CONTEXTUAL INFLUENCES ON VISUAL PROCESSING

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "CONTEXTUAL INFLUENCES ON VISUAL PROCESSING"

Transcription

1 Annu. Rev. Neurosci : doi: /annurev.neuro Copyright c 2002 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved CONTEXTUAL INFLUENCES ON VISUAL PROCESSING Thomas D. Albright and Gene R. Stoner Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Systems Neurobiology Laboratories, The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, California 92037; tom@salk.edu; gene@salk.edu Key Words occlusion, depth-ordering, figure-ground interpretation, filling-in, visual cortex Abstract The visual image formed on the retina represents an amalgam of visual scene properties, including the reflectances of surfaces, their relative positions, and the type of illumination. The challenge facing the visual system is to extract the meaning of the image by decomposing it into its environmental causes. For each local region of the image, that extraction of meaning is only possible if information from other regions is taken into account. Of particular importance is a set of image cues revealing surface occlusion and/or lighting conditions. These informationrich cues direct the perceptual interpretation of other more ambiguous image regions. This context-dependent transformation from image to perception has profound but frequently under-appreciated implications for neurophysiological studies of visual processing: To demonstrate that neuronal responses are correlated with perception of visual scene properties, rather than visual image features, neuronal sensitivity must be assessed in varied contexts that differentially influence perceptual interpretation. We review a number of recent studies that have used this context-based approach to explore the neuronal bases of visual scene perception. INTRODUCTION The Oxford English Dictionary defines context as the parts which immediately precede or follow any particular passage or text and determine its meaning. More generally, context is the whole situation, background, or environment relevant to a particular event, etc., which reveals its meaning. These definitions, of course, beg the question of meaning, to which there are no simple or all-encompassing answers. For our purposes, the definition of meaning is implicit in the central problems of vision, which are identifying the environmental causes of the patterns of light falling on the retinae, and the behavioral significance of those causes for the observer. Accordingly, the meaning of a contour of light on the retina includes, among other things, the particular object in the scene that reflected that pattern and the information that the object conveys about the world. Context, in turn, includes sensory cues that enable the image feature to be assigned to an object, X/02/ $

2 340 ALBRIGHT STONER Figure 1 Illustration of the influence of local sensory context on visual perception. Each image contains a horizontal dark-gray rectangle. Although the rectangles are physically identical, the surrounding features differ in the three images. As a result, the rectangle is perceptually attributed to different environmental causes in the three instances, i.e., it conveys different meanings. (A) The rectangle appears to result from overlap of two surfaces, one of which is transparent (e.g., a piece of tinted glass). (B) The rectangle appears to result from a variation in surface reflectance (e.g., a stripe painted across a flat canvas). (C) The rectangle appears to result from variation in the angle of the surface with respect to the illumination source. These markedly different perceptual interpretations argue for the existence of different neuronal representations. the observer s history with the object, his motivational state, and the reward value of the object to the observer. As a practical matter, extraction of meaning can be identified with perception, and context is the sensory/behavioral/cognitive milieu that influences the way each sensory feature is perceived. Contextual influences on perception are necessarily manifested as interactions, rather than passive associations, between sensory, behavioral, or cognitive elements. For example, the percepts elicited by the images in Figures 1A C are not merely of three different sets of features. On the contrary, the meaning of the dark gray bar is changed dramatically through its interactions with surrounding features. The perceptual whole is more than the sum of the sensory parts. With these definitions as a foundation, we begin our review with a brief historical account of views on the role of context in vision. This history originates with philosophical doctrine and empirical psychology and is offered with the belief that past controversies, misunderstandings, and insights on this topic can guide and inform modern-day visual neuroscience. Indeed, the growing bond between psychology and neuroscience has led to remarkable new discoveries regarding the role of context in the neuronal bases of perception, as well as paradigmatic changes in the way neuronal representations are viewed and approached experimentally. These changes, and the data that support them, are the primary focus of our review. HISTORICAL CONTEXT The Reductionist Psychology of Perception Discussions leading to our present-day understanding of the role of context in perception can be traced to the concept of associationism in the seventeenth-century

3 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 341 philosophy of John Locke (1690). The crux of Locke s view was that perception results from a passive association of ideas, of which immediate sensation is the primary source. Associationism remained a dominant theme in philosophy well into the nineteenth century. Its early proponents adopted the metaphor of mechanical compounding to characterize their conclusion that sensations are linked noninteractively to render the objects of perceptual experience. In the mid nineteenth century, associationism was embraced by many of the great figures in the emerging field of experimental psychology. Wilhelm Wundt (1902), in particular, pressed further the deconstruction of psychological reality by advancing elementism. Whereas the associationists believed percepts to be the product of linked sensations, elementism held the inverse to be true: Any percept may be reduced to independent internal states (sensations) elicited by individual elements of the proximal stimulus (such as luminance or chrominance). Wundt s arguments were highly influential and fitted squarely with the emerging physiology of his time, specifically Johannes Müller s (1838) law of specific nerve energies and the growing belief that different sensory pathways convey specific and distinct perhaps elemental types of information. Despite the prevalence of the Wundtian view, there were dissenting voices. William James (1890) was an associationist to be sure, but he promoted the position that perception is an emergent product of interactions between associated sensations. The Austrian physicist Ernst Mach held similar beliefs. In Contributions to the Analysis of Sensations (1886), Mach discussed context explicitly and provided striking demonstrations of its role in visual perception, which anticipated the Gestalt movement in experimental psychology. Gestalt Psychology Flawed by its failure to account for emergent properties of perception, elementism fell from favor and Gestalt psychology rose in its place. Max Wertheimer [1961 (1912)] founded the Gestalt movement and rejected elementism following his studies of phi motion, now commonly known as apparent motion. This phenomenon is the illusory percept of smooth or continuous motion that results when a stimulus (a spot of light, for example) is displaced by discrete intervals over space and time. (This is, of course, the basis for motion picture photography.) Elementism argues that this motion percept should be reducible to the elemental sensations, which consist of static points of light occurring at different points in space and time. This reduction is impossible, Wertheimer observed; perceived motion under these conditions is thus an emergent property that comes about through the relations between discrete sensations again, the whole is more than the sum of the parts. The Gestaltists offered a set of laws to characterize the sensory interactions underlying perceptual organization. Despite the empirical validity of these laws and the many compelling demonstrations of contextual influences on visual processing, the Gestalt school did not exert a pervasive influence on the developing field of neurobiology in the latter half of the twentieth century. There are several

4 342 ALBRIGHT STONER reasons for this, including the heavy emphasis placed by the Gestaltists on phenomenology and their attendant failure to build firm neurobiological and mechanistic foundations for their beliefs. As a consequence, the Gestalt doctrine was rapidly eclipsed by a new reductionism, the neurophysiology of vision. Neurophysiology of Vision: A Neurobiological Elementism? With the development of techniques for recording neuronal action potentials and the subsequent application of these techniques to the study of the mammalian visual system, the field of visual science underwent a paradigm shift. Fundamental to the conduct and interpretation of neurophysiological experiments has been the classical concept of the receptive field (RF) as a discrete, spatially restricted, representational unit (Hartline & Graham 1932). Implicit in this concept is an assumption of functional independence: Once the information represented by individual RFs is known, it should be possible to deduce how neurons collectively represent the objects of perceptual experience. This assumption of independence, in turn, has fostered a localizationist view, in which different sensory elements are thought to be represented in different brain regions that are mechanistically and functionally independent of one another (e.g., Livingstone & Hubel 1987). Conjunctively and in spite of the many merits of the neurophysiological approach these premises are a throwback to the nineteenth-century views of the associationists and the elementists. An association of sensory attributes represented by functionally independent neurons is reminiscent of mechanical compounding, and the notion that different brain regions represent different attributes smells of Wundtian elementism. Neurophysiological approaches shackled by these premises cannot reveal in toto the neural substrates of perceptual experience for the same reasons that psychological elementism cannot explain the contextual phenomena highlighted by the Gestaltists. As a simple illustration, consider an orientation-selective neuron in primary visual cortex (Hubel & Wiesel 1968). The RF of the neuron illustrated in Figure 2 was characterized without contextual manipulations, and the data clearly reveal how the neuron represents the proximal (retinal) stimulus elements of orientation and direction of motion. From such data, however, it is frankly impossible to know whether this neuron would respond differentially to locally identical image features viewed in different contexts, such as the dark gray bars in Figures 1A C. In other words, the full meaning of the pattern of responses in Figure 2 is unclear, as that pattern is not sufficient to reveal what the neuron conveys about the visual scene. The Emergence of a Neurobiology of Perception The ready ability to record neuronal activity in alert nonhuman primates has allowed one of the most notable achievements of modern neuroscience: the establishment of direct links between the behavior of single neurons and that of the whole organism (e.g., Mountcastle et al. 1972, Newsome et al. 1989; see also Barlow 1972).

5 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 343 Figure 2 Neuronal directional selectivity, as first observed by Hubel & Wiesel (1968) in primary visual cortex (area V1) of rhesus monkey. The neuronal receptive field is indicated by broken rectangles in the left column. The visual stimulus was moved in each of 14 directions (rows A G, opposing directions indicated by arrows) through receptive field. Recorded traces of cellular activity are shown at the right, in which the horizontal axis represents time (2 s/trace) and each vertical line represents an action potential. This neuron responds most strongly to motion up and to the right (row D). (From Hubel & Wiesel Permission from The Physiological Society.)

6 344 ALBRIGHT STONER As impressive as such links are, their relevance to the extraction of meaning from visual images depends upon the meaningfulness of the stimuli being perceived. Unless neurophysiologists use stimuli embodying the semantics of natural images, they will advance with or without the inclusion of behavioral links only a few fledgling steps toward an understanding of the neuronal bases of perception. To illustrate this assertion, imagine attempting to understand the function of language areas of the human brain armed only with nonlanguage stimuli such as frequency-modulated audible noise. Imagine further that, using these stimuli, you make the remarkable discovery that the responses of neurons within Wernicke s area (for example) are correlated with behavioral judgments (of, for instance, whether the frequency modulation was high-to-low or low-to-high). Although this finding would offer an interesting (and perhaps satisfyingly parametric) link between single neurons and perceptual decisions, it seems clear that stimuli constructed of words and sentences would yield results more likely to illuminate language processing. Just as we can only progress so far using nonsense sounds to explore language function, so are we constrained by visual stimuli lacking the rich cue interdependencies that permit the perceptual interpretation of natural scenes. In recognition of this limitation, some have advocated an approach in which neuronal activity is recorded while animals are viewing natural scenes (Gallant et al. 1998, Stanley et al. 1999, Vinje & Gallant 2000). Clearly the sensory features of such scenes are replete with contextual cues, but disentangling the roles of specific cues from among the many that exert parallel influences on neurons and perception is a formidable challenge. Much promise lies in a simpler and more controlled method inspired by the well-documented effects of local context on visual perception such as the influence of one image region on the surface color appearance of an adjacent region (e.g., Shevell 1978, von Helmholtz 2000 [1860/1924], Wachtler et al. 2001) or the influence of depth cues on surface brightness (e.g., Figure 1) (e.g., Gilchrist 1977, Adelson 1993, Purves et al. 1999) and perceived direction of motion (Shimojo et al. 1989, Duncan et al. 2000). By systematically varying one contextual cue at a time, neuronal-perceptual links can be established that reveal how the visual system uses these cues to recover the properties of more complex natural scenes. Using this controlled contextual approach, several recent studies have obtained evidence for parallel influences on neuronal and perceptual sensitivity. Before considering these studies in detail, we note a development that has profoundly impacted the evolution of a context-based approach: the revamping of the concept of the RF. Influences from Beyond the Classical Receptive Field Interest in neuronal effects of context became focused with the discovery of modulatory influences from beyond the classical RF. There were several early indications of such effects (e.g., Barlow 1953, McIlwain 1964, Hubel & Wiesel 1965, Nelson & Frost 1978), suggesting that the classical RF concept was flawed and perhaps limited efforts to understand neural substrates of perception. By the mid- 1980s, however, the accumulation of evidence for response modulation via the

7 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 345 nonclassical RF, or RF surround, reached a critical level, and the topic found a broad audience (Allman et al. 1985). Perhaps the most well-documented nonclassical RF effect is the modulation, by motion in the surround, of neuronal responses in the middle temporal visual area (area MT) of primate cerebral cortex (Figure 3). MT neurons are well known to exhibit a high degree of selectivity for the direction of motion of a stimulus within the classical RF (e.g., Albright 1984). The presence of motion outside the classical RF is, by definition, incapable of eliciting a response. Nonclassical RF motion leads, however, to marked modulation of the response to a stimulus within the classical RF (Figure 4) (Allman et al. 1985, Tanaka et al. 1986, Xiao et al. 1997). Similar surround effects have been reported for other forms of feature contrast, including line orientation (e.g., Gilbert & Wiesel 1990; Knierim & van Essen 1992; Kapadia et al. 2000; Li et al. 2000, 2001) and binocular disparity (Bradley & Andersen 1998). Figure 4 Responses to stimulus motion in the classical receptive field (RF) of a middle temporal (MT) neuron are modulated by motion in the RF surround. (Left) Direction tuning for a moving random dot pattern confined to the classical RF. (Right) Modulation of the response to classical RF motion in the preferred direction as a function of the direction of surround motion. When center and surround motion were in the same direction (0 ), the response to classical RF motion was suppressed. When center and surround moved in opposite directions, the response to classical RF motion was facilitated. (From Allman et al Permission from the Annual Review of Neuroscience, Volume 8, c 1985 by Annual Reviews.)

