Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping"

Transcription

1 Review Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping Laura Lee Colgin, Edvard I. Moser and May-Britt Moser Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience and Centre for the Biology of Memory, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, 7489 Trondheim, Norway Memory interference is a common cause of forgetting. Interference is a byproduct of the need to balance the formation of well-differentiated representations against the ability to retrieve memories from cues that are not identical to the original experience. How the brain accomplishes this has remained elusive. Here we review how insights can be gained from studies of an apparently unrelated phenomenon in the rodent brain remapping in hippocampal place cells. Remapping refers to the formation of distinct representations in populations of place cells after minor changes in inputs to the hippocampus. Remapping might reflect processes involved generally in decorrelation of overlapping signals. These processes might be crucial for storing large numbers of similar experiences with only minimal interference. Introduction One of the major causes of forgetting is memory interference. Memory interference refers to antagonistic processes whereby new memories impair retrieval of previously learned information (retroactive interference) or earlier memories obstruct future learning (proactive interference). Interference is most likely to occur between memories that are similar. For example, if you went fishing on the fjord with your younger sister on Labour Day and then went fishing there again with your older sister on Constitution Day, interference might cause confusion about which holiday was spent with which sister. The brain might employ a neural mechanism that promotes the formation of dissimilar memory representations to guard against such memory interference (Figure 1). A mechanism of this kind would likely involve the hippocampus, a brain region known to be critical for memory formation. The hippocampus came to the forefront of the neuroscience of memory when Scoville and Milner [1] published the first study on H.M., a patient who had most of his two hippocampi surgically removed for treatment of epilepsy. The procedure caused severe memory deficits described as forgetting of the events of daily life as quickly as they occur. The discovery of a brain area of critical importance for memory formation came about at the same time as psychologists, most prominently Donald O. Hebb [2], formulated the first theories of the neural foundations of behaviour. Hebb s work convinced a generation of young behavioural scientists that the most complex functions of Corresponding authors: Colgin, L.L. (laura.colgin@cbm.ntnu.no); Moser, M.-B. (maybm@cbm.ntnu.no). the brain, including learning and memory, could be understood by studying the activity of neuronal populations. The observations on H.M. pointed to a brain area where such studies could begin. This, together with the development of technology for microelectrode recording, paved the way for the first observation of a reliable behavioural correlate in hippocampal neurons the discovery of place cells [3]. Place cells are neurons that are activated selectively when an animal moves through a particular location in space (the place field ). Based on the observation that a considerable fraction of the cell population in the hippocampus are place cells [4], the hippocampus was proposed to function as a spatial map [5], with many of the same properties of the cognitive map proposed by Tolman some decades earlier [6,7]. Hippocampal cells were shown to represent nonspatial information as well [8 11], suggesting that the hippocampal map also stores experiences associated with places in the environment. Implicit in these suggestions was the idea that place cells are essential carriers of hippocampal memory. Today, place cells remain one of the most remarkable neuronal correlates of behaviour in non-sensory and nonmotor areas of the brain. Place cells have taught much, not only about spatial representation in the brain but also about the general working principles of the hippocampus and cortical circuits in general. The use of place cells as a model system for studying neuronal computation rests on two important developments during the 1990s and 2000s. The first is the development of tools and concepts for population analyses. With the emergence of technology for multisite recording from large neuronal ensembles [12,13], it became possible to test theories of neuronal computation in interacting cell assemblies (e.g. [2,14,15]). It was shown that simultaneously recorded place cells provide highly accurate dynamic representations not only of the animal s current location in the environment [12] but also of environments it had experienced in the past [16 19]. This gradual appreciation of population codes in hippocampal place cells was accompanied by a second and equally important development the recognition that place cells are part of a larger system, consisting of several interrelated networks, each with a distinct function in computation of space and memory [20]. The computational functions of some of the subfields of the hippocampus are beginning to be revealed, and place cells are now known to interact with grid cells and head-direction cells in adjacent parahippocampal regions, including the medial entorhinal cortex [21 23]. With these recent /$ see front matter ß 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi: /j.tins Available online 5 August

2 Figure 1. Schematic illustrating how remapping might help prevent interference between similar memories. Separate memory representations for similar experiences can be formed when patterns of neural activity from the entorhinal cortex (EC) are remapped onto largely nonoverlapping cell ensembles in the hippocampus (subfield CA3 in this example). developments, place cells have become one of the best tools for understanding how representations of the outside world are computed in complex neuronal networks. The discovery of remapping One of the major discoveries to come out of studies of hippocampal place cells was the observation that place cells frequently alter their firing patterns in response to apparently minor changes in sensory or cognitive inputs [24]. Place fields can appear, disappear or move to unpredictable locations. Firing rates can also drastically change. Muller and colleagues proposed that such changes in firing activity constitute a remapping of the place cell representation of space. The discovery of remapping emphasised the multi-representational nature of the hippocampus, a property that is ideal for a structure involved in highcapacity memory storage. Moreover, remapping revealed that distinct representations could be produced for similar environments and thus provided researchers with a promising experimental model for studying separation of memories in the brain. In the first quantitative study of remapping, Muller and Kubie [25] set out to test how place cell firing properties would be affected by well-defined changes in the environment. They used a set of simple environments that were variations of the same basic apparatus placed in the same location. For each manipulation, only one aspect of the environment was actively changed while the other features were kept constant. Changing the shape of the testing enclosure from circular to rectangular resulted in a pattern of remapping in which cells either had unrelated fields in the two shapes or had fields in one shape but not the other. When the size of the environment was doubled, approximately half of the cells remapped. In the remaining cells, place fields scaled in size but stayed in the same relative position. Remapping was also observed in a subset of cells when animals were tested in the same environment in light versus darkness [26]. Subsequent studies showed that, in some animals, all simultaneously recorded place cells remapped in response to replacement of a white intramaze cue card with a black one [27] (Figure 2). In another study, remapping was obtained across the entire population of control animals when a familiar testing cylinder was replaced with a novel cylinder of a different colour [28]. At the time of these studies, it was quite remarkable that changes in location-specific firing would occur in response to relatively minor environmental manipulations. However, time has validated these early findings, and there are now numerous reports on remapping in the literature. The emergence of remapping appears to depend upon several factors including extent of differences between environments [29] and animals prior training experience [26,30]. Also, in some cases, remapping can need time to develop. Lever and colleagues [31] reported that repeated exposures to different shapes of enclosures were associated with an increase in remapping across training days, and the probability of remapping also increased with repeated presentations of an altered environment in an earlier study [27]. The studies described above involve remapping related to sensory changes, but motivational state and behavioural context are also important factors for inducing remapping in hippocampal place cells. In one of the first studies supporting this point [32], approximately one-third of cells Figure 2. Colour-coded firing rate maps (yellow = 0 Hz, purple = maximum rate) for hippocampal place cells recorded in a cylinder containing either a white or a black intra-maze cue card. (a) An example place cell recorded across four sessions in a cylinder with either the white or black cue card (as indicated above). Note the difference in place field locations for white and black sessions. (b) Three additional cells recorded in the cylinder for white and black cue card sessions. In these examples, place fields changed location or disappeared in response to the cue card substitution. Modified, with permission, from Bostock et al. [27]. 470

