ADHD- and medication-related brain activation effects in concordantly affected parent child dyads with ADHD

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "ADHD- and medication-related brain activation effects in concordantly affected parent child dyads with ADHD"

Transcription

1 Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48:9 (2007), pp doi: /j x ADHD- and medication-related brain activation effects in concordantly affected parent child dyads with ADHD Jeffery N. Epstein, 1 B.J. Casey, 2 Simon T. Tonev, 3 Matthew C. Davidson, 2 Allan L. Reiss, 4 Amy Garrett, 4 Stephen P. Hinshaw, 5 Laurence L. Greenhill, 6 Gary Glover, 4 Keith M. Shafritz, 7 Alan Vitolo, 5 Lisa A. Kotler, 6 Matthew A. Jarrett, 3 and Julie Spicer 2 1 Department of Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH, USA; 2 Sackler Institute, Weill Medical College of Cornell University, New York, USA; 3 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC, USA; 4 Department of Psychiatry, Stanford University Medical Center, Palo Alto, CA, USA; 5 Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA; 6 Division of Child Psychiatry, Columbia University Medical Center/New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, USA; 7 Department of Psychology, Hofstra University, Hempstead, NY, USA Background: Several studies have documented fronto-striatal dysfunction in children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) using response inhibition tasks. Our objective was to examine functional brain abnormalities among youths and adults with ADHD and to examine the relations between these neurobiological abnormalities and response to stimulant medication. Method: A group of concordantly diagnosed ADHD parent child dyads was compared to a matched sample of normal parent child dyads. In addition, ADHD dyads were administered doubleblind methylphenidate and placebo in a counterbalanced fashion over two consecutive days of testing. Frontostriatal function was measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) during performance of a go/no-go task. Results: Youths and adults with ADHD showed attenuated activity in fronto-striatal regions. In addition, adults with ADHD appeared to activate non-fronto-striatal regions more than normals. A stimulant medication trial showed that among youths, stimulant medication increased activation in fronto-striatal and cerebellar regions. In adults with ADHD, increases in activation were observed in the striatum and cerebellum, but not in prefrontal regions. Conclusions: This study extends findings of fronto-striatal dysfunction to adults with ADHD and highlights the importance of frontostriatal and frontocerebellar circuitry in this disorder, providing evidence of an endophenotype for examining the genetics of ADHD. Keywords: ADHD, adolescence, adulthood, brain imaging, development, fmri, methylphenidate, neuropsychology, children, parents. Abbreviations: MTA: Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD; LNCG: local normative comparison group; CAADID: Conners Adult ADHD Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common developmental disorder of childhood with a prevalence rate of approximately 5 8% in the US (American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Centers for Disease Control, 2005). ADHD and related impairments frequently persist into adolescence and adulthood (Biederman, Mick, & Faraone, 2000; Rasmussen & Gillberg, 2000; Weiss & Hechtman, 1993). Beyond the defining behavioral characteristics of ADHD (i.e., inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity), a large neuropsychological literature has documented several areas of cognitive impairment. Most notably, deficits in response inhibition (i.e., suppression of a prepotent response in favor of an appropriate response; Nigg, 2001) have been reported for children, adolescents, and adults with ADHD using a variety of neuropsychological tasks Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared. (Hervey, Epstein, & Curry, 2004; Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005). Deficits in response inhibition are hypothesized to result from some form of abnormality in the frontostriatal circuitry in the brains of ADHD patients. Evidence for this hypothesis comes from several sources. First, imaging studies with normal participants clearly demonstrate the role of the prefrontal cortices in tasks involving response inhibition (Casey et al., 1997b; Konishi et al., 1999; Konishi, Nakajima, Uchida, Sekihara, & Miyashita, 1998; Menon, Adleman, White, Glover, & Reiss, 2001; Rubia et al., 2001; Tamm, Menon, & Reiss, 2002). Second, patients with frontal lobe lesions have response inhibition deficits similar to those seen in patients with ADHD (Stuss, Murphy, Binns, & Alexander, 2003). Third, several studies have shown correlations between behavioral performance on response inhibition tasks and MRI-based measures of Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA

2 900 Jeffery N. Epstein et al. prefrontal and striatal regions (Casey et al., 1997a; Hill et al., 2003; Rubia, 2002). Taken together, considerable evidence supports fronto-striatal network abnormalities as contributing to observed behavioral deficits in response inhibition in ADHD. Several functional imaging studies have examined differences in brain activity between ADHD and normal participants on response inhibition tasks (Durston et al., 2003; Pliszka et al., 2006; Rubia et al., 1999; Schulz et al., 2004; Smith, Taylor, Brammer, Toone, & Rubia, 2006; Tamm, Menon, Ringel, & Reiss, 2004; Vaidya et al., 1998). A number of these studies have shown lower levels of striatal activation (e.g., caudate nucleus) among children with ADHD relative to controls (Booth et al., 2005; Durston et al., 2003; Rubia et al., 1999; Vaidya et al., 1998, 2005). The majority of studies have found less prefrontal activation in children with ADHD (Booth et al., 2005; Rubia et al., 1999; Rubia, Smith, Brammer, Toone, & Taylor, 2005; Smith et al., 2006; Vaidya et al., 2005), although at least one study has reported the opposite (Durston et al., 2003; Schulz et al., 2004). In those investigations showing less prefrontal activity in ADHD, the pattern has been isolated predominantly within the inferior frontal gyri (IFG; Booth et al., 2005; Durston, Mulder, Casey, Ziermans, & van Engeland, in press; Rubia et al., 1999; Rubia et al., 2005). This pattern of decreased IFG activation has been shown in both children and adolescents with ADHD and has recently been reported in medication-naïve children with ADHD on a mental rotation task (Silk et al., 2005). On the other hand, patterns of activation in regions such as the anterior cingulate cortex have been mixed, with some groups reporting greater activity (Pliszka et al., 2006; Schulz et al., 2004) and others showing less activity (Bush et al., 1999; Rubia et al., 1999; Tamm et al., 2004) in ADHD samples. In addition to examining brain activation differences between patients with ADHD and normal research participants, the effects of stimulant medications have also been examined using brain imaging. A review of previous imaging studies examining the effects of MPH on brain function yields mixed results across adult and pediatric samples. In adults with ADHD, MPH has been associated with increased activation in the cerebellar vermis (Schweitzer et al., 2003), right thalamus (Schweitzer et al., 2004), and precentral gyrus (Schweitzer et al., 2004), and decreased activation in the prefrontal cortex (Schweitzer et al., 2004), precentral gyri (Schweitzer et al., 2003), right claustrum (Schweitzer et al., 2003), and striatum (Matochik, 1994; Schweitzer et al., 2003). These studies have primarily used working memory tasks. In pediatric samples, the pattern of results is somewhat different. Vaidya et al. (1998) found that stimulants increased frontal activation on a response inhibition task in children with ADHD. MPH-related increases in activation in the cerebellar vermis (Anderson, Polcari, Lowen, Renshaw, & Teicher, 2002) and striatum (Shafritz, Marchione, Gore, Shaywitz, & Shaywitz, 2004; Vaidya et al., 1998) have also been noted. However, others have reported MPH-related decreases in striatal activity (Lee et al., 2005). Inconsistencies within the literature and across developmental stages may result from multiple factors. One reason is the broad range of imaging methods (e.g., positron emission tomography (PET), functional magnetic resonance imaging [fmri]) and behavioral paradigms (e.g., stop signal task, go/nogo task) utilized across studies. In particular, pediatric and adult studies are largely different in that adult studies utilize primarily PET while pediatric studies have utilized fmri. To date, no ADHD imaging study has used the same behavioral paradigms with similar imaging methods across pediatric and adult samples. In addition, the sample composition across studies has been quite heterogeneous. A wide variety of inclusion criteria have been used (e.g., multiple ADHD subtypes vs. a single ADHD subytpe; requiring only past evidence of ADHD symptomatology vs. current ADHD symptomatology; samples with medication history vs. medication-naïve patients). To date, samples have been study-specific, serving to increase heterogeneity within most experimental samples. One method for decreasing sample heterogeneity is to utilize an existing, comprehensively defined, and well-studied sample, which (a) allows for accurate description of the study sample (e.g., comorbid disorders), (b) documents the persistence of ADHD symptomatology over time, and (c) ensures common developmental trajectories among the sample. One such existing study sample includes the youths who participated in the Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD (MTA; MTA Cooperative Group, 1999). This sample is one of the most well-defined and comprehensively studied pediatric ADHD samples available. Because children with ADHD symptoms can possess a variety of genetic and/or non-genetic risk factors (Biederman, 2005), another method for increasing homogeneity within an ADHD sample is to utilize a family history of ADHD to select study patients. Seidman and colleagues (Seidman et al., 1995; Seidman, Biederman, Faraone, Weber, & Ouellette, 1997) have shown that adolescents with a family history of ADHD have more neuropsychological deficits, particularly on response inhibition measures, than children without such a family history. Selecting participants based on a family history of ADHD has the capability to produce a more biologically-at risk group of ADHD patients, which would serve to increase the power to detect between-group differences. With this method, an innovative study design may be used in which both youths with ADHD and their affected parents are studied. This design

