Trigeminal high-frequency stimulation produces short- and long-term modification of reflex blink gain

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Trigeminal high-frequency stimulation produces short- and long-term modification of reflex blink gain"

Transcription

1 J Neurophysiol : , 24. First published November 27, 23; doi:.52/jn Trigeminal high-frequency stimulation produces short- and long-term modification of reflex blink gain Michael Ryan, Jaime Kaminer, 2 Patricia Enmore, 4 and Craig Evinger,3 Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, New York; 2 Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, New York; 3 SUNY Eye Institute, Stony Brook, New York; and 4 Program in Neuroscience, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, New York Submitted 7 September 23; accepted in final form 22 November 23 Ryan M, Kaminer J, Enmore P, Evinger C. Trigeminal highfrequency stimulation produces short- and long-term modification of reflex blink gain. J Neurophysiol : , 24. First published November 27, 23; doi:.52/jn Reflex blinks provide a model system for investigating motor learning in normal and pathological states. We investigated whether high-frequency stimulation (HFS) of the supraorbital branch of the trigeminal nerve before the R2 blink component (HFS-B) decreases reflex blink gain in alert rats. As with humans (Mao JB, Evinger C. J Neurosci 2: RC5, 2), HFS-B significantly reduced blink size in the first hour after treatment for rats. Repeated days of HFS-B treatment produced long-term depression of blink circuits. Blink gain decreased exponentially across days, indicating a long-term depression of blink circuits. Additionally, the HFS-B protocol became more effective at depressing blink amplitude across days of treatment. This depression was not habituation, because neither long- nor short-term blink changes occurred when HFS was presented after the R2. To investigate whether gain modifications produced by HFS-B involved cerebellar networks, we trained rats in a delay eyelid conditioning paradigm using HFS-B as the unconditioned stimulus and a tone as the conditioned stimulus. As HFS-B depresses blink circuits and delay conditioning enhances blink circuit activity, occlusion should occur if they share neural networks. Rats acquiring robust eyelid conditioning did not exhibit decreases in blink gain, whereas rats developing low levels of eyelid conditioning exhibited weak, short-term reductions in blink gain. These results suggested that delay eyelid conditioning and long-term HFS-B utilize some of the same cerebellar circuits. The ability of repeated HFS-B treatment to depress trigeminal blink circuit activity long term implied that it may be a useful protocol to reduce hyperexcitable blink circuits that underlie diseases like benign essential blepharospasm. blepharospasm; cerebellum; gain modification; motor learning; trigeminal nerve Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: C. Evinger, Dept. of Neurobiology and Behavior, Stony Brook Univ., Stony Brook, NY ( leslie.evinger@stonybrook.edu). MAMMALS SHARE a common motor control plan for trigeminal reflex blinks (Basso et al. 993, 996; Basso and Evinger 996; Gnadt et al. 997) and exhibit nearly identical patterns of muscle activity for all other types of blinking (Aramideh et al. 994; Evinger et al. 984, 99; Evinger and Manning 993; Kaminer et al. 2). These homologies make trigeminal reflex blinks an ideal model system for investigating motor learning and its relationship with movement disorders. For example, pathological reductions in lid motility and eye irritation initiate motor learning in trigeminal blink circuits (Evinger and Manning 988; Kassem and Evinger 26; Schicatano et al. 22) and eyelid dystonias appear to be exaggerations of this normally adaptive motor learning (Evinger et al. 22; Hallett et al. 28). It is possible to investigate these interactions between motor learning and disease states with high-frequency stimulation (HFS) of the supraorbital branch of the trigeminal nerve (SO) to create a short-term modification of reflex blink gain (Mao and Evinger 2). The HFS paradigm identifies motor learning abnormalities in Parkinson s disease, dystonia, and Tourette syndrome (Battaglia et al. 26; Crupi et al. 28; Quartarone et al. 26; Suppa et al. 2). Determining how these neurological disorders disrupt HFS gain modifications requires identification of the neural bases of this form of motor learning. Short-term HFS-induced modifications occur in trigeminal blink circuits only on the side of the brain receiving HFS. For example, after HFS treatment of the left SO to increase blink gain, stimulating the left SO evokes larger than normal blinks whereas right SO stimulation elicits normal-amplitude blinks. This pattern is analogous to other blink adaptations initiated by reduced eyelid motility (Cossu et al. 999; Evinger et al. 989; Evinger and Manning 988; Kassem and Evinger 26; Manca et al. 2; Schicatano et al. 22; VanderWerf et al. 27). It is unknown, however, whether HFS treatment reproduces the long-term changes in blink gain that occur with reductions in lid motility with facial palsy (Abell et al. 998; Manca et al. 2; VanderWerf et al. 27). We address the long-term effects by examining the consequence of multiple days of HFS treatment on blink reflex gain. Given the role of the cerebellum in gain modifications of blinking and other motor systems (Blazquez et al. 24; Boyden et al. 24; Evinger et al. 989; Hopp and Fuchs 24; Pellegrini and Evinger 997; Raymond et al. 996; Scudder and McGee 23; Soetedjo et al. 28), we delineate some of the critical neural circuits in HFS blink modification by combining HFS treatment designed to depress blink amplitude with cerebellum-dependent delay eyelid conditioning that increases blink circuit activity (Christian and Thompson 23; De Zeeuw and Yeo 25; McCormick and Thompson 984; Thompson 25; Topka et al. 993). We posit that if delay conditioning and HFS-induced modifications utilize overlapping cerebellar circuits, then some occlusion will occur between the two forms of learning. MATERIALS AND METHODS Experiments were performed on 2 male Sprague-Dawley rats (75 55 g) maintained on a reversed 2:2-h light-dark cycle and fed ad libitum. Data were collected during the rats subjective night. All experiments received approval by the Stony Brook University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and complied with all /4 Copyright 24 the American Physiological Society Downloaded from by ${individualuser.givennames} ${individualuser.surname} ( ) on February 9, 28. Copyright 24 American Physiological Society. All rights reserved.

2 MODIFYING REFLEX BLINK GAIN 889 federal, state, and university regulations regarding the use of animals in research. Surgery. Under general anesthesia (9 mg/kg ketamine and mg/kg xylazine), rats were prepared for chronic recording of the orbicularis oculi (OO) EMG (OOemg) and SO stimulation (Dauvergne and Evinger 27; Evinger et al. 993). To record the OOemg, a pair of Teflon-coated wires (.3-in. diameter bare,.55 in. coated; no. 79, A-M Systems, Everett, WA) with mm exposed at the tip were implanted in the OO muscle below the lateral canthus. To stimulate the SO nerve, a nerve cuff containing a pair of stainless steel wires with the insulation removed (.3-in. diameter bare,.55 in. coated; no. 79, A-M Systems) encased in Teflon tubing (-mm diameter; no. 633, Small Parts, Miami, FL) was placed around the SO branch of the trigeminal nerve. Wires were led subcutaneously to a connector embedded in a dental acrylic platform on the skull. The platform was attached to the skull by four stainless steel screws. A silver wire connected to one of the stainless steel screws served as the ground. Rats received an analgesic (ketorolac, 7 mg/kg) for at least 24 h after the surgery. Rats were alert and eating within 24 h of the surgery. The experiments began at least wk after the surgery. Stimulation paradigm. The SO stimulus in all experiments was relative to the minimum current at which a unipolar - s stimulus reliably elicited the R component of a reflex blink, threshold (T). This current was determined at the beginning of each day for each rat A R SO Alone R2 E Pre 2 trials and was held constant throughout that day s experiment. Across all subjects and days tested, the range of threshold currents was 9 A, with a median of 25 A. For each rat, threshold varied little across days. The range of coefficients of variation for thresholds across all rats was.25.35, with a mean of.3.3. All data were collected at twice threshold (2T), a stimulus intensity that evoked a strong R response and an inconsistent and small R2 component in rats (Fig. A) (Basso et al. 993; Dauvergne and Evinger 27; Evinger et al. 993). The HFS train was five 2T SO stimuli presented at 4 Hz. Each day s data collection consisted of five blocks: ) before treatment, 2) treatment condition, 3) immediately after treatment, 4) 3 min after treatment, and 5) 6 min after treatment (Fig. E). Preand posttreatment blocks were the same for all experiments. In these blocks, rats received 2 trials of paired 2T SO stimuli with an interstimulus interval of ms. Thus two blinks were evoked in each nontreatment trial. The first evoked blink was called the condition blink, and the second blink was termed the test blink. The intertrial interval for all data blocks varied pseudorandomly over the range of 2 5 s. Each rat received at least days of a given treatment. Treatment conditions. One group (n ) underwent a blink motor learning paradigm to depress blink amplitude (Mao and Evinger 2). As with the human study, rats in this treatment group received 6 trials of high-frequency SO stimulation that occurred during the R but before the R2 component of the blink (HFS-B). Each treatment ms B HFS-B HFS-B 6 trials 6 trials HFS Cond 6 trials T 2 trials C T3 2 trials T6 2 trials Fig.. Treatment protocols. A: supraorbital branch of the trigeminal nerve (SO)-evoked reflex blink with R and R2 components. B: high-frequency stimulation (HFS) presented before R2 (HFS-B). C: HFS presented after R2 (). D, right: HFS Cond trial, in which HFS-B coterminated with a 3-ms tone. Left: expansion of the same record to show the HFS-B stimulus. E: experimental paradigm. Triangles show a twice-threshold - s supraorbital nerve stimulus. T, T3, T6, immediately, 3 min, and 6 min after treatment. D HFS Cond 25 ms tone 3 ms tone HFS J Neurophysiol doi:.52/jn Downloaded from by ${individualuser.givennames} ${individualuser.surname} ( ) on February 9, 28. Copyright 24 American Physiological Society. All rights reserved.