8 346 ALBRIGHT STONER Although these findings suggested a neuronal substrate through which perceptual context effects might be exerted, there exists an important difference between the definition of context as it has been applied to neuronal RF surround effects and the perceptual definition we have adopted. The neuronal definition is based strictly upon an interesting spatial interaction in the RF, i.e., contextual information from the surround modulates the neuronal response to the classical RF stimulus. By contrast, the perceptual definition of context is based on the ability of contextual information to disambiguate the environmental origins of a sensory stimulus. As we show below, there are phenomena that seem to satisfy both definitions, suggesting that surround modulation may indeed underlie specific types of perceptual disambiguation. However, surround modulation is surely not the only means by which perceptual context effects are implemented neuronally. For example, because surround modulation takes on a very specific spatial form (by definition), it is difficult to imagine how it might account for perceptual effects that occur when contextual information is spatially comingled with stimulus features that are disambiguated by that context [e.g., the ability of a feature s color to disambiguate, both perceptually and neuronally, the motion of that feature (Croner & Albright 1999a; for review see Croner & Albright 1999b). ON THE VARIETIES OF CONTEXTUAL INFLUENCES Broadly defined as sources of information used to identify the meaning of a sensory stimulus the scope of contextual influences on visual processing is vast and may include stimuli present at other points in space or time, as well as effects of attention, memory, and self-movement. For practical reasons, and in an effort to present our arguments in a coherent framework, we have chosen to concentrate on spatial contextual interactions in which information in one region of the visual image influences the interpretation of another region. Except where noted otherwise, the perceptual and neuronal phenomena we have reviewed are based on observations made in primates (humans and monkeys). There is also a common functional theme fundamental to the context-based processes we consider: recovery of the multiple visual scene attributes associated with each single point in the visual image. The optical projection of a threedimensional world onto a two-dimensional retina reduces the information at each spatial location to a single image value, which characterizes the light emanating from that location. Thus, although each value is a direct product of specific surface and lighting conditions in the visual scene, it is only possible to identify those conditions by taking into account the context in which the image value occurs. That context, of course, consists of other values in the image. But how is this context used to infer visual scene content? What are its critical attributes, and how do those attributes reveal surface and lighting relationships?

9 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 347 One way to approach these questions is to rephrase the problem as that of recovering missing information. Indeed, there exists a variety of circumstances, mostly intrinsic to the visual scene, but some intrinsic to the visual system itself, that cause attributes of the visual scene to be literally, fully or partially, missing from view. As an extreme example but one of great functional significance consider what happens when one opaque surface partially blocks the observer s view of another surface. In such cases a single image value arising from the occluder reflects only the properties of that surface. Contextual cues from surrounding spatial regions, however, lead to perceptual decomposition or scission of the image value into visible and implied surfaces and to perceptual interpolation ( filling-in or completion ) of the missing attributes of the occluded surface. A similar recovery of missing information occurs when the foreground surface is not opaque but transparent. Under these conditions each image value from the foreground surface is a physical composite of the separate reflectance and transmission properties of the overlapping surfaces, i.e., the actual properties of those surfaces are missing in the mix. Once again, surrounding contextual cues promote a scission of each image value into perceptually distinct properties of the transparent foreground and background surfaces (see Figure 1A). A precisely analogous recovery process is engaged more generally and ubiquitously owing to the fact that each image value is a product of surface reflectance and illumination. Here also contextual cues promote scission: Just as context enables filling-in of surface properties missing behind an opaque or transparent occluder, context enables the viewer to see behind the illuminant to recover the surface reflectance properties of objects (see Figure 1C). There are several computational steps that are broadly associated with these (and other related) context-mediated recovery processes. These include the interpretation of surface depth-ordering, boundary assignment, and filling-in. How these processes operate, what specific contextual cues are utilized, and what neuronal substrates and mechanisms are employed are the primary topics addressed in the research summarized below. Although the visual scene information supplied by every visual image value is incomplete, this incompleteness, and hence the need for contextual resolution, is nowhere greater than when information about a part of the visual scene is absent altogether. We turn first to a discussion of how these informational gaps are filled-in using contextual cues. FILLING-IN I: USING CONTEXT TO PREDICT MISSING INFORMATION Filling-In the Blind Spot Large enough to conceal 76 full moons (Campbell & Andrews 1992), the blind spot went unnoticed until the seventeenth century (Mariotte 1668). The blind spot is a roughly elliptical ( 5 wide by 8 high) hole in the photoreceptor mosaic

10 348 ALBRIGHT STONER through which information exits (via the optic nerve) and nourishment enters (via blood vessels). The trunks of blood vessels arborize atop each retina, casting shadows and creating angioscotomas (Le Gros 1967). The blind spot and angioscotoma form a large, continuous, irregularly shaped interruption in the central visual field of each eye (sparing the fovea), which, in collaboration with other minor scotomas and those caused by neuronal pathologies, conspire to prevent us from seeing all that lies within our visual field. That the blind spot and scotomas go unnoticed is testament to the importance of filling-in of these visual holes (Ramachandran & Gregory 1991). A question of great importance to our understanding of neuronal representation is whether filling-in is an active process in which spatial context explicitly provides the missing information, or simply ignorance of the fact that anything is absent (e.g., von Helmholtz 2000 [1860/1924], Dennet 1991, Churchland & Ramachandran 1993, Ramachandran 1994, De Weerd et al. 1995). About the blind spot, Helmholtz concluded (contradicting many of his contemporaries), Nothing bright or coloured or dark is to be seen in the gap in the visual field. What we see there is literally nothing a nothing that is not even a hole... (von Helmholtz 2000 [1860/1924], 3:208). Equally emphatically, he asserts...and especially [our italics]...nosensations whatever are transferred from the surrounding neighborhood to the gap. Helmholtz s denial of a role for context in filling-in has not prevailed. On the contrary, in recent years the field has witnessed several intriguing perceptual demonstrations of phenomenal filling-in of the blind spot with information from adjacent regions of the visual field (e.g., Ramachandran & Gregory 1991, Ramachandran 1992), as well as quantitative psychophysical support for an active process (Paradiso & Hahn 1996). For example, He & Davis (2001) showed that filled-in content from the blind spot locus of one eye could suppress real image content from the corresponding visual field locus of the other eye. These investigators reasoned that such binocular rivalry favors an active filling-in mechanism because ignored (i.e., nonactively processed) information should not be able to compete with actively processed information. The most direct evidence for active context-based filling-in comes, however, from neurophysiological studies of the blind spot representation. The blind spots of the two retinae do not correspond to the same parts of visual space, and hence the blind spot representation in primary visual cortex (V1) (see Figure 3) is a large [about 5 10 mm in humans (Tong & Engel 2001)] monocular island within a larger sea of neurons fed by both eyes. Fiorani et al. (1992) found that a bar swept across the blind spot of anesthetized Cebus monkeys (monocular viewing) elicited a response from neurons in these blind spot zones. Neurons with this completion property required stimulation of opposite sides of the blind spot to be activated. Similarly, Komatsu et al. (2000) found that uniform rectangles, with edges well outside the blind spot, elicited responses in the blind spot representation in alert rhesus monkeys. Together these studies indicate that perceptual filling-in of the blind spot results from an active neuronal process fed by informational content present in surrounding regions of the visual

11 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 349 field. Furthermore, filling-in occurs for both oriented features (such as surface boundaries) and homogenous image regions (such as surface interiors). From a functional perspective, this type of process is scarcely surprising, as local spatial context offers the best available clues to the missing content from the visual scene, and thus provides a meaningful resolution to sensory gaps. Filling-In of Acquired or Induced Visual Gaps Monocular retinal lesions, like the blind spot, rob information from a localized region of one eye. Whereas the visual system has had plenty of time to evolve adaptations to the visual gap created by the optic disk, the same is not true of pathological scotomas. Evidence nonetheless suggests that these visual holes are filled-in in humans (Gerrits & Timmerman 1969) and in monkeys with experimentally induced retinal lesions (Murakami et al. 1997). Gerrits et al. (1966) asked whether filling-in extended to artificial scotomas, created by temporarily removing localized information from the retina. These artificial scotomas were formed from a homogenous black patch that was positioned over a restricted region of the retina and surrounded by light. When eye position was fixed relative to the patch, the patch faded over the course of a few seconds and was replaced by a percept of the surrounding light. In subsequent experiments in which the patches were surrounded by dynamic colored texture, Ramachandran & Gregory (1991) found that the color and dynamic properties of the contextual texture filled-in, but color did so faster, implicating the involvement of different mechanisms and perhaps different cortical areas (De Weerd et al. 1998). To explore the neuronal bases of these filling-in effects, De Weerd et al. (1995) positioned similarly constructed artificial scotomas over the classical receptive fields of neurons in cortical areas V1, V2, and V3 (see Figure 3) of alert rhesus monkeys. Following stimulus onset, neuronal activity in areas V2 and V3 (but not V1) gradually increased as if responding to the missing texture. The time course of this climbing activity correlated well with perceptual filling-in, suggesting that these neuronal events in V2 and V3 may underlie the perceptual effect. In a study bearing on mechanism, Pettet & Gilbert (1992) found that, when presented with artificial scotomas, V1 RFs increased in size up to fivefold. Neurons, deprived of information within their classical RF, extend their sensitivity to surrounding regions of visual space. This enlarged field could serve to fill-in the scotoma with information from the surrounding spatial field, just as sensitivity to regions surrounding the blind spot permits neurons in the blind spot representation to fill-in that visual hole. Similar changes in RF size have been seen minutes after focal retinal lesions (Chino et al. 1992, Gilbert & Wiesel 1992). Although longerterm plasticity had been documented in adult animals after restricted deafferentation (e.g., Kaas 1991, Garraghty & Kaas 1992), the results of Pettet & Gilbert s study suggest that perceptual filling-in reflects a form of neuronal plasticity in which visual input alters RF structure and cortical topography from moment to moment. This remarkable suggestion challenges the long-standing view that a neuron s visual RF is a stable entity within the healthy adult.

12 350 ALBRIGHT STONER It should be pointed out that the time scale for perceptual filling-in of artificial scotomas (De Weerd et al. 1998) is not as fast as the seemingly instantaneous perceptual filling-in of the blind spot, nor is it as fast as measured for other types of filling-in (Paradiso & Hahn 1996, Paradiso & Nakayama 1991). This suggests that different neuronal mechanisms are involved in these different types of fillingin (De Weerd et al. 1998). Furthermore, the neuronal effects observed by Pettet & Gilbert (1992) occurred over a period of several minutes, not seconds, and hence the exact relationship between their observed RF changes and perceptual filling-in remains to be determined. Taken together, the different time courses observed for different types of neuronal and perceptual filling-in suggest the involvement of multiple mechanisms. FILLING-IN II: SURFACE OCCLUSION Intrinsic, pathological, and artificial scotomas of the sort discussed above are all characterized by loss of visual information from a fixed retinal locus i.e., the gap moves with the eye or is present only when the eye is in a fixed position. There are, however, other circumstances of transient visual information loss that are caused by surface occlusion in the visual scene. These circumstances are, of course, endemic to normal visual experience, and the resolution of information loss is made possible by context in a manner that is functionally and perhaps mechanistically similar to the resolution of loss from scotomas. Although we provide evidence for common principles, occlusion-based informational gaps differ importantly from those caused by defects intrinsic to the visual system. Occluders elicit a sense of depth-ordered surfaces in the visual scene. Gaps caused by the optic disk and retinal vasculature do not. Accordingly, an observer cannot usually perceive his/her own optic disk or retinal vasculature but generally will see occluding surfaces. It is useful to distinguish two context-based processes in the resolution of information gaps caused by surface occlusion. The first involves establishing which image regions constitute figure and ground (occluder and occluded) using a set of characteristic image cues. The second process, which is intimately tied to the first, involves filling-in or perceptual completion of occluded content from the visual scene and depends upon local contextual cues that offer evidence of that missing content. Surface Depth-Ordering, Figure-Ground Interpretation, and Border Ownership Figure-ground interpretation is coupled to the establishment of the three-dimensional spatial arrangement of surfaces; image regions in the foreground depth plane are generally perceived as figure. There are, of course, well-known sources of depth information (as a scalar quantity) present in the visual image, such as binocular disparity and motion parallax, which serve this function. In addition, there are

13 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 351 image consequences that are specific to surface occlusion, which supply reliable contextual cues for surface depth-ordering (an ordinal quantity). One of the most common of such cues is due to the horizontal displacement of the two eyes, which enables one eye to see part(s) of the visual scene that a foreground surface occludes from the other eye. Human observers are particularly sensitive to this monocular occlusion cue, which is known as Da Vinci stereopsis (Anderson 1994, Nakayama & Shimojo 1990). Occlusion produces several other types of depth-ordering cues that emerge from the distinctive characteristics of overlapping surfaces. One important cue consists of spatially defined X- and T-junctions, which are stereotyped contour arrangements in visual images that generally occur where edges of two surfaces overlap (Kanizsa 1979, Beck et al. 1984). A second cue consists of temporally defined accretion-deletion, which results from the progressive uncovering and/or covering of a background surface by a foreground surface (Kaplan 1969, Stoner & Albright 1995). A third cue consists of the coincident alignment of unconnected line endings or edges, which is indicative of occlusion by a common surface (Kanizsa 1979). The presence of any of these contextual cues generally biases perceived depth-ordering and figure-ground interpretation in human observers. The Gestalt psychologists emphasized a number of additional image cues that lead to figure-ground interpretation but do not offer explicit evidence of occlusion. For example, regions within a closed boundary usually appear as figure (Koffka 1935). Furthermore, if the boundary resembles the profile of a known object, the image region corresponding to the implied object will tend to be seen as figure (Rubin 1921, Peterson 2002). In some of the earliest neurophysiological studies of this topic, Lamme (1995) and Zipser et al. (1996) searched for correlates of figure-ground interpretation in area V1. Stimuli consisting of texture-defined rectangles (perceptual figures defined by boundary closure) were positioned such that individual texture elements drawn from figure or ground were within the classical RF. V1 responses to figure elements were reported to be significantly larger than responses to ground elements. This result implies that figure versus ground is encoded by differential response rate, and that larger spatial context elicits this differential. Using stimuli similar to those of Lamme (1995), however, a more recent study (Rossi et al. 2001) failed to find any response differential in V1 for figure versus ground elements in the classical RF (see also Cumming & Parker 1998). On the other hand, using stimuli consisting of two overlapping rectangles, which possess robust occlusionbased depth-ordering cues (T-junctions), Chang et al. (1999, 2001) detected small populations of both V1 and V2 neurons that responded differentially to the interiors of figure versus ground rectangles. Although differential responses to figure versus ground interiors could constitute the neuronal substrate for the heightened salience of figure versus ground (e.g., Kanizsa 1979), further evidence is needed to resolve the discrepancies between these different studies. The attribution of figures to one side or the other of an image boundary is commonly referred to as assigning border ownership (Koffka 1935, Rubin 1921,

