Oculomotor control and the maintenance of spatially and temporally distributed events in visuo-spatial working memory

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Oculomotor control and the maintenance of spatially and temporally distributed events in visuo-spatial working memory"

Transcription

1 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 56A (7), Oculomotor control and the maintenance of spatially and temporally distributed events in visuo-spatial working memory David G. Pearson and Arash Sahraie University of Aberdeen, Scotland, UK Previous studies have demonstrated that working memory for spatial location can be significantly disrupted by concurrent eye or limb movement (Baddeley, 1986; Smyth, Pearson, & Pendleton, 1988). Shifts in attention alone can also interfere with spatial span (Smyth & Scholey, 1994), even with no corresponding movement of the eyes or limbs (Smyth, 1996). What is not clear from these studies is how comparable is the magnitude of effect caused by different forms of spatial disrupter. Recently, it has been demonstrated that limb movements produce as much interference with spatial span as do reflexive saccades (Lawrence, Myerson, Oonk, & Abrams, 2001). In turn this has led to the hypothesis that all spatially directed movement can produce similar effects in visuospatial working memory. This paper reports the results of five experiments that have contrasted the effect of concurrent eye movement, limb movement, and covert attention shifts on participants working memory for sequences of locations. All conditions involving concurrent eye movement produced significantly greater reduction in span than equivalent limb movement or covert attention shifts with eyes fixated. It is argued that these results demonstrate a crucial role for oculomotor control processes during the rehearsal of location-specific representations in working memory. In the case of the verbal working memory there is a well-documented account of rehearsal in the form of an active articulatory mechanism that revives auditory memory traces stored within a temporary phonological store (Baddeley, 1986, 2000). In contrast, accounts of rehearsal in visuo-spatial working memory are considerably less well developed (Pearson, 2001). Potential candidates for rehearsal in the visuo-spatial domain are processes associated with eye movement, which were initially investigated in a series of studies carried out by Idzikowski, Baddeley, Dimberly, and Park (reported in Baddeley, 1986). They examined the effect on working memory for spatial and verbal material of asking participants to track a sinusoidally moving target with their eyes. They found that eye movements produced a highly significant decline in spatial working memory in contrast to a verbal control and a condition in Requests for reprints should be sent to David G. Pearson, Vision Research Laboratories, Department of Psychology, William Guild Building, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, AB24 2UB, Scotland, UK. d.g.pearson@abdn.ac.uk 2003 The Experimental Psychology Society DOI: /

2 1090 PEARSON AND SAHRAIE which the background to the target moved but the eyes remained stationary. In addition, the interference effect occurred at both encoding and/or retrieval of the spatial material. On the basis of these results Baddeley suggested that rehearsal in visuo-spatial working memory could occur via an active control process linked to oculomotor control that refreshed visuospatial material represented in a passive perceptual input store, in a structure that mirrored the relationship between the phonological store and articulatory loop that had been proposed for verbal working memory. However, other forms of concurrent body movement have also been shown to produce interference in spatial working memory, including sequential tapping of keys (Logie & Marchetti, 1991; Pearson, Logie, & Gilhooly, 1999; Smyth, Pearson, & Pendleton, 1988; Smyth & Pendleton, 1989) and arm movements across an unseen matrix (Quinn, 1991; Quinn & Ralston, 1986). In addition, Johnson (1982) found that simply asking participants to imagine making an arm movement disrupted temporary memory for location, suggesting that the planning of movement was sufficient to produce interference irrespective of whether the movement was actually executed. A possible overlap between spatial rehearsal and the cognitive processes involved during the planning and production of movement has been proposed by Logie (1995), who implicates a spatial inner scribe mechanism in the extraction of information from a visual cache to allow for targeted movement. In Logie s model of visuospatial working memory the production of physical movement, or even just planned production, should interfere with any task that also requires the operation of the inner scribe, including the retention of sequential locations or movements (Logie, 1995; pp ). However, the extent to which actual explicit or implicit motor processes underlie the operation of spatial working memory, particularly regarding the temporary rehearsal of sequences of spatial locations, remains controversial. An alternative explanation is that spatial working memory is disrupted by shifts in spatial attention caused by eye or hand movements rather than the movements themselves. A key study carried out by Smyth and Scholey (1994) demonstrated that spatial recall span was significantly impaired if participants were exposed to visual or auditory signals that appeared in different spatial locations, and that this impairment was increased if they had to point to the location of the signal or make left/right categorical judgements. A follow-up study that controlled for eye movements verified that shifts in spatial attention alone could produce significant disruption of spatial span in the absence of any overt movement of the eyes or limbs (Smyth, 1996). Smyth and colleagues interpreted these results as providing evidence that maintenance of location in spatial working memory involves shifts in spatial attention rather than implicit motor processes. A more detailed theory of spatial rehearsal has since been proposed by Awh and colleagues (Awh, Anllo-Vento, & Hillyard, 2000; Awh & Jonides, 2001). They have argued that spatial rehearsal involves a frontal-parietal system that actively maintains spatial information by means of focal shifts of spatial attention to memorized locations held in working memory. Awh, Jonides, and Reuter-Lorenz (1998) found that when participants were asked to hold a location in working memory this facilitated the visual processing of stimuli appearing in that location relative to stimuli appearing in nonmemorized locations. They also found that disrupting the focus of spatial location during a retention interval significantly impaired participants accuracy on a spatial memory task in comparison to a condition in which the focus of attention was static. On the basis of these results Awh and colleagues have argued that the process of visual selection is a key component of rehearsal in spatial

3 OCULOMOTOR CONTROL AND SPATIAL WORKING MEMORY 1091 working memory, with the cognitive mechanisms involved in spatial selective attention also being used to provide functional markers for location-specific representations in working memory (Awh & Jonides, 2001). Importantly, their account of spatial rehearsal assumes a supramodal basis for attention, in which covert shifts of attention involve processes that are functionally independent from those that mediate oculomotor programming (Awh et al., 1998, 2000). However, other evidence suggests that prefrontal executive processes responsible for the control of attention may be involved in the initiation of planned saccades to a greater extent than a supramodal theory might predict. Roberts, Hager, and Heron (1994) have shown that concurrent mental arithmetic interferes with the execution of antisaccades, but does not appear to interfere with reflexive prosaccades to similar targets. However, a study by Stuyven et al. found that prosaccades could also be vulnerable to interference from a concurrent executive task if the saccades were controlled rather than automatic, suggesting that prefrontal executive processes could be involved in controlled saccade execution across a wider range of tasks than just antisaccades (Stuyven, Van der Goten, Vandierendonck, Claeys, & Crevits, 2000). It is clear from the published literature on spatial working memory that the temporary maintenance of location can be disrupted by a wide range of different secondary tasks, including eye movements, limb movements, and shifts of spatial attention that occur in the absence of eye or limb movements. What is less clear, however, is how comparable these different forms of spatial disrupter are. The magnitude of the different effects could be the same, or alternatively one form of secondary task could produce much higher levels of disruption. In addition, the extent to which the interference caused by different procedures is due to a common cognitive mechanism is unclear. For example, the disruption caused by concurrent eye and limb movement could be entirely due to related shifts in spatial attention, with the processes involved during the control of the movement being largely irrelevant. Some of these issues have been addressed in a recent study carried out by Lawrence, Myerson, Oonk, and Abrams (2001) who examined the effect of saccadic eye movement, limb movement, and saccade inhibition on memory span for spatial locations. They found that limb movements that were performed while maintaining fixation produced as much interference with spatial span as did reflexive saccades, and they concluded from this that the interference caused by eye movement was not due to its visual consequences. Instead Lawrence et al. argue that all spatially directed movement produces similar effects on visuospatial working memory, irrespective of the type of movement being executed. They speculate that the interference caused by spatially directed movements can be due either to interruption of a rehearsal sequence such as a positive feedback loop proposed to link the prefrontal and posterior cortices (Chafee & Goldman-Rakic, 2000; Fuster, 1995), or alternatively to disruption of an attention-based rehearsal process such as that put forward by Awh and colleagues (Awh & Jonides, 2001; Awh et al., 1998). However, there are some aspects of the procedure adopted by Lawrence et al. (2001) that may have mitigated against finding differential effects of eye and limb movement on spatial span. They used a procedure in which the presentation of locations in the primary task is interleaved with performance of the secondary task that is, the first location is presented, participants execute a single reflexive saccade, the second location is presented, another single saccade is executed, and so on. A similar procedure was used for limb movement, in which participants perform single key presses interleaved with presentation of the locations in the