3 exhibited different firing fields when two different tasks, random foraging and moving systematically between goal locations, were performed in the same apparatus located in the same place. When both the task and the apparatus were changed, nearly all of the cells exhibited different place fields. In another study, Moita et al. [33] reported that place cells remapped between two training environments, one which was neutral and another in which rats underwent fear conditioning. In studies in which rats were trained to continuously alternate between left and right turns on T or W mazes [34,35], changes in the firing patterns of cells with fields on the central arm were observed. Some cells fired almost exclusively on either left or right turn trials, and some cells displayed the same fields but changed their firing rates on the two types of trials. Consistent with this, Ferbinteanu and Shapiro [36] found that place cell firing rates in a plus maze increased or decreased according to where the rat started or where it was headed. Remapping that occurs in response to changes in behavioural context might allow the animal to create independent representations of different consequences associated with similar stimuli. In a broader perspective, modulation of place cell activity by behavioural context is in agreement with the idea that place cells are involved in episodic memory operations. Different types of remapping Rate remapping and global remapping The diversity of remapping results seen across multiple studies raises the questions of how and why remapping occurs and what different patterns of remapping might represent in terms of memory encoding. Recently, it was reported that two main categories of remapping signify different types of environmental changes [37]. Training and testing animals in the same location using either different-coloured or different-shaped enclosures resulted in rate remapping, defined as substantial changes in firing rates accompanied by little to no shifts in place field locations (Figure 3, top). When the same animals were tested in identical enclosures placed in different locations, global remapping was observed [37]. Global remapping refers to arbitrary changes in both firing rates and firing fields (Figure 3, bottom) and is similar to the complete remapping described by Muller et al. [24]. Under some circumstances, global remapping can also be induced by salient changes in cue configurations without changing the location of the recording apparatus [27 29,38]. The likelihood of observing global remapping increases if the differences between the environments are more substantial or were exaggerated during the training history. In their 1987 study, Muller and Kubie observed global remapping between rectangular and circular environments in an identical location after animals had been pre-trained in another room where the circular and rectangular enclosures were placed side by side (i.e. in different locations; R.U. Muller and J.L. Kubie, pers. commun.). Wills and colleagues [30] found that global remapping could be more readily obtained within the same place if animals were initially trained in enclosures that differed in shape and colour and were built of different materials. In a later study, global remapping was induced in trials in the same location by changing the shape, colour and construction materials of the recording enclosure and in addition varying the floor texture and type of food reward [29]. Rate remapping and global remapping have been proposed to represent distinct hippocampal encoding systems [37]. In rate remapping, the population of active cells and the location of those cells place fields remain unchanged, suggesting that rate remapping preserves the population code for space and represents nonspatial stimuli through changes in firing rate. Firing rate variations might be a general way for the hippocampus to represent nonspatial aspects of an experience on top of a stable place code. Rate variations can express properties of experiences that change from one trial to the next [10,11] or between particular segments of the trial [34,35]. Rate changes can even reflect slight differences in sensory or motivational inputs on a second-to-second basis within the same trial [39,40]. The degree of rate changes can vary from one condition to the next, possibly as a function of the extent of differences between conditions. Global remapping differs from rate remapping in that it is an all-or-none phenomenon. Although changes in the animal s location virtually always elicit global remapping [37], this coding strategy can also be used to differentiate conditions at a single location [27 29,38]. Whether rate or global remapping takes place might be determined by the degree of difference between experimental conditions, with populations of cells being more likely to express global remapping when environmental changes are relatively substantial. Transitions between representations Most studies of remapping investigated the effects of discrete changes to the environment. Such discrete changes can under some circumstances give rise to slow transformations of hippocampal representations [31]. However, in the natural world, stimuli often change in a continuous manner. The hippocampus must then create dynamically changing representations of continually developing episodes, but how it accomplishes this was not apparent from results of standard remapping studies. Two recent studies [19,30] addressed the subject of how transitions between hippocampal representations occur in response to gradually accumulating changes in the environment. In these experiments, a flexible box that could be configured into many shapes was used as the recording apparatus. Animals were first familiarised with the square and circle versions of the enclosure for several days. Then, on the final test day, the enclosure was morphed through a series of different shapes that were intermediate between square and circle. In the study by Leutgeb and colleagues, rate remapping was observed between the original representations for square and circle enclosures. When the novel intermediate shapes were presented, firing rates increased or decreased gradually as the shapes progressively changed. In the study by Wills and colleagues, a different training protocol was employed to obtain global remapping between initial square and circle representations. Under these circumstances, representations for novel intermediate shapes switched abruptly between the two previously learned representations, falling into one or the other category depending on their degree of similarity to square 471

4 Figure 3. Colour-coded rate maps showing rate remapping and global remapping in six pairs of CA3 place cells (dark blue = 0 Hz; red = maximum firing rate, as shown on the far left and right of each row). Rats were tested in boxes with a different colour configuration in a constant location (rate remapping) or in identical boxes in different locations (A and B) (global remapping). In each panel, the left column shows rate maps for the condition where the cell had the highest peak rate (black or white in the rate remapping condition; room A or room B in the global remapping condition). Peak rates are indicated to the left. The middle column shows rate maps for the same cells in the condition with a lower peak rate. The scale is the same as for the left column. The right column contains the same data as the middle, but the colour maps are now scaled to their own maximum values (indicated to the right of each map). Note that firing locations remained constant in the rate remapping condition, whereas the intensities of firing differed strongly. In the global remapping conditions, both firing locations and firing rates were changed. Modified from Leutgeb et al. [37]. or circle. These results show that small transformations in the environment lead to gradual changes in representations when rate remapping is used to differentiate between stimuli. In global remapping, by contrast, representations are mutually exclusive, and transitions between representations occur abruptly at some point as differences in the environment increase. Results from the two morph studies have important implications for theoretical models of associative memory storage. Some of these models predict that memories are represented as stable activity states in the network known as attractors [15]. When these activity states are discrete and well separated, presentation of stimuli that are similar to previously learned inputs will induce retrieval of the attractor state that is most closely associated with that stimulus pattern [41 43]. Support for discrete attractor networks in hippocampus was provided by the results of Wills et al. [30] in which representations for square and circle shapes were believed to reflect two distinct attractors. By contrast, the results observed in the Leutgeb et al. study [19] are more consistent with an attractor landscape made up of many local attractors along a continuum [44,45]. In this type of scheme, new information can be differentiated via the formation of another local basin of attraction. The threshold for transitioning between neighbouring attractors is very low, such that small external perturbations can easily push the system out of one attractor state and into another [46]. This property of continuous attractors implies that new representations can form in response to minor changes in the environment and be readily associated with previously learned representations having nearby attractor basins. This might facilitate encoding of continuous yet distinguishable sequences or events. Partial remapping Typically, all cells in the place cell population coherently exhibit the same type of remapping in response to environmental manipulations [19,25,28,29]. However, discordance across representations within the population is sometimes observed (e.g. [26,47 50]), and this is known as partial remapping [24]. In partial remapping, different reference frames can be represented by different subsets of cells within the population. In a study in which rats were tested in two identical boxes connected by a corridor, the locations of firing fields in the two boxes were similar for some cells and different for others, indicating that different subsets of cells coded for room and box reference frames [51]. Additionally, in a study employing a rotating arena to dissociate local and global reference frames, different subsets of place cells represented the arena and the stationary background [52]. In a later study, Paz-Villagran and colleagues [53] trained animals in an enclosure consisting of a hexagonal box and a circular box connected by a runway. When the hexagon was replaced with a square, a large majority of cells with fields in that part of the apparatus remapped, whereas only a small percentage of cells with fields in the circle or on the runway remapped, suggesting that different portions of the environment were represented by separate maps. Taken together, the above results suggest that partial remapping might reflect the formation of multiple parallel maps for the various reference frames within a given environment, but these studies did not address the question of whether representations of different reference frames are activated simultaneously. Another study, however, found that different subsets of cells that represented different reference frames were not activated simultaneously [54]. Specifically, during trials in which a variable goal location overlapped with the place 472

