THE EFFECT OF CHANGES IN AVAILABILITY ON FOOD SELECTION AT DIFFERENT LEVELS OF DEPRiVATION. TERRY W. BELKE Mount Allison University

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "THE EFFECT OF CHANGES IN AVAILABILITY ON FOOD SELECTION AT DIFFERENT LEVELS OF DEPRiVATION. TERRY W. BELKE Mount Allison University"

Transcription

1 The Psychological Record, 2000, 50, THE EFFECT OF CHANGES IN AVAILABILITY ON FOOD SELECTION AT DIFFERENT LEVELS OF DEPRiVATION TERRY W. BELKE Mount Allison University TAMMI Y. KWAN Dalhousie University In the present study, two experiments investigated the effect of a change in food availability on selection between two food items at different deprivation levels. In the first study, 6 pigeons chose between a nonpreferred food item (Le., checkers) after a fixed delay and a preferred food item (Le., corn) after an adjusting delay. The delay to the preferred food item adjusted depending upon the pigeon's choices. The dependent measure was the amount of delay added to corn to produce indifference at 70% and 950/0 of body weight. Results showed that less delay had to be added when pigeons were more deprived. In the second study, the availability of both corn and checkers was varied by manipulating the amount of each food item presented to the pigeons at 70% and 95% of body weight. The dependent measure was the amount of each item taken. For the nonpreferred food item, consumption depended on an interaction between deprivation and availability. A decrease in availability only affected checker consumption when body weight was at 70%. Together these two studies showed that the effect of a change in food availability on selectivity was greater when pigeons were more deprived. Optimal foraging theory uses the principle of optimization to predict diet choice. According to optimal foraging theory, if all food items in the environment are ranked according to some measure of profit for the feeder, such as net energy per unit feeding time, then the optimal diet model predicts that an animal should always choose the food item that maximizes the net rate of energy intake per unit time, all other things being equal (Krebs, 1978; Pyke, Pulliam, & Charnov, 1977). However this rule of thumb is qualified by considerations of food availability and deprivation. This research was conducted in the Harvard Psychological Laboratory, William James Hall, Cambridge, MA while Terry Belke was in the graduate program in the Department of Psychology, Harvard University and while Tammi Kwan was in the graduate program in the Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada. Correspondence regarding this article can be sent by mail to Terry W. Belke, Department of Psychology, Mount Allison University, Sackville, New Brunswick, Canada, E4L 1 C7. ( to MTA.CA)

2 530 BELKE AND KWAN In the natural environment, both seasonal and nonseasonal factors (e.g., diseases, fire) contribute variance in the availability of different food items. As food in the environment becomes scarce or difficult to obtain, optimal foraging theory predicts that at some point the animal should give. up on the better alternative in favor of a worse alternative as the better alternative becomes more scarce or difficult to obtain. Thus, food selection or diet choice is a function of -the availability of better or preferred food items while the availability of worse food items is irrelevant. As long as both worse and better food items are readily available, the animal should choose the latter, but as the availability of the latter decreases, the probability that the animal will take the worse food item should increase (Walker, 1987). Previous research on the effects of changes in food availability on food selection is consistent with the predictions of optimal foraging theory. Goss-Custard (1977) showed that selection of a less preferred prey item (a worm) by a shorebird (redshank) depended on the absolute rather than the relative density of the preferred prey item (a shrimp). Lacher, Willig, and Mares (1982) studied the food preference of a florivorous rodent (kerodon rupestris) for different species of leaves at different levels of availability. At the higher level of availability, consumption of preferred leaf types increased while consumption of less preferred leaf types decreased. Schoener (1971) reported that decreased selectivity as a function of decreasing food availability had been observed in fish, weaver birds, blackbirds, swifts, and mollusc. Laboratory studies of selection among food items varying in size or amount rather than type have also found that selection varies ~ith availability. For example, Krebs, Erichsen, Webber, and Charnov (1977), using a conveyor belt to present food items of different sizes to birds, varied the probability of encountering large and small food items. Results showed that dietary specialization occurred when the probability of encountering a better food type was high while dietary generalization occurred when the probability of encountering the better food type was low. Thus, the availability of preferred food items controls selectivity or diet choice. I n contrast, the effect of deprivation on selectivity is less clear. Optimal foraging theory predicts no effect of deprivation on diet selection in the absence of changes in food availability. If an animal is selecting among different food items so as to maximize net energy per unit time when relatively nondeprived, then an increase in deprivation level should not affect food choice. Instead, other aspects of consumption should change, such as an increase in the rate of consumption. Studies of the effects of deprivation on selection between qualitatively different food items presented simultaneously to subjects have found no systematic effects in the absence of changes in food availability. For example, Moon and Zeigler (1979) found no systematic effects of body weight deprivation on selection among different seed types by pigeons.. However, studies of the effects of deprivation on selection of food items where food items are presented successively rather than simultaneously

3 DEPRIVATION, AVAILABILITY, AND FOOD SELECTION 531 have found that the probability of accepting a less profitable food item increases with deprivation. For example, Rechten, Avery, and Stevens (1983) found that deprived or hungry tits were less likely to turn down profitable items and were more likely to accept unprofitable items than were birds that were prefed prior to the session. Thus, the appearance of an effect of deprivation on selectivity seems to depend, in part, on the manner of presentation of the food items to be selected from. Research has documented effects of changes in food availability and deprivation on selection of food items; however, these two variables are not necessarily independent Deprivation and food availability may interact. As food availability decreases, deprivation should increase. According to Schoener (19, 71), the role of deprivation is to create an internal state of "hunger" which functions as a monitor of changes in food availability in the environment. As food availability decreases, the animal becomes hungry and this hunger ''tells the animal what intake is needed by what future time to forestall a given degree of weakness or starvation" (p. 383). Thus, deprivation is related to sensitivity to changes in food availability. The nature of this interaction between deprivation and food availability was investigated in the present study. Of specific interest was whether changes in food availability have similar effects at different deprivation levels. Is a deprived animal more sensitive to changes in food availability than a less deprived animal? Does a unit of change in food availability have different effects at different deprivation levels? The present study investigated the effect of changes in food availability on food preference at different deprivation levels using two different procedures. In the first experiment, the availability of the better or more preferred food item was varied by titrating the delay to the preferred food item. In an adjusting delay procedure (Mazur, 1987, 1988; Mazur, Snyderman, & Coe, 1985), the delay to a preferred food item is increased when the preferred food item is chosen and decreased when the nonpreferred food item is chosen. The delay added to the preferred food item to produce indifference provides a measure of the value of the preferred food item relative to the nonpreferred item. Delays required to produce indifference were assessed at two different deprivation levels. The second experiment varied availability by varying the amount of food that was immediately available. In this procedure, equal amounts of the food items used in Experiment 1 were presented in food cups and the amount was varied at two different levels of deprivation. The amounts of each item taken were measured. Although these procedures are not foraging simulations, generalizing from the tenets of optimal foraging theory one might predict that (a) changes in availability through either delay or amount should increase the probability of selecting the worse food item and (b) that the probability that the animal selects the worse food item should increase under conditions of greater deprivation. That is, in Experiment 1, the indifference delay value should be shorter under conditions of more severe deprivation; and in Experiment 2, the likelihood of the worse food item being taken with a change in availability should be greater under more severe deprivation.

