This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and"

Transcription

1 This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution and sharing with colleagues. Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party websites are prohibited. In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or institutional repository. Authors requiring further information regarding Elsevier s archiving and manuscript policies are encouraged to visit:

2 Behavioural Brain Research 223 (211) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research jo u r n al hom epa ge: Research report Posttrial d-cycloserine enhances the emotional memory of an incentive downshift event Jacob N. Norris 1, Leonardo A. Ortega, Mauricio R. Papini Department of Psychology, Texas Christian University, Fort Worth, TX 76129, USA a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 2 March 211 Received in revised form 28 April 211 Accepted 2 May 211 Available online 7 May 211 Keywords: Incentive downshift d-cycloserine NMDARs Frustration Negative contrast Emotional memory a b s t r a c t The present research was designed to determine whether an incentive downshift event induces an emotional memory that can be modulated by d-cycloserine (DCS), a partial agonist at the glycine site of N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor (NMDAR). DCS has been reported to have memory-enhancing properties in other training situations. Experiments 1 and 2 involved a consummatory successive negative contrast (csnc) situation in which animals are exposed to an incentive downshift involving sucrose solutions of different concentrations. DCS administration (3 mg/kg, ip) immediately after the first 32-to-4% sucrose downshift trial (Experiment 1) retarded recovery of consummatory behavior, but immediately after the first 32-to-6% sucrose downshift trial (Experiment 2) did not affect recovery. There was no evidence that DCS affected consummatory behavior in the absence of an incentive downshift in a manner analogous to a conditioned taste aversion (Experiment 3). These results suggest that activation of NMDARs via the glycine modulatory site enhances the emotional memory triggered by exposure to an incentive downshift event. 211 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Consummatory successive negative contrast (csnc) is the abrupt disruption of consummatory behavior after a downshift from a large to a small incentive, relative to the consummatory behavior of animals exposed only to the small incentive [1]. In a typical csnc experiment, the incentives are 32% and 4% sucrose solutions, and the downshift occurs on Trial 11. The csnc effect indicates that the change in behavior is not an adjustment to the new incentive conditions, but a reduction of consummatory behavior that may be the result of an increase in search behavior [2 4], escape from a frustrating situation [5,6], or release from response inhibition following a surprising devaluation of the incentive [7]. Assuming that the initial rejection of the devalued solution reflects an aversive internal state [8,9], hypothesized that emotional conditioning takes place during the incentive downshift event. The basis for this memory is postulated to be a Pavlovian association between the taste of the downshifted, 4% sucrose solution and the internal state of frustration induced by the downshift. As a result, tasting the 4% sucrose solution would elicit an internal aversive Disclaimer: The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the official policy or position of the Department of the Navy, Department of Defense, nor the U.S. Government. Corresponding author. Tel.: address: m.papini@tcu.edu (M.R. Papini). 1 Current address: Naval Health Research Center, 14 Sylvester Road, San Diego, CA 9216, USA. state that would tend to suppress consummatory behavior and also induce a behavioral switch from consumption to searching or escaping. The associative reactivation of this emotional memory is assumed to retard recovery from csnc beyond the first downshift trial. Emotional memory reactivation is a source of consummatory suppression because animals tend to avoid stimuli associated with negative emotional content, such as the area where they have consumed the devalued sucrose solution (i.e., secondary frustration; [5,1]). The research reported here is concerned with the encoding of this aversive memory of the downshift. The effects of drugs on memory consolidation can be best assessed by posttrial drug administration, that is, by administering the drug immediately after the relevant experience. Memory consolidation is a time-dependent process occurring after acquisition and rendering the memory trace more stable and more resistant to disruption [11]. Because training occurs before drug administration, behavior during training is not affected directly by the drug. Because behavioral testing of drug effects usually occurs a day after drug administration (as in the present experiments), and because many psychoactive drugs are metabolized within hours (e.g., in rats, the half-life of d-cycloserine, used in these experiments, was reported to be 6 7 min; [12,13]), it can be assumed that the drug has been metabolized by the time of testing. As a result, posttrial drug effects on behavior can be more safely attributed to the drug s effect on memory consolidation, rather than to direct drug effects on motor, motivational, or attentional processes active during training and/or testing ([14 17]; but see Experiment 3, below, /$ see front matter 211 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:1.116/j.bbr

3 J.N. Norris et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 223 (211) for a potential alternative of the effects of posttraining drug administration). Evidence consistent with the encoding of a downshift memory is provided by the effects of corticosterone administered immediately after the first downshift trial. Based on extensive research in a variety of learning paradigms, glucocorticoids can be safely assumed to enhance memory consolidation [18 21]. When administered in the csnc situation, immediately after the first downshift trial, corticosterone retards recovery from csnc [22]. Posttraining corticosterone has no effects on consummatory behavior under the following conditions [22,23]: (1) In the absence of a downshift event, as in unshifted controls or when administered after a single exposure to 4% sucrose; thus, corticosterone s effects in the csnc situation cannot be attributed to a conditioned taste aversion to 4% sucrose. (2) When corticosterone is administered 3 h after the first downshift trial, rather than immediately after, thus demonstrating the time-dependent nature of its effects. (3) When corticosterone is administered in an anticipatory contrast situation involving the same solutions used in the csnc situation, but in a procedure known to activate different brain mechanisms from those involved in csnc [2]. Therefore, the selectivity of these corticosterone effects to the csnc situation is consistent with the enhancement of the aversive memory of the downshift event. Other drugs known from other learning paradigms to modulate memory consolidation and tested in the csnc situation under similar conditions do not affect the course of recovery from the incentive downshift as corticosterone does. These include cholinergic drugs (e.g., atropine and physostigmine), known to modulate consolidation of conditioned fear [24 26], but having no effect on csnc [27], and opioids (naloxone, DPDPE, and naltrindole), known to modulate consolidation of conditioned fear [17,28,29], but having no detectable effect on recovery from csnc [3]. Therefore, memory consolidation of the incentive downshift experience is modulated by some, but not all of the neurochemical systems known to be involved in other types of emotional memory. The present experiments were designed to determine whether the N-methyl-d-aspartate glutamate receptor (NMDAR) is involved in memory consolidation in the csnc situation. There is a well established connection between glucocorticoids and NMDARs, although the relationship is complex. For example, glucocorticoids enhance NMDAR activation in hippocampal tissue extracted from neonatal rats [31]. Furthermore, Nair and Bonneau reported that acute restraint stress induced glucocorticoid elevation that, in turn, increased microglia proliferation (a proinflammatory response); this effect was blocked by administration of the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-81. Thus, we hypothesized that the enhancing effects of posttrial corticosterone on csnc described above were mediated by activation of NMDARs. In these experiments, animals received administration of d-cycloserine (DCS) immediately after the first downshift trial following a 32-to-4% sucrose downshift (Experiment 1) or a 32-to-6% sucrose downshift (Experiment 2). Finally, Experiment 3 explored the possibility that DCS s effects acted as an unconditioned stimulus supporting the development of a conditioned taste aversion to the 4% sucrose. DCS is a partial agonist at the glycine binding site (NR1B) of the NMDAR; the endogenous ligand, glycine, has greater affinity to this binding site than DCS, which is thus characterized as a partial agonist. As a result, DCS may act either as an agonist or an antagonist depending on the extracellular concentration of glycine [33]. DCS was selected because there is strong evidence from a variety of learning paradigms that this drug functions as a memory enhancer. For example, in the fear conditioning situation, DCS administration either before or after training sessions has been shown to facilitate extinction [34 37], reduced reinstatement following extinction [38], and increased generalization of extinction to nonextinguished stimuli [35]. In spatial learning situations, DCS improves performance in rats made spatially deficient either by hippocampal lesions [39] or scopolamine-induced amnesia [4,41]. In flavor learning situations, pretrial DCS administration enhanced LiCl-induced conditioned taste aversions [42] and also enhanced fructose-induced conditioned taste preferences [43]. DCS also reversed deficits on eyeblink conditioning in rabbits induced by exposure to inescapable shocks [44] and restored episodic-like memory in an object-recognition task with rats [45]. These references are but a subset of the growing literature on the effects of DCS on learning, all suggesting that, within some constraints (e.g., there is apparently an optimal dose for some of these effects; [46,47]), DCS enhances learning (i.e., the acquisition of new information). Because of the memory enhancing effects reported for both corticosterone and DCS, and because of the connection between glucocorticoid levels and NMDAR activation, we predicted that DCS would have effects in the csnc situation similar to those described above for corticosterone. Furthermore, we assumed that DCS would not affect well-established memories, such as the memory of the preshift solution. Thus, if DCS retards recovery from csnc, this result would be interpreted as an enhancement of the emotional memory of the downshift. 1. Experiment 1 It was hypothesized that posttrial DCS administration would have the same effect on csnc than corticosterone did in previous experiments [22,23]. Therefore, Experiment 1 tested the hypothesis that DCS administration prolongs the csnc effect. A 3 mg/kg dose was chosen based on prior research showing that this dose was effective with rats trained in other tasks [4,47,48]. 2. Method 2.1. Subjects The subjects were 41 male Long-Evans rats bred at the TCU vivarium from an original group purchased from Harlan Laboratories (Indianapolis, IN), approximately 9 days old at the start of the experiment and experimentally naïve. The average ad libitum weight of the entire sample was 45 g (range: g). Rats were housed in a vivarium under a 12:12-h light, dark cycle (lights on at 7: h) and were deprived of food to an 81 84% of the free-food weight. Water was continuously available in each individual cage. Animals were trained during the light phase of the daily cycle 2.2. Apparatus Training was conducted in 4 conditioning boxes (MED Associates, St. Albans, VT) constructed of aluminum and Plexiglas, and measuring 29.4 cm 28.9 cm 24.7 cm (L H W). The floor was made of steel rods.5 cm in diameter and 1.2 cm apart running perpendicular to the feeder wall. A bedding tray filled with corncob bedding was placed below the floor to collect fecal pellets and urine. Against the feeder wall was an elliptical hole 1 cm 2 cm (W H), 3.5 cm from the floor. A sipper tube, 1 cm in diameter, was inserted through this hole. When fully inserted, the sipper tube was flush against the wall. A computer located in an adjacent room controlled the presentation and retraction of the sipper tube, and detected contact with the sipper tube via a circuit involving the steel rods in the floor. Each conditioning box was placed in a sound-attenuating chamber that contained a house light, a speaker to deliver white noise, and a fan for ventilation. Together, the speaker and fan produced noise with an intensity of 8.1 db (SPL, Scale C) 2.3. Procedure Training lasted trials, administered 1 trial per day. Each rat was randomly assigned to one of the conditioning boxes and always trained in that box. The order of training of the 4-rat squads varied across days. After each trial, conditioning boxes were cleaned with a damp paper towel, feces removed, and bedding material replaced as needed. During trials, the houselight, white noise, and fan were on continuously. The trials were divided into a preshift phase ( 1 1) and a postshift phase ( 11 ). Prior to Trial 1, rats were matched by ad libitum weight and then randomly assigned to the downshifted or unshifted condition. After Trial 1, rats exposed to 32% sucrose were matched in terms of overall preshift performance and randomly assigned to one of the two different drug conditions, Saline or 3 mg/kg DCS. The unshifted controls were assigned likewise. This drug dose is frequently reported as effective in prior research (e.g., [37])