5 field of a particular cell, the place cell did not fire, whereas cells that were tied to the goal location reference frame did fire. This result suggests that partial remapping might constitute an unstable state in which the hippocampus constantly fluctuates between maps for different reference frames. Such an unstable state might ultimately transition into a stable configuration. Indeed, in experiments in which local and distal reference frames were rotated in opposite directions, partial remapping patterns in the population evolved over time [47]. Specifically, an increase in novel representations that were not tied to rearrangements of local or distal reference frames was observed across the place cell population after many repetitions of trials. Thus, partial remapping might eventually converge over time toward a coherent population response representing the new relationship between stimuli in the environment. Origins of remapping CA3 To encode an episodic memory, the brain needs to integrate the spatial and nonspatial elements that comprise an individual episode. CA3 has been theorised to carry out this function by acting as an auto-associative memory network [14,41,42,55]. Due to its extensively interconnected recurrent collateral system [56], CA3 is believed to essentially function as a single unit that can associate arbitrary inputs. Sets of concurrent activity, conveying information about where an episode occurred and what occurred there, are associated and stored as a coherent event. The massive interconnectivity in CA3 also allows activation of a portion of the original elements in a memory representation to retrieve the memory representation in its entirety, a process known as pattern completion [41,43,57]. It is easy to imagine that the pattern completion process in CA3 could give rise to memory interference effects if representations for slightly different environments were too similar. Perhaps to counteract these effects, remapping is particularly pronounced in CA3 [37,58]. When identical environments are placed in different locations, resulting in global remapping, completely independent population codes develop in CA3. When different-shaped or -coloured environments are placed in the same location, exceptionally high rate remapping can be seen in CA3, with cells often changing their peak firing rate by an order of magnitude [37]. This strong differentiation of representations might be imposed on CA3 by inputs from dentate gyrus and layer II of medial entorhinal cortex (see below). Dentate gyrus Theoretical models have for several reasons predicted a crucial role for dentate gyrus (DG) in pattern separation, a process that reduces overlap and enhances dissimilarity between representations [42,43]. To reliably produce novel distinct memory representations in response to small changes in input in a model CA3 network, a nonnormally distributed pattern of very strong extrinsic input to CA3 is needed [42]. The unique mossy fiber projection from the DG to CA3 fulfills these requirements. A small number [59] of highly potent [60] synapses combined with sparse firing in the DG [61] create a strong and specific input to CA3 that might be sufficient to orthogonalise incoming patterns of activity. Recent empirical results support this theory. Small differences between environments placed in the same location were associated with rate changes within the different place fields of an individual DG granule cell that were so substantial that rate remapping in the DG was even more pronounced than in CA3 [62]. Another recent study showed that rate remapping between square and circle representations was absent in CA3 in mutant mice lacking the critical NMDA subunit NR1 in the DG [63]. Additionally, a recent human imaging study showed high activation in CA3 and DG during presentation of slightly modified versions of previously acquired stimuli [64]. These results are in line with the theory that DG input is crucial for CA3 pattern separation and further suggest that rate remapping in the hippocampus might originate in the DG. However, it is possible that the origins of rate remapping can be traced back even further, perhaps to lateral entorhinal cortex, which is believed to transmit nonspatial information to hippocampus [65 67]. Medial entorhinal cortex If rate remapping depends on the DG, where and how does global remapping arise? A potential answer to this question is provided by recordings from grid cells in medial entorhinal cortex (MEC). Grid cells are neurons with multiple spatial receptive fields that form a regularly spaced, triangular grid pattern that spans the entirety of a given environment [22]. Changes in environments in a common location that give rise to rate remapping in CA3 produce no changes in grid cell representations in layer II, the MEC layer that provides direct excitatory input to CA3. By contrast, grid cells shift their fields coherently relative to the surrounding environment under conditions in which sensory differences in environments placed in the same location are sufficient to induce global remapping in CA3 [29] (Figure 4a). When identical testing enclosures are presented in different rooms, another manipulation associated with global remapping in CA3, grid cells both shift and rotate their fields coherently. Furthermore, global remapping in CA3 is closely aligned in time with grid realignment in MEC (Figure 4b). These results raise the possibility that global remapping in hippocampus might result from grid realignment in MEC. It is not entirely clear how grid cell realignment could lead to global remapping in hippocampus, but two possible explanations were put forth by Fyhn and colleagues [29]. Both assume that a place cell will only fire when its grid cell inputs sufficiently overlap [68]. The first potential explanation further assumes the existence of column-like processing modules in MEC, an idea that is reasonable considering that neighbouring grid cells have similar spacing, orientation and field size [22]. During remapping, the different modules would undergo different degrees of rotation and translation. A given CA3 place cell would receive input from grid cells in several separate MEC modules along the septotemporal axis. The differential realignment across modules in distinct environments would lead to different patterns of overlap between grid cells and thus produce a new place field, or loss of a 473

6 Figure 4. Remapping and grid realignment in MEC. (a) Examples of colour-coded rate maps (left) for MEC grid cells recorded in square and circle enclosures at a constant location illustrate changes in grid cell firing patterns. A coherent grid cell realignment between square and circle is more clearly visualised in the colour-coded crosscorrelation matrices (Pearson product moment correlations) shown on the right for the same cells. (b) Global remapping in CA3 is closely aligned in time with grid realignment in MEC. Rats were trained to run in the same enclosure in light and darkness, and remapping was observed between the two conditions. When the lights were switched on while the rat was still in the box at the end of a dark session, the maps for the light condition instantly appeared at the same time in CA3 and MEC. Spatial correlations between rate maps for 1 min intervals and the average rate map for the initial light session in its entirety are plotted. Colour-coded rate maps for a pair of simultaneously recorded CA3 and MEC cells are shown (inset). Modified from Fyhn et al. [29]. previously existing place field, in CA3 (i.e. global remapping). The second potential explanation assumes a single continuous map of space in MEC. Identical recording enclosures in different spatial locations, or other conditions that lead to global remapping in CA3, would activate different portions of the universal MEC spatial map. Due to different spacings and orientations between grid cells, points of overlap would vary at different locations in the universal spatial map in MEC, and global remapping would occur in CA3. Considering that the entorhinal hippocampal network is a loop [69,70], one might argue that remapping could begin in CA3, for example, and cycle around to entorhinal cortex. However, there is currently no obvious mechanism to explain how the recruitment of different cell populations in the hippocampus would lead to a coherent shift in grid fields in the MEC. Although it has been proposed that place cell feedback to grid cells might be important for anchoring grids to the environment [71], recent studies have shown that grid cell activity persists for tens of minutes following complete inactivation of hippocampus [72,73]. The slower time course of place field formation in hippocampus compared to MEC [22,58] is also incompatible with a role for place cells in establishing new grid coordinates. These observations suggest that grid fields arise independently from hippocampal activity and thus that grid cell realignment is unlikely to be driven exclusively by place cell remapping. Finally, transfer of representations between hippocampus and entorhinal cortex might not be a linearly sequential process but instead might depend on the background state of the network. Hippocampal output of memory representations to cortex has been reported to occur during sharp wave-related states [74], and CA1 might receive input from CA3 and EC on different phases of theta [75]. These points, so far uninvestigated with regard to remapping, might be important for fully understanding the relationship between remapping in hippocampus and MEC. 474

7 CA1 and onward As described above, an increasing body of evidence suggests that the DG CA3 network plays a large role in the pattern separation process, so it is somewhat puzzling that remapping becomes less robust in CA1 [37,58].Itisof interest to understand the implications of this transformation of representations in CA1 because CA3 output must first pass through CA1 before being transmitted to cortex. According to the auto-associative memory theory, each CA3 cell contains a large amount of information that is vulnerable to degradation as it gets transmitted out of the network. To solve this problem, Treves and Rolls [57] proposed that an intermediate recoding stage is required, and CA1 is hypothesised to fulfill this role. There are more neurons in CA1 than in CA3 [76], and each CA3 pyramidal cell projects to a large number of CA1 pyramidal cells [77,78]. This divergence during recoding is thought to reduce the amount of information that is carried by individual neurons, while at the same time not decreasing the overall information content. It was further suggested that CA1 cells integrate information from CA3 and in this way produce a more conjunctive memory representation [79]. This idea is related to the auto-associative properties of the CA3 network. Retrieval of a memory in its entirety can occur following activation of only part of its elements, suggesting that several different simultaneously active CA3 ensembles make up a memory representation. Each element within the memory is thought to be coded by its own ensemble of CA3 cells with different ensembles linked together by auto-association. Individual CA1 cells would respond to concurrent firing across multiple CA3 ensembles, and consequently CA1 cells would represent combinations of elements of each episodic memory as opposed to individual elements. There is some indirect support for this hypothesis. In addition to the divergent connections described above, there are also convergent connections from CA3 to CA1 [80]. The individual synapses are weak and highly variable [80]. Thus, reliable discharge of a CA1 cell probably requires co-activation of multiple synapses from CA3 within a relatively short window of time, ensuring that CA1 responds preferentially to conjunctive inputs from CA3. As would be expected if CA1 representations were indeed more conjunctive, representations for different environments with common features are more correlated in CA1 than in CA3 [37,58]. It is possible that there is even further convergence and less individual coding as information moves downstream from CA1. Remapping in the subiculum is even less distinct than in CA1 [81], and conjunctive representations of spatial position, head direction and running speed are observed in deep layers of MEC that receive output projections from CA1 and subiculum [23]. Regardless of their conjunctive properties, representations in later processing stages are still likely to effectively separate memories of different environments. A recent study showed that different populations of cells are activated in posterior parietal cortex following experiences in distinct spatial and behavioural contexts [82], suggesting that the distinct memory representations that emerge during hippocampal remapping might be carried downstream to cortical targets. Relationship between remapping and behaviour If remapping is a neural mechanism for memory separation, remapped representations should be associated with different memories and therefore detectable changes in animals behaviour in memory tasks. Dislocation of hippocampal maps that follows displacement of environmental cues is associated with altered performance in place navigation tasks [83 85]. Thus far, however, few studies in the literature have addressed the question of whether remapping would be accompanied by retrieval of different memories. An indirect link between remapping and learning was reported by McHugh and colleagues [63]. In this study, CA3 rate remapping between similar environments was impaired in knockout mice with selective NMDA receptor deletions in DG, and these mice were also deficient at discriminating between similar contexts in a fear conditioning task. However, the results of this study are only correlational, meaning that some additional process might have been disrupted that impaired remapping and contextual conditioning via independent mechanisms. Another test of the relationship between remapping and behavioural performance was conducted by Jeffery and colleagues [86]. In this study, rats were trained to forage in a black square box for grains of rice and then to run to a particular corner when a tone sounded to indicate the imminent delivery of a food reward. When animals reached a criterion of eight consecutive correct choices in the task, the walls of the box were changed from black to white. The majority (85%) of stable place cells remapped in response to this change, meaning that they either started or stopped firing in the new box or shifted the location of their field. Performance following remapping remained significantly above chance. Thus, the authors concluded that place cell remapping is not coupled with navigational behaviour. However, even though task performance was significantly higher than expected after remapping, a significant (p < 0.01) decrease in performance did occur in association with remapping; mean performance decreased from 91% correct in the original black box to 70% correct in the white box. Preserved performance could be at least partially due to the use of a non-hippocampal-dependent beacon strategy [87], meaning that the rat could merely identify a room cue located behind the goal corner and move toward it. Such a strategy would be easy for the animals to implement owing to the richness of extra-maze cues and the simplicity of the box shape [88]. Countering this argument, the authors demonstrated that the task was hippocampal dependent because hippocampal-lesioned animals could not learn to perform above chance levels and were significantly impaired when compared to controls. Still, the lesion group was trained in the early stages of the study, a few weeks before the remapping data were obtained, and thus it is possible that the animals switched from a hippocampal-dependent strategy to a non-hippocampal-dependent strategy during this period. It is also possible that parallel representations were formed in other regions during this interval allowing the task to be solved by brain areas unaffected by remapping. Whatever the case may be, the body of work is too limited at this time to determine whether a link between remapping and behaviour does indeed exist. 475

