. For the 10 g condition, the overall F-value was not significant, F (4,28) = 2.14, P = 0.10; however, a linear contrast confirmed that, like wheel-running rates, lever-pressing rates increased across successive days, F (1,28) = 6.58, P = For the 15 g condition, rates were reduced, but did not significantly differ across days, F (4,28) = 2.54, ns. Median PRPs for and within each prefeed condition are presented in Table 1 and Fig. 1c, respectively. Median rather than mean PRP durations were used because of the sensitivity of means to extreme values. Since parametric analyses of medians are performed with medians of time data in reaction time experiments, a similar approach was taken with PRP medians. Prefeeding had a significant effect on postreinforcement pause duration, F (3,21) = 7.73, P = 0.001, with longer PRPs in the 15 g condition than in the 0 g baseline, t (21) = 4.205, P = Nonparametric analyses yielded similar results, Friedman ANOVA, χ 2 = (3, n = 8), P = 0.001; Wilcoxon matched pairs, 0 g versus 10 g, Z = 1.96, P = 0.05; 0 g versus 15 g, Z = 2.52, P = Within prefeed conditions, median PRP durations were more varied and less systematic than were running and lever-pressing rates. In general, no systematic trends appear evident for any condition. For the 10 g condition, a paired t-test showed that median PRP durations on the last day did not differ significantly from PRPs on the first day (t (7) = 1.57, ns). For the 15 g condition, although

7 T.W. Belke et al. / Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) PRPs do not appear to change systematically across successive days, median PRP duration on the last day was shorter than on the first (t (7) = 3.49, P = 0.01) (Wilcoxon matched pairs, Z = 2.24, P = 0.025). Body weight, measured before prefeeding, for and within each prefeed condition are presented in Table 1 and Fig. 1d, respectively. Analysis of body weight by prefeeding condition revealed a significant effect, F (3,21) = 94.70, P = 0.001, with weight significantly higher than baseline in the 15 g, t (21) = 14.28, P = 0.001, but not the 5 or 10 g conditions, t (21) = 0.76 and 0.85, both ns. Fig. 1d shows that within each condition, body weight was maintained at a constant level over days in the 0, 5, and 10 g prefeeding conditions. In contrast, and as expected, body weight increased across successive days in the 15 g condition. A repeated measures ANOVA with prefeeding condition (0, 5, 10) and days (1 5) the within-subject variables revealed no significant main effect of prefeeding condition, F (2,14) = 0.26, ns, or days, F (4,28) = 0.49, ns and no interaction, F (8,56) = 0.85, ns. However, inclusion of the 15 g prefeeding condition into the analysis yielded significant main effects of prefeeding condition, F (3,21) = 94.54, P = 0.001, and days, F (4,28) = 10.83, P = as well as a significant interaction, F (12,84) = 11.75, P = In addition, a linear contrast confirmed that body weight increased across days in the 15 g condition, F (1,28) = 33.63, P = Fig. 2 presents the hyperbolic curves for the relationship between response and reinforcement rates for each prefeed condition. Table 2 shows the associated estimates of k, Re, and variance accounted for each rat. Fig. 2. Hyperbolic curves relating response and reinforcement rates for the 0, 5, 10, and 15 g prefeed conditions. For this analysis, data for the 0 g condition were averaged from the five sessions that preceded each prefeeding condition. Repeated measures ANOVAs revealed no significant effect of prefeeding on asymptotic levels of responding (k), F (3,21) = 0.86, ns; however, prefeeding did affect Re, F (3,21) = 3.88, P = Relative to baseline, Re was significantly higher in the 15 g condition, Dunnett t (21) = 2.61, P = Increases in the value of Re are generally interpreted as a decrease in the reinforcing efficacy of the programmed reinforcer, the opportunity to run in a wheel. Table 3 shows, for the 5, 10, and 15 g conditions, that the body weights of the animals prior to Table 2 Estimates of k, Re, and variance accounted for each the 0, 5, 10, and 15 g prefeed conditions Rat k Re %VAC 0g 5g 10g 15g 0g 5g 10g 15g 0g 5g 10g 15g A A A A A A A A Mean