4 35 J.N. Norris et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 223 (211) For the two 32-to-4 groups (, ), the 1 preshift trials involved access to a 32% sucrose solution (w/w, prepared by mixing 32 g of commercial sugar for every 68 g of distilled water); the 5 postshift trials involved access to a 4% solution (w/w, 4 g of sugar for every 96 g of distilled water). The two 4-to-4 groups (4/Sal, 4/DCS) received the 4% sucrose solution in all trials. Each trial started with a variable interval of 3 s (range: 45 s). At the end of this interval, the sipper tube was automatically presented. The first recorded contact with the sipper tube initiated a 5-min interval during which the sucrose solution was freely available. At the end of this interval, the sipper tube was retracted and a final variable interval of 3 s (range: 45 s) ended the trial. The dependent variable was the cumulative amount of time in contact with the sipper tube, measured in.5-s units and labeled goal-tracking time. Goal-tracking times were recorded for each entire 5-min trial and also in 5-s bins to analyze within-trial performance. Goal-tracking times were subjected to conventional analysis of variance (ANOVA). The alpha value was set to p <.5 for all statistical tests. DCS or saline was administered immediately following the first downshift trial (Trial 11). Saline animals received an equal-volume injection of isotonic saline. Drug concentrations were adjusted to a 1 ml/kg volume. At the end of the trial, animals were placed back into the transport rack and moved into the holding room where they received the appropriate injection. This took approximately 3 9 s. The following groups were included: (n = 11), (n = 1), 4/Sal (n = 1), and 4/DCS (n = 1). It was assumed that DCS had no direct effects on 12, as DCS s levels in brain and plasma are in the order of 6 7 min [12,13]. DCS was purchased from Sigma-Aldrich Chemicals (Saint Louis, MO). It was freshly dissolved in isotonic saline solution (3 mg/ml) First 1 s Last 1 s 14 4/Sal 4/DCS 3. Results The results are shown in Fig. 1. A Drug Sucrose Preshift Trial analysis revealed a significant change across trials, F(9, 333) = 81.95, p <.1, a main effect of sucrose, F(1, 37) = 1.81, p <.1, and a significant trial by sucrose interaction, F(9, 333) = 3.18, p <.1, but not significant main effect of drug or of drug-related interactions, Fs < 1. A Drug Sucrose Postshift Trial analysis indicated a significant main effect of trial, F(4, 148) = 16.6, p <.1, a significant trial by sucrose interaction, F(4, 148) = 7.14, p <.1, and a trial by sucrose by drug interaction, F(4, 148) = 2.57, p <.1. All other effects were not significant, Fs < 1. To clarify the source of the three-way interaction, one-way ANOVAs for 11 were calculated for pairs of groups involving the same drug condition, but different behavioral treatment: vs. 4/DCS and vs. 4/Sal. For the saline groups, the csnc effect was significant on Trial 11, F(1, 19) = 8.34, p <.1, marginal for Trial 12, F(1, 19) = 4.23, p =.54, and not significant for 13, Fs < 1.49, ps >.23. However, the csnc effect was extended for DCS-treated groups throughout the entire postshift phase, 11. Group contacted the sipper tube Trial 1 4/Sal 4/DCS Fig. 1. Results of Experiment 1. Mean goal-tracking time (±SEM) is plotted as a function of trial, contrast condition, and drug treatment. On 1 1, Groups and received 32% sucrose solution, whereas Groups 4/Sal and 4/DCS received access to 4% sucrose solution. All animals had access to 4% sucrose on 11. Animals were administered either saline (Sal) or d-cycloserine (DCS, 3 mg/kg, ip) immediately after Trial 11, the first downshift trial for Groups and /Sal 4/DCS Fig. 2. Performance during the first (top panel) and last (bottom panel) 1 s of the last preshift trial (Trial 1) and each of the downshift trials ( 11 ) in Experiment 1. Mean goal-tracking time (±SEM) plotted as a function of trial, contrast condition, and drug treatment. Contrast treatments were as described in Fig. 1. significantly less than Group 4/DCS on 11, Fs (1, 18) > 4.86, ps <.5. Further pairwise comparisons were computed, also for 11, between groups with the same behavioral condition, but different drug condition: vs. and 4/DCS vs. 4/Sal. Thus, Group contacted the sipper tube significantly less than Group on Trial 13, F(1, 19) = 1.9, p <.5, marginally on Trial 12, F(1, 19) = 3.79, p =.67, and not significantly on 11, 14, and, Fs (1, 19) < 1.32, ps >.26. However, DCS had no effect on the unshifted controls. The comparisons between Groups 4/DCS and 4/Sal were all not significant for 11, Fs < 2.5, ps >.16. Previous research indicated that consummatory suppression is not evident during the initial 1 s of postshift trials, but it emerges fully during the last 1 s of such trials [49]. Consequently, the within-trial results for the initial and last 1 s of 1 are presented in Fig. 2. In both cases, a Drug (DCS, Sal) Sucrose (32%, 4%) Trial (11 ) analysis yielded a triple interaction that fell short of significance: F(4, 124) = 2.1, p =.85 for the first 1 s, and F(4, 124) = 1.55, p =.191 for the last 1 s. Given the results for the entire session (Fig. 1) and the graphical depiction of the withintrial data (Fig. 2), a second analysis was calculated only for The results were as follows.