3 ADHD frontostriatal dysfunction 901 affords examination of ADHD-specific functional deficits at two different developmental stages. Behaviorally, symptom patterns change with development (Biederman et al., 2000). Also, brain morphology of key brain regions changes over time (Castellanos et al., 2002). By using both youths and their parents, differences across developmental stages may be qualitatively examined which may further our understanding of the developmental course of ADHD-related brain abnormalities and the possible developmental changes in the brain s response to stimulant medication. In the present study, youths from the MTA study serve as the patient sample, and a matched comparison group of normal youths recruited as part of the MTA study serve as controls. To obtain a twogeneration sample, we selected probands based on the presence of an ADHD diagnosis in both the youth and at least one biological parent. Further, the present study utilizes both youths and parents as study participants, allowing analyses of between-group effects and medication effects at two different developmental stages. Our chief objectives were to examine deficits compared to control subjects and medication-related changes in ADHD-related frontostriatal brain circuitry using fmri. Based on previous studies, we predicted between-group differences in fronto-striatal and frontocerebellar circuits, parietal cortices, and anterior cingulate for the ADHD parent child dyads compared to the control dyads. Stimulant medication was predicted to produce brain activation changes especially in the striatal region. Overall study design Sample An initial sample of twenty ADHD youth parent dyads were recruited from 3 of the 7 geographical recruiting sites for the MTA study (i.e., Duke University Medical Center, University of California at Berkeley, and New York State Psychiatric Institute). All children in the MTA study received a diagnosis of ADHD, Combined Type at the time of study recruitment when children were 7 9 years of age (see MTA Cooperative Group, 1999 for MTA study methods and total sample description). At the time of entry into the present study, a DISC-P (version 4.0; Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan, & Schwab- Stone, 2000) was administered. Youths were required to meet DSM-IV ADHD diagnostic criteria for any ADHD subtype based on the current DISC to be included in the study. Biological parents meeting DSM-IV ADHD criteria using the Conners Adult ADHD Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV (CAADID; Epstein, Johnson, & Conners, 2001; Epstein & Kollins, 2006) were included in the study. Youths and their parents in both groups were excluded if they had an estimated IQ below 80, suffered from any neurological disease, had a diagnosis of bipolar disorder, psychosis, or pervasive developmental disorder, or had any history of head trauma. For those participants taking stimulant medication, a washout period (5 multiplied by the half-life of the medication) was required. Participants had to be free of neuroleptic medications for 6 months prior to the study. Nine healthy control dyads matched to these ADHD youths age and sex were recruited from a Local Normative Comparison Group (LNCG) that had been recruited for the MTA study (MTA Cooperative Group, 1999). LNCG children were living in the same communities and attending the same schools as the MTA children at baseline assessment. For the purposes of this study, children in the LNCG group had to have fewer than 3 ADHD symptoms within each DSM-IV ADHD symptom domain as assessed by the DISC parent report. In addition, parents in the LNCG group were matched on gender to the ADHD parent and were required to have fewer than 3 symptoms in each DSM-IV ADHD symptom domain as measured by the CAADID. All youths and their parents in the LNCG groups met the same exclusion criteria as those in the MTA group except for diagnostic status. Imaging protocol After description of the study, written informed consent was obtained for all subjects. All participants were acclimated to the scanning environment on the first day of image acquisition using a mock scanner. See Epstein et al. (in press) for a full description of the simulator protocol. Dyads were scanned on consecutive days. Using a counterbalanced and double-blinded design, dyads were assigned to receive either placebo or immediate-release methylphenidate on the first day of image acquisition; dyads received the complementary condition on the second day of image acquisition. Medication dosage was determined using a 0.3 mg/kg formula with a maximum of 20 mg. Using this formula, all of the participants received a 20 mg dosage except for two youths who received a 17.5 mg dose. The dosage taken was not necessarily the optimal dosage for each individual participant nor was it necessarily equivalent to the dosage regularly taken by some participants prior to study entry. Functional imaging using the behavioral paradigm described above began between 60 and 120 minutes after ingestion and ended between 100 and 160 minutes after ingestion. Dyads engaged in the identical behavioral paradigm on both days of image acquisition. The functional images were collected in minutes each session. The study was approved by Institutional Review Boards at each of the participating institutions.

4 902 Jeffery N. Epstein et al. Behavioral paradigm The behavioral paradigm was a go/no-go task. Participants were required to press the response button with the right index finger for each letter that appeared on the screen except for the letter X. The task consisted of five groups (runs) of 128 trials each run. Each run lasted 5 min, 20 sec. Each letter, approximately 2.5 cm in size, appeared for 500 milliseconds with an inter-stimulus interval of 2000 ms. The letter X occurred on approximately 20% of all trials (n ¼ 125), which were presented randomly throughout the run. Other letters were randomly selected from the alphabet. Performance measures on the go/no-go task were mean reaction time and standard deviation for correct go trials, errors of omission, errors of commission, and d-prime. The signal detection measure, d-prime, reflects the subject s perceptual sensitivity to targets; it is the distance between the signal distribution and noise distribution in standard score units. Higher d-prime values indicate higher levels of signal detection relative to noise (i.e., better discrimination between targets and nontargets). Image acquisition and analysis Subjects were scanned with General Electric 1.5 Tesla fmri scanners (General Electrical Medical Systems, Milwaukee, Wisconsin) at Duke University Medical Center, Stanford University School of Medicine and Cornell University Medical College. 1 A whole brain high resolution T1 weighted anatomic scan ( in-plane resolution, 240 mm field of view; 124 slices at 1.5 mm per slice) was acquired for each subject for transformation and localization of functional data into Talairach space. Functional data were collected with a spiral in-andout sequence (Glover & Law, 2001) (TR ¼ 2500 msec, TE ¼ 40 msec, flip angle ¼ 90, FOV ¼ 240 mm, matrix). Each volume contained 33 oblique slices (3.2 mm thick with 1 mm skip) with an in-plane resolution of mm covering the entire brain. Although the time series sampling interval of TR ¼ 2.5 secs was thus equal to the interstimulus presentation interval, signal detection efficiency was not compromised because the average 1 In order to ensure comparability across sites, all sites used identical scanners and software for imaging. Prior to image acquisition, the same individuals were scanned at all sites in order to ensure similar signal to noise and contrast to noise across sites. Further, all sites scanned identical phantoms on a monthly basis to check and guard against any drift during the course of the study. Lastly, all data were normalized during preprocessing to correct for any small signal to noise variations across site. This cross-site methodology was developed and based on a similar multi-site functional imaging study (Casey et al., 1998). hemodynamic response delay of 5 6 secs is wellsampled at this rate. The BrainVoyager QX software package (Brain Innovations, Maastricht, The Netherlands) was used to perform random effects analyses of the functional data. Preprocessing of the functional data involved three-dimensional motion detection and correction (spatial alignment of all volumes to the first volume by rigid transformation) and linear trend removal. Estimated translation and rotation movements were less than 3 mm for all runs used in the analyses. Subjects who did not have at least 3 runs of usable data were excluded from the study. Functional data were co-registered to the anatomic volume by alignment of corresponding points and manual adjustments to obtain registration. Functional data were then transformed into Talairach space with standard landmarks and were interpolated to a resolution of 1 mm. Signal values in each time course were normalized to z-scores representing a change from the mean signal for that run. The signal values for the correct no-go trials were considered to be the effects of interest and were modeled with a convolution of an ideal boxcar response (assuming a value of 1 for the volume of the no-go task presentation and a value of 0 for the remaining time points) with a linear model of the hemodynamic response (Boynton, Engel, Glover, & Heeger, 1996). These predictors were used to build a design matrix for each time course in the experiment. Only correct trials were included in the matrices and subsequent analyses. Hence, correct no-go trials were contrasted with correct go trials to identify response inhibition-related activation patterns which were compared within groups. A whole brain analysis was conducted, but contrasts were examined only in hypothesized ROIs identified below. Three-dimensional statistical maps were generated by assigning an F value to each voxel corresponding to the correct no-go trials and calculated on the basis of the least mean squares solution of the GLM. Contrast analyses were then performed based on t-score differences between the beta weights of this predictor relative to the mean beta weights for that subject. A p level of.05 was used and multiple comparisons were corrected using a contiguity threshold of 5 acquisition-based voxels (Forman et al., 1995). Analyses were conducted separately for youths and adults to examine between-group and medication effects at two different developmental stages. Analyses could not be feasibly conducted combining or comparing these two developmentally different groups due to the number of differences between parents and children in gender, stimulant medication histories, and methods of recruitment across the youths and adult groups (see Table 1) which could result in erroneous interpretations about developmental differences.

