Genetic Epidemiology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD Index) in Adults

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Genetic Epidemiology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD Index) in Adults"

Transcription

1 Genetic Epidemiology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD Index) in Adults Dorret I. Boomsma*, Viatcheslav Saviouk, Jouke-Jan Hottenga, Marijn A. Distel, Marleen H. M. de Moor, Jacqueline M. Vink, Lot M. Geels, Jenny H. D. A. van Beek, Meike Bartels, Eco J. C. de Geus, Gonneke Willemsen Department of Biological Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Abstract Context: In contrast to the large number of studies in children, there is little information on the contribution of genetic factors to Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in adults. Objective: To estimate the heritability of ADHD in adults as assessed by the ADHD index scored from the CAARS (Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales). Design: Phenotype data from over 12,000 adults (twins, siblings and parents) registered with the Netherlands Twin Register were analyzed using genetic structural equation modeling. Main outcome measures: Heritability estimates for ADHD from the twin-family study. Results: Heritability of ADHD in adults is estimated around 30% in men and women. There is some evidence for assortative mating. All familial transmission is explained by genetic inheritance, there is no support for the hypothesis that cultural transmission from parents to offspring is important. Conclusion: Heritability for ADHD features in adults is present, but is substantially lower than it is in children. Citation: Boomsma DI, Saviouk V, Hottenga J-J, Distel MA, de Moor MHM, et al. (2010) Genetic Epidemiology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD Index) in Adults. PLoS ONE 5(5): e doi: /journal.pone Editor: Pieter H. Reitsma, Leiden University Medical Center, Netherlands Received March 10, 2010; Accepted April 20, 2010; Published May 12, 2010 Copyright: ß 2010 Boomsma et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Funding: This study was supported by Spinozapremie (NWO/SPI ) and Twin-Family Database for Behavior Genetics and Genomics (NWO ). Data collection was funded by the Borderline Personality Disorder Research Foundation and by ZonMW Addiction (Project nr ). M.B. is financially supported by a senior fellowship of the EMGO+ institution. V.S. is supported by the EU Marie Curie Research Training Networks grant (EUTwinS, PI Igor Nenadic, RTN, FP6). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. * dorret@psy.vu.nl Introduction Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a common neurobehavioral disorder that is characterized by inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity. It is associated with considerable social, family, behavioral and cognitive dysfunction and is comorbid with depression, bipolar disorder, anxiety, and substance use [1]. ADHD in adults is often left untreated and significantly correlates with previous married status, unemployment, and European ancestry [2]. Genetic studies (family, twin, and adoption studies) demonstrated a strong genetic component in the etiology of the disorder in children [3 5], but substantially less is known about the heritability of ADHD in adults. This may partly be due to the fact that some of the earlier work suggested that ADHD is rare in adulthood. Nevertheless, a meta-analysis of follow-up studies on ADHD showed that although syndromatic persistence (i.e., the maintenance of full diagnostic status) is low (around 15%), symptomatic persistence (i.e., the maintenance of partial diagnostic status with impairment) is much higher with a persistence rate of 40 60% [6]. In a population screen of nearly 1000 adults, Faraone et al. estimated a prevalence of 2.9% for the narrow and 16.4% for the broad definitions of the ADHD phenotype [7]. Other prevalence estimates vary from study to study and are dependent on the definition and assessment of the phenotype (e.g. self-report or interview data), the gender composition of the sample and the age of the subjects [3,8,9]. In a meta-analysis of prevalence studies that followed the DSM-IV criteria for defining the ADHD phenotype, the pooled prevalence was estimated to be 2.5% (95% confidence interval (CI) %) [9]. It was noted that out of twelve reviewed studies ten have questioned the validity of DSM-IV criteria when applied to adults. Alternative phenotypic definitions led to a significant variation in prevalence of ADHD (between 2.5 and 42.3%) [9]. The metaanalysis also indicated that the prevalence of adult ADHD has a negative association with age, and that this association is modified by the gender composition of the sample. A recent study observed strong familial clustering of ADHD in first-degree relatives of adults with ADHD. Compared to a control group adults with ADHD had significantly more first-degree relatives with ADHD (28% versus 5%) [10]. Children born to parents with ADHD have a seven-fold increased risk for developing the disorder than children born to non-affected parents [11]. One PLoS ONE 1 May 2010 Volume 5 Issue 5 e10621

2 twin study looked at the etiological influences on Attention Problems in young adults, as assessed by the Adult Self Report [12]. The contribution of genetic influences was assessed at three points in time [13]. The mean ages of the participants were 19.6, 21.3, and 22.8 years at waves 1, 2, and 3, respectively. At each age the heritability of Attention Problems was estimated around 40%. The stability in attention problems was mainly due to genetic factors [13]. In this manuscript we present a quantitative genetic study of adult ADHD in a large sample of twin families who are registered with the Netherlands Twin Register [14]. Data were collected in 12,594 adult twins (average age 31 years), their siblings (average age 37 years) and parents (average age 55 years). ADHD was assessed with the CAARS (Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales, Technical Manual) [15], which is a self-report measure suitable for use in large-scale epidemiological studies. Adults with ADHD are the best informants about their symptoms [16] and the self-report form of the CAARS seems to be an appropriate survey method to evaluate ADHD symptoms in a large family-based cohort. The ADHD index was chosen as a phenotype definition due to its ability to distinguish the ADHD adults from the non-clinical cases (a T-score greater than 65) and the simplicity of the survey administration (12 items on the questionnaire) [16]. Heritability was estimated using data from twins, their parents and siblings with structural equation modeling and under different models of familial resemblance. Adding data from siblings to the classical twin design results in an increase in power to detect nonadditive genetic effects [17]. The effects of assortative mating, i.e. the fact that spouses are more similar for a trait or disorder than expected under random assortment, can be detected and accounted for by including data from parents of twins. A small degree of assortative mating is often found for psychiatric disorders [18,19] in population-based samples. However, resemblance between spouses for ADHD symptoms has not yet been reported. When there is spouse resemblance it is necessary to include its effects in a genetic model. Under random mating genetic effects are uncorrelated in parents. Under non-random assortment genetic effects in parents are correlated and the genetic correlations among the first-degree relatives are elevated that may lead to a biased estimate of heritability if this effect is not taken into account [20]. The classical twin design can inform on the influence of shared environment, but is not informative with respect to how much of the environment that is shared among offspring is transmitted from parents to offspring (cultural transmission). It is easily conceivable that growing up with a parent who scores high on a number of ADHD features influences the phenotype of their offspring. By adding phenotypic data from parents to the classical twin design, vertical cultural transmission, reflecting the nongenetic influence of the parents ADHD features on their offspring, can be examined. Because ADHD is expected to have a heritable component, vertical cultural transmission will induce genotypeenvironment correlation [20]. Methods Participants and procedure Phenotype data were collected in participants from the Netherlands Twin Register (NTR) by survey in (survey 7) and in an ongoing study (survey 8) that started in Data collection for survey 7 has been detailed in Distel et al [21]. Briefly, twins and their family members were invited by mail to complete a survey on health, lifestyle, personality and psychopathology. A group of 200 unrelated subjects completed Table 1. Number of participants for genetic analyses. Number Age Men (SD) Age Women (SD) Twin (6412 twins) (11.19) (10.97) (m: 2036+f: 4376) Sister (1020 sisters) (11.81) Brother (602 brothers) (13.67) Mother + father (4560 parents) (7.21) (7.96) doi: /journal.pone t001 the questionnaire the second time after 6-months to obtain retest data. For survey 8, participants could complete the survey online or by paper and pencil. Individuals were invited by letter, which contained a personalized login-name and password to complete the survey online. If the online survey was not completed within 6 weeks, a reminder was sent together with a paper copy of the survey that could be mailed back free of charge. For this study we analyzed the ADHD index data from survey 7 and added data from survey 8 that were available from the (ongoing) online data collection. For 2,519 subjects repeated-measures (i.e. survey 7 and survey 8 internet data) were available from both time points. If two scores were available, the first one (survey 7) was chosen. In total, there were phenotype data available for 15,273 individuals. We excluded offspring of twins (N = 341), subjects with incomplete data on sex (N = 190), who were younger than 18 years or who had no information on age (N = 936), or on zygosity (N = 140), half-siblings (N = 16), non-biological parents (N = 21), the third person in triplets (N = 28) and spouses of twins (N = 969). Two brothers and two sisters per family were included in the analyses; additional remaining siblings were excluded (N = 39). This resulted in a sample of 12,594 subjects. Tables 1 and 2 give details of the sample composition. Zygosity of same-sex twins was based on DNA polymorphisms or on validated survey questions. For 2660 same-sex twins (997 from complete and 666 twins from incomplete pairs) zygosity information was available from DNA testing. The study was approved by the Central Ethics Committee on Research involving human subjects of the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, an Institutional Review Board certified by the US Office of Human Research Protections (IRB number IRB-2991 under Federal-wide Assurance-3703; IRB/institute codes, NTR ). All subjects provided written informed consent. Phenotype Measures In survey 7, the screening self-report (CAARS - S:SV) form of Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS) was included that consists of 30 items accessing ADHD symptom level according to Table 2. Number of twins by zygosity. Twin zygosity Complete pairs Total Number of twins MonoZygotic Males (MZM) DiZygotic Males (DZM) MonoZygotic Females (MZF) DiZygotic Females (DZF) DiZygotic Opposite Sex (DOS) /826 (m/f) doi: /journal.pone t002 PLoS ONE 2 May 2010 Volume 5 Issue 5 e10621

