ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Influence of Compulsivity of Drug Abuse on Dopaminergic Modulation of Attentional Bias in Stimulant Dependence

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Influence of Compulsivity of Drug Abuse on Dopaminergic Modulation of Attentional Bias in Stimulant Dependence"

Transcription

1 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Influence of Compulsivity of Drug Abuse on Dopaminergic Modulation of Attentional Bias in Stimulant Dependence Karen D. Ersche, PhD, CPsychol; Edward T. Bullmore, MB, FRCPsych, PhD, FMedSci; Kevin J. Craig, MBBCh, MRCPsych; Shaila S. Shabbir, BSc; Sanja Abbott, PhD; Ulrich Müller, MD, PhD; Cinly Ooi, PhD; John Suckling, PhD; Anna Barnes, PhD; Barbara J. Sahakian, PhD, DClinPsych, FMedSci; Emilio V. Merlo-Pich, MD; Trevor W. Robbins, PhD, FMedSci, FRS Context: Therearenoeffectivepharmacotherapiesforstimulant dependence but there are many plausible targets for development of novel therapeutics. We hypothesized that dopamine-related targets are relevant for treatment of stimulant dependence, and there will likely be individual differences in response to dopaminergic challenges. Objective: To measure behavioral and brain functional markers of drug-related attentional bias in stimulant-dependent individuals studied repeatedly after shortterm dosing with dopamine D 2 /D 3 receptor antagonist and agonist challenges. Design: Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-groups, crossover design using pharmacological functional magnetic resonance imaging. Setting: Clinical research unit (GlaxoSmithKline) and local community in Cambridge, England. Participants: Stimulant-dependent individuals (n=18) and healthy volunteers (n=18). Interventions: Amisulpride (400 mg), pramipexole dihydrochloride (0.5 mg), or placebo were administered in counterbalanced order at each of 3 repeated testing sessions. Main Outcome Measures: Attentional bias for stimulant-related words was measured during functional magnetic resonance imaging by a drug-word Stroop paradigm; trait impulsivity and compulsivity of dependence were assessed at baseline by questionnaire. Results: Drug users demonstrated significant attentional bias for drug-related words, which was correlated with greater activation of the left prefrontal and right cerebellar cortex. Attentional bias was greater in people with highly compulsive patterns of stimulant abuse; the effects of dopaminergic challenges on attentional interference and related frontocerebellar activation were different between high- and low-compulsivity subgroups. Conclusions: Greater attentional bias for and greater prefrontal activation by stimulant-related words constitute a candidate neurocognitive marker for dependence. Individual differences in compulsivity of stimulant dependence had significant effects on attentional bias, its brain functional representation, and its short-term modulation by dopaminergic challenges. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(6): Author Affiliations are listed at the end of this article. CHRONIC STIMULANT DRUG abuse is associated with major adverse health, social, legal, and economic consequences. 1-3 Yet the pharmacological and psychological treatments available for stimulant dependence arecharacterizedbyhighdropoutandrelapse rates. 4-6 Thereisaclearneedtoimprovetreatment efficacy for the growing number of stimulant-dependent individuals. 3 Advances in neuroimaging research have contributed to a better understanding of the neurobiological basis of stimulant dependence and have indicated some plausible targets for pharmacological intervention. On the hypothesis that stimulant dependence may be associated with a loss of topdown inhibitory control over lower-level reward processing, 7,8 several studies have investigated brain and behavioral responses to tasks, such as the color-word Stroop paradigm, that entail attentional control to override an automatic but incorrect response tendency In studies of stimulant dependence, a drug-related version of the Stroop test has been used to measure the degree of involuntary attention, or attentional bias, toward drug-related words compared with neutral words The increased salience of drug-related cues, as measured by attentional bias in drug-word Stroop paradigms, is thought to contribute to the maintenance of drug-taking behavior 7,24 and has demonstrated predictive value for the risk of relapse. 12,21,25-27 The exact neurochemical mechanisms underpinning attentional bias to 632

2 drug-related stimuli in stimulant-dependent individuals (SDIs) are unknown, but attentional bias on the Stroop paradigm is responsive to dopaminergic modulation Dopamine is generally released in response to reward-predicting or salient events and also in response to drug-related cues in drug-dependent individuals. 34,35 Accumulating evidence indicates that striatal dopamine systems are disrupted in stimulant dependence For example, studies using raclopride positron emission tomography have shown that SDIs have a significantly lower binding potential of striatal dopamine D 2 receptors, which has usually been interpreted as a marker of reduced postsynaptic receptor density. 42 Reduced striatal dopamine receptor density appears to be directly related to decreased metabolic activity in the orbitofrontal cortex, 39,40 a brain area implicated in compulsive behavior and craving. 43 Collectively, these studies suggest that attentional bias predisposing to stimulant dependence could be related to abnormal dopamine transmission and could therefore be treated by drugs targeting dopamine receptors. There are rationales for both dopamine agonist and antagonist drugs, and both have been tried, but the findings are inconsistent, 5,44-47 in most cases providing no more than weak evidence for therapeutic efficacy. One possible explanation for this discrepancy between the theoretical strength of the target and the apparent lack of clinical benefit is that stimulant dependence is a heterogeneous syndrome. There are important psychological dimensions of difference between individuals, which may modulate their response to treatment and attenuate the power of clinical trials involving small numbers of psychologically undifferentiated SDIs. Two psychological dimensions of particular interest in this respect are impulsivity and compulsivity: both are known to be abnormal in SDIs compared with nondependent people, 24,48-50 and both have been linked to dopaminergic mechanisms Impulsivity can be defined as loss of inhibitory control over the response to rewarding or distracting stimuli and may play a role as both a cause and a consequence of substance abuse 55,56 ; compulsivity is defined as persistence or perseverance of behavior in the absence of reward or despite punishment and may be particularly important in the maintenance of drug-taking habits. 54 Both impulsivity and drug-related compulsivity have been linked with alterations in the dopamine system 52,57,58 and are thought to account for the variability of dopaminergic drug effects in SDIs. 51 Herein, we tested 2 hypothetical predictions: (1) attentional bias for drug-related cues, and related brain activation, would be abnormal in people with stimulant dependence and (2) the effects of dopaminergic challenge drugs on attentional bias and related brain systems would be influenced by individual differences in impulsivity and compulsivity among a group of SDIs. We conducted a doubleblind, placebo-controlled, pharmacological functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) study to evaluate the effects on drug-word Stroop task performance of single doses of a dopamine D 2 /D 3 receptor agonist (pramipexole dihydrochloride, 0.5 mg) and a D 2 /D 3 antagonist (amisulpride, 400 mg) administered in a within-subjects crossover design to SDIs and healthy comparison volunteers. METHODS PARTICIPANTS Thirty-six right-handed volunteers participated in this study: 18 individuals with a chronic history of stimulant drug abuse, satisfying the DSM-IV-TR 59 criteria for dependence on either crack cocaine (n=5), cocaine (n=5), or amphetamines (n=8) and 18 matched healthy controls. Stimulant-dependent individuals were non treatment seeking and recruited from the local community. Prior to study enrolment, all participants had a satisfactory medical review and were screened for any other current Axis I psychiatric disorder using the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders. 60 Participants were excluded on the basis of any current Axis I psychiatric disorder, other than substance dependence in SDIs. Participants were also screened for normal appearance of structural MRI scans ( /profile.php?ke220). All volunteers provided written informed consent and received monetary compensation for participation. The study was approved by the Cambridge Research Ethics Committee (REC06/Q0108/130; principal investigator, T.W.R.). SHORT-TERM DOPAMINERGIC DRUG TREATMENTS Amisulpride is a selective antagonist at the dopamine D 2 /D 3 receptors. We administered a single oral dose of 400 mg of amisulpride; no serious adverse events were reported. Pramipexole is a selective agonist at the dopamine D 2 /D 3 receptors 61 ;we administered a single oral dose of 1.5 mg of pramipexole dihydrochloride to the first 6 participants enrolled in the study (3 SDIs, 3 nondependent volunteers). However, this dose of pramipexole was poorly tolerated in the 3 nondependent volunteers, who were unable to perform the tasks at this treatment session because of nausea, vomiting, sweating, and tiredness. Subsequently, the dose of pramipexole dihydrochloride was reduced to 0.5 mg orally for all participants (ie, for 18 controls [including the 3 volunteers who did not tolerate the higher dose] and 15 SDIs). All participants were also administered 30 mg of domperidone orally at each treatment session to prevent emetic effects of dopamine receptor agonism ( BASELINE ASSESSMENTS All participants completed the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale as a measure of self-reported impulsivity. In SDIs, drug-related compulsivity was assessed using the Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale, 62,63 an equivalent to the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale 64 of symptom severity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale measures the amount of time, interference, and distress caused by stimulantrelated thoughts or urges and the efforts to resist them. Obsessive-compulsive tendencies in controls were assessed by the Yale- Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale ( ATTENTIONAL CONTROL TASKS We used 2 versions of the Stroop task as tests of attentional control, based on the paradigm used by Compton et al, 65 in which participants were asked to indicate the font color of a word displayed on the screen using a 4-button box. Each button corresponded to 1 of 4 possible font colors: red, blue, yellow, or 633