5 ADHD frontostriatal dysfunction 903 Table 1 Demographic characteristics ADHD (whole sample) ADHD (matched sample) Control (n ¼ 15) Youths (n ¼ 13) (n ¼ 9) Youths (n ¼ 9) (n ¼ 9) Youths (n ¼ 9) Gender Male (N) Female (N) Ethnicity Caucasian (N) African American (N) Other (N) Age in years Mean (SD) 50.1 (8.1) 17.3 (1.2) 48.6 (9.0) 17.3 (1.2) 46.8 (3.9) 17.4 (1.1) Handedness Right (N) Left (N) Number of each ADHD Subtype Inattentive 6 6* Hyperactive/Impulsive Combined Number with history of N/A N/A stimulant medication Number currently taking N/A N/A stimulant medication Number with specified psychological disorder Eating disorders Mood disorders Obsessive compulsive disorder Posttraumatic stress disorder 1 N/A 1 N/A 0 N/A Social phobia Specific phobia Oppositional defiant disorder N/A 1 N/A 1 N/A 0 Conduct disorder N/A 1 N/A 1 N/A 0 Alcohol use disorders Marijuana use disorders Notes: N/A ¼ not assessed by diagnostic instrument; *One of the children with ADHD did not meet full diagnostic symptom criteria for ADHD. The child had 4 Inattentive symptoms and 0 Hyperactive/Impulsive symptoms. Exclusion of this participant did not alter the study results. Study #1: Between-group effects Methods Subjects. Nine youth parent dyads who were randomly assigned to take placebo on the first day of scanning and the 9 matched LNCG dyads were used to test for between-group differences. ADHD dyads who were randomly assigned to take stimulant medication on the first day of testing were excluded because of the effects of medication on brain function. Demographic data for the matched ADHD and LNCG groups, including gender, ethnicity, age, handedness, diagnostic status, and comorbid disorders, are presented in Table 1. There were no statistically significant differences between the two matched samples (parents or youths) on gender, ethnicity, age, or handedness (all p values >.2). Analyses. Participants were required to perform with greater than 70% accuracy on the behavioral paradigm and have less than a voxel (i.e., less than 3 mm) of head movement for at least three runs to be included in the analyses. The 70% accuracy criterion was derived based on the need to have enough go and no-go trials to obtain a stable hemodynamic response function for each of these trial types. The half voxel of motion criterion is based on Krings et al. (2001). The exclusion of one dyad member excluded the entire dyad from the analyses because of matching criteria. As such, 9 ADHD and 9 normal control dyads (n ¼ 36 subjects) described above met these inclusion criteria. Diagnostic groups were matched on number of runs used in the analysis to prevent differences in statistical power between groups. A whole brain analysis was conducted, but post hoc contrasts were examined in ROIs that have been associated with response inhibition or have been identified in previous imaging studies as being different between ADHD and comparison groups. These areas included the striatum, frontal gyri, anterior cingulate cortex, cerebellum, and posterior parietal gyrus. In ROIs where between-group differences were present, mean beta weights were generated for the

6 904 Jeffery N. Epstein et al. Table 2 T-test results, means and standard deviations for ADHD and LNCG youths and parents on the behavioral task Youths ADHD (n ¼ 9) LNCG (n ¼ 9) Effect size (d) t-tests ADHD (n ¼ 9) LNCG (n ¼ 9) Effect size (d) T-tests Errors of omission 6.44 (8.59) 2.34 (3.03) (8.87) 2.10 (3.30) Errors of commission (14.95) (7.74) (10.56) 8.78 (9.08) Reaction time 439 (69) 352 (30) ** 450 (52) 440 (85) Reaction time SD (56.7) 73.3 (22.5) *** (51.4) (35.9) D-prime 2.70 (1.03) 3.08 (.61) (.99) 3.86 (.52) Note: **p <.01; ***p <.001. ROI using activation maps to define the ROI borders. These beta weights were then correlated with the behavioral measures to examine relations between functional brain activation and performance. Results and discussion Table 2 shows differences between the ADHD and LNCG group s behavioral performance on the go/nogo task conducted in the scanner. Children with ADHD had slower and more variable reaction times (RTs) than children in the LNCG group, but no statistically significant differences were found between parents with ADHD and parents without ADHD. Youths with ADHD showed less brain activation than youths with no history of ADHD during no-go trials in bilateral middle frontal gyrus (Brodmann s areas 9 & 46), right inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann s areas 45 & 47), right inferior parietal lobule (Brodmann s area 40), anterior cingulate (Brodmann s area 32), and bilateral caudate nucleus (Figure 1). Youths with ADHD did not show higher activation compared to LNCG youths in any pre-identified regions of interest. with ADHD also had less activation than controls in right inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann s area 45) and left caudate nucleus. Less activation among parents with ADHD vs. controls was also observed in the left inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann s area 44). On the other hand, parents with ADHD showed more activation in the left inferior parietal lobule (Brodmann s area 40) and anterior cingulate (Brodmann s area 32) than parents without ADHD. 2 See Table 3 for regions of interest, maximum t-value, and Talaraich coordinates. See Tables 6 and 7 for the main effect of condition for each group separately. 3 2 Post hoc correlational analysis showed no within-dyad correlation between levels of activation (i.e., beta values) in youths with levels of activation in their parents (all p values >.05) for any identified region of interest. 3 Whole brain between-group analyses revealed increased right parahippocampal gyrus (BA 20) and decreased left thalamic activation for ADHD youths compared to normal controls. However, these regions did not survive our contiguity threshold. No additional regions of interest were different among parents in whole brain analyses. The relation between behavioral task performance and corresponding brain activation was examined using correlational analyses. Because the trials of interest were no-go trials (i.e., inhibition trials), only errors of commission and d-prime were utilized as performance indicators. Performance on these indicators was correlated with individual beta weights for each individual ROI that was identified in the between-group fmri analyses. Correlations between errors of commission and the various ROIs in parents and children were all non-significant (all p values >.05). However, d-prime was positively correlated with brain activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus in children (r ¼.47, p ¼.048; Brodmann s area 47) and parents (r ¼.66, p ¼.003; Brodmann s area 45). Scatter plots (see Figure 2) of these correlations indicate that the association between right IFG and d-prime was consistent across ADHD and control youths, but was confined to the ADHD participants in the adult sample. D-prime was also positively correlated with left caudate nucleus activation in parents (r ¼.71, p ¼.001). Note that higher values of d-prime indicate better discrimination; hence the more activation in these regions, the better the discrimination. The novel use of concordantly-affected parent child dyads allowed a cross-sectional examination of fronto-striatal abnormalities in manifestations of ADHD at two different ages. We found that youths diagnosed with ADHD showed lower brain activation in fronto-striatal regions on a response inhibition task than comparison youths. of the ADHD sample who themselves met criteria for ADHD showed similar fronto-striatal abnormalities to their children as well as some interesting differences. Consistent with research reporting similar neuropsychological deficits (see reviews by Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996 and Hervey et al., 2004) across childhood and adulthood, these study findings support similar fronto-striatal abnormalities in ADHD patients across youth and adult samples. Further, because the adults and youths in this sample were biologically related, the current pattern of results suggests a possible biologically-based mechanism of transmission (i.e., heritability). Using a family history of ADHD as the basis for inclusion resulted in robust between-group differ-

