on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

Similar documents
PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL'

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight

Sum of responding as a function of sum of reinforcement on two-key concurrent schedules

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14.

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

Herrnstein's equation for the rates of responding during concurrent schedules

Overview. Simple Schedules of Reinforcement. Important Features of Combined Schedules of Reinforcement. Combined Schedules of Reinforcement BEHP 1016

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research

Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of

ASYMMETRY OF REINFORCEMENT AND PUNISHMENT IN HUMAN CHOICE ERIN B. RASMUSSEN AND M. CHRISTOPHER NEWLAND

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

between successive DMTS choice phases.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Behavioural Processes

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

Reinforcer Magnitude and Demand under Fixed-Ratio Schedules with Domestic Hens. Alan Poling 2

Towards a behavioral theory of bias in signal detection

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

Law of effect models and choice between many alternatives

k/2. Thus, Re is measured in the same units

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action

Variation in Herrnsein s r<sub>0</sub> as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate

Stimulus control of topographically tagged responding

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT'

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination

OPTIMALITY AND CONCURRENT VARIABLE-INTERVAL VARIABLE-RATIO SCHEDULES WILLIAM M. BAUM AND CARLOS F. APARICIO

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A.

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

DUKE UNIVERSITY. stants. For concurrent VI VI, a and b are both. one (undermatching); for concurrent VI VR, b

Evidence for a Magnitude Effect in Temporal Discounting With Pigeons

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

NEW YORK UNIVERSITY. as in matching-to-sample or oddity performances, the rate of other, concurrently reinforced responses,

REPEATED MEASUREMENTS OF REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE EFFECTS ON GRADIENTS OF STIMULUS CONTROL' MICHAEL D. ZEILER

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

DRUG EFFECTS ON BEHAVIOR IN TRANSITION: DOES CONTEXT MATTER?

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

ing the fixed-interval schedule-were observed during the interval of delay. Similarly, Ferster

Instrumental Conditioning I

The Matching Law. Definitions. Herrnstein (1961) 20/03/15. Erik Arntzen HiOA Mars, 2015

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Chapter 12 Behavioral Momentum Theory: Understanding Persistence and Improving Treatment

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Schedule Induced Polydipsia: Effects of Inter-Food Interval on Access to Water as a Reinforcer

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1

EFFECTS OF D-AMPHETAMINE ON SIGNALED AND UNSIGNALED DELAYS TO REINFORCEMENT. Lee Davis Thomas

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers

UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 1972

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Duration of components and response rates on multiple fixed-ratio schedules

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning.

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

QUANTIFICATION OF DRUG CHOICE WITH THE GENERALIZED MATCHING LAW IN RHESUS MONKEYS MIKHAIL N. KOFFARNUS AND JAMES H. WOODS

On applying the matching law to between-subject data

AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS. Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

of behavioral contrast. To the extent that an account of contrast is a major concern, therefore, some alternative formulation seems desirable.

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand).

CONTROL OF IMPULSIVE CHOICE THROUGH BIASING INSTRUCTIONS. DOUGLAS J. NAVARICK California State University, Fullerton

On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013)

REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ATTENUATES APOMORPHINE S EFFECTS ON OPERANT PECKING JONATHAN W. PINKSTON AND R. J. LAMB

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

Concurrent Chains Schedules as a Method to Study Choice Between Alcohol Associated Conditioned Reinforcers

Operant matching. Sebastian Seung 9.29 Lecture 6: February 24, 2004

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Transcription:

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY OF AUCKLAND, NEW ZEALAND Five pigeons were trained on a variety of concurrent interval schedules that arranged reinforcements at either fixed or variable times after the last reinforcement. Two measures were obtained: the number of responses on each schedule, and the time spent responding on each schedule. Ratios of response rates on the two schedules did not equal ratios of reinforcement rates when both schedules were variable nor when one was variable and the other fixed. Ratios of times spent responding approximately equalled ratios of reinforcement rates when both schedules were variable, but did not do so when one was fixed. Key words: concurrent schedules, VI schedules, FI schedules, response allocation, time allocation, pecking, pigeons Reliable functional relations between performance and reinforcement parameters on concurrent interval schedules have been summarized by the matching principle (Herrnstein, 1970; Rachlin, 1971), which states that the ratio of response rates or times allocated to responding on each schedule is a function of the ratio of reinforcement rates on each schedule. The general relation (White and Davison, 1973) may be expressed: Pi T, IC R, (1) P1 (rt1 /cr1\a (1 P2 rt2 where P1 and P2 are the number of responses on the two schedules, T1 and T2 are the times allocated to the two schedules, and R1 and R. are the numbers of reinforcements obtained on the two schedules in a session. For concurrent variable-interval variable-interval (conc VI VI) schedules (Baum and Rachlin, 1969), and concurrent fixed-interval fixed-interval (conc Fl FI) schedules in which typical FI or VI response patterns are maintained on both schedules (White and Davison, 1973), both c and a are close to 1.0. For conc Fl VI schedules (Nevin, 1971; Trevett, Davison, and Williams, 1972), c (and possibly a) are less than 1.0 when 'Part of this experiment was submitted by the first author toward the degree of Master of Arts at Auckland University. The work was supported by grants AG 140 PSY 14 and AG 141 PSY 8 from the University Grants Committee to the second author. We thank the cooperative of Stage 3, Masters and Doctoral students who helped conduct this experiment. Reprints and tables of individual data may be obtained from either author, Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Private Bag, Auckland, New Zealand. schedule I is the Fl schedule. White and Davison (1973) also found that where typical Fl or VI-like response patterns were not maintained on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. Trevett et al. obtained a constant proportional preference for the VI schedule over the FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. The value of a was the same for both the conc Fl VI data and conc VI VI control conditions, but the value of c was different by a constant amount between experimental and control conditions. The present experiment systematically replicated the two previous experiments on conc Fl VI performance (Nevin, 1971; Trevett et al., 1972). It differed from these experiments in that the concurrent schedules were arranged according to the switching procedure (Findley, 1958), rather than the two-key procedure. METHOD Subjects Five homing pigeons, numbered 21, 23, 24, 25, and 26, were maintained at 80% + 15 g of their free-feeding weights. The birds had previously been trained on conc Fl Fl schedules (White and Davison, 1973). Apparatus The experimental chamber, situated remote from conventional relay control equipment, was sound-attenuated and external noise was masked by an exhaust fan. A food hopper was situated 10 cm from the grid floor, midway between two translucent response keys 2 cm in 191