3 A Drug-word Stroop task B Color-word Stroop task change sniff coke charlie Drug words blue green red yellow Color words piano song flute trumpet Neutral words dog bear tiger monkey Neutral words C Study design Drug Fixation Color Stimulant words Neutral words Incongruent color words Neutral words Rating of current cravings Fixation Fixation Fixation Fixation Rating of current cravings Scanning time Figure 1. Schematic of the Stroop paradigms. A, In the drug-word Stroop task, participants were asked to identify the font colors of the drug-related words and the neutral words. B, In the color-word Stroop task, participants were asked to identify the font colors of the color words and the neutral words. C, Blocks of drug-related words, color words (which were always incongruent with their font color), and neutral words were interspersed with s of fixation for task presentation during functional magnetic resonance imaging. green. The drug-word Stroop task involved 2 lists of target words: 16 amphetamine-related and 16 cocaine-related words. Both lists were matched with a list of 16 neutral words with regard to length and frequency. Equivalent to the drug-word version, the color-word Stroop task also involved 2 lists of color words, each including 8 distinct color words that were presented twice per. Each color word was always displayed in a font color incongruent with its meaning. Both lists of color words were matched with neutral words for length and frequency (http: // Both the drug-word and the color-word Stroop tasks were presented as a ed periodic paradigm during fmri (Figure 1). This design was chosen because interference effects in emotional Stroop paradigms are thought to rely on a slow disengagement process 66 that generalizes to drug-related words, 67 and the design has therefore been the method of choice in many prior studies investigating attentional interference in an emotional context. 65,68-70 For each word trial, the interstimulus interval was 2.2 seconds, including the presentation of a fixation cross for 0.3 second followed by the presentation of a word for 1.9 seconds, during which time the participants had to register a response by button press. Each of words lasted 35.2 seconds, and 2 s of each word type were followed by a 38-second fixation (Figure 1). The order of the words within each was randomized and the order of the s was counterbalanced across participants and testing sessions. The allocation of the font colors to the words was randomized; each of the 4 colors occurred equally often in each word category. All participants were trained on the task prior to each scanning session with a practice run of at least 1 color-word and 1 of different neutral words to familiarize themselves with the task and to minimize practice effects. Acute Stroop task induced stimulant cravings were measured immediately before and after the Stroop task by asking the following question: How would you rate your current state of craving on a scale from 0 to 100? Task performance was measured in terms of accuracy and latency of response to each trial. Attentional bias for drugrelated words was quantified by an interference score, calculated for each individual as the median latency of correct responses to the drug-related words minus the median latency of correct responses to the neutral words. Likewise, attentional interference in the color-word Stroop task was calculated as the median latency of correct responses to color words minus the median latency of correct responses to neutral words. The median is generally more robust than the mean as a measure of central location in small samples; however, the mean interference scores were also calculated for comparative purposes (Table 1 and Table 2). ACQUISITION OF fmri DATA Whole-brain fmri data were acquired at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, using a Siemens Magnetom Tim Trio whole-body scanner operating at 3 T(Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany). During the performance of each Stroop task, 32 transaxial sections of gradient echo, echoplanar imaging data depicting blood oxygen level dependent contrast were acquired parallel to the intercommissural line with the following parameters: repetition time=2000 milliseconds, echo time=30 milliseconds, flip angle=78, slice thickness=3 mm plus 0.75-mm interslice gap, image matrix size=64 64, and within-plane voxel dimensions=3.0 mm 3.0 mm. Prior to data analysis, the first 5 echoplanar images were discarded to account for T1 equilibration effects. ANALYSIS OF DEMOGRAPHIC, PSYCHOMETRIC, AND BEHAVIORAL DATA Using data from both groups, attentional interference scores, error, and craving data were each analyzed separately using a repeated-measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) model with drug treatment (3 levels: placebo, amisulpride, and pramipexole) as the within-subject factor and group (2 levels: nondependent volunteers and SDIs) as the between-subject factor. Using data from the patient group only, attentional interference was also analyzed using a repeated-measures ANCOVA model with drug treatment (3 levels: placebo, amisulpride, and pramipexole) as the within-subject factor and compulsivity subgroup (2 levels: high and low) as the between-subject factor. In all analyses of attentional interference scores, only data on 634

4 Table 1. Task Performance on the Drug- Stroop and Color- Stroop Tests in Stimulant-Dependent Individuals and Healthy Comparison Volunteers a Dependent Variable Effect df F P Value Drug-word Stroop task Attentional interference score (median latency) Drug 2, Drug group 2, Group 1, b Attentional interference score (mean latency) Drug 2, Drug group 2, Group 1, b Error rate, % Drug 2, Drug group 2, b Group 1, Color-word Stroop task Attentional interference score (median latency) Drug 2, b Drug group 2, Group 1, Attentional interference score (mean latency) Drug 2, Drug group 2, Group 1, Error rate, % Drug 2, Drug group 2, Group 1, Abbreviation: BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory II. 71 a Analysis of covariance modeling of factorial effects on attentional interference and accuracy (covariates: BDI-II total scores, years of education, and plasma levels of pramipexole dihydrochloride). b Significant. Table 2. Task Performance on the Drug- Stroop Test in High- and Low-Compulsivity and High- and Low-Impulsivity Subgroups of Stimulant-Dependent Individuals a Dependent Variable on Drug- Stroop Task Effect df F P Value Compulsivity Attentional interference score (median latency) Drug 2, Drug group 2, b Group 1, b Attentional interference score (mean latency) Drug 2, Drug group 2, b Group 1, b Error rate, % Drug 1.5, Drug group 1.5, Group 1, Impulsivity Attentional interference score (median latency) Drug 2, Drug group 2, Group 1, Attentional interference score (mean latency) Drug 2, Drug group 2, Group 1, Error rate, % Drug 1.5, Drug group 1.5, Group 1, a Analysis of covariance modeling of factorial effects on attentional interference and accuracy (covariate: plasma levels of pramipexole dihydrochloride). b Significant. correctly performed trials were included. To control for group differences in depressive mood, education, and dose of pramipexole, we included the Beck Depression Inventory scores, years of education, and plasma levels of pramipexole as covariates in all analyses involving SDIs and controls; the subgroup analyses (involving SDIs only) included plasma levels of pramipexole as a covariate. Statistical tests were conducted using SPSS (version 13; SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois) and were reported as significant if P.05. ANALYSIS OF fmri DATA Details of the fmri data analysis procedures are briefly summarized herein (full details, /directory/profile.php?ke220). We first identified by wholebrain analysis those brain regions where functional activation was specifically related to the contrast of drug-related vs neutral words and strongly correlated with the behavioral measure of attentional interference (as defined earlier). At each voxel of 635