7 ADHD frontostriatal dysfunction 905 The right side of the image corresponds to the left side of the brain. Figure 1 Diagnosis-based activation differences (control ADHD) during response inhibition for parent and youth participants. For parents, controls show significantly greater activation than ADHD participants in left caudate nucleus and bilaterally in inferior frontal gyrus whereas ADHD participants show greater activation in anterior cingulate and precuneus. For youths, controls show greater activation in anterior cingulate, right inferior frontal gyrus, and bilaterally in caudate nucleus a Table 3 Regions of interest showing significant differences in brain activation between ADHD and normal controls Talairach coordinates Region of interest Brodmann s area x y z Contrast t-value Maximum Volume (mm 3 ) Youth Middle frontal gyrus R 46/ Control>ADHD L 46/9 ) Control>ADHD Inferior frontal gyrus (R) Control>ADHD Anterior cingulate Control>ADHD Inferior frontal gyrus (R) Control>ADHD Caudate nucleus R Control>ADHD L ) Control>ADHD Inferior parietal lobule (R) )43 42 Control>ADHD Anterior cingulate 32 ) ADHD>Control ) Inferior frontal gyrus R Control>ADHD L 44 ) Control>ADHD Caudate nucleus (L) ) Control>ADHD Inferior parietal lobule (L) 40 )47 )67 27 ADHD>Control ) Precuneus 7 0 )70 33 ADHD>Control ) ences in brain activation in our youth sample and also served to identify at least two regions, left caudate nucleus and right inferior frontal gyrus, showing diminished activation among children and parents with ADHD. Right IFG activation was also correlated with performance on the behavioral task. Indeed, the IFG has been repeatedly found to be involved in response inhibition tasks among both control and disordered populations (see review by Aron & Poldrack, 2005). Decreased IFG and caudate activation has also been one of the most consistent ADHD-related findings in fmri studies comparing children with ADHD to normal controls (Booth et al., 2005; Durston et al., 2003; Rubia et al., 1999, 2005; Smith et al., 2006; Vaidya et al., 1998; Vaidya et al., 2005). Also, a recent study report (Sowell et al., 2003) and a review of the structural literature (Swanson, Castellanos, Murias, LaHoste, & Kennedy, 1998) demonstrate significant structural differences in right IFG between children with and without ADHD. Given these past and current findings, IFG functional activation seems to be a candidate endophenotype in examining phenotype-genotype relations in ADHD. Indeed, Durston et al. (in press) have shown a similar concordance in IFG functional

8 906 Jeffery N. Epstein et al. Youths r=.45 r=.82 r=.4 5 r=-.15 Overall r=.47, p<.05 Overall r=.66, p<.01 Figure 2 Correlations (and associated regression lines) between d-prime and right inferior frontal gyrus beta values overall, and as a function of diagnostic group (ADHD, control) for both parent and youth participants abnormalities among children with ADHD and their unaffected siblings. Although some brain regions showed similar patterns of activity across youths with ADHD and their parents, the parents had fewer regional differences from the control parents than did their children with respect to control children. In addition, some brain regions showed higher activation in the parents with ADHD versus the control parents. The different pattern of neurofunctional deficits across youths and adults is quite interesting and raises the question of whether such disparities are evidence of a developmental phenomenon or, rather, an artifact of the experimental methods used for this study. There were several characteristics that differentiated the youth and adult ADHD samples in this study. Most notably, adults were less likely to have received medication for ADHD. Furthermore, parents in the study were recruited based on their child s ADHD status; as a result, the non-referred adult ADHD sample used in this study may have been less severely impaired than the child sample and possibly less severe than adult ADHD samples used in previous studies. There were also differences between the youth and adult samples in terms of gender distribution (more males in youth sample), presence of mood disorders (more mood disorders in the adult sample), history of stimulants (lower usage in the adult sample), and equality of handedness across ADHD and control groups (not a matching variable). Any or all of these variables can be considered confounds in inferring developmental differences from our results. However, in regard to a history of stimulant usage as a confound, at least three fmri studies using medication-naïve children with ADHD have shown activation differences in similar regions of interest as those reported in this paper (e.g., IFG; Rubia et al., 2005; Silk et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2006). It appears that adults with ADHD may recruit alternative brain regions to perform task-related inhibition. Namely, task-related activation in the anterior cingulate was higher for parents with ADHD than control parents. Coupled with the absence of between-groups behavioral deficits, it may be that the adults with ADHD are relying on alternative brain regions to successfully inhibit responses during the task. Recruitment of the anterior cingulate cortex may be an attempt to recruit attentional mechanisms to improve performance (Peterson et al., 1999), perhaps related to response conflict or error monitoring. Interestingly, the finding of increased anterior cingulate cortex activity during successful no-go trials is consistent with results reported by Pliszka et al. (2006) using a pediatric sample. However, Bush et al. (1999) report lower activation in anterior cingulate cortex for adults with ADHD when performing a Counting Stoop task. Our finding of increased activation in anterior cingulate cortex on a behavioral inhibition task should be treated with caution given inconsistency across paradigms. Another developmental difference was the laterality of observed activation differences across youths and adults. Neurobiological models and previous findings largely support a right-lateralized deficit among patients with ADHD (see review by Giedd, Blumenthal, Molloy, & Castellanos, 2001). In our youth sample, most of the differences in brain activation were right lateralized and for those that appeared on the left (e.g., MFG, caudate), there were corresponding differences on the right side. For the adults, however, deficits were observed predominantly on the left side (i.e., lower activation in caudate nucleus and IFG). These findings may suggest

9 ADHD frontostriatal dysfunction 907 involvement of bilateral fronto-striatal circuitry in response inhibition, rather than simple right lateralization of this function due to strategic differences (e.g., use of language to help refrain from making a false alarm) in performing the task. The primary limitation of this study is small sample size. First, because of the decision to use the welldefined and comprehensively assessed MTA sample, potential participants were limited to the MTA sample. The effective sample was further limited to those children who continued to have ADHD into adolescence and who also had a parent with ADHD. Further, the number of data collection sites was limited to three in order to reduce heterogeneity introduced by using multiple scanners. The resulting sample size was small but comparable to other ADHD functional imaging studies. Nevertheless, this small sample may have limited power to detect betweengroup behavioral and brain activation differences. Another limitation was the use of a cross-sectional design to examine possible developmental differences in contrast to using a longitudinal design which would allow within-subjects comparison of developmental changes over time. Also, as commented on above, the youth and parent groups were not matched and displayed several baseline differences on key measures such as medication use history and gender. Study #2: Medication effects Methods Subjects. Of the 20 ADHD dyads, 7 youths and 5 parents were excluded because they were unable to perform with greater than 70% accuracy on the behavioral paradigm and have less than 3 mm of head movement for at least three runs. The remaining 13 youths and 15 parents comprised the sample. See Table 1 for demographic and diagnostic characteristics of the parent and youth samples. Analyses. On- and off-medication contrasts were conducted with a random effects analysis. Placebo and medication data sets were matched on number of runs used in the analysis to prevent differences in statistical power between the two conditions. A whole brain analysis was conducted, but contrasts were examined only in hypothesized ROIs that have been associated with methylphenidate response (i.e., striatum, prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices, and the cerebellum). Results and discussion Table 4 presents the results of paired t-tests examining medication effects on go/no-go task performance. Medication decreased reaction time (RT) variability and improved stimulus discrimination (i.e., d-prime) in both youths and their parents. In addition, medication reduced the number of errors of omission made by parents with ADHD. Comparisons of brain activation during performance of no-go trials showed that ADHD youths, while on MPH compared to placebo, showed a consistent pattern of increased activity across multiple regions. These task-related increases in activity were in the left middle frontal gyrus (Brodmann s area 9), left inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann s area 45), right inferior parietal lobule (Brodmann s area 40), anterior cingulate (Brodmann s area 32), right caudate nucleus and left cerebellum (Figure 3). No regions of interest showed increased activation during placebo days compared to MPH days in the ADHD youths. Correlations between changes in behavioral performance on- and off-medication and brain activation beta values on- and off-medication in activation-defined regions of interest, where medication effects were noted, were non-significant (all p values >.05). Effects of medication on brain activation in parents with ADHD were less widespread. Increased activation was observed for the MPH condition compared to placebo in left caudate nucleus. However, increased activation was observed for placebo vs. MPH in the right inferior parietal lobule (Brodmann s area 40) and left middle frontal gyrus (Brodmann s area 46). See Table 5 for identified regions, volumes, and Talaraich coordinates. There appears to be a benefit of methylphenidate for youths with ADHD as evidenced by better behavioral performance and increased brain activa- Table 4 Paired t-test results, means and standard deviations for the behavioral performance of the ADHD youths and parents on placebo compared to on methylphenidate Youths Placebo (n ¼ 13) MPH (n ¼ 13) Effect size (d) t-tests Placebo (n ¼ 15) MPH (n ¼ 15) Effect size (d) t-tests Errors of omission 6.18 (8.27) 3.35 (7.52) (11.13) 1.44 (2.01) * Errors of commission (14.64) (13.65) (9.27) 7.72 (6.64) Reaction time 434 (80) 407 (42) (57) 403 (51) Reaction time SD (69.2) (40.5) ** (44.9) 78.0 (19.0) * D-prime 2.82 (.92) 3.37 (.84) * 3.12 (.78) 3.83 (.59) *** Note: *p <.05; **p < 01; ***p <.001.

