THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee"

Transcription

1 The Psychological Record, 1997,47, THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pigeons served as subjects in two concurrent-chains experiments in which responding on one or the other of two side keys occasionally produced either a short- ( 10-s) or a long- ( 20-s) duration terminal-link schedule of food reinforcement according to equal variable-interval 30-s initial-link schedules. In Experiment 1, the terminal link was in effect randomly on either key, or always on the opposite key, in relation to the initial link from which it was entered. In addition, terminal-link stimuli were differential keylights, nondifferential keylights, or differential houselights. Preference tended toward indifference with a random location of a terminal link and nondifferential keylights. However, the birds preferred the shorter terminal link when the terminal link was always in effect on the same key, or always on the opposite key, with either differential or nondifferential keylights. The birds also preferred the shorter terminal link with a random location, nondifferential keylights, and differential houselights. In Experiment 2, the terminal link was always in effect on the center key. As before, terminal-link stimuli were differential keylights, nondifferential keylights, or differential houselights. Preference tended toward indifference with nondifferential keylights. However, the birds preferred the shorter terminal link with either differential keylights or differential houselights. Taken together, the data indicate that terminal-link stimuli in concurrent chains function as conditioned reinforcers, rather than simple discriminative stimuli, although other features of the terminal links such as stimulus location act in concert with the visual stimuli to influence preference. The concurrent-chains procedure has long been used in studies of choice (e.g., Autor, 1960,1969; Herrnstein, 1964). Typically, pigeons are presented with two concurrently available response keys, each illuminated by a stimulus correlated with the initial link of a chain schedule of reinforcement. Occasionally, a response on either of the keys produces a terminal-link stimulus in whose presence primary Reprint requests may be sent to Laurel Colton, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Garland Hall, Box 413, Milwaukee, WI

2 146 COLTON AND MOORE reinforcement (e.g., food) occurs according to a designated schedule. Following the delivery of reinforcement, the initial links are reinstated for the next period of choice, and the cycle begins again. An experimental session consists of a series of such exposures, typically until a specified number of reinforcers is obtained. The major dependent variable in the concurrent-chains procedure is the relative response rate in the initial links, or choice proportion, calculated by dividing the rate of initial-link responding on one key by the rate on both keys. A long-standing experimental question concerns the contribution of terminal-link stimuli to responding in concurrent chains. Two possibilities have emerged in the literature. The first is that the stimuli are merely discriminative for a given rate of primary reinforcement and without any conditioned reinforcing strength (Baum, 1973, p. 144). The second is that they function as conditioned reinforcers and actually mediate initiallink choice responding (Williams & Fantino, 1978, p. 86). A number of studies have reported data on this question over the years. For example, Autor (1960, 1969) presented pigeons with a choice between various aperiodic terminal-link schedules (e.g., variable-interval, VI, and variable-time). A terminal link was always located on the same key from which the terminal link was entered. Autor found a higher rate of initiallink responding on the key leading to the higher terminal-link rate of reinforcement. He concluded that responding in concurrent chains was mediated through the conditioned reinforcement provided by the terminallink stimuli, a conclusion that is consistent with a more molecular view of the variables controlling preference in concurrent chains. Herrnstein (1964) also presented pigeons with a choice between aperiodic terminal-link schedules (e.g., VI and variable-ratio), in a procedure similar to Autor's where the terminal link was located on the same key from which the terminal link was entered. He found that the pigeons' initial-link responding was distributed in proportion to the relative rate of terminal-link reinforcement. Herrnstein's results are often said to support a molar view of behavioral processes, according to which (a) the overall correlation between responding and reinforcement is primary, and (b) putative conditioned reinforcers function merely as discriminative stimuli without conditioned reinforcing value of their own (e.g., Baum, 1973, p. 144). Later studies continued the attempt to dissociate the possible roles of terminal-link stimuli in concurrent chains. Williams and Fantino (1978) gave pigeons a choice between terminal links consisting of different fixed-interval () schedules. The terminal-link stimuli could appear randomly on left or right keys. In addition, the terminal-link stimuli appearing on each key were either correlated or uncorrelated with the schedule in effect. The use of the randomly alternating position of the terminal-link key and the use of correlated or uncorrelated terminal-link stimuli allowed Williams and Fantino to examine the effects of overall time to reinforcement independent of any immediate changes in stimuli that could be construed as differential conditioned reinforcement. In

3 EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS 147 some conditions, because the terminal-link stimulus was uncorrelated with left- and right-key schedules, any difference in initial-link responding could be considered a result of the overall delay to reinforcement. Williams and Fantino (1978) found higher preferences for the shorter terminal link when terminal-link stimuli were correlated rather than uncorrelated with schedule duration. They concluded that their findings "support the role of conditioned reinforcement as a determinant of choice in the concurrent-chains procedure" (p. 86). Moreover, their findings were inconsistent with the molar notions that choice behavior is a function only of the primary reinforcement presented at the end of each cycle and that terminal-link stimuli are solely discriminative. In related studies, Omino (1993) and Omino and Ito (1993) examined preference in concurrent chains using a procedure similar to Williams and Fantino (1978). In both Omino (1993) and Omino and Ito (1993), the terminal links were fixed-time (FT) schedules. Entrance into one of the terminal links changed the keylight from white to an "uncued blackout" condition or remained white ("uncued white" condition) while the other key went dark. In both cases, preference for the FT terminallink schedule with the shorter delay to reinforcement increased with increases in absolute values of paired delays in both uncued blackout and uncued white conditions. Furthermore, preference for the shorter delay was higher in the uncued white than in the uncued blackout conditions. Thus, Omino (1993) concluded that "the terminal-link keylight stimulus condition differentially exerts its effect on choice as a conditioned reinforcer in the concurrent-chains procedure" (p. 591). More recently, Alsop, Stewart, and Honig (1994) examined preference using a three-key procedure, where initial links were in effect on the side keys and terminal links on a center key. Fixed-interval terminal-link schedules were varied across conditions. In one case, the center key was always illuminated with white light during the terminal links, regardless of which initial-link schedule had arranged access to the terminal link. In another case, the center key was illuminated with red light if the left initial-link key had arranged access to the terminal link, or blue light if the right initial-link key had arranged access to the terminal link. Preferences for the shorter terminal-link schedule were greater when different keylights were used, a finding consistent with Omino (1993), Omino and Ito (1993), and Williams and Fantino (1978). Based on the procedures in the studies cited above, the relevant research can be organized into a 3 x 4 table in terms of (a) the location of a terminal link relative to its associated initial link and (b) whether an unique exteroceptive stimulus is present during the terminal link. Table 1 presents a summary of terminal-link stimulus arrangements employed in the relevant research on choice in concurrent chains. The horizontal axis of Table 1 indicates the location of the terminal link relative to the location of the initial link from which the terminal link was entered. The first three columns pertain to terminal-link stimulus arrangements in which two keys are used. In the same arrangement, the