5 J.N. Norris et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 223 (211) For the first 1 s (Fig. 2, top panel), the Drug Sucrose Trial (11 13) analysis indicated a significant triple interaction, F(2, 62) = 3.61, p <.4, as well as a significant contrast effect, with downshifted scoring below unshifted controls, F(1, 31) = 9.24, p <.1, and significant change across trials, F(2, 62) = 3.21, p <.5. All other effects were not significant, Fs < 2.43, ps >.9. Pairwise analyses for groups matched by drug condition showed that Groups vs. 4/Sal did not differ on 11 13, Fs(1, 16) < 2.26, ps >., whereas Groups vs. 4/DCS also failed to differ on Trial 11, before DCS administration, F(1, ) = 3.8, p >.9, but the csnc was significant for 12 13, Fs(1, ) > 14.61, ps <.3, after DCS administration. Comparisons for groups matched in terms of the behavioral treatment showed that Group contacted the sipper tube significantly more than on Trial 13, F(1, ) = 7.49, p <.2. On 11 the difference was not significant, F < 1, and on Trial 12 was also not significant, but marginal, F(1, ) = 3.88, p =.68. However, DCS had no detectable effect on the unshifted controls, as indicated by a comparison between Groups 4/Sal vs. 4/DCS for 11 13, Fs < 1. Fig. 2(bottom panel) also shows the results for the last 1 s of 1. The Drug Sucrose Trial (11 13) analysis provided a significant triple interaction, F(2, 62) = 3.71, p <.4, as well as a significant contrast effect, F(1, 31) = 31.88, p <.1, and significant change across trials, F(2, 62) = 4.97, p <.2. All other effects were not significant, Fs < 3.16, ps >.8. Pairwise analyses for groups matched by drug condition showed that Groups vs. 4/Sal differed significantly on 11 12, Fs(1, 16) > 8.1, ps >.2, but not on Trial 13, F < 1, whereas Groups vs. 4/DCS exhibited significant differences in all three postshift trials, Fs(1, ) > 9.59, p >.6. Comparisons for groups matched for behavioral treatment showed that Group contacted the sipper tube significantly more than on Trial 13, F(1, ) = 8.92, p <.1. On 11 the difference was not significant, but marginal, F(1, ) 4.7, p =.62, but on Trial 12 it was not significant, F < 1. DCS had no detectable effect on the unshifted controls on 11 13, Fs(1, ) < 1.37, ps >.25. In summary, DCS administration after the first downshift trial (Trial 11) enhanced suppression of consummatory behavior in subsequent trials. As shown by within-trial analyses, such suppression was detectable during the initial 1 s of subsequent trials ( 12 13), as well as during the last 1 s of the same trials. The fact that DCS-treated animals show consummatory suppression earlier than saline-treated animals is consistent with facilitated retrieval of the emotional memory of the incentive downshift event experienced a day earlier. Such facilitated retrieval seems to have modulated behavior throughout the trial, as assessed by facilitated csnc effect during the last 1 s, relative to saline groups. This result is thus consistent with DCS-induced facilitation of memory retrieval Experiment 2 In the present experiment, the same DCS dose and administration used previously occurred after a 32-to-6% sucrose downshift. The 32-to-6 downshift was used for two reasons. First, a reduction in incentive disparity attenuates csnc [5], thus reducing the opportunity for floor effects in DCS-treated animals. Second, Ruetti et al. [23] showed that posttrial administration of corticosterone enhanced csnc following a 32-to-4% sucrose downshift, but not an 8-to-4% sucrose downshift, concluding that corticosterone s enhancing effects were possible only when an animal experienced a large incentive disparity. Similarly, Daniel et al. [3] showed that naloxone administered prior to the first downshift trial reduced consummatory behavior after a 32-to-6% sucrose downshift, but not after a 16-to-6% sucrose downshift. Likewise, using a less severe downshift will help determine whether the effects of DCS depend on a minimum incentive disparity /Sal 6/DCS Fig. 3. Results of Experiment 2. Goal-tracking time (s) is plotted as a function of trial, contrast condition, and drug treatment. Contrast treatments were as described in Fig. 1, except that the low solution was 6% sucrose. 4. Method 4.1. Subjects and apparatus The subjects were 37 male Long-Evans hooded rats from the TCU vivarium approximately 9 days old at the start of the experiment and experimentally naïve. The average ad libitum weight was 423 g (range: g). Housing, training procedure, apparatus, drug preparation, and route of administration were similar to Experiment Procedure The only difference with respect to the previous experiment was the use of 6% sucrose as the downshifted solution (w/w, prepared by mixing 6 g of sucrose for every 94 g of distilled water). The following groups were included: (n = 9), (n = 9), 6/Sal (n = 9), and 6/DCS (n = 1). All other procedural aspects were as described in the previous experiment. 5. Results The results are shown in Fig. 3. A Drug Sucrose Preshift Trial (1 1) analysis revealed significantly higher goal-tracking scores with 32% sucrose than 6% sucrose, F(1, 33) = 4.83, p <.4, and a significant increase across trials, F(9, 297) = 83.64, p <.1; the main effect for drug and the interactions were all not significant, Fs(9, 297) < 1.56, ps >.12. A Drug Sucrose Postshift Trial (11 ) analysis yielded significant main effects for sucrose, F(1, 33) = 6.67, p <.2, trial, F(4, 132) = 16.45, p <.1, and the trial by sucrose interaction, F(4, 132) = 18.4, p <.1. All other effects were not significant, Fs < 1, including the triple interaction. The results of the within-trial analysis are presented in Fig. 4. Whether in the first or last 1 s, there was no hint of a DCS effect in these data. Drug Sucrose Trial (11 ) analyses, whether for the first or last 1 s, provided no evidence of a triple interaction, Fs < 1. The contrast effect was significant for both the first and last 1 s sets, in terms of a sucrose by trial interaction, F(4, 132) = 6.7, p <.1. The main effect of sucrose as also significant for the last 1 s data set, F(1, 33) = 11.43, p <.3, but not for the first 1 s data set, F < 1. Changes across trials were significant for both analyses, Fs(4, 132) > 3.47, ps <.2. Other factors were not significant, Fs < 1. The same results were obtained with a similar analysis restricted to 11 13, as had been done in Experiment 1. The results of Experiment 2 failed to uncover any effects of DCS after a 32-to-6% sucrose downshift. One implication of these results