8 8 T.W. Belke et al. / Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) 1 10 Table 3 Body weights (g) prior to prefeeding (before), increases in measured weight (g) following the prefeeding period (in), and decrease in weight (g) following the wheel-running session (out) for the 5, 10, and 15 g conditions Rat Prefeed condition 0g 5g 10g 15g Out Before In Out Before In Out Before In Out A3 4.1 (0.6) (0.4) 7.4 (0.5) (1.0) 8.2 (0.7) (0.8) 7.6 (0.4) A9 5.0 (0.4) (0.8) 7.0 (0.8) (1.0) 7.3 (0.7) (1.1) 7.4 (0.7) A (0.5) (1.0) 6.8 (0.4) (1.4) 5.2 (0.9) (1.2) 7.4 (1.3) A (0.8) (0.4) 6.2 (0.8) (1.4) 9.2 (1.0) (2.4) 9.8 (0.9) A (0.8) (0.7) 8.6 (0.2) (0.9) 7.8 (0.6) (1.1) 10.2 (0.9) A (0.6) (0.9) 5.8 (0.8) (1.7) 5.4 (1.8) (0.9) 7.2 (0.6) A (0.7) (1.1) 7.6 (0.6) (0.4) 6.6 (0.6) (1.3) 8.2 (0.7) A (0.9) (0.5) 12.6 (0.4) (1.0) 8.6 (0.9) (1.4) 12.6 (1.2) Mean 4.8 (0.5) (0.8) 7.8 (0.8) (0.8) 7.3 (0.5) (1.5) 8.8 (0.7) For the 0 g condition, only the decrease in weight following the wheel-running session is given. Standard errors are reported in parentheses. prefeeding increase following consumption of the food they were prefed, and decrease following the experimental session (after a session of wheel-running reinforcement). On average, the body weights of the animals increased by 10.00, 18.98, and g in the 5, 10, and 15 g prefeeding conditions, respectively. A repeated measures ANOVA confirmed that body weights increased with amount of prefeeding, F (2,14) = , P = Following sessions of wheel running, weight losses were 4.84, 7.75, 7.30, and 8.80 g on average for the 0, 5, 10, and 15 g prefeeding conditions. Data for the 0 g condition were obtained from the difference between weight going into and coming out of the running wheel taken on the 5 days following the 15 g prefeeding condition. A repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant effect of prefeeding on body weight loss, F (3,21) = 15.46, P = Post hoc comparisons showed that more weight was lost when the animals were prefed than when they were not, t (21) = 4.83, 4.00, and 6.57 all significant at P = Discussion In the present study, prefeeding regulated wheel running and responding for the opportunity to run; however, the effect depended on (1) the amount of prefeeding and (2) an associated change in body weight. Prefeeding the animals 5 g ( 40%) of their daily food allotment prior to a session without a change in body weight did not reduce wheel running or responding for the opportunity to run. Prefeeding 10 g ( 80%) of their daily food allotment under the same conditions, decreased wheel running and lever pressing for the opportunity to run; however, the decrease was transitory and diminished over successive days. Prefeeding the animals 15 g ( 125%) of their daily food allotment prior to a session produced both immediate and sustained decreases in wheel-running and lever-pressing rates and an increase in postreinforcement pauses. Response rate functions showed no effect of prefeeding on response rate asymptotes (k) and an increase in Re in the 15 g condition as the function rose less rapidly toward the asymptote. The increase in Re suggests that the reinforcing efficacy of wheel running was reduced by the 15 g prefeeding load and associated increase in body weight. One potential confound is that feeding the animals before a session added bulk to their digestive systems and that the decline in wheel running could be a function of the added bulk. Going into the wheels, the body weights of the animals increased an average of 10.00, 18.98, and g in the 5, 10, and 15 g conditions, respectively. These weight gains reflect the amount of food plus water consumed during the prefeeding period. Hamilton (1969) showed that while consumption of non-nutritive bulk decreased running in rats, it did not affect lever pressing for heat reinforcement. In the present experiment, the reduc-