14 352 ALBRIGHT STONER Zhou et al. 2000). Border ownership identifies which side of the boundary is seen as the occluder and which side as the occluded background. Several recent studies have found evidence of neuronal correlates of this perceptual process at early stages within the visual pathway. Zhou et al. (2000) stimulated V1, V2, and V4 (see Figure 3) neurons using stimuli that consisted of single rectangles (figure defined by closure) and overlapping pairs of rectangles (figure defined by occlusionbased depth-ordering cues, i.e., T-junctions). These investigators placed the border between figure and ground within the classical RF and varied border ownership, such that the figure lay on one side of the RF or the other. Slightly more than half of the recorded neurons in areas V2 and V4 exhibited significantly different responses for contrast edges as a function of border ownership (Figure 5). A smaller fraction of V1 neurons showed similar effects. The spatial integration of these contextual cues was found to extend to at least 20. Moreover, they emerged almost immediately following stimulus onset, suggesting a rapid conveyance of contextual information from the nonclassical RF. Zhou et al. also found that different cues for border ownership (closure, T-junctions, relative size) yielded similar effects, which indicates that these neurons were encoding border ownership per se, rather than a particular cue for border ownership. Chang et al. (2001) used a different strategy to differentiate selectivity for border ownership from sensitivity to incidental aspects of stimulus geometry. Specifically, stimuli with occlusion cues (T-junctions) yielding ambiguous figure-ground interpretation were compared with those with unambiguous interpretation. The unambiguous stimuli were overlapping rectangles similar to those of Zhou et al. This enlarged stimulus set allowed identification of neurons responding merely to the presence of T-junctions, rather than to depth-ordering per se. With the incorporation of these experimental controls, Chang et al. (2001) confirmed that responses correlated with border ownership existed within V2, although these effects were modest in magnitude and prevalence and were rarely seen in V1. Two additional studies sought neuronal correlates of border ownership using stimuli with occlusion cues local to the stimulating border and thus present in the classical RF. Baumann et al. (1997) stimulated neurons in areas V2 and V3/V3A with spatially defined occlusion cues and observed responses that were consistent with border ownership. Stoner et al. (1998) stimulated neurons in area MT using temporally defined occlusion cues (i.e., accretion-deletion) and found evidence for figure-ground selectivity. Much more work is needed to establish the precise role(s) of these different cortical areas in figure-ground interpretation. The existing neurophysiological evidence suggests, nonetheless, that neuronal representations of figure-ground relationships are pervasive and are manifested early in the visual pathway. Completion of Occluded Information As we have seen, figure-ground interpretation follows the occlusion of one surface by another. Figure-ground interpretation itself leads to another important process: the filling-in of occluded features. There are two types of filling-in associated

15 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 353 Figure 5 Border ownership responses from a V2 neuron. Responses to white squares on gray background are indicated by open bars. Responses to gray squares on white background are indicated by filled bars. Stimuli vary in luminance contrast polarity (i.e., white vs. gray or gray vs. white) across columns and implied border ownership across rows. This cell preferred the border ownership implied by the top row of stimuli in which figures were on the bottom left side of the receptive field (RF). Ellipses indicate classical RF (note that the preferred orientation is that of the short axis of the ellipse). This cell differentiated displays that were identical in an 8 16 region around the RF. (From Zhou et al Copyright 2000 by the Society for Neuroscience.) with occlusion, termed modal and amodal completion (Michotte et al. 1964). Modal completion refers to illusory image features that appear to have resulted from direct stimulation of the visual modality (Kanizsa 1979). The classic Kanizsa triangle (Figure 6) elicits illusory contours, which comprise a case of modal completion driven by contextual cues for surface occlusion. As illustrated in the

16 354 ALBRIGHT STONER Figure 6 The Kanizsa triangle illustrates two forms of filling-in modal and amodal completion elicited by contextual cues for surface occlusion. Modal completion: Coincident alignment of contrast edges and line endings allows for perceptual interpolation of a foreground occluding triangle. Although the inferred foreground occluder and background are physically indistinct, contextual cues enable this missing boundary information to be filled-in. The resulting illusory contours have a sensory-like quality or modal presence. Amodal completion: The existence of a triangular foreground occluder is implied, in part, by the V-shaped segments missing from each of three black features. The occluder implies, in turn, that the black features are actually partially occluded full circles. Similarly, the three black-outlined Vs along the edges of the foreground occluding surface appear to compose a second, partially occluded, triangle. Although observers do not perceive the filled-in occluded features of these objects with the verisimilitude of their unoccluded portions, they are nonetheless fully and directly aware of the presence of occluded features. Moreover, the filled-in occluded features possess very specific forms, which are largely dictated by contextual cues, and observers can readily direct actions toward filled-in features, suggesting that they are explicitly represented in the brain. (From Kanizsa 1979.)

17 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 355 Kanizsa triangle, modal completion usually requires that foreground and occluded background have the same apparent intensity a relatively rare occurrence in natural scenes. [The illusory phi motion discovered by Wertheimer [1961 (1912)] and perceptual filling-in of the blind spot/scotomas (all reviewed above) are also striking examples of modal completion, though unrelated to surface occlusion.] In seminal experiments von der Heydt et al. (1984) explored neuronal correlates of modal completion. Stimuli consisted of illusory contours, which were induced by contextual cues for the presence of a foreground occluding surface (analogous to the case illustrated in Figure 6). The illusory contours were moved through the classical RFs of V1 and V2 neurons by moving the inducing context through the RF surround. Remarkably, when contextual cues were configured to elicit a percept of illusory contours, they also elicited neuronal responses in V2 (but rarely V1) that were comparable to responses elicited by real contours (Figure 7) despite the fact that the illusory contours presented no physical contrast within the classical RF! Redies et al. (1986) obtained similar results from cat cortical areas 17 and 18. Although these data support a model in which modal completion evolves in V2 from real contour signals present in V1, the V1-V2 distinction made by von der Heydt and colleagues has been challenged. Grosof et al. (1993) and Lee & Nguyen (2001) found that a significant fraction of V1 neurons responded to illusory contours (see also Sheth et al. 1996, Ramsden et al. 2001). In addition, the neuronal blind spot filling-in effects reported in V1 by Komatsu et al. (2000) (see above) suggest the existence of a mechanism for contour completion in V1. Lee & Nguyen reported, however, that V1 responses to illusory contours lag behind responses to real contours by about 55 ms, whereas this difference is only 30 ms in area V2. They suggest that this lag could reflect feedback latencies from area V2 or from horizontal connections intrinsic to V1. As we shall see, the debate about the contribution of V1 to modal contour completion is further complicated by recent evidence that V1 neurons represent amodal contours. Amodal completion, which is the second type of filling-in, refers to perceptual interpolation of features seen to lie behind a foreground occluding surface [termed amodal, according to Kanizsa (1979), because the completed information is not verified by any sensory modality ]. Matching the ubiquity of occlusion in the visual environment, amodal completion is much more common, and hence arguably of greater functional significance than the modal variety. The specific features that are amodally completed are determined by contextual cues surrounding the occluding surface. Given that we do not actually see amodal features (unlike modally completed features), it might be thought that they are not explicitly represented. Counter to this supposition, however, are the observations that amodal features have a specific form, surface character, and extent, and are not easily modifiable by cognitive intent (e.g., try imagining the occluded triangle contours in Figure 6 as anything other than straight lines). Moreover, an observer can easily direct a motor action to amodal features (as one might reach behind a book to grab the edge of a desk it partially occludes). Quantitative psychophysical evidence supports these anecdotal phenomena (e.g., Nakayama & Shimojo 1990) all of which argues that amodal features are explicitly represented by the brain.

18 356 ALBRIGHT STONER

19 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 357 Even if explicitly represented, the phenomenally nonvisual character of amodal features would seem to place them in the domain of higher-order visual areas. This appears not to be the case. Sugita (1999) reported neuronal correlates of amodal completion within area V1. When presented with two collinear line segments placed on opposite sides of the classical RF, over 10% of V1 neurons responded if the gap between the segments was placed stereoscopically near (hence consistent with occlusion) (Figure 8). By contrast, these same neurons failed to respond if the gap was positioned stereoscopically behind the line segments, such that amodal completion was prohibited. Sugita found that response latencies to amodal contours were similar to those elicited by real contours presented within the classical RF. On this basis, Sugita concluded that these responses are likely to be mediated by horizontal connections within V1 or perhaps by short-latency feedback connections from V2. The sensitivity of V1 to amodal features implies neuronal X-ray vision ; with the help of contextual cues, area V1 constructs a representation of what lies hidden behind occluders. The apparent qualitative similarity between neuronal representations of amodal, modal, and real contours seems to conflict, however, with their differing perceptual qualities. On the other hand, as we have noted, amodally completed features are tangible enough to expect some processing overlap with real features. The degrees of processing differences and overlap are sure to emerge as research begins to focus on the cellular mechanisms by which context gives rise to these forms of filling-in. INTERRUPTED CONTOURS: FACILITATION AND GROUPING As we have seen, contours can be interrupted in the visual image by a variety of means, including surface occlusion and shadows. Depending upon the visual configuration, contours may be perceptually completed across these gaps (modally or amodally). Two related behavioral paradigms have also been used to explore Figure 7 Responses of a V2 neuron to modal (illusory) contours. Stimuli are shown at left. Ellipses indicate the classical receptive field (RF). Crosses indicate the position of the monkey s fixation point. Each row of dots in the right column corresponds to a forward and backward sweep across the neuronal RF. Each dot represents an action potential. (A) Responses to a real contour. (B) Responses to a modally completed (illusory) contour were qualitatively similar, even though no physical contrast was present in its classical RF. (C) Placing thin intersection lines between black contours and intervening black patch prevented modal completion. Consistent with perception, no response was observed. (D) Spontaneous activity. Numbers below each panel of spike trains indicate mean spike counts per stimulus cycle. (From Peterhans & von der Heydt Copyright 1989 by the Society for Neuroscience.)

20 358 ALBRIGHT STONER Figure 8 Responses of a V1 neuron to amodal contours. (A) Responses to a real contour. (B) Responses to two unconnected line segments (no stimulus in classical receptive field). (C) Responses to two collinear segments with a patch placed stereoscopically in front. (D) Responses to two collinear segments with a patch placed stereoscopically behind. Each stimulus type was visible during periods indicated by bars under each histogram and was swept in each of two directions (left bar, preferred direction; right bar, not preferred direction). This neuron exhibited directionally selective responses to both real (A) and amodally completed (C) contours. Little response was observed for unconnected segments in which amodal completion was not engaged by the presence of a foreground occluding surface (B and D). (From Sugita 1999.) contextual interactions between contour fragments. The first has been used to show that collinear contour fragments tend to be perceptually grouped (Field et al. 1993). The second paradigm of this genre has been used to demonstrate that the processing of oriented contour fragments can be facilitated by flanking collinear contours (Polat & Sagi 1993, 1994; Kapadia et al. 1995). Neurophysiological experiments that aim to identify neuronal correlates of grouping and contour facilitation require assessment of the degree to which the response to an oriented RF stimulus is modulated by contours in the RF surround. Many studies have documented surround effects in area V1 using oriented stimuli

21 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 359 (e.g., Hubel & Wiesel 1965; Bishop et al. 1973; Maffei & Fiorentini 1976; Nelson & Frost 1985; Gulyas et al. 1987; Gilbert & Wiesel 1990; Knierim & van Essen 1992; Li & Li 1994; Kapadia et al. 1995, 2000; Sillito et al. 1995; Polat et al. 1998; Li et al. 2000, 2001). A subset of these have manipulated contour collinearity and have yielded evidence of response facilitation when discontinuous contours are placed collinearly along the long axis of a RF (Maffei & Fiorentini 1976, Nelson & Frost 1985, Kapadia et al. 1995). Most notably, Kapadia et al. (1995, 1999) found that the responses of V1 and V2 neurons to a suprathreshold line within the classical RF can be greatly increased by adding a flanking collinear line in the RF surround. Contour response facilitation could represent a neuronal correlate of perceptual continuity in the face of contour fragmentation caused by occlusion. In an elegant examination of this hypothesis, Bakin et al. (2000) obtained evidence suggesting that the neuronal collinear facilitation effect is associated with amodal completion (see Figure 9). To control the degree of amodal completion, these investigators placed additional lines orthogonally between the RF stimulus and flanking stimuli, and manipulated the relative depth of these lines using binocular disparity cues. The effects of these manipulations on neuronal response facilitation appeared strikingly consistent with amodal completion: Facilitation was blocked by placing orthogonal bars in a depth plane behind the collinear fragments, in a manner suggesting that the fragments lie on separate foreground surfaces (no amodal completion). Facilitation recovered when the orthogonal bars were placed in the near depth plane, in a manner that allows the fragments to lie on the same background surface (amodal completion). This differential effect of orthogonal bar depth was seen by Bakin et al. (2000) in half of the V2 neurons so tested but was rarely seen in area V1. Although interesting, these results are somewhat at odds with those of Sugita (1999). Unlike Bakin et al., Sugita found neuronal correlates of amodal completion in V1. This discrepancy may simply be due to different sampling of the visual field (Sugita s sample was limited to the central 2, whereas that of Bakin et al. extended 3 6 ). Somewhat more puzzling, perhaps, is the fact that Bakin et al. observed neuronal contour facilitation under one condition in which neuronal (and indeed perceptual) amodal completion does not appear to occur (Sugita 1999): two interrupted fragments with no intervening surface (compare the stimulus configuration of Figure 8B with that of 9C). This observation casts doubt on the conclusion that contour facilitation depends upon amodal completion. This intriguing incongruity might best be resolved by examining neuronal correlates of amodal completion and flank facilitation in tandem while simultaneously monitoring perceptual interpretation. These neuronal findings of Gilbert and colleagues (Bakin et al. 2000; Kapadia et al. 1995, 1999) also appear to have a connection to the behavioral phenomena of contrast facilitation (Dresp 1993; Polat & Sagi 1993, 1994; Zenger & Sagi 1996) and contour grouping (Field et al. 1993). Plausible candidates for mediation of these varied contour phenomena are long-range horizontal connections within area V1 (Rockland & Lund 1982, Gilbert & Wiesel 1983, Martin & Whitteridge 1984). These connections are anisotropic and preferentially connect neurons that have collinear RFs (Bosking et al. 1997, Schmidt et al. 1997).

22 360 ALBRIGHT STONER Figure 9 Collinear contextual response facilitation in area V2. Collinear contours in nonclassical receptive field (RF) facilitate responses to RF contour, but not if contextual conditions are inconsistent with amodal completion (but see text). Flanking contour facilitates responses (compare A with C). An orthogonal bar placed in either the same plane (G) as the flank and target stimulus or in the far depth plane (F; 0.16 uncrossed disparity) blocked the flankinduced facilitation of the response to the target stimulus. Conversely, placing the orthogonal bar in the near depth plane (H; 0.16 crossed disparity) did not block this facilitation. (From Bakin et al Copyright 2000 by the Society for Neuroscience.) We have discussed evidence of neuronal filling-in/completion of oriented features and surface properties. We next discuss how completion of moving surfaces might manifest itself as a contextual effect arising from the nonclassical RF of motion-detecting neurons. OCCLUSION AND VISUAL MOTION PROCESSING Although occlusion presents a ubiquitous challenge in the recovery of visual scene properties, it poses a special challenge to the recovery of visual motion. Where occluded, the edge of a moving surface appears terminated in the visual image, yielding a feature that traces an unambiguous trajectory across the image. Because such features and their trajectories are defined, however, not by the moving object, but by the object s coincidental intersection with a foreground occluder (Figure 10),

23 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 361 they provide spurious information about the motion of the object in the scene. For that reason, the visual motion system must distinguish the false motion information provided by features extrinsic to a moving object from the valid information provided by features intrinsic to that object (Nakayama et al. 1989). Shimojo et al. (1989) demonstrated that human subjects utilize depth-ordering cues to distinguish intrinsic and extrinsic features, which in turn enables recovery of the motions of partially occluded objects. Duncan et al. (2000) devised a variant of the classic barber-pole illusion (Wallach 1935) to explore the neuronal substrates of contextual depth-motion interactions. Their barber-diamond stimuli consisted of a moving grating framed by a static diamond-shaped aperture (Figure 11). Two of four textured panels that defined the aperture were placed in front of the grating via stereoscopic depth cues, and the other two were placed behind. These depth manipulations simulated partial occlusion of the grating. The features formed by termination ( terminators ) of the grating stripes at the far panels were seen as intrinsic, and those formed at the near panels were seen as extrinsic to the grating. When the grating was moved horizontally, human observers reported that the grating moved diagonally upwards or downwards, in the direction of the intrinsic terminators. The depth cues at the margins of the moving stimulus thus provided a context for perceptual disambiguation of motion within the interior. [Similar perceptual motion effects can be obtained using simulated shadows, rather than binocular disparity, to elicit depth-ordering (G.R. Stoner, unpublished observations).] Duncan et al. (2000) compared these perceptual effects to the responses of directionally selective neurons in cortical area MT. Barber-diamond stimuli were positioned such that the aperture edges (and hence depth-ordering cues) were outside of the classical RF. Many MT neurons exhibited directionally selective responses consistent with perceived direction of motion, i.e., depth-ordering cues that were restricted to the RF surround predictably altered responses to moving features within the classical RF (Figure 12). These findings challenge previous characterizations of nonclassical receptive field (RF) surround effects within area MT, which have stressed relatively simple feature contrast effects. As noted above, the responses of many MT neurons to motion in the classical RF are modulated by motion in the surround (Figure 4) (Allman et al. 1985). A similar modulation has been reported for binocular disparity (Bradley & Andersen 1998). These two types of intra-modal (motion-motion and depth-depth) interactions may be helpful in signaling cue-specific image discontinuities (Bradley & Andersen 1998) or extracting depth variation based on either motion parallax or binocular disparity (Buracas & Albright 1996, Liu & Kersten 1998, Xiao et al. 1997). Bradley & Andersen (1998) found few interactions between depth and motion, however, which reinforced a view of surrounds as exerting intra-modal and mostly antagonistic modulation of responses to stimulation of the classical RF. In the Bradley & Andersen study, depth and motion were independent stimulus attributes, neither bearing on the interpretation of the other. By contrast, in the barber-diamond stimuli employed by Duncan et al. (2000) (as in natural scenes)

24 362 ALBRIGHT STONER

25 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 363 depth cues provide a context that triggers recovery of occluded scene elements, which in turn modulates the representation of motion. Thus, as was the case for many of the contextual effects reviewed herein, it was the incorporation of occlusion cues that uncovered the sophisticated RF surround interactions observed by Duncan et al. Although the precise mechanisms responsible for the depth-motion interactions of Duncan et al. are unknown, there are some plausible alternatives. One possibility is that the occlusion cues elicit a neuronal representation of the amodally completed grating behind the near panels, as suggested by the findings of Sugita (1999) (reviewed above). These completed contours might then lead to the recruitment of additional motion detectors, which may in turn shift the balance in favor of motion in the direction of intrinsic terminators. A second possible mechanism involves selective pooling of motion signals determined to be intrinsic to the same surface (Stoner & Albright 1993). For example, pooling of the signals arising from the horizontally moving gratings with the diagonally oriented far edge (intrinsic to the grating) would yield the observed diagonal motion of the barber-diamond. A third mechanism involves selective suppression of motion signals arising from extrinsic features (Liden & Pack 1999), which would tilt the balance in favor of intrinsic terminator motions. These three potential mechanisms are, of course, not mutually exclusive. FILLING-IN REVISITED: NORMAL SURFACE PERCEPTION At any one time, only a small proportion of cells (in visual cortex) are likely to be influenced (activated or suppressed), since contours of inappropriate orientation and diffuse light (within the receptive field) have little or no effect on a cell (Quotation from Hubel & Wiesel 1968). Figure 10 Illustration of the contextual dependence of motion perception. Two simple visual scenes are shown, along with image motions they give rise to. (a) A single moving object oriented obliquely and moving downward. Image motion reflects object motion. (b) A single obliquely oriented bar moving to lower right (orthogonal to orientation). The scene also contains occluders, which block the observer s view of the ends of the bar. Resulting visual image motion in (b) is identical to that caused by the scene in (a), although the visual scene motions are clearly different. Observers perceive the different object motions correctly, and they do so because of different spatial contexts, which elicit different interpretations of the terminations of the moving line in the visual image. Terminators in (a) are interpreted as ends of an elongated object, i.e., they are intrinsic features of that object and their downward motions genuinely reflect object motion. Terminators in (b), by contrast, are extrinsic to the moving line (an accident of occlusion), and their downward motions are deemed spurious. Thus, terminator interpretation determines how the image motions are perceived.

26 364 ALBRIGHT STONER Hubel & Wiesel s observation implies that visual neurons only respond to image contrast and that every homogenous region in the visual image is, in effect, a blind spot. Surfaces with uniform reflectance are common and, hence, so are uniform image regions. This poses a profound computational problem and a mystery: How is it that we are not blind to much of the world? Part of the answer might be thought to lie in the functions of luxotonic neurons, which do respond to diffuse sustained light. However, the importance of these neurons is called into question by their relative rarity in the species in which they have been detected (DeYoe & Bartlett 1980, Kayama et al. 1979) and their absence in other species (Kahrilas et al. 1980). Moreover, as others have noted (Walls 1954, Gerrits & Vendrik 1970), the computational problem posed

27 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 365 by uniform regions in the visual image has an obvious parallel with that posed by the blind spot and scotomas, and the same mechanistic considerations and potential neuronal solutions apply. In particular, one can ask whether filling-in of homogenous image regions is an active or passive process. As we have seen, perceptual completion of image gaps caused by the blind spot and scotomas, as well as by surface occlusion, involves an active neuronal process in which the missing features are explicitly reconstructed. It is possible, nonetheless, that our ability to perceive areas of constant coloration is accomplished implicitly (i.e., passively). For example, imagine a gray surface atop a white background. Neurons encoding one spatial location detect one surface border (light-dark); neurons encoding another location detect the complementary discontinuity (dark-light). According to an implicit encoding scheme, border-detecting neurons signal that the surface is darker than the background, while uniformity of Figure 11 Schematic depiction of barber-diamond stimuli used to study the influence of context on perceived motion and its neuronal correlates (a demonstration can be seen at gene/). (a) Stimuli consisted of a moving square-wave grating framed by a static, diamond-shaped aperture. The grating itself was placed in the plane of ocular fixation while four textured panels that defined the aperture were independently positioned in depth using binocular disparity cues. The grating was moved either leftward or rightward on each trial. Each of the four stimulus conditions used was a unique conjunction of direction of grating motion (right vs. left) and depth-ordering configuration. The two conditions illustrated (b and c) were created using rightward-moving gratings; two additional conditions (not shown) were created using leftward-moving gratings. Near and Far identify the depth-ordering of the textured panels relative to the plane of the grating. (b) Upper-right and lower-left panels were placed in the near depth plane, while the upper-left and lower-right panels were in the far depth plane. Line terminators formed at the boundary of near surfaces and grating are classified as extrinsic features resulting from occlusion, and the grating is amodally completed behind the near surface. (Note that gray stripes are not part of the stimulus and are used solely to illustrate amodal completion.) Conversely, line terminators formed at the boundary of the far surfaces and the grating are classified as intrinsic, i.e., they appear to result from the physical termination of the surface upon which the stripes are painted. As a result of this depth-ordering manipulation and ensuing feature interpretation, observers typically perceive the moving grating in (b) as belonging to a surface that slides behind the near panels and across the far panels, i.e., to the upper-right. This direction is identified with motions of intrinsic terminators. The condition shown in (c) contains the same rightward grating motion but employs the depth-ordering configuration that is complementary to (b). In this case observers typically perceive the grating belonging to a surface that is drifting to the lower right. (After Duncan et al Copyright 2000 by the Society for Neuroscience.)

28 366 ALBRIGHT STONER Figure 12 Influence of depth-ordering on direction selectivity of an MT neuron. (a) Neuronal responses to circular gratings moved in each of six directions. These responses (R1 R6) were used to form three different predictions for barber-diamond stimuli (c, d and e). (b) Actual responses to barber-diamond stimuli. Icons illustrate the stimulus configuration for each of four experimental conditions. (c) If depth-ordering in the receptive field surround has no effect, neuronal responses will be influenced solely by the horizontal motion (black arrows) of the grating and, hence, be of the same relative magnitude as responses to circular gratings moved leftward (R2) and rightward (R5), regardless of depth-ordering configuration. (d) If, as expected, neuronal responses are associated with the direction of intrinsic terminator motion (gray arrows), they should be of the same relative magnitude as responses to circular grating moving in the corresponding oblique directions (R1, R3, R4, and R6). (e) If neuronal responses are associated with the direction of extrinsic terminator motion (white arrows), they should be of the same relative magnitude as the intrinsic motion prediction but flipped about the horizontal axis. Observed responses (b) of this neuron to barber-diamond stimuli were more closely correlated with the intrinsic motion prediction than with either the horizontal or extrinsic motion prediction. (From Duncan et al Copyright 2000 by the Society for Neuroscience.)

29 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 367 the interior is signaled by the lack of activity of neurons with RFs positioned over intermediate spatial locations. An analogous proposal can be made for our sense of a temporally continuous world (for related discussion see O Regan 1992). Say your eyes fixate a blue surface, then a red one; neurons sensitive to that chromatic change will respond transiently, and this activation renders a sense of redness even after the response has ended. Taken together, these considerations suggest a brain very stingy with its action potentials, none being wasted on signaling that things have not changed over space or time. Although it is appealingly economical, arguments and evidence against the sufficiency of this implicit encoding scheme are not hard to find. One empirical argument comes from studies of brightness induction. The interior of a gray patch will appear to undergo a brightness change if the surrounding area is temporally modulated from light to dark. If the frequency of that modulation is varied sinusoidally, it will be discovered that this induction effect disappears above about 2.5 Hz (De Valois et al. 1986, Rossi & Paradiso 1996). This temporal cutoff is much lower than that at which the border between the gray patch and surround appear unchanging. The perception of edges and interior brightness can thus be decoupled and we can conclude that the two cannot be mediated by the same pattern of neuronal activity. A more general problem with the implicit coding scheme is that contrast measures at the nearest borders are simply not sufficient to support color and brightness perception of interior zones: As discussed below, the visual system integrates diverse types of information from large regions of visual space to infer the reflectance of a given region of visual space (e.g., Land & McCann 1971, Adelson 1993, Brown & MacLeod 1997, Barnes et al. 1999, Purves et al. 1999, Shevell & Wei 1998, Wachtler et al. 2001) (Figure 13B). Very complicated nontopographic schemes involving the neurons responding to these diverse information sources could, of course, be advanced, but they hardly seem parsimonious. Fortunately, whether there exist topographic neuronal responses related to brightness perception is not a question whose answer is hostage to theory. Paradiso and colleagues ( Rossi et al. 1996, Rossi & Paradiso 1999, MacEvoy & Paradiso 2001) have examined neuronal correlates of brightness induction. In one study they recorded from retina, thalamic lateral geniculate nucleus, and area V1 of anesthetized cats. The experiments were designed to determine whether neurons respond to brightness changes caused by either direct or contextual ( induced ) means. Direct brightness changes were produced by varying the intensity of a central patch of light superimposed on the classical RF. Induced brightness changes were caused by holding the classical RF stimulus constant and varying light intensity in the RF surround. A substantial fraction of V1 neurons unlike those at earlier processing stages exhibited responses that covaried with the brightness of the classical RF stimulus, regardless of whether those brightness changes were caused directly or induced. The findings of Paradiso and colleagues and those from recent related studies (Hung et al. 2001) suggest a topographically explicit, rather than implicit, representation of brightness. This conclusion does not refute the observations of Hubel &

30 368 ALBRIGHT STONER Wiesel (1968) cortical neurons do, in fact, respond to luminance contrast within the classical RF but weakly or not at all to diffuse light. Some cortical neurons are, however, sensitive to luminance contrast cues outside the classical field, and that sensitivity permits them to, in effect, encode the ambiguous properties of homogenous regions within that field. Evidence indicates that the observed topographic neuronal representation of uniform image regions develops progressively from border to interior. In human observers this progressive completion can be observed directly and its time course measured (Paradiso & Hahn 1996, Paradiso & Nakayama 1991). Neuronal evidence from artificial scotoma experiments (reviewed above) (De Weerd et al. 1995) also supports progressive completion, although it is important to note that these artificial scotomas were fixed (at least transiently) relative to eye position, unlike uniform surfaces under normal viewing conditions. RECOVERING SURFACE REFLECTANCE: USING CONTEXT TO SEE BENEATH THE ILLUMINANT Having reviewed evidence for an active neuronal filling-in of uniform image regions, we now turn to function: What is completed, and why? We begin by emphasizing a resemblance to the recovery of visual information lost owing to occlusion. As we have seen, amodal completion answers the question What lies behind? by predicting the content of occluded surfaces. Similarly, completion of uniform image regions identifies what is in front. Although we have portrayed this latter form of completion as propagation of information from edges to interior, the computation required is generally far more complex. As amodal completion must see beneath occluding surfaces, so must the mechanisms of foreground surface completion see beneath the light illuminating the scene in order to recover surface reflectance. To appreciate the magnitude and significance of this problem, recall that the light reaching the retina is a generally a product of two factors: surface reflectance and illumination. Reflectance relates the quantity of light reflected to its wavelength. Reflectance is constant over time (though it may vary over space) for most surfaces and is thus a valuable source of information for object recognition. Illumination is characterized by its intensity as a function of wavelength. Unlike reflectance, however, the illuminant can and often does vary over space and time. Because surface reflectance and illumination properties are confounded in the light reaching the retina, it is impossible to deduce the reflectance of a surface given only the light arising from that surface. Despite this limitation, the primate visual system is quite good at recovering surface reflectance in natural scenes even when faced with significant changes in illumination. (For example, the cover of this book will appear red under many different lighting conditions.) This invariance of color appearance is known as color constancy. Brightness constancy is an achromatic analogue of color constancy, which is likely to result from similar neuronal substrates and mechanisms.

31 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 369 Vision scientists have puzzled over color constancy for centuries. Thomas Young (1807) observed, When a room is illuminated by either the yellow light of a candle, or the red light of a fire, a sheet of writing paper still appears to retain its whiteness. Helmholtz (2000) also recognized the phenomenon and its functional significance: Colors are mainly important for us as properties of objects and as means of identifying objects....we constantly aim to reach a judgment on the object colors and to eliminate differences of illumination. Helmholtz largely attributed the observer s ability to discount the illuminant, and thereby recover surface reflectance, to prior experience with objects in different lighting conditions. Many of Helmholtz s contemporaries and followers offered explanations rooted solely in spatial context (e.g., Hering 1964 [1920], Mach 1886). Contextual Influences on Color Appearance The influence of context on color appearance has long been a popular topic in the field of visual psychophysics, and there now exists an enormous body of data characterizing human sensitivity. One of the most robust effects of context in this domain one that has been known for centuries is the dependency of perceived color on the color of adjacent regions (Figure 13A). From a functional perspective, simple context-induced shifts in color appearance can be viewed as a by-product of a system designed to recover surface reflectance in the face of changing illumination: While an illumination change will alter the spectrum of light arising from a given surface, it does so for every surface so illuminated. Therefore, as noted by the psychologist Hans Wallach (1948), the ratios of spectra from adjacent surfaces generally remain unchanged. Invariant color appearance should thus result from stimuli that possess spectral ratios that are invariant between adjacent image regions. Conversely, in the absence of ratio invariance, perceived color should change, as it indeed does. Neuronal Correlates of Color Appearance Until recently, very little was known of the neuronal bases of color appearance. Although there have been several detailed neuropsychological studies of acquired deficiencies of human color vision (achromatopsia) (for review see Zeki 1990), it has been difficult to discover from these studies brain regions specifically involved in representing perceived color, as opposed to low-level color detection and analysis. The past four decades have also seen numerous electrophysiological studies of neurons in the retina, thalamus, and visual cortex that respond to chrominance and/or luminance contrast within their classical RFs (for review see Lennie & D Zmura 1988). However, most of these studies did not include contextual manipulations that alter color appearance, such as those shown in Figure 13A. Without such manipulations it is impossible to determine whether a neuronal response covaries with the spectral power of the light in the classical RF or, alternatively, with perceived color/brightness. We now turn to a few recent studies that have included appropriate contextual manipulations and have obtained intriguing results.

32 370 ALBRIGHT STONER ACHROMATIC COLOR APPEARANCE (BRIGHTNESS) The complexity of color appearance phenomena is reduced when considering achromatic stimuli, but similar contextual effects and mechanistic interpretations apply. We have already discussed the brightness induction effects reported for neurons in cat area V1 by Paradiso and colleagues (Rossi et al. 1996, Rossi & Paradiso 1999). Not only might these effects underlie perceptual filling-in of the interiors of uniform image regions, but they satisfy one of the criteria for a mechanism that recovers surface reflectance. Specifically, the neuronal response changes when the ratio of intensities changes between adjacent surfaces. If these neurons do, in fact, represent surface reflectance, we should also expect them to exhibit an invariant response under conditions that mimic variations in diffuse illumination, i.e., where contrast remains unchanged. Recent evidence is consistent with this requirement (MacEvoy & Paradiso 2001). CHROMATIC COLOR APPEARANCE Zeki (1983a,b) was the first to attempt to distinguish between neuronal representations of chromatic spectral composition and perceived color. Zeki adopted a stimulus configuration (Figure 13B) that had been a favorite tool of the color theorist Edwin Land for manipulating color appearance (Land & McCann 1971). The stimulus is known as a Mondrian for its resemblance to the style of the painter. These stimuli possess rich chromatic structure and they elicit robust color constancy effects. In Zeki s experiments, one of the colored panels in the Mondrian was placed over the classical RF of a cortical neuron, and the remaining panels were placed in the RF surround. When the color of the illuminant changed, the responses of neurons in area V1 changed in accordance with the wavelength of light falling in the classical RF. By contrast, the responses of a subset of neurons in area V4 (see Figure 3) were unaltered by illumination changes and thus paralleled the perceived constancy of the RF stimulus. Zeki suggested, on the basis of these results, that V4 may be the site at which chromatic context is integrated with the RF stimulus in order to achieve a representation of perceived color. Using simpler stimulus manipulations of the type shown in Figure 13A, Schein & Desimone (1990) also examined the influence of chromatic context on the color selectivity of V4 neurons. Figure 14 illustrates one of their findings. The response of this neuron to the preferred chromatic stimulus placed in the classical RF was dependent upon the color of the surround. To a first approximation, these results are consistent with a mechanism in which the output is a function of chromatic contrast between the classical RF stimulus and the surround. But what is the function of such cells? Schein & Desimone speculated that they may signal the average intensity of the illuminant in specific wave-bands. The response of the cell illustrated, for example, should decline as the redness of the illuminant increases. Cells that serve as wave-band intensity gauges could, by explicitly recovering the properties of the illuminant, permit the illuminant to be discounted and surface reflectance computed (Land 1986). Wachtler et al. (1999) used similarly simple stimuli to re-examine the role of area V1 in color appearance. In contrast to Schein & Desimone (1990) and Rossi

33 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 371 Figure 14 Chromatic selectivity of a V4 neuron is influenced by chromatic context. (Left) Wavelength tuning reveals that the neuron responds optimally to a stimulus of 600 nm in the classical receptive field (RF). (Center) The cell failed to respond to a chromatic background that extended around but did not intrude upon the classical RF. (Right) The chromatic background did, however, modulate the response to the preferred stimulus in the classical RF. To a first approximation, these results are consistent with a mechanism in which the output is a function of chromatic contrast between adjacent stimuli. Moreover, the modulation of response to the classical RF stimulus parallels the context-induced shift in color appearance that occurs for such stimuli. (From Schein & Desimone Copyright 1990 by the Society for Neuroscience.) et al. (1996), Wachtler et al. evaluated the effects of color changes in the RF surround on the full chromatic tuning to the classical RF stimulus. For many V1 neurons the effects were striking. For example, introduction of a reddish surround to a red-preferring neuron had the effect of shifting the chromatic tuning curve away from red and toward the green side of color space. Significantly, the same contextual manipulation causes the central square to appear more greenish to an observer (Wachtler et al. 2001). Interestingly, this type of neuronal response shift is similar to what one would expect from double-opponent color-selective neurons, which have been thought to play a role in color constancy (Conway 2001). The important difference, however, is that the contextual manipulations of Wachtler et al. were highly effective even though they were restricted to the RF surround.

34 372 ALBRIGHT STONER These results suggest that at least some V1 neurons use context to achieve a representation of perceived color. Although this conclusion mirrors that of Rossi et al. (1996) regarding the role of V1 in brightness perception, it is at variance with Zeki s (1983a,b) claim that such contextual effects are not seen until V4. This difference is difficult to reconcile, although it is noteworthy that Wachtler et al. studied V1 responses in alert monkeys, whereas Zeki s subjects were anesthetized. Context and Color: Beyond Spatial Effects We have thus far considered the simple contextual conditions in which the color appearance of a surface is influenced by light arising from adjacent (or nearby) coplanar surfaces. In most real scenes, however, the light arising from surfaces is determined not only by their reflectance and the properties of the illuminant, but also generally by the angle of the surface relative to the observer and the illumination source. One important consequence is that color ratios between surfaces, which provide evidence of reflectance and illumination, can serve as meaningful cues only if interpreted in the additional context of three-dimensional surface configuration. Striking demonstrations and psychophysical confirmation of this important caveat have been provided by Adelson (1993) and Purves et al. (1999) (see also Mach 1886), but nothing is currently known of the neuronal bases of these effects. Although this is an important area for future research, the number and complexity of contextual cue interactions required present a formidable challenge for controlled neurophysiological experiments. CONCLUDING REMARKS Computational considerations and a substantial body of psychophysical and physiological evidence indicate that the visual system employs a well-characterized set of contextual cues to identify the visual scene origins i.e., the meaning of visual image features. A common theme in this review is the use of contextual information to recover visual scene properties lost owing to the superimposition of causes in the visual image. We have, for example, discussed the contextual cues used by the visual system to decompose images into multiple overlapping surfaces and/or lighting conditions. Moreover, we have shown that individual neurons at early stages of cortical processing (e.g., V1 and V2) are involved in the detection of these contextual cues and the representation of recovered information. This recent progress and the attention directed to the role of context represents a critical transition in efforts to understand the neuronal bases of perception. Historically, the reductionism of single-cell physiology has fostered a view of visual neurons as functionally independent windows on the world. In practice, this view has led to an experimental approach that neglects context. The result is that most of the reported responses of visual neurons (e.g., Figure 2) can only be described as neural correlates of the visual image; the full contributions of these neurons to visual scene perception remain unknown.

35 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 373 The transition toward a context-based approach has been inspired, in part, by the discovery of modulatory influences from outside the classical RF. Of even greater importance, perhaps, has been the increased willingness of visual physiologists to turn to experimental psychology for guidance, inspiration, and tools. Embedded in this approach is a renewed focus on function. Accordingly, many of the investigations of neuronal representation reviewed herein are motivated not simply by the question, How does the visual system work? but also by the question, What is the visual system designed to do? The studies we have reviewed are only the tip of an iceberg. The contextual influences we have omitted include, among others, attention and memory, which have been topics of many neuroscientific studies in recent years. There is also a vast realm of contextual effects that are closely related to those we have reviewed, whose manifestations are termed perceptual constancies. These constancies color (reviewed above), size, viewing angle, etc. reflect the recovery of invariant attributes of the visual scene in the face of changing image properties. Further identification of neural correlates of perceptual constancies is an important goal for the future. Finally, in considering future directions, we emphasize that discovery of neuronal correlates is only one step toward understanding the neuronal bases of perception. Evidence of such correlates is now seemingly in abundance but, by contrast, we know very little about how these interesting neuronal properties are implemented. For example, horizontal intrinsic cortical connections are implicated in many phenomena of spatial context, such as filling-in and color appearance effects, but few firm conclusions can be reached. The complexity of cortical wiring and synaptic connectivity/plasticity is indeed daunting. New technologies that enable monitoring of global patterns of neuronal activity and manipulation of that activity within complex circuits nonetheless offer a promising armament for future efforts to reveal the mechanisms underlying contextual influences on visual processing. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Charles Gilbert, Greg Horwitz, Mike Paradiso, and John Reynolds for stimulating discussions and comments on the manuscript. The authors have been supported by grants from the NEI. TDA is an Investigator of the Howard Hughes Medical Institute. The Annual Review of Neuroscience is online at LITERATURE CITED Adelson EH Perceptual organization and the judgment of brightness. Science 262: Albright TD Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. J. Neurophysiol. 52: Allman J, Miezin F, McGuinness E Stimulus specific responses from beyond the

36 374 ALBRIGHT STONER classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 8: Anderson BL The role of partial occlusion in stereopsis. Nature 367: Bakin JS, Nakayama K, Gilbert CD Visual responses in monkey areas V1 and V2 to three-dimensional surface configurations. J. Neurosci. 20: Barlow HB Summation and inhibition in the frog s retina. J. Physiol. 119:69 88 Barlow HB Single units and sensation: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology? Perception 1: Barnes CS, Wei J, Shevell SK Chromatic induction with remote chromatic contrast varied in magnitude, spatial frequency, and chromaticity. Vis. Res. 39: Baumann R, ver der Zwan R, Peterhans E Figure-ground segregation at contours: a neural mechanism in the visual cortex of the alert monkey. Eur. J. Neurosci. 9: Beck J, Prazdny K, Ivry R The perception of transparency with achromatic colors. Percept. Psychophys. 35: Bishop PO, Coombs JS, Henry GH Receptive fields of simple cells in the cat striate cortex. J. Physiol. 231:31 60 Bosking WH, Zhang Y, Schofield B, Fitzpatrick D Orientation selectivity and the arrangement of horizontal connections in tree shrew striate cortex. J. Neurosci. 17: Bradley DC, Andersen RA Centersurround antagonism based on disparity in primate area MT. J. Neurosci. 18: Brown RO, MacLeod DI Color appearance depends on the variance of surround colors. Curr. Biol. 7: Buracas GT, Albright TD Contribution of area MT to perception of three-dimensional shape: a computational study. Vis. Res. 36: Campbell FW, Andrews PR Motion reveals spatial visual defects. Ophthalmic Physiol. Optics 12: Chang G, Stoner GR, Albright TD Neuronal correlates for depth-ordering in area V2. Invest. Opthalmol. Vis. Sci. 40(4): S640 Chang GC, Stoner GR, Albright TD Figure-ground selectivity in macaque V2. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 27:432 Chino YM, Kaas JH, Smith EL, Langston AL, Cheng H Rapid reorganization of cortical maps in adult cats following restricted deafferentation in retina. Vis. Res. 32: Churchland PS, Ramachandran VI Filling-in: why Dennett is wrong. In Dennett and His Critics: Demystifying Mind, ed. B Dahlbom, pp. vii, 247. Oxford/Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Conway BR Spatial structure of cone inputs to color cells in alert macaque primary visual cortex (V-1). J. Neurosci. 21: Croner LJ, Albright TD. 1999a. Segmentation by color influences responses of motionsensitive neurons in the cortical middle temporal visual area. J. Neurosci. 19: Croner LJ, Albright TD. 1999b. Seeing the big picture: integration of image cues in the primate visual system. Neuron 24: Cumming BG, Parker AJ Eye movements explain contextual modulation by binocular disparity in primate V1. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 24:1980 De Valois RL, Webster MA, De Valois KK, Lingelbach B Temporal properties of brightness and color induction. Vis. Res. 26: De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG Perceptual filling-in: a parametric study. Vis. Res. 38: De Weerd P, Gattass R, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG Responses of cells in monkey visual cortex during perceptual filling-in of an artificial scotoma. Nature 377: Dennett DC Consciousness Explained. New York: Little Brown DeYoe EA, Bartlett JR Rarity of luxotonic responses in cortical visual areas of the cat. Exp. Brain. Res. 39: Dresp B Bright lines and edges facilitate the detection of small light targets. Spatial Vis. 7: Duncan RO, Albright TD, Stoner GR

37 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 375 Occlusion and the interpretation of visual motion: perceptual and neuronal effects of context. J. Neurosci. 20: Field DJ, Hayes A, Hess RF Contour integration by the human visual system: evidence for a local association field. Vis. Res. 33: Fiorani JM, Rosa MG, Gattass R, Rocha- Miranda CE Dynamic surrounds of receptive fields in primate striate cortex: a physiological basis for perceptual completion? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 89: Gallant JL, Connor CE, Van Essen DC Neural activity in areas V1, V2 and V4 during free viewing of natural scenes compared to controlled viewing. Neuroreport 9: Garraghty PE, Kaas JH Dynamic features of sensory and motor maps. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2: Gerrits HJ, Timmerman GJ The fillingin process in patients with retinal scotomata. Vis. Res. 9: Gerrits HJM, de Haan B, Vendrick AJH Experiments with retinal stabilized images. Relations between the observations and neural data. Vision Res. 6: Gerrits HJ, Vendrik AJ Simultaneous contrast, filling-in process and information processing in man s visual system. Exp. Brain. Res. 11: Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN Clustered intrinsic connections in cat visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 3: Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN The influence of contextual stimuli on the orientation selectivity of cells in primary visual cortex of the cat. Vis. Res. 30: Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN Receptive field dynamics in adult primary visual cortex. Nature 356: Gilchrist AL Perceived lightness depends on perceived spatial arrangement. Science 195: Grosof DH, Shapley RM, Hawken MJ Macaque V1 neurons can signal illusory contours. Nature 365: Gulyas B, Orban GA, Duysens J, Maes H The suppressive influence of moving textured backgrounds on responses of cat striate neurons to moving bars. J. Neurophysiol. 57: Hartline HK, Graham CH Nerve impulses from single receptors in the eye. J. Cell Comp. Physiol. 1: He S, Davis WL Filling-in at the natural blind spot contributes to binocular rivalry. Vis. Res. 41: Hering E (1920). Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press Hubel DH, Wiesel TN Receptive fields and functional architecture in two nonstriate visual areas (18 and 19) of the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 28: Hubel DH, Wiesel TN Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J. Physiol. 195: Hung CP, Ramsden BM, Chen LM, Roe AW Building surfaces from borders in areas 17 and 18 of the cat. Vis. Res. 41: James W Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt Kaas JH Plasticity of sensory and motor maps in adult mammals. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 14: Kahrilas PJ, Doty RW, Bartlett JR Failure to find luxotonic responses for single units in visual cortex of the rabbit. Exp. Brain Res. 39:11 16 Kanizsa G Organization in Vision: Essays on Gestalt Perception. New York: Praeger Kapadia MK, Ito M, Gilbert CD, Westheimer G Improvement in visual sensitivity by changes in local context: parallel studies in human observers and in V1 of alert monkeys. Neuron 15: Kapadia MK, Westheimer G, Gilbert CD Dynamics of spatial summation in primary visual cortex of alert monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96: Kapadia MK, Westheimer G, Gilbert CD Spatial distribution of contextual interactions in primary visual cortex and in visual perception. J. Neurophysiol. 84: Kaplan GA Kinetic disruption of optical

38 376 ALBRIGHT STONER texture: the perception of depth at an edge. Percept. Psychophys. 6: Kayama Y, Riso RR, Bartlett JR, Doty RW Luxotonic responses of units in macaque striate cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 42: Knierim JJ, van Essen DC Neuronal responses to static texture patterns in area V1 of the alert macaque monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 67: Koffka K Principles of Gestalt Psychology. New York: Harcourt Brace Komatsu H, Kinoshita M, Murakami I Neural responses in the retinotopic representation of the blind spot in the macaque V1 to stimuli for perceptual filling-in. J. Neurosci. 20: Lamme VAF The neurophysiology of figure-ground segregation in primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 15: Land EH Recent advances in retinex theory. Vis. Res. 26:7 21 Land EH, McCann JJ Lightness and retinex theory. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 61:1 11 Le Gros Entopic phemonena. In Le Grand, Yves, 1908 Form and Space Vision. Transl. M Millodot, GG Heath, pp Bloomington: Indiana Univ. Press (From French) Lee TS, Nguyen M Dynamics of subjective contour formation in the early visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98: Lennie P, D Zmura M Mechanisms of color vision. Crit. Rev. Neurobiol. 3: Li CY, Li W Extensive integration field beyond the classical receptive field of cat s striate cortical neurons classification and tuning properties. Vis. Res. 34: Li W, Thier P, Wehrhahn C Contextual influence on orientation discrimination of humans and responses of neurons in V1 of alert monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 83: Li W, Thier P, Wehrhahn C Neuronal responses from beyond the classic receptive field in V1 of alert monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 139: Liden L, Pack C The role of terminators and occlusion cues in motion integration and segmentation: a neural network model. Vis. Res. 39: Liu Z, Kersten D D observers for human 3D object recognition? Vis. Res. 38: Livingstone MS, Hubel DH Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. J. Neurosci. 7: Locke J An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. London: Basset MacEvoy SP, Paradiso MA Lightness constancy in primary visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98: Mach E Contributions to the Analysis of Sensations. Trans. CM Williams, Chicago: Open Court Maffei L, Fiorentini A The unresponsive regions of visual cortical receptive fields. Vis. Res. 16: Mariotte E Nouvelle Découverte Touchant la Veve. Paris: Frederic Leonard. 27 pp. Martin KA, Whitteridge D The relationship of receptive field properties to the dendritic shape of neurones in the cat striate cortex. J. Physiol. 356: McIlwain JT Receptive fields of optic tract axons and lateral geniculate cells: peripheral extent and barbiturate sensitivity. J. Neurophysiol. 27: Michotte AE, Thinès GO, Crabbé G Les Compléments Amodaux des Structures Perceptives. Louvain: Inst. Psychol. Univ. Louvain. 56 pp. Mountcastle VB, LaMotte RH, Carli G Detection thresholds for stimuli in humans and monkeys: comparison with threshold events in mechanoreceptive afferent nerve fibers innervating the monkey hand. J. Neurophysiol. 35: Müller J Elements of Physiology. London: Printed for Taylor & Walton. 2 vols. Murakami I, Komatsu H, Kinoshita M Perceptual filling-in at the scotoma following a monocular retinal lesion in the monkey. Vis. Neurosci. 14: Nakayama K, Shimojo S Toward a neural

39 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 377 understanding of visual surface representation. Cold Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. LV: Nakayama K, Shimojo S, Silverman GH Stereoscopic depth: its relation to image segmentation, grouping, and the recognition of occluded objects. Perception 18:55 68 Nelson JI, Frost BJ Orientation-selective inhibition from beyond the classic visual receptive field. Brain Res. 139: Nelson JI, Frost BJ Intracortical facilitation among co-oriented, co-axially aligned simple cells in cat striate cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 61:54 61 Newsome WT, Britten KH, Movshon JA Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature 341:52 54 O Regan JK Solving the real mysteries of visual perception: the world as an outside memory. Can. J. Psychol. 46: Paradiso MA, Hahn S Filling-in percepts produced by luminance modulation. Vision Res. 36: Paradiso MA, Nakayama K Brightness perception and filling-in. Vis. Res. 31: Peterhans E, von der Heydt R Mechanisms of contour perception in monkey visual cortex. II. Contours bridging gaps. J. Neurosci. 9: Peterson MA On figures, grounds, and varieties of surface completion. In The 31st Carnegie Symposium: Perceptual Organization in Vision: Behavioral and Neural Perspectives, ed. M Behrmann, R Kimchi, C Olson. Mahwah, NJ: Earlbaum. In press Pettet MW, Gilbert CD Dynamic changes in receptive-field size in cat primary visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 89: Polat U, Mizobe K, Pettet MW, Kasamatsu T, Norcia AM Collinear stimuli regulate visual responses depending on cell s contrast threshold. Nature 391: Polat U, Sagi D Lateral interactions between spatial channels: suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments. Vis. Res. 33: Polat U, Sagi D The architecture of perceptual spatial interactions. Vis. Res. 34:73 78 Purves D, Shimpi A, Lotto RB An empirical explanation of the cornsweet effect. J. Neurosci. 19: Ramachandran VS Blind spots. Sci. Am. 266:86 91 Ramachandran VS Phantom limbs, neglect syndromes, repressed memories, and Freudian psychology. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 37: Ramachandran VS, Gregory RL Perceptual filling in of artificially induced scotomas in human vision. Lett. Nat. 350: Ramsden BM, Hung CP, Roe AW Real and illusory contour processing in area V1 of the primate: a cortical balancing act. Cereb. Cortex 11: Redies C, Crook JM, Creutzfeldt OD Neuronal responses to borders with and without luminance gradients in cat visual cortex and dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. Exp. Brain Res. 61: Rockland KS, Lund JS Widespread periodic intrinsic connections in the tree shrew visual cortex. Science 215: Rossi AF, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex of macaques. J. Neurosci. 21: Rossi AF, Paradiso MA Temporal limits of brightness induction and mechanisms of brightness perception. Vis. Res. 36: Rossi AF, Paradiso MA Neural correlates of perceived brightness in the retina, lateral geniculate nucleus, and striate cortex. J. Neurosci. 19: Rossi AF, Rittenhouse CD, Paradiso MA The representation of brightness in primary visual cortex. Science 273: Rubin E Visuell Wahrgenommene Figuren; Studien in Psychologischer Analyse. Kobenhavn: Gyldendalske Boghandel Schein SJ, Desimone R Spectral properties of V4 neurons in the macaque. J. Neurosci. 10: Schmidt KE, Kim DS, Singer W, Bonhoeffer T, Lowel S Functional specificity

40 378 ALBRIGHT STONER of long-range intrinsic and interhemispheric connections in the visual cortex of strabismic cats. J. Neurosci. 17: Sheth BR, Sharma J, Rao SC, Sur M Orientation maps of subjective contours in visual cortex. Science 274: Shevell SK The dual role of chromatic backgrounds in color perception. Vis. Res. 18: Shevell SK, Wei J Chromatic induction: border contrast or adaptation to surrounding light? Vis. Res. 38: Shimojo S, Silverman GH, Nakayama K Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion. Vis. Res. 29: Sillito AM, Grieve KL, Jones HE, Cudeiro J, Davis J Visual cortical mechanisms detecting focal orientation discontinuities. Nature 378: Stanley GB, Li FF, Dan Y Reconstruction of natural scenes from ensemble responses in the lateral geniculate nucleus. J. Neurosci. 19: Stoner GR, Albright TD Image segmentation cues in motion processing: implications for modularity in vision. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5: Stoner GR, Albright TD Depth from occlusion and non-fourier motion. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 21:511 Stoner GR, Duncan RO, Albright TD Area MT responses to contrast-modulated stimuli may encode depth-from-dynamic occlusion. Invest. Opthalmol. Vis. Sci. 39:S904 Sugita Y Grouping of image fragments in primary visual cortex. Nature 401: Tanaka K, Hikosaka K, Saito H, Yukie M, Fukada Y, Iwai E Analysis of local and wide-field movements in the superior temporal visual areas of the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 6: Tong F, Engel SA Interocular rivalry revealed in the human cortical blind-spot representation. Nature 411: Vinje WE, Gallant JL Sparse coding and decorrelation in primary visual cortex during natural vision. Science 287: von der Heydt R, Peterhans E, Baumgartner G Illusory contours and cortical neuron responses. Science 224: von Helmholtz H (1860/1924). Treatise on Psychological Optics, trans. JPC Southall. Rochester, NY: Opt. Soc. Am. Wachtler T, Albright TD, Sejnowski TJ Nonlocal interactions in color perception: nonlinear processing of chromatic signals from remote inducers. Vis. Res. 41: Wachtler T, Sejnowski TJ, Albright TD Responses of cells in macaque V1 to chromatic stimuli are compatible with human color constancy. Soc. Neurosci. Abs. 25(S7.8):4 Wallach H Uber visuell wahrgenommene Bewegungsrichtung. Psychol. Forsch. 20: Wallach H Brightness constancy and the nature of achromatic colors. J. Exp. Psychol. 38: Walls G The filling-in process. Am. J. Optom. 31: Wertheimer M (1912). Classics in Psychology, ed. T Shipley, pp New York: Phil. Library Wundt WM Principles of Physiological Psychology. Trans. EB Titchener. New York: Macmillan (from German) Xiao DK, Marcar VL, Raiguel SE, Orban GA Selectivity of macaque MT/V5 neurons for surface orientation in depth specified by motion. Eur. J. Neurosci. 9: Young T A Course of Lectures on Natural Philosophy and the Mechanical Arts, 2 vols. London: Johnson Zeki S. 1983a. Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: the reaction of cells in monkey visual cortex to wavelengths and colours. Neuroscience 9: Zeki S. 1983b. Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: the responses of wavelength-selective and colour-coded cells in monkey visual cortex to changes in wavelength composition. Neuroscience 9: Zeki S Colour vision and functional specialisation in the visual cortex. Discuss. Neurosci. 6:11 64

41 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS ON VISION 379 Zenger B, Sagi D Isolating excitatory and inhibitory nonlinear spatial interactions involved in contrast detection. Vis. Res. 36: Zhou H, Friedman HS, von der Heydt R Coding of border ownership in monkey visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 20: Zipser K, Lamme VA, Schiller PH Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 16:

42 Figure 3 Organization of visual cortex in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta), which has been the primary subject in studies of the neuronal substrates of contextual influences on visual perception. (A) Lateral view of the monkey brain, which illustrates the locations of some of the known visual areas. Primary visual cortex (area V1) is located on the occipital pole (left); extrastriate cortical visual areas extend anteriorally (rightward) and are labeled by their commonly used abbreviations. Indicated borders of visual areas (dashed lines) are approximate. Some sulci have been partially opened ( yellow regions) to show visual areas that lie buried within these sulci. PO, parieto-occipital sulcus; ST, superior temporal sulcus. (B) Visual areas are hierarchically organized. The image illustrates some of the anatomical connections known to exist from the retina through visual cortex. Except where indicated by arrows, anatomical connections are known to be reciprocal. RGC, retinal ganglion cell layer; LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus; M, magnocellular subdivisions; P 1 and P 2, parvocellular subdivisions; MT, middle temporal; MST, medial superior temporal; FST, fundus superior temporal; PP, posterior parietal cortex; VIP, ventral intraparietal; STP, superior temporal polysensory.

43 Figure 13 Contextual influences on color appearance. (A) The colors of the two central squares are identical, but the one on the right appears greener than the one on the left. This difference in appearance of the central squares is caused by the physical difference between the larger background contexts. This illusion, which has been known for centuries, is caused by the contrast difference between the two image regions and is a product of a mechanism designed to recover the reflectance of surfaces. Neurons in area V1 exhibit context-induced shifts in their sensitivity to receptive field stimuli, which mirror changes in color appearance (Rossi et al. 1996, Wachtler et al. 1999, MacEvoy & Paradiso 2001). (B) Mondrian stimulus used to examine influence of context on perceptual and neuronal sensitivity to color (Land & McCann 1971, Zeki 1983a).

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing C Contextual Influences in Visual Processing TAI SING LEE Computer Science Department and Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Synonyms Surround influence;

More information

What is mid level vision? Mid Level Vision. What is mid level vision? Lightness perception as revealed by lightness illusions

What is mid level vision? Mid Level Vision. What is mid level vision? Lightness perception as revealed by lightness illusions What is mid level vision? Mid Level Vision March 18, 2004 Josh McDermott Perception involves inferring the structure of the world from measurements of energy generated by the world (in vision, this is

More information

Principals of Object Perception

Principals of Object Perception Principals of Object Perception Elizabeth S. Spelke COGNITIVE SCIENCE 14, 29-56 (1990) Cornell University Summary Infants perceive object by analyzing tree-dimensional surface arrangements and motions.

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Javier R. Movellan Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0515 movellan@inc.ucsd.edu Thomas

More information

Psychology of visual perception C O M M U N I C A T I O N D E S I G N, A N I M A T E D I M A G E 2014/2015

Psychology of visual perception C O M M U N I C A T I O N D E S I G N, A N I M A T E D I M A G E 2014/2015 Psychology of visual perception C O M M U N I C A T I O N D E S I G N, A N I M A T E D I M A G E 2014/2015 EXTENDED SUMMARY Lesson #4: Oct. 13 th 2014 Lecture plan: GESTALT PSYCHOLOGY Nature and fundamental

More information

Perceptual Grouping: It s Later Than You Think. Stephen E. Palmer 1

Perceptual Grouping: It s Later Than You Think. Stephen E. Palmer 1 Perceptual Grouping: It s Later Than You Think Stephen E. Palmer 1 Psychology Department, University of California, Berkeley, California Abstract Recent research on perceptual grouping is described with

More information

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1 Announcements exam 1 this Thursday! review session: Wednesday, 5:00-6:30pm, Meliora 203 Bryce s office hours: Wednesday, 3:30-5:30pm, Gleason https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zdw7pvgz0um M Cells M cells

More information

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes The Puzzle of Object and Scene Perception The stimulus on the receptors is ambiguous. Inverse projection problem: An image on the retina can be caused by an infinite

More information

Attention and Blind-Spot Phenomenology

Attention and Blind-Spot Phenomenology Attention and Blind-Spot Phenomenology Liang Lou & Jing Chen Department of Psychology Grand Valley State University Allendale, Michigan 49401 U.S.A. loul@gvsu.edu chenj@gvsu.edu Copyright (c) Liang Lou

More information

Vision Seeing is in the mind

Vision Seeing is in the mind 1 Vision Seeing is in the mind Stimulus: Light 2 Light Characteristics 1. Wavelength (hue) 2. Intensity (brightness) 3. Saturation (purity) 3 4 Hue (color): dimension of color determined by wavelength

More information

Perception Outline Chapter 6, Psychology, David G Meyers, 7 th Edition

Perception Outline Chapter 6, Psychology, David G Meyers, 7 th Edition Perception Outline Chapter 6, Psychology, David G Meyers, 7 th Edition By transforming sensation into perception we create meaning - Selective Attention - Perception comes to us moment by moment 1. Selective

More information

Lesson 5 Sensation, Perception, Memory, and The Conscious Mind

Lesson 5 Sensation, Perception, Memory, and The Conscious Mind Lesson 5 Sensation, Perception, Memory, and The Conscious Mind Introduction: Connecting Your Learning The beginning of Bloom's lecture concludes his discussion of language development in humans and non-humans

More information

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013 Definition Slides Sensation = the process by which our sensory receptors and nervous system receive and represent stimulus energies from our environment. Perception = the process of organizing and interpreting

More information

= add definition here. Definition Slide

= add definition here. Definition Slide = add definition here Definition Slide Definition Slides Sensation = the process by which our sensory receptors and nervous system receive and represent stimulus energies from our environment. Perception

More information

Consciousness The final frontier!

Consciousness The final frontier! Consciousness The final frontier! How to Define it??? awareness perception - automatic and controlled memory - implicit and explicit ability to tell us about experiencing it attention. And the bottleneck

More information

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück The Binding Problem This lecture is based on following articles: Adina L. Roskies: The Binding Problem; Neuron 1999 24: 7 Charles M. Gray: The Temporal Correlation Hypothesis of Visual Feature Integration:

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

Adventures into terra incognita

Adventures into terra incognita BEWARE: These are preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. Chapter VI. Adventures into terra incognita In primary visual cortex there are neurons

More information

Pre-Attentive Visual Selection

Pre-Attentive Visual Selection Pre-Attentive Visual Selection Li Zhaoping a, Peter Dayan b a University College London, Dept. of Psychology, UK b University College London, Gatsby Computational Neuroscience Unit, UK Correspondence to

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

The Standard Theory of Conscious Perception

The Standard Theory of Conscious Perception The Standard Theory of Conscious Perception C. D. Jennings Department of Philosophy Boston University Pacific APA 2012 Outline 1 Introduction Motivation Background 2 Setting up the Problem Working Definitions

More information

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization THE VISUAL SYSTEM Higher Visual Processing Martin Paré Assistant Professor Physiology & Psychology Most of the cortex that covers the cerebral hemispheres is

More information

GESTALT PSYCHOLOGY AND

GESTALT PSYCHOLOGY AND GESTALT PSYCHOLOGY AND CONTEMPORARY VISION SCIENCE: PROBLEMS, CHALLENGES, PROSPECTS JOHAN WAGEMANS LABORATORY OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF LEUVEN, BELGIUM GTA 2013, KARLSRUHE A century of Gestalt

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

Dynamics and Modeling in Cognitive Science - I

Dynamics and Modeling in Cognitive Science - I Dynamics and Modeling in Cognitive Science - I Narayanan Srinivasan Centre of Behavioural and Cognitive Sciences University of Allahabad, India Outline General introduction to Cognitive Science Problem

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception Competing Frameworks in Perception Lesson II: Perception module 08 Perception.08. 1 Views on perception Perception as a cascade of information processing stages From sensation to percept Template vs. feature

More information

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception Competing Frameworks in Perception Lesson II: Perception module 08 Perception.08. 1 Views on perception Perception as a cascade of information processing stages From sensation to percept Template vs. feature

More information

SEMINAR IN COGNITION Object and surface perception Fall 2001

SEMINAR IN COGNITION Object and surface perception Fall 2001 SEMINAR IN COGNITION Object and surface perception Fall 2001 Course: Psych 637 (16: 830: 637) Time : W 2:50 5:30 Code : 35020 Place : Psy-301, Busch Instructor : Manish Singh Office Hours: Office : 119

More information

Object vision (Chapter 4)

Object vision (Chapter 4) Object vision (Chapter 4) Lecture 8 Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Spring 2015 1 Outline for today: Chap 3: adaptation Chap 4: intro to object vision gestalt

More information

Visual Perception 6. Daniel Chandler. The innocent eye is blind and the virgin mind empty. - Nelson Goodman. Gestalt Principles of Visual Organization

Visual Perception 6. Daniel Chandler. The innocent eye is blind and the virgin mind empty. - Nelson Goodman. Gestalt Principles of Visual Organization Visual Perception 6 Daniel Chandler The innocent eye is blind and the virgin mind empty. - Nelson Goodman Gestalt Principles of Visual Organization In discussing the 'selectivity' of perception I have

More information

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get IAT 355 Perception 1 Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get Why we need to understand perception The ability of viewers to interpret visual (graphical) encodings of information and

More information

Computer Vision. Gestalt Theory. Gestaltism. Gestaltism. Computer Science Tripos Part II. Dr Christopher Town. Principles of Gestalt Theory

Computer Vision. Gestalt Theory. Gestaltism. Gestaltism. Computer Science Tripos Part II. Dr Christopher Town. Principles of Gestalt Theory A B C Computer Vision Computer Science Tripos Part II Dr Christopher Town A B Gestalt Theory D C Gestalt: a meaningful whole or group Whole is greater than the sum of its parts Relationships among parts

More information

Review #6 ( )

Review #6 (  ) Review #6 ( http://www.appsychology.net ) ( Reproduced with Permission from Ben McIlwain [Author] ) Questions 1. You typically fail to consciously perceive that your own nose is in your line of vision.

More information

Sensation vs. Perception

Sensation vs. Perception PERCEPTION Sensation vs. Perception What s the difference? Sensation what the senses do Perception process of recognizing, organizing and dinterpreting ti information. What is Sensation? The process whereby

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2007

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2007 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2007 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect, which you ll acknowledge when you turn in your exam. Each multiple-choice question is worth 1 point and the value of the other

More information

Amodal Completion in Visual Search Preemption or Context Effects?

Amodal Completion in Visual Search Preemption or Context Effects? PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Amodal Completion in Visual Search Preemption or Context Effects? Robert Rauschenberger, 1 Mary A. Peterson, 1 Fauzia Mosca, 2,3 and Nicola Bruno 3 1 University of

More information

Sensation and Perception

Sensation and Perception Sensation and Perception 1 Chapters 4 of the required textbook Introduction to Psychology International Edition bv James Kalat (2010) 9 th Edition EXPECTED LEARNING OUTCOMES After studying this chapter,

More information

Sensation and Perception

Sensation and Perception 1 Sensation and Perception DR. ARNEL BANAGA SALGADO, Doctor of Psychology (USA) FPM (Ph.D.) Psychology (India) Doctor of Education (Phl) Master of Arts in Nursing (Phl) Master of Arts in Teaching Psychology

More information

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J. The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem By Andrew Laguna, S.J. Outline I. Introduction II. The Visual System III. What is the Binding Problem? IV. Possible Theoretical Solutions

More information

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Frank Tong Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Office: Room 3-N-2B Telephone: 609-258-2652 Fax: 609-258-1113 Email: ftong@princeton.edu Graduate School Applicants

More information

Visual Perception. Agenda. Visual perception. CS Information Visualization January 20, 2011 John Stasko. Pre-attentive processing Color Etc.

Visual Perception. Agenda. Visual perception. CS Information Visualization January 20, 2011 John Stasko. Pre-attentive processing Color Etc. Topic Notes Visual Perception CS 7450 - Information Visualization January 20, 2011 John Stasko Agenda Visual perception Pre-attentive processing Color Etc. Spring 2011 CS 7450 2 1 Semiotics The study of

More information

Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions. Arthur Shapiro and Dejan Todorovic, Editors. Chapter xx. Illusory Color Spread from Apparent Motion

Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions. Arthur Shapiro and Dejan Todorovic, Editors. Chapter xx. Illusory Color Spread from Apparent Motion Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions Arthur Shapiro and Dejan Todorovic, Editors Chapter xx Illusory Color Spread from Apparent Motion Carol M. Cicerone 1, Professor Emeritus, and Donald D. Hoffman, Professor

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features Dikran J. Martin Psychology 110 Name: Date: Lecture Series: Chapter 3 Sensation and Perception: Pages: 31 Making Contact with the World around Us TEXT: Baron, Robert A. (2001). Psychology (Fifth Edition).

More information

PSYC 441 Cognitive Psychology II

PSYC 441 Cognitive Psychology II PSYC 441 Cognitive Psychology II Session 5 Theories of Perception Lecturer: Dr. Benjamin Amponsah, Dept., of Psychology, UG, Legon Contact Information: bamponsah@ug.edu.gh College of Education School of

More information

Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory

Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory 1 Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory Imagine that you are watching a game of tennis on television and someone accidentally mutes the sound. You will probably notice that following the game becomes

More information

Understanding Users. - cognitive processes. Unit 3

Understanding Users. - cognitive processes. Unit 3 Understanding Users - cognitive processes Unit 3 Why do we need to understand users? Interacting with technology involves a number of cognitive processes We need to take into account Characteristic & limitations

More information

On the Time of Peripheral Sensations and Voluntary Motor Actions. Text

On the Time of Peripheral Sensations and Voluntary Motor Actions. Text On the Time of Peripheral Sensations and Voluntary Motor Actions DOUGLAS M. SNYDER ABSTRACT Libet's notions of backwards referral for peripheral sensations and unconscious cerebral initiative accompanying

More information

Perceptual Imagination and Perceptual Memory

Perceptual Imagination and Perceptual Memory 1 Perceptual Imagination and Perceptual Memory An Overview Fiona Macpherson The essays in this volume explore the nature of perceptual imagination and perceptual memory. How do perceptual imagination and

More information

A Direct Object of Perception

A Direct Object of Perception E-LOGOS Electronic Journal for Philosophy 2015, Vol. 22(1) 28 36 ISSN 1211-0442 (DOI 10.18267/j.e-logos.411),Peer-reviewed article Journal homepage: e-logos.vse.cz A Direct Object of Perception Mika Suojanen

More information

Sensation & Perception PSYC420 Thomas E. Van Cantfort, Ph.D.

Sensation & Perception PSYC420 Thomas E. Van Cantfort, Ph.D. Sensation & Perception PSYC420 Thomas E. Van Cantfort, Ph.D. Objects & Forms When we look out into the world we are able to see things as trees, cars, people, books, etc. A wide variety of objects and

More information

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes PSY382-Hande Kaynak, PhD 2/13/17 Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes 1 2 Figure 5-1 p96 3 Figure 5-2 p96 4 Figure 5-4 p97 1 Why Is It So Difficult to Design a Perceiving Machine? The stimulus on the

More information

Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection

Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection Vijay Laxmi, J. N. Carter and R. I. Damper Image, Speech and Intelligent Systems (ISIS) Research Group Department of Electronics and Computer Science University

More information

Computational Architectures in Biological Vision, USC, Spring 2001

Computational Architectures in Biological Vision, USC, Spring 2001 Computational Architectures in Biological Vision, USC, Spring 2001 Lecture 11: Visual Illusions. Reading Assignments: None 1 What Can Illusions Teach Us? They exacerbate the failure modes of our visual

More information

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Baran Çürüklü Department of Computer Science and Engineering Mälardalen University Västerås S-721 23, Sweden Abstract. In this paper

More information

2/3/17. Visual System I. I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex

2/3/17. Visual System I. I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex 1 Visual System I I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex 2 1 2/3/17 Window of the Soul 3 Information Flow: From Photoreceptors

More information

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Selective Attention Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form,

More information

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3 Perception Chapter 8, Section 3 Principles of Perceptual Organization The perception process helps us to comprehend the confusion of the stimuli bombarding our senses Our brain takes the bits and pieces

More information

Asymmetries in ecological and sensorimotor laws: towards a theory of subjective experience. James J. Clark

Asymmetries in ecological and sensorimotor laws: towards a theory of subjective experience. James J. Clark Asymmetries in ecological and sensorimotor laws: towards a theory of subjective experience James J. Clark Centre for Intelligent Machines McGill University This talk will motivate an ecological approach

More information

Note:- Receptors are the person who receives any images from outer environment.

Note:- Receptors are the person who receives any images from outer environment. Concept According to Oxford Advanced Learner s Dictionary, Perception means the way you notice things especially with the senses. Perception is the process of organizing and attempting to understand the

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Shaw - PSYC& 100 Lilienfeld et al (2014) - Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception: How we sense and conceptualize the world

Shaw - PSYC& 100 Lilienfeld et al (2014) - Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception: How we sense and conceptualize the world Name: 1 Shaw - PSYC& 100 Lilienfeld et al (2014) - Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception: How we sense and conceptualize the world 1 Distinguish between sensation and perception. Include as part of your answer

More information

The Structuralist Approach

The Structuralist Approach The Structuralist Approach Approach established by Wundt (1830-1920) States that perceptions are created by combining elements called sensations Popular in mid to late 19 th century Wundt studied conscious

More information

Inner Vision: Seeing the Mind s Eye

Inner Vision: Seeing the Mind s Eye William James prize article : Seeing the Mind s Eye Abstract Does imagining a visual pattern involve the same mechanisms as actually seeing that pattern? If so, what are the functional consequences of

More information

On the implementation of Visual Attention Architectures

On the implementation of Visual Attention Architectures On the implementation of Visual Attention Architectures KONSTANTINOS RAPANTZIKOS AND NICOLAS TSAPATSOULIS DEPARTMENT OF ELECTRICAL AND COMPUTER ENGINEERING NATIONAL TECHNICAL UNIVERSITY OF ATHENS 9, IROON

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Today: Visual perception, leading to higher-level vision: object recognition, word perception.

Today: Visual perception, leading to higher-level vision: object recognition, word perception. 9.65 - Cognitive Processes - Spring 2004 MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences Course Instructor: Professor Mary C. Potter 9.65 February 9, 2004 Object recognition HANDOUT I. Why is object recognition

More information

Lecture 2.1 What is Perception?

Lecture 2.1 What is Perception? Lecture 2.1 What is Perception? A Central Ideas in Perception: Perception is more than the sum of sensory inputs. It involves active bottom-up and topdown processing. Perception is not a veridical representation

More information

Chapter 1. Abstraction

Chapter 1. Abstraction Chapter 1 Abstraction There are many functions that we can assign to the brain, and more specifically to the cerebral cortex, that thin sheet containing billions of cells that envelopes the cerebral hemispheres.

More information

Response profiles to texture border patterns in area V1

Response profiles to texture border patterns in area V1 Visual Neuroscience (2000), 17, 421 436. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2000 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238000 $12.50 Response profiles to texture border patterns in area V1 HANS-CHRISTOPH NOTHDURFT,

More information

1.1 FEATURES OF THOUGHT

1.1 FEATURES OF THOUGHT SEC 1 Page 1 of 7 1.1 FEATURES OF THOUGHT Thought can refer to the ideas or arrangements of ideas that result from thinking, the act of producing thoughts, or the process of producing thoughts. Despite

More information

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System Lecturer: Jitendra Malik Scribe: Ryan White (Slide: layout of the brain) Facts about the brain:

More information

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention Chapter 6 Attention Attention William James, in 1890, wrote Everyone knows what attention is. Attention is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2003

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2003 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2003 Read each question carefully and answer it completely. Pay careful attention to the point value of questions so that you allocate your time appropriately (1 point = 1 minute).

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Spatial Distribution of Contextual Interactions in Primary Visual Cortex and in Visual Perception

Spatial Distribution of Contextual Interactions in Primary Visual Cortex and in Visual Perception Spatial Distribution of Contextual Interactions in Primary Visual Cortex and in Visual Perception MITESH K. KAPADIA, 1,2 GERALD WESTHEIMER, 1 AND CHARLES D. GILBERT 1 1 The Rockefeller University, New

More information

THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES

THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES IN THE VISUAL CORTICAL HIERARCHY JOHAN WAGEMANS LABORATORY OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF LEUVEN, BELGIUM DIPARTIMENTO DI PSICOLOGIA, UNIVERSITÀ DI MILANO-BICOCCA,

More information

Local Image Structures and Optic Flow Estimation

Local Image Structures and Optic Flow Estimation Local Image Structures and Optic Flow Estimation Sinan KALKAN 1, Dirk Calow 2, Florentin Wörgötter 1, Markus Lappe 2 and Norbert Krüger 3 1 Computational Neuroscience, Uni. of Stirling, Scotland; {sinan,worgott}@cn.stir.ac.uk

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect, so keep your eyes foveated on your own exam. I tend to think of a point as a minute, so be sure to spend the appropriate amount

More information

The Perceptual Experience

The Perceptual Experience Dikran J. Martin Introduction to Psychology Name: Date: Lecture Series: Chapter 5 Sensation and Perception Pages: 35 TEXT: Lefton, Lester A. and Brannon, Linda (2003). PSYCHOLOGY. (Eighth Edition.) Needham

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect. Read each question carefully and answer it completely. Multiple-choice questions are worth one point each, other questions

More information

Perceptual Organization (II)

Perceptual Organization (II) (II) Introduction to Computational and Biological Vision CS 202-1-5261 Computer Science Department, BGU Ohad Ben-Shahar Why do things look they way they do? [Koffka 1935] External (Environment) vs. Internal

More information

Selectivity and Spatial Distribution of Signals From the Receptive Field Surround in Macaque V1 Neurons

Selectivity and Spatial Distribution of Signals From the Receptive Field Surround in Macaque V1 Neurons J Neurophysiol 88: 2547 2556, 2002; 10.1152/jn.00693.2001. Selectivity and Spatial Distribution of Signals From the Receptive Field Surround in Macaque V1 Neurons JAMES R. CAVANAUGH, 2 WYETH BAIR, 1,2

More information

9.65 Sept. 12, 2001 Object recognition HANDOUT with additions in Section IV.b for parts of lecture that were omitted.

9.65 Sept. 12, 2001 Object recognition HANDOUT with additions in Section IV.b for parts of lecture that were omitted. 9.65 Sept. 12, 2001 Object recognition HANDOUT with additions in Section IV.b for parts of lecture that were omitted. I. Why is visual perception difficult? II. Basics of visual perception A. Gestalt principles,

More information

Visual Perception. Agenda. Visual perception. CS Information Visualization August 26, 2013 John Stasko. Pre-attentive processing Color Etc.

Visual Perception. Agenda. Visual perception. CS Information Visualization August 26, 2013 John Stasko. Pre-attentive processing Color Etc. Topic Notes Visual Perception CS 7450 - Information Visualization August 26, 2013 John Stasko Agenda Visual perception Pre-attentive processing Color Etc. Fall 2013 CS 7450 2 1 Semiotics The study of symbols

More information

Stimulus any aspect of or change in the environment to which an organism responds. Sensation what occurs when a stimulus activates a receptor

Stimulus any aspect of or change in the environment to which an organism responds. Sensation what occurs when a stimulus activates a receptor Chapter 8 Sensation and Perception Sec 1: Sensation Stimulus any aspect of or change in the environment to which an organism responds Sensation what occurs when a stimulus activates a receptor Perception

More information

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice Multiple Choice 1. Which structure is not part of the visual pathway in the brain? a. occipital lobe b. optic chiasm c. lateral geniculate nucleus *d. frontal lobe Answer location: Visual Pathways 2. Which

More information

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye: Problems confronting the visual system: Visual Physiology image contains a huge amount of information which must be processed quickly. image is dim, blurry and distorted. Light levels vary enormously.

More information

Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence. Edwin Blake

Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence. Edwin Blake Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence Edwin Blake edwin@cs.uct.ac.za This Talk 2 Perception Bottom-up Top-down Integration Presence Bottom-up Top-down BIPs Presence arises from

More information

Perceptual Grouping in a Self-Organizing Map of Spiking Neurons

Perceptual Grouping in a Self-Organizing Map of Spiking Neurons Perceptual Grouping in a Self-Organizing Map of Spiking Neurons Yoonsuck Choe Department of Computer Sciences The University of Texas at Austin August 13, 2001 Perceptual Grouping Group Two! Longest Contour?

More information

Object recognition and hierarchical computation

Object recognition and hierarchical computation Object recognition and hierarchical computation Challenges in object recognition. Fukushima s Neocognitron View-based representations of objects Poggio s HMAX Forward and Feedback in visual hierarchy Hierarchical

More information

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2 Name: ID # ID: A Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2 Multiple Choice (38 questions, 1 point each) Identify the letter of the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

More information

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth Journal of Vision (2010) 10(4):1, 1 27 http://journalofvision.org/10/4/1/ 1 Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth Anna Seydell David C. Knill Julia Trommershäuser Department

More information

B.A. II Psychology - Paper A. Form Perception. Dr. Neelam Rathee. Department of Psychology G.C.G.-11, Chandigarh

B.A. II Psychology - Paper A. Form Perception. Dr. Neelam Rathee. Department of Psychology G.C.G.-11, Chandigarh B.A. II Psychology - Paper A Form Perception Dr. Neelam Rathee Department of Psychology G.C.G.-11, Chandigarh Form Perception What it is? How do we recognize an object? (form perception) 2 Perception of

More information

Object Perception Perceiving and Recognizing Objects

Object Perception Perceiving and Recognizing Objects Object Perception Perceiving and Recognizing Objects Extrastriate cortex Outside V1 in occipital lobe Dorsal pathway Ventral pathway Modular organization of visual areas associated with object recognition

More information