8 Conclusions Much has been learned over the past two decades regarding the conditions that lead to place cell remapping, and these results have bolstered the view that place cells convey much more than just spatial information. Remapping incorporates information about space together with particular events and contexts that arise within that space, a function that is essential for episodic memory processing. Remapping also serves as a pattern separation mechanism that is likely important for reducing interference between related memories. Pattern separation is a general process that occurs throughout the brain. For example, cells in lateral prefrontal cortex respond differentially to different categories of stimuli even when stimuli features are highly similar [89]. Pattern separation occurs in cerebellum, where it is thought to involve divergence of incoming activity onto a large number of granule cells [90]. It is likely that pattern separation principles are implemented in many if not all cortical networks. Understanding hippocampal remapping might thus help us understand computation throughout the brain. References 1 Scoville, W.B. and Milner, B. (1957) Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 20, Hebb, D.O. (1949) The Organization of Behavior. Wiley 3 O Keefe, J. and Dostrovsky, J. (1971) The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 34, O Keefe, J. (1976) Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat. Exp. Neurol. 51, O Keefe, J. and Nadel, L. (1978) The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Clarendon Press 6 Tolman, E.C. (1932) Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men. Century 7 Tolman, E.C. (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 55, Ranck, J.B., Jr (1973) Studies on single neurons in dorsal hippocampal formation and septum in unrestrained rats. I. Behavioral correlates and firing repertoires. Exp. Neurol. 41, Young, B.J. et al. (1994) Correlates of hippocampal complex-spike cell activity in rats performing a nonspatial radial maze task. J. Neurosci. 14, Hampson, R.E. et al. (1993) Hippocampal cell firing correlates of delayed-match-to-sample performance in the rat. Behav. Neurosci. 107, Wood, E.R. et al. (1999) The global record of memory in hippocampal neuronal activity. Nature 397, Wilson, M.A. and McNaughton, B.L. (1993) Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science 261, Harris, K.D. et al. (2003) Organization of cell assemblies in the hippocampus. Nature 424, Marr, D. (1971) Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 262, Hopfield, J.J. (1982) Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 79, Wilson, M.A. and McNaughton, B.L. (1994) Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science 265, Nakazawa, K. et al. (2002) Requirement for hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors in associative memory recall. Science 297, Jarosiewicz, B. and Skaggs, W.E. (2004) Hippocampal place cells are not controlled by visual input during the small irregular activity state in the rat. J. Neurosci. 24, Leutgeb, J.K. et al. (2005) Progressive transformation of hippocampal neuronal representations in morphed environments. Neuron 48, Moser, E.I. et al. (2008) Place cells, grid cells, and the brain s spatial representation system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, Taube, J.S. et al. (1990) Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis. J. Neurosci. 10, Hafting, T. et al. (2005) Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature 436, Sargolini, F. et al. (2006) Conjunctive representation of position, direction, and velocity in entorhinal cortex. Science 312, Muller, R.U. et al. (1991) Spatial firing correlates of neurons in the hippocampal formation of freely moving rats. In Brain and Space (Paillard, J., ed.), pp , Oxford University Press 25 Muller, R.U. and Kubie, J.L. (1987) The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells. J. Neurosci. 7, Quirk, G.J. et al. (1990) The firing of hippocampal place cells in the dark depends on the rat s recent experience. J. Neurosci. 10, Bostock, E. et al. (1991) Experience-dependent modifications of hippocampal place cell firing. Hippocampus 1, Kentros, C. et al. (1998) Abolition of long-term stability of new hippocampal place cell maps by NMDA receptor blockade. Science 280, Fyhn, M. et al. (2007) Hippocampal remapping and grid realignment in entorhinal cortex. Nature 446, Wills, T.J. et al. (2005) Attractor dynamics in the hippocampal representation of the local environment. Science 308, Lever, C. et al. (2002) Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature 416, Markus, E.J. et al. (1995) Interactions between location and task affect the spatial and directional firing of hippocampal neurons. J. Neurosci. 15, Moita, M.A. et al. (2004) Putting fear in its place: remapping of hippocampal place cells during fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 24, Wood, E.R. et al. (2000) Hippocampal neurons encode information about different types of memory episodes occurring in the same location. Neuron 27, Frank, L.M. et al. (2000) Trajectory encoding in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex. Neuron 27, Ferbinteanu, J. and Shapiro, M.L. (2003) Prospective and retrospective memory coding in the hippocampus. Neuron 40, Leutgeb, S. et al. (2005) Independent codes for spatial and episodic memory in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Science 309, Hayman, R.M. et al. (2003) Context-specific acquisition of location discrimination by hippocampal place cells. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, Fenton, A.A. and Muller, R.U. (1998) Place cell discharge is extremely variable during individual passes of the rat through the firing field. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, Olypher, A.V. et al. (2002) Properties of the extra-positional signal in hippocampal place cell discharge derived from the overdispersion in location-specific firing. Neuroscience 111, McNaughton, B.L. and Morris, R.G.M. (1987) Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information storage within a distributed memory system. Trends Neurosci. 10, Treves, A. and Rolls, E.T. (1992) Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input systems to the hippocampal CA3 network. Hippocampus 2, McClelland, J.L. and Goddard, N.H. (1996) Considerations arising from a complementary learning systems perspective on hippocampus and neocortex. Hippocampus 6, Tsodyks, M. and Sejnowski, T. (1995) Associative memory and hippocampal place cells. Int. J. Neural Syst. 6, Tsodyks, M. (2005) Attractor neural networks and spatial maps in hippocampus. Neuron 48, Samsonovich, A. and McNaughton, B.L. (1997) Path integration and cognitive mapping in a continuous attractor neural network model. J. Neurosci. 17, Shapiro, M.L. et al. (1997) Cues that hippocampal place cells encode: dynamic and hierarchical representation of local and distal stimuli. Hippocampus 7, Tanila, H. et al. (1997) Discordance of spatial representation in ensembles of hippocampal place cells. Hippocampus 7,