9 T.W. Belke et al. / Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) tion in wheel running could reflect digestive overload, but it is unlikely that changes in lever pressing and postreinforcement pausing are due to added bulk. The results of the present study also appear consistent with those obtained by Taylor (1973) for rats running in a runway. In the absence of a change in body weight prefeeding reduced running speed, though not significantly. In the presence of a change in body weight, prefeeding significantly reduced running speed. The results for the 10 g condition are consistent with the conclusions from Boer (1989) that wheel running is sensitive to food supply without local changes in body weight. That is, in the present study, prefeeding the animals 10 g of food reduced wheel running and this occurred without an increase in weight. However, the decrease in wheel running was transitory with 10 g prefeeding, but was sustained in the 15 g condition where prefeeding generated weight gain. One problem is that wheel-running rates did not systematically decrease as body weight increased from 280 to 300 g on average. This suggests that there could be a lag between changes in body weight and changes in wheel running; such a lag would have implications for Boer s (1989) claim that 4-day changes in food supply regulate wheel running without changes in local body weight. Although we did not find co-variation of wheel-running rates with body weight over the 5 days of 15 g prefeeding, wheel running did decline for these same rats when body weight was raised from 80 to 100% of ad libitum weight (Belke, 1996). These data also address the effect of postsession feeding on wheel running and responding for the opportunity to run. In the present experiment, feeding occurred shortly after the completion of a session; one possibility is that within session lever pressing for wheel running and bouts of running were a chain maintained by postsession feeding as the terminal reinforcement. The results from the present study are inconsistent with this account. In the 10 g condition, running and lever-pressing for the opportunity to run were maintained despite the fact that animals received almost all of their daily food allotment before the session (only 1 2 g postsession). Furthermore, we would have expected the animals to show declines in running and lever pressing over successive days in the 10 g condition as the animal repeatedly experienced the greatly reduced postsession feeding. Instead, running and lever pressing increased. Finally, in the 15 g condition, where the animals received no postsession feeding, lever-pressing and wheel-running rates were only reduced by an average of 25 and 25.5%, respectively. In general, the effect of prefeeding on motivation for physical activity is consistent with the biobehavioural analysis of activity anorexia proposed by Epling and Pierce (1991; also Pierce and Epling, 1996). A change in food supply sufficient to stop food-related searching or dispersal would have involved gains in food intake sufficient to produce stable gains in body weight. If the amount of food produced only transitory weight gains (i.e., through gut-fullness), then the motivation to travel would not decline. Both of these effects were demonstrated in the present experiment for the 10 and 15 g prefeeding conditions. Accordingly, animals would vary their range of foraging based on food density (e.g., quantity and frequency of meals), and abandon the patch only when reduced food supply began to impact body weight. Overall, motivation for physical activity would be most sensitive to changes in food allocation that were sufficient to produce gains (or losses) in animals body weights. Taken together, the results suggest that motivation for physical activity is mediated through changes in food intake and body weight. That is, gains in food consumption decrease the motivation for wheel running; reduced motivation for physical activity is, however, more sustained if food intake is sufficient to increase body weight. In other words, enduring changes in the motivation for physical activity occur when food intake is greater than day-to-day fluctuations in food supply related to foraging. References Belke, T.W., The effect of a change in body weight on running and responding reinforced by the opportunity to run. Psychol. Rec. 46, Belke, T.W., Studies of wheel-running reinforcement: parameters of Herrnstein s (1970) response-strength equation vary with schedule order. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 73, Boer, D.P., Determinants of excessive activity in activity anorexia. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. University of Alberta, Edmonton. Collier, G.H., Work: a weak reinforcer. Trans. NY Acad. Sci. 32, Epling, W.F., Pierce, W.D., Solving the Anorexia Puzzle: A Scientific Approach. Hogrefe and Huber, Toronto, Ont.

10 10 T.W. Belke et al. / Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) 1 10 Fleshler, M., Hoffman, H.S., A progression for generating variable interval schedules. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 5, Hamilton, C.L., Ingetion of nonnutritive bulk and wheel running in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 69, Herrnstein, R.J., On the law of effect. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 13, Herrnstein, R.J., Formal properties of the matching law. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 21, Heyman, G.M., Monaghan, M.M., Effects of changes in response requirement and deprivation on the parameters of the matching law equation: new data and review. J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Process. 13, Jakubczak, L.F., Age differences in the effects of terminal food deprivation (starvation) on activity, weight loss, and survival of rats. J. Gerontol. 22, Kanarek, R.B., Collier, G.H., Self-starvation: a problem of overriding the satiety signal? Physiol. Behav. 30, Moskowitz, M.J., Running-wheel activity in the white rat as a function of combined food and water deprivation. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 52, Mrosovsky, N., Barnes, D.S., Anorexia, food deprivation and hibernation. Physiol. Behav. 12, Mrosovsky, N., Sherry, D.F., Animal anorexias. Science 207, Pierce, W.D., Activity anorexia: biological, behavioral, and neural levels of selection. Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 551. Pierce, W.D., Epling, W.F., Boer, D.P., Deprivation and satiation: the interrelations between food and wheel running. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 46, Pierce, W.D., Epling, W.F., Theoretical developments in activity anorexia. In: Epling, W.P., Pierce, W.D. (Eds.), Activity Anorexia: Theory, Research, and Treatment. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp Price, E.O., Food deprivation effects on the running-wheel activity of wild and domestic Norway rats. Behav. Process. 1, Pyke, G.H., Pulliam, H.R., Charnov, E.L., Optimal foraging: a selective review of theory and tests. Quart. Rev. Biol. 52, Russell, J.C., Epling, W.F., Pierce, W.D., Amy, R., Boer, D., Induction of voluntary prolonged running by rats. J. Appl. Physiol. 63, Sclafani, A., Rendel, A., Food deprivation-induced activity in dietary obese, dietary lean, and normal-weight rats. Behav. Biol. 24, Taylor, C., Effects of prefeeding on performance with body weight held constant. Learn. Motivat. 4, Treichler, F.R., Hall, J.F., The relationship between deprivation weight loss and several measures of activity. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 55, Wilkinson, G.N., Statistical estimation in enzyme kinetics. Biochem. J. 80,