4 1092 PEARSON AND SAHRAIE primary task. Such a procedure has been rarely used in the majority of other studies that have examined visuo-spatial working memory, in which performance of the secondary task is typically confined to a retention interval that occurs after the encoding stage (Baddeley, 1986; Logie, 1995). This has the advantage of allowing more precise interpretation of any effects of intervening task on encoding and retention processes. A single eye or limb movement may not produce comparable effects to those when a succession of movements has to be generated. The interleaving technique may allow participants to carry out covert rehearsal, as can occur with the effects of articulatory suppression on verbal span if the rate of articulation is too slow (Baddeley, 1986). In addition, the interleaving of primary and secondary tasks may cause general interference at the executive level that may contribute to masking differential effects of eye and limb movements. The current paper presents a series of experiments that have attempted to comprehensively determine the comparative effects of eye movements, covert attention shifts with maintained fixation, and limb movements, on memory span for sequentially presented spatial locations. The main objective of the study was to compare the level of interference caused by saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movement with that caused by discrete and continuous covert attention shifts. Although previous studies (i.e., Awh et al., 1998; Smyth & Scholey, 1994) have demonstrated that covert shifts in attention alone can significantly disrupt spatial memory, it is unclear whether the magnitude of this disruption is equivalent in behavioural terms to that caused by concurrent eye or limb movements. It is also unclear whether smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movements can be treated as equivalent in terms of the levels of interference produced in spatial memory (Idzihowski et al., in Baddeley, 1986; Hale, Myerson, Rhee, Weiss, & Abrams, 1996; Lawrence et al., 2001; Quinn & McConnell, 1996). Although it might be expected that pursuit and saccadic movements produce similar attentional demands, functional imaging studies have demonstrated differences between the levels of activity generated by the two types of eye movement within the frontal eye fields (Petit, Clark, Ingeholm, & Haxby, 1997). It has also been shown that accuracy of pointing to justextinguished visual targets is strongly influenced by the type of eye movement (saccadic or pursuit) that is made to the target (Honda, 1984). This could result in differences in the behavioural effects of pursuit and saccadic movements on spatial recall span. Comparison of the effect of different types of secondary task on spatial span can be seen as an essential step in understanding the cognitive mechanisms that underlie temporary memory for spatial location. Although eye movement has been shown to be related to spatial memory, the exact nature of this relationship is unclear, and there is a need for the relative effects of eye movements, limb movements, and covert attention shifts on spatial span to be clearly demonstrated. In an attempt to address these issues five experiments were carried out using a repeated measures design. Participants GENERAL METHOD FOR EXPERIMENTS 1 5 The use of repeated measures in which participants act as their own control provides the most effective means to examine the comparative effects of different secondary tasks, as differential strategy use or other confounding factors are considerably reduced relative to a mixed or between-subjects design. In

5 OCULOMOTOR CONTROL AND SPATIAL WORKING MEMORY 1093 light of this it was decided that the same volunteers should participate in all five planned experiments. The experiments were carried out separately over a 15-month period in the order reported, with each testing session lasting 2 3 hours per participant. The participants were 12 right-handed females with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The age range was 22 to 43 years (mean age years). All participants were postgraduate students at the University of Aberdeen who took part in the experiments on a voluntary basis. Informed consent was obtained from all the participants, and ethical approval was obtained from the Departmental Ethics Committee at the University of Aberdeen. Apparatus and method Participants spatial span was measured using an irregular array of nine blocks based on the Corsi Blocks test (De Renzi & Nichelli, 1975), and modified for display on a PC monitor. The method allowed the presentation of spatially distributed and temporally separated events, with each event consisting of a transient change in luminance of a block from grey to black. Participants viewed binocularly, a uniform grey background (26 cd/m 2 ) from a viewing distance of 57 cm. A headrest was used to reduce irrelevant movement. The monitor screen measured 37 cm (w) 26 cm (h), and the Corsi blocks display (28 cm 21 cm) was positioned centrally on the uniform field. Each block measured 3.6 cm square and was defined by a 2-mm border. For each trial the sequence of events was as follows: The array of blocks appeared, and participants fixated on a cross positioned at the centre of the screen. The series of transient visual events was denoted by a momentary change of square luminance to 0.65 cd/m 2 for 250 ms followed by return to the background luminance for the same period. The selection of blocks to be highlighted was carried out randomly by the computer with the restriction that no block was highlighted twice in each trial. At the end of the sequence the blocks disappeared signalling the start of a 5-s retention interval. The reappearance of the blocks signalled the end of the retention interval, which was also signalled by a short audio tone. Participants recalled the sequence of presented events by pointing and pressing the mouse key on previously highlighted blocks in an identical order to that of presentation. A summary of these events is given in Figure 1. The recall of the sequence always took place with no restrictions on eye movements. To ensure compliance with the experimental instructions both horizontal and vertical eye movements were monitored and recorded using electro-oculograms (EOG). Some examples of EOG traces from different experimental conditions are illustrated in Figure 2. The EOG signals were displayed on a separate computer screen throughout the testing session. Calibration of exact position of gaze was accomplished by the measurement of the EOG signals when participants sequentially fixated on four calibration points on the screen at the start of the experimental run. Accurate and consistent measurement of spatial span was integral to the investigations reported here. Span was calculated using a staircase procedure adapted from previously published studies (Hulme, Maughan, & Brown, 1991; Logie, Della Sala, Laiacona, Chalmers, & Wynn, 1996; Logie & Pearson, 1997). We also took multiple estimates of span for each condition to reduce any confounding effects produced by the random highlighting of blocks for each trial (Berch, Krikorian, & Huha, 1998; Kemps, 1999; Smirni, Villardita, & Zappala, 1983). At the start of each condition, participants carried out a series of practice trials to ensure understanding and compliance with the experimental instructions. Once performance was judged to be stable they continued with five separate runs for every condition. For each experimental run trials were blocked in groups of three. In the first block only two sequential transient events were presented per trial. Following the retention interval the participant reported the position and order of the visual events. If at least two out of the three trials were correctly identified then an additional block was presented on each trial. The number of transient visual events was increased sequentially in the consecutive blocks, and the experimental run was terminated if more than two trials were reported incorrectly in any one block.