9 49 Knierim, J.J. (2002) Dynamic interactions between local surface cues, distal landmarks, and intrinsic circuitry in hippocampal place cells. J. Neurosci. 22, Anderson, M.I. and Jeffery, K.J. (2003) Heterogeneous modulation of place cell firing by changes in context. J. Neurosci. 23, Skaggs, W.E. and McNaughton, B.L. (1998) Spatial firing properties of hippocampal CA1 populations in an environment containing two visually identical regions. J. Neurosci. 18, Zinyuk, L. et al. (2000) Understanding hippocampal activity by using purposeful behavior: place navigation induces place cell discharge in both task-relevant and task-irrelevant spatial reference frames. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97, Paz-Villagran, V. et al. (2004) Independent coding of connected environments by place cells. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, Gothard, K.M. et al. (1996) Binding of hippocampal CA1 neural activity to multiple reference frames in a landmark-based navigation task. J. Neurosci. 16, Rolls, E.T. (1987) Information representation, processing and storage in the brain: analysis at the single neuron level. In The Neural and Molecular Bases of Learning (Changeux, J.-P. and Konishi, M., eds), pp , Wiley 56 Amaral, D.G. and Witter, M.P. (1989) The three-dimensional organization of the hippocampal formation: a review of anatomical data. Neuroscience 31, Treves, A. and Rolls, E.T. (1994) Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory. Hippocampus 4, Leutgeb, S. et al. (2004) Distinct ensemble codes in hippocampal areas CA3 and CA1. Science 305, Amaral, D.G. et al. (1990) Neurons, numbers and the hippocampal network. Prog. Brain Res. 83, Henze, D.A. et al. (1997) Large amplitude miniature excitatory postsynaptic currents in hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons are of mossy fiber origin. J. Neurophysiol. 77, Jung, M.W. and McNaughton, B.L. (1993) Spatial selectivity of unit activity in the hippocampal granular layer. Hippocampus 3, Leutgeb, J.K. et al. (2007) Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science 315, McHugh, T.J. et al. (2007) Dentate gyrus NMDA receptors mediate rapid pattern separation in the hippocampal network. Science 317, Bakker, A. et al. (2008) Pattern separation in the human hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus. Science 319, Naber, P.A. et al. (1997) Parallel input to the hippocampal memory system through peri- and postrhinal cortices. Neuroreport 8, Hargreaves, E.L. et al. (2005) Major dissociation between medial and lateral entorhinal input to dorsal hippocampus. Science 308, Leutgeb, J.K. et al. (2008) Hippocampal Rate Coding Depends on Input from the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 68 Solstad, T. et al. (2006) From grid cells to place cells: a mathematical model. Hippocampus 16, Iijima, T. et al. (1996) Entorhinal-hippocampal interactions revealed by real-time imaging. Science 272, Kloosterman, F. et al. (2003) Electrophysiological characterization of interlaminar entorhinal connections: an essential link for re-entrance in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, O Keefe, J. and Burgess, N. (2005) Dual phase and rate coding in hippocampal place cells: theoretical significance and relationship to entorhinal grid cells. Hippocampus 15, Bonnevie, T. et al. (2006) Misalignment of Entorhinal Grid Fields after Hippocampal Inactivation. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 73 Hafting, T. et al. (2008) Hippocampus-independent phase precession in entorhinal grid cells. Nature 453, Chrobak, J.J. and Buzsaki, G. (1996) High-frequency oscillations in the output networks of the hippocampal-entorhinal axis of the freely behaving rat. J. Neurosci. 16, Hasselmo, M.E. et al. (2002) A proposed function for hippocampal theta rhythm: separate phases of encoding and retrieval enhance reversal of prior learning. Neural Comput. 14, Boss, B.D. et al. (1987) On the numbers of neurons in fields CA1 and CA3 of the hippocampus of Sprague-Dawley and Wistar rats. Brain Res. 406, Ishizuka, N. et al. (1990) Organization of intrahippocampal projections originating from CA3 pyramidal cells in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 295, Li, X.G. et al. (1994) The hippocampal CA3 network: an in vivo intracellular labeling study. J. Comp. Neurol. 339, Rolls, E.T. (1996) A theory of hippocampal function in memory. Hippocampus 6, Sayer, R.J. et al. (1990) The time course and amplitude of EPSPs evoked at synapses between pairs of CA3/CA1 neurons in the hippocampal slice. J. Neurosci. 10, Sharp, P.E. (1997) Subicular cells generate similar spatial firing patterns in two geometrically and visually distinctive environments: comparison with hippocampal place cells. Behav. Brain Res. 85, Burke, S.N. et al. (2005) Differential encoding of behavior and spatial context in deep and superficial layers of the neocortex. Neuron 45, Lenck-Santini, P.P. et al. (2002) Relationships between place cell firing fields and navigational decisions by rats. J. Neurosci. 22, Lenck-Santini, P.P. et al. (2001) Evidence for a relationship between place-cell spatial firing and spatial memory performance. Hippocampus 11, Kubie, J.L. et al. (2007) Changes in goal selection induced by cue conflicts are in register with predictions from changes in place cell field locations. Behav. Neurosci. 121, Jeffery, K.J. et al. (2003) Preserved performance in a hippocampaldependent spatial task despite complete place cell remapping. Hippocampus 13, Pearce, J.M. et al. (1998) Hippocampal lesions disrupt navigation based on cognitive maps but not heading vectors. Nature 396, Poucet, B. et al. (2004) Spatial navigation and hippocampal place cell firing: the problem of goal encoding. Rev. Neurosci. 15, Freedman, D.J. et al. (2001) Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science 291, Marr, D. (1969) A theory of cerebellar cortex. J. Physiol. 202,

Distinct Ensemble Codes in Hippocampal Areas CA3 and CA1

Distinct Ensemble Codes in Hippocampal Areas CA3 and CA1 Distinct Ensemble Codes in Hippocampal Areas CA3 and CA1 Stefan Leutgeb, 1 Jill K. Leutgeb, 1 Alessandro Treves, 1,2 May-Britt Moser, 1 Edvard I. Moser 1* 1 Centre for the Biology of Memory, MTFS, Norwegian

More information

Navigation: Inside the Hippocampus

Navigation: Inside the Hippocampus 9.912 Computational Visual Cognition Navigation: Inside the Hippocampus Jakob Voigts 3 Nov 2008 Functions of the Hippocampus: Memory and Space Short term memory Orientation Memory consolidation(h.m)

More information

Space and brain: a tabula rasa for the sense of direction. Paul A. Dudchenko

Space and brain: a tabula rasa for the sense of direction. Paul A. Dudchenko Space and brain: a tabula rasa for the sense of direction Paul A. Dudchenko University of Stirling Psychology, School of Natural Sciences Stirling, FK9 4LA United Kingdom Phone: +44 131 650 3531 p.a.dudchenko@stir.ac.uk

More information

EDVARD MOSER AND THE GRID CELLS MICROSTRUCTURE OF A SPATIAL MAP IN THE ENTORHINAL CORTEX

EDVARD MOSER AND THE GRID CELLS MICROSTRUCTURE OF A SPATIAL MAP IN THE ENTORHINAL CORTEX FAMOUS RESEARCHERS EDVARD MOSER AND THE GRID CELLS MICROSTRUCTURE OF A SPATIAL MAP IN THE ENTORHINAL CORTEX Y. TSENKINA EDVARD I. MOSER is a professor of neuroscience and a director of the Norwegian Centre

More information

Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior. James L. McClelland Stanford University

Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior. James L. McClelland Stanford University Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior James L. McClelland Stanford University A Playwright s Take on Memory What interests me a great deal is the mistiness of the past Harold Pinter,

More information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Table 1. Quantitative features of EC neuron dendrites

Supplementary Information Supplementary Table 1. Quantitative features of EC neuron dendrites Supplementary Information Supplementary Table 1. Quantitative features of EC neuron dendrites Supplementary Table 2. Quantitative features of EC neuron axons 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Layer distribution

More information

ASSOCIATIVE MEMORY AND HIPPOCAMPAL PLACE CELLS

ASSOCIATIVE MEMORY AND HIPPOCAMPAL PLACE CELLS International Journal of Neural Systems, Vol. 6 (Supp. 1995) 81-86 Proceedings of the Neural Networks: From Biology to High Energy Physics @ World Scientific Publishing Company ASSOCIATIVE MEMORY AND HIPPOCAMPAL

More information

Molecular and Circuit Mechanisms for Hippocampal Learning

Molecular and Circuit Mechanisms for Hippocampal Learning Molecular and Circuit Mechanisms for Hippocampal Learning Susumu Tonegawa 1 and Thomas J. McHugh 1 The hippocampus is crucial for the formation of memories of facts and episodes (Scoville and Milner 1957;

More information

Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior

Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior Modeling of Hippocampal Behavior Diana Ponce-Morado, Venmathi Gunasekaran and Varsha Vijayan Abstract The hippocampus is identified as an important structure in the cerebral cortex of mammals for forming

More information

Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex

Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex Thomas Klausberger Dept. Cognitive Neurobiology, Center for Brain Research, Med. Uni. Vienna The hippocampus is a key brain circuit for certain forms of memory

More information

SPATIAL PROCESSING IN THE BRAIN: The Activity of Hippocampal Place Cells

SPATIAL PROCESSING IN THE BRAIN: The Activity of Hippocampal Place Cells Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001. 24:459 86 Copyright c 2001 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved SPATIAL PROCESSING IN THE BRAIN: The Activity of Hippocampal Place Cells Phillip J. Best, 1 Aaron M. White,

More information

Place Cells, Grid Cells, and the Brain s Spatial Representation System

Place Cells, Grid Cells, and the Brain s Spatial Representation System ANNUAL REVIEWS Further Click here for quick links to Annual Reviews content online, including: Other articles in this volume Top cited articles Top downloaded articles Our comprehensive search Annu. Rev.

More information

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT

Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition. Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT Hippocampal mechanisms of memory and cognition Matthew Wilson Departments of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Biology MIT 1 Courtesy of Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.