4 532 BELKE AND KWAN Experiment 1 Method Subject Selection Pigeons were selected for the study based on demonstration of a stable moderate to strong preference for corn over Purina pigeon checkers. Prior to the selection procedure, the pigeons were on ad-lib feeding conditions. Pigeons were fed 30 g of a mixture of assorted seeds and Purina pigeon checkers once a day. During the selection phase, 10 pigeons were presented with 30 g of corn and 30 g of checkers in a two-compartment feeding tray attached to the home cages. Each pigeon was given access to the corn and checkers for 2 min. After 2 min, the tray was removed and the remaining amount of each food type was weighed. To eliminate the possibility of side preferences, the side on which each food item was presented was reversed across days. In order for a pigeon to be selected into the present study, the pigeon nad to consume more corn than checkers each day over a period of 10 consecutive days. Table 1 shows the total amounts of each food item taken by each selected pigeon over the 10 days. Table 1 Total Amounts (g) Consumed by Pigeons Over 1 O-Day Testing Period Pigeon Corn Checkers P P P P P P Based on this procedure, 6 pigeons were selected as subjects. All pigeons had prior experience with operant conditioning procedures. The pigeons had free access to water and grit in their home cages, but were food deprived to, and maintained at, body weights of 70% and 95% of their ad-lib weights. Pigeons were not run on days when their body weights deviated more than 10 g from their target weight. Supplemental food, which contained various seeds and grains, including corn and checkers, was given when needed following an experimental session. Miller (1976) reported that the composition of a postsession food supplement produced no differential effects on preference. Apparatus The experimental chamber was 32 cm long, 29 cm wide, and 30 cm high. Three response keys were mounted in the front wall of the chamber. Each key was 20.5 cm above the floor. Response keys were 2 cm in diameter and ~eparated by 7 cm, center to center. The force required for

5 DEPRIVATION, AVAILABILITY, AND FOOD SELECTION 533 microswitch closure was approximately 1.5 N. A food hopper was located below each response key. Each hopper was illuminated by two 6-W white lights when in operation. The chamber was illuminated by two 6-W white lights located in a box mounted on the top of the chamber. The chamber was located in a sound-attenuating box with a fan for ventilation and to mask extraneous noise. A PDP-8A computer in another room was used to control experimental events and record responses. Procedure Informal observations of pigeons in the colony revealed that Purina pigeon checkers tend to accumulate in the food cups of birds that are at or near ad-lib weight. During feeding, the seeds and grains were selected out and the pigeon checkers remained in the food cup. New birds introduced to the loft also showed this behavior. In contrast, experimental birds at less than ad-lib body weights do not leave pigeon checkers in their food cups, but do tend to pick out seeds and grains first. These casual observations suggest that pigeons prefer other food items over pigeon checkers. Unpopped popcorn, readily available at a local supermarket, was chosen as an alternative to pigeon checkers. The subject selection procedure revealed that corn was preferred over checkers. Information on the constituents and nutritional value of corn and checkers is provided in Appendix A. Pigeons were divided into two groups. Each group received a different order of deprivation conditions. For pigeons in Group 1, the order of body weights was 70%, 95%, and 70%. For pigeons in Group 2, the order was 95%, 70%, and 95%. The experiment employed an adjusting-delay procedure to measure changes in the relative value of the two food reinforcers as a function of deprivation. In this procedure, each session consisted of 64 trials. The trials were grouped into blocks of four trials. Each block consisted of two forced-choice trials followed by two free-choice trials. Each trial began with the center key illuminated red. The purpose of the center key was to ensure that the pigeon's head was approximately equidistant from both side keys on the free-choice trials. On free-choice trials, a response on the center key illuminated the two side keys and extinguished illumination of the center key. For Group 1 birds, the left key was illuminated blue and the right key was illuminated white. Key illumination colors were reversed for birds in Group 2. Despite differences in illumination color, the left hopper always produced pigeon checkers and the right hopper always presented corn. An intertrial interval (ITI) of 30 s was programmed between trials. Continuing with free-choice trials, a response on the keylight associated with checkers initiated a standard delay of 5 s. During the passage of this delay, the keylight flashed on and off twice a second. After 5 s elapsed, the keylight stopped flashing and the checker hopper was activated for a 3-s duration. If the pigeon pecked the keylight associated with corn, the adjusting delay was initiated. Again, the keylight flashed

6 534 BELKE AND KWAN and following the elapse of the adjusting delay, the corn hopper was activated for 3 s. The next trial began after the ITI elapsed. On forced-choice trials, a single peck on the center key illuminated either the keylight associated with checkers or the keylight associated with corn. A response on whichever key was illuminated initiated the associated delay and provided the appropriate reinforcement following passage of the delay. Responses on the nonilluminated keys had no effect. Following the termination of the hopper presentation, the ITI began. Of two forced-choice trials in each block of four trials, one involved the standard delay to checkers and the other involved the adjusting delay to corn. The order of presentation of these two forced-choice trials within each block of four trials was randomly varied. The delay associated with corn was adjusted following completion of the free-choice trials. If corn was chosen on both free-choice trials, then the delay associated with corn was increased by 1 s. If checkers were chosen on both trials, the delay to corn was decreased by 1 s. Finally, if corn was chosen on one trial and checkers on the other, then the delay remained unchanged. The delay remained in effect for the subsequent block of trials. At the start of the first session, the delay associated with corn was set at 1 s. For subsequent sessions, the adjusting delay always began at the last value from the previous session. The delay on the adjusting alternative could not be decreased to a value less than 1 s. Stability criteria. For the first condition, a minimum of 25 sessions had to occur before performance could be judged stable. For subsequent conditions, the minimum was 15 sessions. After the minimum number of sessions had been met, a condition was terminated when several stability criteria were met. To assess stability, each session was divided into two blocks of 32 trials. A mean delay was calculated from the eight delays obtained from the free-choice trials in each 32-trial block. Performance was judged stable when the following criteria were met: (1) Neither the highest nor the lowest single block means of a condition could occur in the last six blocks, (2) the mean adjusting delay across the last six blocks could not be the highest nor the lowest six-block mean of the condition, and (3) the mean delay of the last six blocks could not differ from the mean delay of the preceding six blocks by more than 1 s. The stable value was the average of the two six-block means used to judge stability. In this experiment, a condition was not terminated until these criteria were met three times and the indifference delay reported for a condition was the average of the three stable values. Results Table 2 presents the mean and standard deviations for the indifference delay values for each pigeon by body weight level. The number of sessions to stability are also presented. For Group 1, the mean delays across the 70%, 95%, and 70% body weight conditions were 8.96, 14.01, and s, respectively. For Group 2, the mean delays across the 95%, 70%, and 95%