6 1094 PEARSON AND SAHRAIE Figure 1. Diagram of screenshots depicting the order and timing of events in a two-event trial in the baseline condition used for Experiments 1 to 5. Events run in direction of arrow and represent (1) initial presentation of array of blocks, (2) presentation of first event indicated by transient change in luminance of block (250 ms), (3) presentation of second event (interstimulus time of 250 ms), (4) 5-s retention interval with presentation of uniform field with fixation point (screenshots from retention intervals in continuous pursuit, Experiment 1, and visual noise, Experiment 3, conditions are also illustrated), (5) block array reappears and participants indicate position and order of presented locations using the PC mouse. The span for an individual experimental run was calculated as the average of the number of events per trial in the last three correctly identified trials. The participant s span for a condition was then calculated as the average of the results from the last four out of five experimental runs recorded (the first was treated as a practice run and not included in the analysis). The control condition for each of the five experimental sessions was when the participant fixated on a fixation crosshair placed on a grey background during the retention interval. Each experiment started by baseline measurements followed by two or three experimental conditions. The order of presentation of conditions was counterbalanced across participants. EXPERIMENT 1 Effects on spatial span of smooth pursuit eye movements and continuous attention shifts with maintained fixation The first experiment examined the effect on spatial span of smooth pursuit eye movements during the retention interval, compared with covert attention shifts with maintained fixation. Smooth pursuit eye movements have been shown previously to disrupt spatial memory (Baddeley, 1986). It has also been demonstrated that attention shifts with maintained fixation

7 OCULOMOTOR CONTROL AND SPATIAL WORKING MEMORY 1095 Figure 2. Examples of horizontal EOG traces for all eye movement conditions, as well as baseline and no eye movement with eyes shut conditions. also disrupt spatial memory (Awh et al., 1998; Smyth, 1996). The aim of the experiment was to compare the magnitude of these two effects against each other. Method Secondary tasks In the smooth pursuit condition participants were asked to track an oscillating ball with sinusoidally modulated speed moving in the centre of the screen. The ball appeared in the same position as the fixation point at the beginning of the retention interval and moved right or left on a randomly determined basis. Participants tracked the ball continuously over the duration of the 5-s retention interval. The ball took one second to move from one side of the screen to the other. In the attention shift with maintained fixation condition the ball moved in an identical fashion at the top of the screen. The crosshair fixation point was present throughout the retention interval. Participants were instructed to maintain fixation while focusing their attention on the ball at the top of the screen. A screenshot from the pursuit movement condition is presented in Figure 1. Participants EOG traces were monitored continuously during both conditions to ensure compliance with the experimental instructions. Any deviation from the instructions would result in the given trial being re-run. The advantage of this procedure was that it explicitly prevented participants from protecting primary task performance by trading off performance in the secondary task.

8 1096 PEARSON AND SAHRAIE Results and discussion Mean spans in each condition were 5.09 (SD = 0.85) for baseline, 3.43 (SD = 1.19) for pursuit, and 4.29 (SD = 0.88) for continuous attention shifts. Participants compliance with experimental instructions was excellent, and it was not necessary to omit or re-run any trials. The results were analysed by a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) that revealed a main effect of condition, F(2, 22) = 33.22, p <.001. Post hoc comparisons by Tukey HSD tests revealed that all differences were significant at the.001 level. The results of the first experiment show that smooth pursuit eye movement during a retention interval produces significantly greater disruption in spatial span than do continuous attention shifts carried out while the eyes remain fixated. However, unlike the smooth pursuit condition there was no objective measure of participants compliance with experimental instructions in the attention shift condition. Despite the fact that all of the participants indicated that they had complied with the experimental instructions, it remained a possibility that on some or all of the trials they may not have attended to the moving target. Experiment 2 addressed this issue by repeating the procedure of Experiment 1, but with the addition of requiring participants to monitor and respond to colour changes of the target in both pursuit and attention shift conditions. EXPERIMENT 2 Effects on spatial span of smooth pursuit eye movements and continuous attention shifts with additional monitoring and report of target colour changes Method Secondary tasks The secondary tasks used in Experiment 1 were modified to include random colour changes of the target from white to red. Each colour change lasted 200 ms, with up to three changes occurring over the 5-s retention interval. There was at least a 750-ms delay between target changes. Participants were instructed to make a mouse click as quickly as possible each time a colour change occurred. These responses were recorded to allow a measure of participants compliance with the experimental instructions in the continuous attention shift only condition. All other aspects of the procedure were identical to those of Experiment 1. Results and discussion Mean spans in each condition were 4.99 (SD = 0.49) for baseline, 3.20 (SD = 0.96) for pursuit, and 3.87 (SD = 1.05) for continuous attention shifts. It was not necessary to re-run or omit any trials. The results were analysed by a one-way ANOVA that revealed a main effect of condition, F(2, 22) = 28.01, p <.001. Post hoc Tukey comparisons revealed that all differences were significant at the.02 level. Mean accuracy of detecting target colour changes in the attention shift condition was 98.66% and 100% in the smooth pursuit condition, confirming that participants were attending to the moving target as instructed. The classification of errors included false alarms as well as failure to respond to a target change.

9 OCULOMOTOR CONTROL AND SPATIAL WORKING MEMORY 1097 These results replicate the finding reported in Experiment 1, in which smooth pursuit eye movement during a retention interval produced significantly greater disruption of spatial span than continuous attention shifts carried out with the eyes fixated. Participants high level of accuracy for responding to target changes in the continuous attention shift condition demonstrates that this difference cannot be explained by a failure to comply with the experimental instructions. EXPERIMENT 3 Effects on spatial span of articulatory suppression and dynamic visual noise The results of the previous two experiments have shown that continuous eye movement produces significantly greater disruption of spatial span than do continuous covert shifts of attention with eyes fixated. What is not clear, however, is whether the eye movements themselves are a crucial factor in producing these results. An alternative explanation is that any requirement of participants to produce a rapid concurrent response will lead to a significant reduction in span. In order to address this, a third experiment was carried out in which participants carried out articulatory suppression during the retention interval. Articulatory suppression is a widely used technique for disrupting the operation of verbal working memory (Andrade, 2001; Baddeley, 1986), but it is believed to have a minimal effect on visuo-spatial working memory (Logie, 1995; Smyth et al., 1988). Therefore, if continuous eye movement only disrupts the operation of spatial working memory, it can be predicted that there should be no comparable effect produced by concurrent articulatory suppression. A second alternative to eye movement as the cause of disruption is that movement of the eye produces a change in the retinal image. This change alone could reduce span, either by a masking effect (Sperling, 1967; Turvey, 1973), or perhaps by potentially disrupting the operation of a cognitive retention strategy based on visual mental imagery (Andrade, Kemps, Werniers, May, & Szmalec, 2002; Smyth & Pendleton, 1989). In order to investigate this a secondary task condition was added in which a rapidly changing visual noise display filled the screen during the retention interval while participants maintained fixation. In addition to acting as a perceptual mask, dynamic visual displays of this type have also been shown to significantly disrupt tasks that rely on the formation of mental images (McConnell & Quinn, 2000; Quinn & McConnell, 1996; Smyth & Waller, 1998). Method Secondary tasks Articulatory suppression consisted of participants repeating the word go once every half a second, following procedures previously reported in the literature (i.e., Baddeley, 1986; Murray, 1968). The audio signals from a metronome were used to demonstrate to participants the correct rate at which to perform the suppression during the practice trials. No audio cue was provided during the experiments, and the experimenters used the visual signals from the metronome to ensure participant compliance. Any trials in which performance of the secondary task fell below 100% were re-run. Participants were instructed to begin suppression immediately at the start of the retention interval and to perform it continuously throughout the 5-s duration.