3 DSM-IV criteria [15] and contains 12 items of the ADHD index. In survey 7 all 30 items were assessed; in survey 8 only the 12 items of the ADHD index were included. The ADHD index is designed to identify the adults in a population that are likely to be diagnosed with ADHD. Each of the 12 items of the ADHD index was scored on the scale from 0 to 4, missing items were handled as per CAARS manual recommendations [15]. Genetic modeling Twin-family studies make use of the different degree of genetic relatedness of family members to estimate the relative contribution of genes and environment to the variance of a trait. Monozygotic (MZ) twins are genetically (nearly) identical. Under random mating dizygotic (DZ) twins and non-twin sibling share on average 50% of their segregating genes, while parents and offspring share exactly 50% of their autosomal genes. In quantitative genetic analyses, phenotypic variance is modeled as a function of genetic (G), shared (C), and non-shared environmental (E) influences. Genetic variance can be additive (A), indicating that the effects of alleles are additive, or non-additive (dominance; D) referring to the interaction among alleles. All genetic effects are shared by MZ twins. DZ twins and siblings are correlated 0.5 for their additive genetic values, as are parents and offspring. DZ twins and siblings are correlated 0.25 for their non-additive genetic values, for parents and offspring this correlation is zero. Under non-random mating the correlations among family members for additive genetic values may increase [19]. Twin correlations provide a first impression of the relative contribution of A, C, D and E. The more similar MZ twins are in their phenotypes compared to DZ twins and non-twin siblings, the more variance in a trait is caused by genetic effects. When the DZ correlation is less than half the MZ correlation, there is evidence for D. Differences within MZ twin pairs are due to E which also includes measurement error. Genetic analyses were carried out using structural equation modeling in Mx [22] using raw-data maximum likelihood estimation of parameters. In a saturated model (with input the 868 data matrix of 2 twins, 2 sisters, 2 brothers, mother and father data) we first tested for sex differences in means and variances, for age regression on the ADHD index and for heterogeneity of correlations among family members. The extent to which A, D and E influence the variance in the ADHD index was estimated with the genetic modeling. First, a simple ADE variance decomposition model was fitted to the data, in which the correlation between spouses was simply estimated, but not allowed to influence any other parameters in the model. This would be the case, for example, when living together leads to spousal resemblance. Next, the spousal correlation was modeled as due to phenotypic assortment and several models of familial resemblance were evaluated, allowing for the effects of phenotypic assortative mating. In this last series of models we also evaluated the importance of cultural transmission, in addition to genetic transmission from parents to offspring. Statistical testing The standard approach to the comparison of different models is by means of likelihood-ratio tests, by subtracting the negative log likelihood (-2LL) for a more restricted model from the -2LL for a more general model. This yields a statistic that is distributed as x 2 with degrees of freedom (df ) equal to the difference in the number of parameters in the two models. If the x 2 -test yields a significant p-value (e.g. lower than 0.05 or 0.01), the constrained model is deemed not significantly worse. However, the chi-squared value is inflated as the sample size increases. For this reason, big discrepancies in small samples may not be significant whereas small differences in big sized samples are significant. Given the large sample size in this study (.12,000 subjects) decisions about goodness-of-fit were based on the RMSEA (Root Mean Square Error of Approximation) which is obtained as: RMSEA =!(F0/df), where F0 is the chosen fit function [23]. Good models have an RMSEA of.05 or less. Models whose RMSEA is.10 or more fit poorly. Results Test-retest data were available for 2,519 subjects who took part in NTR surveys 7 and 8. The average age was (SD = 13.66) at the first and (SD = 13.77) at the second assessments. The correlation between the two assessments was There were no differences in average scores for ADHD index (7.75 (SD = 3.79) and 7.54 (SD = 3.87)) between the two assessments. The six month test -retest correlation assessed on 200 unrelated individuals who took part in survey 7 was Figure 1 shows the distribution of the ADHD index in men and women. For this figure, ADHD index scores were transformed into the T-scores within each sex group. A T-score of.65 is considered significant in clinical groups [15]. In the whole sample, 6.8% of women and 7.4% of men had a T-score over 65. In both sexes, the distribution of scores showed a good approximation to a normal distribution with some skewness in the right tail (high end of the scale). The estimates of means from a saturated model (corrected and uncorrected for age) are given in Table 3 for male and female offspring and for the parents. The estimates of familial correlations are given in Table 4. Model tests are summarized in Table 5. The regression of ADHD index on age (in years) was negative, as expected, and was the same in males and females. The point estimate was 20.02, which is non-significant when judged by RMSEA (,0.05). Women score somewhat higher on the ADHD index than men. There is no generation difference when the regression on age is taken into account. The difference between the sexes is small and the significant chi-squared test is likely to be caused by the very large sample size. Both the sex differences in means and in SD are not significant when judged by RMSEA (,0.05). Table 4 presents the familial correlations for ADHD index in MZ and DZ twins, in sib-sib and twin-sib pairs, in parent-offspring pairs and in parents. All correlations are given conditional on sex. MZM (monozygotic male twin pairs) and MZF (monozygotic female twin pairs) correlations are moderate (0.34 and 0.38, respectively). Correlations in first-degree relatives are somewhat smaller than half the MZ correlations. It is of note that parentoffspring correlations are not lower than DZ or sibling correlations, suggesting that there is little evidence that a different set of genes influences the phenotype in older subjects. For all firstdegree relative pairs (male-male, female-female and opposite-sex pairs) correlations could be constrained to be equal and were estimated at 0.09 (male pairs), 0.19 (female pairs) and 0.12 (opposite-sex pairs). The confidence intervals around these correlations overlap and a formal test of equality showed that all correlations between first-degree relatives could be constrained to be the same. The estimate of the correlation in first-degree relatives is 0.14 (95% CI = ). The correlation in MZM and MZF can also be constrained to be the same and is estimated at 0.36 (95% CI = ). The correlation between ADHD index scores of parents was estimated at We explored if the best explanation for this correlation would be phenotypic assortment or whether spouses who live together for a longer period of time start to increasingly resemble each other. The PLoS ONE 3 May 2010 Volume 5 Issue 5 e10621