5 A 250 Behavioral performance during drug-word Stroop task Amisulpride Placebo Pramipexole B Anterior 200 Median Interference Score, ms ± 1 SE z = 10 z = 8 0 z = Controls Drug Users z = 14 Posterior C Correlation between attentional bias for drug-words and brain activation D 40 Task-related brain activation during drug-work Stroop task Amisulpride Placebo Pramipexole Median Interference Score, ms Mean BOLD Response ± 1 SE r = 0.69, P < BOLD Response During Drug- Stroop Task 0 Controls Drug Users Figure 2. Behavioral performance during the drug-word Stroop paradigm and task-related activation of a frontocerebellar system associated with attentional bias for drug words (in all participants). A, Drug users showed a significant attentional bias for drug-related words, as reflected in higher attentional interference scores compared with the healthy comparison volunteers. Attentional bias was measured by each volunteer s median response latency of correctly identified colors of drug-related words minus the median response latency of correctly identified colors of matched neutral words. Pramipexole was given as pramipexole dihydrochloride. B, The red voxels indicate brain regions activated by the contrast between drug-related words and neutral words; yellow voxels indicate brain regions within this system where activation was positively correlated with attentional interference scores on the drug-word Stroop task. C, Scatterplot of median attentional interference score (y-axis) vs functional activation of the brain regions associated with attentional bias for drug words (x-axis), which are the left ventral prefrontal cortex (Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates [x, y, z] in millimeters: 46, 26, 12; 44, 22, 8; and 40, 6, 30) and right cerebellum (22, 80, 40). The spatial coordinates refer to the peak voxel where the effect size is greatest. BOLD indicates blood oxygen level dependent. D, Comparison of mean task-related (BOLD) activation in brain regions associated with attentional bias between stimulant-dependent individuals and healthy volunteers. Stimulant users show overactivation in the left ventral prefrontal cortex and right cerebellum compared with controls. these behaviorally relevant frontal and cerebellar regions, we tested the factorial effects of diagnostic group and dopaminergic drug challenge by ANCOVA using all data on both groups. Finally, we also tested each voxel of this frontocerebellar system to investigate factorial effects of compulsivity (high or low) and dopaminergic drug challenge by ANCOVA in the patient group only. These anatomically focused factorial analyses of the fmri data are formally analogous to parallel factorial analyses of behavioral data on attentional interference (described earlier). All data sets from both Stroop tasks for SDIs and controls under all drug conditions were initially preprocessed to correct for effects of subject motion, differential slice timing, and differences in global means At the first single-subject level of analysis, a time series regression model specified 2 key contrasts between correctly performed trials: (1) drug-related words vs neutral words in the drug-word s and (2) color words vs neutral words in the color-word s. These contrasts are equivalent to those used to generate attentional interference 636

6 Table 3. Demographic, Psychological, and Baseline Personality Measures for the Groups of 18 Stimulant-Dependent Individuals and 18 Healthy Volunteers Mean (SD) [range] Group Healthy Comparison Volunteers Stimulant-Dependent Individuals F df P Value Age, y 32.7 (6.9) 34.3 (7.2) , Sex ratio (male:female) 15:3 15:3 a a.99 Ethnic ratio (white:afro Caribbean) b 17:1 16:2 a a.99 Employment ratio (employed:unemployed) 17:1 9:9 a a.007 Verbal intelligence quotient (NART) (6.0) (8.1) , Education, y 12.4 (1.8) 11.2 (1.0) , BIS-11 total score 62.0 (7.2) 82.0 (9.5) , MADRS total score 0.9 (2.4) 5.6 (8.2) , BDI-II total score 1.1 (2.4) 9.3 (11.1) , YBOCS total score 0.1 (0.5) OCDUS total score 26.5 (7.9) Duration of stimulant abuse, y 11.7 (7.4) [2-26] Frequency of stimulant abuse, d/wk 5.4 (2.0) Age at onset of stimulant abuse, y 20.5 (5.4) [14-35] Abbreviations: BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory II 71 ; BIS-11, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale 76 ; MADRS, Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale 77 ; NART, National Adult Reading Test 78 ; OCDUS, Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale 62 ; YBOCS, Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. 64 a Fisher exact test. b Ethnicity was recorded according to the standard practices of the study funder and sponsor. Ethnic background was neither an inclusion nor an exclusion criterion. scores in the behavioral data and are therefore most specific to the cognitive control processes of interest. This design matrix, convolved with a hemodynamic response function, 75 was regressed at each voxel onto the corrected time series. The resulting maps of the model coefficients normalized by their standard errors for each individual were mapped into the standard space of the Montreal Neurological Institute echo-planar imaging template by linear affine transformation to obtain a group activation map. These activation maps included all participants under all drug treatments to identify brain regions that were significantly activated by contrast 1 (drug-word Stroop task) and by contrast 2 (color-word Stroop task) using a permutation test on spatial statistics ( We used the activation map of the drug-word Stroop task contrast to define a mask (shown as red voxels in Figure 2) and regressed the median attentional interference score for each individual on the individual activation statistics at each voxel in this mask. This procedure identified a set of voxels that were both generically activated by the task and significantly associated with variability in attentional interference (shown as yellow voxels in Figure 2). The mean activation statistic over all voxels associated with attentional interference was calculated for each participant and used as a dependent variable in ANCOVA models that tested the effects of group, drug, and drug group interaction on brain activation in these regions. The same regional activation statistics were also used as dependent variables in ANCOVA models that tested the effects of subgroup (eg, high or low compulsivity), drug, and drug subgroup interaction on brain activation. RESULTS BASELINE IMPULSIVITY, COMPULSIVITY, AND MOOD As expected from previous research, 49 SDIs reported significantly higher levels of impulsivity on the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale than controls. Stimulant-dependent individuals also generally reported high levels of compulsivity of stimulant abuse on the Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale, whereas healthy controls did not show strong obsessive-compulsive traits, as assessed by the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Table 3). Although participants with clinical depression were excluded from the study, SDIs reported higher scores on depressive mood, as assessed at baseline by the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale 77 and corroborated by serial scores on the Beck Depression Inventory II 71 before each treatment session (F 2,34 =21.50; P.001). SUBJECTIVE EFFECTS OF DRUG TREATMENT Dopaminergic challenge drugs had no significant effects on subjective alertness, contentedness, or calmness scales. There were also no significant group differences or drug group interactions on any of these measures ( /profile.php?ke220). There was no evidence for induction of craving by performance of the drug-word Stroop task and no effect of dopaminergic challenge drugs on self-reported craving. BEHAVIORAL DATA FOR THE STROOP TASKS In the drug-word Stroop paradigm, SDIs showed a marked attentional bias toward drug-related cues compared with controls (Figure 2A). Thus, across all 3 short-term treatment conditions, SDIs demonstrated significantly longer latency than controls in correctly identifying the font colors of drug-related words compared with their responses to neutral words (Table 1). Drug users did not make more errors on the drug-word Stroop task than controls. The significant drug group interaction for accuracy was driven by controls who showed a tendency to 637