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 00, 79, 6 NUMBER (MARCH) RESPONDING FOR SUCROSE AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: EFFECTS OF SUCROSE CONCENTRATION AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCER DURATION

More information

81 - k R1. (1 ) R1 + Re

81 - k R1. (1 ) R1 + Re 484 BELKE AND NEUBAUER increase in the release of dopamine in the mesolimbic pathway that is associated with the biological basis of reward (Lambert, 1992; Watson, Trujillo, Herman, & Akil, 1989). The

More information

Food and Water Intake and Weight Regulation in the Pigeon

Food and Water Intake and Weight Regulation in the Pigeon Physiology and Behavior Vol. 8, pp. 127-134. Brain Research Publications, Inc., 1972. Printed in U.S.A. Food and Water Intake and Weight Regulation in the Pigeon H. PHILIP ZEIGLER 1, H.L. GREEN AND J.SIEGEL

More information

k/2. Thus, Re is measured in the same units

k/2. Thus, Re is measured in the same units JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR INCREASING AND SIGNALING BACKGROUND REINFORCEMENT: EFFECT ON THE FOREGROUND RESPONSE-REINFORCER RELATION TERRY W. BELKE AND GENE M. HEYMAN HARVARD UNIVERSITY

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand).

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). http://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/ Research Commons at the University of Waikato Copyright Statement: The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). The thesis

More information

THE EFFECT OF CHANGES IN AVAILABILITY ON FOOD SELECTION AT DIFFERENT LEVELS OF DEPRiVATION. TERRY W. BELKE Mount Allison University

THE EFFECT OF CHANGES IN AVAILABILITY ON FOOD SELECTION AT DIFFERENT LEVELS OF DEPRiVATION. TERRY W. BELKE Mount Allison University The Psychological Record, 2000, 50, 529-541 THE EFFECT OF CHANGES IN AVAILABILITY ON FOOD SELECTION AT DIFFERENT LEVELS OF DEPRiVATION TERRY W. BELKE Mount Allison University TAMMI Y. KWAN Dalhousie University

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior.

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR TECHNIQUES FOR ESTABLISHING SCHEDULES WITH WHEEL RUNNING AS REINFORCEMENT IN RATS IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA 1993, 60, 219-238 NUMBER 1

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (3), 264-270 Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pullman, Washington 99163 Five pigeons

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a Effects of Food Deprivation on Memory Performance. Jacob. L. Kerewaro Abstract The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)

More information

Interaction of social and individual learning in food preferences of Norway rats

Interaction of social and individual learning in food preferences of Norway rats ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1, 62, 41 46 doi:1.16/anbe..1721, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Interaction of social and individual learning in food preferences of Norway rats BENNETT G. GALEF,

More information

Ethanol regulated preference in rats

Ethanol regulated preference in rats Psychopharmacology (1993) 112:259 269 Psychopharmacology Springer-Verlag 1993 Ethanol regulated preference in rats Gene M. Heyman Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge,

More information

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction PUNISHMENT A ND RECO VER Y D URING FIXED-RA TIO PERFORMA NCE' NATHAN H. AZRIN2 ANNA STATE HOSPITAL When a reinforcement is delivered according to a fixed-ratio schedule, it has been found that responding

More information

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 5, NUMBF- 4 OCITOBER, 1 962 THE EFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES OF PRIMARY AND CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT JOAN G. STEVENSON1 AND T. W. REESE MOUNT HOLYOKE

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 93 102 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND

More information

FALSIFICATION OF MATCHING THEORY: CHANGES IN THE ASYMPTOTE OF HERRNSTEIN S HYPERBOLA AS A FUNCTION OF WATER DEPRIVATION J.JMCDOWELL AND JESSE DALLERY

FALSIFICATION OF MATCHING THEORY: CHANGES IN THE ASYMPTOTE OF HERRNSTEIN S HYPERBOLA AS A FUNCTION OF WATER DEPRIVATION J.JMCDOWELL AND JESSE DALLERY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1999, 72, 251 268 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) FALSIFICATION OF MATCHING THEORY: CHANGES IN THE ASYMPTOTE OF HERRNSTEIN S HYPERBOLA AS A FUNCTION OF WATER DEPRIVATION

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules

Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules Animal Learning & Behavior 1977, 5 (1),11-114 Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pul/man, Washington

More information

Variability as an Operant?