4 Figure 1. Phenotype distribution of the ADHD index in men and women. ADHD index scores were transformed into T-scores separately by sex. doi: /journal.pone g001 correlation between the absolute difference in ADHD scores and the mean age of the couple (assuming that older parents have been together for a longer period of time) was (N = 1,319). The pattern of familial correlations in Table 4 uggests that the familial resemblance that is observed is likely to primarily be explained by genetic factors. A correlation of 0.36 in MZ twins and of 0.14 in first-degree relatives suggests that genetic dominance (non-additivity) may play a role, although dominance would predict lower correlations in parents and their offspring than in siblings and this is not what was seen in the data. Genetic variance components modeling confirmed that the heritability of ADHD index scores is explained by additive genetic factors. In the ADE model, the contributions of A and D to the variance in ADHD index were 29% and 6% respectively. The contribution of D could be constrained at zero and heritability was 33% (95% CI = ). Next, a model that in addition to genetic also included the cultural transmission paths from parents to offspring was fitted to the twin-family data (Figure 2). This model also Table 3. Maximum likelihood estimates for means and standard deviations for ADHD index in offspring and parents and regression estimate on age. Mean ADHD (not age corrected) Mean ADHD (age corrected) SD ADHD (age corrected) Age regression Male offspring (m) Father Female offspring (f) Mother doi: /journal.pone t003 Table 4. Maximum likelihood estimates of familial correlations. r ADHD index 95% CI MZ twins MZ Male MZ Female Male first-degree relatives DZ Male brother - male twin brother - brother father -son Female first-degree relatives DZ Female sister - female twin sister - sister mother -daughter Female-Male first-degree relatives DZ Opposite Sex brother - female twin sister - brother sister male twin mother - son father -daughter Parents (father mother) doi: /journal.pone t004 PLoS ONE 4 May 2010 Volume 5 Issue 5 e10621

5 Table 5. Test results from fitting saturated and genetic models to ADHD data. Model NP* -2LL Versus model x 2 df p RMSEA 1 Saturated model ** Saturated no age regression No sex diff age regression NS Means and SD 4 Means equal within sex NS 5 All means equal SD equal within sex All SD equal Equal familial correlations 8 in opposite-sex relatives NS 9 in male relatives NS 10 in female relatives NS 11 in all first-degree relatives in MZ twins NS Variance components 13 ADE model NS 14 AE model NS Phenotypic assortment 15 ADE & cultural transmission NS 16 ADE, no cultural transmission NS 17 AE, no cultural transmission *NP: Number of estimated parameters. **Saturated model: 17 familial correlations, 4 means, 4 SD, 2 age regression = 27 parameters; -2LL = -2*Log-Likelihood; df = degrees of freedom for x 2 test; NS = not significant; RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation. doi: /journal.pone t005 included the genetic effects induced by phenotypic assortment specified as a copath [24] which represents an extrinsic correlation that influences the covariance structure of both antecedent and subsequent factors, but does not contribute to their variance and vertical cultural transmission. Model comparisons (Table 5 model 16 compared to 15) indicated that cultural transmission was not significant. Also, constraining genetic dominance at zero was allowed as judged by the low RMSEA. The final model included an estimate for phenotypic assortment and for additive genetic and unique environmental variance components (4.62 and 10.68, respectively), leading to a heritability estimate of 30%. Discussion This is the first large-scale genetic epidemiology study to report on the heritability of self-rated ADHD symptoms in adults. We assessed ADHD features with the ADHD index, based on the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales in a twin-family sample of over 12,000 adults. Stability of the phenotype was 0.66 both over a 6-month and a 2-year period. When employing a T-score cutoff of.65 the prevalence of adult ADHD was 6.8% in women and 7.4% in men. These prevalence estimates correspond well with those from an epidemiological study using screening and diagnostic assessment of ADHD in 10 countries which reported an overall prevalence of 3.4% and a prevalence for The Netherlands of 5% [25]. The heritability of the ADHD index, when corrected for assortative mating, was estimated to be 30%. There was no difference in heritability between men and women and also not between the parental (average age 55 years) and the offspring generations (average age 34 years). In fact, the parent-offspring correlations were of similar magnitude as the sib-sib and DZ twin correlations, providing evidence that the same set of genes may be playing a role across the adult life-span. The heritability estimate in adults is substantially lower than the heritability of ADHD and Attention Problems in children. A large longitudinal study of children aged 3 12 year estimated heritability for Attention Problems at 75% at all ages [26] and this is representative of nearly all studies carried out in children [27,28]. It is unclear if the lower heritability in adults should be explained by age-by-genotype interaction (whereby the expression of the genotype depends on age) or by the fact that ratings of ADHD in children are usually based on parental, teacher or clinician s reports whereas in adults the phenotype is based on self-report. The age at which heritability decreases from the high estimates in childhood to the lower estimate as reported in the current study may be somewhere during adolescence. A longitudinal study in young adult twins (18 30 years) of self-reported Attention Problems estimated its heritability at 40% [13]. It can be hypothesized that environment during childhood, in so far as it is relevant to ADHD and Attention Problems, is more uniform than later in life, when variation in environment is larger. For example, during childhood a significant amount of time is spend in school by all children. The lower heritability estimate in adults thus may mainly reflect an increase in the contribution of unique environmental influences. The effect of these influences may depend on genotype (genotype-environment interaction). If not explicitly modeled, GE interaction will increase the variance due PLoS ONE 5 May 2010 Volume 5 Issue 5 e10621

6 Figure 2. Path diagram of phenotypic assortment model with genetic and cultural transmission from parents to offspring. Squares represent the phenotypes of a DZ twin pair (P T1 and P T2 ) with one extra sibling (P Sib ), and both parents (P F and P M ). Latent factors are represented by circles and include A (additive genetic factor), D (dominance genetic factor), and E (non-shared environment). F represents vertical cultural transmission whereby the phenotype of the parents influences the environment of their offspring. Assortment of parents is modeled as a copath (i). Simultaneous genetic and cultural inheritance induces a correlation (s) between this environmental factor F and the genetic factor A. Path coefficients a, d and e represent the influence of latent factors on the phenotype. The variance due to vertical cultural transmission is represented by r and the variance of additive genetic factors by g. doi: /journal.pone g002 to unique environment, and thus decrease estimates of heritability [29,30]. In contrast, the effect of interactions between shared environment and genotype will be included in the genetic variance component. For children there have been reports of interaction between maternal smoking or drinking during pregnancy (by definition an exposure that is shared by twins and likely to be shared among siblings) and offspring genotype [31 33]. If effects of such early exposures tend to wane as children grow up, they may also contribute to the higher heritability estimates in children. The large difference between childhood and adult ADHD heritability is remarkable if we compare these results to those for IQ and cognition. ADHD tends to be associated with a lower IQ [34], but whereas childhood ADHD is highly heritable, childhood IQ shows a rather low heritability (,25% at age 5) which increases to around 70 to 80% in adults [35]. The developmental patterns of association and whether or not these associations are mediated by genes that influence both IQ and ADHD remain to be investigated. This study analyzed data from over 12,000 adult subjects. Although the large sample size offered the possibility to test for different models of genetic and cultural inheritance, a limitation of the present study is that the large sample size precluded a clinical diagnosis of the phenotype. Another limitation might be that survey studies in volunteer samples increasingly suffer from nonresponse. Distel et al. [21] analyzed data from one of the surveys (survey 7) that supplied data for the current analyses and looked at responders from families characterized by a high degree of cooperation (defined as most invited family members taking part in the study) and a low degree of cooperation (defined as one or only a few family members taking part). For genetic (or familial) traits, the data from responders from low-responsive families serve as a proxy for the missing data of their non-responding family members. CAARS data (inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive scales) were included in the analyses. Outcomes tended to be slightly more favorable for individuals from highly cooperative families compared to individuals from less cooperative families, but after correction for multiple testing this effect was not significant for the ADHD scales. These results confirmed those from an earlier study [36] that looked at differences in Attention Problems [12] between subjects from highly and less cooperative families. Non-response bias thus may play a role for ADHD, but its effects are unlikely to be large. It has been speculated that the heritability of ADHD, especially of the persistent form, may be higher in adulthood than in childhood [37,38]. This paper signifies that in a general population sample the heritability of ADHD features in adults is substantially lower than that in children. A similar conclusion was drawn when analyzing longitudinal data on Attention Problems in young adults [13]. However, in the current study we could not take into consideration whether or not the adults who took part in the NTR survey studies might have suffered from ADHD in childhood. Currently we are collecting CAARS data in young adult twins who were registered with the Netherlands Twin Register as newborns and for whom parental and teacher data on Attention Problems and Overactive Behavior have been collected from age 3 onwards [27,39,40]. This will allow examination of heterogeneity in heritability estimates by stratification of the adult sample into subgroups with persistent and non-persistent ADHD. This study clearly established the moderate stability and heritability of ADHD in adults. It paves the way for future investigations needed to fully understand the complex architecture of ADHD, such as studies on the heterogeneity of the phenotype, genome-wide linkage and association studies in adults and investigations of the effects of genotype-environment interaction and correlation. As detailed by recent reviews and meta-analyses [4,37,41 43] attempts to localize and identify genes underlying the vulnerability to ADHD have not yet been very successful. In spite of the lower heritability of adult ADHD as compared to childhood ADHD, we can now attempt to find genetic polymorphisms for ADHD in adults. The NTR-Biobank [44,45] has collected DNA and RNA samples in over 9500 participants and genotyping is currently under way for a large subsample. Other efforts in adults include the IMpACT initiative [37]. The contribution of (large) numbers of genes across childhood and adulthood can be investigated with genome-wide profiling [46]. In this approach, the effect of multiple SNPs are tested rather than the effects of individual SNPs. These SNPs are not required to reach a genomewide significance level by themselves, but their combined effect is captured in a genome-wide genetic risk score which can be compared across samples. Acknowledgments We thank all participants from the Netherlands Twin Register. We thank Dr JJS Kooij for careful reading of the manuscript. Author Contributions Conceived and designed the experiments: DIB EJCdG GW. Performed the experiments: MAD MHMdM LMG JHDAvB MB. Analyzed the data: DIB VS JJH MHMdM JMV. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: JJH MAD MHMdM JMV MB EJCdG. Wrote the paper: DIB VS. PLoS ONE 6 May 2010 Volume 5 Issue 5 e10621