7 Table 4. Brain Regions Differentially Activated During the Drug- Stroop Task in Drug Users and Healthy Volunteers, Irrespective of Drug Treatment Size (Voxel) Max F a x, y, z Max Coordinates, mm b AAL Regions Regions Associated With Greater Activation During Indication of Font Color of Drug-Related s vs Neutral s , 42, 12 Bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (orbital, opercula, and triangular parts), left middle frontal gyrus (orbital part), rolandic operculum, insula, and superior temporal gyrus , 50, 14 Bilateral middle frontal gyrus (orbital part), superior frontal gyrus (medial part), rectus gyrus, and anterior cingulate gyrus , 80, 46 Right cerebellum , 52, 30 Bilateral posterior cingulate gyrus, precuneus, and left median cingulate gyrus , 22, 4 Left thalamus and caudate nucleus , 46, 4 Left middle temporal gyrus , 32, 6 Right middle temporal gyrus and inferior temporal gyrus , 12, 36 Left precentral gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, and inferior frontal gyrus (opercula part) , 22, 64 Bilateral supplementary motor area and superior frontal gyrus (medial part) Regions Associated With Deactivation During Indication of Font Color of Drug-Related s vs Neutral s , 8, 10 Bilateral caudate, thalamus, hippocampus, posterior cingulate, precuneus, calcarine gyrus, left parahippocampus, left lingual gyrus, left fusiform gyrus, and right cuneus , 8, 8 Right superior temporal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, rolandic operculum postcentral gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, Herschel gyrus, insula, and putamen , 2, 48 Bilateral cingulate gyrus, supplementary motor area, and paracentral lobule , 72, 54 Right precuneus, cuneus, superior parietal gyrus, and superior occipital gyrus , 36, 52 Right inferior parietal gyrus, postcentral gyrus, and supramarginal gyrus , 12, 72 Left precentral gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, paracentral lobule, and postcentral gyrus Abbreviations: AAL, Automated Anatomical Labeling 79 ; Max, maximum. a Max F denotes the maximum F statistic (test statistics for task-related activation) over all voxels in each region. b Coordinates are given in Montreal Neurological Institute space. Table 5. Brain Regions Differentially Activated During the Color- Stroop Task in Drug Users and Healthy Volunteers, Irrespective of Drug Treatment Size (Voxel) Max F a x, y, z Max Coordinates, mm b AAL Regions Regions Associated With Greater Activation During Indication of Font Color of Incongruent Color s vs Neutral s , 46, 40 Left cerebellum and vermis Regions Associated With Deactivation During Indication of Font Color of Incongruent Color s vs Neutral s , 56, 10 Bilateral middle frontal gyrus (orbital part), superior frontal gyrus (medial part), gyrus rectus, and anterior cingulate gyrus Abbreviations: AAL, Automated Anatomical Labeling 79 ; Max, maximum. a Max F denotes the maximum F statistic (test statistics for task-related activation) over all voxels in each region. b Coordinates are given in Montreal Neurological Institute space. make fewer errors while taking amisulpride relative to placebo (F 1,14 =3.48; P=.08). On the color-word Stroop paradigm, no main effect of group and no drug group interaction were identified (Table 1). fmri DATA FOR THE DRUG-WORD AND COLOR-WORD STROOP TASKS The brain regions activated by drug words (relative to neutral words) included the left ventral and dorsal prefrontal cortex and precentral cortex; anterior cingulate, medial prefrontal, and premotor cortex; posterior cingulate and medial posterior parietal cortex; bilateral middle and inferior temporal cortex; left thalamus and caudate nucleus; and right cerebellum (Table 4 and Within this pattern of brain activation, attentional bias for drug words was most strongly associated with greater activation of the left ventral prefrontal cortex (Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates [x, y, z] in millimeters: 46, 26, 12; 44, 22, 8; and 40, 6, 30) and right cerebellum (x, y, z: 22, 80, 40) (Figure 2B and C). We fitted an ANCOVA model to test for significant main and interactive effects of group and dopaminergic drug challenge on these regions of activation. Only the effect of group was significant: SDIs had greater task-related activation of the left ventral prefrontal cortex and right cerebellum compared with controls (F 1,31 =6.40; P.05) (Figure 2D and /directory/profile.php?ke220). In the color-word Stroop task, the only brain area activated in all participants was the left cerebellum (x, y, z: 4, 46, 40) (Table 5). We did not find a significant relationship with attentional interference scores in this locus of activation and therefore did not investigate it any further. 638

8 A Median Interference Score, ms ± 1 SE Behavior performance during drug-word Stroop task Amisulpride Placebo Pramipexole B BOLD Response ± 1 SE Task-related brain activation during drug-word Stroop task Low Compulsive High Compulsive Low Compulsive High Compulsive Figure 3. Behavioral performance and task-related activation of a frontocerebellar system during the drug-word Stroop task in high- and low-compulsivity drug users. Pramipexole was given as pramipexole dihydrochloride. A, High-compulsivity drug users showed a different profile in response to short-term dopaminergic treatment compared with low-compulsivity drug users, as reflected in greater attentional bias for drug-related words relative to neutral words. Attentional bias was measured by each volunteer s median response latency of correctly identified colors of drug-related words minus the median response latency of correctly identified colors of matched neutral words. B, Dopaminergic drug effects on the left prefrontal cortex during the drug-word Stroop task are modulated by compulsivity of stimulant dependence. Box plots show functional activation associated with attentional bias for drug-related words in the left prefrontal cortex (Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates [x, y, z] in millimeters: 46, 26, 12; 44, 22, -8; and 40, 6, 30) and right cerebellum (22, 80, 40) in high- and low-compulsivity subgroups, indicating differential effects of pramipexole. The spatial coordinates refer to the peak voxel where the effect size is greatest. EFFECTS OF IMPULSIVITY AND COMPULSIVITY ON STIMULANT DEPENDENCE To investigate possible effects of individual differences in baseline measurements of impulsivity on drugrelated attentional bias, we divided the SDIs into low- and high-impulsivity subgroups by a median split on their total Barratt Impulsiveness Scale scores. The high- and low-impulsivity subgroups were not significantly different on any other demographic or clinical variables (http: // There were no significant differences between impulsivity subgroups in latency or accuracy of response to the drug-word Stroop test and there were no significant drug impulsivity subgroup interactions (Table 2). Likewise, we also divided the SDIs into high- and lowcompulsivity subgroups by a median split on their total Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale scores; the compulsivity subgroups were not significantly different on any other baseline variables ( However, there was a significant subgroup effect on attentional interference during the drug-word Stroop test; the high-compulsivity subgroup demonstrated significantly greater attentional bias to drug-related related words (Figure 3A) (Table 2). There was also a significant drug compulsivity subgroup interaction whereby the dopaminergic agents had markedly different effects on attentional bias in high- and lowcompulsivity subgroups. Post hoc analysis indicated that this interaction was due to different effects of pramipexole in the 2 subgroups: low-compulsivity SDIs showed no attentional bias, whereas in high-compulsivity SDIs, response latencies for drug-related words were increased (F 1,15 =9.80; P.05). The effects of compulsivity on attentional bias for drugrelated words were followed up by an analysis of subgroup effects on brain activation in the frontocerebellar regions previously defined. We found a significant drug compulsivity subgroup interaction on brain activation in the left ventral prefrontal cortex and right cerebellum (F 2,30 =3.36; P.05) (Figure 3B). Post hoc analysis showed that this interaction also was mainly driven by pramipexole, which caused a significant reduction in frontocerebellar activation compared with placebo in the low-compulsivity subgroup (t 8 =2.34; P.05) and the opposite effect of increased prefrontal activation, in the highcompulsivity subgroup (t 8 = 0.77; P.05). COMMENT Using 2 versions of the Stroop paradigm for testing attentional control, we confirmed that SDIs had significantly greater attentional bias in favor of drug-related cues, 20,21,23,63,80 reflected in greater attentional interference scores on the drug-word Stroop test (Figure 2A). Moreover, this impairment of attentional control in people with stimulant dependence was specific to drug-related cues and did not generalize to their performance on the color-word Stroop task. Thus, it seems that there is a clinically relevant impairment of attentional control in stimulant dependence 21 that does not simply reflect the global impairment in attentional function that has been associated with stimulant dependence. 81,82 We were also able to show, using fmri, that greater attentional bias for drug-related words was associated with greater activation of the right cerebellum, a brain area that is implicated in Stroop task interference control, and the left ventral prefrontal cortex or inferior 639