More information

Encoding Spatial Context: A Hypothesis on the Function of the Dentate Gyrus-Hilus System

Encoding Spatial Context: A Hypothesis on the Function of the Dentate Gyrus-Hilus System Encoding Spatial Context: A Hypothesis on the Function of the Dentate Gyrus-Hilus System A. Minai, ECECS Department, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH J. Best, Department of Psychology, Miami University,

More information

A model of the interaction between mood and memory

A model of the interaction between mood and memory INSTITUTE OF PHYSICS PUBLISHING NETWORK: COMPUTATION IN NEURAL SYSTEMS Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 12 (2001) 89 109 www.iop.org/journals/ne PII: S0954-898X(01)22487-7 A model of the interaction between

More information

Synaptic plasticity and hippocampal memory

Synaptic plasticity and hippocampal memory Synaptic plasticity and hippocampal memory Tobias Bast School of Psychology, University of Nottingham tobias.bast@nottingham.ac.uk Synaptic plasticity as the neurophysiological substrate of learning Hebb

More information

Chapter 6: Hippocampal Function In Cognition. From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Chapter 6: Hippocampal Function In Cognition. From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Chapter 6: Hippocampal Function In Cognition From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Grid Cell The Hippocampus Serves a Role in Multimodal Information Processing and Memory

More information

Toward a spiking model of the Hippocampus and Entorhinal Cortex

Toward a spiking model of the Hippocampus and Entorhinal Cortex Toward a spiking model of the Hippocampus and Entorhinal Cortex Emily Mankin Abstract A framework for a spiking model of Entorhinal Cortex, and the Dentate Gyrus and CA3 subfields of the hippocampus is

More information

Place cells, spatial maps and the population code for memory Stefan Leutgeb, Jill K Leutgeb, May-Britt Moser and Edvard I Moser

Place cells, spatial maps and the population code for memory Stefan Leutgeb, Jill K Leutgeb, May-Britt Moser and Edvard I Moser Place cells, spatial maps and the population code for memory Stefan Leutgeb, Jill K Leutgeb, May-Britt Moser and Edvard I Moser The study of population dynamics in hippocampal place cells has emerged as

More information

Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function

Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function Behavioural Brain Research 89 (1997) 1 34 Review article Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function Michael E. Hasselmo

More information

Acetylcholine again! - thought to be involved in learning and memory - thought to be involved dementia (Alzheimer's disease)

Acetylcholine again! - thought to be involved in learning and memory - thought to be involved dementia (Alzheimer's disease) Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function Michael E. Hasselmo * and Bradley P. Wyble Acetylcholine again! - thought to

More information

Place fields and the cognitive map

Place fields and the cognitive map This is the pre-peer reviewed version of the following article: Place fields and the cognitive map Paul A. Dudchenko 1,2 and Emma R. Wood 2 which has been published at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hipo.22450/full

More information

From the GPS to HM: Place Cells, Grid Cells, and Memory

From the GPS to HM: Place Cells, Grid Cells, and Memory HIPPOCAMPUS 25:719 725 (2015) COMMENTARY From the GPS to HM: Place Cells, Grid Cells, and Memory James J. Knierim* ABSTRACT: A longstanding debate in hippocampus research has revolved around how to reconcile

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

Prior Knowledge and Memory Consolidation Expanding Competitive Trace Theory

Prior Knowledge and Memory Consolidation Expanding Competitive Trace Theory Prior Knowledge and Memory Consolidation Expanding Competitive Trace Theory Anna Smith Outline 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Background in Memory Models Models of Consolidation The Hippocampus Competitive Trace Theory

More information

Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making

Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making A Unifying Computational Neuroscience Approach Edmund T. Rolls University of Oxford Department of Experimental Psychology Oxford England OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Contents

More information

Cerebral Cortex. Edmund T. Rolls. Principles of Operation. Presubiculum. Subiculum F S D. Neocortex. PHG & Perirhinal. CA1 Fornix CA3 S D

Cerebral Cortex. Edmund T. Rolls. Principles of Operation. Presubiculum. Subiculum F S D. Neocortex. PHG & Perirhinal. CA1 Fornix CA3 S D Cerebral Cortex Principles of Operation Edmund T. Rolls F S D Neocortex S D PHG & Perirhinal 2 3 5 pp Ento rhinal DG Subiculum Presubiculum mf CA3 CA1 Fornix Appendix 4 Simulation software for neuronal

More information

Beta Oscillations and Hippocampal Place Cell Learning during Exploration of Novel Environments Stephen Grossberg 1

Beta Oscillations and Hippocampal Place Cell Learning during Exploration of Novel Environments Stephen Grossberg 1 Beta Oscillations and Hippocampal Place Cell Learning during Exploration of Novel Environments Stephen Grossberg 1 1 Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems Center for Adaptive Systems, and Center of

More information

Anatomy of the Hippocampus

Anatomy of the Hippocampus Anatomy of the Hippocampus Lecture 3.2 David S. Touretzky September, 2015 Human Hippocampus 2 Human Hippocampus 3 Hippocampus Means Seahorse Dissected human hippocampus next to a specimen of hippocampus

More information

Integration of the Sensory Inputs to Place Cells: What, Where, Why, and How?

Integration of the Sensory Inputs to Place Cells: What, Where, Why, and How? HIPPOCAMPUS 17:775 785 (2007) COMMENTARY Integration of the Sensory Inputs to Place Cells: What, Where, Why, and How? Kathryn J. Jeffery* ABSTRACT: The hippocampal place cells are a highly multimodal class

More information

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields.

Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Theta sequences are essential for internally generated hippocampal firing fields. Yingxue Wang, Sandro Romani, Brian Lustig, Anthony Leonardo, Eva Pastalkova Supplementary Materials Supplementary Modeling

More information

CRISP: Challenging the Standard Framework of Hippocampal Memory Function

CRISP: Challenging the Standard Framework of Hippocampal Memory Function CRISP: Challenging the Standard Framework of Hippocampal Memory Function Laurenz Wiskott Mehdi Bayati Sen Cheng Jan Melchior Torsten Neher supported by: Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft - Sonderforschungsbereich

More information

Limbic system outline

Limbic system outline Limbic system outline 1 Introduction 4 The amygdala and emotion -history - theories of emotion - definition - fear and fear conditioning 2 Review of anatomy 5 The hippocampus - amygdaloid complex - septal

More information

REPRESENTATION OF OBJECT-IN-CONTEXT WITHIN MOUSE HIPPOCAMPAL NEURONAL ACTIVITY. Herborg Nanna Ásgeirsdóttir. A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of

REPRESENTATION OF OBJECT-IN-CONTEXT WITHIN MOUSE HIPPOCAMPAL NEURONAL ACTIVITY. Herborg Nanna Ásgeirsdóttir. A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of REPRESENTATION OF OBJECT-IN-CONTEXT WITHIN MOUSE HIPPOCAMPAL NEURONAL ACTIVITY by Herborg Nanna Ásgeirsdóttir A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of The Charles E. Schmidt College of Science In Partial Fulfillment

More information

BBS-D _ Mather_ Houghton. The Dentate Gyrus and the Hilar Revised

BBS-D _ Mather_ Houghton. The Dentate Gyrus and the Hilar Revised BBS-D-15-00899_ Mather_ Houghton The Dentate Gyrus and the Hilar Revised Conor Houghton Department of Computer Science, University of Bristol, UK Department of Computer Science, Merchant Venturers Building,

More information

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D.