Variability as an Operant? The Behavior Analyst 2012, 35, 243 248 No. 2 (Fall) Variability as an Operant? Per Holth Oslo and Akershus University College Correspondence concerning this commentary should be addressed to Per Holth,

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 89 (2012) 212 218 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes j o ur nal homep age : www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Providing a reinforcement history

More information

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia AM. ZOOI.OCIST, 12:507-512 (1972). Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia PAUL DOWNEY AND BEHRUS JAHAN-PARWAR Biology Department, Clark University, Worcester, Massachusetts 01610 and Worcester Foundation

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

The Effects of Circadian Entrainment on Operant Conditioning

The Effects of Circadian Entrainment on Operant Conditioning Illinois Wesleyan University Digital Commons @ IWU Honors Projects Psychology 199 The Effects of Circadian Entrainment on Operant Conditioning Karen M. Lionello '9 Illinois Wesleyan University Recommended

More information

NUMBER: /2007

NUMBER: /2007 Purpose PAGE 1 OF 8 The purpose of this policy is to create clear guidelines and procedures regarding the use of diet control in behavioral studies so that they match USDA guidelines. This procedure is

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

Fitting a Single-Phase Model to the Post-Exercise Changes in Heart Rate and Oxygen Uptake

Fitting a Single-Phase Model to the Post-Exercise Changes in Heart Rate and Oxygen Uptake Fitting a Single-Phase Model to the Post-Exercise Changes in Heart Rate and Oxygen Uptake R. STUPNICKI, T. GABRYŚ, U. SZMATLAN-GABRYŚ, P. TOMASZEWSKI University of Physical Education, Warsaw, Poland Summary

More information

The effects of caffeine on operant behavior in the Mongolian gerbil

The effects of caffeine on operant behavior in the Mongolian gerbil University of Richmond UR Scholarship Repository Honors Theses Student Research 1989 The effects of caffeine on operant behavior in the Mongolian gerbil Stephanie L. Myers Follow this and additional works

More information

INDUCED POLYDIPSIA1 JOHN L. FALK UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN. a large number of these small meals. Under. CRF, inter-reinforcement time is short and

INDUCED POLYDIPSIA1 JOHN L. FALK UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN. a large number of these small meals. Under. CRF, inter-reinforcement time is short and JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 9, NUMBER I JANUARY, 1966 THE MOTIVATIONAL PROPERTIES OF SCHEDULE- INDUCED POLYDIPSIA1 JOHN L. FALK UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN Schedule-induced polydipsia

More information

Overview. Simple Schedules of Reinforcement. Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement. Combined Schedules of Reinforcement BEHP 1016

Overview. Simple Schedules of Reinforcement. Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement. Combined Schedules of Reinforcement BEHP 1016 BEHP 1016 Why People Often Make Bad Choices and What to Do About It: Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement F. Charles Mace, Ph.D., BCBA-D University of Southern Maine with Jose Martinez-Diaz,

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

Transfer of persistence across reinforced behaviors

Transfer of persistence across reinforced behaviors Animal Learning & Behavior 1979, 7 (4), 493-498 Transfer of persistence across reinforced behaviors ROBERT EISENBERGER University ofdelaware, Newark, Delaware 19711 ROBERT TERBORG Calvin College, Grand

More information

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1998, 70, 165 183 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

Department of Psychology, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48104

Department of Psychology, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48104 BEHAVIORAL BIOLOGY, 8, 749-754 (1973), Abstract No. I228R Effects of Prior Exposure to Animate Objects on Approach Tendency in Chicks SUSAN SAEGERT and D. W. RAJECKI 1 Department of Psychology, The University

More information

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 42, No. 4, 401 414 2329-8456/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000112 Contextual

More information

CORRELATED DELAY OF REINFORCEMENT 1

CORRELATED DELAY OF REINFORCEMENT 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1961, Vol. 54, No. 2, 196-203 CRRELATED DELAY F REINFRCEMENT 1 It is well established that variations in an operant response can be differentiated by