9 frontal gyrus, a brain area that is implicated in the retrieval of semantic knowledge and the processing of word meaning This neurocognitive association was demonstrated by the contrast between SDIs and controls: SDIs had significantly greater frontocerebellar activation (Figure 2D) as well as significantly greater attentional interference scores (Figure 2A) and attentional interference was positively correlated with frontocerebellar activation over all subjects in both groups (Figure 2C). Although abnormal inferior frontal gyrus activation during the disorder-related Stroop paradigm has been identified in various groups of psychiatric patients, 68,69,93 overactivation of left inferior frontal gyrus when identifying font colors of drug-related words might be specific for stimulant dependence. Left inferior frontal gyrus overactivation in stimulant users has also been reported in response to cocaine-related videos 94 or imagery 95 relative to neutral cues or when a stimulant drug was administered unexpectedly. 96 COMPULSIVITY OF DRUG ABUSE AND FRONTOCEREBELLAR SYSTEMS We also found that SDIs with a highly compulsive pattern of stimulant drug abuse generally had greater attentional bias for drug-related words (Figure 3A). Moreover, the high- and low-compulsivity subgroups differed markedly in their response to single-dose challenges of pramipexole and amisulpride: most saliently, pramipexole increased attentional interference and frontocerebellar activation in highly compulsive SDIs, whereas in lowcompulsivity SDIs, pramipexole had the opposite effect, tending to reduce or normalize attentional interference and frontocerebellar activation. How can we explain the different effects of these drugs on attentional interference and related brain activation in high- and lowcompulsivity subgroups of SDIs? At a mechanistic level, the normalizing effect of pramipexole on attentional bias and related frontocerebellar systems in low-compulsive SDIs could be achieved by 1 of 2 main actions. If there is relatively deficient striatal dopamine neurotransmission in this subgroup, consistent with findings recently reviewed by Narendran and Martinez, 97 that impairs the function of the associated corticostriatal loops, the dopamine D 2 /D 3 agonist could restore normal function via its postsynaptic action. 98 Alternatively, given that the SDIs were not in a withdrawal state, it is conceivable that they were exhibiting upregulation of striatal dopamine neurotransmission at the synapse and that the relatively low dose of the dopamine agonist restored normal functioning by its presynaptic action at dopamine D 2 autoreceptors. 99,100 However, the D 2 /D 3 receptor antagonist, amisulpride, generally had opposite effects to those of pramipexole, perhaps favoring the former hypothesis. Clearly, neither of these hypotheses could account for the opposite effects of pramipexole in the highcompulsivity subgroup. One hypothesis that might explain this is that highly compulsive individuals have developed a dopamine receptor sensitivity that could lead to an exaggerated and pathological response to pramipexole. Similar observations of postsynaptic supersensitivity have been described in patients with Parkinson disease who compulsively overdose their dopamine agonist medication. 58 The consistent finding of reduced D 2 /D 3 receptor binding in stimulant dependence 36,38,97,101 argues against this interpretation. However, recent preclinical studies have demonstrated that a long-term regimen of amphetamine exposure does lead to an increase in activation of the high-affinity state of striatal D 2 receptors. 102 Regardless of these considerations, it is evident that heterogeneity in response to dopaminergic agents clearly has to be taken into account when interpreting the studies of dopamine neurotransmission in stimulant abusers and indeed may help to resolve some of the inconsistencies in the field. 97 THERAPEUTIC IMPLICATIONS One general therapeutic implication of these results is that dopaminergic drugs might have value in controlling the attentional bias toward stimulant-related cues that has previously been shown to predict relapse following a period of abstinence. 21 For example, dopaminergic agents could reduce the risk of attentional capture by relapseprovoking cues and thereby promote maintenance of abstinence. The observation that we did not find a relationship between attentional bias and self-reported craving may not be unusual given that many aspects of compulsive drug taking occur through automatic or habitual processes. 54,103,104 Our data further suggest that the therapeutic effects of dopaminergic drugs could be strongly influenced by the baseline compulsivity of stimulant drug use (which can vary considerably within a group homogenously defined as satisfying DSM-IV criteria for stimulant drug dependence). This finding also has potential implications for early clinical trial design in the development of new candidate therapeutics for stimulant dependence: greater statistical power for tests of therapeutic efficacy might be achieved by sampling psychologically stratified, eg, high- or low-compulsivity, groups of patients. METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES Strengths of the study include the balanced, parallelgroups, crossover design; the use of psychological test paradigms theoretically focused on cognitive mechanisms of special relevance to drug dependence; the choice of pharmacologically contrasting dopaminergic challenge drugs; and the well-characterized group of stimulant users. Weaknesses of the study include modest sample size, which has probably constrained the statistical power of the experiment, although the repeated-measures design generated 108 (36 3) behavioral and neuroimaging observations to assess factorial effects of interest. This is a large number of observations by comparison with other neuroimaging studies of stimulant dependence. The dose of pramipexole dihydrochloride was reduced during the course of the experiment, from 1.5 mg to 0.5 mg, because of the occurrence of adverse effects at the starting dose. This difference in dose was associated with proportional changes in pharmacokinetic measures of exposure, which were subsequently included as covariates 640

10 in ANCOVA models to control for any effects of dose on behavioral or brain functional differences between groups. Although SDIs were carefully selected according to eligibility criteria that excluded comorbid Axis I disorders and alcohol and opiate dependence, there was still a degree of heterogeneity within the SDI group in terms of their stimulant drug of choice (cocaine or amphetamine) and its source (3 amphetamine users were prescribed amphetamine for harm reduction). This clinical heterogeneity motivates our analysis of individual differences in compulsivity and impulsivity of drug use; however, it also presents a constraint on the generalizability of our results. The observation that high- and lowcompulsivity SDIs did not differ in terms of drug-taking histories or clinical symptoms may seem surprising but similar observations have been made with regard to craving in the presence of drug-related cues, which seems not to be reflected in clinical or demographic variables but in biological differences of cue-induced dopamine release in the striatum. 35 In relation to the cognitive tests, one may argue that a lack of familiarity of drug words in controls may have affected the results. This seems unlikely since previous research has shown that the interference effect for the drug-word Stroop task is specific to drug-dependent individuals and independent from the familiarity of words. 105,106 It is still not clear whether drug-related compulsivity is similar to compulsive behavior observed in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder, a disorder that has also been associated with dopamine dysfunction In our healthy controls, however, the Yale- Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale has been shown to be insensitive for obsessive-compulsive behaviors at a subclinical level. Further studies may consider using other measures such as the Padua Inventory, 110 which assesses the spectrum rather than the severity of obsessivecompulsive symptoms. Moreover, behavioral measures of compulsivity in stimulant dependence need to be developed to validate the individual differences in compulsivity of drug use measured herein by a self-report instrument (Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale). For example, future studies of SDIs might usefully also include more general cognitive measures of attentional set shifting or reversal learning as more objective markers of compulsive perseveration. Submitted for Publication: March 19, 2009; final revision received September 16, 2009; accepted September 17, Author Affiliations: Brain Mapping Unit, Department of Psychiatry (Drs Ersche, Bullmore, Müller, Ooi, Suckling, Barnes, and Sahakian), Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute (Drs Ersche, Bullmore, Craig, Abbott, Müller, Ooi, Suckling, Barnes, Sahakian, and Robbins), and Department of Experimental Psychology (Drs Craig, Abbott, and Robbins), University of Cambridge, and GlaxoSmithKline, Clinical Unit Cambridge (Dr Bullmore), Cambridge, and GlaxoSmithKline, Clinical Sciences Study Operations, Harlow (Ms Shabbir), England; and GlaxoSmithKline, Neurosciences Centre of Excellence for Drug Discovery, Verona, Italy (Dr Merlo- Pich). Dr Craig is now affiliated with P1vital Ltd, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, England. Correspondence: Karen D. Ersche, PhD, CPsychol, University of Cambridge, Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Psychiatry, Herchel Smith Building, Cambridge CB2 0SZ, England (ke220@cam.ac.uk). Author Contributions: Drs Ersche and Bullmore contributed equally to this work. Financial Disclosure: This work was funded and sponsored by GlaxoSmithKline grant RG45422 and conducted within the GlaxoSmithKline Clinical Unit Cambridge, Cambridge, England. Dr Bullmore is employed part time by the University of Cambridge and part time by GlaxoSmithKline plc and is a shareholder of GlaxoSmithKline. Dr Craig is full-time employed by P1vital Ltd and Ms Shabbir and Dr Merlo-Pich are full-time employed by GlaxoSmithKline plc. Dr Abbott has provided computing services for GlaxoSmithKline. Dr Suckling has received grant support from GlaxoSmithKline. Dr Müller has received research grant support from Janssen-Cilag and honoraria or travel expenses from Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Pharmacia-Upjohn, and UCB Pharma. Dr Sahakian has provided consulting services to Cambridge Cognition Ltd, GlaxoSmithKline, Novartis, AstraZeneca, and Eli Lilly; she holds shares of CeNeS and has received honoraria for grand rounds in psychiatry at Massachusetts General Hospital (continuing medical education credits) and for speaking at the International Conference on Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia and Mood Disorders (2007). Dr Sahakian is also on the Medical Research Council Neurosciences and Mental Health Board and the Science Coordination Team for the Foresight Project on Mental Capital and Wellbeing and receives an honorarium as an associate editor of Psychological Medicine.Dr Robbins has provided consulting services for Cambridge Cognition Ltd and the pharmaceutical industry, including Eli Lilly and GlaxoSmithKline, and has also received honoraria from Roche, Merck Sharp and Dohme, Lundbeck, and GlaxoSmithKline. He is a past recipient of research grants from GlaxoSmithKline and Pfizer and holds shares from CeNeS and share options from Cambridge Cognition Ltd and Allon Therapeutics. He also receives an editorial honorarium from Springer-Verlag (Psychopharmacology). Funding/Support: Software development was supported by a Human Brain Project grant from the National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering. Dr Ersche is a recipient of the Betty Behrens Research Fellowship at Clare Hall, University of Cambridge. Dr Sahakian is supported by a Wellcome Trust programme grant. Dr Robbins is supported by a Wellcome Trust programme grant and is a recipient of the James S. McDonnell Foundation Collaborative Award. Previous Presentations: Data in preliminary form were presented at the International Symposium on Attention and Performance XXII; July 17, 2008; Stow, Vermont; the 21st European College for Neuropsychopharmacology Annual Conference; September 3, 2008; Barcelona, Spain; and the Collegium Internationale Neuro-Psychopharmacologicum Regional Meeting on Addiction and Psychosis; April 26, 2009; Edinburgh, Scotland. 641