192 BRENDA LOBB and M. C. DA VISON diameter, 13 cm apart, and 22.5 cm from the floor. Both keys could be transilluminated by colored lights. Pecks on lighted keys exceeding about 0.1 N closed a microswitch behind the keys and provided auditory feedback from a relay inside the chamber. The equipment was that used by White and Davison (1973). Procedure As all subjects had extensive histories of training on concurrent schedules, no key-peck or schedule training was necessary. The animals were placed directly onto the first experimental condition (Table 1). Concurrent schedules were arranged according to the changeover method (Findley, 1958) in which a response on the left-hand (white) key alternated schedules and their associated (red or green) stimuli on the right-hand key. The two keys are known as the changeover and main keys respectively. The schedules arranged on the main key in the various parts of the experiment are shown in Table 1. In all conditions, the concurrent schedules were mutually independent and were operative simultaneously, so that the animals had continuous access via the changeover key to both schedules and their associated main-key stimuli. Bursts of responding on the changeover key were discouraged by arranging that changeover responses were effective only after at least one response on the main key following a changeover key peck. A changeover delay (Herrnstein, 1961) specified that a response on the main key could not be reinforced, even if a reinforcement had been arranged by the schedule, until 3 sec had elapsed after any response to the changeover key. The number of reinforcements per session was varied in the different experimental conditions to maintain a session time of not more than 50 min (Table 1). Supplementary feed of mixed grain was given, when necessary, immediately after daily training sessions. During reinforcement, which consisted of 3-sec access to wheat, the magazine was illuminated and the keys were dark and inoperative. All VI schedules used were derived from arithmetic progressions of the form a, a + d, a + 2d, etc., and were composed of 12 randomized intervals. In all conditions, the value of the shortest interval was 10 sec. For all conditions, the number of responses, the time spent responding, the number of reinforcements obtained on each schedule, and the number of effective changeovers, were recorded. Daily response data were calculated as the relative number of responses to the red main key (responses on the red main key divided by total responses to the main key). Conditions were changed when all animals had reached a stability criterion five, not necessarily consecutive, times. To satisfy the criterion, it was necessary that the median relative number of responses for five sessions did not differ by more than 0.05 from the median of the previous five sessions. RESULTS All measures of response allocation, time allocation, number of reinforcements, and number of changeovers between the schedules were obtained from the final five sessions of each experimental condition. These data are shown as single-session means in Table 1. Ratios of numbers of responses to the two schedules are shown in Figure 1 for both the individual animals and the group as a function of the ratio of obtained reinforcement rates on the two schedules. Both coordinates are logarithmic (Baum and Rachlin, 1969), and the data from conc Fl VI and conc VI VI schedules are shown separately. Straight lines were fitted to the logarithmic data by the method of least squares, and the equations of the best-fitting lines are shown in the figure. If the ratio of response rates on the two schedules equalled the ratio of reinforcement rates on the two schedules (c = a = 1.0 in Equation 1), the fitted line would have a slope of 1.0 and a logarithmic intercept of 0.0. Neither set of data from this experiment is close to this equality. In analyzing the present results, Sign Tests (Siegel, 1956) were used to determine whether differences in slopes or intercepts were significant. The Sign Test requires that all five comparisons of individual animal data be in the same direction for significance at p = 0.05. On this criterion, the slopes of the fitted lines (and thus the value of a required in Equation 1) were significantly less than 1.0. Further, the slopes of the fitted lines to the conc Fl VI data were significantly smaller than those fitted to the conc VI VI data. Analysis of the size of the intercepts to the fitted lines (the logarithm of c in Equation 1) shows that they were significantly smaller for the conc Fl VI data than for the conc VI VI data. All comparisons not spe-

CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES 193 Table 1 Sequence of experimental conditions, number of sessions training, number of responses emitted and time allocated, number of changeovers, and number of reinforcements obtained on the two concurrent schedules. All these data are averaged over the last five sessions of training for the five birds. All time data are in seconds. Main Key Schedules No. of Responses Times Reinforcements Change- Red Green Sessions Red Green Red Green Red Green overs FI 120 VI 120 26 1419 2240 1121 1430 21.9 23.1 68 FI 120 VI 30 19 230 1264 193 784 8.5 41.5 51 Fl 120 VI 240 23 1860 1405 1375 963 22.3 10.7 86 Fl 120 VI 60 21 738 1570 432 1009 12.8 27.2 79 Fl 120 VI 480 25 2200 1103 1677 810 24.0 6.0 71 VI 240 VI 60 17 804 2351 389 1753 9.4 40.6 78 VI 60 VI 240 18 2173 773 1677 426 40.4 9.6 76 Fl 300 VI 75 20 545 1931 285 1509 6.8 28.2 59 Fl 75 VI 300 27 1405 990 1026 754 27.9 7.1 65 VI 300 VI 75 21 363 1676 184 1337 5.6 29.5 37 VI 75 VI 300 23 1681 487 1243 268 29.6 5.4 48 VI 120 VI 30 18 433 1118 232 850 9.5 40.5 53 VI 120 VI 480 25 2322 622 2668 411 24.0 6.0 62 VI 120 VI 60 20 693 1467 387 1071 13.1 26.9 74 VI 120 VI 240 22 2033 1118 1477 682 22.4 10.6 89 VI 180 VI 90 22 823 1758 496 1381 11.4 23.6 71 VI 90 VI 180 20 1638 813 1300 562 23.9 11.1 65 Fl 180 VI 90 21 876 1603 546 1319 11.3 23.7 81 FI 90 VI 180 20 1315 1109 926 860 23.5 11.5 93 cifically mentioned are not significant according to the Sign Test. The data on time spent responding on the two schedules were analyzed in the same way as the response data, and the ratio of times allocated as a function of the ratio of obtained reinforcement rates on logarithmic coordinates is shown in Figure 2. Again, straight lines were fitted to the data by the method of least squares. For the time-allocation measures, the slopes of the lines fitted to the conc VI VI data are close to, but significantly greater than 1.0; those fitted to the conc Fl VI data are significantly smaller than 1.0 (Sign Tests). Hence, the difference between the slopes of the lines fitted to these data (conc VI VI and conc Fl VI) is also significant. As for the response data, the size of the intercept to the fitted lines was significantly smaller for the conc Fl VI data than for the conc VI VI data. The slopes of the lines fitted to the time-allocation data were significantly steeper than those fitted to the response-allocation data for both types of concurrent schedules, although there was no significant difference in the values of the intercepts obtained from time and response measures. DISCUSSION The present results on performance in conc Fl VI schedules, as compared to performance in conc VI VI schedules, broadly replicate previous results on response and time allocation (Nevin, 1971; Trevett et al., 1972), even though the procedure for concurrent scheduling used here was different from those used previously. But certain important details of the results are different. The slopes of the functions relating log response-rate ratios to log reinforcement-rate ratios for conc Fl VI performance, and thus the required value of a in Equation 1, were found by Nevin (1971) to be about 0.5, by Trevett et al. to be about 0.62, and in the present experiment to be about 0.68. We shall take these data as estimates of an exponent, a, for conc Fl VI performance of 0.63, the mean value of 11 animals in these experiments. Trevett et al. reported that the functions relating log response-rate ratios to log reinforcement-rate ratios were displaced below the origin (0,0). For example, when Fl and VI schedules with the same mean interval were concurrently arranged, the animals responded more on the VI schedule than on the Fl schedule. Such a result gives a value of c in Equa-

194 BRENDA LOBB and M. C. DAVISON o CONC. VI VI x CONC. Fl VI 0 wcky I- w flj z 0 w ck: 0 -J -0.5 0 05-05 0 0.5 LOG REINFORCEMENT RATE RATIO Fig. 1. The logarithm of the ratio of response rates on each schedule as a function of the logarithm of the ratio of the reinforcement rates obtained on each schedule. Data for conc Fl VI schedules and conc VI VI schedules are shown separately, and straight lines were fitted to each by the method of least squares. The equation of these lines, in logarithmic terms, is shown by each. tion of less than 1.0 when schedule is the FI schedule. Nevin found such a displacement for only one bird, and a displacement in the other direction for one of the other two birds. On this point, the present data strongly support Trevett et al.'s results, the logarithmic intercepts for each bird being negative. The absolute size of the obtained intercept may be influenced by a bias toward one or other key or key color (Baum and Rachlin, 1969), and the amount of bias may be obtained from the intercept of the line fitted to the response data for the conc VI VI conditions (Figure 1). For the present data, this bias was very small, hav-

CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES 195 0 0.5-0000 05y* 1.07 0.05 ' x < ~~~~~~~~x 0 cr. * 0.71x - 0411 xx~~~ -0.5, u.79x 0-0i12 30.5 0 X ox GROUP 0 24 ~~~~o0.5 0.5p0 y n 1.07x -002/0 y a 1.08x - 0.01, 0[ x X 0 y 0.72xx-0.16 x yo0.65x -015 _o0. 0~~~~~~~~~ 0~~~~~~~~~ -0.5 0 005-0.5 0 O*5 LOG REINFORCEMENT RATE RATIO Fig. 2. The logarithm of the ratio of times allocated to responding to each schedule as a function of the logarithm of the ratio of reinforcement rates obtained on each schedule. Data for conc FI VI schedules and conc VI VI schedules are shown separately, and straight lines were fitted to each by the method of least squares. The equation of these lines, in logarithmic terms, is shown by each.