4 148 COLTON AND MOORE Table 1 Studies Concerning Conditioned Reinforcement and Preference in Concurrent Chains and Possible Terminal-Link Stimulus Conditions Location of terminal-link stimulus Nature of terminal- Same Random Opposite Center link stimulus (two-key) (two-key) (two-key (three-key) Cued Autor (1960,1969) Williams & Alsop et al. Herrnstein (1964) Fantino (1978) (1994) Uncued Omino (1993) Williams & Alsop et al. Omino & Ito (1993) Fantino (1978) (1994) Houselight >< >< terminal link was always located on the same key from which the terminal link was entered. This procedure is the conventional procedure used in the vast majority of concurrent chains studies. In the random arrangement, the terminal link appears randomly on the same key or the opposite key from which the terminal link was entered. In the opposite arrangement, the terminal link always appears on the opposite key from which the terminal link was entered. The fourth column pertains to arrangements in which three keys are used. In the center arrangement, the initial links appear on the two side keys, and a terminal link is always in effect on the center key. The vertical axis of Table 1 indicates the nature of the terminallink stimuli. Here, we will attempt to maintain consistency by using the terminology of the previous studies. In cued conditions, the terminallink stimuli are differential keylights (i.e., different keylight colors associated with each terminal link). In uncued conditions, the terminal-link stimuli are nondifferential keylights. In houselight conditions, the terminal-link stimuli are differential houselights. Table 1 presents the six relevant studies noted above according to the terminal-link stimulus arrangements used: same-cued (Autor, 1960, 1969; Herrnstein, 1964), same-uncued (Omino, 1993; Omino & Ito, 1993), random-cued and -uncued (Williams & Fantino, 1978), and center-cued and -uncued (Alsop et ai., 1994). The present research sought to determine the generality of the prior results and also fill in the unexamined cells of the matrix in Table 1 in order to clarify the role of terminal-link stimuli. Experiment 1 employed a modified two-key version of the concurrent-chains procedure. Pigeons chose between terminal links consisting of schedules of different durations. In some conditions, terminal links appeared randomly on either the left or the right key. In other conditions, they always appeared on the key opposite that on which initial-link responding had accessed the terminal link. Behavior was examined in cued conditions, uncued conditions, and with differential houselights. Differential houselights were

5 EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS 149 used in recognition of prior research indicating that such stimuli influence choice responding (Brownstein & Pliskoff, 1968). Thus, Experiment 1 permitted the investigation of two previously unexamined terminal-link stimulus conditions and the effects of terminal-link keylight stimuli on stimulus control of initial-link responding. It also examined the effect of differential houselights on terminal-link stimulus control in a manner not previously used. Experiment 2 employed a three-key version of the concurrent-chains procedure much like that of Alsop et al. (1994). Pigeons chose between terminal links consisting of schedules of different durations. Terminallink keylight stimuli and schedules were in effect on a center key. As in Experiment 1, behavior was examined in cued conditions, uncued conditions, and with differential houselights. Thus, in addition to examining the generality of the findings of Alsop et al. (1994) using a three-key arrangement, Experiment 2 determined whether preference develops in this center-key arrangement when nondifferential keylights and differential houselights are employed. Data suggesting that patterns of preference and other characteristics of initial-link responding remain relatively constant across all manipulations would indicate that overall rate of primary reinforcement in the terminal links is central, independent of any contribution of conditioned reinforcement. These data would suggest that the primary role of terminal-link stimuli is simply to enhance discrimination of terminal-link events (Baum, 1973; Schneider, 1972; Schuster, 1969). However, data suggesting that preferences are higher in cued conditions than in uncued conditions would indicate that conditioned reinforcement does contribute to responding, as reported by Williams and Fantino (1978) and others (see Williams, 1994, for a review). General Method Subjects Four adult male pigeons of mixed breeds and different experimental histories (R-222, R-213, R-1580, R-187) served as subjects. The birds were maintained at approximately 80% of their free-feeding weights. Sessions were conducted at approximately the same time of day, 6 days per week, under 23-h food deprivation. Apparatus Three-key experimental chambers for pigeons were used. Only the two side keys were used in Experiment 1, and all three keys were used in Experiment 2. Conventional electromechanical equipment located in an adjacent room controlled events and recorded the data. Houselights were used only on specific occasions for differential cues, rather than for general illumination. These houselights were either red or green 11 OV AC, 6-W light bulbs, illuminated only during the terminal links. The bulbs were located behind a plastic diffusing panel at the top rear of the experimental chambers.

6 150 COLTON AND MOORE General Procedure The initial links in both Experiments 1 and 2 were equal, interdependent, variable-interval (VI) 30-s schedules (Stubbs & Pliskoff, 1969). The schedules were interdependent in the sense that whenever one terminal link was assigned, the other initial-link schedule became inoperative. This control procedure ensured that the pigeons entered each terminal link equally often. During the initial links, the keylights were white. With the exceptions noted below, an 10-s schedule was in effect during one terminal link and an 20-s schedule was in effect during the other. During a terminal link, responding on the operative key produced a primary reinforcer (3-s access to mixed grain) according to the schedule in effect. The initial links were reinstated after each primary reinforcer. As in Williams and Fantino (1978), but unlike Alsop et al. (1994), no changeover delay was used. Each session terminated after the delivery of 40 reinforcers. Each condition was in effect until a stability criterion was satisfied. This criterion required that the birds perform in at least 15 sessions. The final nine sessions were divided into three blocks of three sessions each. A standard choice proportion (Chung & Herrnstein, 1967; Herrnstein, 1961) was calculated for each session and a mean choice proportion obtained for each block of sessions. Responding was considered stable when choice proportions did not differ by more than.05 and showed no increasing or decreasing trend across blocks. The data of primary interest were the asymtotic preference levels for each condition, expressed as the average choice proportion for the nine stable sessions. All four birds served in both experiments. Pigeons R-222 and R-187 served first in Experiment 1 and then in Experiment 2. Pigeons R-213 and R-1580 served first in Experiment 2 and then in Experiment 1. Experiment 1 Procedure In Experiment 1, all key arrangements involved only the two side keys. Behavior was examined in seven conditions appearing in Table 2: Same-cued, Same-uncued, Random-cued, Random-uncued, Oppositecued, Opposite-uncued, and Houselight. With the exception of the houselight condition, these conditions are presented schematically in Figure 1. In cued conditions, the terminal-link stimulus in effect when a terminal link was entered from the left initial link was a green light, and from the right initial link, a red light. In uncued conditions, a circle was in effect during the terminal links, regardless of whether the terminal link was entered from the left or right initial link. In the opposite arrangement (Figure 1, third row), the terminal link always appeared on the opposite key from which the terminal link was entered. The effects of this manipulation have never been examined before. Thus, these conditions attempted to extend the study of Williams and Fantino (1978) and to determine whether the location of the terminal link influenced terminal-link stimulus control of initial-link responding.