10 1098 PEARSON AND SAHRAIE The dynamic visual noise condition was based on previous displays reported in the literature (Pearson et al., 1999; Quinn & McConnell, 1996). During the retention interval the screen was filled with a display of approximately 2924 black and white circles per second (see Figure 1). The display ran at a frame rate of 85 Hz, with approximately 34 new items appearing per frame at random locations excluding the fixation crosshair. The fixation point was visible throughout, and participants were instructed to maintain fixation and ignore the changing display as much as possible. In all other respects the procedure for both secondary task conditions was identical to that for the baseline condition described previously. Performance of the secondary tasks was monitored by the experimenters throughout testing to ensure compliance with the experimental instructions and prevent any primary task/secondary task trade-off. Results and discussion Mean spans in each condition were 4.95 (SD = 0.57) for baseline, 4.85 (SD = 0.98) for articulatory suppression, and 5.01 (SD = 0.67) for dynamic visual noise. It was not necessary to re-run or omit any trials. The results were analysed by a one-way ANOVA that revealed no significant effect of condition (F <1,ns). Neither concurrent articulatory suppression during the retention interval nor the presence of dynamic visual noise led to any significant reduction in span in comparison to the baseline condition. The results of Experiment 3 suggest that the reduction in spatial span caused by continuous eye movement cannot be accounted for simply in terms of the demands caused by producing a regular response, or by changes in the retinal image during the retention interval. The absence of any significant effect by suppression and visual noise also suggests that participants are not relying on a visual imagery strategy or verbal recoding in order to remember the spatial sequences (Avons & Mason, 1999; Smyth & Pendleton, 1989). EXPERIMENT 4 Effects on spatial span of saccadic eye movements, discrete attention shifts, and limb movements The previous experiments have shown that continuous eye movements produce significantly greater reduction in spatial span than do equivalent covert attention shifts with eyes fixated. However, it is not clear whether other forms of eye movement can also produce this effect. As discussed in the Introduction, functional imaging studies have demonstrated differences in underlying neuronal mechanisms mediating saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements (Petit et al., 1997). Differences between the different types of eye movement have also been shown for pointing accuracy to just-extinguished visual targets (Honda, 1984). Therefore, it is of interest to investigate whether the saccadic eye movements can produce the same effect on spatial span as can continuous eye movements. In order to investigate this, a fourth experiment was carried out in which continuous eye movements and attention shifts were replaced by saccadic eye movements and equivalent discrete attention shifts with the eyes fixated. A third secondary task was added in the form of concurrent limb movement in which participants tapped between two buttons using their preferred hand. According to Lawrence et al. (2001) all spatially directed movement should produce similar effects in spatial working

11 memory, irrespective of whether the eyes or limbs are being moved. This experiment allows a direct test of this hypothesis. If Lawrence et al. (2001) are correct, then it should be expected that there would be no significant difference in the level of disruption caused by saccadic eye movement and limb movement. Alternatively, if it is the case that eye movements produce a unique form of interference in spatial working memory, then it can be predicted that the saccadic eye movement condition will produce a significant or even a greater reduction in span than either concurrent limb movements or covert discrete shifts of attention. Method Secondary tasks In the saccadic eye movement condition participants were asked to track a ball that appeared alternating in position left and right of the screen. The ball appeared in the same position as the fixation point at the beginning of the retention interval and then appeared on the right or left on a randomly determined basis. Participants were instructed to keep their eyes focused on the ball throughout the duration of the retention interval. As soon as the ball disappeared from one side of the screen and reappeared on the other, participants had to shift their eyes as quickly as possible to the new position. The ball s movement mimicked the amplitude and frequency of the continuous tracking condition used in Experiments 1 and 2. The discrete attention shift condition was identical to the saccadic movement condition, except that the crosshair fixation point was present throughout the retention interval. Participants were instructed to maintain fixation while focusing their attention on the ball as it alternated position between the left and right of the screen. In both the saccadic and discrete attention shift conditions participants were required to monitor and report changes in the target colour from white to red as described in the procedure for Experiment 2. In the limb movement condition two blocks were positioned in front of the computer monitor at the same spatial separation as the amplitude of the moving ball in the saccadic and attention shift conditions. Participants were instructed to tap between the two blocks with their preferred hand at the same frequency as the moving ball in the other conditions. As in Experiment 3, a metronome was used to aid participants in establishing the correct rate of movement, but this was not audible during the actual experimental trials. During the retention interval the monitor screen remained blank except for the fixation point. Participants were required to maintain fixation throughout performance of the limb movements to ensure that any interference caused by the tapping was not due to extraneous visual processing. As in the previous experiments, participants EOG traces were monitored continuously during both conditions to ensure compliance with the experimental instructions. The experimenters also monitored participants secondary task performance in the limb movement condition using a light metronome (which was not visible to participants). Any deviation from the instructions or fall in secondary task performance from 100% would result in the given trial being re-run. Results and discussion OCULOMOTOR CONTROL AND SPATIAL WORKING MEMORY 1099 Mean spans in each condition were 5.21 (SD = 0.57) for baseline, 2.52 (SD = 1.20) for saccadic, 4.20 (SD = 0.81) for limb movement, and 3.82 (SD = 0.90) for discrete attention shifts. It was not necessary to re-run or omit any trials. The results were analysed by a one-way ANOVA that revealed a main effect of condition, F(3,33) = 41.05; p <.001. Post hoc Tukey comparisons revealed that there was no significant difference between the discrete attention shift and limb movement conditions (p >.41). All other comparisons were significant at the