More information

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques C O N F I D E N T I A L Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques DATE This study was conducted under the terms of a Materials Transfer and Services Agreement between NeuroDetective International and

More information

HEDONIC AND MOTORIC EFFECTS OF

HEDONIC AND MOTORIC EFFECTS OF JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR A PARAMETRIC EVALUATION OF THE HEDONIC AND MOTORIC EFFECTS OF DRUGS: PIMOZIDE AND AMPHETAMINE GENE M. HEYMAN HARVARD UNIVERSITY 1983, 40, 113-122 NUMBER

More information

EFFECTS OF ECONOMY TYPE, REINFORCER MAGNITUDE, AND NICOTINE ON THE ESSENTIAL VALUE OF FOOD IN RATS

EFFECTS OF ECONOMY TYPE, REINFORCER MAGNITUDE, AND NICOTINE ON THE ESSENTIAL VALUE OF FOOD IN RATS EFFECTS OF ECONOMY TYPE, REINFORCER MAGNITUDE, AND NICOTINE ON THE ESSENTIAL VALUE OF FOOD IN RATS By RACHEL CASSIDY A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information

ALLISTON K. REID Duke University, Durham, North Carolina

ALLISTON K. REID Duke University, Durham, North Carolina Animal Learning & Behavior 1985, 13 (3), 321-326 Schedule induction and the temporal distributions of adjunctive behavior on periodic water schedules ALLISTON K. REID Duke University, Durham, North Carolina

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1974 Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer Richard H. Weiss Western

More information

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 3-2016 Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Andrew R. Craig Utah State University

More information

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Behavioural Processes 66 (2004) 213 233 Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Raymond C. Pitts, Stacy

More information

Variation in Herrnsein s r<sub>0</sub> as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate

Variation in Herrnsein s r<sub>0</sub> as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate Illinois Wesleyan University From the SelectedWorks of James Dougan 1985 Variation in Herrnsein s r0 as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate James Dougan, Illinois Wesleyan University

More information

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR LICK-SHOCK CONTINGENCIES IN THE RATT1 VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY 1972, 17, 119-125 NUMBER I (JANUARY) Hungry rats were allowed to lick an 8%

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Illinois Wesleyan University Digital Commons @ IWU Honors Projects Psychology 2000 Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Sara J. Estle '00 Illinois Wesleyan University Recommended Citation

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning Operant Conditioning The nature of reinforcement: Premack s probability differential theory Response deprivation

More information

fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1

fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1 BEHAVIORAL AND NEURAL BIOLOGY 36, 298-303 (1982) BRIEF REPORT Food-Aversion Conditioning in Japanese Monkeys (Macaca fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1 TETSURO MATSUZAWA Primate Research Institute,

More information

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL'

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1973, 2, 393-43 NUMBER 3 (NOVEMBER) PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' M. C. DAVISON AND W. TEMPLE UNIVERSITY OF AUCKLAND AND

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 84 (2010) 511 515 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Changes in stimulus control during guided skill

More information

Acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia by rats in proximity to upcoming food delivery

Acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia by rats in proximity to upcoming food delivery Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 491 499 Acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia by rats in proximity to upcoming food delivery GINESA LÓPEZ-CRESPO and MIGUEL RODRÍGUEZ Universidad de Almería, Almería,

More information

ANNA STATE HOSPITAL. peated measurement of the attack. A mechanical

ANNA STATE HOSPITAL. peated measurement of the attack. A mechanical JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 8, NUMBER 3 MAY, 1965 THE OPPORTUNITY FOR AGGRESSION AS AN OPERANT REINFORCER DURING AVERSIVE STIMULATION' N. H. AZRIN, R. R. HUTCHINSON, AND R.

More information

PHYSIOEX 3.0 EXERCISE 33B: CARDIOVASCULAR DYNAMICS

PHYSIOEX 3.0 EXERCISE 33B: CARDIOVASCULAR DYNAMICS PHYSIOEX 3.0 EXERCISE 33B: CARDIOVASCULAR DYNAMICS Objectives 1. To define the following: blood flow; viscosity; peripheral resistance; systole; diastole; end diastolic volume; end systolic volume; stroke

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

Furthermore, added choline may exert relatively little effect when. naturally occurring lipotropic factors are present in appreciable amounts

Furthermore, added choline may exert relatively little effect when. naturally occurring lipotropic factors are present in appreciable amounts 343 6I2.352.2:547.922 THE EFFECTS OF CHOLESTEROL AND CHOLINE ON LIVER FAT BY C. H. BEST AND JESSIE H. RIDOUT (From the School of Hygiene, University of Toronto) (Received January 27, 1936) THE results