7 EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS 151 Table 2 Experiment 1: Sequence of Conditions and Terminal-Link Schedules for Each Subject Terminal-link schedule after entry from: Sequence for each subject -- Condition Left key Right key R-222 R-213 R-1580 R-187 Same-cued 10 s 20 s 1, 13 2,13 1, 13 2, s 10 s Same-uncued 10 s 20 s s 10 s Opposite-cued 10 s 20 s s 10 s Opposite-u ncued 10 s 20 s s 10 s Random-cued 10 s 20 s s 10 s Random-uncued 10 s 20 s s 10 s Houselight 10 s 20 s s 10 s Note. Birds R-222 and R-187 were not exposed to the houselight condition in Experiment 1. In the houselight condition, the location of a terminal link was random, and the keylights were nondifferential (i.e., a circle) as in the random-uncued condition. However, a red houselight was in effect during a terminal link entered from the right initial-link key, and a green houselight was in effect during a terminal link entered from the left initiallink key. The effects of this manipulation also have never been examined before. The manipulation sought to determine whether differential houselights would control initial-link responding, in the absence of differential keylights and reliable location of terminal-link keylight stimuli. As indicated in Table 2, an initial determination of preference was made between 10-s and 20-s schedules. Then, a reversal was conducted in which the schedule that was formerly 10 s became 20 s, and vice versa. A reversal served to demonstrate experimental control by the conditions in effect. In Figure 1, these initial and reversed assignments of schedules to the terminal links are respectively indicated by two numbers for each (i.e., 10 and 20). Each subject was exposed to its conditions in a different order, and reversals were performed immediately after initial determinations of preference. All subjects began with the same-cued and same-uncued conditions. However, they were exposed to these conditions only once, because these conditions were common to both Experiments 1 and 2. For ease of comparison, all four birds are listed in Table 2. Upon completing all experimental conditions in Experiments 1 and 2, all subjects were exposed to a final same-cued condition in which left and right terminal links were 10 sand 20 s, respectively. This final determination confirmed performance in the same-cued condition.

8 152 COLTON AND MOORE Same-cued VI 30 VI 30 Same-uncued VI 30 VI30 W 10/20 20/10 10/20 20/10 Random-cued VI 30 VI 30 W Random-uncued VI 30 VI 30 W 10/20 (p-.s) 20/10 (p=-.5) 10/20 (p=.5) 20/10 (p=.5) Opposite-cued VI 30 VI 30 Opposite-uncued VI 30 VI 30 10/20 20/10 10/20 20/10 Figure 1. Key arrangements used in Experiment 1. After the behavior of Pigeons R-213 and R-1580 was examined under the same, random, and opposite arrangements, these two birds were exposed to the houselight condition. The keylight stimuli in this condition were the same as those in the random-uncued condition, except that a green housejight was in effect when a terminal link was entered from the left key, and a red housejight was in effect when a terminal link was entered from the right key. Results Figure 2 presents the average asymptotic preferences in terms of the shorter terminal-link schedule ( 10 s) for all four birds. The preferences in each condition are averaged across the last nine stable

9 EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS 153 Cued o Uncued 1.0 R-222 R (]) (.) ~ (]) ~ a.. R-187 R Same Opposite Random Same Opposite Random Houselight TERMINAL-LINK ARRANGMENT Figure 2. Experiment 1: Average asymptotic preference (choice proportions for 10-s over 20-s terminal-link schedule) in cued and uncued key arrangements for each subject. sessions for initial determinations and reversals. The only exception was the same-cued condition for which preferences in initial, reversed, and final determinations were averaged. Shaded bars represent cued conditions and unshaded bars represent uncued conditions. The same-cued and sameuncued conditions are shown for all four birds. The houselight condition for Pigeons R-213 and R-1S80 is represented by a shaded bar. Figure 2 shows that with the same arrangement, all four birds preferred the 10-s schedule in both the same-cued and sameuncued conditions. The finding of equivalent preference in these two conditions contrasts with that of Omino (1993), who found somewhat lower preferences in the same-uncued condition (his "uncued white" condition).

10 154 COLTON AND MOORE In the opposite arrangement, all four birds again preferred the 10- s schedule in both the opposite-cued and opposite-uncued conditions. Note that the average preference exhibited by R-1580 in the oppositecued condition is slightly less than that exhibited in the opposite-uncued condition (see Figure 2). No other birds exhibited this pattern. In the random arrangement, all four birds preferred the 10-s schedule in the random-cued condition. However, three of the four birds tended toward indifference in the random-uncued condition. The only exception was the slight preference exhibited by R-187. Inspection of the data in Table 3 indicates that any preferences that appeared during initial determinations in this condition were unreliable in that they never fully reversed for Pigeons R-222, R-213, and R Table 3 Experiment 1: Absolute and Relative Response-Rate and Reinforcement Data T-L I-L T-L T-L schedule (s) resp/min resp/min IRI (s) Bird Condition L R L R L R L R P (SO) Sessions R-222 Same-cued (.02) (.03) 30 Same-uncued (.06) (.04) 25 Random-cued (.04) (.04) 18 Random-uncued (.04) (.04) 15 Opposite-cued (.04) (.08) 16 Opposite-uncued (.01) (.14) 30* Final Same-cued (.03) 16 R-187 Same-cued (.03) (.01) 15 Same-uncued (03) (.05) 30* Random-cued (.02) (.04) 19 Random-uncued (.04) (.10) 30* Opposite-cued (.09) (.05) 24 Opposite-uncued (.04) (.05) 24 Final Same-cued (.03) 23 R-1580 Same-cued (. 02) (.01) 23 Same-uncued (.02) (.04) 28 Random-cued (.01) (.03) 23 Random-uncued (.04) (.03) 18