7 References 1. McIntosh D, Kutcher S, Binder C, Levitt A, Fallu A, et al. (2009) Adult ADHD and comorbid depression: A consensus-derived diagnostic algorithm for ADHD. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 5: Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, et al. (2006) The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry 163: Faraone SV, Doyle AE (2001) The nature and heritability of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 10: , viii ix. 4. Franke B, Neale BM, Faraone SV (2009) Genome-wide association studies in ADHD. Hum Genet 126: Derks EM, Hudizak JJ, Boomsma DI (2009) Genetics of ADHD, Hyperactivity, and Attention Problems. In: Kim Y-K, ed. Handbook of Behavior Genetics. New York: Springer. pp Faraone SV, Biederman J, Mick E (2006) The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychol Med 36: Faraone SV, Biederman J (2005) What is the prevalence of adult ADHD? Results of a population screen of 966 adults. J Atten Disord 9: Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA (2007) The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry 164: Simon V, Czobor P, Balint S, Meszaros A, Bitter I (2009) Prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 194: Antshel KM, Faraone SV, Maglione K, Doyle A, Fried R, et al. (2009) Is adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder a valid diagnosis in the presence of high IQ? Psychol Med 39: Moss SB, Nair R, Vallarino A, Wang S (2007) Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. Prim Care 34: , v. 12. Achenbach TM (1997) Manual for the young adult self-report and young adult behavior checklist. BurlingtonVT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. pp van den Berg SM, Willemsen G, de Geus EJ, Boomsma DI (2006) Genetic etiology of stability of attention problems in young adulthood. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 141B: Boomsma DI, de Geus EJ, Vink JM, Stubbe JH, Distel MA, et al. (2006) Netherlands Twin Register: from twins to twin families. Twin Res Hum Genet 9: Conners CK, Erhardt D, Sparrow E (1999) Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS). North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health systems Inc. 144 p. 16. Kooij JJS, Boonstra AM, Swinkels SHN, Bekker EM, de Noord I, et al. (2008) Reliability, Validity, and Utility of Instruments for Self-Report and Informant Report Concerning Symptoms of ADHD in Adult Patients. J Atten Disord 11: Posthuma D, Boomsma DI (2000) A note on the statistical power in extended twin designs. Behav Genet 30: Maes HH, Neale MC, Kendler KS, Hewitt JK, Silberg JL, et al. (1998) Assortative mating for major psychiatric diagnoses in two population-based samples. Psychol Med 28: van Grootheest DS, van den Berg SM, Cath DC, Willemsen G, Boomsma DI (2008) Marital resemblance for obsessive-compulsive, anxious and depressive symptoms in a population-based sample. Psychol Med 38: Cavalli-Sforza LL, Bodmer WF (1971) Quantitative characters, polygenic inheritance, and environmental interactions. The Genetics of Human Populations. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman and Company. pp Distel MA, Ligthart L, Willemsen G, Nyholt DR, Trull TJ, et al. (2007) Personality, health and lifestyle in a questionnaire family study: a comparison between highly cooperative and less cooperative families. Twin Res Hum Genet 10: Neale MC, Booker SM, Xie G, Maes HH (2006) Mx: Statistical Modeling. Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry. 204 p. 23. Browne MW, Cudeck R (1993) Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, eds. Testing Structural Equation Models (SAGE Focus Editions): Sage Publications, Inc. pp Cloninger CR (1980) Interpretation of intrinsic and extrinsic structural relations by path analysis: theory and applications to assortative mating. Genetical Research 36: Fayyad J, De Graaf R, Kessler R, Alonso J, Angermeyer M, et al. (2007) Crossnational prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Br J Psychiatry 190: Rietveld MJ, Hudziak JJ, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt CE, Boomsma DI (2004) Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to 12. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45: Derks EM, Hudziak JJ, van Beijsterveldt CE, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI (2004) A study of genetic and environmental influences on maternal and paternal CBCL syndrome scores in a large sample of 3-year-old Dutch twins. Behav Genet 34: Waldman ID, Gizer IR (2006) The genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin Psychol Rev 26: Molenaar PC, Boomsma DI (1987) Application of nonlinear factor analysis to genotype-environment interaction. Behav Genet 17: Purcell S (2002) Variance components models for gene-environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Res 5: Brookes KJ, Mill J, Guindalini C, Curran S, Xu X, et al. (2006) A common haplotype of the dopamine transporter gene associated with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder and interacting with maternal use of alcohol during pregnancy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63: Kahn RS, Khoury J, Nichols WC, Lanphear BP (2003) Role of dopamine transporter genotype and maternal prenatal smoking in childhood hyperactiveimpulsive, inattentive, and oppositional behaviors. J Pediatr 143: Ficks CA, Waldman ID (2009) Gene-environment interactions in attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 11: Polderman TJ, de Geus EJ, Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, van Leeuwen M, et al. (2009) Attention problems, inhibitory control, and intelligence index overlapping genetic factors: a study in 9-, 12-, and 18-year-old twins. Neuropsychology 23: Haworth CM, Wright MJ, Luciano M, Martin NG, de Geus EJ, et al. (2009) The heritability of general cognitive ability increases linearly from childhood to young adulthood. Mol Psychiatry;In press. 36. Vink JM, Willemsen G, Stubbe JH, Middeldorp CM, Ligthart RS, et al. (2004) Estimating non-response bias in family studies: application to mental health and lifestyle. Eur J Epidemiol 19: Banaschewski T, Becker K, Scherag S, Franke B, Coghill D (2010) Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an overview. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 19: Faraone SV (2004) Genetics of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 27: Hudziak JJ, Althoff RR, Derks EM, Faraone SV, Boomsma DI (2005) Prevalence and genetic architecture of Child Behavior Checklist-juvenile bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry 58: Derks EM, Hudziak JJ, Van Beijsterveldt CE, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI (2006) Genetic analyses of maternal and teacher ratings on attention problems in 7-year-old Dutch twins. Behav Genet 36: Gizer IR, Ficks C, Waldman ID (2009) Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic review. Hum Genet 126: Zhou K, Dempfle A, Arcos-Burgos M, Bakker SC, Banaschewski T, et al. (2008) Meta-analysis of genome-wide linkage scans of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B: Asherson P, Zhou K, Anney RJ, Franke B, Buitelaar J, et al. (2008) A highdensity SNP linkage scan with 142 combined subtype ADHD sib pairs identifies linkage regions on chromosomes 9 and 16. Mol Psychiatry 13: Boomsma DI, Willemsen G, Sullivan PF, Heutink P, Meijer P, et al. (2008) Genome-wide association of major depression: description of samples for the GAIN Major Depressive Disorder Study: NTR and NESDA biobank projects. Eur J Hum Genet 16: Willemsen G, de Geus EJC, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt CEMT, Brooks AI, et al. (2010) The Netherlands Twin Register Biobank: a resource for genetic epidemiological studies. Twin Res Hum Genet;In press. 46. Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL, Visscher PM, O Donovan MC, et al. (2009) Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 460: PLoS ONE 7 May 2010 Volume 5 Issue 5 e10621

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H

More information

Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): A Longitudinal Multiple Informant Study of Problem Behavior

Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): A Longitudinal Multiple Informant Study of Problem Behavior Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): A Longitudinal Multiple Informant Study of Problem Behavior Meike Bartels, 1 C. E. M. (Toos) van Beijsterveldt, 1 Eske M. Derks, 1 Therese M. Stroet, 1 Tinca J.