10 908 Jeffery N. Epstein et al. Youths The right side of the image corresponds to the left side of the brain. Figure 3 Stimulant medication-related activation differences (on- vs. off-meds) during response inhibition for parent and youth participants. For parents, being on medication was related to significantly greater activation in left caudate nucleus. For youths, medication was related bilateral caudate and inferior parietal lobe activity a tion in fronto-striatal structures when on stimulant medication compared to placebo. This increased inactivation was observed in adults with ADHD for the striatum, but not the prefrontal cortex. In addition, adults with ADHD showed a medication-related decrease in activation of the right parietal lobe. The activation of striatal regions as a result of stimulant medication across youth and adults is consistent with the pharmacological properties of stimulants (Volkow, Wang, Fowler, & Ding, 2005), the existing literature (Vaidya et al., 1998), and our study hypotheses. MPH blocks dopamine reuptake in striatal areas and possibly blocks reuptake of norepinephrine in the cortices, thereby leading to medication-related increases in striatal activation. The increased activation in both prefrontal cortex and striatum observed in this youth sample is similar to the increase in activation observed in a sample of children with ADHD using fmri and a similar task (Vaidya et al., 1998). In addition to the medication effects reported in frontostriatal structures, the present study also reports medication-related increases in cerebellar activation among the youths. A similar effect was seen in the adults but this effect did not pass our statistical contiguity threshold, suggesting that the cerebellar effect is less pronounced and less widespread for adults. That stimulant medications would increase activation in the cerebellum is not surprising. The cerebellum has many inputs from the ventral tegmental area (Ikai, Takada, & Mizuno, 1994; Ikai, Takada, Shinonaga, & Mizuno, 1992), Table 5 Regions of interest showing significant differences in brain activation between medication and placebo conditions among youths and parents with ADHD Region of interest Brodmann s Area Talairach coordinates x y z Contrast Max. t-value Volume (mm 3 ) Youth Anterior cingulate MPH>Placebo Orbital frontal cortex (R)* MPH>Placebo Inferior frontal gyrus (L) 45 ) MPH>Placebo Caudate nucleus (R) MPH>Placebo Middle frontal gyrus (L) 9 ) MPH>Placebo Globus pallidus (R)* MPH>Placebo Caudate nucleus (L)* ) MPH>Placebo Inferior parietal lobule (R) )45 46 MPH>Placebo Cerebellum (L) )12 )62 )21 MPH>Placebo Middle frontal gyrus (L)* 46 ) Placebo>MPH Caudate nucleus (L) ) MPH>Placebo Hippocampus (L) )24 )18 )9 MPH>Placebo Inferior parietal lobule (R) 40 )37 )28 37 Placebo>MPH ) Cerebellum (L)* )2 )60 )17 MPH>Placebo *did not meet statistical contiguity threshold.

11 ADHD frontostriatal dysfunction 909 Table 6 Regions of interest showing significant differences in brain activation in control participants Table 7 Regions of interest showing significant differences in brain activation in participants with ADHD Region of interest Brodmann s area Talairach coordinates x y z Maximum t-value Region of interest Brodmann s area Talairach coordinates x y z Maximum t-value Youth Inferior frontal gyrus R R L 45 ) Middle frontal gyrus R L 9 ) Caudate nucleus (L) ) Globus Pallidus R L )18 ) Inferior parietal lobule (R) R ) L 40 )46 ) Cerebellum (L) )27 )65 ) Anterior cingulate R L 32 ) Orbital (R) Hippocampus (L) Inferior frontal gyrus R R L 46 ) Middle frontal gyrus R L 9 ) Caudate nucleus (R) Globus Pallidus R L ) Inferior parietal lobule (R) R ) L 40 )40 ) Cerebellum (L) )27 )64 ) Anterior cingulate R L 32 ) Orbital (L) ) Hippocampus (L) 31 )22 ) Youth Inferior frontal gyrus R R L 45 ) Middle frontal gyrus R L 9 ) Caudate nucleus (R) Globus Pallidus R L ) Inferior parietal lobule (R) R ) L 40 )45 ) Cerebellum (L) )22 )52 ) Anterior cingulate R L 32 ) Orbital (R) ) Hippocampus (L) 29 )29 ) Inferior frontal gyrus R R L 45 ) Middle frontal gyrus R L 46 ) Caudate nucleus (R) Globus Pallidus R L ) Inferior parietal lobule (R) R ) L 40 )53 ) Cerebellum (L) )36 )55 ) Anterior cingulate R L 32 ) Orbital (R) Hippocampus (L) ) which is a dopaminergic center of the brain. Using PET, increased cerebellar activity as a result of MPH has been shown with normal adults (Volkow et al., 1998). This study s documentation of increases in cerebellar activation as a result of MPH administration does differ from previous studies in that this effect was shown using task-related BOLD response. Typically, fmri studies have not included cerebellar regions in their imaging field of view. Hence, the present findings emphasize the importance of collecting whole brain images when examining activation patterns, and especially medication effects, in patients with ADHD. A limitation of the current study is that children and adults with ADHD did not necessarily take an individually-determined optimal dose of MPH on the day of scanning. Rather, a mg/kg formula (with a maximum cut-off of 20 mg MPH) was used to assign dosages to individual participants. Because weight does not correlate with optimal dose (Rapport, Du- Paul, & Kelly, 1989) and because patients have variable dose response curves (Rapport et al., 1987), dosing according to weight does not always result in an optimal dosage for each individual. General discussion Results from the above studies support prevailing theoretical notions that ADHD is caused by a neurobiological deficit localized to prefrontal and striatal regions of the brain. Several studies have been

12 910 Jeffery N. Epstein et al. conducted examining response inhibition-related brain activation in children with ADHD. This study extends findings of fronto-striatal dysfunction to adults with ADHD and in individuals with genetic vulnerability for the disorder. A stimulant medication trial among the ADHD youths and their parents showed that among youths, stimulant medication increases activation in the striatum, as well as in cerebellar regions. Increases in activation were observed in prefrontal regions in the youth, but not the adults. In addition, there was a medicationrelated increase in activation in hippocampus for the adults, unlike the youths. A clear strength of this study was the use of the MTA sample for recruitment of participants. All of the MTA participants with ADHD had a documented history of an ADHD, Combined Type diagnosis between 7 and 9 years of age. Further, children in this sample were assessed approximately every 2 years until the present so that persistence of ADHD symptomatology has been documented. This differs from most ADHD imaging samples where study inclusion is usually, but not always, based upon symptoms present at the time of the study. Such a single time point diagnosis may yield increased sample heterogeneity, with the likelihood of multiple etiologies (Biederman, 2005). Our study sample was further restricted to youths and parents with a family history of ADHD. This recruitment strategy increases the likelihood that the observed findings represent the effects of stimulant medications on youths and adults whose ADHD symptomatology is biologically based and shared within dyads. Because the sample used for the present study was selected based on genetic association, the study results provide several clues as to possible endophenotypic characteristics that may be used to study the genetics and pharmacogenetics of ADHD. Indeed, several investigators (e.g., Doyle et al., 2005) have described the advantages of using endophenotypes to search for genetic associations with ADHD. In addition, several research studies report on the relationships between specific candidate genes and response to stimulant medication among patients with ADHD (see McGough, 2005 for review). Some of the outcomes in the present study (i.e., d-prime on a response inhibition task, fronto-striatal activation) demonstrated between-group and medicationrelated effects across developmentally different samples. Relating these endophenotypes to genetic markers has the potential to identify genetic predictors of ADHD and possibly stimulant response. Acknowledgements The grant was supported by a set of NIH collaborative grants: MH (Epstein), MH (Casey), MH (Reiss), MH (Hinshaw), and MH (Greenhill) as well as grant #RR09784 (Glover). Correspondence to Jeffery N. Epstein, Cincinnati Children s Hospital Medical Center, 3333 Burnet Ave, ML 10006, Cincinnati, OH 45229; Tel: (513) ; Fax: (513) ; jeff.epstein@cchmc.org References American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders, 4th edn. Washington, DC: Author. Anderson, C.M., Polcari, A., Lowen, S.B., Renshaw, P.F., & Teicher, M.H. (2002). Effects of methylphenidate on functional magnetic resonance relaxometry of the cerebellar vermis in boys with ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, Aron, A.R., & Poldrack, R.A. (2005). The cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: Relevance for genetic research in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57, Biederman, J. (2005). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A selective overview. Biological Psychiatry, 57, Biederman, J., Mick, E., & Faraone, S.V. (2000). Agedependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and symptom type. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, Booth, J.R., Burman, D.D., Meyer, J.R., Lei, Z., Trommer, B.L., Davenport, N.D., et al. (2005). Larger deficits in brain networks for response inhibition than for visual selective attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, Boynton, G.M., Engel, S.A., Glover, G.H., & Heeger, D.J. (1996). Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, Bush, G., Frazier, J.A., Rauch, S.L., Seidman, L.J., Whalen, P.J., Jenike, M.A., et al. (1999). Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder revealed by fmri and counting Stroop. Biological Psychiatry, 45, Casey, B.J., Castellanos, F.X., Giedd, J.N., Marsh, W.L., Hamburger, S.D., Schubert, A.B., et al. (1997a). Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, Casey, B.J., Cohen, J.D., O Craven, K., Davidson, R.J., Irwin, W., Nelson, C.A., et al. (1998). Reproducibility of fmri results across four institutions using a spatial working memory task. Neuroimage, 8, Casey, B.J., Trainor, R.J., Orendi, J.L., Schubert, A.B., Nystrom, L.E., Giedd, J.N., et al. (1997b). A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a go-no-go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, Castellanos, F.X., Lee, P.L., Sharp, W., Jeffries, N.O., Greenstein, D.K., Clasen, L.S., et al. (2002). Developmental brain trajectories of brain volume abnormal-