11 EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS 155 R-1S80 Opposite-cued (.06) (.07) 15 Opposite-uncued (.03) (.01) 15 Final Same-cued (.02) 15 Houselight (.03) (.02) 17 R-213 Same-cued (.03) (.02) 21 Same-uncued (.05) 30* (.03) 18 Random-cued (.06) (.03) 25 Random-uncued (.03) (.06) 30* Opposite-cued (.03) (.03) 23 Opposite-uncued (.03) (.03) 20 Final Same-cued (.00) 27 Houselight (.03) (.06) 19 Note. Data presented for each pigeon and condition are: IRI = interreinforcement interval; P = asymptotic level of preference; SO = choice proportion standard deviation; where T-L = terminal-link and I-L = initial-link. The choice proportion (P) is expressed in terms of the 10-s terminal link. Choice proportions for the shorter terminal-link schedule ( 10 s) were calculated, e.g., L / (L + R), where Land R represent the number of responses during left and right initial links, respectively. Absolute response rates during the initial links were calculated, e.g., L / total time in initial links. Absolute response rates during the terminal links were calculated, e.g., rate during 10 / (rate during 10 + rate during 20). Interreinforcement intervals were calculated, e.g., total time in left terminal link / number of reinforcements obtained in left terminal link. * Preference failed to stabilize within 30 sessions. In the houselight condition, both birds reliably preferred the 10- s schedule. The preferences here were exhibited even though the birds were responding on a key which appeared in a random location with a nondifferential keylight. Table 3 displays additional data from Experiment 1. Absolute response rates during the initial links were again averaged across the final nine sessions of the initial determinations and reversals. These absolute response rates were higher on the key leading to the 10-s terminal-link schedule in all conditions except the random-uncued condition. Rather, in this condition, response rates were often higher during the initial link leading to the longer terminal-link schedule ( 20 s). For three of the four birds, initial-link response rates did not reverse with reversals in terminal links for this random-uncued condition. In this condition, only R-187 responded with higher rates during whichever initial link led to the shorter terminal-link schedule. Recall that this bird also slightly preferred whichever key was correlated with the shorter terminal-link schedule in initial and reversed random-uncued conditions.

12 156 COLTON AND MOORE Table 3 also presents absolute response rates during the terminal links, averaged across the final nine sessions of the initial determinations and reversals. Figure 3 presents relative response rates during the terminal links, expressed in terms of the shorter 10-s terminal link, for each condition. All relative response-rate data were averaged over initial determinations and reversals. Absolute response rates during the terminal links showed no systematic trends across conditions. Relative response rates during the 10-s terminal link were often higher in cued conditions (>.50, as shown in Figure 3) than in uncued conditions (approximately.50, as shown in Figure 3). However, some conditions produced the opposite result (for example, compare same-cued and -uncued conditions for Pigeons R-222, R-213, and R-1580). C/) ~ c: :::i 1.0 cts c: EO.5 ~ ~ 0> c: "C ::J 0.0 Cl... cts a: 1.0 C/) c: o Q. C/) a: 0.5 > -..::; ~ a: 0.0 R-222 R-187 Cued o Uncued R-213 R-1580 Same Opposite Random Same Opposite Random Houselight TERMINAL-LINK ARRANGEMENT Figure 3. Experiment 1: Relative terminal-link responses/min expressed in terms of the shorter ( 10-s) terminal-link schedule and averaged over initial determinations and reversals.

13 EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS 157 Interreinforcement intervals (IRis) were calculated (in seconds) from the final nine sessions of terminal-link data for each condition. Table 3 shows that the obtained I Rls were approximately 10 sand 20 s, corresponding to terminal-link schedules of 10 sand 20 s, respectively. After subjects had completed all conditions, a final same-cued ( 10 vs. 20) assessment reconfirmed preferences for the shorter terminallink schedule as observed previously. Table 3 includes choice-proportion standard deviations and the number of sessions required for preferences to stabilize in each condition of Experiment 1. No systematic differences in either of these measures emerged between conditions. Cases in which preferences failed to stabilize within 30 sessions occurred in uncued conditions only and are indicated by asterisks in Table 3. Experiment 2 Procedure The same four birds served in Experiment 2. Behavior was examined in the four comparisons listed in Table 4: Same-cued, Centercued, Center-uncued, and Houselight. With the exception of the houselight condition, these conditions appear in Figure 4. The same key arrangement was described in Experiment 1 and employed two keys (Figure 4, first row). Once again, in this arrangement, terminal-link stimuli appeared on the same key on which entry had been gained. The second key arrangement involved three keys, and was called the center arrangement (Figure 4, second row). In this arrangement, terminal links were in effect on the center key rather than on the two side keys. Behavior was examined in cued conditions (Figure 4, left column) and uncued conditions (Figure 4, right column). In the center-cued condition, a green light was in effect on the center key when a terminal link was entered from the left initial-link key, and a red light was in effect Table 4 Experiment 2: Sequence of Conditions and Terminal-Link Schedules for Each Subject Terminal-link schedule after entry from: Sequence for each subject Condition Left key Right key R-222 R-213 R-1580 R Same-cued 10 s 20 s s 10 s Center-cued 10 s 20 s s 10 s Center-uncued 10 s 20 s s 10 s Houselight 10 s 20 s s 10 s 7 7 Note. Birds R-213 and R-1580 were not exposed to the houselight condition in Experiment 2.

14 158 COLTON AND MOORE 10/20 20/10 Center-cued VI 30 VI 30 Center-uncued VI 30 VI 30 10/20 1 0/20 Figure 4. Key arrangements used in Experiment 2. when a terminal link was entered from the right initial-link key. In the center-uncued condition, a circle was in effect on the center key, regardless of the initial link from which the terminal link was entered. These conditions examined the generality of Alsop et al. (1994). Pigeons R-222 and R-187 were also exposed to the center arrangement with nondifferential keylights (i.e., the center-uncued condition) and with differential houselights. In this condition, a circle was in effect on the center key, regardless of which initial link had resulted in terminal-link access. However, if the terminal link was entered from the left key, a green houselight was in effect, and if the terminal link was entered from the right key, a red houselight was in effect. For each of the four combinations of location and terminal-link stimuli, an initial determination of preference was made between 10-s and 20-s schedules. Then, a reversal of these terminal-link schedules was performed, in which the schedule that was formerly 10 s became 20 s, and vice versa. A reversal served to demonstrate experimental control by the conditions in effect. In Figure 4, these initial and reversed assignments of schedules to the terminal links are respectively indicated by two numbers for each : 10 and 20. Again, the birds were exposed to these conditions in a different order (see Table 4), with the exception that

15 EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS 159 reversals were performed immediately after initial determinations of preference. For ease of comparison, all four birds are listed in Table 4. In summary, Experiment 2 attempted to examine the generality of Alsop et al. (1994), and to determine whether preference would develop in the center-key arrangement with nondifferential keylights and differential houselights. Results Figure 5 presents the average asymptotic preferences in terms of the shorter terminal-link schedule ( 10 s) for all four birds. The preferences in each condition are averaged across the last nine stable 1.0 R-222 Cued o Uncued R U ~ 0.0 '- '+- CD CD 1.0 '- CL R-187 R Same Center Houselight Same Center TERMINAL-LINK ARRANGMENT Figure 5. Experiment 2: Average asymptotic preference (choice proportion for 10-s over 20-s terminal-link schedule) in cued and uncued key arrangements for each subject.