196 BRENDA LOBB and M. C. DAVISON ing a logarithmic value of -0.01. The negative sign shows that the bias was toward the green key color. Thus, the true intercept of the fitted line to the conc Fl VI response data is (-0.15 - (-0.01)), which equals -0.14, showing that the negative intercept found for the present data is not a result of stimulus bias. For Trevett et al.'s data, a similar calculation gives a value of -0.15. The agreement between these two results is excellent, but both seem incompatible with those reported by Nevin. We can only suggest that Nevin's results were affected by an unmeasured key bias. On the basis of these results, it seems that conc Fl VI schedule response allocation may be characterized by the following form of Equation 1: =-0 72~I.(2) P2 R2( The value of c = 0.72 is the antilog of the logarithmic intercepts found here and by Trevett et al. Trevett et al. reported that straight lines fitted to the logarithmic response and reinforcement rate ratios for conc VI VI schedules did not have the expected slope of 1.0 (a = 1.0 in Equation 1). Schneider (1973) also obtained a slope of less than 1.0 in conc VI VI schedules. The same was found in the present results, although the slope was nearer 1.0 than was found by Trevett et al. (0.82 versus 0.69) or Schneider (0.6). Like Trevett et al., we are unable to explain why the slope was not 1.0, as would be expected from many previous reports. Schneider noted that procedural differences do not appear to account for the discrepancy. We would like to point out, however, that response allocation slopes of less than 1.0 may be the rule rather than the exception. An analysis of previously reported data on conc VI VI response allocation (Catania, 1963; Herrnstein, 1961; Hollard and Davison, 1971; Schneider, 1973; Shull and Pliskoff, 1967; Silberberg and Fantino, 1970; and Trevett et al., 1972) showed that 22 of the 26 individual animals used in these experiments gave a value of a in Equation 1 of less than 1.0. The other four showed values close to, or somewhat above, 1.0. This clearly shows that the normally reported equality between response ratios and reinforcement-rate ratios (Herrnstein, 1961; 1970) is incorrect, and is due mainly to a reliance on graphs in which relative measures (responses or reinforcements on a key divided by total responses or reinforcements) are shown. A slight ogival deviation on such graphs may represent a considerable deviation from unit slope on more sensitive log ratio coordinates (cf., Baum, 1974). It is of interest to examine how overall response rates on each key changed between the conc Fl VI and the conc VI VI conditions. Previous research with single schedules (Schneider, 1969) showed that overall rates are lower for Fl schedules, as compared with VI schedules providing the same overall reinforcement rate. Such an effect is not found in the present data. Figure 3 shows the overall response rates (number of responses divided by total session time) on each schedule for all conditions in which the first schedule was either Fl 120-sec or VI 120-sec. As would be expected (Catania, 1963), the response rate on each schedule is a direct function of the reinforcement rate on that schedule, and an inverse function of the reinforcement rate on the alternate, concurrent schedule. Figure 3 shows that the response rate on the Fl 120-sec schedules was similar to that on the VI 120-sec schedules when these were concurrent with various VI schedules. However, the response rate on the concurrent VI schedule in which the reinforcement rate was varied was higher when the alternate schedule Conc. Fl 120-sec (C) VI x-sec (o) Conc. VI 120-sec (a) VI x-sec (a) 80r z 60 w a-40 0. tn w ck20 I 0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 RFTS. PER MIN. ON VARIED SCHEDULE Fig. 3. The overall response rate on each schedule (number of responses divided by total session time) as a function of the reinforcement rate on the varied schedule. The data used were those obtained when one schedule was either VI 120-sec or Fl 120-sec, and the concurrently arranged VI schedule was varied.

CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES 197 was an Fl than when it was a VI schedule. In other words, response ratios on conc VI VI schedules differ from those on conc Fl VI schedules because, in the latter case, animals respond more on the VI schedule, rather than less on the Fl schedule, in comparison with the former case. The present data on time allocation in conc Fl VI schedules also replicate in general the results of Trevett et al. The slope of the function relating log time-allocation ratios to log reinforcement-rate ratios (Figure 2) is 0.72, compared with the value of 0.66 reported by Trevett et al. The log intercepts of the bestfitting lines are less than zero in both experiments, -0.28 for Trevett et al. and -0.16 for the present data. Taking the mean values for the nine birds in these two experiments, we obtain the following form of Equation 1: T- = 0.59 (R) * (3) Comparison of Equations (2) and (3), which are averages across experiments, suggests that response and time measures in conc Fl VI performance may be (1) equivalent with respect to the value of a, but (2) not so with respect to the value of c. But, since neither of these conclusions is supported by the present data, we will take both response and time measures of conc Fl VI performance as giving an equally accurate estimate of both a and c. The functions relating log time-allocation ratios to log reinforcement-rate ratios for the conc VI VI data gave a slope slightly greater than 1.0, and greater than the slope found for the conc FI VI data. Again, investigation of data previously reported on time allocation in conc VI VI schedules showed that slopes greater than 1.0 often occurred. The present data do not support the conclusion that conc Fl VI performance differs from conc VI VI performance only in that c =, 1.0 in the former case (Trevett et al., 1972). The value of a in Equation 1, which may be interpreted as a measure of sensitivity of the dependent variable to the independent variable, is larger for conc VI VI performance. The value of a is also greater for time-allocation measures than response-allocation measures, both here and in many previous studies (Catania, 1963; Hollard and Davison, 1971; Shull and Pliskoff, 1967; Silberberg and Fantino, 1970; Trevett et al., 1972). In view of the use to which the simple matching law (response ratio equals reinforcement ratio) has been put, for instance as a yardstick for concurrent-chains research, it is clear that more detailed research is needed on concurrent interval schedule performance. REFERENCES Baum, W. M. On two types of deviation from the matching law: bias and undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 22, 231-242. Baum, W. M. and Rachlin, H. C. Choice as time allocation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 861-874. Catania, A. C. Concurrent performances: reinforcement interaction and response independence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 253-263. Findley, J. D. Preference and switching under concurrent scheduling. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1958, 1, 123-144. Herrnstein, R. J. Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1961, 4, 267-272. Herrnstein, R. J. On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1970, 13, 243-266. Hollard, V. D. and Davison, M. C. Preference for qualitatively different reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 16, 375-380. Nevin, J. A. Rates and patterns of responding with concurrent fixed-interval and variable-interval reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 16, 241-247. Rachlin, H. C. On the tautology of the matching law. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1971, 15, 249-251. Schneider, B. A. A two-state analysis of fixed-interval responding in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimnental Analysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 677-687. Schneider, J. W. Reinforcer effectiveness as a function of reinforcer rate and magnitude: a comparison of concurrent performances. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1973, 20, 461-471. Shull, R. L. and Pliskoff, S. S. Changeover delay and concurrent schedules: some effects on relative performance measures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1967, 10, 517-527. Siegel, S. Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1956. Silberberg, A. and Fantino, E. Choice, rate of reinforcement, and the changeover delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1970, 13, 187-197. Trevett, A. J., Davison, M. C., and Williams, R. J. Performance in concurrent interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1972, 17, 369-374. White, A. J. and Davison, M. C. Performance in concurrent fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1973, 19, 147-153. Received 18 March 1974. (Final Acceptance 10 April 1975.)