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome. Authors Year Patients Male gender (%) Mean age (range) Adults/ Children

More information

Brain Imaging Data of ADHD

Brain Imaging Data of ADHD August 01, 2004 Brain Imaging Data of ADHD Amir Raz, Ph.D. The past two decades have ushered in a new era of methodological advances in tools for noninvasive imaging of the living brain. The information

More information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis (OA). All subjects provided informed consent to procedures

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Hart H, Radua J, Nakao T, Mataix-Cols D, Rubia K. Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity

More information

Risk-taking and the adolescent brain: who is at risk?

Risk-taking and the adolescent brain: who is at risk? Developmental Science 10:2 (2007), pp F8 F14 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00579.x FAST-TRACK REPORT Blackwell Publishing Ltd Risk-taking and the adolescent brain: who is at risk? Adriana Galvan, 1 Todd

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Supplemental informations Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Stéphanie Lefebvre, PhD^1,2, Guillaume Baille, MD^4, Renaud Jardri MD, PhD 1,2 Lucie Plomhause, PhD

More information

The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fmri Study

The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fmri Study NeuroImage 16, 449 453 (2002) doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1074, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fmri Study S. Durston,*, K. M.

More information

Passport control a bit carried away. appreciated the advice forgot to talk to the manager, next thing I know my fmri thankfully, when aroused things back to normal Inattentive impaired children and adolescents:

More information

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is Article Response Inhibition in Adolescents Diagnosed With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder During Childhood: An Event-Related fmri Study Kurt P. Schulz, Ph.D. Jin Fan, Ph.D. Cheuk Y. Tang, Ph.D.

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

Neural and behavioral correlates of expectancy violations in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

Neural and behavioral correlates of expectancy violations in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48:9 (2007), pp 881 889 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01754.x Neural and behavioral correlates of expectancy violations in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

More information

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Supplemental Information Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression Roselinde H. Kaiser, Ph.D., Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli, Ph.D., Daniel G. Dillon, Ph.D., Franziska Goer, B.S., Miranda

More information

Supplementary materials. Appendix A;

Supplementary materials. Appendix A; Supplementary materials Appendix A; To determine ADHD diagnoses, a combination of Conners' ADHD questionnaires and a semi-structured diagnostic interview was used(1-4). Each participant was assessed with

More information

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis International Journal of Innovative Research in Computer Science & Technology (IJIRCST) ISSN: 2347-5552, Volume-2, Issue-6, November-2014 Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using

More information

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Subjects The experiment included twelve subjects: ten sighted subjects and two blind. Five of the ten sighted subjects were expert users of a visual-to-auditory

More information

Brain Activation Gradients in Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Related to Persistence of ADHD in Adolescent Boys

Brain Activation Gradients in Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Related to Persistence of ADHD in Adolescent Boys Brain Activation Gradients in Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Related to Persistence of ADHD in Adolescent Boys KURT P. SCHULZ, PH.D., JEFFREY H. NEWCORN, M.D., JIN FAN, PH.D., CHEUK Y. TANG, PH.D., AND

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. is the most common neurobehavioral

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. is the most common neurobehavioral ORIGINAL ARTICLE Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Methylphenidate and Placebo in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder During the Multi-Source Interference Task George Bush, MD, MMSc; Thomas

More information

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder A Neuro-Anatomical Approach to diagnosis and treatment

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder A Neuro-Anatomical Approach to diagnosis and treatment Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder A Neuro-Anatomical Approach to diagnosis and treatment Damon Lipinski, Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist Center for Pediatric Neuropsychology What is Attention? Different

More information

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and dissociative disorder among abused children

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and dissociative disorder among abused children Blackwell Publishing AsiaMelbourne, AustraliaPCNPsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences1323-13162006 Folia Publishing Society2006604434438Original ArticleADHD and dissociation in abused childrent. Endo et

More information

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition Outline Functional MRI Mapping Cognition Michael A. Yassa, B.A. Division of Psychiatric Neuro-imaging Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Why fmri? fmri - How it works Research

More information

9/13/2018. Neurobiological Aspects of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria

9/13/2018. Neurobiological Aspects of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria Neurobiological Aspects of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Neil P. Jones 7th Annual Conference on ADHD and Executive Function September 14, 218 Diagnosis Child

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Redlich R, Opel N, Grotegerd D, et al. Prediction of individual response to electroconvulsive therapy via machine learning on structural magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA

More information

Presenter Disclosure Information

Presenter Disclosure Information 12 th International Congress on Schizophrenia Research Presenter Disclosure Information I will not discuss off label use and/or investigational use in my presentation. I have no financial relationships

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Green SA, Hernandez L, Tottenham N, Krasileva K, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M. The neurobiology of sensory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. Published

More information

Neural development of selective attention and response inhibition

Neural development of selective attention and response inhibition NeuroImage 20 (2003) 737 751 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Neural development of selective attention and response inhibition James R. Booth, a,b,c, * Douglas D. Burman, a Joel R. Meyer, b Zhang Lei, d

More information

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and prenatal. smoke and alcohol exposure

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and prenatal. smoke and alcohol exposure Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and prenatal smoke and alcohol exposure Regina Schrama 1, Patrick de Zeeuw 2 1 Master program Neuroscience and Cognition, Behavioural Neuroscience, Child and Adolescent

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Jónsdóttir, S. (2006). ADHD and its relationship to comorbidity and gender. s.n.

Citation for published version (APA): Jónsdóttir, S. (2006). ADHD and its relationship to comorbidity and gender. s.n. University of Groningen ADHD and its relationship to comorbidity and gender Jónsdóttir, Sólveig IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite

More information

A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control

A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control Developmental Science 5:4 (2002), pp F9 F16 A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control Blackwell Publishers Ltd Sarah Durston, 1,2 Kathleen M. Thomas, 1 Yihong Yang, 3 Aziz M. Ulu, 4 Robert

More information

Pharmacological MRI in the assessment of monoaminergic function Schouw, M.L.J.

Pharmacological MRI in the assessment of monoaminergic function Schouw, M.L.J. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Pharmacological MRI in the assessment of monoaminergic function Schouw, M.L.J. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Schouw, M. L. J. (2013).

More information

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 SPECIAL ISSUE WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TE US ABOUT AND DIS? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 By: Angelika Dimoka Fox School of Business Temple University 1801 Liacouras Walk Philadelphia, PA

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/324/5927/646/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves Modulation of the vmpfc Valuation System Todd A. Hare,* Colin F. Camerer, Antonio

More information

4.6. In ADHD: What are the most frequent comorbid disorders? 4.1. How is ADHD defined? What clinical manifestations does the disorder have?

4.6. In ADHD: What are the most frequent comorbid disorders? 4.1. How is ADHD defined? What clinical manifestations does the disorder have? 4. ADHD Questions to be answered: 4.1. How is ADHD defined? What clinical manifestations does the disorder have? 4.2. Etiopathogeny of ADHD. What are the main risk factors? 4.3. In ADHD: Are there neuropsychological

More information

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is characterized

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is characterized REVIEW Cool Inferior Frontostriatal Dysfunction in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Versus Hot Ventromedial Orbitofrontal-Limbic Dysfunction in Conduct Disorder: A Review Katya Rubia Attention-deficit/hyperactivity

More information

Differential Prefrontal Cortex Activation During Inhibitory Control in Adolescents With and Without Childhood Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Differential Prefrontal Cortex Activation During Inhibitory Control in Adolescents With and Without Childhood Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Neuropsychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 19, No. 3, 390 402 0894-4105/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.390 Differential Prefrontal Cortex Activation During

More information

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group 1 Supplemental Data Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group Additional inclusion criteria for the major depressive disorder group were: age of onset of

More information

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA The Effect of Cognitive Functioning, Age, and Molecular Variables on Brain Structure Among Carriers of the Fragile X Premutation: Deformation Based Morphometry Study Naomi J. Goodrich-Hunsaker*, Ling M.