3 89 MODIFYING REFLEX BLINK GAIN trial consisted of a single SO stimulus at 2T to evoke a reflex blink followed by five 2T SO stimuli delivered at 4 Hz before the onset of the R2 component of the OOemg activity (Fig. B). The latency between first SO stimulus and HFS was adjusted for each rat based on their average R2 latency. The second group (n 8) underwent delay eyelid conditioning (HFS Cond) in which the HFS-B stimulus served as the unconditioned stimulus (US) and a tone served as the conditioned stimulus (CS) (Fig. D). Three rats in the HFS Cond group also contributed to the HFS-B group data. Each of the 6 paired trials consisted of a 3-ms CS with a 4-ms rise time that coterminated with the HFS-B US. Four rats were trained with a 2-kHz tone and the other four with an 8-kHz tone. Both tones were 85 db. Two rats in each CS group developed robust conditioning (see Fig. 4A). During the HFS Cond treatment each day, rats received paired CS-US trials followed by a CS alone on the eleventh trial and a single 2T SO stimulus on the twelfth trial. We presented 6 of these 2 trial blocks each day, delivering a total of 6 CS HFS-B, 6 CS alone, and 6 SO alone trials. Thus rats in the HFS-Cond treatment received the same number of HFS-B stimuli as rats in the HFS-B alone condition. In the third group (n 4), HFS occurred after the R2 component of the blink reflex (). This protocol did not modify blink amplitude in humans (Mao and Evinger 2) and served as a control for the number of stimuli delivered in the other treatment conditions. Rats received 6 trials of stimulation in which the HFS occurred after termination of the R2 blink component (Fig. C). Latency between first SO blink evoking stimulus and HFS was adjusted for each rat based on the average latency and duration of the R2 component. Data collection and analysis. Reflex and conditioned blinks were monitored as the rats moved freely in their home cage in a darkened room during their subjective night. OOemg signals were amplified (A-M Systems model 7; four-channel differential amplifier), filtered at.3 5 khz, collected at 4 khz per channel (DT 283, Data Translation, Marlboro, MA; 2-bit analog-to-digital resolution), and stored for later off-line analysis on laboratory-developed software. Blink amplitude was determined by integrating the rectified OOemg activity between the beginning and end of each blink component (Dauvergne and Evinger 27; Evinger et al. 99; Pellegrini et al. 995; Pellegrini and Evinger 995). Because stimulus intensity was constant within a day, changes in blink amplitude after HFS were changes in trigeminal blink reflex gain. We normalized within-day treatment-induced changes in blink amplitude by dividing both the average pre-hfs and the average post-hfs EMG amplitude by the median pre-hfs EMG amplitude for that day. As there were no consistent differences in data between post-hfs blocks, we combined them for analysis. We quantified the effect of the HFS treatment by subtracting the normalized pre-hfs blink amplitude from the normalized post-hfs blink amplitude and termed this measure relative gain change. A negative value demonstrated a decrease in blink gain, whereas a positive value indicated an increased blink gain. We calculated two measures of long-term modifications induced by HFS treatment. For each day of treatment, we determined the mean amplitude of 2T SO-evoked blinks before treatment relative to the median pretreatment amplitude on the first day of testing. If treatment produced a long-term depression in the trigeminal blink amplitude, then pretreatment blink amplitude should decrease across days of HFS treatment. Second, to determine whether the HFS treatment created a larger relative change after repeated days of treatment, we plotted the relative change each day as a function of the number of days of treatment. If the treatment became more effective at modifying trigeminal reflex blink gain after repeated treatments, then there should be greater relative change across days of treatment. In addition to reflex blinks, conditioned responses (CRs) in the HFS Cond treatment were analyzed. OOemg activity in the first 35 ms of tone onset was designated as an alpha response and not analyzed. A CR was defined as OOemg activity 5 standard deviations above the baseline OOemg activity determined from the ms of OOemg activity prior to tone onset. Trials in which OOemg baseline activity was disrupted by other activities, e.g., face cleaning, were not analyzed. Percent CRs was calculated as the number of trials exhibiting a CR divided by the total number of useable CS trials. Statistical tests of significance (P.5) were performed with SPSS software (SPSS, Chicago, IL) using an independent-samples t-test or a paired-samples t-test. Data are presented as means SE. Regressions were performed with linear, power, logarithmic, and exponential curves. The function generating the highest correlation was employed to describe the data. RESULTS In humans, the HFS-B condition depressed the R2 component of subsequent blinks (Mao and Evinger 2). The R response, however, is not reported in this study because the R response is inconsistent and provides very little lid closure in primates (Evinger et al. 99; Snow and Frith 989). In mammals other than primates, however, the R component provides most of the lid closure (Pellegrini et al. 995), whereas the R2 response is weak and inconsistent (Anor et al. 996, 2; Basso et al. 993; LeDoux et al. 997; Pellegrini et al. 995). Thus we investigated changes in R magnitude. Averaged across all rats and days, HFS-B treatment significantly depressed subsequent condition R and test R responses by % [t (5) 9.7, P.; Fig. 2A, left] and % [t (5) 2.2, P.5; Fig. 2A, left], respectively, relative to pre-hfs-b R values. Before HFS treatment, a condition R2 blink response occurred on slightly more than half of the trials, 57 3%. After HFS-B treatment, the probability of a condition R2 occurring fell to 8.4%, significantly less than before treatment [t (5) 5.3, P.]. As with humans, treatment did not alter the R2 component of blinks (Mao and Evinger 2); treatment in the rat also did not significantly affect R blink amplitude (Fig. 2A, right). Averaged across all rats and days of treatment, treatment produced an insignificant % decrease [t (4).53, P.5; Fig. 2A, right] and % increase [t (4).46, P.5; Fig. 2A, right] in condition R and test R blink amplitude, respectively. Thus, as occurred with the human R2, the timing of the HFS relative to the R2 determined the effect of HFS on trigeminal blink circuits. In the rat, HFS-B treatment created a short-term reduction in R blink gain. Averaging across days, however, obscured two long-term modifications in trigeminal blink circuits created by repeated HFS-B treatment. First, R blink amplitude collected before HFS-B treatment decreased with repeated days of HFS-B treatment (Fig. 2B). Normalizing the mean R blink amplitude before HFS-B each day to the median R blink amplitude before HFS-B on the first day of treatment revealed a significant exponential decrease across days of HFS-B treatment for both condition R (r 2.49, P.5; Fig. 2B) and test R (r 2.4, P.5; Fig. 2B) blink amplitude. The second long-term modification caused by repeated HFS-B treatment was the enhanced ability of HFS-B treatment to cause relative gain decreases (r 2.3; Fig. 2C). Over the first 3 days of HFS-B treatment, the rats showed an average relative change in condition R amplitude of 6%, whereas over the last 3 days of HFS-B treatment the average relative change was 27%. The J Neurophysiol doi:.52/jn Downloaded from by ${individualuser.givennames} ${individualuser.surname} ( ) on February 9, 28. Copyright 24 American Physiological Society. All rights reserved.