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A Hippocampus Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D. Common (Distributed) Model of Memory Processes Time Course of Memory Processes Long Term Memory DECLARATIVE NON-DECLARATIVE Semantic Episodic Skills

More information

Hippocampal Place Cells, Context, and Episodic Memory

Hippocampal Place Cells, Context, and Episodic Memory Hippocampal Place Cells, Context, and Episodic Memory David M. Smith* and Sheri J.Y. Mizumori HIPPOCAMPUS 16:000 000 (2006) ABSTRACT: Although most observers agree that the hippocampus has a critical role

More information

Hippocampal Place Cells, Context, and Episodic Memory

Hippocampal Place Cells, Context, and Episodic Memory Hippocampal Place Cells, Context, and Episodic Memory David M. Smith* and Sheri J.Y. Mizumori HIPPOCAMPUS 16:716 729 (2006) ABSTRACT: Although most observers agree that the hippocampus has a critical role

More information

Hippocampal and Neocortical Interactions During Context Discrimination: Electrophysiological Evidence From the Rat

Hippocampal and Neocortical Interactions During Context Discrimination: Electrophysiological Evidence From the Rat Hippocampal and Neocortical Interactions During Context Discrimination: Electrophysiological Evidence From the Rat Sheri J.Y. Mizumori, 1 * David M. Smith, 2 and Corey B. Puryear 1 HIPPOCAMPUS 17:851 862

More information

Y a-t il un pilote dans le cerveau? Données récentes Sur les bases neuronales de l orientation spatiale

Y a-t il un pilote dans le cerveau? Données récentes Sur les bases neuronales de l orientation spatiale Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Marseille NeuroSTIC Grenoble, June 23 24, 2016 Y a-t il un pilote dans le cerveau? Données récentes Sur les bases neuronales de l orientation spatiale Why is it important

More information

Mnemonic Contributions of Hippocampal Place Cells

Mnemonic Contributions of Hippocampal Place Cells CHAPTER 5 Mnemonic Contributions of Hippocampal Place Cells Sherri J. Y. Mizumori, D. M. Smith, and C. B. Puryear Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195 I. INTRODUCTION The

More information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 1) The hippocampus is a cortical structure in the medial portion of the temporal lobe (medial temporal lobe in primates. a) What is the main function of the hippocampus? The hippocampus

More information

Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation

Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation So far we have talked about the hippocampus and: -coding of spatial locations in rats -declarative (explicit) memory -experimental evidence

More information

Motor systems III: Cerebellum April 16, 2007 Mu-ming Poo

Motor systems III: Cerebellum April 16, 2007 Mu-ming Poo Motor systems III: Cerebellum April 16, 2007 Mu-ming Poo Population coding in the motor cortex Overview and structure of cerebellum Microcircuitry of cerebellum Function of cerebellum -- vestibulo-ocular

More information

Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma

Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma Paper: Abraham, Wickliffe C., and Anthony Robins. "Memory retention the synaptic stability versus plasticity dilemma." Trends in neurosciences

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1 SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Figure 1 The supralinear events evoked in CA3 pyramidal cells fulfill the criteria for NMDA spikes, exhibiting a threshold, sensitivity to NMDAR blockade, and all-or-none

More information

CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation

CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation Article CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation Joshua P. Neunuebel 1,3,4 and James J. Knierim 1,2,3,

More information

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B Cortical Analysis of Visual Context Moshe Bar, Elissa Aminoff. 2003. Neuron, Volume 38, Issue 2, Pages 347 358. Visual objects in context Moshe Bar.

More information

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories? CASE 49 A 43-year-old woman is brought to her primary care physician by her family because of concerns about her forgetfulness. The patient has a history of Down syndrome but no other medical problems.

More information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 1) Hippocampus is a cortical structure in the medial portion of the temporal lobe (medial temporal lobe in primates. a) What is the main function of the hippocampus? The hippocampus

More information

Self-organizing continuous attractor networks and path integration: one-dimensional models of head direction cells

Self-organizing continuous attractor networks and path integration: one-dimensional models of head direction cells INSTITUTE OF PHYSICS PUBLISHING Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 13 (2002) 217 242 NETWORK: COMPUTATION IN NEURAL SYSTEMS PII: S0954-898X(02)36091-3 Self-organizing continuous attractor networks and path

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 7: Network models Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis II 6 Single neuron

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. A proposed architecture for the neural representation of spatial context

ARTICLE IN PRESS. A proposed architecture for the neural representation of spatial context Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews xx (2004) xxx xxx www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev A proposed architecture for the neural representation of spatial context Kathryn J. Jeffery*, Michael I. Anderson,

More information

A Neural Coding Scheme Formed by the Combined Function of Gamma and Theta Oscillations

A Neural Coding Scheme Formed by the Combined Function of Gamma and Theta Oscillations Schizophrenia Bulletin vol. 34 no. 5 pp. 974 980, 2008 doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn060 Advance Access publication on June 16, 2008 A Neural Coding Scheme Formed by the Combined Function of Gamma and Theta Oscillations

More information

Spatial View Cells and the Representation of Place in the Primate Hippocampus

Spatial View Cells and the Representation of Place in the Primate Hippocampus Spatial View Cells and the Representation of Place in the Primate Hippocampus Edmund T. Rolls* Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, England HIPPOCAMPUS 9:467 480 (1999)

More information

Synfire chains with conductance-based neurons: internal timing and coordination with timed input

Synfire chains with conductance-based neurons: internal timing and coordination with timed input Neurocomputing 5 (5) 9 5 www.elsevier.com/locate/neucom Synfire chains with conductance-based neurons: internal timing and coordination with timed input Friedrich T. Sommer a,, Thomas Wennekers b a Redwood

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

A cognitive map for object memory in the hippocampus

A cognitive map for object memory in the hippocampus Research A cognitive map for object memory in the hippocampus Joseph R. Manns 1,3 and Howard Eichenbaum 2 1 Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322, USA; 2 Center for Memory

More information

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems Compare Chap 31 of Purves et al., 5e Chap 24 of Bear et al., 3e Larry Wittie Computer Science, StonyBrook University http://www.cs.sunysb.edu/~cse511

More information

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003 Name: Student #: BEFORE YOU BEGIN!!! 1) Count the number of pages in your exam. The exam is 8 pages long; if you do not

More information

Cerebellum and spatial cognition: A connectionist approach

Cerebellum and spatial cognition: A connectionist approach Cerebellum and spatial cognition: A connectionist approach Jean-Baptiste Passot 1,2, Laure Rondi-Reig 1,2, and Angelo Arleo 1,2 1- UPMC Univ Paris 6, UMR 7102, F-75005 Paris, France 2- CNRS, UMR 7102,

More information

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Ch 8. Learning and Memory Ch 8. Learning and Memory Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by H.-S. Seok, K. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang Biointelligence

More information

Discordance of Spatial Representation in Ensembles of Hippocampal Place Cells

Discordance of Spatial Representation in Ensembles of Hippocampal Place Cells Discordance of Spatial Representation in Ensembles of Hippocampal Place Cells Heikki Tanila, 1 Matthew L. Shapiro, 2 and Howard Eichenbaum 3 * 1 Department of Neuroscience and Neurology, University of

More information

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25 Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP Reading: BCP Chapter 25 Memory Systems Learning is the acquisition of new knowledge or skills. Memory is the retention of learned information. Many different

More information

Computational approach to the schizophrenia: disconnection syndrome and dynamical pharmacology

Computational approach to the schizophrenia: disconnection syndrome and dynamical pharmacology Computational approach to the schizophrenia: disconnection syndrome and dynamical pharmacology Péter Érdi1,2, Brad Flaugher 1, Trevor Jones 1, Balázs Ujfalussy 2, László Zalányi 2 and Vaibhav Diwadkar

More information

From spike frequency to free recall: How neural circuits perform encoding and retrieval.

From spike frequency to free recall: How neural circuits perform encoding and retrieval. Hasselmo et al. 1 From spike frequency to free recall: How neural circuits perform encoding and retrieval. Michael Hasselmo, Bradley P. Wyble, Robert C. Cannon Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience

More information

The Neurophysiology of Memory

The Neurophysiology of Memory The Neurophysiology of Memory WENDY A. SUZUKI a,c AND HOWARD EICHENBAUM b a Center for Neural Science, New York University, 4 Washington Place, Room 809, New York, New York 10003, USA b Laboratory of Cognitive

More information

11/2/2011. Basic circuit anatomy (the circuit is the same in all parts of the cerebellum)

11/2/2011. Basic circuit anatomy (the circuit is the same in all parts of the cerebellum) 11/2/2011 Neuroscientists have been attracted to the puzzle of the Cerebellum ever since Cajal. The orderly structure, the size of the cerebellum and the regularity of the neural elements demands explanation.