6 352 J.N. Norris et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 223 (211) First 1 s 12 Last 1 s /Sal 6/DCS Fig. 4. Performance during the first (top panel) and last (bottom panel) 1 s of the last preshift trial (Trial 1) and each of the downshift trials ( 11 ) in Experiment 2. Mean goal-tracking time (±SEM) plotted as a function of trial, contrast condition, and drug treatment. Contrast treatments were as described in Fig. 1, except that the low solution was 6% sucrose. is that a minimum disparity between preshift and postshift sucrose concentrations is required for DCS administered immediately after the first downshift trial to affect recovery from csnc. Notice that the failure to detect an effect of posttrial DCS administration occurred despite the presence of a significant csnc effect after a 32-to-6% sucrose downshift Experiment 3 Experiment 1 showed that DCS administered immediately after the first downshift trial prolonged the csnc effect in subsequent trials. Whereas these results provide support for the hypothesis that activation of NMDA receptors using DCS enhances csnc by facilitating the consolidation and subsequent retrieval of the emotional memory of the incentive downshift event, one alternative remains to be evaluated. Nunnink et al. [37] reported that pairing DCS at 3 mg/kg with access to saccharine reduced preference against water in subsequent two-bottle tests, compared to a saline control. Although the experiment lacked an unpaired control, it is plausible that the effect of DCS on csnc may be the result of a conditioned taste aversion, rather than of emotional memory consolidation. This analysis is described in detail in similar experiments involving other drugs administered after the first downshift trial in the csnc preparation [23,3,51]. Briefly, the 4% sucrose solution is relatively novel for downshifted animals and may act as an efficient conditioned stimulus for the effects induced by DCS administration, which would be the unconditioned stimulus. This factor would be less prominent in unshifted controls because of their extensive preexposure to 4% sucrose during preshift trials (i.e., latent inhibition reduces conditioned taste aversions; [52]). Experiment 2 provided data inconsistent with the conditioned taste aversion hypothesis because DCS had no measurable effect on the subsequent consumption of 6% sucrose. However, because of the different concentrations used in Experiments 1 and 2 (4% vs. 6% sucrose) the role of this factor cannot be safely excluded. For example, the lesser disparity between 6 and 32% sucrose, compared to 4 and 32% sucrose, may have rendered the 6% solution relatively less novel and, therefore, relatively less likely to enter into an association with the consequences of DCS administration. Experiment 3 evaluated the conditioned taste aversion hypothesis by pairing 4% sucrose to DCS in the absence of an incentive downshift, but in a situation that was otherwise the same as that used in Experiment 1. Moreover, unlike in Nunnink et al. s [42] experiment, the present study included a nonassociative control group that received exposure to both 4% sucrose and DCS, but in an unpaired fashion. If the effects of DCS observed in previous experiments are mediated by a conditioned taste aversion, rather than memory consolidation, then DCS should result in a similar suppression of consummatory behavior in the absence of the downshift event. 6. Method 6.1. Subjects and apparatus The subjects were 36 male, experimentally naïve Long-Evans hooded rats, approximately 9 days old at the start of the experiment. The average ad libitum weight for the entire sample was 397 g (range: g). Rats were maintained as described in Experiment 1. The training apparatus and drug preparation were also the same described previously Procedure Training lasted 3 daily trials, scheduled to be identical to trials in the csnc experiments reported above. Subjects received 4% sucrose solution in every trial. Prior to Trial 1, rats were matched by ad libitum weight and then randomly assigned to one of three conditions: Paired (n = 12), Unpaired (n = 12), or Saline (n = 12). All groups received two injections; the first injection was administered immediately at the end of Trial 1 and the second injection was administered 3 h after the end of Trial 1. The first injection was saline for Groups Unpaired and Saline, and DCS (3 mg/kg, ip) for Group Paired. The second injection was saline for Groups Paired and Saline, but DCS (3 mg/kg, ip) for Group Unpaired. Thus, groups were matched in terms of the number and temporal distribution of injections; moreover, Groups Paired and Unpaired were also matched in terms of exposure to DCS. 7. Results The results are shown in Fig. 5. A Group Trial (1 3) analysis showed a significant increase of behavior across trials, F(2, 66) = 67.77, p <.1, but groups were not significantly different, F(2, 33) = 1.14, p >.3, as was the interaction effect, F < 1. Oneway ANOVAs for each trial yielded not significant group effect for 1 3, Fs(2, 35) < 1.57, ps >.22. The not significant difference obtained on Trial 1 indicates that subjects were behaviorally

7 J.N. Norris et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 223 (211) Goal-Tracking Time (s) Paired Unpaired Saline Fig. 5. Results of Experiment 3. Goal-tracking time (s) is plotted as a function of trial, and drug treatment. On 1 3 Paired, Unpaired, and Saline groups received 4% sucrose solution. Group Paired received DCS (3 mg/kg, ip) immediately after Trial 1 and saline 3 h after Trial 1. Group Unpaired received saline immediately after Trial 1 and DCS (3 mg/kg, ip) 3 h after Trial 1. Group Saline received saline injections immediately after and 3 h after Trial 1. All groups were equated in terms of the number and temporal distribution of injections; Groups Paired and Unpaired were also equated in terms of DCS administration. Notice that none of the groups was exposed to an incentive downshift in Experiment 3. matched before the treatment. These results provide no evidence that the effects of DCS reported in Experiment 1 were the result a conditioned taste aversion. 8. General discussion The current studies demonstrated that the partial NMDAR agonist DCS retarded recovery from csnc when administered immediately following the first trial involving a 32-to-4% sucrose incentive downshift. DCS failed to induce conditioned taste aversion, suggesting that its suppressive effects on consummatory behavior were not the result of an acquired rejection of the relatively novel 4% sucrose solution in downshifted animals. Furthermore, DCS had no detectable effect on the consummatory behavior of nonshifted rats. These results suggest that the effects of DCS on csnc were not caused by performance factors (i.e., perceptual, motivational, or motor effects). Therefore, the effects of DCS were restricted to animals that had some experience with an incentive downshift event an interpretation consistent with a mnemonic effect of DCS. These results were interpreted to support the hypothesis that NMDA receptor activation via the glycinemodulatory site enhances the consolidation, hence facilitating retrieval of the emotional aversive memory induced by the unexpected incentive downshift event. The effects of posttrial DCS administration were interpreted as acting on mnemonic processes that presumably occur after DCS administration, but conclude prior to next day s trial. It is implausible that DCS is still active a day after its administration [12,13]. Behavioral evidence from the present experiments (e.g., unshifted groups in both experiments) and from fear conditioning experiments [48] suggests that the dose used here (3 mg/kg, ip) does not have measurable behavioral effects 24 h after its administration. When DCS was selected for study (see Section 1 for rationale), it was assumed that DCS facilitated memory consolidation based mainly on results from the fear conditioning situation. Recent evidence suggests that the effects of DCS on fear extinction reflect the erasure of the original fear acquisition, rather than an enhancement of fear extinction learning [53]. Mao et al. [53] reported that DCS facilitated the reversal of postsynaptic changes caused as a result of fear extinction, as assessed in terms of a decreased surface expression of GluR1, a subunit of AMPA( -amino-3-hydroxy-5- methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid) receptors, a type of receptor known to mediate conditioning [54]. Thus, DCS may act to facilitate NMDA-mediated endocytosis of AMPA receptors, erasing learning. This memory-erasure hypothesis can be applied to the csnc situation as follows. With posttrial administration (Experiment 1), it could be argued that DCS erased the emotional memory of the downshift event acquired during the first downshift trial. This would predict a csnc effect on the second downshift trial (Trial 12) that would be essentially identical to that of the first downshift trial (Trial 11). Instead, Experiment 1 showed that DCS-treated animals exhibited greater early suppression on the second and third downshift trials ( 12 13) than saline-treated animals, an effect contradicting the memory erasure hypothesis. Moreover, this hypothesis cannot adequately explain the effects of DCS on maze learning, conditioned place preference, and conditioned taste aversion reported by others [42,43,47]. The effects of DCS on csnc reported here were assumed to be mediated by an increase in the activity of NMDA receptors, which would lead to the strengthening of the emotional memory of the downshift event. However, available evidence suggests that DCS could act in some cases as an antagonist. For example, Hood et al. [33] have shown that DCS s binding to the NMDA receptor is only approximately 4 5% as effective as glycine. Thus, under an appropriate combination of DCS dose and extracellular glycine levels, DCS could actually reduce NMDA-receptor effectiveness. Along these lines, Saul skaya and Solov eva [55] reported that glycine levels in the nucleus accumbens are transiently elevated following forced changes in feeding behavior in rats. This is especially relevant to the current studies for two reasons. First, the nucleus accumbens shows increased c-fos transcription (a transcription factor for immediate early gene expression correlated with synaptic changes such as growth and protein kinase activity) and reduced dopamine efflux following 32-to-4% sucrose downshift [56,57]. Second, increased glycine levels in Saul skaya and Solov eva s [55] study were observed selectively when animals were exposed to both feeding and a fear-inducing stimulus previously paired with shock-induced pain. Thus, it is plausible that the forced rearrangement of feeding behavior caused by incentive downshift in the csnc situation acted to increase glycine levels, thus converting DCS into an antagonist at the glycine site. Despite these potential interactions of DCS and extracellular glycine levels, the present results do not support the hypothesis that DCS acted as an antagonist. Experiments 1 and 2 showed that the effectiveness of DCS scaled to the magnitude of the downshift. DCS interfered with recovery from csnc following a 32-to-4% sucrose downshift, but had no detectable effect following a 32- to-6% sucrose downshift. If 3 mg/kg DCS acted as an antagonist, then the effects of posttrial DCS administration following these two levels of incentive downshift should have yielded similar results. However, if DCS at 3 mg/kg acted as an agonist, then its effects should be directly related to the size of the disparity between preshift and postshift incentives. Therefore, the results reported here indicate that DCS functioned as an agonist in the csnc preparation. The present results are consistent with the hypothesis that DCS enhanced the consolidation of the emotional memory of the incentive downshift event. This is so because DCS retarded recovery from csnc, a consequence consistent with the strengthening of an emotional memory. Because posttrial administration of corticosterone also retarded recovery from csnc [22,23], it is tempting to argue that these two effects are related. The memory enhancing effects of stress hormones like corticosterone, epinephrine, and norepinephrine are well documented [,17]. These mnemonic effects of stress hormones are mediated by glutamatergic receptors in the brain. For example, norepinephrine levels enhance contextual fear conditioning in mice by phosphorylating GluR1 subunits of the AMPA receptor [54]. Similar interactions occur between glucocorticoid administration and activation of NMDA receptors. For example, restraint stress enhances allodynia (increased pain