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Brain imaging of the neural systems affected in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

Brain imaging of the neural systems affected in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder THEMED ARTICLE y ADHD For reprint orders, please contact reprints@expert-reviews.com Key Paper Evaluation Brain imaging of the neural systems affected in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

More information

Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of

Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of Introduction Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of scientific inquiry for over 100 years. In particular, this topic has had significant interest in the past

More information

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Brain electrical activity methods description and explanation of method state effects trait effects Positron emission tomography

More information

EEG based biomarkers in pediatric neuropsychiatry: ADHD autism (ASD)

EEG based biomarkers in pediatric neuropsychiatry: ADHD autism (ASD) EEG based biomarkers in pediatric neuropsychiatry: ADHD autism (ASD) Neuropsychologist PhD Geir Ogrim NORWAY geir.ogrim@so-hf.no Affiliations Neuroteam, Child psychiatry service, Østfold Hospital Trust

More information

Ippeita Dan on behalf of RISTEX ADHD Diagnosis Consortium Chuo University, Tokyo, Japan

Ippeita Dan on behalf of RISTEX ADHD Diagnosis Consortium Chuo University, Tokyo, Japan Functional near-infrared spectroscopy on the go for clinical application along with recent technical development for enhancing its potential With emphasis on neuropharmacological assessment on children

More information

ADHD & PTSD. ADHD Across the Lifespan March 18, 2017 Andrea E. Spencer, MD.

ADHD & PTSD. ADHD Across the Lifespan March 18, 2017 Andrea E. Spencer, MD. ADHD & PTSD ADHD Across the Lifespan March 18, 2017 Andrea E. Spencer, MD Disclosures Neither I nor my partner has a relevant financial relationship with a commercial interest to disclose. Acknowledgments

More information

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT Why delaying the onset of alcohol and other drug use during adolescence is so important Aaron White, PhD Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research National Institute

More information

Technetium-99m HMPAO brain SPECT in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Technetium-99m HMPAO brain SPECT in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder ORIGINAL ARTICLE Annals of Nuclear Medicine Vol. 16, No. 8, 527 531, 2002 Technetium-99m HMPAO brain SPECT in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Gamze CAPA KAYA,* Aynur PEKCANLAR,**

More information

Efficiency of the Prefrontal Cortex During Working Memory in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Efficiency of the Prefrontal Cortex During Working Memory in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Efficiency of the Prefrontal Cortex During Working Memory in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder MARGARET A. SHERIDAN, M.A., STEPHEN HINSHAW, PH.D., AND MARK D`ESPOSITO, M.D. ABSTRACT Objective: Previous

More information

Can Brain Scans See Depression?

Can Brain Scans See Depression? Can Brain Scans See Depression? By BENEDICT CAREY They seem almost alive: snapshots of the living human brain. Not long ago, scientists predicted that these images, produced by sophisticated brainscanning

More information

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Attention, Academic Achievement and the Brain Boss

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Attention, Academic Achievement and the Brain Boss Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Attention, Academic Achievement and the Brain Boss James B. Hale, PhD, MEd, ABSNP, ABPdN Visiting Professor of Educational Neuroscience Division of Psychology

More information

Summary of findings from the previous meta-analyses of DTI studies in MDD patients. SDM (39) 221 Left superior longitudinal

Summary of findings from the previous meta-analyses of DTI studies in MDD patients. SDM (39) 221 Left superior longitudinal Supplemental Data Table S1 Summary of findings from the previous meta-analyses of DTI studies in MDD patients Study Analysis Method Included studies, n MDD (medicated) HC Results (MDDHC)

More information

Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Brain and Cognition. journal homepage:

Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Brain and Cognition. journal homepage: Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) 73 83 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Brain and Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Differences in feedback- and inhibition-related neural activity

More information

Neuroimaging in Clinical Practice

Neuroimaging in Clinical Practice Neuroimaging in Clinical Practice John Gabrieli Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences & Martinos Imaging Center at the McGovern Institute for Brain Research, MIT Disclosures Neither I nor my spouse/partner

More information

Week 2: Disorders of Childhood

Week 2: Disorders of Childhood Week 2: Disorders of Childhood What are neurodevelopmental disorders? A group of conditions with onset in the developmental period Disorders of the brain The disorders manifest early in development, often

More information

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of Placebo effects in fmri Supporting online material 1 Supporting online material Materials and Methods Study 1 Procedure and behavioral data We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was

More information

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Overview of the SIIPS1 development. The development of the SIIPS1 consisted of individual- and group-level analysis steps. 1) Individual-person

More information

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Methylphenidate (Ritalin) and Dopamine

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Methylphenidate (Ritalin) and Dopamine Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Methylphenidate (Ritalin) and Dopamine 625 Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Methylphenidate (Ritalin) and Dopamine C J Vaidya, Georgetown

More information

Use of Multimodal Neuroimaging Techniques to Examine Age, Sex, and Alcohol-Related Changes in Brain Structure Through Adolescence and Young Adulthood

Use of Multimodal Neuroimaging Techniques to Examine Age, Sex, and Alcohol-Related Changes in Brain Structure Through Adolescence and Young Adulthood American Psychiatric Association San Diego, CA 24 May 2017 Use of Multimodal Neuroimaging Techniques to Examine Age, Sex, and Alcohol-Related Changes in Brain Structure Through Adolescence and Young Adulthood

More information

Biological Risk Factors

Biological Risk Factors Biological Risk Factors Ms Angelina Crea Provisional Psychologist Academic Child Psychiatry Unit Royal Children s Hospital Professor Alasdair Vance Head Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics

More information

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective MILITARY PSYCHOLOGY, 21:(Suppl. 1)S108 S112, 2009 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0899-5605 print / 1532-7876 online DOI: 10.1080/08995600802554748 Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future:

More information

The shift in nosology from the Diagnostic PROCEEDINGS FROM CHILDHOOD TO ADOLESCENCE: DIAGNOSIS AND COMORBIDITY ISSUES * Thomas J. Spencer, MD ABSTRACT

The shift in nosology from the Diagnostic PROCEEDINGS FROM CHILDHOOD TO ADOLESCENCE: DIAGNOSIS AND COMORBIDITY ISSUES * Thomas J. Spencer, MD ABSTRACT FROM CHILDHOOD TO ADOLESCENCE: DIAGNOSIS AND COMORBIDITY ISSUES * Thomas J. Spencer, MD ABSTRACT Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) tends to manifest differently in adolescents than in children,

More information

Impact of Comorbidities on Self-Esteem of Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Impact of Comorbidities on Self-Esteem of Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder The International Journal of Indian Psychology ISSN 2348-5396 (e) ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) Volume 3, Issue 3, No.1, DIP: 18.01.011/20160303 ISBN: 978-1-365-03416-9 http://www.ijip.in April - June, 2016 Impact

More information

ADHD in the Preschool Aged Child

ADHD in the Preschool Aged Child ADHD in the Preschool Aged Child (PATS) 11/2/2013 Stephen Meister MD, MHA, FAAP The Edmund N Ervin Pediatric Center (PATS) National Institute of Mental Health study First papers published in 2006 after

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Braver et al. 10.1073/pnas.0808187106 SI Methods Participants. Participants were neurologically normal, righthanded younger or older adults. The groups did not differ in gender breakdown

More information

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism Justin H G Williams Morven McWhirr Gordon D Waiter Cambridge Sept 10 th 2010 Joint attention in autism Declarative and receptive aspects initiating

More information

Meta-analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies of Inhibition and Attention in Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Meta-analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies of Inhibition and Attention in Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder META-ANALYSIS Meta-analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies of Inhibition and Attention in Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Exploring Task-Specific, Stimulant Medication, and Age

More information

Attention & Planning. Prevalence. ADHD and Attention. Incidence. Incidence. ADHD: Updates and Practical Suggestions 4/4/2018

Attention & Planning. Prevalence. ADHD and Attention. Incidence. Incidence. ADHD: Updates and Practical Suggestions 4/4/2018 ADHD: Updates and Practical Suggestions Attention & Planning Christopher Boys, PhD Associate Professor of Pediatrics University of Minnesota ADHD and Attention Children with ADHD engage in high risk behaviors

More information

EEG Power Spectrum Analysis in Children with ADHD

EEG Power Spectrum Analysis in Children with ADHD Yonago Acta medica 2016;59:169 173 Original Article EEG Power Spectrum Analysis in Children with ADHD Akira Kamida, Kenta Shimabayashi, Masayoshi Oguri, Toshihiro Takamori, Naoyuki Ueda, Yuki Koyanagi,

More information

The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load

The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load Vogan et al. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 2014, 6:19 RESEARCH Open Access The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive

More information

Determining Medication Treatment. Neuropsychological Impairment Matter?