More information

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H PSYC H O PAT H O LO G Y : PSYC H I AT R I C P R O B LEMS A N D T H E A S SO C I

More information

Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale

Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48:11 (27), pp 18 187 doi:1.1111/j.1469-761.27.1783.x Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale Tinca J.C. Polderman,

More information

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins Behavior Genetics, Vol. 33, No. 5, September 2003 ( 2003) Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt,

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Relation Between Attention Problems and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Relation Between Attention Problems and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Behav Genet (2008) 38:11 23 DOI 10.1007/s10519-007-9178-8 ORIGINAL RESEARCH Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Relation Between Attention Problems and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

More information

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Risk factors for the development and outcome of childhood psychopathology SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Chapter 147 In this chapter I present a summary of the results of the studies described in this thesis followed

More information

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION Chapter 8 GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION A Longitudinal Study in 3- to 12-Year-Old Children Dorret I. Boomsma, Ph.D. C.E.M. van Beijsterveldt, Ph.D. Meike Bartels, Ph.D. James

More information

The Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Oppositional Defiant Behavior: A Multi-informant Twin Study

The Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Oppositional Defiant Behavior: A Multi-informant Twin Study The Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Oppositional Defiant Behavior: A Multi-informant Twin Study JAMES J. HUDZIAK, M.D., ESKE M. DERKS, M.A., ROBERT R. ALTHOFF, M.D., PH.D., WILLIAM COPELAND,

More information

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement Psychological Methods 2007, Vol. 12, No. 4, 451 466 CORRECTED APRIL 10, 2008; SEE LAST PAGE Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 1082-989X/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.4.451

More information

NETHERLANDS TWIN REGISTER. Dorret Boomsma

NETHERLANDS TWIN REGISTER. Dorret Boomsma NETHERLANDS TWIN REGISTER Dorret Boomsma [di.boomsma@vu.nl] I will discuss NTR: a longitudinal twin-family study Why twins Not only twins: Multigeneration An example: ACTION Netherlands Twin Register Netherlands

More information

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance Twin Research (1999) 2, 99 107 1999 Stockton Press All rights reserved 1369 0523/99 $12.00 http://www.stockton-press.co.uk/tr Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of

More information

More boys than girls with attention deficit hyperactivity

More boys than girls with attention deficit hyperactivity Why More Boys Than Girls With ADHD Receive Treatment: A Study of Dutch Twins Eske M. Derks, 1 James J. Hudziak, 2,3 and Dorret I. Boomsma 1 1 Department of Biological Psychology,Vrije Universiteit,Amsterdam,

More information

Heritability of Anxious-Depressive and Withdrawn Behavior: Age-Related Changes During Adolescence

Heritability of Anxious-Depressive and Withdrawn Behavior: Age-Related Changes During Adolescence NEW RESEARCH Heritability of Anxious-Depressive and Withdrawn Behavior: Age-Related Changes During Adolescence Diane J. Lamb, M.Sc., Christel M. Middeldorp, M.D., Ph.D., Catarina E.M. van Beijsterveldt,

More information

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder DOI 10.1007/s10519-014-9670-x ORIGINAL RESEARCH Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder J. L. Ebejer S. E. Medland J.

More information

Evidence for shared genetic influences on selfreported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian twins

Evidence for shared genetic influences on selfreported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian twins Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Open Access Publications 2008 Evidence for shared genetic influences on selfreported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian

More information

Childhood problem behavior and parental divorce: evidence for gene environment interaction

Childhood problem behavior and parental divorce: evidence for gene environment interaction Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol (2012) 47:1539 1548 DOI 10.1007/s00127-011-0470-9 ORIGINAL PAPER Childhood problem behavior and parental divorce: evidence for gene environment interaction Sylvana Robbers

More information

Genetic influences on disordered eating behaviour are largely independent of body mass index

Genetic influences on disordered eating behaviour are largely independent of body mass index Acta Psychiatr Scand 2008: 117: 348 356 All rights reserved DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01132.x Copyright Ó 2007 The Authors Journal Compilation Ó 2007 Blackwell Munksgaard ACTA PSYCHIATRICA SCANDINAVICA

More information

The existence, prevalence, and taxonomy of juvenile bipolar

The existence, prevalence, and taxonomy of juvenile bipolar Longitudinal Stability of the CBCL-Juvenile Bipolar Disorder Phenotype: A Study in Dutch Twins Dorret I. Boomsma, Irene Rebollo, Eske M. Derks, Toos C.E.M. van Beijsterveldt, Robert R. Althoff, David C.

More information

The aim of this study was to investigate the reliability

The aim of this study was to investigate the reliability Jacqueline M. Langendonk, 1 C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt, 1 Silvia I. Brouwer, 1 Therese Stroet, 1 James J. Hudziak, 1,2 and Dorret I. Boomsma 1 1 Department of Biological Psychology,Vrije Universiteit,Amsterdam,

More information

ARTICLE. Genetic and Environmental Influences on Individual Differences in Sedentary Behavior During Adolescence

ARTICLE. Genetic and Environmental Influences on Individual Differences in Sedentary Behavior During Adolescence ONLINE FIRST ARTICLE Genetic and Environmental Influences on Individual Differences in Sedentary Behavior During Adolescence A Twin-Family Study Niels van der Aa, PhD; Meike Bartels, PhD; Saskia J. te

More information

Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study

Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study JAMES J. HUDZIAK, M.D., WILLIAM COPELAND, B.A., LAWRENCE P. RUDIGER, PH.D., THOMAS M. ACHENBACH, PH.D., ANDREW C. HEATH, D.PHIL., AND RICHARD

More information

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Self-Report Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms in Dutch Adolescents at Ages 12, 14, and 16

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Self-Report Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms in Dutch Adolescents at Ages 12, 14, and 16 Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Self-Report Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms in Dutch Adolescents at Ages 12, 14, and 16 DANIËL S. VAN GROOTHEEST, M.D., MEIKE BARTELS, PH.D., CATARINA E.M. VAN

More information

Trends in adolescent alcohol use: effects of age, sex and cohort on prevalence and heritabilityadd_

Trends in adolescent alcohol use: effects of age, sex and cohort on prevalence and heritabilityadd_ RESEARCH REPORT doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03612.x Trends in adolescent alcohol use: effects of age, sex and cohort on prevalence and heritabilityadd_3612 518..527 Lot M. Geels 1,2, Meike Bartels 1,2,

More information

Discontinuous Traits. Chapter 22. Quantitative Traits. Types of Quantitative Traits. Few, distinct phenotypes. Also called discrete characters

Discontinuous Traits. Chapter 22. Quantitative Traits. Types of Quantitative Traits. Few, distinct phenotypes. Also called discrete characters Discontinuous Traits Few, distinct phenotypes Chapter 22 Also called discrete characters Quantitative Genetics Examples: Pea shape, eye color in Drosophila, Flower color Quantitative Traits Phenotype is

More information

In the classic twin design, estimation of genetic and

In the classic twin design, estimation of genetic and A Test of the Equal Environment Assumption (EEA) in Multivariate Twin Studies Eske M. Derks, 1 Conor V. Dolan, 2 and Dorret I. Boomsma 1 1 Department of Biological Psychology,Vrije Universiteit,Amsterdam,

More information

Stable Genetic Effects on Symptoms of Alcohol Abuse and Dependence from Adolescence into Early Adulthood