8 354 J.N. Norris et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 223 (211) sensitivity to nonpainful stimuli) induced by neuropathic pain and this effect is mediated by glucocorticoid release. Corticosterone administration often has similar effects to those of restraint stress and its effects on allodynia are eliminated by coadministration of memantine, an NMDA receptor antagonist [58]. Because the effects of corticosterone in other situations are mediated by glutamatergic activity [32], we hypothesize that the retardation of recovery from csnc induced by corticosterone and DCS reflects effects at two different stages of the same cascade leading to enhanced consolidation of the emotional memory of the downshift. Acknowledgements The research reported here was partially supported by SERC grant # 831, TCU School of Science and Engineering. The authors thank C. Torres, A. E. Mustaca, and M. F. Lopez-Seal for valuable comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. References [1] Vogel JR, Mikulka PJ, Spear NE. Effects of shifts in sucrose and saccaharine concentrations on licking behavior in the rat. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology 1968;66: [2] Flaherty CF. Incentive relativity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; [3] Elliott MH. The effect of change of reward on the maze performance of rats. Univ Calif Pub Psychol 1928;4:19 3. [4] Pecoraro N, Timberlake W, Tinsley M. Incentive downshifts evoke search behavior in rats (Rattus norvegicus). J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Proc 1999;25: [5] Daly HB. Reinforcing properties of escape from frustration aroused in various learning situations. Psychology of Learning & Motivation 1974;8: [6] Norris JN, Pérez-Acosta A, Ortega LA, Papini MR. Naloxone facilitates appetitive extinction and eliminates escape from frustration. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior 29;94:81 7. [7] Gray JA, McNaughton N. The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. 2nd Edition Oxford UK: Oxford University Press; 2. [8] Papini MR. Comparative psychology of surprising nonreward. Brain Behavior & Evolution 23;62: [9] Papini MR, Wood M, Daniel AM, Norris JN. Reward loss as psychological pain. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy 26;6: [1] Amsel A. Frustration theory. An analysis of dispositional learning and memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; [11] McGaugh JL. Memory: a century of consolidation. Science 2;287: [12] Baran H, Gramer M, Löscher W. Alterations in plasma and brain amino acids after administration of the glycine/nmda receptor partial agonist, d-cycloserine, to mice and rats. European Journal of Pharmacology 1995;273: [13] Löscher W, Wlaz P, Rundfeldt C, Baran H, Hönack D. Anticonvulsant effects of the glycine/nmda receptor ligands d-cycloserine and d-serine but not R-(+)-HA-966 in amygdala-kindled rats. British Journal of Pharmacology 1994;112: [14] Breen RA, McGaugh JL. Faciliation of maze learning with posttrial injections of picrotoxin. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology 1961;54: [] Gold PE. Memory-enhancing drugs. In: Byrne JH, editor. Learning and memory: a comprehensive reference, vol. 3. New York: Elsevier; 28. p [16] McGaugh JL. Memory and emotion. New York: Columbia University Press; 23. [17] McGaugh JL, Roozendaal B. Memory modulation. In: Byrne JH, editor. Learning and memory: a comprehensive reference, vol. 3. New York: Elsevier; 28. p [18] Cordero MI, Sandi C. A role for brain glucocorticoid receptors in contextual fear conditioning: dependence upon training intensity. Brain Research 1998;786:11 7. [19] Flood JF, Vidal D, Bennett EL, Orme AE, Vasquez S, Jarvik ME. Memory facilitating and anti-amnesic effects of corticosteroids. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior 1978;8:81 7. [2] Hui GK, Figueroa IR, Poytress BS, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL, Weinberger N. Memory enhancement of classical fear conditioning by post-training injections of corticosterone in rats. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory 24;81: [21] Kovács GL, Telegdy G, Lissák K. Dose-dependent action of corticosteroids on brain serotonin content and passive avoidance behavior. Hormones & Behavior 1977;8:5 65. [22] Bentosela M, Ruetti E, Muzio R, Mustaca AE, Papini MR. Administration of corticosterone after the first downshift trial enhances consummatory successive negative contrast. Behavioral Neuroscience 26;12: [23] Ruetti E, Justel N, Mustaca AE, Papini MR. Posttrial corticosterone administration enhances the effects of incentive downshift: exploring the boundaries of this effect. Behavioral Neuroscience 29;123: [24] Baratti CM, Kopf S. The post-training memory enhancement induced by physostigmine and oxotremorine in mice is not state-dependent. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory 1996;65: [25] Liu TL, Liang KC. Posttraining infusion of cholinergic drugs into the ventral subiculum modulated memory in an inhibitory avoidance task: Interaction with the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory 29;91: [26] Todd J, Kesner RP. Effects of posttraining injection of cholinergic agonists and antagonists into the amygdala on retention of passive avoidance training in rats. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology 1978;92: [27] Bentosela M, D Ambros M, Altamirano A, Muzio RN, Baratti C, Mustaca AE. Posttrial administration of cholinergic drugs does not affect consummatory successive negative contrast in rats. International Journal of Comparative Psycholology 25;18:91 9. [28] McGaugh JL, Introini-Collison IB, Juler RG, Izquierdo I. Stria terminalis lesions attenuate the effects of posttraining naloxone and betaendorphin on retention. Behavioral Neuroscience 1986;1: [29] McGaugh JL, Introini-Collison IB, Nagahara AH. Memory-enhancing effects of posttraining naloxone: involvement of -noradrenergic influences in the amygdaloid complex. Brain Research 1988;446: [3] Daniel AM, Ortega L, Papini MR. Role of the opioid system in incentive downshift situations. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory 29;92: [31] Takahashi T, Kimoto T, Tanabe N, Hattori TA, Yasumatsu N, Kawato S. Corticosterone acutely prolonged N-methyl-daspartate receptor-mediated Ca 2+ elevation in cultured rat hippocampal neurons. Journal of Neurochemistry 22;83: [32] Nair A, Bonneau RH. Stress-induced elevation of glucocorticoids increases microglia proliferation through NMDA receptor activation. J Neuroimmunol 26;171: [33] Hood WF, Compton RP, Monahan JB. d-cycloserine: a ligand for the N-methyld-aspartate coupled glycine receptor has partial agonist characteristics. Neuroscience Letters 1989;98:91. [34] Bouton ME, Vurbic D, Woods AM. d-cycloserine facilitates context-specific fear extinction learning. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory 28;9: [35] Ledgerwood L, Richardson R, Cranney J. Effect of d-cycloserine on extinction of conditioned freezing. Behavioral Neuroscience 23;117: [36] Lee J, Milton A, Everitt B. Reconsolidation and extinction of conditioned fear, inhibition and potentiation. Journal of Neuroscience 26;26:1 6. [37] Walker DL, Ressler KJ, Lu K, Davis M. Facilitation of conditioned fear extinction by systemic administrator or intra-amygdala infusions of d-cycloserine as assessed with fear-potentiated startle in rats. Journal of Neuroscience 22;22: [38] Ledgerwood L, Richardson R, Cranney J. d-cycloserine and the facilitation of extinction of conditioned fear: consequences for reinstatement. Behavioral Neuroscience 24;119: [39] Schuster GM, Schmidt WJ. d-cycloserine reverses the working memory impairment of hippocampal-lesioned rats in a spatial learning task. European Journal of Pharmacology 1992;224:97 8. [4] Sirvio J, Ekonsalo T, Riekkinen P, Lahtinen H, Riekkinen P. d-cycloserine a modulator of the N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor improves spatial learning in rats treated with muscarinic antagonist. Neuroscience Letters 1992;146:2 8. [41] Zajaczkowski W, Danysz W. Effects of d-cycloserine and aniracetam on spatial learning in rats with entorhinal cortex lesions. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior 1997;56:21 9. [42] Nunnink M, Davenport RA, Ortega B, Houpt TA. d-cycloserine enhances conditioned taste aversion learning in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior 27;87: [43] Golden GL, Houpt TA. NMDA receptor in conditioned flavor-taste preference learning: Blockade by MK-81 and enhancement by d-cycloserine. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior 27;86: [44] Waddell J, Mallimo E, Shors T. d-cycloserine reverses the detrimental effects of stress on learning in females and enhances retention in males. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 21;93:31 6. [45] Zlomuzica A, De Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Dere E. NMDA receptor modulation by d-cycloserine promotes episodic-like memory in mice. Psychopharmacology 27;193:53 9. [46] D Souza DC, Charney DS, Krystal JH. Glycine site agonists of the NMDA receptor: a review. CNS Drug Review 1995;1: [47] Quartermain D, Mower J, Rafferty M, Herting R, Lanthorn T. Acute but not chronic activation of the NMDA-coupled glycine receptor with d-cycloserine facilitates learning and retention. European Journal of Pharmacology 1994;257:7 12. [48] Woods AM, Bouton ME. d-cycloserine facilitates extinction but does not eliminate renewal of the conditioned emotional response. Behavioral Neuroscience 26;12: [49] Norris JN, Daniel AM, Papini MR. Spontaneous recovery of consummatory behavior, but not consummatory contrast. Learning & Motivation 28;39: [5] Papini MR, Pellegrini S. Scaling relative incentive value in consummatory behavior. Learning and Motivation 26;37:

9 J.N. Norris et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 223 (211) [51] Wood MD, Norris JN, Daniel AM, Papini MR. Trial-selective effects of U5,488H, a -opioid receptor agonist, on consummatory successive negative contrast. Behavioural Brain Research 28;193: [52] Lubow RE. Conditioned taste aversion and latent inhibition: a review. In: Reilly S, Schachtman TR, editors. Conditioned taste aversion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 29. p [53] Mao SC, Hsiao YH, Gean PW. Extinction training in conjunction with a partial agonist of the glycine site on the NMDA receptor erases memory trace. Journal of Neuroscience 26;26: [54] Hu H, Real E, Takamiya K, Kang MG, Ledoux J, Huganir RL, et al. Emotion enhances learning via norepinephrine regulation of AMPA-receptor trafficking. Cell 27;131: [55] Saul skaya NB, Solov eva NA. Tetrodotoxin-dependent glycine release in the rat nucleus accumbens during correction of feeding behavior. Neuroscience & Behavioral Physiology 25;35:8 9. [56] Genn RF, Ahn S, Phillips AG. Attenuated dopamine efflux in the rat nucleus accumbens during successive negative contrast. Behavioral Neuroscience 24;118: [57] Pecoraro N, Dallman MF. c-fos after incentive shifts, expectancy incredulity and recovery. Behavioral Neuroscience 25;119: [58] Alexander JK, DeVries AC, Kigerl KA, Dahlman JM, Popovich PG. Stress exacerbates neuropathic pain via glucocorticoid and NMDA receptor activation. Brain, Behavior & Immunology 29;23:851 6.

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Anisomycin Disrupts Consummatory Behavior after Incentive Downshift via Conditioned Taste Aversion

Anisomycin Disrupts Consummatory Behavior after Incentive Downshift via Conditioned Taste Aversion International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 14, 1, 71-84, 2014 Printed in Spain. All rights reserved. Copyright 2014 AAC Anisomycin Disrupts Consummatory Behavior after Incentive Downshift

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Effects of housing on consummatory successive negative contrast in rats: wirebottom cages versus polycarbonate tubs

Effects of housing on consummatory successive negative contrast in rats: wirebottom cages versus polycarbonate tubs Effects of housing on consummatory successive negative contrast in rats: wirebottom cages versus polycarbonate tubs Michael Wood, MS 1, Alan M. Daniel, MS 1, Egeenee Daniels, DVM 2 & Mauricio R. Papini,

More information

Opioid Receptors Regulate the Extinction of Pavlovian Fear Conditioning

Opioid Receptors Regulate the Extinction of Pavlovian Fear Conditioning Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 117, No. 6, 1292 1301 0735-7044/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1292 Opioid Receptors Regulate the

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival? Threats are much better reinforcers? Fear is a prime

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival Threats are much better reinforcers Fear is a prime

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on Fear Extinction

Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on Fear Extinction Behavioral Neuroscience 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 123, No. 2, 303 314 0735-7044/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014988 Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on

More information

Abstract. R. Roesler 1, J. Quevedo 1, C. Rodrigues 1, M. Madruga 1, M.R.M. Vianna 1 and M.B.C. Ferreira 2

Abstract. R. Roesler 1, J. Quevedo 1, C. Rodrigues 1, M. Madruga 1, M.R.M. Vianna 1 and M.B.C. Ferreira 2 Brazilian Amygdaloid Journal non-nmda of Medical receptors and Biological and memory Research expression (1999) 32: 349-353 ISSN 0100-879X Short Communication 349 Increased training prevents the impairing

More information

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Hippocampal Pyramidal Neuron of Mice and Rats Figure 1 Open Field Apparatus Open Field Behavior

More information

CHAPTER 8 COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING

CHAPTER 8 COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING CHAPTER 8 COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING A tentative classification of learning phenomena Learning Phenomena General Specific Nonassociative Habituation Short term Long term? Sensitization

More information

Do Rats Consume Ethanol to Regulate a Negative Emotion Induced by a Successive Negative Contrast Procedure?

Do Rats Consume Ethanol to Regulate a Negative Emotion Induced by a Successive Negative Contrast Procedure? Seton Hall University erepository @ Seton Hall Seton Hall University Dissertations and Theses (ETDs) Seton Hall University Dissertations and Theses Spring 5-14-2016 Do Rats Consume Ethanol to Regulate

More information

The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory JERRY W. RUDY University of Colorado, Boulder Sinauer Associates, Inc. Publishers Sunderland, Massachusetts 01375 Table of Contents CHAPTER 1 Introduction: Fundamental

More information

Infusions of AP5 into the basolateral amygdala impair the formation, but not the expression, of step-down inhibitory avoidance

Infusions of AP5 into the basolateral amygdala impair the formation, but not the expression, of step-down inhibitory avoidance Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research (2) 33: 829-834 Amygdaloid NMDA receptors and fear memory ISSN 1-879X 829 Infusions of AP5 into the basolateral amygdala impair the formation, but not

More information

Scaling relative incentive value in consummatory behavior

Scaling relative incentive value in consummatory behavior Learning and Motivation 37 (2006) 357 378 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Scaling relative incentive value in consummatory behavior Mauricio R. Papini a,, Santiago Pellegrini b a Department of Psychology,

More information

Memory enhancement of classical fear conditioning by post-training injections of corticosterone in rats

Memory enhancement of classical fear conditioning by post-training injections of corticosterone in rats Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 81 (2004) 67 74 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynlme Memory enhancement of classical fear conditioning by post-training injections of corticosterone in rats Gabriel K. Hui,

More information

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear Historical views that thought and emotion were processed separately in the brain Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: So far, fear is the best understood What is fear? Dictionary: A feeling of agitation and

More information

The Contribution of the Amygdala to Reward-Related Learning and Extinction

The Contribution of the Amygdala to Reward-Related Learning and Extinction Chapter 14 The Contribution of the Amygdala to Reward-Related Learning and Extinction Rose Chesworth and Laura Corbit Additional information is available at the end of the chapter Abstract There has been

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Amygdala Strengthening of Cortical Memory Representations

Amygdala Strengthening of Cortical Memory Representations Chapter 7 Amygdala Strengthening of Cortical Memory Representations Candice M. Chavez, James L. McGaugh and Norman M. Weinberger Additional information is available at the end of the chapter http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/53677

More information

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet University of Amsterdam Supplementary Figures and Legends Day 1 Day 2 Day 3