12 1100 PEARSON AND SAHRAIE.001 level. Mean accuracy of detecting target colour changes in both the discrete attention shift and saccadic conditions was 100%, confirming that participants were attending to the moving target as instructed. These results show that saccadic eye movements performed during a retention interval produce significantly greater reduction in span than do either discrete covert attention shifts or limb movements that are performed at the same amplitude and frequency as the eye movements. These findings do not support the claim made by Lawrence et al. (2001) that all spatially directed movement produces similar effects in visuo-spatial working memory. However, the absence of any significant difference between the discrete attention shift and limb movement conditions provides support for the hypothesis that the disruption caused by limb movement is due to related shifts in attention rather performance of the movement itself (Awh & Jonides, 2001; Awh et al., 1998; Lawrence et al., 2001; Smyth, 1996). EXPERIMENT 5 Effects on spatial span of saccadic eye movement with eyes closed, eyes closed with no movement, and free eye movement conditions The previous experiments have established that concurrent eye movement during a retention interval produces significantly greater reduction in spatial span than do comparable conditions in which limb movement or attention shifts are performed with the eyes fixated. However, it remains uncertain as to the extent to which the eye movements themselves are responsible for this effect rather than a related factor. The use of dynamic visual noise in Experiment 3 established that changes to the retinal image during the retention interval did not lead to a reduction in span. What is not clear, however, is whether eye movements performed without any corresponding shift in retinal image will still lead to a reduction in spatial span. To investigate this, the final experiment required participants to execute saccadic movements with their eyes closed during the retention interval. In addition to removing any shift in retinal image as the eyes move, this condition also had the advantage that the eye movements were not executed in response to visual targets. A large number of studies have demonstrated that shifts of attention precede saccadic eye movements to visual targets (Shepherd, Findlay, & Hockey, 1986; Chelazzi, Biscaldi, Corbetta, Peru, & Berlucchi, 1995). In the present experiment the saccades were self-generated by the participant and were not directed to specific targets in their visual field. Of course, requiring participants to shut their eyes during the retention interval might in itself disrupt performance. To control for this a condition was added in which participants were instructed to close their eyes but to attempt to keep them as still as possible. The final condition in the experiment was one in which the fixation point was removed, and participants were allowed to make any voluntary eye movements they wished during the retention interval. As Baddeley (1986) has suggested that participants may use overt eye movement as a rehearsal mechanism to maintain sequences of locations, requiring participants to fixate during the retention interval might in itself lead to a reduction in spatial span. The final condition was designed to test this.

13 Method Secondary tasks OCULOMOTOR CONTROL AND SPATIAL WORKING MEMORY 1101 In the saccadic movement with eyes closed condition participants were initially trained using a metronome to make saccades with the same frequency as that used in the saccadic movement condition in the previous experiment. EOG signals were monitored to ensure that participants executed the eye movements correctly. During the actual experimental trials the metronome was not present, and participants initiated each saccade themselves, with EOG signal monitoring ensuring that the correct number of saccades were carried out for each trial. Participants were instructed to close their eyes and begin saccadic movement as soon as the retention interval began, and to continue this until the short audio tone indicated the end of the retention interval. The second condition was identical to the first, except that participants were instructed to keep their eyes as still as possible during the retention interval. Again, the EOG signals were monitored to ensure compliance with these instructions. Finally, in the free eye movement condition the crosshair fixation point was removed during the retention interval, leaving a blank screen. Participants were told that they were free to make any eye movements they wished during the interval. However, EOG signals were still recorded so as to provide an indication of how participants responded to these instructions. All other aspects of the procedure were the same as those in the previous experiments. Results and discussion Mean spans in each condition were 5.30 (SD = 0.67) for baseline, 3.13 (SD = 1.13) for saccades with eyes closed, 4.93 (SD = 0.51) for eyes closed with no movement, and 5.06 (SD = 0.61) for free eye movement. It was not necessary to re-run or omit any trials. The results were analysed by a one-way ANOVA that revealed a main effect of condition, F(3, 33) = 41.79; p <.001. Post hoc Tukey comparisons revealed that the eyes shut with no movement and free eye movement conditions did not significantly differ either from each other (p >.93) or from the baseline condition (p >.33). All other comparisons were significant at the.001 level. These results demonstrate that saccadic eye movements significantly reduce span even when they are not directed towards targets that are present in the visual field. The lack of any effect from the eyes shut with no movement condition demonstrates that this disruption is not simply due to participants closing their eyes. The results from the free eye movement condition show that the presence of a fixation point during the retention interval does not in itself reduce participants spatial span. However, analysis of the EOG recordings showed that participants did not appear to be making any form of overt eye movement during the retention interval, even though they were free to do so. Although there was some evidence in the practice trials that participants started by adopting a strategy in which they used eye movements to attempt to retain the locations, this was rapidly abandoned in favour of a strategy in which the eyes were kept as still as possible. Indeed, the EOG signals in this condition were indistinguishable from the signals recorded in the baseline condition that required fixation. In addition, when questioned participants indicated it was their subjective experience that overt eye movements were actually making it harder to retain the presented locations accurately. These results are discussed further in the General Discussion section below.

14 1102 PEARSON AND SAHRAIE COMPARISON OF CONDITIONS ACROSS EXPERIMENTS 1 TO 5 In all five of the experiments described previously the eye movement conditions produced significantly greater reduction in span than did comparable attention shift or limb movement conditions. However, as discussed in the introduction, it is important to compare the relative size of these decrements across different experiments. Figure 3 shows the mean baseline performance for the 12 participants across the five experimental testing sessions. ANOVA revealed no significant change in baseline span across different sessions either for participants as a group (F <1,ns) or when considering the performance of each participant separately: for each participant, 0.43 < F(4, 15) < 3.01; p = ns. There is no indication of a practice effect developing across experiments. These results indicate that the procedure adopted for sampling participants spatial span was reliable. Following this a percentage change in performance for each condition was calculated by subtracting the mean span for each condition from the baseline span established in the same testing session. These scores are presented in Figure 4. Dependent samples t tests were carried out to compare performance across conditions. Interference caused by the saccadic eye movement condition was significantly greater than both the smooth pursuit condition, t(11) = 3.133, p <.01, and the eyes shut and saccadic movement condition, t = 2.26, p <.05. Performance in the saccadic movement with eyes closed condition did not significantly differ from the smooth pursuit eye movement condition, t(11) = 0.410, p >.69, but did cause significantly greater reduction in span than did continuous shifts in attention, t(11) = 2.628, p <.02, discrete attention shifts, t(11) = 2.564, p <.02, and concurrent limb movement, t(11) = 5.535, p <.001. Figure 3. Illustration of the mean span, standard deviation, and standard error of participants baseline performance across Experiments 1 to 5.