Stable Genetic Effects on Symptoms of Alcohol Abuse and Dependence from Adolescence into Early Adulthood Behav Genet (2012) 42:40 56 DOI 10.1007/s10519-011-9488-8 ORIGINAL RESEARCH Stable Genetic Effects on Symptoms of Alcohol Abuse and Dependence from Adolescence into Early Adulthood Jenny H. D. A. van Beek

More information

Flexible Mx specification of various extended twin kinship designs

Flexible Mx specification of various extended twin kinship designs Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Open Access Publications 2009 Flexible Mx specification of various extended twin kinship designs Hermine H. Maes Virginia Commonwealth University

More information

MARC Project 4: Australian Children of Alcoholic Female Twins

MARC Project 4: Australian Children of Alcoholic Female Twins MARC Project 4: Australian Children of Alcoholic Female Twins Mary Waldron, Valerie S. Knopik, Theodore Jacob, Anne Glowinski, Nicholas Martin, & Andrew Heath Background Although it has been widely embraced

More information

The Genetic Basis of Problem Behavior in 5-Year-Old Dutch Twin Pairs

The Genetic Basis of Problem Behavior in 5-Year-Old Dutch Twin Pairs Behavior Genetics, Vol. 34, No. 3, May 2004 ( 2004) The Genetic Basis of Problem Behavior in 5-Year-Old Dutch Twin Pairs C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt, 1,4 F. C. Verhulst, 2 P. C. M. Molenaar, 3 and D. I.

More information

In Australian twins participating in three different

In Australian twins participating in three different Heritability and Stability of Resting Blood Pressure in Australian Twins Jouke-Jan Hottenga, 1 John B. Whitfield, 2 Eco J. C. de Geus, 1 Dorret I. Boomsma, 1 and Nicholas G. Martin 2 1 Department of Biological

More information

Sex Differences in Genetic Architecture of Complex Phenotypes?

Sex Differences in Genetic Architecture of Complex Phenotypes? Sex Differences in Genetic Architecture of Complex Phenotypes? Jacqueline M. Vink*, Meike Bartels, Toos C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt, Jenny van Dongen, Jenny H. D. A. van Beek, Marijn A. Distel, Marleen

More information

The latent class structure of ADHD is stable across informants

The latent class structure of ADHD is stable across informants Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Open Access Publications 2006 The latent class structure of ADHD is stable across informants Robert R. Althoff University of Vermont William

More information

Researchers probe genetic overlap between ADHD, autism

Researchers probe genetic overlap between ADHD, autism NEWS Researchers probe genetic overlap between ADHD, autism BY ANDREA ANDERSON 22 APRIL 2010 1 / 7 Puzzling link: More than half of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder meet the diagnostic

More information

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children J. Intell. 2015, 3, 160-167; doi:10.3390/jintelligence3040160 Brief Report OPEN ACCESS Journal of Intelligence ISSN 2079-3200 www.mdpi.com/journal/jintelligence Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations

More information

Genetic Influences on Childhood IQ in 5- and 7-Year-Old Dutch Twins

Genetic Influences on Childhood IQ in 5- and 7-Year-Old Dutch Twins DEVELOPMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 14{\), 115-126 Copyright 1998, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Genetic Influences on Childhood IQ in 5- and 7-Year-Old Dutch Twins Dorret I. Boomsma and G. Caroline M.

More information

Aconsistent finding from twin studies is that the

Aconsistent finding from twin studies is that the SPECIAL SECTION UNDERSTANDING THE ASTHMA EPIDEMIC: CAN TWIN STUDIES HELP? An Exploration of Gene Environment Interaction and Asthma in a Large Sample of 5-Year-Old Dutch Twins Toos C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt

More information

Dan Koller, Ph.D. Medical and Molecular Genetics

Dan Koller, Ph.D. Medical and Molecular Genetics Design of Genetic Studies Dan Koller, Ph.D. Research Assistant Professor Medical and Molecular Genetics Genetics and Medicine Over the past decade, advances from genetics have permeated medicine Identification

More information

The association of current smoking behavior with the smoking behavior of parents, siblings, friends and spouses

The association of current smoking behavior with the smoking behavior of parents, siblings, friends and spouses Blackwell Science, LtdOxford, UKADDAddiction1360-0443 2003 Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs98Original ArticleFamilial and social risk factors for smokingjacqueline M. Vink

More information

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study Diabetologia (2013) 56:1031 1035 DOI 10.1007/s00125-013-2859-3 SHORT COMMUNICATION Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study S. Carlsson & A. Ahlbom & P. Lichtenstein

More information

Birth Order and Sibling Gender Ratio of a Clinical Sample of Children and Adolescents Diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Birth Order and Sibling Gender Ratio of a Clinical Sample of Children and Adolescents Diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Birth Order and Sibling Gender Ratio of a Clinical Sample Original Article Birth Order and Sibling Gender Ratio of a Clinical Sample of Children and Adolescents Diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity

More information

Summary & general discussion

Summary & general discussion Summary & general discussion 160 chapter 8 The aim of this thesis was to identify genetic and environmental risk factors for behavioral problems, in particular Attention Problems (AP) and Attention Deficit

More information

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating Cynthia M. Bulik,* Patrick F. Sullivan, and Kenneth S. Kendler Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics of Virginia

More information

Genetic and Cultural Transmission of Smoking Initiation: An Extended Twin Kinship Model

Genetic and Cultural Transmission of Smoking Initiation: An Extended Twin Kinship Model Behav Genet (2006) 36:795 808 DOI 0.007/s059-006-9085-4 ORIGINAL PAPER Genetic and ultural ransmission of Smoking Initiation: An Extended win Kinship Model Hermine H. Maes Æ Michael. Neale Æ Kenneth S.

More information

Causes of individual differences in adolescent optimism: a study in Dutch twins and their siblings

Causes of individual differences in adolescent optimism: a study in Dutch twins and their siblings Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2015) 24:1381 1388 DOI 10.1007/s00787-015-0680-x ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Causes of individual differences in adolescent optimism: a study in Dutch twins and their siblings Rezan

More information

Australian children of alcoholic female twins

Australian children of alcoholic female twins Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Posters 2005: Alcoholism and Comorbidity 2005 Australian children of alcoholic female twins Wendy S. Slutske Follow this and additional works

More information

IV. Additional information regarding diffusion imaging acquisition procedure

IV. Additional information regarding diffusion imaging acquisition procedure Data Supplement for Ameis et al., A Diffusion Tensor Imaging Study in Children with ADHD, ASD, OCD and Matched Controls: Distinct and Non-distinct White Matter Disruption and Dimensional Brain-Behavior

More information

Personality traits are developmentally dynamic constructs

Personality traits are developmentally dynamic constructs Nine-Year Stability of Type D Personality: Contributions of Genes and Environment NINA KUPPER, PHD, DORRET I. BOOMSMA, PHD, ECO J. C. DE GEUS, PHD, JOHAN DENOLLET, PHD, AND GONNEKE WILLEMSEN, PHD Objective:

More information

Chapter 12 Discussion & Summary

Chapter 12 Discussion & Summary Chapter 12 Discussion & Summary volume 1 number 01 pages 117-124 september 18 th - 2008 published once 117 Discussion & Summary This thesis describes the study of genetic and environmental influences on

More information

No effect of classroom sharing on educational achievement in twins: a prospective, longitudinal cohort study

No effect of classroom sharing on educational achievement in twins: a prospective, longitudinal cohort study No effect of classroom sharing on educational achievement in twins: a prospective, longitudinal cohort study Tinca Jc Polderman, Meike Bartels, Frank C Verhulst, Anja C Huizink, Catherina Em Van Beijsterveldt,

More information

Original Article The prevalence and factors affecting attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among school children in Khartoum State

Original Article The prevalence and factors affecting attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among school children in Khartoum State Original Article The prevalence and factors affecting attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among school children in Khartoum State Amira Mohammed Osman (1), Ilham Mohammed Omer (2), Abdalla Abderahman

More information

Today s Topics. Cracking the Genetic Code. The Process of Genetic Transmission. The Process of Genetic Transmission. Genes

Today s Topics. Cracking the Genetic Code. The Process of Genetic Transmission. The Process of Genetic Transmission. Genes Today s Topics Mechanisms of Heredity Biology of Heredity Genetic Disorders Research Methods in Behavioral Genetics Gene x Environment Interactions The Process of Genetic Transmission Genes: segments of