4 Relative Change Pre HFS re Day Relative Change A * B C HFS-B 3 2 RC RT Days of HFS-B R2 Probability MODIFYING REFLEX BLINK GAIN 4 D HFS-B E F Days of HFS Days of 89 Fig. 2. A: average relative change in condition R (RC) and test R (RT) blink amplitude following HFS treatment. B: average pre-hfs blink amplitude relative to median pre-hfs blink amplitude on day for RC and RT as a function of days of HFS-B treatment. Lines are best fit exponentials. C: average relative change for RC and RT as a function of days of HFS-B treatment. Lines are logarithmic best fits. D: average condition R2 (R2C) probability as a function of days of HFS-B and treatment. Lines are best fit linear regressions. E: average pre-hfs blink amplitude relative to median pre-hfs blink amplitude on day for RC and RT as a function of days of treatment. F: average relative change for RC and RT as a function of days of treatment. *P.5, P.. Error bars are SE Days of HFS-B Days of test R showed a similar, but smaller, change in relative gain across days of treatment (r 2.4; Fig. 2C). Repeated days of HFS-B decreased the probability of an R2 occurring before HFS-B treatment (r 2.73, P.; Fig. 2D). Thus the decreasing R blink amplitude and R2 probability demonstrated that repeated HFS-B treatment reduced trigeminal drive on blink circuits. In addition, repeated HFS-B treatment made daily HFS-B treatment more effective at decreasing blink gain. Consistent with the absence of short-term effects (Fig. 2A, right), repeated days of treatment failed to produce long-term changes. Repeated days of treatment did not change pretreatment amplitude relative to day pretreatment blink amplitude for condition R (r 2.3, P.5; Fig. 2E) or test R (r 2.2, P.5; Fig. 2E). Likewise, R2 probability did not change with repeated days of treatment (r 2.7, P.5; Fig. 2D). There were also no consistent modifications of relative change across days for condition R (r 2.3, P.5; Fig. 2F) or test R (r 2.27, P.5; Fig. 2F). Indeed, the variability of the data demonstrated that when the HFS occurred after the R2 component it produced no consistent effect on blink amplitude (Fig. 2, E and F). Thus the absence of long-term modifications with treatment demonstrated that the long-term reductions in blink gain created by HFS-B treatment were the result of the timing of HFS relative to the R2 and not generalized habituation. Repeated HFS-B or treatment did not modify blink threshold (Fig. 3A). For each rat and condition, every day s reflex blink threshold was normalized to the threshold current on the first day of treatment. These relative thresholds were averaged across rats for each day of treatment. Blink threshold did not change significantly with repeated days of HFS-B treatment (r 2.32, P.5; Fig. 3A), indicating that the long-term changes in trigeminal blink amplitude were not the result of a change in blink threshold. Likewise, repeated treatment did not alter blink threshold (r 2.3, P.5; Fig. 3A). Thus the HFS-B paradigm produced both shortand long-term decrease in the gain of trigeminal reflex blinks without affecting blink threshold. Delay eyelid conditioning (HFS Cond). The cerebellum plays a critical role in eyelid conditioning (Christian and Thompson 23; De Zeeuw and Yeo 25; Raymond et al. 996; Thompson 25) and trigeminal reflex blink adaptation (Chen and Evinger 26; Evinger and Manning 988; Pellegrini and Evinger 997). If short- and long-term HFS-B depression of trigeminal reflex blinks involves some of the same cerebellar neurons responsible for delay eyelid conditioning, then some occlusion should occur when HFS-B treatment and delay eyelid conditioning are combined, because HFS-B depresses blink circuits (Fig. 2A, left) whereas eyelid conditioning increases the activity in blink circuits. If occlusion occurred between the two paradigms, then we expected that rats failing to develop eyelid conditioning would generate small gain decreases whereas rats that developed robust eyelid conditioning would not show blink gain changes with HFS Cond treatment. Using 7% CRs as a threshold for the devel- J Neurophysiol doi:.52/jn Downloaded from by ${individualuser.givennames} ${individualuser.surname} ( ) on February 9, 28. Copyright 24 American Physiological Society. All rights reserved.

5 892 MODIFYING REFLEX BLINK GAIN Relative Blink Threshold A B HFS-B <7% HFS Cond >7% HFS Cond Days of HFS Fig. 3. A: mean blink threshold relative to day blink threshold as a function of days of HFS-B and treatment. B: mean blink threshold relative to day blink threshold as a function of days of HFS Cond treatment for rats achieving 7% conditioned responses (CRs) and rats achieving 7% CRs. Error bars are SE. opment of robust eyelid conditioning, we found four rats that consistently achieved 7% CRs (Fig. 4A) and four rats that failed to attain this level of conditioning ( 7% CRs; Fig. 4A). Averaged across all days and rats that exhibited 7% CRs, condition R amplitude decreased by.8 5.3% after HFS Cond [t (49) 2.25, P.5; Fig. 4B]. As predicted, this decrease in condition R amplitude was significantly smaller than the depression produced by HFS-B treatment alone [t (43) 3., P.]. For rats that achieved 7% CRs, however, neither condition (RC) nor test (RT) R amplitude changed significantly [t (5).2, P.5, RC; t (5).2, P.5, RT; Fig. 4B, 7%]. Comparing blink depression caused by HFS-B treatment with that caused by HFS Cond treatment in the three rats that participated in both experiments reinforced the differences between the two treatments on blink gain. The blink depression with HFS-B treatment for these three rats was not different from that for rats that received HFS-B treatment alone [RC, t (4).6, P.5; RT, t (4).5, P.5; Fig. 4B, HFS-B*]. In the HFS Cond treatment, these rats exhibited less blink depression than they developed in the HFS-B treatment and were similar to the 7% group (Fig. 4B, HFS-Cond*). Thus combining the HFS-B stimulus with eyelid conditioning disrupted both forms of motor learning. The long-term changes with repeated HFS Cond treatment were smaller than those with HFS-B treatment. Looked at across days of HFS Cond treatment, neither the 7% nor the 7% group exhibited a significant change in pre-hfs condition R amplitude (Fig. 4C; 7% r 2.4, P.5; 7% r 2.4, P.5). Blink threshold also did not change across days of HFS Cond (Fig. 3B; r 2.28, P.5, 7%; r 2.22, P.5, 7%). There was a trend for relative change to become more negative across days of HFS Cond treatment for the 7% group (Fig. 4D; r 2.8, P.5) but to become more positive for the 7% group (Fig. 4D; r 2.2, P.5). The trend for larger relative changes across days of treatment for the 7% group was confirmed by averaging relative change across days (Fig. 4E). Averaged over the first 4 days of HFS Cond treatment there was no short-term relative change for rats in the 7% group [.3., t (5).26, P.5; Fig. 4E, 7%], but averaged over days 8 2 the relative change was.23.8 [t (5) 3., P.; Fig. 4E, 7%]. Thus it took at least 8 days of HFS Cond treatment before these rats exhibited a significant short-term decrease in condition R blink gain. In contrast, HFS-B treatment produced a relative change in condition R blink gain of.2.4 [t (43) 4.9, P.; Fig. 4E, HFS-B] in the first 4 days of treatment, and by days 8 2 the mean relative change was.3.5 [t (23) 6., P.] over all of the rats receiving HFS-B. In rats that achieved 7% CRs and rats receiving treatment, there were no significant changes in condition R gain in either the first 4 or the last 4 days of treatment (Fig. 4E). Consistent with occlusion occurring between HFS-B and eyelid conditioning, rats that failed to acquire robust eyelid conditioning only slowly developed short-term gain reductions and never exhibited long-term changes and rats that acquired robust eyelid conditioning never exhibited shortor long-term reductions in blink gain. DISCUSSION HFS treatment in which the 4-Hz SO stimulus train occurs before the R2 component of a SO-evoked blink produces both short- and long-term decreases in trigeminal reflex blink amplitude (Fig. 2, Fig. 4E) independent of short- or long-term habituation (Abel et al. 998; Ferguson et al. 978; Hirano et al. 996; Sanes and Ison 983). For example, matched for the number of HFS-B trains does not cause short (Fig. 2A)- or long (Fig. 2, E and F)-term decreases in blink amplitude. Further evidence against habituation causing the blink depression demonstrated here is that potentiation rather than habituation develops when the HFS occurs during the R2 blink component in humans (Battaglia et al. 26; Crupi et al. 28; Kranz et al. 23; Mao and Evinger 2; Quartarone et al. 26). Independent of the number of HFS trains, however, habituation can occur when the intertrial interval used to collect pre- and post-hfs data is s (Zeuner et al. 2) instead of the 2-s intertrial intervals used in this and other HFS studies. Thus the timing of the HFS relative to the R2 is critical in creating the short- and long-term decreases in blink reflex amplitude. This reduction is a modification of reflex blink gain, because blink amplitude decreases while the blinkevoking stimulus remains constant (Fig. 3A). HFS treatment in humans (Battaglia et al. 26; Crupi et al. 28; Mao and Evinger 2; Quartarone et al. 26; Suppa et al. 2; Zeuner et al. 2) focuses exclusively on changes in the R2 component of SO-evoked blinks (Evinger et al. 99; Snow and Frith 989). In this study, we primarily measured changes in the R component because it produces most of the lid closure in nonprimate mammals (Pellegrini et al. 995). Our data demonstrated that the R showed the same pattern of gain changes as the R2 in human HFS paradigms. The demonstration that R and R2 responses of SO-evoked blinks arise from J Neurophysiol doi:.52/jn Downloaded from by ${individualuser.givennames} ${individualuser.surname} ( ) on February 9, 28. Copyright 24 American Physiological Society. All rights reserved.