More information

Representational Switching by Dynamical Reorganization of Attractor Structure in a Network Model of the Prefrontal Cortex

Representational Switching by Dynamical Reorganization of Attractor Structure in a Network Model of the Prefrontal Cortex Representational Switching by Dynamical Reorganization of Attractor Structure in a Network Model of the Prefrontal Cortex Yuichi Katori 1,2 *, Kazuhiro Sakamoto 3, Naohiro Saito 4, Jun Tanji 4, Hajime

More information

Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001

Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001 Brain and Cognitive Sciences 9.96 Experimental Methods of Tetrode Array Neurophysiology IAP 2001 An Investigation into the Mechanisms of Memory through Hippocampal Microstimulation In rodents, the hippocampus

More information

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück The Binding Problem This lecture is based on following articles: Adina L. Roskies: The Binding Problem; Neuron 1999 24: 7 Charles M. Gray: The Temporal Correlation Hypothesis of Visual Feature Integration:

More information

Transitions between dierent synchronous ring modes using synaptic depression

Transitions between dierent synchronous ring modes using synaptic depression Neurocomputing 44 46 (2002) 61 67 www.elsevier.com/locate/neucom Transitions between dierent synchronous ring modes using synaptic depression Victoria Booth, Amitabha Bose Department of Mathematical Sciences,

More information

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140 Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140 Second midterm is next Tuesday!! Covers lectures 7-12 (Synaptic transmission, NT & receptors, intracellular signaling & synaptic plasticity). Review session is on Monday (Nov

More information

PERSPECTIVES. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity, spatial memory and anxiety

PERSPECTIVES. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity, spatial memory and anxiety OPINION Hippocampal synaptic plasticity, spatial memory and anxiety David M. Bannerman, Rolf Sprengel, David J. Sanderson, Stephen B. McHugh, J. Nicholas P. Rawlins, Hannah Monyer and Peter H. Seeburg

More information

Reversed and forward buffering of behavioral spike sequences enables retrospective and prospective retrieval in hippocampal regions CA3 and CA1

Reversed and forward buffering of behavioral spike sequences enables retrospective and prospective retrieval in hippocampal regions CA3 and CA1 Neural Networks 21 (2008) 276 288 www.elsevier.com/locate/neunet 2008 Special Issue Reversed and forward buffering of behavioral spike sequences enables retrospective and prospective retrieval in hippocampal

More information

Cell Responses in V4 Sparse Distributed Representation

Cell Responses in V4 Sparse Distributed Representation Part 4B: Real Neurons Functions of Layers Input layer 4 from sensation or other areas 3. Neocortical Dynamics Hidden layers 2 & 3 Output layers 5 & 6 to motor systems or other areas 1 2 Hierarchical Categorical

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Basic properties of compound EPSPs at

Supplementary Figure 1. Basic properties of compound EPSPs at Supplementary Figure 1. Basic properties of compound EPSPs at hippocampal CA3 CA3 cell synapses. (a) EPSPs were evoked by extracellular stimulation of the recurrent collaterals and pharmacologically isolated

More information

Supplementary Figure 1 Information on transgenic mouse models and their recording and optogenetic equipment. (a) 108 (b-c) (d) (e) (f) (g)

Supplementary Figure 1 Information on transgenic mouse models and their recording and optogenetic equipment. (a) 108 (b-c) (d) (e) (f) (g) Supplementary Figure 1 Information on transgenic mouse models and their recording and optogenetic equipment. (a) In four mice, cre-dependent expression of the hyperpolarizing opsin Arch in pyramidal cells

More information

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature,

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature, Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain, Nature, Vol. 435, pp. 1102-7. Sander Vaus 22.04.2015 The study

More information

Suppl. Information Supplementary Figure 1. Strategy/latency analysis of individual mice during maze learning. a,

Suppl. Information Supplementary Figure 1. Strategy/latency analysis of individual mice during maze learning. a, Goal-oriented searching mediated by ventral hippocampus early in trial-and-error learning Ruediger, S, Spirig, D., Donato, F., Caroni, P. Suppl. Information Supplementary Figure 1. Strategy/latency analysis

More information

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Theories of Sensory, short-term & long-term memories Memory & brain Cellular bases

More information

Functional Differences in the Backward Shifts of CA1 and CA3 Place Fields in Novel and Familiar Environments

Functional Differences in the Backward Shifts of CA1 and CA3 Place Fields in Novel and Familiar Environments Functional Differences in the Backward Shifts of CA1 and CA3 Place Fields in Novel and Familiar Environments Eric D. Roth 1,2, Xintian Yu 2, Geeta Rao 2, James J. Knierim 2,3 * 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

UNINFORMATIVE MEMORIES WILL PREVAIL

UNINFORMATIVE MEMORIES WILL PREVAIL virtute UNINFORMATIVE MEMORIES WILL PREVAIL THE STORAGE OF CORRELATED REPRESENTATIONS AND ITS CONSEQUENCES Emilio Kropff SISSA, Trieste di Studi Avanzati Internazionale Superiore - e conoscenza seguir

More information

TNS Journal Club: Interneurons of the Hippocampus, Freund and Buzsaki

TNS Journal Club: Interneurons of the Hippocampus, Freund and Buzsaki TNS Journal Club: Interneurons of the Hippocampus, Freund and Buzsaki Rich Turner (turner@gatsby.ucl.ac.uk) Gatsby Unit, 22/04/2005 Rich T. Introduction Interneuron def = GABAergic non-principal cell Usually

More information

Losing one's way in a digital world: VR studies on navigation

Losing one's way in a digital world: VR studies on navigation Losing one's way in a digital world: VR studies on navigation Albert Postma, Jessie Bullens and Ineke J. M. van der Ham Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, the Netherlands

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

The role of phase synchronization in memory processes

The role of phase synchronization in memory processes The role of phase synchronization in memory processes Juergen Fell and Nikolai Axmacher Abstract In recent years, studies ranging from single-unit recordings in animals to electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography

More information

A Neural Model of Context Dependent Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex

A Neural Model of Context Dependent Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex A Neural Model of Context Dependent Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex Sugandha Sharma (s72sharm@uwaterloo.ca) Brent J. Komer (bjkomer@uwaterloo.ca) Terrence C. Stewart (tcstewar@uwaterloo.ca) Chris

More information

Computational Analysis of the Role of the Hippocampus in Memory

Computational Analysis of the Role of the Hippocampus in Memory HZPPOCAMPUS, VOL. 4, NO. 3, PAGES 374-391, JUNE 1994 Computational Analysis of the Role of the Hippocampus in Memory Alessandro Treves and Edmund T. Rolls Department of Experimental Psychology, University

More information

Dual Phase and Rate Coding in Hippocampal Place Cells: Theoretical Significance and Relationship to Entorhinal Grid Cells

Dual Phase and Rate Coding in Hippocampal Place Cells: Theoretical Significance and Relationship to Entorhinal Grid Cells Dual Phase and Rate Coding in Hippocampal Place Cells: Theoretical Significance and Relationship to Entorhinal Grid Cells John O Keefe 1 * and Neil Burgess 1,2 HIPPOCAMPUS 15:853 866 (2005) ABSTRACT: We

More information

Temporal Encoding of Place Sequences by Hippocampal Cell Assemblies

Temporal Encoding of Place Sequences by Hippocampal Cell Assemblies Neuron 50, 145 157, April 6, 2006 ª2006 Elsevier Inc. DOI 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.023 Temporal Encoding of Place Sequences by Hippocampal Cell Assemblies George Dragoi 1,2, * and György Buzsáki 1, * 1

More information

Synap&c Plas&city. long-term plasticity (~30 min to lifetime) Long-term potentiation (LTP) / Long-term depression (LTD)

Synap&c Plas&city. long-term plasticity (~30 min to lifetime) Long-term potentiation (LTP) / Long-term depression (LTD) Synap&c Plas&city synaptic connectivity constantly changes in response to activity and other factors During development: provides the basic wiring of the brain s circuits Throughout rest of life: basis

More information

Some Thoughts on Place Cells and the Hippocampus

Some Thoughts on Place Cells and the Hippocampus Some Thoughts on Place Cells and the Hippocampus Jeffrey S. Taube* Department of Psychology, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire HIPPOCAMPUS 9:452 457 (1999) ABSTRACT: This commentary addresses five

More information

Research Article Associative Memory Storage and Retrieval: Involvement of Theta Oscillations in Hippocampal Information Processing

Research Article Associative Memory Storage and Retrieval: Involvement of Theta Oscillations in Hippocampal Information Processing Neural Plasticity Volume 2011, Article ID 683961, 15 pages doi:10.1155/2011/683961 Research Article Associative Memory Storage and Retrieval: Involvement of Theta Oscillations in Hippocampal Information

More information

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus Central Visual Pathways V1/2 NEUR 3001 dvanced Visual Neuroscience The Lateral Geniculate Nucleus () is more than a relay station LP SC Professor Tom Salt UCL Institute of Ophthalmology Retina t.salt@ucl.ac.uk

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR In Physiology Today What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may

More information

Comprehensive computational model for memory processing in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex circuitry

Comprehensive computational model for memory processing in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex circuitry Comprehensive computational model for memory processing in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex circuitry A. Project Scope (1) Describe the relevance of the scientific problem within the biological and

More information

Cholinergic suppression of transmission may allow combined associative memory function and self-organization in the neocortex.

Cholinergic suppression of transmission may allow combined associative memory function and self-organization in the neocortex. Cholinergic suppression of transmission may allow combined associative memory function and self-organization in the neocortex. Michael E. Hasselmo and Milos Cekic Department of Psychology and Program in

More information

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch!

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch! Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch! How do we perceive objects held in the hand?! Touch receptors deconstruct objects to detect local features! Information is transmitted in parallel

More information