Determining Medication Treatment. Neuropsychological Impairment Matter? Determining Medication Treatment Response in ADHD: Does Neuropsychological Impairment Matter? James B. Hale, PhD, MEd, ABPdN Pediatric Neuropsychologist Center for Teaching Brain Literacy Olympia, WA Contact:

More information

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver NRSC January 28, 2010 Costs: Health, Crime, Productivity Costs in billions of dollars (2002) $400 $350 $400B legal

More information

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4 Table S1: Brain regions involved in the adapted classification learning task Brain Regions x y z Z Anterior Cingulate

More information

Book review. Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS). By C.K. Conners, D. Erhardt, M.A. Sparrow. New York: Multihealth Systems, Inc.

Book review. Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS). By C.K. Conners, D. Erhardt, M.A. Sparrow. New York: Multihealth Systems, Inc. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 431 437 Book review Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS). By C.K. Conners, D. Erhardt, M.A. Sparrow. New York: Multihealth Systems, Inc., 1999 1. Test

More information

The Multi-Source Interference Task: The Effect of Randomization

The Multi-Source Interference Task: The Effect of Randomization Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27:711 717, 2005 Copyright Taylor & Francis Ltd. ISSN: 1380-3395 DOI: 10.1080/13803390490918516 NCEN 1380-3395 Journal of Clinical and Experimental

More information

Is ADHD Always a Childhood Onset Disorder? Towards Understanding Adult Onset ADHD

Is ADHD Always a Childhood Onset Disorder? Towards Understanding Adult Onset ADHD Is ADHD Always a Childhood Onset Disorder? Towards Understanding Adult Onset ADHD Joseph Biederman, MD Professor of Psychiatry Harvard Medical School Chief, Clinical and Research Programs in Pediatric

More information

CONNERS K-CPT 2. Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

CONNERS K-CPT 2. Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. CONNERS K-CPT 2 Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. Assessment Report Name/ID: Jen Sample / 334 Age: 5 Gender: Female Birth Date: June 30, 2008 Grade: Administration

More information

Kevin T. Blake, Ph.D., P.L.C. Tucson, Arizona Cross Country Education Brentwood, Tennessee. All Rights Reserved Kevin T. Blake, Ph.D., P.L.C.

Kevin T. Blake, Ph.D., P.L.C. Tucson, Arizona Cross Country Education Brentwood, Tennessee. All Rights Reserved Kevin T. Blake, Ph.D., P.L.C. Developmentally Disconnected: Evidence-Based Tools for Transforming Social Competence March 2014 Update Kevin T. Blake, Ph.D., P.L.C. Tucson, Arizona Cross Country Education Brentwood, Tennessee 1 Neurobiofeedback

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Online supplementary information for: Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Joel L. Voss, Brian D. Gonsalves, Kara D. Federmeier,

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation

Cover Page. The handle  holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/32078 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation Author: Pannekoek, Nienke Title: Using novel imaging approaches in affective disorders

More information

Sex influences on material-sensitive functional lateralization in working and episodic memory: Men and women are not all that different

Sex influences on material-sensitive functional lateralization in working and episodic memory: Men and women are not all that different www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 32 (2006) 411 422 Sex influences on material-sensitive functional lateralization in working and episodic memory: Men and women are not all that different Kristen

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M.

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M. Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making Johanna M. Jarcho 1,2 Elliot T. Berkman 3 Matthew D. Lieberman 3 1 Department of Psychiatry

More information

Supporting Information. Demonstration of effort-discounting in dlpfc

Supporting Information. Demonstration of effort-discounting in dlpfc Supporting Information Demonstration of effort-discounting in dlpfc In the fmri study on effort discounting by Botvinick, Huffstettler, and McGuire [1], described in detail in the original publication,

More information

For more than 100 years, extremely hyperactive

For more than 100 years, extremely hyperactive 8 W H A T W E K N O W AD/HD Predominantly Inattentive Type For more than 100 years, extremely hyperactive children have been recognized as having behavioral problems. In the 1970s, doctors recognized that

More information

The Adolescent Developmental Stage

The Adolescent Developmental Stage The Adolescent Developmental Stage o Physical maturation o Drive for independence o Increased salience of social and peer interactions o Brain development o Inflection in risky behaviors including experimentation

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Huijser, C. (2011). Neuroimaging studies in pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder

Citation for published version (APA): Huijser, C. (2011). Neuroimaging studies in pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Neuroimaging studies in pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder Huijser, C. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Huijser, C. (2011). Neuroimaging

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Brain Gray Matter Deficits at 33-Year Follow-up in Adults With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Established in Childhood

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Brain Gray Matter Deficits at 33-Year Follow-up in Adults With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Established in Childhood ORIGINAL ARTICLE Brain Gray Matter Deficits at 33-Year Follow-up in Adults With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Established in Childhood Erika Proal, PhD; Philip T. Reiss, PhD; Rachel G. Klein,

More information

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H PSYC H O PAT H O LO G Y : PSYC H I AT R I C P R O B LEMS A N D T H E A S SO C I

More information

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Supplementary Information Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex David V. Smith 1-3, Benjamin Y. Hayden 1,4, Trong-Kha Truong 2,5, Allen W. Song 2,5, Michael L.

More information

Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks

Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks Jinglei Lv 1,2, Dajiang Zhu 2, Xintao Hu 1, Xin Zhang 1,2, Tuo Zhang 1,2, Junwei Han 1, Lei Guo 1,2, and Tianming Liu 2 1 School

More information

Translational Clinical Science

Translational Clinical Science Translational Clinical Science Alasdair Vance Head, Academic Child Psychiatry Department of Paediatrics University of Melbourne Royal Children s Hospital Outline of Presentation 1. Translational Clinical

More information

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Supplemental Information Table of Contents 2 Behavioral Data 2 Table S1. Participant demographics

More information

VIII. 10. Right Temporal-Lobe Contribution to the Retrieval of Family Relationships in Person Identification

VIII. 10. Right Temporal-Lobe Contribution to the Retrieval of Family Relationships in Person Identification CYRIC Annual Report 2009 VIII. 10. Right Temporal-Lobe Contribution to the Retrieval of Family Relationships in Person Identification Abe N. 1, Fujii T. 1, Ueno A. 1, Shigemune Y. 1, Suzuki M. 2, Tashiro

More information

Dorsolateral and Orbital Frontal Subcortical. Differential Diagnosis and Treatment

Dorsolateral and Orbital Frontal Subcortical. Differential Diagnosis and Treatment Dorsolateral and Orbital Frontal Subcortical Circuit Impairment in ADHD: Implications for Differential Diagnosis and Treatment James B. Hale, PhD, ABPdN Pediatric Neuropsychologist Professor of Paediatrics,

More information

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions

Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Supplementary Material S3 Further Seed Regions Figure I. Changes in connectivity with the right anterior insular cortex. (A) wake > mild sedation, showing a reduction in connectivity between the anterior

More information

Objective Measures of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Pilot Study

Objective Measures of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Pilot Study Objective Measures of -Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Pilot Study Itai Berger MD 1 and Gil Goldzweig PhD 2 1 Neuro-Pediatric Unit, Hadassah-Hebrew University Medical Center, Jerusalem, Israel 2 Clinical

More information

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Disclaimers Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Paul Glasier, Ph.D. Licensed Psychologist I have no relevant financial relationships with the manufacturers(s) of any commercial products(s)

More information

A Peek Inside the Mystery. ADD/ADHD Students. June, 2015 Presented by: Bryan Harris, Ed.D.

A Peek Inside the Mystery. ADD/ADHD Students. June, 2015 Presented by: Bryan Harris, Ed.D. A Peek Inside the Mystery Understanding andteaching ADD/ADHD Students June, 2015 Presented by: Bryan Harris, Ed.D. But, by the time think about it, I ve Ive already done it. Question: Can you identify

More information

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders: Are Children Being Overmedicated?

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders: Are Children Being Overmedicated? U.S. Department of Health and Human Services National Institutes of Health National Institute of Mental Health September 26, 2002 NOTE TO WRITERS AND EDITORS: Dr. Richard Nakamura, Acting Director of the

More information