More information

Strategies to Develop Food Frequency Questionnaire

Strategies to Develop Food Frequency Questionnaire Strategies to Develop Food Frequency www.makrocare.com Food choices are one of the health related behaviors that are culturally determined. Public health experts and nutritionists have recognized the influence

More information

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES by John W. Brelsford, Jr. and Richard C. Atkinson TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 July 21, 1967 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES!, Reproduction

More information

Applicability to foraging simulation of a reinforcement schedule controlling the response energy of pigeons

Applicability to foraging simulation of a reinforcement schedule controlling the response energy of pigeons Learn Behav (2013) 41:425 432 DOI 10.3758/s13420-013-0117-7 Applicability to foraging simulation of a reinforcement schedule controlling the response energy of pigeons Masanori Kono Published online: 15

More information

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1987, 47, 57-72 CHOICE BETWEEN RELIABLE AND UNRELIABLE OUTCOMES: MIXED PERCENTAGE-REINFORCEMENT IN CONCURRENT CHAINS MARCIA L. SPETCH AND ROGER DUNN DALHOUSIE

More information

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee The Psychological Record, 1997,47,145-166 THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pigeons served

More information

USE OF INFRARED THERMOGRAPHY TO EVALUATE DIFFERENCES IN MEAN BODY SURFACE TEMPERATURE AND RADIANT HEAT LOSS IN GROWING PIGS

USE OF INFRARED THERMOGRAPHY TO EVALUATE DIFFERENCES IN MEAN BODY SURFACE TEMPERATURE AND RADIANT HEAT LOSS IN GROWING PIGS Swine Day 2000 Contents USE OF INFRARED THERMOGRAPHY TO EVALUATE DIFFERENCES IN MEAN BODY SURFACE TEMPERATURE AND RADIANT HEAT LOSS IN GROWING PIGS J. A. Loughmiller, M. F. Spire 1, M. D. Tokach, S. S.

More information

Schwabe, Emery & Griffith (1938), basing oxygen consumption on body

Schwabe, Emery & Griffith (1938), basing oxygen consumption on body J. Physiol. (1963), 169, pp. 851-861 851 With 3 text-figures Printed in Great Britain THE BASAL METABOLIC RATE OF COLD-ADAPTED RATS BY A. C. L. HSIEH From the Department of Physiology, University of Hong

More information

Learning: Chapter 7: Instrumental Conditioning

Learning: Chapter 7: Instrumental Conditioning Learning: Chapter 7: Instrumental Conditioning W. J. Wilson, Psychology November 8, 2010 1. Reinforcement vs. Contiguity Thorndike (a reinforcement theorist): Law of effect: positive consequences strengthen

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

The Effects of Dairy on Subjective Appetite and Post-prandial Glycemia Harvey Anderson University of Toronto

The Effects of Dairy on Subjective Appetite and Post-prandial Glycemia Harvey Anderson University of Toronto The Effects of Dairy on Subjective Appetite and Post-prandial Glycemia Harvey Anderson University of Toronto Dairy Research Symposium 2018: Transferring Results for Action February 9, 2018 Château Laurier

More information

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2005, 83, 281 296 NUMBER 3(MAY) MORPHINE TOLERANCE AS A FUNCTION OF RATIO SCHEDULE: RESPONSE REQUIREMENT OR UNIT PRICE? CHRISTINE E. HUGHES, STACEY C. SIGMON,

More information

Learning. Association. Association. Unit 6: Learning. Learning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Different Types of Learning

Learning. Association. Association. Unit 6: Learning. Learning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Different Types of Learning Unit 6: Learning Learning Learning relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience experience (nurture) is the key to learning Different Types of Learning Classical -learn by association

More information

DIETARY ENERGY DENSITY AND GROWING-FINISHING PIG PERFORMANCE AND PROFITABILITY

DIETARY ENERGY DENSITY AND GROWING-FINISHING PIG PERFORMANCE AND PROFITABILITY Swine Day 2003 Contents DIETARY ENERGY DENSITY AND GROWING-FINISHING PIG PERFORMANCE AND PROFITABILITY M.G. Young, M.D. Tokach, S.S. Dritz 1, J.M. DeRouchey, R.D. Goodband, and J.L. Nelssen Summary A retrospective

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS Behavioural Processes xxx (2010) xxx xxx

ARTICLE IN PRESS Behavioural Processes xxx (2010) xxx xxx Behavioural Processes xxx (2010) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Demand for sucrose in the genetically obese