7 DEPRIVATION, AVAILABILITY, AND FOOD SELECTION 535 body weight conditions were 8.38, 6.21, and s, respectively. Both groups displayed the same pattern of shorter delays at 70% than at 95% and changes in delays covaried with changes in body weight. Table 2 Group 1 P57 P306 P453 Mean Group 2 P48 P423 P496 Mean Mean I ndifference Delays for Corn (s), Standard Deviations ( ), and Sessions to 8tability (8) for Each Pigeon by Body Weight Level Body Weight Level 70% S 950/0 S 70% 6.87(1.14) (0.92) (0.43) (0.82) (0.99) (0.47) 8.53 (1.16) (0.45) (4.55) /0 8 70% 8 95% 9.51 (1.39) (0.32) (0.48) 8.88 (0.30) (0.83) (0.11) 6.75 (0.46) (0.14) (1.68) For the purpose of statistical analysis, values for the same body weight conditions in Table 2 were averaged and the resultant values were tested using a paired t test. The results of this analysis showed that indifference delays were significantly shorter (t(5) = 3.62, P =.015} at 70% (M = 7.87 s) than at 95% (M = s) body weight. On average, indifference values for corn were approximately 5 s less in the 70% body weight condition. This result implies that the difference in the value of corn relative to checkers was less under conditions of greater deprivation. Finally, Pig'eons 48 and 496 both produced longer indifference delays upon return to 950/0 body weight than when initially put into this condition. This may reflect an order effect. Previous experience with more severe deprivation may enhance the value of a preferred food item when the pigeon is returned to a less deprived state. Discussion In general, the results showed that the difference in relative value between the preferred and nonpreferred food items decreased as deprivation increased. This interpretation follows the theoretical basis of the adjusting delay procedure in which value is a function of the magnitude and delay of reinforcement (Mazur, 1987). An alternative interpretation is that pigeons were willing to wait longer for the preferred food item when they were less deprived. This interpretation suggests that impulsivity increased with deprivation. However, Logue and Pena Correal (1985) failed to find an effect of deprivation on self-control in pigeons. Regardless of the interpretation, this finding is consistent with the hypothesis that selectivity decreases rather than increases with deprivation.

8 536 BELKE AND KWAN Experiment 2 Experiment 1 used delay as a measure of value in order to investigate the effects of a change in the availability of a preferred food item on the value of the preferred food item relative to the nonpreferred item at different deprivation levels. Availability was varied by adjusting the delay to the preferred food item. However, availability can also be altered by decreasing amount rather than by increasing delay. A change in availability based on amount can be accomplished by simply decreasing the amount of food given to pigeons in a food cup. An advantage of changing availability in this way is that it allows separate assessment of the effects of availability, deprivation, and an availability by deprivation interaction. Method Subjects With the exception of Pigeon 453, the same pigeons were used in Experiment 2. Pigeon 453 died in the 12-month interval between the first and second experiments. Apparatus The experiment was carried out in the pigeon colony room with the birds in their home cages. Water and grit were continuously available. Food preferences were tested using a 14-cm x 6.5-cm x 6.5-cm metal food container designed to attach to an opening on the home cage. The portion of the container where the food was placed was partitioned into two equal areas (6.5 cm x 3 cm x 4 cm). The pigeon was able to access both areas through a 6.2-cm x 4.7-cm opening at the rear of the container. A stopwatch was used to measure time and a scale was used to measure food amount and pigeon weights. Procedure The pigeons were kept in the same groups as in Experiment 1. Birds in Group 1 were tested at 70% and then 95% ad-lib body weight whereas birds in Group 2 were tested in the opposite order. When the birds reached their target weights, food preference tests began. At each deprivation level, high and low food availability tests were conducted. In the high availability test, pigeons were presented with 30 g of corn and 30 g of checkers. After 2 min of access, the food container was removed and the amount of each item remaining was measured. Eight test sessions were conducted. On alternate days, the sides on which the corn and checkers were placed were reversed to control for side preferences. In the low food availability test, an amount equivalent to 750/0 of the mean total amount of both items taken during the high availability test was calculated. Subjects were then presented with half that amount in checkers and half in corn. Eight test sessions were again conducted with the sides reversed between test sessions. After 2 min of access to both food items, the remainder of each item was weighed and recorded.

9 DEPRIVATION, AVAILABILITY, AND FOOD SELECTION 537 \~ Results Table 3 presents the amounts of corn and checkers taken by each pigeon ~nder each condition. Because availability was manipulated as a percentage of the amounts consumed in the high availability condition and the amounts consumed in the high availability conditions differed as a function of deprivation level, this raises the possibility of spurious effects if the data are analyzed solely in terms of amounts consumed. Wherever appropriate, the data are analyzed in terms of both grams consumed and percentage of the amount offered that was consumed. Pigeon P57 P306 P48 P423 P496 Mean Table 3 Mean Amounts (g) of Corn (Co) and Checkers (Ch) Consumed Under Different Levels of Deprivation and Availability Deprivation Level 70% 95% Availability Availability High Low High Low Co Ch Co Ch Co Ch Co (9) (9) (10) (10) (10) (9.6) Ch 0.50 (4) 2:63 (6) 0.13 (5) 0.00 (5) 1.00 (5) 0.85 (5) Note. The amounts (g) of each food type offered under conditions of low food availability are given in parentheses. Low availability amounts were 75% of the amounts consumed under conditions of high availability divided equally between the two food types. In high availability conditions, 30 g of each food type was offered. First, with availability held constant at a high level, the effect of deprivation on the rate of eating can be seen in the amount of food consumed per min. At 95% body weight, pigeons consumed on average 6.59 g/min compared to g/min at 70% body weight. A paired t test showed that the rate of eating significantly increased with deprivation, «4) = 11.87, P < In terms of an effect of availability, less food was consumed in the low (M = g) than the high availability condition (M = g); however, because less food was available in the low availability condition, this effect is best interpreted in terms of percentage of amount offered that was consumed. In the high and low availability conditions, pigeons were presented with 120 g and 29.2 g of food, respectively. When amounts offered are taken into consideration, pigeons consumed 32.7% of the food offered in the high availability condition and 81.7% in the low availability condition. Thus, the effect of a change in availability was to increase the percentage of available food that was consumed. However, even if the pigeons had consumed the same absolute amount of food in both conditions, this effect of availability on the percentage of food consumed would be expected to occur.