6 MODIFYING REFLEX BLINK GAIN 893 Percent CRs Pre HFS re Day Relative Change 75 5 A 25 <7% >7% C.5 <7% >7% E Days of HFS Conditioning Days - 4 Days Days of HFS Conditioning -A ** HFS-B <7% >7% Relative Change Relative Change B.2 HFS-B HFS-B* HFS-Cond* <7% >7% D * Days of HFS Conditioning * RC RT * <7% >7% Fig. 4. A: % CRs as a function of days of HFS Cond for rats that achieved 7% CRs and rats that achieved 7% CRs over 2 days of HFS Cond treatment. B: average relative change in RC and RT blink amplitude following HFS treatment for all HFS-B rats, HFS-B treatment for the 3 rats that participated in both HFS-B and HFS Cond treatments (HFS-B*), HFS Cond treatment for the 3 rats that participated in both HFS-B and HFS Cond treatments (HFS-Cond*), and for all 7% and all 7% rats. C: mean pre- HFS blink amplitude relative to median day pre-hfs blink amplitude as a function of days of HFS Cond treatment for rats that achieved 7% CRs and rats achieving 7% CRs. D: mean relative change as a function of days of HFS Cond treatment for rats in the 7% and 7% groups. Lines are best fit linear regressions. E: mean relative change over days 4 and days 8 2 for all rats in the HFS-B,, 7%, and 7% groups. Error bars are SE. *P.5, **P., P.. different regions of the trigeminal complex and involve different pools of medullary interneurons (Cruccu et al. 25; Ongerboer de Visser 983; Ongerboer de Visser and Kuypers 978; Pellegrini et al. 995) suggests that the blink changes created by HFS treatment reflect modifications in second-order trigeminal neurons and/or OO motoneurons. Repeated HFS-B treatment produced a cumulative depression of trigeminal reflex blink circuits. Over days of HFS-B treatment, trigeminal blink drive fell exponentially, so that a 2T SO stimulus evoked smaller blinks (Fig. 2B). HFS-B treatment also became increasingly effective at reducing blink gain over days of HFS-B treatment (Fig. 2C). Just as facial palsy creates a long-term enhancement of blink circuits (Cossu et al. 999; Manca et al. 2; VanderWerf et al. 27), repeated HFS-B treatment produced a longterm depression of blink circuits. Thus repeated HFS-B treatment created long-term modifications of trigeminal blink circuits and blink gain equivalent to those caused by facial palsy. The cerebellum plays a major role in gain modification of trigeminal reflex blinks, saccadic eye movements, and the vestibuloocular reflex (Blazquez et al. 24; Boyden et al. 24; Lisberger 988; Miles and Lisberger 98; Pellegrini and Evinger 997; Rambold et al. 22; Raymond et al. 996; Robinson et al. 22; Scudder and McGee 23; Soetedjo and Fuchs 26; Straube et al. 2). The present data suggest that the cerebellum also participates in gain modification created by HFS-B treatment. The activity of interpositus neurons changes with trigeminal blink gain modification (Chen and Evinger 26) and delay eyelid conditioning (Freeman and Nicholson 2; Gould and Steinmetz 996; Kim and Thompson 997; Nicholson and Freeman 24). If HFS-B gain modification also involves some of the same pools of interpositus neurons as delay eyelid conditioning, then competition between HFS-B treatment to depress blink circuits and delay eyelid conditioning to excite blink circuits should affect both types of learning. In one group of rats, 7%, robust conditioning did not occur, as evidenced by the inability to exhibit at least 7% CRs in 2 days of conditioning. Thus the excitatory drive created by delay conditioning competed only partially with the decreased drive of HFS-B on blink gain. This competition slowed the development of short-term changes in blink gain and prevented long-term changes (Fig. 4). Rats developing 7% CRs exhibited rates of eyelid conditioning similar to those reported in other studies of eyelid conditioning (Freeman et al. 27; Green et al. 2; Lee and Kim 24; Weiss and Thompson 99). In these rats, eyelid conditioning won the competition for neural resources, blocking short- and long-term blink depression normally produced by HFS-B treatment. The long-term changes created by repeated days of HFS-B treatment suggest the possibility of reducing trigeminal activity in the focal dystonia benign essential blepharospasm (BEB), involving trigeminal overactivity (Hallett et al. 28) and cerebellar abnormalities (Kerrison et al. 23; Obermann et al. 27). The visually disabling spasms of lid closure that characterize BEB appear to be an exaggeration of blink adaptations to dry eye or eye irritation (Evinger et al. 22; Hallett et al. 28) initiated in trigeminal blink circuits (Henriquez and Evinger 27; Schicatano et al. 22) and probably sustained J Neurophysiol doi:.52/jn Downloaded from by ${individualuser.givennames} ${individualuser.surname} ( ) on February 9, 28. Copyright 24 American Physiological Society. All rights reserved.