15 OCULOMOTOR CONTROL AND SPATIAL WORKING MEMORY 1103 Figure 4. Illustration of the percentage change from baseline for all conditions across Experiments 1 to 5. Conditions are identified as follows: (A) free eye movement; (B) eyes shut with no movement; (C) visual noise; (D) articulatory suppression; (E) continuous covert attention shifts; (F) continuous covert attention shift plus colour change monitoring and report; (G) discrete covert attention shifts; (H) spatial tapping; (I) smooth pursuit eye movement; (J) smooth pursuit eye movement plus colour change monitoring and report; (K) saccadic eye movement with eyes shut; (L) saccadic eye movement with eyes open. The interference caused by concurrent limb movement was not significantly different from performance in the discrete attention shift condition, t(11) = 0.257, p >.802, and continuous and discrete attention shift conditions did not significantly differ from each other, t(11) =.257, p >.802. Finally, there was no significant difference in performance between continuous eye movements with and without response to target colour change, t(11) = 0.194, p >.849, and continuous attention shifts with and without responses, t(11) = 1.386, p >.193. This demonstrates that any additional executive load imposed by the requirement to monitor target colour changes was not in itself disruptive to participants performance. Comparison across the conditions reveals a pyramid of effects that is summarized in Figure 5. All conditions that involved eye movement (I, J, K, and L) produced significantly greater reduction in spatial span than did conditions that involved shifts in attention with the eyes fixated (E, F, G, and H). The greatest reduction from baseline occurred in the saccadic eye movement condition, which produced significantly greater disruption in comparison to all other conditions. In an attempt to explore these findings further, an examination was carried out on the effect of different secondary tasks on the types of error made by participants during recall of the spatial sequences. This was done by categorizing the first recall error in a trial as either a spatial (S) or a temporal (T, order) error. In any given trial participants were recalling a sequence of spatially and temporally distributed events. If the first error in the trial was due to the participant reporting the incorrect temporal order of that event, then it was termed a temporal error (as they had still reported a location that was part of the correct sequence, but not the correct

16 1104 PEARSON AND SAHRAIE Figure 5. Illustration of the hierarchy of conditions and corresponding distribution of temporal and spatial errors occurring for the last two incorrectly recalled trials in each experimental run. order in which it had occurred). On the other hand, if the participant reported a block that had not been presented in the original sequence (i.e., a location error), it was termed a spatial error. Any subsequent errors after the first one could not be reliably categorized as being spatial or temporal and were ignored. Errors for all conditions were categorized in this way for the last two incorrect trials that had caused a particular experimental run to be terminated. The results of this analysis are also summarized in Figure 5, which shows that the distribution of type of error maps on to the pyramid of effects previously described. Repeated measures one-way ANOVAs were used to analyse these results. The proportions of spatial and temporal errors in conditions A, B, C, and D were 25% (S) and 75% (T), which were not significantly different from those found in the baseline conditions (F <1;ns). The ratio of errors for conditions involving covert shifts in attention was 42.4% (S) and 57.6% (T). Although there were no significant differences in these ratios between conditions E, F, and G, involving shifts in attention and limb movements, there were significantly more spatial errors than in the baseline condition and conditions A, B, C, and D, F(1, 82) = 15.3; p <.001. In turn, eye movement conditions produced higher spatial errors than those including shifts in attention alone. These ratios were 51.6% (S) and 48.4% (T) for the smooth pursuit conditions and saccadic movement with eyes shut (I, J, and K), and 61.5% (S) and 38.5% (T) for saccadic eye movements (L). Again, the incidence of spatial error for eye movement conditions was significantly higher than those including shifts in spatial attention alone, F(1, 170) = 6.27; p <.01. In all conditions, the first error of recall occurred nearly a third of the way through the sequence (grand average mean 3.11, SD = 0.11). There were no significant differences across any of the conditions for the position in the sequence of the first error. The theoretical probability of making a spatial error in the saccadic eye movement condition was also determined. On average, three visual events had been presented for these

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Selective Interference With the Maintenance of Location Information in Working Memory

Selective Interference With the Maintenance of Location Information in Working Memory Neuropsychology Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1996, Vol. I0, No. 2, 228-240 08944105/96/$3.00 Selective Interference With the Maintenance of Location Information in Working

More information

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning GSTF Journal of Psychology (JPsych) Vol. No., August 5 Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning Hironori Oto 79 Received 6 Jul 5 Accepted 9 Aug 5 Abstract This paper

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Pupillary correlates of covert shifts of attention during working memory maintenance

Pupillary correlates of covert shifts of attention during working memory maintenance Atten Percept Psychophys (217) 79:782 795 DOI 1.3758/s13414-16-1272-7 Pupillary correlates of covert shifts of attention during working memory maintenance Nash Unsworth 1 & Matthew K. Robison 1 Published

More information

September 25, Measures of facilitated responding (priming) Lexical decision

September 25, Measures of facilitated responding (priming) Lexical decision 1 Memory I: Basic Findings September 25, 2008 2 3 Major Historical Landmarks Basic Phenomena Hermann Ebbinghaus Uber das Gedächtniss (1885): first major treatise on empirical study of memory Bartlett (1932):

More information

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory

Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience. Working memory Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience Working memory 1 What is working memory? Brief, immediate memory for information we are currently processing. Closely related to attention: attending to something is often

More information

AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the

AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the U.S. Office of Naval Research 1 Introduction During

More information

Rehearsal in Spatial Working Memory

Rehearsal in Spatial Working Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Human Perception and Performance 0096-1523/98/$3.00 1998, Vol. 24, No. 3, 780-790 Rehearsal in Spatial

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article Research Article VISUAL SEARCH REMAINS EFFICIENT WHEN VISUAL WORKING MEMORY IS FULL Geoffrey F. Woodman, Edward K. Vogel, and Steven J. Luck University of Iowa Abstract Many theories of attention have

More information

Archived at the Flinders Academic Commons:

Archived at the Flinders Academic Commons: Archived at the Flinders Academic Commons: http://dspace.flinders.edu.au/dspace/ This is the authors version of an article published in Memory. The original publication is available by subscription at:

More information

Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration

Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:1694 1709 DOI 10.3758/s13414-011-0135-5 Spatial working memory load affects counting but not subitizing in enumeration Tomonari Shimomura & Takatsune Kumada Published

More information

Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation

Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (1), 154-158 Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation JOO-SEOK HYUN AND STEVEN J. LUCK University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa In mental rotation, a

More information

Effects of gaze-aversion on visual-spatial imagination. Lucy Markson. Kevin B. Paterson. School of Psychology. University of Leicester

Effects of gaze-aversion on visual-spatial imagination. Lucy Markson. Kevin B. Paterson. School of Psychology. University of Leicester Gaze-aversion & visual-spatial imagination In press, British Journal of Psychology Effects of gaze-aversion on visual-spatial imagination Lucy Markson & Kevin B. Paterson School of Psychology University

More information

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1040-1050 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1040 Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching ERINA SAEKI AND SATORU

More information

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object Supplemental materials for: The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object David Whitney*, David A. Westwood, & Melvyn A. Goodale* *Group on Action and Perception, The

More information

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

CANTAB Test descriptions by function CANTAB Test descriptions by function The 22 tests in the CANTAB battery may be divided into the following main types of task: screening tests visual memory tests executive function, working memory and

More information

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS Franziska Kopp* #, Erich Schröger* and Sigrid Lipka # *University of Leipzig, Institute of General Psychology # University of Leipzig,

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction Marc Pomplun and Sindhura Sunkara Department of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts at Boston 100 Morrissey

More information

Individual and developmental differences in working memory across the life span

Individual and developmental differences in working memory across the life span Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1999, 6 (1), 28-40 Individual and developmental differences in working memory across the life span LISA JENKINS, JOEL MYERSON, SANDRA HALE, and ASTRID F. FRY Washington University,

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Background Maintenance: Modality specificity and buffers Interference resolution: Distraction and

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Current Biology, in press Supplemental Information: Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Grigorios Sotiropoulos, Aaron R. Seitz, and Peggy Seriès Supplemental Data Detailed

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

The Role of Vision in the Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Evidence From Blind and Sighted People

The Role of Vision in the Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Evidence From Blind and Sighted People Neuropsychology 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 24, No. 5, 674 679 0894-4105/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019594 The Role of Vision in the Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Evidence From Blind and

More information

Motor and visual codes interact to facilitate visuospatial memory performance

Motor and visual codes interact to facilitate visuospatial memory performance Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (6), 1189-1193 Motor and visual codes interact to facilitate visuospatial memory performance Marvin Chum University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada Harold Bekkering

More information

Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking

Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking Miki Matsumuro (muro@cog.human.nagoya-u.ac.jp) Kazuhisa Miwa (miwa@is.nagoya-u.ac.jp) Graduate School of Information

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

This is a repository copy of Modality specificity and integration in working memory: Insights from visuospatial bootstrapping.