10 538 BELKE AND KWAN A repeated measures analysis of variance with deprivation level (70%, 95%) and food availability (high, low) as within-subject factors was performed on the amounts of checkers consumed. Although this analysis showed significant main effects of deprivation, F(1, 4) = 13.67, P <.05, and availability, F(1, 4) = 21.96, P <.05, these main effects were generated as a function of a significant deprivation x availability interaction, F(1, 4) = 67.00, P <.01. Figure 1 shows the mean amounts of checkers consumed as a function of deprivation and availability. In parentheses beside each data point are the mean number of grams offered and the percentage of the grams offered that the grams consumed represent. Analyses of these data as either grams consumed or percentage consumed produce the same pattern of results and yield the same interpretation. First, from Figure 1, it is apparent that deprivation had no effect on checkers consumed when' availability of both food items was high, t(4) = -c 0) E ::::J Ul c::: 0 u (f) ~ CJ.) ~ u Q) L:. U "Po 4 Ul E co ~ t!) 2 (30, 5.6%) 0 0 High availability Low availability 92'.6 % ) 70% 95% Percent ad-lib body weight (30, 3.40/0) (5, 15.80/0) Figure 1. Mean amounts (g) of pigeon checkers consumed under different levels of availability and deprivation. Mean amounts offered (g) and the percentage of the amount offered that the amount consumed represents are shown in the parentheses.

11 DEPRIVATION, AVAILABILITY, AND FOOD SELECTION , P =.64 for both grams consumed and percentages. Nor did a change in availability of both items affect consumption of checkers when deprivation was less severe (Le., 95%), ~4) = 0.44, P =.69 for grams, ~4) =. 2.14, P =.10 for percent checkers consumed. However, when the availability of both items was low, deprivation strongly affected checker consumption, ~4) = 6.5, P <.01 for grams consumed and ~4) = 5.34, P <.01 for percent checkers consumed. When deprivation was more severe (Le., 70%), a decrease in availability strongly increased both grams consumed, ~4) = 6.15, P <.01, and percent checkers consumed, ~4) = 11.70, P <.001. Consumption of the preferred food item (i.e., corn) was not analyzed in a similar manner because when availability was low, birds typically ate all the corn that was offered, but differed in whether or not checkers were also consumed. In this case, the amounts of corn consumed in terms of grams consumed differed only as a function of the procedure for manipulating availability and as such would produce spurious effects. Similarly, when availability was high, birds almost exclusively ate corn regardless 9f the deprivation level. Consequently, the difference in amounts consumed reflect the effect of deprivation on the rate of eating. Discussion Experiment 2 further explicated the roles played by deprivation, availability, and food type. Corn was preferred to checkers under all conditions; however, the conditions differed with respect to whether or not checkers were consumed. Checkers were consumed when availability was low and deprivation was severe, but not when availability was high regardless of deprivation level, or when availability was low and deprivation was mild. In other words, selectivity diminished only under conditions of decreased availability and increased deprivation. The results from Experiment 2 can be reconciled with those of Experiment 1 by conceptualizing the amounts of the preferred item given in Experiment 2 as equivalent to delays of the preferred food item that were either less than or greater than the indifference delay values that varied as a function of deprivation. Under conditions of mild deprivation, the amounts given in both the high and low availability conditions would be equivalent to delay values less than the indifference point. At both levels of availability, corn was chosen, checkers were not. Under conditions of more severe deprivation, the indifference delay would be shorter and the change in availability would cross the indifference delay value. In the high availability condition, corn was chosen, checkers were not; in the low availability condition, both corn and checkers were chosen. General Discussion In general, the results from the present experiments demonstrate, as suggested by Schoener (1971), an interaction between availability and

12 540 BELKE AND KWAN deprivation on food selectivity. Changes in food availability had different effects on the probability of choosing the less preferred food item at different deprivation levels. In Experiment 1, less delay had to be added to the preferred food item to produce indifference. in choice between the two items in the condition of greater deprivation. In Experiment 2, the decline in availability of both food items markedly increased the likelihood of eating the less preferred food item, but only under conditions of greater deprivation. Similar results occurred whether availability of the food items was decreased by increasing the delay to the preferred food item or decreasing the amounts of both items. ' It could be argued that the increased consumption of checkers in the low availability condition at the greater deprivation level may reflect an attempt to maintain a constant overall level of food intake. However, the fact that pigeons did not increase their consumption of checkers in the less severe deprivation condition contradicts this interpretation. Finally, although neither procedure is a foraging simulation, the effects of deprivation and availability on food selection in these experimental procedures appear consistent with those that optimal foraging theory would predict for more ecologically valid contexts. The direct effect of deprivation was to increase rate of food consumption. In Experiment 2, rate of food consumption doubled when body weight was decreased from 95% to 70%. However, as observed by Moon and Zeigler (1979), although rate of consumption changed, preference did not. At both deprivation levels corn was strongly preferred over checkers. - References GOSS-CUSTARD, J. D. (1977). Responses of the redshank, Tringa Totanus, to the absolute and relative prey densities of two prey items. Journal of Animal Ecology, 46, KREBS, J. R. (1978). Optimal foraging: Decision rules for predators. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology (pp ). Oxford: Blackwell. KREBS, J. R., ERICHSEN, J. T., WEBBER, J. I., & CHARNOV, E. L. (1977). Optimal prey selction in the great tit (Parus majot). Animal Behavior, 25, LOGUE, A. W., & PENA-CORREAL, T. E. (1985). The effect of food deprivation on self control. Behavioural Processes, 10, LACHER, T. E., Jr., WILLIG, M. R., & MARES, M. A. (1982). Food preferences as a function of resource abundance with multiple prey types: An experimental analysis of optimal foraging theory. American Naturalist, 120, MAZUR, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior: Vol. 5. The effect of delay and intervening events on reinforcement value (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. MAZUR, J. E. (1988). Choice between small certain and large uncertain reinforcers. Animal Learning & Behavior, 16,