7 894 MODIFYING REFLEX BLINK GAIN by modifications in cerebellar activity (Evinger et al. 989; Pellegrini and Evinger 997). Other characteristics of BEB, photophobia and excessive blinking, also reflect exaggerated excitability of trigeminal blink circuits (Dolgonos et al. 2; Kaminer et al. 2). A single day of HFS-B treatment, however, does not produce significant clinical changes in lid symptoms, although patients report feeling considerably better after HFS-B (Kranz et al. 23). The lack of a significant reduction in lid abnormalities with a single treatment may reflect the inability of one HFS-B treatment to modify cerebellar circuits significantly. Repeated HFS-B treatment, however, clearly modifies cerebellar circuits to create an exponential reduction in the activity of trigeminal blink circuits and with repetition becomes increasingly effective at short-term gain decreases. This long-term change may be effective at reducing spasms of lid closure and other trigeminal abnormalities. Because BEB patients exhibit enhanced learning in HFS paradigms (Quartarone et al. 26), repeated HFS-B treatment may produce more long-term blink circuit depression than occurs in normal subjects. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Donna Schmidt and Pratibha Thakur for technical assistance and Dr. Alice S. Powers, Pratibha Thakur, and Mala Ananth for helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. Present address of M. Ryan: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Bungtown Rd., Cold Spring Harbor, NY 724. GRANTS This work was supported by grants from the National Eye Institute (EY- 739) and the Thomas Hartman Center for Parkinson Research to C. Evinger and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant F3 NS to J. Kaminer. DISCLOSURES No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the author(s). AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS Author contributions: M.R., J.K., and C.E. conception and design of research; M.R., J.K., and P.E. performed experiments; M.R., J.K., P.E., and C.E. analyzed data; M.R., J.K., and C.E. interpreted results of experiments; M.R., J.K., P.E., and C.E. edited and revised manuscript; M.R., J.K., P.E., and C.E. approved final version of manuscript; C.E. prepared figures; C.E. drafted manuscript. REFERENCES Abel K, Waikar M, Pedro B, Hemsley D, Geyer M. Repeated testing of prepulse inhibition and habituation of the startle reflex: a study in healthy human controls. J Psychopharmacol 2: , 998. Abell KM, Baker RS, Cowen DE, Porter JD. Efficacy of gold weight implants in facial nerve palsy: quantitative alterations in blinking. Vision Res 38: , 998. Anor S, Espadaler JM, Monreal L, Mayhew IG. Electrically induced blink reflex in horses. Vet Rec 39: , 996. Anor S, Espadaler JM, Pastor J, Pumarola M. Electrically induced blink reflex and facial motor nerve stimulation in beagles. J Vet Intern Med 4: , 2. Aramideh M, Ongerboer de Visser BW, Devriese PP, Bour LJ, Speelman JD. Electromyographic features of levator palpebrae superioris and orbicularis oculi muscles in blepharospasm. Brain 7: 27 38, 994. Basso MA, Evinger C. An explanation for reflex blink hyperexcitability in Parkinson s disease. II. Nucleus raphe magnus. J Neurosci 6: , 996. Basso MA, Powers AS, Evinger C. An explanation for reflex blink hyperexcitability in Parkinson s disease. I. Superior colliculus. J Neurosci 6: , 996. Basso MA, Strecker RE, Evinger C. Midbrain 6-hydroxydopamine lesions modulate blink reflex excitability. Exp Brain Res 94: 88 96, 993. Battaglia F, Ghilardi MF, Quartarone A, Bagnato S, Girlanda P, Hallett M. Impaired long-term potentiation-like plasticity of the trigeminal blink reflex circuit in Parkinson s disease. Mov Disord 2: , 26. Blazquez PM, Hirata Y, Highstein SM. The vestibulo-ocular reflex as a model system for motor learning: what is the role of the cerebellum? CR Biol 3: 88 92, 24. Boyden ES, Katoh A, Raymond JL. Cerebellum-dependent learning: the role of multiple plasticity mechanisms. Annu Rev Neurosci 27: 58 69, 24. Chen FP, Evinger C. Cerebellar modulation of trigeminal reflex blinks: interpositus neurons. J Neurosci 26: , 26. Christian KM, Thompson RF. Neural substrates of eyeblink conditioning: acquisition and retention. Learn Mem : , 23. Cossu G, Valls-Sole J, Valldeoriola F, Munoz E, Benitez P, Aguilar F. Reflex excitability of facial motoneurons at onset of muscle reinnervation after facial nerve palsy. Muscle Nerve 22: 64 62, 999. Cruccu G, Iannetti GD, Marx JJ, Thoemke F, Truini A, Fitzek S, Galeotti F, Urban PP, Romaniello A, Stoeter P, Manfredi M, Hopf HC. Brainstem reflex circuits revisited. Brain 28: , 25. Crupi D, Ghilardi MF, Mosiello C, Di Rocco A, Quartarone A, Battaglia F. Cortical and brainstem LTP-like plasticity in Huntington s disease. Brain Res Bull 75: 7 4, 28. Dauvergne C, Evinger C. Experiential modification of the trigeminal reflex blink circuit. J Neurosci 27: , 27. De Zeeuw CI, Yeo CH. Time and tide in cerebellar memory formation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 5: , 25. Dolgonos S, Ayyala H, Evinger C. Light-induced trigeminal sensitization without central visual pathways: another mechanism for photophobia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 52: , 2. Evinger C, Basso MA, Manning KA, Sibony PA, Pellegrini JJ, Horn AK. A role for the basal ganglia in nicotinic modulation of the blink reflex. Exp Brain Res 92: 57 55, 993. Evinger C, Manning KA. A model system for motor learning: adaptive gain control of the blink reflex. Exp Brain Res 7: , 988. Evinger C, Manning KA. Pattern of extraocular muscle activation during reflex blinking. Exp Brain Res 92: 52 56, 993. Evinger C, Manning KA, Sibony PA. Eyelid movements. Mechanisms and normal data. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 32: 387 4, 99. Evinger C, Mao JB, Powers AS, Kassem IS, Schicatano EJ, Henriquez VM, Peshori KR. Dry eye, blinking, and blepharospasm. Mov Disord 7, Suppl 2: S75 S78, 22. Evinger C, Pellegrini JJ, Manning KA. Adaptive gain modification of the blink reflex. A model system for investigating the physiologic bases of motor learning. Ann NY Acad Sci 563: 87, 989. Evinger C, Shaw MD, Peck CK, Manning KA, Baker R. Blinking and associated eye movements in humans, guinea pigs, and rabbits. J Neurophysiol 52: , 984. Ferguson IT, Lenman JA, Johnston BB. Habituation of the orbicularis oculi reflex in dementia and dyskinetic states. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 4: , 978. Freeman JH, Halverson HE, Hubbard EM. Inferior colliculus lesions impair eyeblink conditioning in rats. Learn Mem 4: , 27. Freeman JH Jr, Nicholson DA. Developmental changes in eye-blink conditioning and neuronal activity in the cerebellar interpositus nucleus. J Neurosci 2: 83 89, 2. Gnadt JW, Lu SM, Breznen B, Basso MA, Henriquez VM, Evinger C. Influence of the superior colliculus on the primate blink reflex. Exp Brain Res 6: , 997. Gould TJ, Steinmetz JE. Changes in rabbit cerebellar cortical and interpositus nucleus activity during acquisition, extinction, and backward classical eyelid conditioning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 65: 7 34, 996. Green JT, Rogers RF, Goodlett CR, Steinmetz JE. Impairment in eyeblink classical conditioning in adult rats exposed to ethanol as neonates. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 24: , 2. Hallett M, Evinger C, Jankovic J, Stacy M. Update on blepharospasm: report from the BEBRF International Workshop. Neurology 7: , 28. Henriquez VM, Evinger C. The three-neuron corneal reflex circuit and modulation of second-order corneal responsive neurons. Exp Brain Res 79: 69 72, 27. J Neurophysiol doi:.52/jn Downloaded from by ${individualuser.givennames} ${individualuser.surname} ( ) on February 9, 28. Copyright 24 American Physiological Society. All rights reserved.