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis (OA). All subjects provided informed consent to procedures

More information

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group 1 Supplemental Data Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group Additional inclusion criteria for the major depressive disorder group were: age of onset of

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. Authors Year Patients Male gender (%) Mean age (range) Adults/ Children

More information

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Brain electrical activity methods description and explanation of method state effects trait effects Positron emission tomography

More information

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Table S1: Brain regions involved in the adapted classification learning task Brain Regions x y z Z Anterior Cingulate

More information

Influence of Compulsivity of Drug Abuse on Dopaminergic Modulation of Attentional Bias in Stimulant Dependence

Influence of Compulsivity of Drug Abuse on Dopaminergic Modulation of Attentional Bias in Stimulant Dependence Europe PMC Funders Group Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010 June ; 67(6): 632 644. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.60. Influence of Compulsivity of Drug Abuse

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Green SA, Hernandez L, Tottenham N, Krasileva K, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M. The neurobiology of sensory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. Published

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/324/5927/646/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves Modulation of the vmpfc Valuation System Todd A. Hare,* Colin F. Camerer, Antonio

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Cocaine dependence: a fast-track for brain ageing?

Cocaine dependence: a fast-track for brain ageing? Europe PMC Funders Group Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Mol Psychiatry. 2013 February ; 18(2): 134 135. doi:10.1038/mp.2012.31. Cocaine dependence: a fast-track for brain ageing?

More information

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Supplemental Information Table of Contents 2 Behavioral Data 2 Table S1. Participant demographics

More information

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Neuron, Volume 70 Supplemental Information Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Luke J. Chang, Alec Smith, Martin Dufwenberg, and Alan G. Sanfey Supplemental Information

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Redlich R, Opel N, Grotegerd D, et al. Prediction of individual response to electroconvulsive therapy via machine learning on structural magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA

More information

Supplementary Information Appendix: Default Mode Contributions to Automated Information Processing

Supplementary Information Appendix: Default Mode Contributions to Automated Information Processing Supplementary Information Appendix: Default Mode Contributions to Automated Information Processing Authors: Deniz Vatansever a,b,c,1, David K. Menon a,d, Emmanuel A. Stamatakis a,b,d Affiliations: a Division

More information

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Supplemental informations Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Stéphanie Lefebvre, PhD^1,2, Guillaume Baille, MD^4, Renaud Jardri MD, PhD 1,2 Lucie Plomhause, PhD

More information

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy Methods Participants We examined a group of twelve male pathological gamblers (ten strictly right handed, all smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy males,

More information

Validation of non- REM sleep stage decoding from resting state fmri using linear support vector machines

Validation of non- REM sleep stage decoding from resting state fmri using linear support vector machines Validation of non- REM sleep stage decoding from resting state fmri using linear support vector machines Altmann A. 1,2,7 *, Schröter M.S. 1,3 *, Spoormaker V.I. 1, Kiem S.A. 1, Jordan D. 4, Ilg R. 5,6,

More information

2017, Joule Inc. or its licensors Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors.

2017, Joule Inc. or its licensors Online appendices are unedited and posted as supplied by the authors. Results Validation: Reproducibility Figure S1. Reproducibility of the results of small-world parameters. Differences in topological properties of functional brain networks between bulimia nervosa (BN)

More information

Supplementary Materials for

Supplementary Materials for Supplementary Materials for Folk Explanations of Behavior: A Specialized Use of a Domain-General Mechanism Robert P. Spunt & Ralph Adolphs California Institute of Technology Correspondence may be addressed

More information

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver NRSC January 28, 2010 Costs: Health, Crime, Productivity Costs in billions of dollars (2002) $400 $350 $400B legal

More information

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia 1 Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia Christine I. Hooker, Taylor L. Benson, Anett Gyurak, Hong Yin,

More information

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism Justin H G Williams Morven McWhirr Gordon D Waiter Cambridge Sept 10 th 2010 Joint attention in autism Declarative and receptive aspects initiating

More information

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Overview of the SIIPS1 development. The development of the SIIPS1 consisted of individual- and group-level analysis steps. 1) Individual-person

More information

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Supplementary Information Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex David V. Smith 1-3, Benjamin Y. Hayden 1,4, Trong-Kha Truong 2,5, Allen W. Song 2,5, Michael L.