16 160 COLTON AND MOORE sessions for initial determinations and reversals. The only exception was the same-cued condition for which preferences in initial, reversed, and final determinations were averaged. The shaded bars represent cued conditions, and the unshaded bars represent uncued conditions. The houselight condition for R-222 and R-187 is represented by a shaded bar. Figure 5 shows that all four birds preferred the 10-s terminal-link schedule in the same-cued condition. Figure 5 also shows that all four subjects preferred the 10-s terminal-link schedule in the center-cued condition. Their preferences here were comparable to those in the same arrangement. However, three of the four birds tended toward indifference in the center-uncued condition, a result similar to that obtained with the random-uncued condition in Experiment 1. The slight preferences that did appear in the center-uncued condition were unreliable in that they never fully reversed for R-213, R-1580, and R-187 (see Table 5). A fourth bird (R-222), however, did exhibit a slight preference for whichever key was correlated with the shorter terminal-link schedule during both initial determinations and reversals. These data are consistent with those of Alsop et al. (1994). Table 5 Experiment 2: Absolute and Relative Response-Rate and Reinforcement Data T-L I-L T-L T-L schedule (s) resp/min resp/min IRI (s) Bird Condition L R L R L R L R P (s) Sessions R-222 Same-cued (.02) (.03) 30 Center-cued (.03) (.01) 19 Center-uncued (.03) (.02) 28 Final Same-cued (.03) 16 Houselight (.03) (.09) 30* R-187 Same-cued (.03) (.01) 15 Center-cued (.04) (.02) 20 Center-uncued (.03) (.03) 23 Final Same-cued (.03) 23 Houselight (.03) (.01) 30* R-1580 Same-cued (.02) (.01) 23 Center-cued (.06) (.02) 20 Center-uncued (.07) 30* (.04) 16 Final Same-cued (.02) 15

17 EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS 161 R-213 Same-cued (.03) (.02) 21 Center-cued (.03) (.02) 15 Center-uncued (.05) (.09) 24 Final Same-cued (.00) 27 Note. Data presented for each pigeon and condition are: IRI = interreinforcement interval; P = asymptotic level of preference; SO = choice proportion standard deviation; where T-L = terminal-link and I-L = initial-link. The choice proportion (P) is expressed in terms of the 10-s terminal link. Choice proportions for the shorter terminal-link schedule ( 10 s) were calculated, e.g., L / (L + R), where Land R represent the number of responses during left and right initial links, respectively. Absolute response rates during the initial links were calculated, e.g., L / total time in initial links. Absolute response rates during the terminal links were calculated, e.g., rate during 10 / (rate during 10 + rate during 20). Interreinforcement intervals were calculated, e.g., total time in left terminal link / number of reinforcements obtained in left terminal link. * Preference failed to stabilize within 30 sessions. Finally, Figure 5 shows that R-222 and R-187 preferred the 10-s schedule in the houselight condition of the center-key arrangement employing nondifferential keylights. The preferences in this condition were just as robust as in the center-cued condition. The outcome of this manipulation has not been reported before. Table 5 presents further data from Experiment 2. Absolute response rates during the initial links were calculated using data from the final nine sessions of each condition. Table 5 presents initial-link absolute response rates across conditions. Higher response rates on the key leading to the 10-s terminal-link schedule occurred in same- and center-cued conditions. However, no such trend occurred in the center-uncued condition. Rather, in this condition, response rates were often higher during the initial link leading to the longer terminal-link schedule ( 20 s). For three of the four birds, initial-link response rates did not reverse when terminal-link conditions were reversed. In the center-uncued condition, only R-222 responded with higher rates during whichever initial link led to the shorter terminal-link schedule. Recall that this bird also slightly preferred whichever key was correlated with the shorter terminal-link schedule in initial and reversed center-uncued conditions. Absolute response rates during the terminal links were also calculated using data from the final nine sessions of each condition. Terminal-link absolute response rates for each condition appear in Table 5. Figure 6 presents relative response rates, expressed in terms of the shorter 10-s terminal link, for each condition. All relative response rate data were averaged over initial determinations and reversals. Relative response rates during the 10-s terminal link were higher in cued conditions (>.50, as shown in Figure 6) than in uncued conditions (approximately.50, as shown in Figure 6). IRis were calculated (in seconds) from the final nine sessions of terminal-link data for each condition. As in Experiment 1, obtained IRis were approximately 10 sand 20 s, corresponding to terminal-link schedules of 10 sand 20 s, respectively.

18 162 COLTON AND MOORE Cued o Uncued C/) ~ c :.:J 1.0 al c: E 0.5 ~ ~ 0) C c ~ 0.0 o..- cts a: C/) c o a. w 1.0 0:: 0.5 > ~ as c:: 0.0 Same R-222 R-213 R-187 R-1580 Center Houselight Same TERMINAL-LINK ARRANGEMENT Center Figure 6. Experiment 2: Relative terminal-link responses/min expressed in terms of the shorter ( 10-s) terminal-link schedule and averaged over initial determinations and reversals. After subjects had completed all conditions, a final same-cued ( 10 vs. 20) assessment reconfirmed clear preferences for the shorter terminal-link schedule as observed previously. Table 5 includes the standard deviations for the choice proportions and the number of sessions required in each condition of Experiment 2. No systematic differences in either of these measures emerged between conditions. Cases in which preferences failed to stabilize within 30 sessions occurred in uncued conditions only and are indicated by asterisks in Table 5.