8 MODIFYING REFLEX BLINK GAIN 895 Hirano C, Russell AT, Ornitz EM, Liu M. Habituation of P3 and reflex motor (startle blink) responses to repetitive startling stimuli in children. Int J Psychophysiol 22: 97 9, 996. Hopp JJ, Fuchs AF. The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity. Prog Neurobiol 72: 27 53, 24. Kaminer J, Powers AS, Horn KG, Hui C, Evinger C. Characterizing the spontaneous blink generator: an animal model. J Neurosci 3: , 2. Kassem IS, Evinger C. Asymmetry of blinking. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 47: 95 2, 26. Kerrison JB, Lancaster JL, Zamarripa FE, Richardson LA, Morrison JC, Holck DE, Andreason KW, Blaydon SM, Fox PT. Positron emission tomography scanning in essential blepharospasm. Am J Ophthalmol 36: , 23. Kim JJ, Thompson RF. Cerebellar circuits and synaptic mechanisms involved in classical eyeblink conditioning. Trends Neurosci 2: 77 8, 997. Kranz G, Shamim EA, Lin PT, Kranz GS, Hallett M. Long-term depression-like plasticity of the blink reflex for the treatment of blepharospasm. Mov Disord 22: , 23. LeDoux MS, Lorden JF, Weir AD, Smith JM. Blink reflex to supraorbital nerve stimulation in the cat. Exp Brain Res 6: 4 2, 997. Lee T, Kim JJ. Differential effects of cerebellar, amygdalar, and hippocampal lesions on classical eyeblink conditioning in rats. J Neurosci 24: , 24. Lisberger SG. The neural basis for motor learning in the vestibulo-ocular reflex in monkeys. Trends Neurosci : 47 52, 988. Manca D, Munoz E, Pastor P, Valldeoriola F, Valls-Sole J. Enhanced gain of blink reflex responses to ipsilateral supraorbital nerve afferent inputs in patients with facial nerve palsy. Clin Neurophysiol 2: 53 56, 2. Mao JB, Evinger C. Long-term potentiation of the human blink reflex. J Neurosci 2: RC5, 2. McCormick DA, Thompson RF. Cerebellum: essential involvement in the classically conditioned eyelid response. Science 223: , 984. Miles FA, Lisberger SG. Plasticity in the vestibulo-ocular reflex: a new hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci 4: , 98. Nicholson DA, Freeman JH Jr. Selective developmental increase in the climbing fiber input to the cerebellar interpositus nucleus in rats. Behav Neurosci 8: 6, 24. Obermann M, Yaldizli O, De Greiff A, Lachenmayer ML, Buhl AR, Tumczak F, Gizewski ER, Diener HC, Maschke M. Morphometric changes of sensorimotor structures in focal dystonia. Mov Disord 22: 7 23, 27. Ongerboer de Visser BW. Comparative study of corneal and blink reflex latencies in patients with segmental or with cerebral lesions. Adv Neurol 39: , 983. Ongerboer de Visser BW, Kuypers HG. Late blink reflex changes in lateral medullary lesions. An electrophysiological and neuro-anatomical study of Wallenberg s Syndrome. Brain : , 978. Pellegrini JJ, Evinger C. The trigeminally evoked blink reflex. II. Mechanisms of paired-stimulus suppression. Exp Brain Res 7: 8 96, 995. Pellegrini JJ, Evinger C. Role of cerebellum in adaptive modification of reflex blinks. Learn Mem 4: 77 87, 997. Pellegrini JJ, Horn AK, Evinger C. The trigeminally evoked blink reflex. I. Neuronal circuits. Exp Brain Res 7: 66 8, 995. Quartarone A, Sant Angelo A, Battaglia F, Bagnato S, Rizzo V, Morgante F, Rothwell JC, Siebner HR, Girlanda P. Enhanced long-term potentiation-like plasticity of the trigeminal blink reflex circuit in blepharospasm. J Neurosci 26: 76 72, 26. Rambold H, Churchland A, Selig Y, Jasmin L, Lisberger SG. Partial ablations of the flocculus and ventral paraflocculus in monkeys cause linked deficits in smooth pursuit eye movements and adaptive modification of the VOR. J Neurophysiol 87: , 22. Raymond JL, Lisberger SG, Mauk MD. The cerebellum: a neuronal learning machine? Science 272: 26 3, 996. Robinson FR, Fuchs AF, Noto CT. Cerebellar influences on saccade plasticity. Ann NY Acad Sci 956: 55 63, 22. Sanes JN, Ison JR. Habituation and sensitization of components of the human eyeblink reflex. Behav Neurosci 97: , 983. Schicatano EJ, Mantzouranis J, Peshori KR, Partin J, Evinger C. Lid restraint evokes two types of motor adaptation. J Neurosci 22: , 22. Scudder CA, McGee DM. Adaptive modification of saccade size produces correlated changes in the discharges of fastigial nucleus neurons. J Neurophysiol 9: 26, 23. Snow BJ, Frith RW. The relationship of eyelid movement to the blink reflex. J Neurol Sci 9: 79 89, 989. Soetedjo R, Fuchs AF. Complex spike activity of purkinje cells in the oculomotor vermis during behavioral adaptation of monkey saccades. J Neurosci 26: , 26. Soetedjo R, Kojima Y, Fuchs AF. Complex spike activity in the oculomotor vermis of the cerebellum: a vectorial error signal for saccade motor learning? J Neurophysiol : , 28. Straube A, Deubel H, Ditterich J, Eggert T. Cerebellar lesions impair rapid saccade amplitude adaptation. Neurology 57: 25 28, 2. Suppa A, Belvisi D, Bologna M, Marsili L, Berardelli I, Moretti G, Pasquini M, Fabbrini G, Berardelli A. Abnormal cortical and brain stem plasticity in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Mov Disord 26: 73 7, 2. Thompson RF. In search of memory traces. Annu Rev Psychol 56: 23, 25. Topka H, Valls-Sole J, Massaquoi SG, Hallett M. Deficit in classical conditioning in patients with cerebellar degeneration. Brain 6: , 993. VanderWerf F, Reits D, Smit AE, Metselaar M. Blink recovery in patients with Bell s palsy: a neurophysiological and behavioral longitudinal study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 48: 23 23, 27. Weiss C, Thompson RF. The effects of age on eyeblink conditioning in the freely moving Fischer-344 rat. Neurobiol Aging 2: , 99. Zeuner KE, Knutzen A, Al-Ali A, Hallett M, Deuschl G, Bergmann TO, Siebner HR. Associative stimulation of the supraorbital nerve fails to induce timing-specific plasticity in the human blink reflex. PLoS One 5: e362, 2. J Neurophysiol doi:.52/jn Downloaded from by ${individualuser.givennames} ${individualuser.surname} ( ) on February 9, 28. Copyright 24 American Physiological Society. All rights reserved.

Enhanced Long-Term Potentiation-Like Plasticity of the Trigeminal Blink Reflex Circuit in Blepharospasm

Enhanced Long-Term Potentiation-Like Plasticity of the Trigeminal Blink Reflex Circuit in Blepharospasm 716 The Journal of Neuroscience, January 11, 2006 26(2):716 721 Neurobiology of Disease Enhanced Long-Term Potentiation-Like Plasticity of the Trigeminal Blink Reflex Circuit in Blepharospasm Angelo Quartarone,

More information

Experiential Modification of the Trigeminal Reflex Blink Circuit

Experiential Modification of the Trigeminal Reflex Blink Circuit 10414 The Journal of Neuroscience, September 26, 2007 27(39):10414 10422 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Experiential Modification of the Trigeminal Reflex Blink Circuit Céline Dauvergne 1 and Craig Evinger

More information

The influence of age in peripheral facial palsy on brainstem reflex excitability

The influence of age in peripheral facial palsy on brainstem reflex excitability Original Article The influence of age in peripheral facial palsy on brainstem reflex excitability M. E. Kiziltan, N. Uzun, G. Kiziltan, F. K. Savrun Department of Neurology, Istanbul University Cerrahpasa

More information

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Sensory Modulation of the Blink Reflex in Patients With Blepharospasm. exhibit an abnormal excitability of the blink reflex

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Sensory Modulation of the Blink Reflex in Patients With Blepharospasm. exhibit an abnormal excitability of the blink reflex ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Sensory Modulation of the Blink Reflex in Patients With Blepharospasm Evelia Gómez-Wong, MD; Maria J. Martí, MD; Eduardo Tolosa, MD; Josep Valls-Solé, MD Objective: To measure the

More information

Cerebellar Cortex Lesions Prevent Acquisition of Conditioned Eyelid Responses

Cerebellar Cortex Lesions Prevent Acquisition of Conditioned Eyelid Responses The Journal of Neuroscience, December 15, 1999, 19(24):10940 10947 Cerebellar Cortex Lesions Prevent Acquisition of Conditioned Eyelid Responses Keith S. Garcia, Philip M. Steele, and Michael D. Mauk Department

More information

The blink reflex recovery cycle differs between essential and presumed psychogenic blepharospasm

The blink reflex recovery cycle differs between essential and presumed psychogenic blepharospasm The blink reflex recovery cycle differs between essential and presumed psychogenic blepharospasm P. Schwingenschuh, MD P. Katschnig, MD M.J. Edwards, MD, PhD J.T.H. Teo, MD, PhD L.V.P. Korlipara, MD, PhD

More information

Eyeblink cross-habituation between tactile and acoustic systems in humans

Eyeblink cross-habituation between tactile and acoustic systems in humans Psychobiology 1997,250), 66-70 Eyeblink cross-habituation between tactile and acoustic systems in humans WILLIAM A DYCUS and ALICE SCHADE POWERS St. John's University, Jamaica, New York To study cross-habituation

More information

What the cerebellum computes

What the cerebellum computes 222 Review TRENDS in Neurosciences Vol.26 No.4 April 2003 What the cerebellum computes Tatsuya Ohyama, William L. Nores, Matthew Murphy and Michael D. Mauk Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, and W.M.