More information

This article analyzes the effect of classroom separation

This article analyzes the effect of classroom separation Does Sharing the Same Class in School Improve Cognitive Abilities of Twins? Dinand Webbink, 1 David Hay, 2 and Peter M. Visscher 3 1 CPB Netherlands Bureau for Economic Policy Analysis,The Hague, the Netherlands

More information

Genetic and environmental influences on obsessive-compulsive symptoms in adults: a population-based twin-family study

Genetic and environmental influences on obsessive-compulsive symptoms in adults: a population-based twin-family study Psychological Medicine, 2007, 37, 1635 1644. f 2007 Cambridge University Press doi:10.1017/s0033291707000980 First published online 25 June 2007 Printed in the United Kingdom Genetic and environmental

More information

SEMs for Genetic Analysis

SEMs for Genetic Analysis SEMs for Genetic Analysis Session 8, Lecture 4 2/3/06 Basic Genetic Analysis of Twin Samples Using SEMs SEMs and variance explained Motivating example Simple correlation test The heritability concept Structural

More information

Interaction of Genes and the Environment

Interaction of Genes and the Environment Some Traits Are Controlled by Two or More Genes! Phenotypes can be discontinuous or continuous Interaction of Genes and the Environment Chapter 5! Discontinuous variation Phenotypes that fall into two

More information

Twin singleton differences in intelligence?

Twin singleton differences in intelligence? Twin Research (2000) 3, 83 87 2000 Macmillan Publishers Ltd All rights reserved 1369 0523/00 $15.00 y www.nature.com/tr Twin singleton differences in intelligence? Daniëlle Posthuma 1, Eco JC De Geus 1,

More information

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL COVARIATION BETWEEN AUTISTIC TRAITS AND BEHAVIORAL PROBLEMS. Dorret I. Boomsma 1. BT Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL COVARIATION BETWEEN AUTISTIC TRAITS AND BEHAVIORAL PROBLEMS. Dorret I. Boomsma 1. BT Amsterdam, the Netherlands. GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL COVARIATION BETWEEN AUTISTIC TRAITS AND BEHAVIORAL PROBLEMS Rosa A. Hoekstra 1*, Meike Bartels 1, James J. Hudziak 2, Toos C.E.M. Van Beijsterveldt 1, Dorret I. Boomsma 1. 1 Department

More information

Sports Participation during Adolescence: A Shift from Environmental to Genetic Factors

Sports Participation during Adolescence: A Shift from Environmental to Genetic Factors BASIC SCIENCES Original Investigations Sports Participation during Adolescence: A Shift from Environmental to Genetic Factors JANINE H. STUBBE, DORRET I. BOOMSMA, and ECO J. C. DE GEUS Department of Biological

More information

We examined the contribution of genetic and

We examined the contribution of genetic and Heritability and Stability of Resting Blood Pressure Jouke-Jan Hottenga, 1 Dorret I. Boomsma, 1 Nina Kupper, 1 Danielle Posthuma, 1 Harold Snieder, 2 Gonneke Willemsen, 1 and Eco J. C. de Geus 1 1 Department

More information

Frequency of Truancy at High School: Evidence for Genetic and Twin Specific Shared Environmental Influences

Frequency of Truancy at High School: Evidence for Genetic and Twin Specific Shared Environmental Influences Journal of Adolescent Health 45 (2009) 579 586 Original article Frequency of Truancy at High School: Evidence for Genetic and Twin Specific Shared Environmental Influences Niels van der Aa, M.Sc.*, Irene

More information

The finding of a significant gene by environment

The finding of a significant gene by environment The Serotonin Transporter Gene Length Polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and Life Events: No Evidence for an Interaction Effect on Neuroticism and Anxious Depressive Symptoms Christel M. Middeldorp, 1,2,3 Eco J.

More information

Continuity and Change in Preschool ADHD Symptoms: Longitudinal Genetic Analysis with Contrast Effects

Continuity and Change in Preschool ADHD Symptoms: Longitudinal Genetic Analysis with Contrast Effects Behavior Genetics, Vol. 35, No. 2, March 2005 (Ó 2005) Continuity and Change in Preschool ADHD Symptoms: Longitudinal Genetic Analysis with Contrast Effects Thomas S. Price, 1,4 Emily Simonoff, 2 Philip

More information

Nature Genetics: doi: /ng Supplementary Figure 1

Nature Genetics: doi: /ng Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Illustrative example of ptdt using height The expected value of a child s polygenic risk score (PRS) for a trait is the average of maternal and paternal PRS values. For example,

More information

Mx Scripts Library: Structural Equation Modeling Scripts for Twin and Family Data

Mx Scripts Library: Structural Equation Modeling Scripts for Twin and Family Data Behavior Genetics, Vol. 35, No. 4, July 2005 (Ó 2005) DOI: 10.1007/s10519-005-2791-5 Mx Scripts Library: Structural Equation Modeling Scripts for Twin and Family Data D Posthuma 1 and D. I. Boomsma 1 Received

More information

Interaction of Genes and the Environment

Interaction of Genes and the Environment Some Traits Are Controlled by Two or More Genes! Phenotypes can be discontinuous or continuous Interaction of Genes and the Environment Chapter 5! Discontinuous variation Phenotypes that fall into two

More information

Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to12

Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to12 Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45:3 (2004), pp 577 588 Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to12 M.J.H. Rietveld, 1 J.J. Hudziak,

More information

Attention Deficit Disorder. Evaluation Scale-Home Version 16. The Attention Deficit Disorders. Evaluation Scale-School Version 17

Attention Deficit Disorder. Evaluation Scale-Home Version 16. The Attention Deficit Disorders. Evaluation Scale-School Version 17 The Development of an Educational and Screening Instrument for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in a Pediatric Residency Program Stephen P. Amos, Ph.D., Robert Wittler, M.D., Corrie Nevil, M.D.,

More information

Heritability of Working in a Creative Profession

Heritability of Working in a Creative Profession Behav Genet (2017) 47:298 304 DOI 10.1007/s10519-016-9832-0 ORIGINAL RESEARCH Heritability of Working in a Creative Profession Mark Patrick Roeling 1 Gonneke Willemsen 2 Dorret I. Boomsma 2 Received: 6

More information

Combined Linkage and Association in Mx. Hermine Maes Kate Morley Dorret Boomsma Nick Martin Meike Bartels

Combined Linkage and Association in Mx. Hermine Maes Kate Morley Dorret Boomsma Nick Martin Meike Bartels Combined Linkage and Association in Mx Hermine Maes Kate Morley Dorret Boomsma Nick Martin Meike Bartels Boulder 2009 Outline Intro to Genetic Epidemiology Progression to Linkage via Path Models Linkage

More information

Genes, Diseases and Lisa How an advanced ICT research infrastructure contributes to our health

Genes, Diseases and Lisa How an advanced ICT research infrastructure contributes to our health Genes, Diseases and Lisa How an advanced ICT research infrastructure contributes to our health Danielle Posthuma Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research VU Amsterdam Most human diseases are heritable

More information

Resemblance between Relatives (Part 2) Resemblance Between Relatives (Part 2)

Resemblance between Relatives (Part 2) Resemblance Between Relatives (Part 2) Resemblance Between Relatives (Part 2) Resemblance of Full-Siblings Additive variance components can be estimated using the covariances of the trait values for relatives that do not have dominance effects.