More information

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Supplemental Information Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Roselinde H. Kaiser, Ph.D., Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli, Ph.D., Daniel G. Dillon, Ph.D., Franziska Goer, B.S., Miranda

More information

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis International Journal of Innovative Research in Computer Science & Technology (IJIRCST) ISSN: 2347-5552, Volume-2, Issue-6, November-2014 Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using

More information

Supplementary Digital Content

Supplementary Digital Content Supplementary Digital Content Contextual modulation of pain in masochists: involvement of the parietal operculum and insula Sandra Kamping a, Jamila Andoh a, Isabelle C. Bomba a, Martin Diers a,b, Eugen

More information

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition Outline Functional MRI Mapping Cognition Michael A. Yassa, B.A. Division of Psychiatric Neuro-imaging Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Why fmri? fmri - How it works Research

More information

Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship to trait anxiety

Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship to trait anxiety brain research 1481 (2012) 13 36 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship

More information

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex in dual-task performance Fabienne Collette a,b, Laurence 01ivier b,c, Martial Van der Linden a,d, Steven Laureys b, Guy Delfiore b, André Luxen

More information

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Figure I. Changes in connectivity with the right anterior insular cortex. (A) wake > mild sedation, showing a reduction in connectivity between the anterior

More information

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain 1 Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain Emily Falk, Elliot Berkman, Traci Mann, Brittany Harrison, Matthew Lieberman This document contains: Example

More information

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Online supplementary information for: Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Joel L. Voss, Brian D. Gonsalves, Kara D. Federmeier,

More information

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Master Thesis Christa Nijnens September 16, 2009 University of Utrecht Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience Department of Neuroscience

More information

Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk

Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk... and corresponding risk prediction errors Peter Bossaerts 1 Contents 1. Reward Encoding And The Dopaminergic System 2. Reward Prediction Errors And TD (Temporal

More information

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, 2017 The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain David Reyher, MSW, CAADC Behavioral Health Program Director Alvernia University Defining Addiction

More information

Passport control a bit carried away. appreciated the advice forgot to talk to the manager, next thing I know my fmri thankfully, when aroused things back to normal Inattentive impaired children and adolescents:

More information

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review Jim Morrill, MD, PhD MGH Charlestown HealthCare Center Massachusetts General Hospital Disclosures Neither I nor my spouse/partner has a relevant financial relationship

More information

DOES THE D 3 DOPAMINE RECEPTOR (D 3 r)play A ROLE IN ADDICTION? Isabelle Boileau, PhD

DOES THE D 3 DOPAMINE RECEPTOR (D 3 r)play A ROLE IN ADDICTION? Isabelle Boileau, PhD DOES THE D 3 DOPAMINE RECEPTOR (D 3 r)play A ROLE IN ADDICTION? Isabelle Boileau, PhD Acknowledgments Human Neurochemical Pathology Lab Stephen J. Kish, PhD Jun Chao Tong, PhD Centre for Movement Disorders

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Selective Abnormal Modulation of Hippocampal Activity During Memory Formation in First-Episode Psychosis

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Selective Abnormal Modulation of Hippocampal Activity During Memory Formation in First-Episode Psychosis ORIGINAL ARTICLE Selective Abnormal Modulation of Hippocampal Activity During Memory Formation in First-Episode Psychosis Amélie M. Achim, PhD; Marie-Claude Bertrand, PhD; Hazel Sutton, BSc; Alonso Montoya,

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation

Cover Page. The handle  holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/32078 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Author: Pannekoek, Nienke Title: Using novel imaging approaches in affective disorders

More information

National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) Understanding Drug Abuse and Addiction: What Science Says

National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) Understanding Drug Abuse and Addiction: What Science Says National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) Understanding Drug Abuse and Addiction: What Science Says Last Updated February 2016 https://www.drugabuse.gov 1 Table of Contents Understanding Drug Abuse and Addiction:

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. is the most common neurobehavioral

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. is the most common neurobehavioral ORIGINAL ARTICLE Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Methylphenidate and Placebo in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder During the Multi-Source Interference Task George Bush, MD, MMSc; Thomas

More information

doi: /brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease

doi: /brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease doi:10.1093/brain/aws024 Brain 2012: 135; 1165 1179 1165 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Altered brain mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Huntington s disease Marianne J. U. Novak, 1 Jason D.

More information

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is characterized

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is characterized REVIEW Cool Inferior Frontostriatal Dysfunction in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Versus Hot Ventromedial Orbitofrontal-Limbic Dysfunction in Conduct Disorder: A Review Katya Rubia Attention-deficit/hyperactivity

More information

There are, in total, four free parameters. The learning rate a controls how sharply the model

There are, in total, four free parameters. The learning rate a controls how sharply the model Supplemental esults The full model equations are: Initialization: V i (0) = 1 (for all actions i) c i (0) = 0 (for all actions i) earning: V i (t) = V i (t - 1) + a * (r(t) - V i (t 1)) ((for chosen action

More information

Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP

Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP Neurobiology of Addiction JeanAnne Johnson Talbert, DHA, APRN BC, FNP, CARN AP Disclosures This speaker has no conflicts of interest to disclose Objectives Define drug abuse and addiction Identify the

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

DIADEM Instructions for Use

DIADEM Instructions for Use Software Version: 1.0.0 Document Version: 3.0 July 2017 Contents 1 Intended Use... 4 2 Scanning... 5 2.1 Automatic Operation... 5 2.2 Suitable Scans... 5 2.2.1 Protocols... 5 2.2.2 Brain Coverage... 5

More information

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of Placebo effects in fmri Supporting online material 1 Supporting online material Materials and Methods Study 1 Procedure and behavioral data We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was

More information

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults Supplementary Material Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance across cognitive domains in older adults Emily E. Shaw 1,2, Aaron P. Schultz 1,2,3, Reisa A.

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Newman et al. 10.1073/pnas.1510527112 SI Results Behavioral Performance. Behavioral data and analyses are reported in the main article. Plots of the accuracy and reaction time data

More information

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri Magnetic Resonance Imaging 24 (2006) 181 185 Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri Birgit Abler a, 4, Alard Roebroeck b, Rainer Goebel b, Anett

More information

MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION. Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre

MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION. Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre 1 MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre Reward 2 Humans, as well as other organisms engage in behaviours that are rewarding The pleasurable feelings provide positive reinforcement

More information

Data Analysis. Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning. Delay vs. Trace Conditioning. Discrimination and Reversal. Complex Discriminations

Data Analysis. Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning. Delay vs. Trace Conditioning. Discrimination and Reversal. Complex Discriminations What is Fear Conditioning? Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning Information and prediction: Animals use environmental signals to predict the occurrence of biologically significant events. Similar

More information

Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders 2012, 2:11

Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders 2012, 2:11 Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders This Provisional PDF corresponds to the article as it appeared upon acceptance. Fully formatted PDF and full text (HTML) versions will be made available soon. Neural

More information

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest Experimental Design Kate Watkins Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford With thanks to: Heidi Johansen-Berg Joe Devlin Outline Choices for experimental paradigm Subtraction / hierarchical

More information

Study No: Title : Rationale: Phase: Study Period: Study Design: Centres: Indication: Treatment: Objectives: Statistical Methods: Sample Size

Study No: Title : Rationale: Phase: Study Period: Study Design: Centres: Indication: Treatment: Objectives: Statistical Methods: Sample Size The study listed may include approved and non-approved uses, formulations or treatment regimens. The results reported in any single study may not reflect the overall results obtained on studies of a product.

More information

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Subjects The experiment included twelve subjects: ten sighted subjects and two blind. Five of the ten sighted subjects were expert users of a visual-to-auditory

More information

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M.

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M. Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making Johanna M. Jarcho 1,2 Elliot T. Berkman 3 Matthew D. Lieberman 3 1 Department of Psychiatry

More information

9/13/2018. Neurobiological Aspects of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria

9/13/2018. Neurobiological Aspects of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria Neurobiological Aspects of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Neil P. Jones 7th Annual Conference on ADHD and Executive Function September 14, 218 Diagnosis Child

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16 SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16 women and 16 men. One female had to be excluded from brain data analyses because of strong movement

More information

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References

Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References Title of file for HTML: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Tables and Supplementary References Supplementary Information Supplementary Figure 1. The mean parameter

More information

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Available online at www.sciencedirect.com R NeuroImage 18 (2003) 806 810 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Technical Note Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Andrea Mechelli,

More information

Moving the brain: Neuroimaging motivational changes of deep brain stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder Figee, M.