More information

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material Supplemental Material Recording technique Multi-unit activity (MUA) was recorded from electrodes that were chronically implanted (Teflon-coated platinum-iridium wires) in the primary visual cortex representing

More information

The Origin of Downbeat Nystagmus

The Origin of Downbeat Nystagmus The Origin of Downbeat Nystagmus An Asymmetry in the Distribution of On-Directions of Vertical Gaze-Velocity Purkinje Cells SARAH MARTI, a DOMINIK STRAUMANN, a AND STEFAN GLASAUER b a Neurology Department,

More information

Classical conditioning using stimulation of the inferior olive as the unconditioned stimulus

Classical conditioning using stimulation of the inferior olive as the unconditioned stimulus Proc. Nati. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 83, pp. 5349-5353, July 1986 Psychology Classical conditioning using stimulation of the inferior olive as the unconditioned stimulus (learning/neural plasticity/acquisition/extinction/rabbit)

More information

doi: /brain/awn317 Brain 2009: 132;

doi: /brain/awn317 Brain 2009: 132; doi:10.1093/brain/awn317 Brain 2009: 132; 502 510 502 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY Voluntary, spontaneous and reflex blinking in patients with clinically probable progressive supranuclear palsy Matteo

More information

Cerebellum: Origins and Development

Cerebellum: Origins and Development Cerebellum: Origins and Development Found in all vertebrates Dorsal lip of developing medulla (rhombencephalon) Near terminations of vestibular (VIII) and lateral line afferents, which sense fluid displacement

More information

Following the seminal proposal of Hebb1 and many others, acquired learning

Following the seminal proposal of Hebb1 and many others, acquired learning Associative Learning and Long-Term Potentiation in Rodents: José M. Delgado-, MD, PhD Long-Term Potentiation (LTP) and Learning-Dependent Changes in Synaptic Strength Following the seminal proposal of

More information

Links Between Single-Trial Changes and Learning Rate in Eyelid Conditioning

Links Between Single-Trial Changes and Learning Rate in Eyelid Conditioning Cerebellum () : DOI.7/s--9-8 ORIGINAL PAPER Links Between Single-Trial Changes and Learning Rate in Eyelid Conditioning Andrei Khilkevich & Hunter E. Halverson & Jose Ernesto Canton-Josh & Michael D. Mauk,

More information

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010

Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Timing and the cerebellum (and the VOR) Neurophysiology of systems 2010 Asymmetry in learning in the reverse direction Full recovery from UP using DOWN: initial return to naïve values within 10 minutes,

More information

Non-therapeutic and investigational uses of non-invasive brain stimulation

Non-therapeutic and investigational uses of non-invasive brain stimulation Non-therapeutic and investigational uses of non-invasive brain stimulation Robert Chen, MA, MBBChir, MSc, FRCPC Catherine Manson Chair in Movement Disorders Professor of Medicine (Neurology), University

More information

Surface recording of muscle activity

Surface recording of muscle activity 3 rd Congress of the European Academy of Neurology Amsterdam, The Netherlands, June 24 27, 2017 Hands-on Course 5 Electromyography: Surface, needle conventional and single fiber - Level 1-2 Surface recording

More information

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction Instructor: Paul Wolfgang Faculty sponsor: Yuan Shi, Ph.D. Andrey Mavrichev CIS 4339 Project in Computer Science May 7, 2009 Research work was completed in collaboration with Michael Tobia, Kevin L. Brown,

More information

11/2/2011. Basic circuit anatomy (the circuit is the same in all parts of the cerebellum)

11/2/2011. Basic circuit anatomy (the circuit is the same in all parts of the cerebellum) 11/2/2011 Neuroscientists have been attracted to the puzzle of the Cerebellum ever since Cajal. The orderly structure, the size of the cerebellum and the regularity of the neural elements demands explanation.

More information

CEREBELLUM: ESSENTIAL INVOLVEMENT IN A SIMPLE LEARNED RESPONSE A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE PROGRAM IN NEUROSCIENCES AND THE

CEREBELLUM: ESSENTIAL INVOLVEMENT IN A SIMPLE LEARNED RESPONSE A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE PROGRAM IN NEUROSCIENCES AND THE 1 CEREBELLUM: ESSENTIAL INVOLVEMENT IN A SIMPLE LEARNED RESPONSE A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE PROGRAM IN NEUROSCIENCES AND THE COMMITTEE ON GRADUATE STUDIES OF STANFORD UNIVERSITY IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information

The late blink reflex response abnormality due to lesion of the lateral tegmental field

The late blink reflex response abnormality due to lesion of the lateral tegmental field Brain (1997), 120, 1685 1692 The late blink reflex response abnormality due to lesion of the lateral tegmental field M. Aramideh, 1,2 B. W. Ongerboer de Visser, 2 J. H. T. M. Koelman, 2 C. B. L. Majoie

More information

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory Systems Neuroscience November 29, 2016 Memory Gabriela Michel http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Forms of memory Different types of learning & memory rely on different brain structures

More information

Hemifacial spasm. Parkinson's Disease Center and Movement Disorders Clinic

Hemifacial spasm. Parkinson's Disease Center and Movement Disorders Clinic Parkinson's Disease Center and Movement Disorders Clinic 7200 Cambridge Street, 9th Floor, Suite 9A Houston, Texas 77030 713-798-2273 phone www.jankovic.org Hemifacial spasm Diagnosis Hemifacial spasm

More information

Conditioned Eyeblink Response Consists of Two Distinct Components

Conditioned Eyeblink Response Consists of Two Distinct Components Conditioned Eyeblink Response Consists of Two Distinct Components MAGNUS IVARSSON AND PÄR SVENSSON Department of Physiological Sciences, Section for Neuroscience, Lund University, S-223 62 Lund, Sweden

More information

A Mechanism for Savings in the Cerebellum

A Mechanism for Savings in the Cerebellum The Journal of Neuroscience, June 1, 2001, 21(11):4081 4089 A Mechanism for Savings in the Cerebellum Javier F. Medina, Keith S. Garcia, and Michael D. Mauk W. M. Keck Center for the Neurobiology of Learning

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999 TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL A Senior Honors Thesis Presented By Vanessa E. Castagna June 999 999 by Vanessa E. Castagna ABSTRACT TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: A TEST OF

More information

TREATMENT-SPECIFIC ABNORMAL SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN EARLY PARKINSON S DISEASE

TREATMENT-SPECIFIC ABNORMAL SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN EARLY PARKINSON S DISEASE TREATMENT-SPECIFIC ABNORMAL SYNAPTIC PLASTICITY IN EARLY PARKINSON S DISEASE Angel Lago-Rodriguez 1, Binith Cheeran 2 and Miguel Fernández-Del-Olmo 3 1. Prism Lab, Behavioural Brain Sciences, School of

More information

Biomarkers in Schizophrenia

Biomarkers in Schizophrenia Biomarkers in Schizophrenia David A. Lewis, MD Translational Neuroscience Program Department of Psychiatry NIMH Conte Center for the Neuroscience of Mental Disorders University of Pittsburgh Disease Process

More information

IN PRESS Gating of neural error signals during motor learning

IN PRESS Gating of neural error signals during motor learning IN PRESS Gating of neural error signals during motor learning Rhea Kimpo (Stanford University), Jacob Rinaldi (Stanford University), Christina Kim (Stanford University), Hannah Payne (Stanford University),

More information

Classical Conditioning

Classical Conditioning What is classical conditioning? Classical Conditioning Learning & Memory Arlo Clark-Foos Learning to associate previously neutral stimuli with the subsequent events. Howard Eichenbaum s Thanksgiving Pavlov

More information

Symptomatic and essential palatal tremor.

Symptomatic and essential palatal tremor. 52 5ournal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1996;6:52-525 Human Motor Control Section, Medical Neurology Branch, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of

More information

Electrical recording with micro- and macroelectrodes from the cerebellum of man

Electrical recording with micro- and macroelectrodes from the cerebellum of man Electrical recording with micro- and macroelectrodes from the cerebellum of man D. GRAHAM SLAUGHTER, M.D., BLAINE S. NASHOLD, Jn., M.D., AND GEORGE G. SOMJEN, M.D. The Division of Neurosurgery, and the

More information

Temporal Patterns of Inputs to Cerebellum Necessary and Sufficient for Trace Eyelid Conditioning

Temporal Patterns of Inputs to Cerebellum Necessary and Sufficient for Trace Eyelid Conditioning J Neurophysiol 104: 627 640, 2010. First published May 19, 2010; doi:10.1152/jn.00169.2010. Temporal Patterns of Inputs to Cerebellum Necessary and Sufficient for Trace Eyelid Conditioning Brian E. Kalmbach,

More information

CEREBELLUM-DEPENDENT LEARNING: The Role of Multiple Plasticity Mechanisms

CEREBELLUM-DEPENDENT LEARNING: The Role of Multiple Plasticity Mechanisms Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2004. 27:581 609 doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144238 Copyright c 2004 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved CEREBELLUM-DEPENDENT LEARNING: The Role of Multiple Plasticity

More information

Original Article. Annals of Rehabilitation Medicine

Original Article. Annals of Rehabilitation Medicine Original Article Ann Rehabil Med 2013;37(6):839-847 pissn: 2234-0645 eissn: 2234-0653 http://dx.doi.org/10.5535/arm.2013.37.6.839 Annals of Rehabilitation Medicine Electrophysiologic Investigation During

More information

Discharge Profiles of Abducens, Accessory Abducens, and Orbicularis Oculi Motoneurons During Reflex and Conditioned Blinks in Alert Cats