13 DEPRIVATION, AVAILABILITY, AND FOOD SELECTION 541 MAZUR, J. E., SNYDERMAN, M., & COE, D. (1985). Influences of delay and rate of reinforcement on discrete trial choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, MILLER, H. L., Jr. (1976). Matching-based hedonic scaling in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, MOON, R. D., & ZEIGLER, H. P. (1979). Food preferences in the pigeon (Columbia livia). Physiology and Behavior, 22, PYKE, G. H., PULLIAM, H. R., & CHARNOV, E. L. (1977). Optimal foraging: a selective review of theory and tests. Quarterly Review of Biology, 52, RECHTEN, C., AVERY, M., & STEVENS, A. (1983). Optimal prey selection - why do great tits show partial preferences. Animal Behavior, 31, SCHOENER, T. W. (1971). Theories of feeding strategies. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 11, WALKER, S. (1987). Animal learning: An introduction. New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul Inc. Appendix A Nutritional Analysis and Iingredients for Purina Pigeon Checkers and Corn Purina Pigeon Chow Checkers Analysis: (0/0 weight by ounce) Crude Protein 15.0% Crude Fat 2.50/0 Crude Fiber 5.5% Calories/gram = 2.78 Ingredients: Grain products, plant protein products, forage products, animal protein products, process grain by-products, vitamin A supplement, D activated animal sterol (source of vitamin D), ethoxyquin (a preservative), vitamin E supplement, riboflavin supplement, DL-methionine, vitamin 812 supplement, menadione sodium bisulfate (source of vitamin K activity), sodium selenite, choline chloride, calcium pantothenate, niacin supplement, calcium carbonate, dicalcium phosphate, manganous oxide, calcium iodate, copper oxide, zinc oxide Corn Analysis: Protein Carbohydrate Fat (% weight by ounce) 60/0 40% 15% Calories/gram = 3.62

14 542 BELKE AND KWAN

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

FOWL POWER. Available in 50# Bags

FOWL POWER. Available in 50# Bags FOWL POWER Crude Protein min 16.00% Lysine min 0.65% Methionine min 0.20% Crude Fat min 3.00% Crude Fiber max 6.00% Calcium min 1.40% Calcium max 1.90% Phosphorus min 0.40% Salt max 0.30% INGREDIENTS:

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

16% HOG RATION Feed as a Complete Growing & Finishing Ration

16% HOG RATION Feed as a Complete Growing & Finishing Ration 16% HOG RATION Feed as a Complete Growing & Finishing Ration Crude Protein min 16.00% Lysine min 0.70% Crude Fat min 3.50% Crude Fiber max 7.00% Calcium min 0.40% Calcium max 0.90% Salt max 0.70% Selenium

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Dairy Feed 14% - Textured

Dairy Feed 14% - Textured Dairy Dairy Feed 14% - Textured Guaranteed Analysis CRUDE PROTEIN, not less than... 14.0 % CRUDE FAT, not less than... 2.0 % CRUDE FIBER, not more than... 3.0 % ADF, not more than... 4.00 % CALCIUM (Ca),

More information

HORSE FEED. Available in 50# Bags

HORSE FEED. Available in 50# Bags HORSE FEED Crude Protein min 12.00% Crude Fiber max 9.50% Calcium min 0.50% Phosphorus min 0.45% Copper min 33 PPM Zinc min 190 PPM Vitamin A min 8,200 IU/LB INGREDIENTS: Processed Grain By-Products, Grain

More information

Rancher s Choice Adult Dog. Achieve Performance Dog Food

Rancher s Choice Adult Dog. Achieve Performance Dog Food Rancher s Choice Adult Dog 21% Protein - 9% Fat Achieve Performance Dog Food Rancher s Choice Pet Food is the right choice for your pets. You and your pet are receiving Complete a high nutrition quality

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

Effect of short-term prefeeding and body weight on wheel running and responding reinforced by the opportunity to run in a wheel

Effect of short-term prefeeding and body weight on wheel running and responding reinforced by the opportunity to run in a wheel Behavioural Processes 67 (2004) 1 10 Effect of short-term prefeeding and body weight on wheel running and responding reinforced by the opportunity to run in a wheel Terry W. Belke a,, W. David Pierce b,

More information

Ranchers Choice AN -18%

Ranchers Choice AN -18% Ranchers Choice AN -18% All Purpose For Cattle, Sheep, Horses Guaranteed Analysis CRUDE PROTEIN, not less than... 18.0% CRUDE FAT, not less than... 4.00% CRUDE FIBER, not more than... 1.00% CALCIUM (Ca),

More information

18% Pig Starter Pellet

18% Pig Starter Pellet 18% Pig Starter Pellet MEDICATED Increased rate of gain and improved feed efficiency. Active Drug Ingredient Oxytetracycline... 50 gms/ton Guaranteed Analysis CRUDE PROTEIN, not less than... 18.0% LYSINE,

More information

POULTRY PREMIUM FEED. Features & Benefits. Learn More at BuckeyeNutrition.com CRUMBLES & PELLETS

POULTRY PREMIUM FEED. Features & Benefits. Learn More at BuckeyeNutrition.com CRUMBLES & PELLETS POULTRY PREMIUM FEED CRUMBLES & PELLETS Features & Benefits Formulated with all-natural vegetable-based ingredients Supports healthy growth and development Medication-free formula BIG 4 PELLETS FOR TURKEYS

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 12-1991 Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Applicability to foraging simulation of a reinforcement schedule controlling the response energy of pigeons

Applicability to foraging simulation of a reinforcement schedule controlling the response energy of pigeons Learn Behav (2013) 41:425 432 DOI 10.3758/s13420-013-0117-7 Applicability to foraging simulation of a reinforcement schedule controlling the response energy of pigeons Masanori Kono Published online: 15

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 93 102 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

Animal Nutrition. What s in a soybean? Activity 1: Soybean nutrients

Animal Nutrition. What s in a soybean? Activity 1: Soybean nutrients Animal Nutrition name: date: class: Welcome to the GrowNextGen Animal Nutrition Plug-and-Play Lesson. This lesson will take approximately three class periods (120 minutes total) and will enhance and expand

More information

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 00, 79, 6 NUMBER (MARCH) RESPONDING FOR SUCROSE AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: EFFECTS OF SUCROSE CONCENTRATION AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCER DURATION

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

Ohio State University Eastern Agriculture Research Station Belle Valley, Ohio Male vs Female Confinement Lamb Finishing

Ohio State University Eastern Agriculture Research Station Belle Valley, Ohio Male vs Female Confinement Lamb Finishing Ohio State University Eastern Agriculture Research Station Belle Valley, Ohio Male vs Female Confinement Lamb Finishing Clif Little, Associate Professor/Extension Educator, Guernsey County Wayne Shriver,

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

Introduction. Hypothesis

Introduction. Hypothesis Introduction Effects of Feeding Babcock B300 Laying Hens Conventional Sanfandila Layer Diets Compared to Diets Containing 10% Norgold DDGS on Performance and Egg Quality Corn distiller s dried grains with

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (3), 264-270 Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pullman, Washington 99163 Five pigeons

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

GUIDELINES FOR USE OF NUTRITION AND HEALTH CLAIMS

GUIDELINES FOR USE OF NUTRITION AND HEALTH CLAIMS 1 CAC/GL 23-1997 GUIDELINES FOR USE OF NUTRITION AND HEALTH CLAIMS CAC/GL 23-1997 Nutrition claims should be consistent with national nutrition policy and support that policy. Only nutrition claims that

More information

Clif Little, Associate Professor/Extension Educator, Guernsey/Noble Counties Wayne Shriver, Manager, EARS & Staff

Clif Little, Associate Professor/Extension Educator, Guernsey/Noble Counties Wayne Shriver, Manager, EARS & Staff Ohio State University Eastern Agriculture Research Station (EARS) Belle Valley, Ohio Ewe Lamb Confinement vs Pasture Finishing & Wether Lamb Feed Trial Clif Little, Associate Professor/Extension Educator,

More information

Feeds and Feeding What are the nutritional needs of my poultry? Jessie Flores 2017 Mid-Atlantic Small Flock Poultry Expo

Feeds and Feeding What are the nutritional needs of my poultry? Jessie Flores 2017 Mid-Atlantic Small Flock Poultry Expo Feeds and Feeding What are the nutritional needs of my poultry? Jessie Flores 2017 Mid-Atlantic Small Flock Poultry Expo What is a nutrient? There are six classes of nutrients: 1. Carbohydrates 2. Proteins

More information

Broiler Nutrition Specifications

Broiler Nutrition Specifications Broiler Nutrition Specifications 2 Introduction 3 Table 1: Nutrition Specifications for As-Hatched Broilers - Target Live Weight

More information

Dairy. GUARANTEED ANALYSIS Crude Protein (min) % Crude Fat (min) % Crude Fiber (max)...5.0%

Dairy. GUARANTEED ANALYSIS Crude Protein (min) % Crude Fat (min) % Crude Fiber (max)...5.0% Dairy L&K Non-Medicated Calf Grower 16% Re-formulated Crude Protein (min)...16.0% Crude Fat (min)... 3.0% Grain Products, Plant Protein Products, Processed Grain By-Products, Cane Molasses, Processed Bakery

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 89 (2012) 212 218 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes j o ur nal homep age : www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Providing a reinforcement history

More information

PRODUCT BROCHURE. Proudly made in British Columbia, Canada. Kasiks uses all natural, regionally sourced ingredients

PRODUCT BROCHURE. Proudly made in British Columbia, Canada. Kasiks uses all natural, regionally sourced ingredients Simply Inspired By Nature PRODUCT BROCHURE Proudly made in British Columbia, Canada Kasiks uses all natural, regionally sourced ingredients When we first started our company in 1989, it was always the

More information

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 348 370 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Chris Hope and Angelo Santi Wilfrid

More information

We want youloknow about. nutrition labels on food. Oregon State University Extension Service

We want youloknow about. nutrition labels on food. Oregon State University Extension Service We want youloknow about nutrition labels on food % Oregon State University Extension Service NUTRITION LABELING Many food processors are putting nutrition information on their food labels. All fortified

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

FATS AND PELLETS IN BROILER RATIONS. G. H. Arscott

FATS AND PELLETS IN BROILER RATIONS. G. H. Arscott 32 FATS AND PELLETS IN BROILER RATIONS By G. H. Arscott \.5 RA140.4 v. OCT 9 1958 41' Miscellaneous Paper 37 April 1957 Agricultural Experiment Station Oregon State College Corvallis p FATS AND PELLETS

More information

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 74-84 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.74 Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure PATRICK VAN ROOYEN AND ANGELO

More information

How Do I Supplement My Livestock With Minerals? Part IV

How Do I Supplement My Livestock With Minerals? Part IV How Do I Supplement My Livestock With s? Part IV Randy Wiedmeier, Regional Livestock Specialist, South-Central Area All of our livestock animals possess a specific appetite or craving for sodium. As demonstrated

More information

Free Choice Sheep Mineral

Free Choice Sheep Mineral Free Choice Sheep Mineral Sheep on pasture Guaranteed Analysis CALCIUM (Ca), not less than... 12.5 % CALCIUM (Ca), not more than... 13.5 % PHOSPHORUS (P), not less than... 9.80 % SALT (NaCl), not less

More information

C. N. Groesbeck, R. D. Goodband, M. D. Tokach, S. S. Dritz 2, J. L. Nelssen, J. M. DeRouchey, B. W. James, T. P. Keegan, and K. R.

C. N. Groesbeck, R. D. Goodband, M. D. Tokach, S. S. Dritz 2, J. L. Nelssen, J. M. DeRouchey, B. W. James, T. P. Keegan, and K. R. Swine Day 2004 INTERACTIVE EFFECTS BETWEEN PANTOTHENIC ACID AND RACTOPAMINE HCl (PAYLEAN ) ON GROWTH PERFORMANCE AND CARCASS CHARACTERIS- TICS OF GROWING-FINISHING PIGS 1 C. N. Groesbeck, R. D. Goodband,

More information

Determining the threonine requirement of the high-producing lactating sow. D.R. Cooper, J.F. Patience, R.T. Zijlstra and M.

Determining the threonine requirement of the high-producing lactating sow. D.R. Cooper, J.F. Patience, R.T. Zijlstra and M. 66 Determining the threonine requirement of the high-producing lactating sow D.R. Cooper, J.F. Patience, R.T. Zijlstra and M. Rademacher Introduction There are two steps in the design of a feeding strategy.

More information

Optimal foraging theory

Optimal foraging theory Optimal foraging theory Christopher Furlong/Getty Images 2007 John Vucetich John Ascher 2006-2010 Where is Moriarty going? Purpose of trip: buy ice cream for home Moriarty wants to get home as fast as

More information

BROILER. Nutrition Specifications. An Aviagen Brand

BROILER. Nutrition Specifications. An Aviagen Brand BROILER 708 Nutrition Specifications 2014 An Aviagen Brand Introduction Nutrition specifications for Ross 708 broilers are given in the following tables for a range of production and market situations

More information

AVIAN - FARE Balanced Diet for Omnivorous Birds Including- Storks, Ibis, Cranes, Hornbills, Finches, Robins etc.

AVIAN - FARE Balanced Diet for Omnivorous Birds Including- Storks, Ibis, Cranes, Hornbills, Finches, Robins etc. AVIAN - FARE Balanced Diet for Omnivorous Birds Including- Storks, Ibis, Cranes, Hornbills, Finches, Robins etc. Porcine By-Products, Poultry Meal, Fish Meal, Dried Bakery Products, Shrimp Meal, Dried

More information

Efficiency of Utilization of Dietary Phosphorus By Caged Turkey Breeder Hens When Fed Rations Supplemented With Live Yeast Culture

Efficiency of Utilization of Dietary Phosphorus By Caged Turkey Breeder Hens When Fed Rations Supplemented With Live Yeast Culture POULTRY NUTRITION Efficiency of Utilization of Dietary Phosphorus By Caged Turkey Breeder Hens When Fed Rations Supplemented With Live Yeast Culture R.H. Thayer, R.F. Burkitt, R.D. Morrison and E. E. Murray

More information

An Alternative Molting Procedure By Don Bell, Poultry Specialist (emeritus) University of California, Riverside

An Alternative Molting Procedure By Don Bell, Poultry Specialist (emeritus) University of California, Riverside 5/18/2005 UC Research For Non-feed-removal Molting 1 An Alternative Molting Procedure By Don Bell, Poultry Specialist (emeritus) University of California, Riverside (A supplement to Farm Evaluation of

More information

MyPlate.gov Assignment

MyPlate.gov Assignment Name: Class Hour: MyPlate.gov Assignment Directions: In 2011 the USDA came out with MyPlate.gov, a way to help Americans live a more healthy life. In this assignment you will explore the site and see how

More information

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Summer 2005 Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli William

More information

Full file at Designing a Healthful Diet

Full file at   Designing a Healthful Diet THOM.4427.cp02.p008-012_vpdf 10/26/06 7:09 PM Page 8 Chapter 2 Designing a Healthful Diet Chapter Summary A healthful diet provides the proper combination of energy and nutrients and has four characteristics:

More information

Department of Animal Sciences, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida 32611

Department of Animal Sciences, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida 32611 Performance of Commercial Laying Hens when Six Percent Corn Oil Is Added to the Diet at Various Ages and with Different Levels of Tryptophan and Protein 1 R. S. Antar, R. H. Harms, 2 M. Shivazad, 3 D.

More information

Log on to your PUCC account and set up your Brill diet formulation files. You need to do the following. Your TA will guide you through the process.

Log on to your PUCC account and set up your Brill diet formulation files. You need to do the following. Your TA will guide you through the process. ANSC 324 Spring, 2007 Laboratory Exercise #3 Due Date: COMPLETE IN LAB (no point value) This laboratory exercise will emphasize ingredient and nutrient selection for computerized diet formulation. You

More information

USING A SELF-CONTROL TRAINING PROCEDURE TO INCREASE APPROPRIATE BEHAVIOR MARK R. DIXON AND LINDA J. HAYES

USING A SELF-CONTROL TRAINING PROCEDURE TO INCREASE APPROPRIATE BEHAVIOR MARK R. DIXON AND LINDA J. HAYES JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1998, 31, 203 210 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 1998) USING A SELF-CONTROL TRAINING PROCEDURE TO INCREASE APPROPRIATE BEHAVIOR MARK R. DIXON AND LINDA J. HAYES UNIVERSITY OF NEVADA

More information

The Evaluation of Dehulled Canola Meal as a Replacement for Soybean Meal in the Diets of Growing and Finishing Pigs

The Evaluation of Dehulled Canola Meal as a Replacement for Soybean Meal in the Diets of Growing and Finishing Pigs The Evaluation of Dehulled Canola Meal as a Replacement for Soybean Meal in the Diets of Growing and Finishing Pigs J.F. Patience, D. Gillis and C.F.M. de Lange Executive Summary The major restriction

More information

Soy Protein Concentrate as a Substitute for Fishmeal in the Feed for Black Carp

Soy Protein Concentrate as a Substitute for Fishmeal in the Feed for Black Carp Key Words: Black carp, soy protein concentrate, soybean meal, 80:20 pond technology, China Soy Protein Concentrate as a Substitute for Fishmeal in the Feed for Black Carp INTRODUCTION Results of the 2008

More information

Swine: Feeding 1. Randy Walker 2

Swine: Feeding 1. Randy Walker 2 Swine: Feeding 1 Randy Walker 2 Table 1. Maximum Amount of Different Feeds for Various Rations % Complete Ration Feed Gestation Lactation Starter Grower Finisher Alfalfa meal 90 10 0 5 5 Barley 80 80 25

More information

Joint Mobility brand Canine Formula. Nutrition formulated to promote joint health. A dog owner s guide from your veterinarian and Purina.

Joint Mobility brand Canine Formula. Nutrition formulated to promote joint health. A dog owner s guide from your veterinarian and Purina. JOINT MOBILITY C A N I N E F O R M U L A Joint Mobility Canine Formula Nutrition formulated to promote joint health. A dog owner s guide from your veterinarian and Purina. Purina Veterinary Diets are sold

More information

Whey-Grown Yeast as a Protein Source for Baby Pigs

Whey-Grown Yeast as a Protein Source for Baby Pigs Whey-Grown Yeast as a Protein Source for Baby Pigs Y. J. Ajeani, C. V. Maxwell, F. N. Owens, D. Holbert, K. B. Poling and J. S. Schooley Story in Brief The nutritive value of whey-grown yeast for baby

More information

INTRODUCTION TO ADVANCED NUTRITION TECHNOLOGY, INC.

INTRODUCTION TO ADVANCED NUTRITION TECHNOLOGY, INC. 2 Advanced Nutrition Technology, Inc. P.O. Box 171 Rock Valley, Iowa 51247 Phone: (712) 439-1932 or (877) 393-1987 Fax: (712) 439-2830 INTRODUCTION TO ADVANCED NUTRITION TECHNOLOGY, INC. Advanced Nutrition

More information

Recommended Resources: The following resources may be useful in teaching

Recommended Resources: The following resources may be useful in teaching Unit C: Poultry Management Lesson 1: Nutrients for Maintenance, Growth and Reproduction Student Learning Objectives: Instruction in this lesson should result in students achieving the following objectives:

More information

Emeraid Intensive Care HDN

Emeraid Intensive Care HDN Emeraid Intensive Care HDN Balanced nutrition for critically ill felines Emeraid Intensive Care HDN Feline or Highly Digestible Nutrition is a semi-elemental therapeutic diet developed by veterinarians

More information

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia AM. ZOOI.OCIST, 12:507-512 (1972). Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia PAUL DOWNEY AND BEHRUS JAHAN-PARWAR Biology Department, Clark University, Worcester, Massachusetts 01610 and Worcester Foundation

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

Product Information:

Product Information: Product Information: Pro-Phree 1 of 5 Nutrition support of infants and toddlers who require extra calories, minerals, and vitamins and/or protein restriction. Use under medical supervision. Protein-free

More information

7200 Poultry Premix: Poultry concentrate with MHA and Levucell SC Yeast.

7200 Poultry Premix: Poultry concentrate with MHA and Levucell SC Yeast. Additives: Availa 4 : Co, Cu, Mn and Zn amino acid complex source (registered trademark of ZinPro Performance Minerals ). Levucell SC: Active dry yeast Saccharomyces cerevisiae (registered trademark of

More information

Exp Research Report. Digestibility of energy and concentration of digestible and metabolizable energy in high

Exp Research Report. Digestibility of energy and concentration of digestible and metabolizable energy in high Exp. 582 Research Report Digestibility of energy and concentration of digestible and metabolizable energy in high protein and conventional DDGS fed to growing pigs. C. D. Espinosa and H. H. Stein University

More information

Interaction of social and individual learning in food preferences of Norway rats

Interaction of social and individual learning in food preferences of Norway rats ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1, 62, 41 46 doi:1.16/anbe..1721, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Interaction of social and individual learning in food preferences of Norway rats BENNETT G. GALEF,

More information

Discounting in pigeons when the choice is between two delayed rewards: implications for species comparisons

Discounting in pigeons when the choice is between two delayed rewards: implications for species comparisons Original Research Article published: 17 August 2011 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00096 Discounting in pigeons when the choice is between two delayed rewards: implications for species comparisons Amanda L. Calvert,

More information

nutrition, vitamin levels in other ingredients and level of metabolic precursors in the diet. Summary

nutrition, vitamin levels in other ingredients and level of metabolic precursors in the diet. Summary Swine Day 2001 Contents INFLUENCE OF INCREASING NIACIN ON GROWTH PERFORMANCE AND CARCASS CHARACTERISTICS OF GROW-FINISH PIGS REARED IN A COMMERCIAL ENVIRONMENT 1 D. E. Real, J. L. Nelssen, J. A. Unruh,

More information

Product Information: Tyrex -1

Product Information: Tyrex -1 Product Information: Tyrex -1 1 of 5 Nutrition support of infants and toddlers with tyrosinemia types I, II or III. Phenylalanine- and tyrosine-free. Use under medical supervision. Phenylalanine- and tyrosine-free

More information

There are six general classes of nutrients needed in the horse s diet: water carbohydrates fats protein minerals vitamins.

There are six general classes of nutrients needed in the horse s diet: water carbohydrates fats protein minerals vitamins. HORSE NUTRITION Nutrients A nutrient is defined as any feed constituent that is necessary to support life. The following is a list of functions that nutrients perform in the horse's body: source of energy

More information

: /18

: /18 612.461.23: 616-001.17/18 SOME OBSERVATIONS ON THE COMPARATIVE EFFECTS OF COLD AND BURNS ON PROTEIN METABOLISM IN RATS. By G. H. LATHE 1 and R. A. PETERS. From the Department of Biochemistry, Oxford. (Received

More information

Imitation and Affordance Learning by Pigeons (Columba livia)

Imitation and Affordance Learning by Pigeons (Columba livia) Journal of Comparative Psychology Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 117, No. 4, 414 419 0735-7036/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.4.414 Imitation and Affordance

More information

Product Information: Propimex -1

Product Information: Propimex -1 Product Information: Propimex -1 1 of 5 Nutrition support of infants and toddlers with propionic or methylmalonic acidemia. Methionine- and valine-free; low in isoleucine and threonine. Use under medical

More information

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Learning and Motivation 39 (2008) 71 94 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Luís Oliveira,

More information

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a Effects of Food Deprivation on Memory Performance. Jacob. L. Kerewaro Abstract The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

Digestion of the Bison

Digestion of the Bison Digestion of the Bison Roll Call: Because the stomachs of the ruminant and monogastric animals are so different, their diets also vary. Let's compare the diets. Tell me what foods each of these animals

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

A Brief Overview of Catfish Nutrition

A Brief Overview of Catfish Nutrition Vol. 24, No. 16 Research Report September 2015 A Brief Overview of Catfish Nutrition Edwin H. Robinson and Menghe H. Li INTRODUCTION Modern catfish feeds are based on data derived from extensive nutritional

More information

Unit C: Poultry Management. Lesson 1: Nutrients for Maintenance, Growth and Reproduction

Unit C: Poultry Management. Lesson 1: Nutrients for Maintenance, Growth and Reproduction Unit C: Poultry Management Lesson 1: Nutrients for Maintenance, Growth and Reproduction 1 1 Carbohydrates Fats Minerals Nutrients Proteins Vitamins Water Terms 2 2 I. Nutrients are the chemical substances

More information

Module 1 Nutrition Basics. Exam 1 B

Module 1 Nutrition Basics. Exam 1 B Module 1-2 Test 1B 1 Module 1 Nutrition Basics PLEASE DO NOT MARK ON THIS COPY. USE YOUR SCANTRONS TO MARK UNSWERS. True/False Exam 1 B 1. Appetite is defined as the psychological desire for food. 2. Cellulose

More information

Product Information: Ketonex -1

Product Information: Ketonex -1 Product Information: 1 of 5 Nutrition support of infants and toddlers with maple syrup urine disease (MSUD). Isoleucine-, leucine- and valine-free. Use under medical supervision. Branched-chain amino acid-free

More information

STANDARDIZATION ORGANIZATION FOR G.C.C (GSO)

STANDARDIZATION ORGANIZATION FOR G.C.C (GSO) ھيئة التقييس لدول مجلس التعاون لدول الخليج العربية STANDARDIZATION ORGANIZATION FOR G.C.C (GSO) Final Draft GSO5/FDS/CAC./2013 اشتراطات األغذية ذات اإلدعاءات التغذوية والصحية Nutritional and Health Claims

More information

Example 1: Label of a complete feed (with minimum voluntary labelling particulars)

Example 1: Label of a complete feed (with minimum voluntary labelling particulars) Example 1: Label of a complete feed (with minimum voluntary Product Code 4567 Bloggs Broiler Grower Complete feed for feeding to growing chickens of 14 to 24 days of age Protein 21% Methionine 0.6% Fibre

More information

Protein Deposition in Growing and Finishing Pigs

Protein Deposition in Growing and Finishing Pigs 1 Protein Deposition in Growing and Finishing Pigs DETERMINING WHOLE BODY PROTEIN DEPOSITION RATES IN PIGS. Mark L. Lorschy, Doug A. Gillis, John F. Patience and Kees de Lange. Summary There is controversy

More information