19 EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS 163 General Discussion The present two experiments examined the effects of terminal-link stimulus arrangements on preference in concurrent chains. In Experiment 1, preferences were low or nonexistent in the randomuncued condition. In contrast, preferences were strong and reliable in the same-cued, same-uncued, opposite-cued, opposite-uncued, and random-cued conditions. Strong and reliable preferences were also observed in the houselight condition. In Experiment 2, preferences were low or nonexistent in the center-uncued condition. In contrast, preferences were strong and reliable in the same-cued and center-cued conditions. Strong and reliable preferences were also observed under the houselight condition. These findings were observed when there was little variation in the obtained terminal-link IRis. Primary reinforcement was reliably obtained after about 10 s in the 10-s terminal link, and after about 20 s in the 20-s terminal link. Taken together, these results confirm the earlier reports of Williams and Fantino (1978), Omino (1993), Omino and Ito (1993), and Alsop et al. (1994). In addition, they extend the findings to procedures involving an opposite arrangement, and to those involving differential houselights. The results indicate that terminal-link stimuli were more than just discriminative about the IRI. If a characteristic stimulus, such as a differential keylight or houselight, was correlated with terminal-link IRI, the birds' initial-link responding came under its control. These characteristic terminal-link stimuli may be regarded as conditioned reinforcers, a view consistent with that of Williams and Fantino (1978). Williams and Fantino (1978) reasoned that when terminal-link schedules were associated with different stimuli (cued conditions), differences in both delayed reinforcement and immediate conditioned reinforcement should influence preference in the initial links. However, when terminal-link schedules were associated with the same stimuli (uncued conditions), differences in immediate conditioned reinforcement should be eliminated and initial-link responding should differ only with respect to overall delay of reinforcement. The findings of the present study are in agreement with this reasoning. In all cued conditions, when the nature and/or location of the terminal-link keylight stimuli were consistent, preferences for the shorter terminal-link schedule were reliably high. In uncued conditions, when terminal-link keylight stimuli were nondifferential in nature but were consistent and differential in their location, (i.e., same- and opposite-uncued conditions), preferences for the shorter terminal-link schedule were also reliably high. However, when terminal-link keylight stimuli were both uncued and nondifferential in location (i.e., center- and random-uncued conditions), preferences tended toward indifference. Thus, the present research supports the majority of studies concerning the effect of terminal-link stimuli on preference and shows that such stimuli may serve as conditioned reinforcers, rather than just discriminative stimuli for terminal-link IRis.

20 164 COLTON AND MOORE A variety of findings in the stimulus control literature also testify to the importance of consistency between stimulus and response. For example, in the area of animal discrimination learning, Bitterman (1966) noted that Yerkes' (1907) original apparatus required the rat merely to run past the discriminative stimulus to reach a goal box containing food. Unfortunately, this "Yerkes box" resulted in very poor discrimination learning. However, a jumping apparatus developed by Lashley (1930) allowed the rat to make contact with the stimuli to be discriminated; the discriminative stimuli were also the locations of the required response. The rat jumped directly at one of two movable stimulus windows in order to access food located on a platform behind the window. This refinement yielded better discrimination learning. Indeed, Bitterman, Tyler, and Elam (1955) showed that rats learn a discrimination (e.g., vertical versus horizontal stripes, large circle versus small circle) with fewer errors in a Lashley jumping stand if the stimuli are located on the stimulus-windows rather than adjacent to the windows. Discrimination learning studies using auditory stimuli also support the influence of stimulus location on stimulus control under certain conditions. For example, Harrison, Iversen, and Pratt (1977) found that stimulus control in monkeys developed better when the auditory stimulus was closer to, rather than separate from, the manipulandum (see also Dobrezeska, Szweijkowska, and Konorski's study using dogs as subjects, cited in Davey, 1986, p. 213). Thus, when a noise burst was presented through the same key on which a response was reinforced, stimulus control was achieved in 1 or 2 sessions. However, 10 sessions were required to achieve stimulus control in two other conditions in which (a) the noise burst was presented through one key and responding on the other key was reinforced and (b) reinforcement was contingent upon responding on a key nearer the sound source. Indeed, the connection between stimulus presentation and response is said to account, in part, for the effect of spatial arrangement of cues, response, and reinforcement on the learning of discriminations (Hearst & Jenkins, 1974). In particular, research on sign-tracking has examined how behavior directed toward (or away from) stimuli as a result of the relation between the stimulus and reinforcement (or the absence of reinforcement) brings an organism into contact with an informative stimulus. Sign-tracking also involves an overt response directed toward that stimulus. In the case of a feature-positive effect, behavior seems to come under the control of a localized stimulus more readily when the appearance or presentation of the stimulus is correlated with reinforcement than when the disappearance or absence of the stimulus is correlated with reinforcement. Like the earlier studies, the present experiments varied the relation between the nature and location of terminal-link stimuli and the location of the preceding initial-link response. Holding the location of visual stimuli constant from initial to terminal links clearly facilitated the development of systematic responding in the present research.

21 EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS 165 In conclusion, the findings of this study indicate that initial-link responding may be influenced by more than just terminal-link primary reinforcement. Other terminal-link variables (Le., the nature and location of terminal-link stimuli) playa role as well. A conditioned-reinforcing value may be conferred upon terminal-link stimuli by various parameters of the link such that initial-link responding is reinforced to a greater or lesser degree by the stimuli (Fantino, 1977). Thus, the present study joins Williams and Fantino (1978), Alsop, Stewart, and Honig (1994), and amino (1993) in showing that conditioned reinforcement contributes significantly to choice in concurrent chains. References ALSOP, B., STEWART, K. E., & HONIG, K. (1994). Cued and uncued terminal links in concurrent-chains schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, AUTOR, S. M. (1960). The strength of conditioned reinforcers as a function of frequency and probability of reinforcement. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University. AUTOR, S. M. (1969). The strength of conditioned reinforcers as a function of frequency and probability of reinforcement. In D. P. Hendry (Ed.), Conditioned reinforcement (pp }. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press. BAUM, W. M. (1973). The correlation-based law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 20, BITTERMAN, M. E. (1966). Animal learning. In J. B. Sidowski (Ed.), Experimental methods and instrumentation in psychology (pp ). New York: McGraw-Hili, Inc. BITTERMAN, M. E., TYLER, D. W., & ELAM, C. B. (1955). Simultaneous and successive discrimination under identical stimulating conditions. American Journal of Psychology, 68, BROWNSTEIN, A. J., & PLiSKOFF, S. S. (1968). Some effects of relative reinforcement rate and changeover delay in response-independent concurrent schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, CHUNG, S., & HERRNSTEIN, R. J. (1967). Choice and delay of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, DAVEY, G. (Ed.). (1966). Animal learning and conditioning (pp ). Baltimore: University Park Press. FANTINO, E. (1977). Conditioned reinforcement: Choice and information. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp ). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. HARRISON, J. M., IVERSEN, S. D., & PRATT, S. R. (1977). Control of responding by location of auditory stimuli: Adjacency of sound and response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 28, HEARST, E., & JENKINS, H. M. (1974). Sign tracking: The stimulus-reinforcer relation and direct action. Austin, TX: Psychonomic Society.