This is a repository copy of Modality specificity and integration in working memory: Insights from visuospatial bootstrapping. This is a repository copy of Modality specificity and integration in working memory: Insights from visuospatial bootstrapping. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/81571/

More information

This version was downloaded from Northumbria Research Link:

This version was downloaded from Northumbria Research Link: Citation: Jenkins, Laura (2016) The Functional Working Memory Architecture of Visual Change Detection Tasks: Are These Tasks Visual Domain Specific? Doctoral thesis, Northumbria University. This version

More information

THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT

THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT Tajuk Bab/Chapter title 1 THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT Teo Sieak Ling and Yeo Kee Jiar shireleneteoh@hotmail.my kjyeo_utm@yahoo.com Abstract Extensive scientific studies in human

More information

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK?

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK? CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK? Bobby Nguyen 1, Yan Zhuo 2 & Rui Ni 1 1 Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA 2 Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy

More information

Consolidating working memory: Enhancing cognitive performance through effective encoding

Consolidating working memory: Enhancing cognitive performance through effective encoding Consolidating working memory: Enhancing cognitive performance through effective encoding Donna Bayliss Neurocognitive Development Unit School of Psychology University of Western Australia 1 Consolidation

More information

Binding across space and time in visual working memory

Binding across space and time in visual working memory Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (3), 292-303 doi:10.3758/mc.38.3.292 Binding across space and time in visual working memory PAUL JOHAN KARLSEN University of Tromsø, Tromsø, Norway RICHARD J. ALLEN University

More information

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 04/24/2018: Lecture 05-2 Note: This Powerpoint presentation

More information

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making Matthew A J Apps 1,2, Laura Grima 2, Sanjay Manohar 2, Masud Husain 1,2 1 Nuffield Department of Clinical Neuroscience,

More information

Competition in visual working memory for control of search

Competition in visual working memory for control of search VISUAL COGNITION, 2004, 11 6), 689±703 Competition in visual working memory for control of search Paul E. Downing and Chris M. Dodds University of Wales, Bangor, UK Recent perspectives on selective attention

More information

The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: Patterns of selective interference in subitizing and counting

The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: Patterns of selective interference in subitizing and counting Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (4), 675-681 The role of working memory in spatial enumeration: Patterns of selective interference in subitizing and counting LANA M. TRICK University of Guelph,

More information

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory

Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Psychon Bull Rev (2017) 24:1651 1657 DOI 10.3758/s13423-017-1226-7 BRIEF REPORT Consolidation and restoration of memory traces in working memory Sébastien De Schrijver 1 & Pierre Barrouillet 1 Published

More information

The role of response selection and input monitoring in solving simple arithmetical products

The role of response selection and input monitoring in solving simple arithmetical products Journal Memory & Cognition 2005,?? 33 (?), (8),???-??? 1472-1483 The role of response selection and input monitoring in solving simple arithmetical products MAUD DESCHUYTENEER and ANDRÉ VANDIERENDONCK

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms

Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms Daryl Fougnie and René Marois Vanderbilt

More information

Shifting Covert Attention to Spatially Indexed Locations Increases Retrieval Performance of Verbal Information

Shifting Covert Attention to Spatially Indexed Locations Increases Retrieval Performance of Verbal Information Shifting Covert Attention to Spatially Indexed Locations Increases Retrieval Performance of Verbal Information Anja Prittmann (anja.prittmann@psychologie.tu-chemnitz.de) Technische Universität Chemnitz,

More information

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? Keyed. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 55B (1), 1 25 Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? J.H. Wearden, A. Parry, and L. Stamp University of

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest Experimental Design Kate Watkins Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford With thanks to: Heidi Johansen-Berg Joe Devlin Outline Choices for experimental paradigm Subtraction / hierarchical

More information

The role of spatial working memory in inhibition of return: Evidence from divided attention tasks

The role of spatial working memory in inhibition of return: Evidence from divided attention tasks Perception & Psychophysics 2003, 65 (6), 970-981 The role of spatial working memory in inhibition of return: Evidence from divided attention tasks ALAN D. CASTEL and JAY PRATT University of Toronto, Toronto,

More information

The contribution of working memory resources to arithmetic performance

The contribution of working memory resources to arithmetic performance The contribution of working memory resources to arithmetic performance Doris Chi Mei Leung BSc(Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy at the University of Western Australia,

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1 The attentional window configures to object boundaries University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 2 ABSTRACT When

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory?

Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Running Head: Visual Working Memory and the CDA Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? Nick Berggren and

More information

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search VISUAL COGNITION, 0000, 00 (0), 111 Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search Walter R. Boot Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee,

More information

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING George J. Andersen, Rui Ni Department of Psychology University of California Riverside Riverside, California, USA E-mail: Andersen@ucr.edu E-mail: ruini@ucr.edu

More information

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making Jan M. Wiener (jwiener@bournemouth.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University Poole, BH12 5BB, UK Olivier De

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Memory. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Memory. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin AQA A Level Psychology Topic Companion Memory Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin AQA A LEVEL Psychology topic companion: MEMORY Page 2 Contents Memory The multi-store model 3 Types of long-term memory 9 The working

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

Psycholinguistics Psychological Mechanisms

Psycholinguistics Psychological Mechanisms Psycholinguistics Psychological Mechanisms Psychological Mechanisms Information processing system Processing Serial and parallel processing Top-down and bottom-up processing Automatic and controlled processes

More information

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (7), 1236-1247 doi: 10.3758/MC/36.7.1236 An action sequence held in memory can interfere with response selection of a target stimulus, but does not interfere with response activation

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Using Peripheral Processing and Spatial Memory to Facilitate Task Resumption

Using Peripheral Processing and Spatial Memory to Facilitate Task Resumption Using Peripheral Processing and Spatial Memory to Facilitate Task Resumption Raj M. Ratwani 1,2, Alyssa E. Andrews 2, Malcolm McCurry 1, J. Gregory Trafton 1,2, Matthew S. Peterson 2 Naval Research Laboratory

More information

Short-Term Memory Demands of Reaction-Time Tasks That Differ in Complexity

Short-Term Memory Demands of Reaction-Time Tasks That Differ in Complexity Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 190, Vol. 6, No. 2, 375-39 Short-Term Memory Demands of Reaction-Time Tasks That Differ in Complexity Gordon D. Logan Erindale College,

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 284 (2015) 167 178 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report How coordinate and categorical

More information

Enhanced visual perception near the hands

Enhanced visual perception near the hands Enhanced visual perception near the hands Bachelor thesis Marina Meinert (s0163430) Supervisors: 1 st supervisor: Prof. Dr. Ing. W. B. Verwey 2 nd supervisor: Dr. M. L. Noordzij External supervisor: Dr.