Discharge Profiles of Abducens, Accessory Abducens, and Orbicularis Oculi Motoneurons During Reflex and Conditioned Blinks in Alert Cats Discharge Profiles of Abducens, Accessory Abducens, and Orbicularis Oculi Motoneurons During Reflex and Conditioned Blinks in Alert Cats JOSÉ A. TRIGO, AGNÈS GRUART, AND JOSÉ M. DELGADO-GARCÍA Laboratorio

More information

Physiology & Behavior

Physiology & Behavior Physiology & Behavior 96 (2009) 399 411 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Physiology & Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/phb Associative and non-associative blinking in classically

More information

An Explanation for Reflex Blink Hyperexcitability in Parkinson s Disease. I. Superior Colliculus

An Explanation for Reflex Blink Hyperexcitability in Parkinson s Disease. I. Superior Colliculus The Journal of Neuroscience, November 15, 1996, 16(22):7308 7317 An Explanation for Reflex Blink Hyperexcitability in Parkinson s Disease. I. Superior Colliculus Michele A. Basso, 1 Alice S. Powers, 3

More information

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey 3.6.QH Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey Versnel, Huib; Zwiers, Marcel; Van Opstal, John Department of Biophysics University of Nijmegen Geert Grooteplein 655

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival? Threats are much better reinforcers? Fear is a prime

More information

Variety of muscle responses to tactile stimuli

Variety of muscle responses to tactile stimuli Variety of muscle responses to tactile stimuli Julita Czarkowska-Bauch Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, 3 Pasteur St., 02-093 Warsaw, Poland Abstract. Influences

More information

Parallel Acquisition of Awareness and Trace Eyeblink Classical Conditioning

Parallel Acquisition of Awareness and Trace Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Research Parallel Acquisition of Awareness and Trace Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Joseph R. Manns, 1 Robert E. Clark, 2 and Larry R. Squire 3,4,5 1 Department of Psychology, University of California,

More information

Water immersion modulates sensory and motor cortical excitability

Water immersion modulates sensory and motor cortical excitability Water immersion modulates sensory and motor cortical excitability Daisuke Sato, PhD Department of Health and Sports Niigata University of Health and Welfare Topics Neurophysiological changes during water

More information

Cerebellum: little brain. Cerebellum. gross divisions

Cerebellum: little brain. Cerebellum. gross divisions Cerebellum The anatomy of the cerebellum and its gross divisions Its principal input and output pathways The organization of the cerebellar cortex Role of climbing vs. mossy fibre input The parallel-fibre/

More information

Treatment for Nystagmus

Treatment for Nystagmus ISSN: 2234-0971 Treatment for Nystagmus Seong-Hae Jeong Department of Neurology, Chungnam National University Hospital, Daejeon, Korea Treatment for Nystagmus Seong-Hae Jeong Chungnam National University

More information

Multiple sites of extinction for a single learned response

Multiple sites of extinction for a single learned response Multiple sites of extinction for a single learned response Brian E. Kalmbach and Michael D. Mauk J Neurophysiol 107:226-238, 2012. First published 21 September 2011; doi:10.1152/jn.00381.2011 You might

More information

To Blink or Not to Blink: Fine Cognitive Tuning of the Defensive Peripersonal Space

To Blink or Not to Blink: Fine Cognitive Tuning of the Defensive Peripersonal Space The Journal of Neuroscience, September 12, 2012 32(37):12921 12927 12921 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive To Blink or Not to Blink: Fine Cognitive Tuning of the Defensive Peripersonal Space C. F. Sambo, 1,2

More information

Lauer et al Olivocochlear efferents. Amanda M. Lauer, Ph.D. Dept. of Otolaryngology-HNS

Lauer et al Olivocochlear efferents. Amanda M. Lauer, Ph.D. Dept. of Otolaryngology-HNS Lauer et al. 2012 Olivocochlear efferents Amanda M. Lauer, Ph.D. Dept. of Otolaryngology-HNS May 30, 2016 Overview Structural organization Responses Hypothesized roles in hearing Olivocochlear efferent

More information

The Cerebellum. Outline. Overview Structure (external & internal) Micro-circuitry of the cerebellum Cerebellum and motor learning

The Cerebellum. Outline. Overview Structure (external & internal) Micro-circuitry of the cerebellum Cerebellum and motor learning The Cerebellum P.T Ji Jun Cheol Rehabilitation Center 1 HansarangAsan Hospital. Outline Overview Structure (external & internal) Micro-circuitry of the cerebellum Cerebellum and motor learning 2 1 Cerebellum

More information

EYE POSITION FEEDBACK IN A MODEL OF THE VESTIBULO-OCULAR REFLEX FOR SPINO-CEREBELLAR ATAXIA 6

EYE POSITION FEEDBACK IN A MODEL OF THE VESTIBULO-OCULAR REFLEX FOR SPINO-CEREBELLAR ATAXIA 6 EYE POSITION FEEDBACK IN A MODEL OF THE VESTIBULO-OCULAR REFLEX FOR SPINO-CEREBELLAR ATAXIA 6 J. H. Anderson 1, M. C. Yavuz 2, B. M. Kazar 3, P. Christova 1, C. M. Gomez 4 1 Department of Otolaryngology,

More information

Initial localization of the acoustic conditioned stimulus projection system to the cerebellum essential for classical eyelid conditioning

Initial localization of the acoustic conditioned stimulus projection system to the cerebellum essential for classical eyelid conditioning Proc. Nati. Acad. Sci. SA Vol. 84, pp. 3531-3535, May 1987 Psychology nitial localization of the acoustic conditioned stimulus projection system to the cerebellum essential for classical eyelid conditioning

More information

Neural Learning Rules for the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex

Neural Learning Rules for the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex The Journal of Neuroscience, November 1, 1998, 18(21):9112 9129 Neural Learning Rules for the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex Jennifer L. Raymond and Stephen G. Lisberger Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Department

More information

The individual animals, the basic design of the experiments and the electrophysiological

The individual animals, the basic design of the experiments and the electrophysiological SUPPORTING ONLINE MATERIAL Material and Methods The individual animals, the basic design of the experiments and the electrophysiological techniques for extracellularly recording from dopamine neurons were

More information

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title A Continuum of Learning and Memory Research Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9q3031wx Journal International Journal of Comparative Psychology,

More information

Adaptation of the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Amblyopia

Adaptation of the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Amblyopia Adaptation of the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Amblyopia Carol A. Wesfall and Clifton M. Schor Adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) is demonstrated by changes in gain in response to discrepancies

More information

The Cerebellum: A Neuronal Learning Machine? Jennifer L. Raymond, Stephen G. Lisberger, Michael D. Mauk*

The Cerebellum: A Neuronal Learning Machine? Jennifer L. Raymond, Stephen G. Lisberger, Michael D. Mauk* IIIIIIII ARTICLE -9" The Cerebellum: A Neuronal Learning Machine? Jennifer L. Raymond, Stephen G. Lisberger, Michael D. Mauk* Comparison of two seemingly quite different behaviors yields a surprisingly

More information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 1) The hippocampus is a cortical structure in the medial portion of the temporal lobe (medial temporal lobe in primates. a) What is the main function of the hippocampus? The hippocampus

More information

Motor systems.... the only thing mankind can do is to move things... whether whispering or felling a forest. C. Sherrington

Motor systems.... the only thing mankind can do is to move things... whether whispering or felling a forest. C. Sherrington Motor systems... the only thing mankind can do is to move things... whether whispering or felling a forest. C. Sherrington 1 Descending pathways: CS corticospinal; TS tectospinal; RS reticulospinal; VS

More information

Research Perspectives in Clinical Neurophysiology

Research Perspectives in Clinical Neurophysiology Research Perspectives in Clinical Neurophysiology A position paper of the EC-IFCN (European Chapter of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology) representing ~ 8000 Clinical Neurophysiologists

More information

For more information about how to cite these materials visit

For more information about how to cite these materials visit Author(s): Peter Hitchcock, PH.D., 2009 License: Unless otherwise noted, this material is made available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-commercial Share Alike 3.0 License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/3.0/

More information

Efficacy of gold weight implants in facial nerve palsy: quantitative alterations in blinking

Efficacy of gold weight implants in facial nerve palsy: quantitative alterations in blinking Vision Research 38 (1998) 3019 3023 Efficacy of gold weight implants in facial nerve palsy: quantitative alterations in blinking Karen M. Abell a, Robert S. Baker a,b, *, David E. