Autism is one of the most prevalent developmental

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Autism is one of the most prevalent developmental"

Transcription

1 JSLHR Research Article Language and False-Belief Task Performance in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder M. Jeffrey Farrar, a Hye Kyeung Seung, b and Hyeonjin Lee c Purpose: Language is related to false-belief (FB) understanding in both typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The current study examined the role of complementation and general language in FB understanding. Of interest was whether language plays similar or different roles in the groups FB performance. Method: Participants were 16 typically developing children (mean age = 5.0 years; mental age = 6.7) and 18 with ASD (mean age = 7.3 years; mental age = 8.3). Children were administered FB and language tasks (say- andthink-complements), receptive and expressive vocabulary tests, and relative clauses. Results: When mental age and receptive and expressive vocabulary were used as separate covariates, the typical control group outperformed the children with ASD in FB task performance. Chi-square analyses indicated that passing both complementation tasks was linked to the FB understanding of children with ASD. Children with ASD who passed FB tasks all passed say- and thinkcomplement tasks. However, some children in the control group were able to pass the FB tasks, even if they failed the say- and think-complement tasks. Conclusion: The results indicate that children with ASD relied more on complement understanding to pass FB than typically developing children. Results are discussed regarding the developmental pathways for FB understanding. Autism is one of the most prevalent developmental disorders. Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) present deficits in two domains: deficits in social communication, and repetitive patterns of behaviors/ restricted interests and activities (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Children with ASD who are high functioning (i.e., have near average cognitive level) experience social deficits, language delay, language pragmatic deficits, and executive function difficulties (Gernsbacher, Morson, & Grace, 2015; Rapin & Dunn, 2003; Tager-Flusberg, 2007). These social and pragmatic difficulties often have been attributed to theory of mind deficits in children with ASD (Baron-Cohen, 1995; Happé, 1993). It has long been recognized that theory of mind consists of many components, and children with ASD do not always show difficulty in all aspects of it (Broekhof et al., 2015). Furthermore, a Psychology Department, University of Florida, Gainesville b Department of Human Communication Studies, California State University, Fullerton c Department of Early Childhood Education, Yeungnam University, North Gyeongsang, Korea Correspondence to M. Jeffrey Farrar: farrar@ufl.edu Editor: Sean Redmond Associate Editor: Patricia Eadie Received December 8, 2015 Revision received May 6, 2016 Accepted January 11, limitations in theory of mind development do not account for all characteristics of ASD (Tager-Flusberg, 2007). For instance, pragmatic language difficulties may be more related to basic language skills, such as syntax, rather than to theory of mind (e.g., Gernsbacher & Pripas-Kapit, 2012; Whyte, Nelson, & Shcerf, 2014). Despite the expansion of the explanations of ASD and some of its characteristics, children with autism often struggle in different theory of mind tasks (e.g., Hutchins et al., 2016). The current study explored how language contributes to theory of mind development. The role of language in facilitating theory of mind development, particularly falsebelief (FB) understanding, has generated considerable debate about both children with ASD and typically developing children (J. G. de Villiers & de Villiers, 2014; Durrleman et al., 2016; San Juan & Astington, 2012). In both groups, language is related to FB performance. However, there is disagreement regarding whether general language or sentential complements is related to FB understanding and whether this relationship varies across populations. Theory of Mind Theory of mind has been extensively examined in typically developing children (S. A. Miller, 2015; Wellman, 2014). An important achievement in its development is FB Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time of publication. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol July 2017 Copyright 2017 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 1999

2 understanding, the recognition that different people can have conflicting beliefs about the same situation, such as the content of a box. For instance, in the unexpected content task, children are shown a familiar crayon box and asked about its content. After predicting that there are crayons inside, the child is shown that there are actually pennies inside. After putting the pennies back, children are asked what a puppet will think is in the box even though he has never looked inside. If children understand FBs, they will predict that she will think there are crayons in the box, even though they know there are pennies. Typically developing children pass FB tasks by approximately 4.5 to 5.0 years of age (Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2001). In contrast, certain clinical populations, such as children with specific language impairment, ASD, and hearing impairment, are delayed in FB reasoning (P. A. de Villiers, 2005; Farrar et al., 2009; Schick, de Villiers, de Villiers, & Hoffmeister, 2007; Siegal & Peterson, 2008; Tager-Flusberg, 2007). A variety of explanations have been offered for the development of FB understanding, including a specialized theory of mind module (Baron-Cohen, 1995), executive function (Devine & Hughes, 2014; Tager- Flusberg, 2007), and language (Milligan, Astington, & Dack, 2007). Language and False-Belief Several different explanations regarding the influence of language on FB reasoning have been proposed. P. A. de Villiers (2005) views the development of sentential complement structures as necessary for representing a FB. Mental state verbs (e.g., think, believe) contain a proposition that builds into a mental representation an embedded clause structure (e.g., John believes that it is a dog, but it is a wolf. ). This complementation structure allows a split between what John believes and the reality of the situation, which is the essence of FB comprehension. A memory for complements task is typically used to assess mastery of complements using both mental state verbs (e.g., think) and communication verbs (told, say; e.g., J. G. de Villiers & Pyers, 2002). For example, children are shown a picture and provided a verbal description, such as She thought/said it was a bug in her hair and are then shown a second picture with the description but it was really a leaf. The child is then asked: What did she think/ say was in her hair? The use of communication verbs (e.g., say, tell), instead of mental state verbs (e.g., think) in the memory for complements task is important for testing the role of complement structures in FB. If mental state verbs are used, the task reflects both semantic (meaning of think) and syntactic structure components, making it difficult to separate the role of complement semantics from complement syntax per se. The use of communication verbs tests theroleofcomplementsyntaxseparatefrommeaning. Furthermore, say-complements develop earlier than thinkcomplements and may be a precursor to FB reasoning (P. A. de Villiers, 2005; Perner, Zauner, & Sprung, 2005). They also provide a more stringent test of the role of complementation because memory for false complements involving mental state verbs may actually reflect an understanding of FB (Tardif, So, & Kaciroti, 2007). Numerous studies have supported the importance of complementation in FB reasoning for typically developing children. In a longitudinal study of 28 typically developing children (age 3 5 years), the children s performance on a memory for complements task predicted the children s later FB more so than other language measures (J. G. de Villiers & Pyers, 2002; Low, 2010). Other researchers, however, have suggested that complementation is not required but rather that general language (semantic, syntax) is sufficient (Slade & Ruffman, 2005). These studies have demonstrated that general language using comprehensive standardized assessments (e.g., Test of Early Language Development [Hresko, Reid, & Hammill, 1999]; Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals [Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 1992]; Astington & Jenkins, 1999), mean length of utterance as a morphosyntactic measure (J. Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991), Index of Productive Syntax (IPSYN; Scarborough, 1990), and receptive/ expressive vocabulary (Ng, Cheung, & Xiao, 2010; Slade & Ruffman, 2005) were related to FB performance, but complementation played no unique role or was not examined. In a similar way, social constructivist approaches to FB argue that children s participation in linguistically mediated social interactions, involving mental state talk, facilitates FB understanding by highlighting different perspectives on situations (Nelson, 2005; Ruffman, 2014). Thus, there is clear support for the role of language in typically developing children, although there is continuing debate regarding what aspects of language are necessary (Astington & Baird, 2005; Slade & Ruffman, 2005). A meta-analysis of 107 studies of typically developing children reported that all language measures, vocabulary, syntax, and complementation were significantly associated with FB, with the largest effect for complementation. However, only four of the studies had included complementation measures (Milligan et al., 2007). Language is also related to FB in clinical populations. Studies of verbal children with ASD (Fisher, Happé, & Dunn, 2005; Hale & Tager-Flusberg, 2005; Peterson, Wellman, & Liu, 2005), specific language impairment (Farrant, Fletcher, & Mayberry, 2006; C. Miller, 2006), and who are deaf and have hearing parents (Schick et al., 2007) all show FB delays that are related to their language ability. As with typically developing children, the issue of whether complementation or general language accounts for FB understanding in children with ASD has been a focus of interest, although the number of studies is limited. Of particular interest are studies that examined the impact of both general language and complementation in the same study. Tager-Flusberg (2000) compared the roles of receptive vocabulary, as assessed by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Fourth Edition (PPVT-4; L. M. Dunn & Dunn, 2007), complementation structures involving communication verbs (e.g., say), and those involving mental state verbs (e.g., think) in predicting FB for children with ASD 2000 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol July 2017

3 or intellectual disability. For children with ASD, complementation involving communication verbs was the only predictor of FB. For the children withanintellectualdisability, complementation involving mental verbs (e.g., think) was the only predictor of FB. Lind and Bowler (2009) compared children with ASD, children with a learning disability, and typically developing children. For children with ASD, complement syntax of say and told predicted performance on the unexpected locations FB task after controlling for vocabulary, but not the unexpected contents task. Complement syntax was not related to FB performance in the comparison control groups. For these children, general vocabulary was sufficient. Complementation is related to FB in children with ASD who speak other languages as well. For instance, mental verb (e.g., think) complementation played a significant role in FB task performance in Korean-speaking children with ASD (Lee et al., 2009). Complementation of both communication and cognition verbs was uniquely related to FB understanding in a sample of 17 French-speaking children and adolescents with ASD on verbal FB tasks but not on nonverbal ones (Durrleman & Franck, 2015). Performance on FB tasks in the ASD group was also significantly lower than that in the control group, but not on nonverbal FB and control tasks. However, a subsequent study did report an effect of complementation on nonverbal FB reasoning (Durrleman et al., 2016). In the only longitudinal study of children with ASD, Tager-Flusberg and Joseph (2005) included both complementation and general language measures (IPSYN) as predictors of FB. Age and IPSYN at Time 1 were the strongest predictors of FB at Time 2. Complement understanding involving communication verbs accounted for a unique additional variance. Thus, across studies there is some inconsistency in which aspect of language is associated with FB in terms of the type of FB task, verbs used, and populations studied (e.g., Cheung, 2006; Low, 2010). The general pattern for children with ASD is that sentential complement syntax may play a necessary role in FB, with some studies finding that only say-complements are linked to FB performance (Tager-Flusberg, 2007) whereas others find that both sayand think-complementation are related (Durrelmann & Franck, 2015). The purpose of the current study was to further examine the role of language in FB understanding in children with ASD. Of particular interest was whether language affects FB development differently in children with ASD than in typically developing children. There is some evidence that complementation is critical to FB understanding in children with ASD (Durrleman & Franck, 2015; Lind & Bowler, 2009; Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, 2005). The present study used multiple language measures to assess current language levels. FB performance was compared between ASD and control groups on two types of FB tasks (change of location and change of content tasks) because some studies have demonstrated different effects depending on the FB task (Lind & Bowler, 2009). The study aimed to evaluate the contribution of a set of language variables (receptive vocabulary, expressive vocabulary, complementation, and parents perception of their child s overall syntax) on FB task performance. Furthermore, because children with ASD differ from typically developing children on several dimensions, such as mental age, it is important to either match or control for these factors. Two specific research questions were addressed: (a) What is the relationship between FB performance and language in children with ASD and in typically developing younger children? (b) Does general language and complementation differentially affect FB understanding in the two groups of children? Method Participants Flyers about the study were sent to agencies (regional center, psychology private practice clinics, speech-language clinics, workshops, special educators) in a large urban community. Informed consent forms were mailed to parents who were interested in having their children participate in the study. A total of 42 children were recruited: 24 children with high-functioning ASD (i.e., IQ standard scores above 70 and verbal communicators) between ages 4 and 11 years in the ASD group, and 18 typically developing children between ages 3 and 7 years in the control group. Participants in the ASD group were diagnosed by community professionals on the basis of DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). It should be noted that the data collection was completed before the release of the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The diagnosis of those in the ASD group was confirmed by having parents complete the Lifetime form of the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Rutter, Bailey, Berument, Lord, & Pickles, 2003) and a clinical observation by the second author, who has experience with ASD diagnosis as a certified and licensed speech-language pathologist. Bishop and Norbury (2002) reported a good agreement between total cutoff scores on the Autism Diagnostic Interview Revised (ADI-R; Le Couteur, Lord, & Rutter, 2003) and the SCQ. Several children in the control group were siblings of the children in another ASD study conducted by the second author. Consistent with the ASD population, the number of male participants in the ASD group outnumbered the female participants (15 boys and three girls). In the control group, the male participants also outnumbered the female participants (14 boys and two girls). Of the 24 recruits for the ASD group, six were excluded for various reasons. Three recruits did not meet an autism diagnosis on the basis of SCQ score and clinical observation, two had IQ scores lower than 70 and low vocabulary scores ( 2 SD on PPVT-4 and/or Expressive Vocabulary Test Second Edition (EVT-2; Williams, 2007), and one discontinued after vocabulary testing. Thus, the final sample size of the ASD group consisted of 18 children with the following heritages: three Asian, six European, six Hispanic, and Farrar et al.: Language and False-Belief in ASD 2001

4 three Middle Eastern (mean age = 7.3 years; range: 4 11 years). Of the 18 recruits for the control group, two were excluded: One child withdrew from participation after completing vocabulary tests, and one child s IQ score was lower than 70. Thus, the final sample size of the control group consisted of 16 children with the following heritages: seven Asian, five European, three Hispanic, and one Middle Eastern (mean age = 5 years; range: 3 7 years). Materials and Procedures Trained graduate students in the Communicative Disorders program tested the participants at a university speech-hearing clinic room equipped with a one-way mirror. Each participant came to the clinic three times. The participants were tested individually while their parents observed their children s testing through the one-way mirror. Baseline Testing Each participant took a vocabulary test during the first visit and a cognitive test during the second visit. Participants receptive vocabulary level was tested using Form A of the PPVT-4 and expressive vocabulary level was tested using the EVT-2. Age-equivalent scores of the PPVT-4 and the EVT-2 were used for analysis. Participants cognitive level was assessed using a brief IQ score on the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Second Edition (KABC-II; Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004). The KABC-II was selected because it reduces ethnically and culturally based differences. The brief IQ scores were derived from the Mental Processing Index, which focuses on mental processing instead of acquired knowledge. The Mental Processing Index consists of sequential processing, simultaneous processing, learning ability (ages 3 6 years), and planning ability (administered to children ages 7 18 years). The subtests administered (i.e., Atlantis, Conceptual Thinking, Story Completion, Face Recognition, Number Recall, Rover, Rebus, Triangle, Word Order, and Pattern Reasoning) varied depending on the age of the participants. Composite IQ standard scores and subscale scores were obtained from the subtests administered. The mental age for each child was computed and was used in the analyses in order to control for any group differences. Parents in both groups completed the Lifetime form of thescq.attheendoftheirfirstvisit,theresearchassistant gave the SCQ to the parents and had them complete it at home and bring it to their second visit. A total score higher than 15 on the SCQ is indicative of an ASD diagnosis. Parents also completed the Children s Communication Checklist Second Edition (CCC-2; Norbury, Nash, Baird, & Bishop, 2004), which screens overall communication in speech, syntax, semantics, coherence, initiation, scripted language, context, nonverbal communication, social relations, and interest. The CCC-2 is appropriate to use for children older than 4 years of age who can produce sentences. Therefore, three parents of 3-year-olds in the control group did not complete the CCC-2. General Communication Composite standard scores and scaled scores were obtained. General Communication Composite and syntax subscale scores were used for analysis in order to assess general language abilities. The syntax subscale scores were used in order to examine whether syntax, rather than complementation perse,wasassociatedwith FB understanding. Experimental Tasks Each participant received the experimental tasks during the third visit. Four sets of stimuli (Batteries A D) were presented to each participant, and the order of the stimuli presentation was counterbalanced. Each set consisted of two FB tasks (change of location and change of content), a language task (say- and think-complementation), and a control language task (relative clause task). The inclusion of the relative clause control task was to assess whether any relation between complementation and FB was a result of complementation per se and not general grammatical ability. A blank screen was presented at the beginning of each task. Each task was presented with a brief narrative while showing cartoonlike pictures on a computer screen. After presenting an event picture on the computer screen, a research assistant read a brief script of the presented picture (see the Appendix for sample stimuli). Change of location task. A sample narrative was: John was playing with a toy plane. John wanted to drink water. John put the toy plane in his backpack. Then he went to the kitchen. John s father went into the room. He thought the toy plane might break in the backpack. So, John s father took the toy plane from the backpack and put it in a box. Then John s father left the room. John went to the room to play with the toy plane. Following the presentation of a narrative, four questions were asked. Q1. When John goes to the room, where would he look for the toy plane? (FB) Q2. Why did John look there? Q3. Where did John put the toy plane? Q4. Where is the toy plane now? Q1 above was the FB question and questions 2, 3, and 4 were control questions to assess the participant s memory. Change of content task. An examiner presented a closed chocolate box and asked the participant What do you think is inside the box? The box was then opened to reveal there are really keys in the box. The box was then closed while the child was looking. Following this presentation, the examiner asked the following five questions. Q1. What is inside the box? Q2. When you first saw the box, all closed up like this, what did you think was inside the box? (FB) Q3. Can you remember what was in the box? The experimenter takes a puppet out of a closed bag and lets her look at the closed box Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol July 2017

5 Q4. The puppet has never looked inside the box. What does she think is inside the box? (FB) Q5. Why does she think that? Responses on two of the above questions (Q2: self as an agent; Q4: a puppet as an agent) assessed FB understanding. Questions 1 and 3 were control questions. Complementation language tasks. A sample narrative was: Paul s parents were talking. Paul s father asked, What is Paul doing? Paul s mother said that Paul was sleeping. However, Paul s mother thought that Paul was playing computer games. Following the presentation of the narrative, two questions were asked. Q1: What did Paul s mother say Paul was doing? Q2. What did Paul s mother think Paul was doing? Responses to Q1 and Q2 were scored for data analysis. Relative clause control language tasks. Asample narrative was: This is Helen. These are Helen s friends. She is brushing her hair. She is brushing her teeth. Helen hugs the girl who is brushing her hair. Following the presentation of the narrative, language comprehension question was asked. Q1. Who did Helen hug? If the participants did not respond to Q1, Q2 was presented. Q2. Did Helen hug the girl who was brushing her hair? Or did Helen hug the girl who was brushing her teeth? Response to Q1 or Q2 was scored for analysis. Scoring Raw scores, age-equivalent scores, and standard scores were obtained for the PPVT-4 and the EVT-2. Ageequivalent scores were used for analysis because we were interested in the absolute level of performance. For each subtest of the KABC-II, raw scores, scaled scores, percentile ranks, and age-equivalent scores were also computed. Subscale standard scores of sequential, simultaneous, learning, planning processes and composite IQ score (i.e., Mental Processing Index) were also obtained. The KABC-II provides only the composite IQ score and does not provide the age-equivalent score of the composite IQ. Therefore, mental age was derived as a mean score of each subtest age-equivalent score administered on the KABC-II as suggested in the manual (p. 46). The General Communication Composite standard score and syntax subscale score were obtained for the CCC-2. The range of scores for the FB location task was 0 4, the FB content task was 0 8 (4 for self as an agent, and 4 for puppet as an agent). The range of scores for the FB location control task was 0 8, and the FB content control task was 0 4. To be credited with passing the FB task, children had to pass the control questions. If they failed the control questions on a specific FB task, they were not credited with passing that task. If they missed all the control questions, they were excluded from analyses. We also removed those children who did not pass the control questions but passed the FB tasks. This resulted in a loss of one child from each group. For language tasks, the range for the say-complement was 0 4, the think-complement was 0 4, and the range for the relative clause (language control task) was 0 4). All raw scores were converted to percentages of correct responses for analyses. Trained research assistants completed scoring, and the second author checked their accuracy. Any disagreements were resolved by discussion. Results Baseline Data All data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS; IBM Corp, 2014). For all analyses, we conducted Bonferroni corrections to guard against Type 1 errors. For the baseline data on participant characteristic variables, the error rate was set at.01 per variable and the inflation of the error rates was controlled by dividing.01 by the number of comparisons per variable. The four participant characteristic variables were age (chronological age and mental age), IQ (KABC-II composite and subscale standard scores), SCQ, and CCC-2 (General Communication Composite and syntax subscale scores). A descriptive summary of participants characteristics within the ASD and control groups is presented in Table 1. These characteristics were compared between groups using analyses of variance. The SCQ scores were significantly higher in the ASD group than in the control group, F(1, 31) = 53.13, p =.000, η 2 p =.64. This result is consistent with the diagnosis of ASD as shown by the mean total score higher than 15. Children in the ASD group were older than those in the control group, F(1, 32) = , p =.001, η 2 p =.30. However, their mental ages were similar, F(1, 31) = 3.70, p =.064. We matched the participants on their overall cognitive level by using their mental age. The IQ composite score between groups was not significant, F(1, 32) = 7.02, p =.012 (error rate is.0025 by dividing.01 by four comparisons). Overall, their cognitive level was similar. It should be noted that KABC-II subscale scores were based on n = 18 in the ASD group and n = 13 in the control group because three participants in the control group were 3 years old and subscale scores were not available for children less than 4 years of age. The planning subscale score is applicable only to children older than 7 years of age (n = 9 in the ASD group, and n = 2 in the control group). The overall communication composite score on the CCC-2 of the children in the ASD group was significantly lower than the scores in the control group, F(1, 27) = 30.31, p =.000, η 2 p =.54. The mean of the CCC-2 syntax subscale score in the ASD group is below 1 SD and lower than the scores in the control group. However, the syntax subscale score was not statistically different between the ASD Farrar et al.: Language and False-Belief in ASD 2003

6 Table 1. Participant characteristics. Characteristic ASD group (n = 18) Control group (n = 16) M SD M SD p Age * Mental age KABC-II a Sequential Simultaneous Learning Planning b SCQ c * CCC-2 d * Syntax Note. ASD = autism spectrum disorder; KABC-II = Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Second Edition; SCQ = Social Communication Questionnaire; CCC-2 = Children s Communication Checklist Second Edition. a n = 13 in the control group for three of the four KABC-II subtest scores. b n = 9 in ASD group, n = 2 in control group; total score > 15 indicates ASD. c CCC-2 General Communication Composite standard score (M = 100, SD = 15). d CCC-2 Syntax scaled score (M = 10, SD = 3). *p <.0025 (adjusting the number of comparisons). and control groups, F(1, 26) = 5.59, p =.026. It should be noted that six of the participants did not have CCC-2 scores because their chronological age did not meet the age range (4 16 years). Table 2 provides the means and standard deviations on the FB and language measures. The overall percentage correct response rates were examined using analyses of variance and are shown in Figure 1. The percent correct on the total FB task performance was not significantly different between the ASD and control groups, F(1, 30) = 1.06, p =.31, or between the FB location and content tasks. The differences between the FB location and content task performance were further examined within each group using paired t tests, and there were no significant differences between the ASD group, t(16) = 1.81, p =.088, and the control group, t(14) = 1.24, p =.24.Thereceptiveand expressive vocabulary age-equivalent scores also did not differ between groups, nor were comparisons between sayand think-complement task performances significantly different between the ASD and control groups. Paired t tests between say- and think-complement were not significant in either the ASD, t(16) =.35, p =.73, or control groups, t(14) =.53, p =.61. Finally, there were no group differences on the relative clause control task, t(30) =.59, p =.46. False-Belief Understanding and Language Task Performance Of primary interest was the relation of vocabulary, syntax, and complementation development to FB performance in the ASD and control groups. Our findings from analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) and Pearson chi-square analyses are described in the following sections. ANCOVA Analyses To explore the contribution of language to FB understanding, a series of separate ANCOVAs were conducted controlling for the effect of mental age and language measures (i.e., receptive and expressive vocabulary, say- and think-complement, relative clauses) as covariates on total FB performance and the FB location and content tasks. As shown in Table 3, controlling for mental age resulted in significant group differences in FB scores. FB estimated means were significantly higher in the control group Table 2. False-belief and language measures (percent correct). ASD group (n = 17) Control group (n = 15) Measure M SD M SD p False-belief total False-belief location False-belief content Receptive vocabulary a Expressive vocabulary a Complementation-say Complementation-think Relative clauses Note. ASD = autism spectrum disorder. a Age-equivalent score. p <.004 (adjusting the number of comparisons) Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol July 2017

7 Figure 1. Percent correct on false-belief and complementation tasks. ASD = autism spectrum disorder; FB = false-belief; FBL = false-belief location; FBC = false-belief content; RELCLA = relative clause. language, and mental age are related. Of particular interest was whether the ASD and control groups differed on FB performance, controlling for complementation performance. Controlling for either the say- or think-complement had no impact on FB scores for the groups, as shown in Table 3. Finally, although not reported in Table 3, no FB differences were found when CCC-2 syntax or relative clauses were used as covariates (all ps >.16). compared with the ASD group. As shown in Table 3, these effects were found for the total and location FB scores, but not for the content task scores. Similar significant effects were found when receptive and expressive vocabulary scores were used as covariates in separate ANCOVAs. Children with ASD scored lower than the control group in estimated FB total and location performance scores. No group differences were found for the FB content task. Thus, controlling for mental age and vocabulary created a suppressor effect, indicating that FB, Pearson Chi-Square Analyses We further examined the association between the number of children who passed or failed the say-complement and the think-complement tasks (i.e., percentage of correct performance 75%) and passed or failed the FB tasks. Separate chi-square analyses were performed for the children with ASD and the control group for the two complement tasks. As seen in Table 4, passing the say-complement task was associated with passing the FB tasks in the ASD group. Every child with ASD who passed the FB task also passed the say-complement task, and no one who failed the saycomplement task passed the FB tasks. There were children with ASD who passed the say-complement task but failed the FB tasks. There are likely other cognitive achievements, such as inhibitory control, that also contribute to understanding FB, particularly with children with ASD (Devine & Hughes, 2014; Tager-Flusberg, 2007). However, in the control group, 29% of the children who failed the saycomplement task were still able to pass the FB task, and 75% of the children who passed the FB task passed the saycomplement task. Table 3. ANCOVA results for false-belief (FB) performance (percentage correct) in ASD and control groups. Covariate and DV F-ratio groups p Estimated means Control ASD Partial η p 2 Power Mental age a FB total FB location FB content Receptive vocabulary a FB total * FB location * FB content * Expressive vocabulary a FB total FB location FB content Say-complement b FB total FB location FB content Think-complement b FB total FB location FB content Note. DV = dependent variable (FB total, FB location, FB content); ASD = autism spectrum disorder. a Covariate is significant. Receptive and expressive vocabulary scores are age-equivalent scores. b Covariate is not significant. *Significance level set at.003. Farrar et al.: Language and False-Belief in ASD 2005

8 Table 4. Chi-square analyses of likelihood of passing false-belief (FB) tasks as a function of say-complement scores. Group Say fail Say pass Total ASD FB fail FB pass Total χ 2 (1, n = 17) = p <.05 Control FB fail FB pass Total χ 2 (1, n = 15) = p = ns Note. Finally, Table 5 displays the chi-square results for the think-complement. The association of passing thinkcomplement with passing FB tasks was statistically significant in the ASD group but not in the control group. As with the say-complement task, every child with ASD who passed the think-complementation task passed the FB task. However, in the control group, 50% of the children who passed the think-complement task failed FB tasks. As with say-complementation, passing the think-complementation task was not necessary to pass FB tasks in the control group. Summary Although there were no raw group differences in FB performance, once mental age and receptive or expressive vocabulary age-equivalent scores were controlled for in separate ANCOVAs, the children with ASD scored lower than the control group on the total and location FB measures. These covariates operated as a suppressor effect of the group differences in FB understanding. In contrast, controlling for either complement measure did not result in any significant difference in the ASD and control groups. Table 5. Chi-square analyses of likelihood of passing false-belief (FB) tasks as a function of think-complement scores. Group Think fail Think pass Total ASD FB fail FB pass Total χ 2 (1, n = 17) = 386. p <.05 Control FB fail FB pass Total χ 2 (1, n = 15) =.134, p=ns Note. ASD = autism spectrum disorder. ASD = autism spectrum disorder. The chi-square analyses indicated, however, that complementation was critical for children with ASD to pass the FB tasks. Not a single child with ASD passed the FB tasks without passing both the say- and think-complement tasks. This was not the case for control children, who were able to pass the FB tasks without necessarily passing the complementation task. Discussion The present study investigated whether general language and complementation abilities differentially affected FB understanding in children with ASD compared with a typically developing control group. In initial group comparisons, the groups did not differ on FB understanding. In a series of ANCOVAs, the children with ASD scored significantly lower on FB tasks compared with the control group. To be specific, when mental age and receptive or expressive vocabulary age scores were used as covariates, there were significant FB performance differences between the ASD and control groups. These results suggest that controlling for mental age and vocabulary affected FB development. This has been demonstrated in numerous studies of typically developing children and children with ASD (Cheung, 2006; see Milligan et al., 2007, for a review). One limitation of the use of vocabulary measures as a proxy for general language is that it does not assess general syntactic ability, which is also related to FB (Slade & Ruffman, 2005). In the current study, controlling for a parental measure of children s expressive syntax level and relative clause performance was unrelated to FB understanding. However, a more standardized assessment of syntax may be informative in future studies. Of particular theoretical interest was whether controlling for complementation affected FB understanding. Prior studies have suggested that memory for complements is particularly critical for children with ASD. Although studies have differed on finding whether say- orthink-complements are required, Tager-Flusberg and Joseph (2005) reported that complementation measures involving communication verbs (say and told) were the only predictor of FB in children with ASD. Lind and Bowler (2009) found that complement syntax with communication verbs predicted performance on the unexpected locations FB task after controlling for vocabulary, but not on the unexpected contents task. Lind and Bowler did not test for mental state verbs. In contrast, Lee et al. (2009) reported that mental state verb (think) complements, but not say-complements, were a significant contributor to FB task performance in Korean-speaking children with ASD. A recent study of French-speaking children found effects of both say- and think-complementation (Durreleman & Franck, 2015). In the present study, controlling for either complement measure did not affect group difference in FB performance in the ANCOVAs. The chi-square analyses, however, clearly demonstrated that both say- and think-complements were significantly associated with FB understanding in children with ASD. Children with ASD who passed FB tasks 2006 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol July 2017

9 (defined as 75% or higher correct responses), all passed say- andthink-complement tasks. The children with ASD who failed say- and think-complement tasks all failed the FB tasks. There were children with ASD who passed the complement task but failed the FB tasks. Complementation may be a necessary but not sufficient requirement for FB understanding at least for children with ASD. For the control group, passing say- or think-complements was not required for passing the FB task. P. A. de Villiers (2005) argued that complementation is required for FB understanding for typically developing children, whereas others argue that general language is sufficient (e.g., Slade & Ruffman, 2005). Studies have found support for both perspectives (de Villiers & Pyers, 2002; Nelson, 2005). Why does complementation play a stronger role in children with autism? One interpretation is that general language measures, such as receptive vocabulary, reflect participation in social interactions involving mental state talk (Nelson, 2005). For instance, typically developing children whose mothers reported a preference for mental state language in their conversations performed better on FB tasks (Peterson & Slaughter, 2003; Tompkins, 2015a; see Ruffman, 2014, for a review). Children with ASD have difficulties in the social use of language, which limits their participation in rich linguistic exchanges (Hughes & Leekham, 2004; Whyte, Nelson, & Shcerf, 2014). In one study, mothers of children with ASD made fewer cognitive and affective clarifications in their narratives. Although their use of cognitive clarifications was related to FB understanding of typically developing children, this was not the case for the children with ASD (Slaughter, Peterson, & Mackintosh, 2007). This may explain, in part, why the impact of language on FB understanding for children with ASD differs from FB understanding for typically developing children. Children with ASD rely on the acquisition of complementation as a mental model for FB understanding. Typically developing children can learn about the mind from conversations with others. Children with ASD have more difficulty engaging in these conservations about another s mind. Thus, Tager-Flusberg and colleagues (Tager-Flusberg, 2007; Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, 2005) argued that complement understanding is crucial to FB understanding in children with ASD. According to their argument, complementation is needed to bootstrap their mental-representational capacity because they typically do not have the other social cognitive skills that could contribute to a socially constructed FB (Tager-Flusberg, 2007, p. 301). That is, complementation is needed for representing a FB, which is consistent with the current study. Longitudinal investigations of children with ASD, which are extremely rare, should be conducted to confirm the direction of the effects. Furthermore, studies of the effects of mental state talk on FB performance for ASD are needed. Finally, in the ANCOVAs, we also found different effects of vocabulary on the change of location tasks compared with the change of content tasks. Lind and Bowler (2009) found similar differences in FB performance depending on the type of task. They suggested that the change of location and change of content tasks have different levels of complexity, involving narratives and the types of questions asked, which may require different language skills to pass them. Further studies are needed to identify the exact source of these differences across FB tasks (see Ng et al., 2010). Clinical Implications/Future Directions Future studies should address some of the other limitations of the current study by including (a) more homogeneous age ranges in both groups (ages 6 8 years in the ASD group; ages 4 6 years in the control group), (b) ADI-R and/or the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) measures, (c) direct syntax measures rather than parent report measures (CCC-2 syntax), and (d) longitudinal studies with larger sample sizes to investigate the emergence of FB in both populations. Participants should understand the semantic distinction between say- and think-complements as well as syntactic differences between them. Our tasks are not completely independent. In future studies, the linguistic stimuli should be more carefully selected to avoid any overlap between complement stimuli. These results also have intervention implications for children with ASD (Hutchins & Prelock, 2008). A few training studies have examined whether training complement understanding affects FB performance in typically developing children (Hale & Tager-Flusberg, 2003; Lohmann & Tomasello, 2003) or low-income preschoolers (Tompkins, 2015b). Hale and Tager-Flusberg (2003) reported that typically developing children who received training on sentential complements improved performance on sentential complements as well as on a FB task. Thus, FB as an identified deficit skill could be targeted in a training study for children with ASD by training on complementation, although it certainly would be expected that not all children could be trained because of various demographic or cognitive characteristics. The result of training studies in typically developing children provides a basis for exploring the effectiveness of training on FB performance in children with ASD. References American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Astington, J., & Baird, J. (Eds.). (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, / Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bishop, D. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: A study using standardized diagnostic instruments. Journal of Farrar et al.: Language and False-Belief in ASD 2007

10 Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, / Broekhof, E., Ketelaar, L., Stockman, L., van Zijp, A., Bos, G. N., & Rieffe, C. (2015). The understanding of intentions, desires and beliefs in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, Cheung, H. (2006). False belief and language comprehension in Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, j.jecp de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (2014). The role of language in theory of mind development. Topics in Language Disorders, 34, de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17, S (02) de Villiers, P. A. (2005). The role of language in theory-of-mind development: What deaf children tell us. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp ). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta analysis. Child Development, 85, Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Telsa, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children s understanding of other people s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62, Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Fourth Edition. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson. Durrleman, S., Burnel, M., Thommen, E., Foudon, N., Sonie, S., Reboul, A., & Fourneret, P. (2016). The language cognition interface in ASD: Complement sentences and false belief reasoning. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 21, Durrleman, S., & Franck, F. (2015). Exploring links between language and cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Complement sentences, false belief, and executive functioning. Journal of Communication Disorders, 54, j.jcomdis Farrant, B. M., Fletcher, J., & Mayberry, M. T. (2006). Specific language impairment, theory of mind, and visual perspective taking: Evidence for simulation theory and the developmental role of language. Child Development, 77, Retrieved from Farrar, M. J., Johnson, B., Tompkins, V., Easters, M., Zilisi- Medus, A., & Benigno, J. P. (2009). Language and theory of mind in preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 42, doi.org/ /j.jcomdis Fisher, N., Happé, F., & Dunn, J. (2005). The relationship between vocabulary, grammar, and false belief task performance in children with autistic spectrum disorders and children with moderate learning difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, Gernsbacher, M. A., Morson, E. M., & Grace, E. J. (2015). Language development in autism. In G. Hickok & S. Small (Eds.), Neurobiology of language (pp ). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. Gernsbacher, M. A., & Pripas-Kapit, S. R. (2012). Who s missing the point? A commentary on claims that autistic persons have a specific deficit in figurative language comprehension. Metaphor and Symbol, 27, Hale, C., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science, 6, Hale, C., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Social communication in children with autism: The relationship between theory of mind and discourse development. Autism, 9, Happé, F. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), Hresko,W.P.,Reid,D.K.,&Hammill,D.D.(1999). Test of Early Language Development Third Edition (TELD-3). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Hughes, C., & Leekham, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development. Social Development, 13, j x Hutchins, T. L., & Prelock, P. A. (2008). Supporting theory of mind development: Considerations and recommendations for professionals providing services to individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Topics in Language Disorders, 28, Hutchins, T. L., Prelock, P. A., Morris, H., Benner, J., LaVigne, T., & Hoza, B. (2016). Explicit vs. applied theory of mind competence: A comparison of typically developing males, males with ASD, and males with ADHD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 21, IBM Corp. (2014). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 23.0) [Computer software]. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Second Edition. Circle Pine, MN: AGS Publishing. Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview Revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Lee, H., Seung, H., Farrar, M. J., Jeong, S., Kim, K., & Gwon, E. (2009). The relationship between false belief and sentential complement in Korean-speaking children with autism. The Korean Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22(2), Lind, S. E., & Bowler, D. M. (2009). Language and theory of mind in autism spectrum disorder: The relationship between complement syntax and false belief task performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, doi.org/ /s y Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74, Low, J. (2010). Preschoolers implicit and explicit false-belief understanding: Relations with complex syntactical mastery. Child Development, 81, j x Miller, C. (2006). Developmental relationship between language and theory of mind. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15, (2006/014) Miller, S. A. (2015). Theory of mind: The silver anniversary edition. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 41, Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, j x 2008 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol July 2017

11 Nelson, K. (2005). Language pathways into the community of minds. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp ). New York, NY: Oxford University Press Ng, L., Cheung, H., & Xiao, W. (2010). False belief, complementation language, and contextual bias in preschoolers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34, Norbury, C. F., Nash, M., Baird, G., & Bishop, D. M. (2004). Using a parental checklist to identify diagnostic groups in children with communication impairment: A validation of the Children s Communication Checklist 2. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39, doi.org/ / Perner, J., Zauner, P., & Sprung, M. (2005). What does that have to do with point of view? Conflicting desires and want in German. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp ). New York, NY: Oxford University Press Peterson, C., & Slaughter, V. (2003). Opening windows into the mind: Mothers preferences for mental state explanations and children s theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 18, Peterson, C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory of mind development for children with deafness or autism. Child Development, 76, Rapin, I., & Dunn, M. (2003). Update on the language disorders of individuals on the autistic spectrum. Brain & Development, 25, Ruffman, T. (2014). To belief or not belief: Children s theory of mind. Developmental Review, 34, /j.dr Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Berument, S. K., Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ), Lifetime. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. San Juan, V., & Astington, J. W. (2012). Bridging the gap between implicit and explicit understanding: How language development promotes the processing and representation of false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Index of Productive Syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11(1), S ) Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development, 78, x Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (1992). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool (CELF-P). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Siegal, M., & Peterson, C. C. (2008). Language and theory of mind in atypically developing children: Evidence from studies of deafness, blindness, and autism. In C. Sharp, P. Fonagy & I. Goodyer (Eds.), Social cognition and developmental psychopathology (pp ). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Slade, L., & Ruffman, T. (2005). How language does (and does not) relate to theory of mind: A longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory and false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, / X21332 Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C., & Mackintosh, E. (2007). Mind what mother says: Narrative input and theory of mind in typical children and those on the autism spectrum. Child Development, 78, X21332 Tager-Flusberg, H. (2000). Language and understanding minds: Connections in autism. In. S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager- Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism and developmental cognitive neurosciences (pp ). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Evaluating the theory-of-mind hypothesis of autism. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. (2005). How language facilitates the acquisition of false belief understanding in children with autism. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp ). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Tardif, T., So, C., & Kaciroti, N. (2007). Language and false belief: Evidence for general, not specific, effects in Cantonesespeaking preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 43, Tompkins, V. (2015a). Improving low-income preschoolers theory of mind: A training study. Cognitive Development, 36, Tompkins, V. (2015b). Mothers cognitive state talk during shared book reading and children s later false belief understanding. Cognitive Development, 36, j.cogdev Wellman, H. (2014). Making minds. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, Whyte, E. M., Nelson, K. E., & Scherf, K. S. (2014). Idiom, syntax, and advanced theory of mind abilities in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57, Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive Vocabulary Test Second Edition. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson. Farrar et al.: Language and False-Belief in ASD 2009

12 Appendix (p. 1 of 4) Sample Stimuli Figure A1. John plays with the toy plane. Figure A2. John wanted to drink water Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol July 2017

13 Appendix (p. 2 of 4) Sample Stimuli Figure A3. John put the toy plane in his backpack. Figure A4. John went to the kitchen. Farrar et al.: Language and False-Belief in ASD 2011

14 Appendix (p. 3 of 4) Sample Stimuli Figure A5. John s father went into the room. He thought the toy plane might break in the backpack. Figure A6. John s father took the toy plane from the backpack and put it in a box Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol July 2017

15 Appendix (p. 4 of 4) Sample Stimuli Figure A7. John s father left the room. Figure A8. John went to the room to play with the toy plane. Farrar et al.: Language and False-Belief in ASD 2013

Brief Report: An Independent Replication and Extension of Psychometric Evidence Supporting the Theory of Mind Inventory

Brief Report: An Independent Replication and Extension of Psychometric Evidence Supporting the Theory of Mind Inventory DOI 10.1007/s10803-016-2784-7 BRIEF REPORT Brief Report: An Independent Replication and Extension of Psychometric Evidence Supporting the Theory of Mind Inventory Kathryn J. Greenslade 1,2 Truman E. Coggins

More information

Acknowledgements. Theory of Mind. Theory of Mind Evaluation and Clinical Applications. Theory of Mind and Language. Evaluating Theory of Mind

Acknowledgements. Theory of Mind. Theory of Mind Evaluation and Clinical Applications. Theory of Mind and Language. Evaluating Theory of Mind Theory of Mind Evaluation and Clinical Applications Allison Bean, M.S., CCC-SLP Beth Walker, M.A., CCC-SLP/A Karla McGregor, Ph.D., CCC-SLP University of Iowa Acknowledgements University of Iowa Word Learning

More information

Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups

Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups Megan Gresholdt Allison M. Haskill, Ph.D Augustana College Megan-Gresholdt@augustana.edu AllisonHaskill@augustana.edu What is Theory of Mind (ToM)?

More information

Durham Research Online

Durham Research Online Durham Research Online Deposited in DRO: 15 October 2010 Version of attached le: Accepted Version Peer-review status of attached le: Peer-reviewed Citation for published item: Lind, S.E. and Bowler, D.M.

More information

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers!

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers! Marisa Filipe Dezembro de 2013 pdpsi10020@fpce.up.pt EXCL/MHC-LIN/0688/2012 Summary Introduction: Prosodic Abilities in Children and Young Adults with Typical & Non-Typical Development Prosodic abilities

More information

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: In school-aged children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), what oral narrative elements differ from their typically developing peers? Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University

More information

Pragmatic language impairments

Pragmatic language impairments Pragmatic language impairments Dorothy Bishop Wellcome Principal Research Fellow Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford 1 Oral communication involves: COMPREHENSION decoding speech

More information

The Development of Theory of Mind in Early Childhood*

The Development of Theory of Mind in Early Childhood* SOCIAL COGNITION The Development of Theory of Mind in Early Childhood* 1 Janet Wilde Astington, PhD, 2 Margaret J. Edward, MA 1 Institute of Child Study, University of Toronto, Canada 2 School District

More information

Factors Influencing How Parents Report. Autism Symptoms on the ADI-R

Factors Influencing How Parents Report. Autism Symptoms on the ADI-R Factors Influencing How Parents Report Autism Symptoms on the ADI-R Diana Wexler Briarcliff High School Diana Wexler Briarcliff High School 1 Abstract Background: The Autism Diagnostic Interview - Revised

More information

Language and Theory of Mind in Children with Hearing Impairment - A Preliminary Study

Language and Theory of Mind in Children with Hearing Impairment - A Preliminary Study ToM in HI JISHA 26 (1), 36-42 Language and Theory of Mind in Children with Hearing Impairment - A Preliminary Study Meera S. S. 1, Thulasi Prasad 2 & Rashmi. J. Bhat 3 Abstract Theory of Mind (ToM) is

More information

",! " ", " " " " "!!! " $.

,!  ,     !!!  $. 27 35 5,6, Theory of Mind,,!, #!! Theory of Mind, ToM Theory of Mind (ToM)! ToM!!! $ Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 985; Astington &)! (Jenkins, 995; Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2,, $ #,# (joint attention)

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs DSM-IV to DSM-5: Primary Changes Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs Laurie Swineford, PhD CCC-SLP Washington State University DSM-IV Previously we used the

More information

Language Disorders in Greek 5, May 2014

Language Disorders in Greek 5, May 2014 Mental state terms and the role of working memory in high functioning autistic children s narrative production ELENI BALDIMTSI 1, ELENI PERISTERI 1 & IANTHI-MARIA TSIMPLI 1,2 1 ARISTOTLE UNIVERSITY OF

More information

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Parkland College A with Honors Projects Honors Program 2015 Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Hannah Li Parkland College Recommended Citation Li, Hannah, "Social and Pragmatic Language

More information

Faculty Advisor: Dr. Sandra Gillam. Abstract. 1. Introduction

Faculty Advisor: Dr. Sandra Gillam. Abstract. 1. Introduction Proceedings of The National Conference On Undergraduate Research (NCUR) 2015 Eastern Washington University, Cheney, WA April 16-18, 2015 Improving The Use Of Mental State Verbs By Children With Autism

More information

Research on theory of mind in deaf education

Research on theory of mind in deaf education Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Independent Studies and Capstones Program in Audiology and Communication Sciences 2011 Research on theory of mind in deaf education Ashley

More information

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document.

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document. Title Understanding how children think: A theory of mind perspective Author(s) Carolyn Tan-Niam S. L. Source REACT, 1998(1), 27-31 Published by National Institute of Education (Singapore) This document

More information

Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome

Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome Jessica Klusek, MS CCC-SLP FPG Child Development Institute (FPG) University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (UNC) Molly Losh, PhD Northwestern

More information

Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences

Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences DATE: October 13, 2018 PRESENTED BY: Jill Dolata, PhD, CCC-SLP & Cynthia Green, MS, CCC-SLP Goals Describe

More information

Critical Review: Late Talkers : What Can We Expect?

Critical Review: Late Talkers : What Can We Expect? Critical Review: Late Talkers : What Can We Expect? Ian Gallant M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate Western University: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders This critical review examines two specific questions

More information

social communication disorder: identification with the CCC-2? Courtenay Frazier Norbury University College London

social communication disorder: identification with the CCC-2? Courtenay Frazier Norbury University College London social communication disorder: identification with the CCC-2? Courtenay Frazier Norbury University College London my PhD plan identify diagnostic criteria for pragmatic language impairment that would reliably

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

Cover Page. The handle   holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/19149 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Author: Maljaars, Janne Pieternella Wilhelmina Title: Communication problems in children

More information

Is Theory of Mind Understanding Impaired in Males with Fragile X Syndrome?

Is Theory of Mind Understanding Impaired in Males with Fragile X Syndrome? J Abnorm Child Psychol (2007) 35:17 28 DOI 10.1007/s10802-006-9077-0 ORIGINAL PAPER Is Theory of Mind Understanding Impaired in Males with Fragile X Syndrome? Cathy M. Grant Ian Apperly Chris Oliver Published

More information

Longitudinal Associations Between Children's Understanding of Emotions and Theory of Mind

Longitudinal Associations Between Children's Understanding of Emotions and Theory of Mind Longitudinal Associations Between Children's Understanding of Emotions and Theory of Mind By: Marion O Brien, Jennifer Miner Weaver, Jackie A. Nelson, Susan D. Calkins, Esther M. Leerkes, Stuart Marcovitch

More information

Receptive Language Skills among Younger, Adolescent, and Adults with Down Syndrome: The Use of the Growth-Scale- Vocabulary as a Measure

Receptive Language Skills among Younger, Adolescent, and Adults with Down Syndrome: The Use of the Growth-Scale- Vocabulary as a Measure Research Article imedpub Journals www.imedpub.com Journal of Childhood & Developmental Disorders ISSN 2472-1786 DOI: 10.4172/2472-1786.100078 Abstract Receptive Language Skills among Younger, Adolescent,

More information

Reading Between the Lines: An Intervention Tool for Teaching Theory of Mind

Reading Between the Lines: An Intervention Tool for Teaching Theory of Mind University of Redlands InSPIRe @ Redlands Undergraduate Honors Theses Theses, Dissertations & Honors Projects 2015 Reading Between the Lines: An Intervention Tool for Teaching Theory of Mind Megan Jansen

More information

Brief Report: Prompted Pretend Play in Autism1

Brief Report: Prompted Pretend Play in Autism1 Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 27, No., 997 Brief Reports Brief Report: Prompted Pretend Play in Autism Tony Charman2 University College London Simon Baron-Cohen University of Cambridge

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Perspect Lang Learn Educ. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 May 3.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Perspect Lang Learn Educ. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 May 3. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Perspect Lang Learn Educ. 2008 October ; 15(3): 119 126. doi:10.1044/lle15.3.119. Language Outcomes of Late Talking Toddlers at Preschool

More information

The Mind Behind the Message: Advancing Theory-of-Mind Scales for Typically Developing Children, and Those With Deafness, Autism, or Asperger Syndrome

The Mind Behind the Message: Advancing Theory-of-Mind Scales for Typically Developing Children, and Those With Deafness, Autism, or Asperger Syndrome Child Development, March April 2012, Volume 83, Number 2, Pages 469 485 The Mind Behind the Message: Advancing Theory-of-Mind Scales for Typically Developing Children, and Those With Deafness, Autism,

More information

Theory of Mind and Language in Children With Cochlear Implants

Theory of Mind and Language in Children With Cochlear Implants Theory of Mind and Language in Children With Cochlear Implants Ethan Remmel Kimberly Peters Western Washington University Thirty children with cochlear implants (CI children), age range 3 12 years, and

More information

Language is the behavior of the brain; communication is the behavior of the body and brain. Financial Disclosure

Language is the behavior of the brain; communication is the behavior of the body and brain. Financial Disclosure Financial Disclosure Asperger Syndrome & HFA Informal Dynamic Social Thinking Assessment Michelle Garcia Winner www.socialthinking.com I am affiliated with Think Social Publishing. I publish multiple books

More information

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) DSM-5 (Criteria and Major Changes for SLP-Related Conditions) Individuals meeting the criteria will be given a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder with three levels of severity based on degree of support

More information

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Assessment and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder For this month s topic, I am excited to share my recent experience using the fifth edition of the Diagnostic

More information

VERBAL INFERENCE IN SCHOOL AGE CHILDREN WITH ASD, LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT, & TYPICAL DEVELOPMENT

VERBAL INFERENCE IN SCHOOL AGE CHILDREN WITH ASD, LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT, & TYPICAL DEVELOPMENT VERBAL INFERENCE IN SCHOOL AGE CHILDREN WITH ASD, LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT, & TYPICAL DEVELOPMENT 2014 ASHA Convention Orlando, Florida November 21, 2014 Margaret Yuk, Kelli Linder, Allison Haskill, & Sophia

More information

An investigation of oral narratives in children with High Functioning Autism: aspects of microstructure and macrostructure

An investigation of oral narratives in children with High Functioning Autism: aspects of microstructure and macrostructure An investigation of oral narratives in children with High Functioning Autism: aspects of microstructure and macrostructure E L E N I B A L D I M T S I 1, E L E N I P E R I S T E R I 1, & I A N T H I T

More information

FOUNDATIONS FOR LITERACY: AN EARLY LITERACY INTERVENTION FOR DHH CHILDREN

FOUNDATIONS FOR LITERACY: AN EARLY LITERACY INTERVENTION FOR DHH CHILDREN FOUNDATIONS FOR LITERACY: AN EARLY LITERACY INTERVENTION FOR DHH CHILDREN Amy Lederberg, Susan Easterbrooks, Stacey Tucci, Victoria Burke, Elizabeth Miller Georgia State University Carol Connor, Arizona

More information

Individual Differences in Theory of Mind from Preschool to Adolescence: Achievements and Directions. Claire Hughes. Rory T. Devine

Individual Differences in Theory of Mind from Preschool to Adolescence: Achievements and Directions. Claire Hughes. Rory T. Devine Individual Differences in Theory of Mind from Preschool to Adolescence: Achievements and Directions Claire Hughes Rory T. Devine University of Cambridge Key words: theory of mind, individual differences,

More information

Language Outcomes for Preverbal Toddlers with Autism

Language Outcomes for Preverbal Toddlers with Autism Studies in Literature and Language Vol. 4, No. 3, 2012, pp. 1-7 DOI:10.3968/j.sll.1923156320120403.3528 ISSN 1923-1555[Print] ISSN 1923-1563[Online] www.cscanada.net www.cscanada.org Language Outcomes

More information

Down Syndrome and Autism

Down Syndrome and Autism Down Syndrome and Autism Lina Patel, Psy.D. Sie Center for Down Syndrome Children s Hospital Colorado University of Colorado School of Medicine Lina.Patel@childrenscolorado.org Financial Disclosure Objectives

More information

Diagnosis Advancements. Licensee OAPL (UK) Creative Commons Attribution License (CC-BY) Research study

Diagnosis Advancements. Licensee OAPL (UK) Creative Commons Attribution License (CC-BY) Research study Page 1 of 6 Diagnosis Advancements Relationship between Stereotyped Behaviors and Restricted Interests (SBRIs) measured on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) and diagnostic results. C Schutte

More information

Lecture 6: The Whorfian Hypothesis (contd.); autism spectrum disorders and language

Lecture 6: The Whorfian Hypothesis (contd.); autism spectrum disorders and language Lecture 6: The Whorfian Hypothesis (contd.); autism spectrum disorders and language Learning goals: o Understand what is meant by the Whorfian Hypothesis, and be able to illustrate it with (controversial)

More information

COGNITIVE AND AFFECTIVE THEORY OF MIND IN CHILDREN AND YOUNG ADOLESCENTS

COGNITIVE AND AFFECTIVE THEORY OF MIND IN CHILDREN AND YOUNG ADOLESCENTS COGNITIVE AND AFFECTIVE THEORY OF MIND IN CHILDREN AND YOUNG ADOLESCENTS A Thesis Submitted to the Committee on Graduate Studies in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science

More information

Children s Communication Checklist - 2: a validation study

Children s Communication Checklist - 2: a validation study Publié dans Revue Tranel (Travaux neuchâtelois de linguistique) 42, 53-63, 2005 qui doit être utilisée pour toute référence à ce travail Children s Communication Checklist - 2: a validation study Courtenay

More information

Making inferences from text: it s vocabulary that matters

Making inferences from text: it s vocabulary that matters Making inferences from text: it s vocabulary that matters Rebecca Lucas 1 Courtenay Frazier Norbury Department of Psychology Royal Holloway University of London 1 corresponding author Address for correspondence:

More information

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D.

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. Autism Spectrum Disorder Neurodevelopmental disorder Reflects understanding of the etiology of disorder as related to alterations

More information

Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype?

Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype? Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Communication Disorders 41 (2008) 319 336 Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype? Andrew

More information

Autistic Disorder. Asperger s Disorder: Problems: Higher Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders: Revealing Communication Deficits. Therefore, we must:

Autistic Disorder. Asperger s Disorder: Problems: Higher Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders: Revealing Communication Deficits. Therefore, we must: Higher Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders: Revealing Communication Deficits Glenis Benson, Ph.D. American Speech and Hearing Association Chicago, IL November 20, 2008 Problems: Assumption that language

More information

Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders

Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders J. Experimental Child Psychology 90 (2005) 142 171 www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders

More information

Lexical and Grammatical Skills in Toddlers on the Autism Spectrum Compared to Late Talking Toddlers

Lexical and Grammatical Skills in Toddlers on the Autism Spectrum Compared to Late Talking Toddlers J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:1065 1075 DOI 10.1007/s10803-010-1134-4 ORIGINAL PAPER Lexical and Grammatical Skills in Toddlers on the Autism Spectrum Compared to Late Talking Toddlers Susan Ellis Weismer

More information

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: Can joint attention, imitation, and/or play skills predict future language abilities of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)? Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology)

More information

The DSM-IV-TR diagnoses autism as having the symptoms of. qualitative impairment in social interaction, qualitative impairments in

The DSM-IV-TR diagnoses autism as having the symptoms of. qualitative impairment in social interaction, qualitative impairments in Lena Treadway Final Treatment Plan April 27, 2011 Diagnostic Related Group: The DSM-IV-TR diagnoses autism as having the symptoms of qualitative impairment in social interaction, qualitative impairments

More information

Joanna Bailes M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Joanna Bailes M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: Can imitation, joint attention and the level of play in preschool years predict later language outcomes for children with autism spectrum disorder? Joanna Bailes M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate

More information

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 14: Autism

9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood. Lecture 14: Autism 9.85 Cognition in Infancy and Early Childhood Lecture 14: Autism Schedule Monday, November 21st -- mirror neuron debate; research drafts back Wednesday, November 23rd -- Happy Thanksgiving; no class Monday,

More information

Overview. Clinical Features

Overview. Clinical Features Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Clinical Features Overview Diagnostic & Statistical Manual IV (DSM IV) Prevalence Course of Onset Etiology Early Recognition Early Recognition

More information

Critical Review: What Presenting Speech and Language Characteristics of Late Talkers Distinguish Those Who Recover from Those Who Do Not?

Critical Review: What Presenting Speech and Language Characteristics of Late Talkers Distinguish Those Who Recover from Those Who Do Not? Critical Review: What Presenting Speech and Language Characteristics of Late Talkers Distinguish Those Who Recover from Those Who Do Not? Melissa Dumoulin M.Cl.Sc. Speech-Language Pathology Candidate University

More information

Peer Perception in Autism. Kathryn McVicar, MD Assistant Professor Clinical Pediatrics and Neurology Albert Einstien College of Medicine

Peer Perception in Autism. Kathryn McVicar, MD Assistant Professor Clinical Pediatrics and Neurology Albert Einstien College of Medicine Peer Perception in Autism Kathryn McVicar, MD Assistant Professor Clinical Pediatrics and Neurology Albert Einstien College of Medicine Peer Perception and Autism What is autism? A review of specific deficits

More information

Age of diagnosis for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Reasons for a later diagnosis: Earlier identification = Earlier intervention

Age of diagnosis for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Reasons for a later diagnosis: Earlier identification = Earlier intervention Identifying Autism Spectrum Disorders: Do You See What I See? Age of diagnosis for Autism Spectrum Disorders 1970 s it was around 5-6 years of age 1980 s it was around 4-5 years of age presently the mean

More information

Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism

Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism By Lauren Lowry Hanen Certified SLP and Clinical Staff Writer Several years ago I worked on a diagnostic assessment team. I remember the first

More information

Specific Language Impairment and Perspective Taking: Delayed Development of Theory of Mind, Visual and Emotional Perspective Taking

Specific Language Impairment and Perspective Taking: Delayed Development of Theory of Mind, Visual and Emotional Perspective Taking Research Article imedpub Journals http://www.imedpub.com Journal of Childhood & Developmental Disorders Specific Language Impairment and Perspective Taking: Delayed Development of Theory of Mind, Visual

More information

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Marci Laurel, MA, CCC-SLP mlaurel@salud.unm.edu and Services (UCEDD) 1 Training Objectives Participants will: 1. Name at least three communication

More information

Categorization in ASD: The Role of Typicality and Development

Categorization in ASD: The Role of Typicality and Development Unless otherwise noted, the publisher, which is the American Speech-Language- Hearing Association (ASHA), holds the copyright on all materials published in Perspectives on Language Learning and Education,

More information

Is theory of mind understanding impaired in males with fragile X Syndrome? Grant, C., Apperly, I. & Oliver, C

Is theory of mind understanding impaired in males with fragile X Syndrome? Grant, C., Apperly, I. & Oliver, C Is theory of mind understanding impaired in males with fragile X Syndrome? Grant, C., Apperly, I. & Oliver, C Cerebra Centre for Neurodevelopmental Disorders, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham

More information

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc Evaluating Educational Services for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders Erik Mayville, Ph.D., BCBA-D The Institute for Educational Planning, LLC Connecticut Center for Child Development, Inc. & mayville@iepinc.org

More information

CLINICAL BOTTOM LINE Early Intervention for Children With Autism Implications for Occupational Therapy

CLINICAL BOTTOM LINE Early Intervention for Children With Autism Implications for Occupational Therapy Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, J.,... Varley, J. (2010). Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: The Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics,

More information

JOHN C. THORNE, PHD, CCC-SLP UNIVERSITY OF WASHINGTON DEPARTMENT OF SPEECH & HEARING SCIENCE FETAL ALCOHOL SYNDROME DIAGNOSTIC AND PREVENTION NETWORK

JOHN C. THORNE, PHD, CCC-SLP UNIVERSITY OF WASHINGTON DEPARTMENT OF SPEECH & HEARING SCIENCE FETAL ALCOHOL SYNDROME DIAGNOSTIC AND PREVENTION NETWORK OPTIMIZING A CLINICAL LANGUAGE MEASURE FOR USE IN IDENTIFYING SIGNIFICANT NEURODEVELOPMENTAL IMPAIRMENT IN DIAGNOSIS OF FETAL ALCOHOL SPECTRUM DISORDERS (FASD) JOHN C. THORNE, PHD, CCC-SLP UNIVERSITY OF

More information

Typical Gesture Development

Typical Gesture Development By Lauren Lowry Hanen Certified Speech-Language Pathologist and Hanen Staff Member In last month s WigWag Minute, I wrote about gestures, and about how examining children s gesture use can help us identify

More information

Language Comprehension Profiles of Young Adolescents With Fragile X Syndrome

Language Comprehension Profiles of Young Adolescents With Fragile X Syndrome AJSLP Article Language Comprehension Profiles of Young Adolescents With Fragile X Syndrome Ashley Oakes, a,b Sara T. Kover, c and Leonard Abbeduto a,b Purpose: In this study, the authors sought to characterize

More information

Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children Jones, Anna C.; Gutierrez, Roberto; Ludlow, Amanda

Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children Jones, Anna C.; Gutierrez, Roberto; Ludlow, Amanda Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children Jones, Anna C.; Gutierrez, Roberto; Ludlow, Amanda DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2015.06.005 License: Creative Commons: Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs

More information

Research Article The Michigan Autism Spectrum Questionnaire: A Rating Scale for High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders

Research Article The Michigan Autism Spectrum Questionnaire: A Rating Scale for High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders Hindawi Publishing Corporation Autism Research and Treatment Volume 2013, Article ID 708273, 5 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2013/708273 Research Article The Michigan Autism Spectrum Questionnaire: A

More information

Early Educational Placement and Later Language Outcomes for Children With Cochlear Implants

Early Educational Placement and Later Language Outcomes for Children With Cochlear Implants Otology & Neurotology 31:1315Y1319 Ó 2010, Otology & Neurotology, Inc. Early Educational Placement and Later Language Outcomes for Children With Cochlear Implants *Jean Sachar Moog and Ann E. Geers *Moog

More information

AUTISM. What is it? How does it affect a student s learning? What do we do about it? Patricia Collins MS CCC-SLP

AUTISM. What is it? How does it affect a student s learning? What do we do about it? Patricia Collins MS CCC-SLP AUTISM What is it? How does it affect a student s learning? What do we do about it? Patricia Collins MS CCC-SLP Autism spectrum disorder is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by deficits in social

More information

ENGAGEMENT IN ROLE PLAY, PRETENSE, AND ACTING CLASSES PREDICT ADVANCED THEORY OF MIND SKILL IN MIDDLE CHILDHOOD*

ENGAGEMENT IN ROLE PLAY, PRETENSE, AND ACTING CLASSES PREDICT ADVANCED THEORY OF MIND SKILL IN MIDDLE CHILDHOOD* IMAGINATION, COGNITION AND PERSONALITY, Vol. 30(3) 249-258, 2010-2011 ENGAGEMENT IN ROLE PLAY, PRETENSE, AND ACTING CLASSES PREDICT ADVANCED THEORY OF MIND SKILL IN MIDDLE CHILDHOOD* THALIA R. GOLDSTEIN

More information

Autism is best characterized as a spectrum of developmental

Autism is best characterized as a spectrum of developmental Research Article Measuring Pragmatic Language in Speakers With Autism Spectrum Disorders: Comparing the Children s Communication Checklist 2 and the Test of Pragmatic Language Joanne Volden Linda Phillips

More information

Introductory Workshop. Research What is Autism? Core Deficits in Behaviour. National Autistic Society Study - UK (Barnard, et. al, 2001).

Introductory Workshop. Research What is Autism? Core Deficits in Behaviour. National Autistic Society Study - UK (Barnard, et. al, 2001). Schedule 1.! What is Autism and How is it treated? Intervention and Outcomes, New views on Research and Possibilities 2.! How do typical children develop Referencing, Regulation, Play and Peer Relationships?

More information

Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children

Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children Confronting the Language Barrier: Theory of Mind in Deaf Children 1 Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children Anna C. Jones a, Roberto Gutierrez b, Amanda K. Ludlow b c a Deafness,CognitionandLanguageResearchCentre,UniversityCollegeLondon,LondonWC1H0PD,UnitedKingdom

More information

Joint Attention. Joint Attention. Joint Attention. Joint Attention and ASD: Enhancing Language and Social Competence

Joint Attention. Joint Attention. Joint Attention. Joint Attention and ASD: Enhancing Language and Social Competence The and ASD: Enhancing Language and Social Competence Joanne Gerenser, Ph.D., CCC-SLP The ability to coordinate attention between people and objects Loveland & Landry (1986) An attentionalstate during

More information

Determining Effective Communication Through Motoric Analysis in Nonverbal Children With Autism

Determining Effective Communication Through Motoric Analysis in Nonverbal Children With Autism Determining Effective Communication Through Motoric Analysis in Nonverbal Children With Autism Voula Constantarakos, MHSc.,CCC-SLP Marla Rolleri, MS, CCC-SLP Megan Weisbrode, MS, OTR/L November 17, 2006

More information

Reading Accuracy vs. Reading Comprehension in Two Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. A Senior Honors Thesis. Ellen Hambley

Reading Accuracy vs. Reading Comprehension in Two Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. A Senior Honors Thesis. Ellen Hambley Reading Accuracy vs. Reading Comprehension in Two Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder A Senior Honors Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for graduation with research distinction

More information

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1 Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1 Please use the following citation when referencing this work: McGill, R. J. (2013). Test review: Children s Psychological Processing Scale (CPPS). Journal of Psychoeducational

More information

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome 5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome Published in: Neuropsychologia, 2008, 47 (3), 652-656. Chapter 5 Abstract The semantic and phonemic fluency performance of adults with high functioning

More information

Categorization Ability, but Not Theory of Mind, Contributes to Children s Developing Understanding of Expertise

Categorization Ability, but Not Theory of Mind, Contributes to Children s Developing Understanding of Expertise Categorization Ability, but Not Theory of Mind, Contributes to Children s Developing Understanding of Expertise Judith H. Danovitch (j.danovitch@louisville.edu) Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences,

More information

Investigating the development of core cognitive skills in autism: a 3-year. prospective study

Investigating the development of core cognitive skills in autism: a 3-year. prospective study Investigating the development of core cognitive skills in autism: a 3-year prospective study Elizabeth Pellicano Pellicano, E. (2010). The development of core cognitive skills in autism: a 3-year prospective

More information

Bilingual Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder from Newcomer Families

Bilingual Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder from Newcomer Families Bilingual Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder from Newcomer Families March 2018 Authors Research Partners Johanne Paradis Krithika Govindarajan Keren Hernandez Dept of Linguistics, University

More information

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Alex Rice M.Cl.Sc SLP Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/1176170/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Mindblind Eyes: An Absence of Spontaneous Theory of Mind in Asperger Syndrome Atsushi Senju,* Victoria Southgate, Sarah White,

More information

Language Comprehension Predicts Later Cognitive Ability and Symptom Severity in Toddlers with ASD

Language Comprehension Predicts Later Cognitive Ability and Symptom Severity in Toddlers with ASD Language Comprehension Predicts Later Cognitive Ability and Symptom Severity in Toddlers with ASD McGarry, E., Fiorello, K., Heldenberg, S., Reifler, A., Klaiman, C., Saulnier, C., & Lewis, M.. Marcus

More information

Narrative as a clinical assessment

Narrative as a clinical assessment Narrative as a clinical assessment NICOLA BOTTING LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION SCIENCE CITY UNIVERSITY LONDON Overview Background about narrative Different types of narrative Narrative in relation to diagnosis

More information

The effect of social stories on reduction of challenging behaviours in autistic children

The effect of social stories on reduction of challenging behaviours in autistic children Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 15 (2011) 351 355 WCES-2011 The effect of social stories on reduction of challenging behaviours in autistic children Ahmad

More information

COMPARATIVE STUDY OF EARLY CHILDHOOD HIGH- RECEPTIVE-EXPRESSIVE LANGUAGE DISORDER FUNCTION AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL MIXED

COMPARATIVE STUDY OF EARLY CHILDHOOD HIGH- RECEPTIVE-EXPRESSIVE LANGUAGE DISORDER FUNCTION AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL MIXED COMPARATIVE STUDY OF EARLY CHILDHOOD HIGH- FUNCTION AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL MIXED RECEPTIVE-EXPRESSIVE LANGUAGE DISORDER Pinchen Yang, Yuh-Jyh Jong, 1 Hsiu-Yi Hsu, 1 and Cheng-Sheng Chen Departments of

More information

Delayed language development is often the first symptom

Delayed language development is often the first symptom AJSLP Research Article Sentence Comprehension in Boys With Autism Spectrum Disorder Sara T. Kover, a Eileen Haebig, a Ashley Oakes, b Andrea McDuffie, b Randi J. Hagerman, b and Leonard Abbeduto b Purpose:

More information

Sign Language and Early Childhood Development

Sign Language and Early Childhood Development University of Arkansas, Fayetteville ScholarWorks@UARK Rehabilitation, Human Resources and Communication Disorders Undergraduate Honors Theses Rehabilitation, Human Resources and Communication Disorders

More information

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not.

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not. Autism Summary Autism What is Autism? The Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a developmental disability that can have significant implications on a child's ability to function and interface with the world

More information

Background on the issue Previous study with adolescents and adults: Current NIH R03 study examining ADI-R for Spanish speaking Latinos

Background on the issue Previous study with adolescents and adults: Current NIH R03 study examining ADI-R for Spanish speaking Latinos Sandy Magaña Background on the issue Previous study with adolescents and adults: brief description of study examining comparison between whites and Latinos in on the ADI-R Current NIH R03 study examining

More information

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Overview Diagnostic Criteria Current: Diagnostic & Statistical

More information

Early Interventions for ASD: State of the Science

Early Interventions for ASD: State of the Science Early Interventions for ASD: State of the Science Connie Kasari, Ph.D. University of California, Los Angeles Malaysian Regional Conference April 22-23 Today What is the evidence for current early interventions?

More information

Communication and Language: An Overview. Amanda Colvin, MS, CCC-SLP Hillary Mecham, MS, CCC-SLP

Communication and Language: An Overview. Amanda Colvin, MS, CCC-SLP Hillary Mecham, MS, CCC-SLP Communication and Language: An Overview Amanda Colvin, MS, CCC-SLP Hillary Mecham, MS, CCC-SLP Communication vs. Language Communication can be defined as: the transmission of thoughts or feelings from

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorders

Autism Spectrum Disorders Autism Spectrum Disorders ASD=Autism Spectrum Disorder EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICES IN THE SCHOOLS Impairments in communication Impairments in social interaction Restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Den Bosch: Boxpress

Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Den Bosch: Boxpress UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Parigger, E.M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language

More information

City, University of London Institutional Repository

City, University of London Institutional Repository City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Botting, N. & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2008). The role of language, social cognition, and social skill in the functional social

More information

2018 Gatlinburg Conference Symposium Submission SS-20

2018 Gatlinburg Conference Symposium Submission SS-20 Symposium Title: IDD in the Family: Insights from Observed Parent-Youth Interactions Chair: Bruce Baker 1 Discussant: Laura Lee McIntyre 2 Overview: Youth with intellectual and developmental disabilities

More information