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Encoding of Information in Auditory Displays: Initial Research on Flexibility and Processing Codes in Dual-task Scenarios

Encoding of Information in Auditory Displays: Initial Research on Flexibility and Processing Codes in Dual-task Scenarios PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS AND ERGONOMICS SOCIETY 52nd ANNUAL MEETING 2008 1820 Encoding of Information in Auditory Displays: Initial Research on Flexibility and Processing Codes in Dual-task Scenarios

More information

Working Memory Capacity and the Antisaccade Task: Individual Differences in Voluntary Saccade Control

Working Memory Capacity and the Antisaccade Task: Individual Differences in Voluntary Saccade Control Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2004, Vol. 30, No. 6, 1302 1321 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.6.1302

More information

Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm

Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm VISUAL COGNITION, 2002, 9 (4/5), 421 457 Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm Peter Wühr and Jochen Müsseler Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research,

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Chapter 3. Working Memory

Chapter 3. Working Memory + Chapter 3 Working Memory + Atkinson and Shiffrin s (1971) Modal Model Environmental Input Sensory Registers Detect sensory input from the various modalities Visual, auditory, haptic, etc. Memories are

More information

Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied

Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (2), 269-274 Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied GEOFFREY F. WOODMAN Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee and STEVEN J. LUCK

More information

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set In press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set Timothy J. Wright 1, Walter

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Stocks and losses, items and interference: A reply to Oberauer and Süß (2000)

Stocks and losses, items and interference: A reply to Oberauer and Süß (2000) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2000, 7 (4), 734 740 Stocks and losses, items and interference: A reply to Oberauer and Süß (2000) JOEL MYERSON Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri LISA JENKINS University

More information

To See or Not to See: The Visual Component of Complex Mental Arithmetic

To See or Not to See: The Visual Component of Complex Mental Arithmetic To See or Not to See: The Visual Component of Complex Mental Arithmetic Wendy Ann Deslauriers (wadeslau@connect.carleton.ca) Centre for Applied Cognitive Research, Institute of Cognitive Science, Carleton

More information

Short-Term and Working Memory. Outline. What is memory? Short-term memory Working memory Working memory and the brain. Chapter 5

Short-Term and Working Memory. Outline. What is memory? Short-term memory Working memory Working memory and the brain. Chapter 5 Short-Term and Working Memory Chapter 5 Outline Short-term memory Working memory Working memory and the brain What is memory? The Persistence of Memory -Salvador Dali Intuitions about memory Memory for

More information

INTRODUCTION METHODS

INTRODUCTION METHODS INTRODUCTION Deficits in working memory (WM) and attention have been associated with aphasia (Heuer & Hallowell, 2009; Hula & McNeil, 2008; Ivanova & Hallowell, 2011; Murray, 1999; Wright & Shisler, 2005).

More information

The Working Memory Model (WMM) By Karys, Emily V, Dan C, Liam

The Working Memory Model (WMM) By Karys, Emily V, Dan C, Liam The Working Memory Model (WMM) By Karys, Emily V, Dan C, Liam So what is the Working Memory Model? The Working Memory Model (WMM) is one explanation for how the short term memory (STM) functions and is

More information

How should you study for Friday's exam?

How should you study for Friday's exam? How should you study for Friday's exam? re-read textbook? re-read lecture slides? study guide? NO! these are passive. Use active study. Test yourself by Take the practice quizzes in Moodle Create your

More information

Developmental Evidence for Working Memory as Activated Long-Term Memory

Developmental Evidence for Working Memory as Activated Long-Term Memory Commentary on: Ruchkin D.S., Grafman J., Cameron K., Berndt R.S. (2003). Working Memory Retention Systems: A State of Activated Long-Term Memory. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, vol. 26, p. 250. Developmental

More information

Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI?

Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI? Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI? Franklin P. Tamborello, II Phillip H. Chung Michael D. Byrne Rice University Department of Psychology 61 S. Main,

More information

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Nick Berggren and Martin Eimer Abstract During the retention of

More information

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use Amanda Hahn (achahn30@gmail.com) Takashi Yamauchi (tya@psyc.tamu.edu) Na-Yung Yu (nayungyu@gmail.com) Department of Psychology, Mail

More information

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Limits to Iconic Memory 0 Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory Ronald A. Rensink Departments of Psychology and Computer Science University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4 Canada Running Head: Limits

More information

Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning

Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning Capacity Limits in Mechanical Reasoning Mary Hegarty Department of Psychology University of California, Santa Barbara Santa Barbara, CA 93106 hegarty@psych.ucsb.edu Abstract This paper examines capacity

More information

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory Eiichi Hoshino 1,2, Ken Mogi 2, 1 Tokyo Institute of Technology, Department of Computational Intelligence and Systems Science. 4259

More information

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness

Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (3), 490-494 Retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tests: The role of test awareness GINO CAMP, DIANE PECHER, and HENK G. SCHMIDT Erasmus University Rotterdam,

More information

This version was downloaded from Northumbria Research Link:

This version was downloaded from Northumbria Research Link: Citation: Hamilton, Colin (2013) The Development of Visuo Spatial Working Memory in Children. In: Working Memory, Developmental Differences, Component Processes and Improvement Mechanisms. NOVA, New York,

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Distinct Capacity Limits for Attention and Working Memory Evidence From Attentive Tracking and Visual Working Memory Paradigms Daryl Fougnie and René Marois Vanderbilt

More information

Temporary Suppression of Visual Processing in an RSVP Task: An Attentional Blink?

Temporary Suppression of Visual Processing in an RSVP Task: An Attentional Blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1992. Vol. 18. No. 3. 849-860 Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-1523/92/S3.00 Temporary Suppression

More information

Memory Development. Cognitive Development

Memory Development. Cognitive Development Memory Development Cognitive Development Memory as information storage Memory Why does our memory sometimes fail us? Memory Schachter s Seven Sins of Memory 1. Transience 2. Absent-Mindedness 3. Blocking

More information

Psychology Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d e. 2. a b c d e 14. a b c d e

Psychology Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d e. 2. a b c d e 14. a b c d e 1 7 Psychology 3450 - Midterm Exam October 20, 2010 Answer Sheet Version A Name: MUN ID: Indicate your answer by crossing out the appropriate letter. Answer 20 questions only. 1. a b c d e 13. a b c d

More information

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition 1 Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition Intro Memory Empirical Studies Interf MR Scan LTM Codes DCT Imagery & Spatial Cognition Rel Org Principles ImplEnc SpatEq Neuro Imaging Critique StruEq Prop

More information

Verbal and Spatial Working Memory in School-Age Children: Developmental Differences in Susceptibility to Interference

Verbal and Spatial Working Memory in School-Age Children: Developmental Differences in Susceptibility to Interference Developmental ftychology 1997, Vol. 33, No. 2, 364-371 Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0012-1649/97/S3.00 Verbal and Spatial Working emory in School-Age Children: Developmental

More information