More information

Limbic system outline

Limbic system outline Limbic system outline 1 Introduction 4 The amygdala and emotion -history - theories of emotion - definition - fear and fear conditioning 2 Review of anatomy 5 The hippocampus - amygdaloid complex - septal

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

Food restriction: enhancing effects on drug reward and striatal cell signaling

Food restriction: enhancing effects on drug reward and striatal cell signaling Food restriction: enhancing effects on drug reward and striatal cell signaling K.D. Carr Departments of Psychiatry & Pharmacology NYU School of Medicine Common Neural Substrates for Incentive-Motivating

More information

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson, H Djafari Marbini, AJ Toner, TW Robbins, BJ Everitt Departments of

More information

Reward devaluation disrupts latent inhibition in fear conditioning

Reward devaluation disrupts latent inhibition in fear conditioning Reward devaluation disrupts latent inhibition in fear conditioning Luís Gonzalo De la Casa, Auxiliadora Mena, Juán Carlos Ruiz-Salas, Esperanza Quintero & Mauricio R. Papini Learning & Behavior ISSN 1543-4494

More information

Blocked and test-stimulus exposure effects in perceptual learning re-examined

Blocked and test-stimulus exposure effects in perceptual learning re-examined Behavioural Processes xxx (2004) xxx xxx Blocked and test-stimulus exposure effects in perceptual learning re-examined M del Carmen Sanjuan a,, Gumersinda Alonso b, James Byron Nelson b a Universidad del

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala 11 December 1997 Nature 390, 604-607 (1997) Macmillan Publishers Ltd. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala MICHAEL T. ROGAN, URSULA V. STÄUBLI & JOSEPH E. LEDOUX

More information

It has been suggested that the use of anxiolytics is contraindicated

It has been suggested that the use of anxiolytics is contraindicated Effects of D-Cycloserine on Extinction: Translation From Preclinical to Clinical Work Michael Davis, Kerry Ressler, Barbara O. Rothbaum, and Rick Richardson Administration of benzodiazepines or serotonin

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory Systems Neuroscience November 29, 2016 Memory Gabriela Michel http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Forms of memory Different types of learning & memory rely on different brain structures

More information

Behavioural memory reconsolidation of food and fear memories

Behavioural memory reconsolidation of food and fear memories ARTICL Received 11 Apr 211 Accepted 21 Sep 211 Published 18 Oct 211 DOI: 1.138/ncomms1515 Behavioural memory reconsolidation of food and fear memories Charlotte R. Flavell 1, David J. Barber 1 & Jonathan

More information

Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949

Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949 Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras Lecture 5: Memory Consolidation Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949 McGaugh, 2 Squire: retention of TV shows after ECS At least 2 kinds

More information

Supplementary Methods

Supplementary Methods 1 Supplementary Methods Social Preference Test Subjects Seventy-four Long-Evans, male rats served as subjects (S-rats) in the foodpreference test, with 40 assigned to the CXT-Same (CXT-S) Condition and

More information

Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral Amygdala Lesions in Rats?

Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral Amygdala Lesions in Rats? Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 76, 268 283 (2001) doi:10.1006/nlme.2001.4042, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral

More information

Doctor of Philosophy

Doctor of Philosophy i INVESTIGATING REACTIVITY TO INCENTIVE DOWNSHIFT AS A CORRELATED RESPONSE TO SELECTION FOR HIGH ALCOHOL PREFERENCE AND A DETERMINANT OF RASH ACTION AND ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION A Dissertation Submitted to

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioural effects of ketamine in non-stressed and stressed mice. Naive C57BL/6 adult male mice (n=10/group) were given a single dose of saline vehicle or ketamine (3.0 mg/kg,

More information

The vocal expression of feeding motivation and frustration in the domestic laying hen, Gallus gallus domesticus

The vocal expression of feeding motivation and frustration in the domestic laying hen, Gallus gallus domesticus Applied Animal Behaviour Science 69 2000 265 273 www.elsevier.comrlocaterapplanim The vocal expression of feeding motivation and frustration in the domestic laying hen, Gallus gallus domesticus Patrick

More information

Effects of Caffeine on Memory in Rats

Effects of Caffeine on Memory in Rats Western University Scholarship@Western 2015 Undergraduate Awards The Undergraduate Awards 2015 Effects of Caffeine on Memory in Rats Cisse Nakeyar Western University, cnakeyar@uwo.ca Follow this and additional

More information

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION INTRODUCTION Epidemiologic reports indicate that mood disorders in children and adolescents are quite common, with up to 70% of depressed children and adolescents experiencing a recurrence within 5 years

More information

Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and

Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and Gallo Taste Learning and Memory in Aging Milagros Gallo, PhD Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and in humans involves conscious recall. 1 Visual recognition memory is

More information

Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning

Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning PART II NEUROSCIENCE 1996, UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning Rudolf Cardinal St John s College, Cambridge. Supervised by Dr C.H. Bennett and Professor N.J.

More information

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories Liz Miller Molly McGinnis Lindsey Richardson A research thesis submitted in partial completion of PSY451 senior research thesis, at Wofford

More information

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning

Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning Learn Behav (2011) 39:79 86 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0022-x Effect of extended training on generalization of latent inhibition: An instance of perceptual learning Gabriel Rodríguez & Gumersinda Alonso Published

More information

Appetitive Pavlovian goal-tracking memories reconsolidate only under specific conditions

Appetitive Pavlovian goal-tracking memories reconsolidate only under specific conditions Research Appetitive Pavlovian goal-tracking memories reconsolidate only under specific conditions Amy C. Reichelt 1 and Jonathan L.C. Lee School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham

More information

Emotional Hormones and Memory Modulation

Emotional Hormones and Memory Modulation Emotional Hormones and Memory Modulation 933 Emotional Hormones and Memory Modulation J L McGaugh and B Roozendaal, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA ã 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Introduction

More information

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT ACTA NEUROBIOL: EXP. 1988, 48: 117-121 Short communication LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT W. Jeffrey WILSON and Jennifer C. HALL Department of Psychological

More information

What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment. University of Mississippi

What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment. University of Mississippi LT Memory and the Amygdala 1 Running Head: Role of the amygdala in long term memory What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment University of Mississippi LT Memory and the Amygdala

More information

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus Behavioral Neuroscience 1995. Vol. 109, No. 2, 366-370 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/95/$3.00 BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/328/5983/1288/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Induction of Fear Extinction with Hippocampal-Infralimbic BDNF Jamie Peters, Laura M. Dieppa-Perea, Loyda M. Melendez,

More information

TITLE: Glutamate transmission enhancement for treatment of PTSD

TITLE: Glutamate transmission enhancement for treatment of PTSD AD Award Number: W81XWH-8-1-83 TITLE: Glutamate transmission enhancement for treatment of PTSD PRINCIPAL INVESTIGATOR: Victoria Risbrough, Ph.D. CONTRACTING ORGANIZATION: Regents of the University of California

More information

A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors

A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors Learning & Behavior 29, 37 (4), 35-31 doi:1.3758/lb.37.4.35 A learned flavor preference persists despite the extinction of conditioned hedonic reactions to the cue flavors DOMINIC M. DWYER Cardiff University,

More information

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(2),199-203 Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning PETER C. HOLLAND and DESWELL T. FORBES University ofpittsburgh, Pittsburgh,

More information

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories? CASE 49 A 43-year-old woman is brought to her primary care physician by her family because of concerns about her forgetfulness. The patient has a history of Down syndrome but no other medical problems.