More information

MOLECULAR EPIDEMIOLOGY Afiono Agung Prasetyo Faculty of Medicine Sebelas Maret University Indonesia

MOLECULAR EPIDEMIOLOGY Afiono Agung Prasetyo Faculty of Medicine Sebelas Maret University Indonesia MOLECULAR EPIDEMIOLOGY GENERAL EPIDEMIOLOGY General epidemiology is the scientific basis of public health Descriptive epidemiology: distribution of disease in populations Incidence and prevalence rates

More information

University of Groningen. Children of bipolar parents Wals, Marjolein

University of Groningen. Children of bipolar parents Wals, Marjolein University of Groningen Children of bipolar parents Wals, Marjolein IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

An Introduction to Quantitative Genetics I. Heather A Lawson Advanced Genetics Spring2018

An Introduction to Quantitative Genetics I. Heather A Lawson Advanced Genetics Spring2018 An Introduction to Quantitative Genetics I Heather A Lawson Advanced Genetics Spring2018 Outline What is Quantitative Genetics? Genotypic Values and Genetic Effects Heritability Linkage Disequilibrium

More information

Familial Mental Retardation

Familial Mental Retardation Behavior Genetics, Vol. 14, No. 3, 1984 Familial Mental Retardation Paul L. Nichols ~ Received 18 Aug. 1983--Final 2 Feb. 1984 Familial patterns of mental retardation were examined among white and black

More information

The Influence of Religion on Alcohol Use Initiation: Evidence for Genotype X Environment Interaction

The Influence of Religion on Alcohol Use Initiation: Evidence for Genotype X Environment Interaction Behavior Genetics, Vol. 29, No. 6, 1999 The Influence of Religion on Alcohol Use Initiation: Evidence for Genotype X Environment Interaction Judith R. Koopmans, 1,3 Wendy S. Slutske, 2 G. Caroline M. van

More information

BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND SUBTYPES OF ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER WITH COMORBIDITIES

BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND SUBTYPES OF ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER WITH COMORBIDITIES BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND SUBTYPES OF ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER WITH COMORBIDITIES Ruu-Fen Tzang 1,2 and Yue-Cune Chang 3 1 Department of Psychiatry, Mackay Memorial Hospital, 2 Mackay Medicine,

More information

Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: a quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs

Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: a quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 53:1 (2012), pp 73 80 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02467.x Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: a quantitative

More information

Psych 3102 Introduction to Behavior Genetics

Psych 3102 Introduction to Behavior Genetics Psych 3102 Introduction to Behavior Genetics Lecture 12 Quantitative analysis Covariance between relatives Sources of covariance between relatives covariance a measure of shared variance (how similar the

More information

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability Eric Turkheimer and Erin E. Horn In 3, our lab published a paper demonstrating that the heritability

More information

Attention-deficit hyperactive disorder in school-aged children in Saudi Arabia

Attention-deficit hyperactive disorder in school-aged children in Saudi Arabia DOI 10.1007/s00431-010-1190-y ORIGINAL PAPER Attention-deficit hyperactive disorder in school-aged children in Saudi Arabia Mohammed M. J. Alqahtani Received: 31 December 2009 / Accepted: 9 March 2010

More information

Lummaa et al. (2007) presented historical data collected

Lummaa et al. (2007) presented historical data collected ARTICLES Males Do Not Reduce the Fitness of Their Female Co-Twins in Contemporary Samples Sarah E. Medland, 1,2* John C. Loehlin, 3 Gonneke Willemsen, 4 Peter K. Hatemi, 1,2 Mathew C. Keller, 5 Dorret

More information

The comparison of behavioral and emotional problems in children with a bipolar parent and children with healthy parents in Zahedan, Iran, 2011

The comparison of behavioral and emotional problems in children with a bipolar parent and children with healthy parents in Zahedan, Iran, 2011 The comparison of behavioral and emotional problems in children with a bipolar parent and children with healthy parents in Zahedan, Iran, 2011 Mahboubeh Firoozkouhi Moghaddam, Nour Mohammad Bakhshani,

More information

Variation (individual differences): Stature (in cm) in Dutch adolescent twins

Variation (individual differences): Stature (in cm) in Dutch adolescent twins Variation (individual differences): Stature (in cm) in Dutch adolescent twins 400 Men 700 Women 600 300 500 400 200 300 100 Std. Dev = 8.76 Mean = 179.1 200 100 Std. Dev = 6.40 Mean = 169.1 0 N = 1465.00

More information

Polygenic risk scores for smoking: predictors for alcohol and cannabis use?

Polygenic risk scores for smoking: predictors for alcohol and cannabis use? bs_bs_banner RESEARCH REPORT doi:10.1111/add.12491 Polygenic risk scores for smoking: predictors for alcohol and cannabis use? Jacqueline M. Vink 1,2, Jouke Jan Hottenga 1, Eco J. C. de Geus 1, Gonneke

More information

HEREDITARY INFLUENCES ON DEVELOPMENT

HEREDITARY INFLUENCES ON DEVELOPMENT HEREDITARY INFLUENCES ON DEVELOPMENT HEREDITARY INFLUENCES ON DEVELOPMENT Genotype: genes that one inherits Phenotype: how one s genotype is expressed in observable or measurable characteristics PRINCIPLES

More information

Mendelian & Complex Traits. Quantitative Imaging Genomics. Genetics Terminology 2. Genetics Terminology 1. Human Genome. Genetics Terminology 3

Mendelian & Complex Traits. Quantitative Imaging Genomics. Genetics Terminology 2. Genetics Terminology 1. Human Genome. Genetics Terminology 3 Mendelian & Complex Traits Quantitative Imaging Genomics David C. Glahn, PhD Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center & Department of Psychiatry, Yale University July, 010 Mendelian Trait A trait influenced

More information

Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood

Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood CE ACTIVITY Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood Melissa A. Munn, MPE 1,2 * Michael C. Stallings, PhD 1,2 Soo Hyun Rhee, PhD 1,2 Laura E. Sobik, PhD

More information

A DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVE ON THE ETIOLOGY OF ALCOHOL USE AND COMORBID TRAITS

A DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVE ON THE ETIOLOGY OF ALCOHOL USE AND COMORBID TRAITS A DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVE ON THE ETIOLOGY OF ALCOHOL USE AND COMORBID TRAITS LOT GEELS Reading committee: Prof. dr. C.V. Dolan Prof. dr. E.M. Derks Prof. dr. A.C. Huizink Prof. dr. R.C.M.E. Engels Dr.

More information

Nature and nurture in. Department of Biological Psychology, Neuroscience Campus / EMGO + institute VU University, Amsterdam The Netherlands

Nature and nurture in. Department of Biological Psychology, Neuroscience Campus / EMGO + institute VU University, Amsterdam The Netherlands Nature and nurture in behavioral problems Eco de Geus Department of Biological Psychology, Neuroscience Campus / EMGO + institute VU University, Amsterdam The Netherlands Topics Quantitative Genetics Genetic

More information

PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ

PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ USADA s mission is to protect the rights of clean athletes. Some athletes need to use stimulants to manage ADD/ADHD and the anti-doping community acknowledges and respects

More information

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences University of Lethbridge Research Repository OPUS Faculty Research and Publications http://opus.uleth.ca Lalumière, Martin 2005 Behavioral genetics: The study of differences Lalumière, Martin L. Department

More information

The Inheritance of Complex Traits

The Inheritance of Complex Traits The Inheritance of Complex Traits Differences Among Siblings Is due to both Genetic and Environmental Factors VIDEO: Designer Babies Traits Controlled by Two or More Genes Many phenotypes are influenced

More information

Individuals wishing to seek an evaluation for ADHD

Individuals wishing to seek an evaluation for ADHD 9 WHAT WE KNOW Diagnosis of ADHD in Adults Individuals wishing to seek an evaluation for ADHD should use this information and resource sheet as a set of guidelines for what to expect from the clinician

More information

The Symptom Trajectory of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Korean School-Age Children

The Symptom Trajectory of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Korean School-Age Children ORIGINAL ARTICLE https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2017.11.01.1 Print ISSN 1738-3684 / On-line ISSN 1976-3026 OPEN ACCESS The Symptom Trajectory of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Korean School-Age

More information

Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms in preschool children: discrepancy between parent and teacher evaluations

Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms in preschool children: discrepancy between parent and teacher evaluations Environ Health Prev Med (2009) 14:150 154 DOI 10.1007/s12199-008-0075-4 REGULAR ARTICLE Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms in preschool children: discrepancy between

More information

Liability Threshold Models

Liability Threshold Models Liability Threshold Models Frühling Rijsdijk & Kate Morley Twin Workshop, Boulder Tuesday March 4 th 2008 Aims Introduce model fitting to categorical data Define liability and describe assumptions of the

More information

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives)

Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Genetics of Behavior (Learning Objectives) Recognize that behavior is multi-factorial with genetic components Understand how multi-factorial traits are studied. Explain the terms: incidence, prevalence,

More information