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Differences in Taste Sensitivity to. Linoleic Acid between Male and Female Rats

Differences in Taste Sensitivity to. Linoleic Acid between Male and Female Rats Taste Sensitivity to Linoleic Acid 1 Differences in Taste Sensitivity to Linoleic Acid between Male and Female Rats Cameron Corbin Rebecca Dover Brittany Lewis Kimberly Smith Submitted as partial fulfillment

More information

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change*

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2 (4), 262-266 Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change* ALAN RUSSELLt and PETER H. GLOW University ofadelaide,

More information

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY Volume 6, Number 2 Submitted: September 8, 2000 Resubmitted: January 22, 2001 Accepted: January 23, 2001 Publication date: January 26, 2001 THE EFFICACY OF REINFORCEMENT

More information

This article was published in an Elsevier journal. The attached copy is furnished to the author for non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the author s institution, sharing

More information

Challenges of High Speed Splicing

Challenges of High Speed Splicing Challenges of High Speed Splicing Richard S. Tetro V.P. Technical Director Black Clawson Converting Machinery Inc. Fulton NY http://bcconverting.com ABSTRACT: Reliability is a key objective of any continuous

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 261-268 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) CONCURRENT SCHEULES OF PRIMARY AN CONITIONE REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' ONAL W. ZIMMERMAN CARLETON UNIVERSITY Rats responded

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Demand under Fixed-Ratio Schedules with Domestic Hens. Alan Poling 2

Reinforcer Magnitude and Demand under Fixed-Ratio Schedules with Domestic Hens. Alan Poling 2 Reinforcer Magnitude and Demand under Fixed-Ratio Schedules with Domestic Hens Amber A. Grant 1, T. Mary Foster 1, William Temple 1, Surrey Jackson, 1 Jennifer Kinloch 1 and Alan Poling 2 Affiliation of

More information

REINFORCEMENT1 UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN. this procedure required that an animal emit a. pause of at least 15 min, was found to be sensitive

REINFORCEMENT1 UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN. this procedure required that an animal emit a. pause of at least 15 min, was found to be sensitive JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR USE OF PROGRESSIVE FIXED-RATIO PROCEDURES IN THE ASSESSMENT OF INTRACRANIAL REINFORCEMENT1 RICHARD E. KEESEY AND MICHAEL D. GOLDSTEIN UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN

More information

Standard Operating Procedure for Cycle Ergometry on Monarks

Standard Operating Procedure for Cycle Ergometry on Monarks Standard Operating Procedure for Cycle Ergometry on Monarks Effective date: 26.07.2017 Review due date: 26.04.2019 Original Author Name: Richard Metcalfe Position: Ph.D. Student Date: 14.12.2012 Reviewer

More information

Pharmacokinetics of drug infusions

Pharmacokinetics of drug infusions SA Hill MA PhD FRCA Key points The i.v. route provides the most predictable plasma concentrations. Pharmacodynamic effects of a drug are related to plasma concentration. Both plasma and effect compartments

More information

Classical and Instrumental Conditioning. Lecture 8

Classical and Instrumental Conditioning. Lecture 8 Classical and Instrumental Conditioning Lecture 8 1 Basic Procedure for Classical Conditioning CS (Bell) US (Meat Powder) CR (Salivation) UR (Salivation) 2 Acquisition Major Phenomena of Classical Conditioning

More information

Discounting in pigeons when the choice is between two delayed rewards: implications for species comparisons

Discounting in pigeons when the choice is between two delayed rewards: implications for species comparisons Original Research Article published: 17 August 2011 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00096 Discounting in pigeons when the choice is between two delayed rewards: implications for species comparisons Amanda L. Calvert,

More information

Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Short Duration Performance Measures Compared to the Wilkinson Auditory Vigilance Task

Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Short Duration Performance Measures Compared to the Wilkinson Auditory Vigilance Task Sleep, 1(2): 169-176 1978 Raven Press, New York Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Short Duration Performance Measures Compared to the Wilkinson Auditory Vigilance Task Marilyn Glenville, Roger Broughton,

More information

Changes in Metabolism in Response to Varying Energy Intake in a Marine Mammal, the Steller Sea Lion

Changes in Metabolism in Response to Varying Energy Intake in a Marine Mammal, the Steller Sea Lion Changes in Metabolism in Response to Varying Energy Intake in a Marine Mammal, the Steller Sea Lion David A. S. Rosen* Andrew W. Trites Marine Mammal Research Unit, Fisheries Centre, University of British

More information