22 166 COLTON AND MOORE HERRNSTEIN, R. J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, HERRNSTEIN, R. J. (1964). Secondary reinforcement and the rate of primary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, LASHLEY, K. S. (1930). The mechanics of vision. Part I. A method for rapid analysis of pattern vision in the rat. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 37, OMINO, T. (1993). A quantitative analysis of sensitivity to the conditioned reinforcing value of terminal-link stimuli in a concurrent-chains schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, OMINO, T., & ITO, M. (1993). Choice and delay of reinforcement: Effects of terminal-link stimuli and response conditions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, SCHNEIDER, J. W. (1972). Choice between two-component chained and tandem schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18, SCHUSTER, R. H. (1969). A functional analysis of conditioned reinforcement. In D. P Hendry (Ed.), Conditioned reinforcement (pp ). Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press. STUBBS, D. A., & PLiSKOFF, S. S. (1969). Concurrent responding with a fixed relative rate of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, WILLIAMS, B. A., & FANTINO, E. (1978). Effects on choice of reinforcement delay and conditioned reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, WILLIAMS, B. A. (1994). Conditioned reinforcement: Experimental and theoretical issues. The Behavior Analyst, 17, YERKES, R. W. (1907). The dancing mouse. New York: MacMillan.

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (4),380-384 Copyright 1978 by The Psychonornic Society, nc. Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information BRUCE A. WALD Utah State University, Logan,

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1987, 47, 57-72 CHOICE BETWEEN RELIABLE AND UNRELIABLE OUTCOMES: MIXED PERCENTAGE-REINFORCEMENT IN CONCURRENT CHAINS MARCIA L. SPETCH AND ROGER DUNN DALHOUSIE

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 12-1991 Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio

More information

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT'

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT' D. ALAN STUBBS AND PHILIP J. SILVERMAN UNIVERSITY OF MAINE, ORONO AND WORCESTER

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL'

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1973, 2, 393-43 NUMBER 3 (NOVEMBER) PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' M. C. DAVISON AND W. TEMPLE UNIVERSITY OF AUCKLAND AND

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

ing the fixed-interval schedule-were observed during the interval of delay. Similarly, Ferster

ing the fixed-interval schedule-were observed during the interval of delay. Similarly, Ferster JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAIOR 1969, 12, 375-383 NUMBER 3 (MAY) DELA YED REINFORCEMENT ERSUS REINFORCEMENT AFTER A FIXED INTERAL' ALLEN J. NEURINGER FOUNDATION FOR RESEARCH ON THE NEROUS

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

CONTROL OF IMPULSIVE CHOICE THROUGH BIASING INSTRUCTIONS. DOUGLAS J. NAVARICK California State University, Fullerton

CONTROL OF IMPULSIVE CHOICE THROUGH BIASING INSTRUCTIONS. DOUGLAS J. NAVARICK California State University, Fullerton The Psychological Record, 2001, 51, 549-560 CONTROL OF IMPULSIVE CHOICE THROUGH BIASING INSTRUCTIONS DOUGLAS J. NAVARICK California State University, Fullerton College students repeatedly chose between

More information

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (3), 264-270 Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pullman, Washington 99163 Five pigeons

More information

Stimulus control of topographically tagged responding

Stimulus control of topographically tagged responding Animal Learning& Behavior 1979, 7 (3),333-338 Stimulus control of topographically tagged responding K. GEOFFREY WHITE and STEVEN B. BRAUNSTEIN Victoria University, Wellington, New Zealand Pigeons' responses

More information

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Illinois Wesleyan University Digital Commons @ IWU Honors Projects Psychology 2000 Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Sara J. Estle '00 Illinois Wesleyan University Recommended Citation

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 89 (2012) 212 218 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes j o ur nal homep age : www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Providing a reinforcement history

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING JOURNL OF THE EXPERMENTL NLYSS OF BEHVOR 1979, 32, 121-127 NUMBER (JULY) CQUSTON OF STMULUS CONTROL WHLE NTRODUCNG NEW STMUL N FDNG LNNY FELDS THE COLLEGE OF STTEN SLND fter establishing a discrimination

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (1986) 38B, 81-95 Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon P. J. Durlach and N. J. Mackintosh Department of Experimental Psychology, University

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2005 April ; 31(2): 213 225. Timing in Choice Experiments Jeremie Jozefowiez and Daniel T. Cerutti

More information

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Summer 2005 Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli William

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER UNIVERSITY OF IOWA 1969, 12, 451-461 NUMBER

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules

Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules Animal Learning & Behavior 1977, 5 (1),11-114 Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pul/man, Washington

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

of behavioral contrast. To the extent that an account of contrast is a major concern, therefore, some alternative formulation seems desirable.

of behavioral contrast. To the extent that an account of contrast is a major concern, therefore, some alternative formulation seems desirable. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AN EQUATION FOR BEHAVIORAL CONTRAST BEN A. WILLIAMS AND JOHN T. WIXTED UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SAN DIEGO 1986, 45, 47-62 NUMBER 1 (JANUARY) Pigeons were

More information

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 74-84 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.74 Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure PATRICK VAN ROOYEN AND ANGELO

More information

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, 215 231 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS

More information

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Behavioural Processes 66 (2004) 213 233 Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Raymond C. Pitts, Stacy

More information

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, 152 162 2329-8456/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000053 Memory Systems

More information

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Learning and Motivation 39 (2008) 71 94 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Luís Oliveira,

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 11, 79-91 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2010) 79 Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats Rogelio Escobar National Autonomous University of Mexico

More information

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION By JIN HO YOON A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY

More information

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A.

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December

More information

Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task

Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task Analysis of Gambling Behavior Volume 11 Article 2 2017 Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task Nathaniel C. Rice 1. United States Army Medical Research Institute of Chemical Defense,

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Transfer Across Delayed Discriminations: II. Differences in the Substitutability of Initial Versus Test Stimuli

Transfer Across Delayed Discriminations: II. Differences in the Substitutability of Initial Versus Test Stimuli Joulmal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/98/$3.00 1998, VoL 24, No. 1, 47-59 Transfer Across Delayed Discriminations:

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE By ANTHONY L. DEFULIO A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez The Psychological Record, 28, 58, 57 528 Some Effects of Magnitude of Reinforcement on Persistence of Responding Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman The University of Minnesota Angel Jimenez The University

More information

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, & JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR1 1982, 38, 71-85 NUMBER I (JULY) MEMORY FOR RECENT BEHAVIOR IN THE PIGEON STEPHEN P. KRAMER CORRECTIONS DIVISION, DRAPER, UTAH Variations of the symbolic

More information

Concurrent Chains Schedules as a Method to Study Choice Between Alcohol Associated Conditioned Reinforcers

Concurrent Chains Schedules as a Method to Study Choice Between Alcohol Associated Conditioned Reinforcers Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 1-2012 Concurrent Chains Schedules as a Method to Study Choice Between Alcohol Associated Conditioned Reinforcers Corina

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 261-268 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) CONCURRENT SCHEULES OF PRIMARY AN CONITIONE REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' ONAL W. ZIMMERMAN CARLETON UNIVERSITY Rats responded

More information

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2009, Vol. 35, No. 3, 293 311 2009 American Psychological Association 0097-7403/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013926 Resistance to Change Within

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 348 370 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Chris Hope and Angelo Santi Wilfrid

More information

Variation in Herrnsein s r<sub>0</sub> as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate

Variation in Herrnsein s r<sub>0</sub> as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate Illinois Wesleyan University From the SelectedWorks of James Dougan 1985 Variation in Herrnsein s r0 as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate James Dougan, Illinois Wesleyan University

More information

Instrumental Conditioning I

Instrumental Conditioning I Instrumental Conditioning I Basic Procedures and Processes Instrumental or Operant Conditioning? These terms both refer to learned changes in behavior that occur as a result of the consequences of the

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 8-2012 Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Meredith

More information

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research TWO-KEY CONCURRENT RESPONDING: CHOICE AND DELAYS OF REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS Presented to The Faculty of the Division of Graduate Studies and Research By George

More information

EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT. Lee Davis Thomas

EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT. Lee Davis Thomas EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT Lee Davis Thomas A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington for Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

Variability as an Operant?

Variability as an Operant? The Behavior Analyst 2012, 35, 243 248 No. 2 (Fall) Variability as an Operant? Per Holth Oslo and Akershus University College Correspondence concerning this commentary should be addressed to Per Holth,

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(4),621-629 Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives THOMAS R. ZENTALL University ofkentucky, Lexington,

More information

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 93 102 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND

More information

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 217, 17, 123 135 NUMBER 1 (JANUARY) SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 1

More information

The influence ofbrief stimuli uncorrelated with reinforcement on choice between variable-ratio schedules

The influence ofbrief stimuli uncorrelated with reinforcement on choice between variable-ratio schedules Animal Learning & Behavior /993. 2/ (2). /59-/67 The influence ofbrief stimuli uncorrelated with reinforcement on choice between variable-ratio schedules PHIL REED and VAL SZZUDLO University ollege London

More information

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 122 2001 Participating Faculty Professor James Dickson (dickson@stolaf.edu) Professor Dana Gross (grossd@stolaf.edu)

More information

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2015, 103, 269 287 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG

More information

Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects

Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects Journal of Eye Movement Research 2(5):3, 1-6 Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects Candido V. B. B. Pessôa Edson M. Huziwara Peter

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior.

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR TECHNIQUES FOR ESTABLISHING SCHEDULES WITH WHEEL RUNNING AS REINFORCEMENT IN RATS IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA 1993, 60, 219-238 NUMBER 1

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS The Psychological Record, 2000, 50, 429-442 LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS LUIS A. PEREZ-GONZALEZ, JOSEPH E. SPRADLIN, and KATHRYN J. SAUNDERS University of Oviedo, Spain

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules

Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules Animal Learning & Behavior 1979, 7 (2), 203-210 Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules NANCY K. INNIS University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada

More information

Spatially Diffuse Inhibition Affects Multiple Locations: A Reply to Tipper, Weaver, and Watson (1996)

Spatially Diffuse Inhibition Affects Multiple Locations: A Reply to Tipper, Weaver, and Watson (1996) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1996, Vol. 22, No. 5, 1294-1298 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/%/$3.00 Spatially Diffuse Inhibition

More information

Backward associations: Differential learning about stimuli that follow the presence versus the absence of food in pigeons

Backward associations: Differential learning about stimuli that follow the presence versus the absence of food in pigeons Antmal Learntng & Behavtor 1989, 17 (3), 280-290 Backward associations: Differential learning about stimuli that follow the presence versus the absence of food in pigeons ELIOT HEARST Indiana University,

More information

Schedules of Reinforcement

Schedules of Reinforcement Schedules of Reinforcement MACE, PRATT, ZANGRILLO & STEEGE (2011) FISHER, PIAZZA & ROANE CH 4 Rules that describe how will be reinforced are 1. Every response gets SR+ ( ) vs where each response gets 0

More information

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) AMERICAN UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 009, 9, 367 377 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) WITHIN-SUBJECT REVERSIBILITY OF DISCRIMINATIVE FUNCTION IN THE COMPOSITE-STIMULUS CONTROL OF BEHAVIOR STANLEY J. WEISS,

More information

Further Tests of Response-Outcome Associations in Differential-Outcome Matching-to-Sample

Further Tests of Response-Outcome Associations in Differential-Outcome Matching-to-Sample Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1«7, Vol. 23, No. 2, 171-182 Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7*>3/97/J3.00 Further Tests of Response-Outcome

More information

edited by Derek P. Hendry'

edited by Derek P. Hendry' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR INFORMATION ON CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT A review of Conditioned Reinforcement, edited by Derek P. Hendry' LEWIS R. GOLLUB2 UNIVERSITY OF MARYLAND 1970,

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT AND RESPONSE STRENGTH TIMOTHY A. SHAHAN

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT AND RESPONSE STRENGTH TIMOTHY A. SHAHAN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2010, 93, 269 289 NUMBER 2(MARCH) CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT AND RESPONSE STRENGTH TIMOTHY A. SHAHAN UTAH STATE UNIVERSITY Stimuli associated with primary

More information

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 5, NUMBF- 4 OCITOBER, 1 962 THE EFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES OF PRIMARY AND CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT JOAN G. STEVENSON1 AND T. W. REESE MOUNT HOLYOKE

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Learn Behav. 2009 November ; 37(4): 357 364. doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.357. Behavioral Momentum and Relapse of Extinguished Operant Responding

More information

ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND

ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2008, 89, 157 167 NUMBER 2(MARCH) ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND IDAHO STATE UNIVERSITY

More information

NEW YORK UNIVERSITY. as in matching-to-sample or oddity performances, the rate of other, concurrently reinforced responses,

NEW YORK UNIVERSITY. as in matching-to-sample or oddity performances, the rate of other, concurrently reinforced responses, JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR CONCURRENT PERFORMANCES: RATE AND ACCURACY OF FREE-OPERANT ODDITY RESPONDING1 A. CHARLES CATANIA AND RICARDO DOBSON2 NEW YORK UNIVERSITY 1972, 17, 25-35

More information

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a Effects of Food Deprivation on Memory Performance. Jacob. L. Kerewaro Abstract The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)

More information