More information

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2004, Vol. 30, No. 2, 104 117 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.2.104

More information

Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors and Pavlovian Fear Conditioning: Role of NMDA and Opioid Receptors

Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors and Pavlovian Fear Conditioning: Role of NMDA and Opioid Receptors Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 121, No. 5, 1043 1052 0735-7044/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.5.1043 Temporal-Difference Prediction Errors

More information

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Psicológica (2004), 25, 45-65. Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Matías López*, Raúl Cantora*, and Luis Aguado** 1 * Universidad de

More information

Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place

Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place Preference Stephanie Willis, Jonnique Adjmul, Shabaaz Sandhu, Antoniette M. Maldonado-Devincci, Cheryl Kirsten ABSTRACT The present

More information

Insular Cortex-Amygdala Dialogue During Taste Recognition Memory Formation

Insular Cortex-Amygdala Dialogue During Taste Recognition Memory Formation The highlight for April is by Dr. Federico Bermudez-Rattoni from the Institute of Cellular Physiology, National University of Mexico in Mexico City, Mexico. Dr. Bermudez-Rattoni s work over the past 25

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

Effects of an extinguished CS on competition with another CS

Effects of an extinguished CS on competition with another CS Behavioural Processes 72 (2006) 14 22 Effects of an extinguished CS on competition with another CS Carla H. Bills a, Marsha Dopheide a, Oskar Pineño b, Todd R. Schachtman a, a University of Missouri, USA

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

NEUROBIOLOGY ALCOHOLISM

NEUROBIOLOGY ALCOHOLISM NEUROBIOLOGY ALCOHOLISM THERE HAS BEEN A MAJOR THEORETICAL SHIFT IN MEDICATION DEVELOPMENT IN ALCOHOLISM Driven by animal models of intermittent ethanol administration followed by termination, then access

More information

Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling

Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling Research Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling Kate M. Wassum, 1,2,4 Sean B. Ostlund, 1,2 Bernard W. Balleine, 3

More information

Glutamate Overview. How can one neurotransmitter have so many diverse functions?

Glutamate Overview. How can one neurotransmitter have so many diverse functions? tamate Overview How can one neurotransmitter have so many diverse functions? Darryle Schoepp, Ph.D. Senior Vice President and Franchise Head, Neuroscience Control of Excitability via Amino Acid Neurotransmitters

More information

Molecular mechanisms of memory reconsolidation

Molecular mechanisms of memory reconsolidation Molecular mechanisms of memory reconsolidation Natalie C. Tronson and Jane R. Taylor Abstract Memory reconsolidation has been argued to be a distinct process that serves to maintain, strengthen or modify

More information

Learning and Motivation

Learning and Motivation Learning and Motivation 4 (29) 178 18 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Learning and Motivation journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Context blocking in rat autoshaping: Sign-tracking

More information

Associative learning

Associative learning Introduction to Learning Associative learning Event-event learning (Pavlovian/classical conditioning) Behavior-event learning (instrumental/ operant conditioning) Both are well-developed experimentally

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology

Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology xxx (2009) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yfrne Review Glucocorticoids and the

More information

Instructions for use

Instructions for use Title A neuroeconomic theory of bidirecti addiction Author(s) Takahashi, Taiki Citation edical Hypotheses, 75(4): 356-358 Issue Date 2010-10 Doc URLhttp://hdl.handle.net/2115/44260 Right Type article (author

More information

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman BIOMED 509 Executive Control Cambridge University Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan UNM SOM JL Brigman 4-7-17 Symptoms and Assays of Cognitive Disorders Symptoms of Cognitive Disorders Learning & Memory

More information

Science & Technologies COMPARISON THE EFFECTS OF TACRINE AND GALANTAMINE ON ACTIVE AVOIDANCE TEST IN RATS WITH DIAZEPAM-AMNESIA MODEL

Science & Technologies COMPARISON THE EFFECTS OF TACRINE AND GALANTAMINE ON ACTIVE AVOIDANCE TEST IN RATS WITH DIAZEPAM-AMNESIA MODEL COMPARISON THE EFFECTS OF TACRINE AND GALANTAMINE ON ACTIVE AVOIDANCE TEST IN RATS WITH DIAZEPAM-AMNESIA MODEL Darinka Dimitrova, Damianka Getova Medical University Plovdiv, Medical Faculty, 4002 Plovdiv,

More information

The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties

The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties The Form of a Conditioned Stimulus Can Influence the Degree to Which It Acquires Incentive Motivational Properties Paul J. Meyer*, Elizabeth S. Cogan, Terry E. Robinson Department of Psychology, University

More information

Insular Cortex and Consummatory Successive Negative Contrast in the Rat

Insular Cortex and Consummatory Successive Negative Contrast in the Rat Butler University Digital Commons @ Butler University Scholarship and Professional Work COPHS College of Pharmacy & Health Sciences 2009 Insular Cortex and Consummatory Successive Negative Contrast in

More information

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(3),363-367 An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock WLLAM C. GORDON and ROBERT R. MOWRER University ofnew Mexico, Albuquerque, New

More information

Traumatic events such as military combat, motor vehicle

Traumatic events such as military combat, motor vehicle Recent fear is resistant to extinction Stephen Maren* and Chun-hui Chang* *Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Program, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1043 Communicated by Richard F.

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Nat Neurosci. 2006 August ; 9(8): 1004 1006. Maternal presence serves as a switch between learning fear and attraction in infancy

More information

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING Pergamon Neuroscience Vol. 83, No. 3, pp. 837 841, 1998 Copyright 1998 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved PII: S0306-4522(97)00457-0 0306 4522/98 $19.00+0.00

More information

MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION. Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre

MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION. Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre 1 MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre Reward 2 Humans, as well as other organisms engage in behaviours that are rewarding The pleasurable feelings provide positive reinforcement

More information

TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE. The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in

TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE. The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in 1 NAME COURSE TITLE 2 TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in dopamine neurotransmitter, up-regulation

More information

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1989, Vol. 15, No. 3, 232-241 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-740389$00.75 Contextual Effects in Conditioning,

More information

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University Undergraduate Honors Theses 5-2014 Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? Tarra L. Baker Follow

More information

Introduction the basics of psychological learning and memory theory. From Mechanisms of Memory by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Introduction the basics of psychological learning and memory theory. From Mechanisms of Memory by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Introduction the basics of psychological learning and memory theory. From Mechanisms of Memory by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Definitions Learning: The acquisition of an altered behavioral response due to an

More information

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning

Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (2), 213-219 Extinction and retraining of simultaneous and successive flavor conditioning THOMAS HIGGINS and ROBERT A. RESCORLA University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

More information

Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior. James L. McClelland Stanford University

Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior. James L. McClelland Stanford University Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior James L. McClelland Stanford University A Playwright s Take on Memory What interests me a great deal is the mistiness of the past Harold Pinter,

More information

The contribution of latent inhibition to reduced generalization after pre-exposure to the test stimulus

The contribution of latent inhibition to reduced generalization after pre-exposure to the test stimulus Behavioural Processes 71 (2006) 21 28 The contribution of latent inhibition to reduced generalization after pre-exposure to the test stimulus Maria del Carmen Sanjuan a,, Gumersinda Alonso b, James Byron

More information

Authors: Kyle Z. Pasquariello, Marina Han, Cagla Unal, Paul J. Meyer. Behavioural Brain Research

Authors: Kyle Z. Pasquariello, Marina Han, Cagla Unal, Paul J. Meyer. Behavioural Brain Research Accepted Manuscript Title: Adrenergic Manipulation Inhibits Pavlovian Conditioned Approach Behaviors Authors: Kyle Z. Pasquariello, Marina Han, Cagla Unal, Paul J. Meyer PII: S0166-4328(17)31334-7 DOI:

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) Classical vs. Instrumental The modern view is that these two types of learning involve similar

More information

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Behavioural Brain Research 110 (2000) 97 108 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Stephen Maren *, William Holt Department of Psychology

More information

ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon Limbic System II. David P. Wolfer MD

ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon Limbic System II. David P. Wolfer MD ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon 05.05.2014 Limbic System II David P. Wolfer MD Institute of Anatomy, University of Zurich Institute for Human Movement Sciences and Sport, ETH Zurich http://www.dpwolfer.ch

More information

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR LICK-SHOCK CONTINGENCIES IN THE RATT1 VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY 1972, 17, 119-125 NUMBER I (JANUARY) Hungry rats were allowed to lick an 8%

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

J. A. Gray s reinforcement sensitivity theory and frustrative nonreward: a theoretical note on expectancies in reactions to rewarding stimuli

J. A. Gray s reinforcement sensitivity theory and frustrative nonreward: a theoretical note on expectancies in reactions to rewarding stimuli Personality and Individual Differences 32 (2002) 1247 1253 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid J. A. Gray s reinforcement sensitivity theory and frustrative nonreward: a theoretical note on expectancies in reactions

More information

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats Physiology & Behavior 76 (2002) 117 129 The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats Peter C. Holland a, *, Gorica D. Petrovich b, Michela Gallagher b a Department

More information