Moving the brain: Neuroimaging motivational changes of deep brain stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder Figee, M. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Moving the brain: Neuroimaging motivational changes of deep brain stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder Figee, M. Link to publication Citation for published

More information

Distinct valuation subsystems in the human brain for effort and delay

Distinct valuation subsystems in the human brain for effort and delay Supplemental material for Distinct valuation subsystems in the human brain for effort and delay Charlotte Prévost, Mathias Pessiglione, Elise Météreau, Marie-Laure Cléry-Melin and Jean-Claude Dreher This

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 197 (2009) 186 197 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report Top-down attentional

More information

Table 1. Summary of PET and fmri Methods. What is imaged PET fmri BOLD (T2*) Regional brain activation. Blood flow ( 15 O) Arterial spin tagging (AST)

Table 1. Summary of PET and fmri Methods. What is imaged PET fmri BOLD (T2*) Regional brain activation. Blood flow ( 15 O) Arterial spin tagging (AST) Table 1 Summary of PET and fmri Methods What is imaged PET fmri Brain structure Regional brain activation Anatomical connectivity Receptor binding and regional chemical distribution Blood flow ( 15 O)

More information

Andy C.H. Lee a,b,, Trevor W. Robbins b, Stephen Smith c, Gemma A. Calvert c, Irene Tracey c, Paul Matthews c, Adrian M. Owen a. 1.

Andy C.H. Lee a,b,, Trevor W. Robbins b, Stephen Smith c, Gemma A. Calvert c, Irene Tracey c, Paul Matthews c, Adrian M. Owen a. 1. Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 2420 2437 Evidence for asymmetric frontal-lobe involvement in episodic memory from functional magnetic resonance imaging and patients with unilateral frontal-lobe excisions Andy

More information

BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION

BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION TREVOR.W. ROBBINS Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge Many drugs of abuse, including stimulants such as amphetamine and cocaine, opiates

More information

Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons

Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons Supplementary Information for: Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons Steven W. Kennerley, Timothy E. J. Behrens & Jonathan D. Wallis Content list: Supplementary

More information

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch. The Frontal Lobes Readings: KW Ch. 16 Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions E.L. Highly organized college professor Became disorganized, showed little emotion, and began to miss deadlines Scores on intelligence

More information

Task-Related Functional Connectivity Analysis of Emotion Discrimination in a Family Study of Schizophrenia

Task-Related Functional Connectivity Analysis of Emotion Discrimination in a Family Study of Schizophrenia Schizophrenia Bulletin doi:10.1093/schbul/sbx004 Task-Related Functional Connectivity Analysis of Emotion Discrimination in a Family Study of Schizophrenia Vina M. Goghari*,1, Nicole Sanford 2,3, Michael

More information

Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions

Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions Anneke. Goudriaan 1,2, Ph.D. Ruth van Holst 1,2, M.Sc. Dick

More information

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA The Effect of Cognitive Functioning, Age, and Molecular Variables on Brain Structure Among Carriers of the Fragile X Premutation: Deformation Based Morphometry Study Naomi J. Goodrich-Hunsaker*, Ling M.

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Braver et al. 10.1073/pnas.0808187106 SI Methods Participants. Participants were neurologically normal, righthanded younger or older adults. The groups did not differ in gender breakdown

More information

Biomarkers Workshop In Clinical Trials Imaging for Schizophrenia Trials

Biomarkers Workshop In Clinical Trials Imaging for Schizophrenia Trials Biomarkers Workshop In Clinical Trials Imaging for Schizophrenia Trials Research focused on the following areas Brain pathology in schizophrenia and its modification Effect of drug treatment on brain structure

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Histograms of original and phase-randomised data

Supplementary Figure 1. Histograms of original and phase-randomised data Log Supplementary Figure 1. Histograms of original and phase-randomised data BOLD signals histogram Denoised signals histogram Activity-inducing signals histogram Innovation signals histogram 20 15 10

More information

Brain-wide functional inter-hemispheric disconnection is a potential. biomarker for schizophrenia and distinguishes it from depression

Brain-wide functional inter-hemispheric disconnection is a potential. biomarker for schizophrenia and distinguishes it from depression Supplementary Materials Brain-wide functional inter-hemispheric disconnection is a potential biomarker for schizophrenia and distinguishes it from depression Shuixia Guo, Keith M. Kendrick, Jie Zhang,

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way Mortality and morbidity attributable to use of addictive substances in the United States. The Association of American Physicians from 1999 60 million tobacco smokers in the U.S. 14 million dependent on

More information

Results. NeuRA fmri March 2017

Results. NeuRA fmri March 2017 Introduction With cognitive, sensory or motor stimulation, specific brain regions are activated, requiring higher energy use and higher levels of blood flow. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri)

More information

TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE. The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in

TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE. The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in 1 NAME COURSE TITLE 2 TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in dopamine neurotransmitter, up-regulation

More information

Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf

Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf Supplementary Material Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf Ella Striem-Amit 1*, Jorge Almeida 2,3, Mario Belledonne 1, Quanjing Chen 4, Yuxing Fang

More information

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Name: Student #: BEFORE YOU BEGIN!!! 1) Count the number of pages in your exam. The exam is 8 pages long; if you do not

More information

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus FRONTAL LOBE Central Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Cingulate Lateral Lateral View Medial View Motor execution and higher cognitive functions (e.g., language production, impulse inhibition, reasoning

More information

The Neurobiology of Addiction

The Neurobiology of Addiction The Neurobiology of Addiction Jodi Gilman, Ph.D. Center for Addiction Medicine Massachusetts General Hospital Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry Harvard Medical School What is Addiction? commonly

More information

Brain Imaging Studies on the Cognitive, Pharmacological and Neurobiological Effects of Cannabis in Humans: Evidence from Studies of Adult Users

Brain Imaging Studies on the Cognitive, Pharmacological and Neurobiological Effects of Cannabis in Humans: Evidence from Studies of Adult Users Send Orders for Reprints to reprints@benthamscience.ae REVIEW ARTICLE Current Pharmaceutical Design, 2016, 22, 1-14 1 Brain Imaging Studies on the Cognitive, Pharmacological and Neurobiological Effects

More information

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder A Neuro-Anatomical Approach to diagnosis and treatment

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder A Neuro-Anatomical Approach to diagnosis and treatment Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder A Neuro-Anatomical Approach to diagnosis and treatment Damon Lipinski, Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist Center for Pediatric Neuropsychology What is Attention? Different

More information

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 341 356 Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Joseph B. Sala a,, Pia Rämä a,c,d, Susan M.

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

Unit 3: The Biological Bases of Behaviour

Unit 3: The Biological Bases of Behaviour Unit 3: The Biological Bases of Behaviour Section 1: Communication in the Nervous System Section 2: Organization in the Nervous System Section 3: Researching the Brain Section 4: The Brain Section 5: Cerebral

More information

How to report my result using REST slice viewer?

How to report my result using REST slice viewer? How to report my result using REST slice viewer? Han Zhang Center for Cognition and Brain Disorders, Hangzhou Normal University napoleon1982@gmail.com 2013/12/30 Commonly, you got an activation for functional

More information

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion Kevin N. Ochsner, a, * Rebecca D. Ray, b Jeffrey C. Cooper, b Elaine R. Robertson, b Sita Chopra,

More information

Supplementary materials. Appendix A;

Supplementary materials. Appendix A; Supplementary materials Appendix A; To determine ADHD diagnoses, a combination of Conners' ADHD questionnaires and a semi-structured diagnostic interview was used(1-4). Each participant was assessed with

More information

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation. REVIEW OF FRONTAL LOBE STRUCTURES Main organization of frontal cortex: 1. Motor area (precentral gyrus). 2. Premotor & supplementary motor areas (immediately anterior to motor area). Includes premotor,

More information

The Cognitive Control of Memory: Age Differences in the Neural Correlates of Successful Remembering and Intentional Forgetting

The Cognitive Control of Memory: Age Differences in the Neural Correlates of Successful Remembering and Intentional Forgetting The Cognitive Control of Memory: Age Differences in the Neural Correlates of Successful Remembering and Intentional Forgetting Avery A. Rizio, Nancy A. Dennis* The Pennsylvania State University, Department

More information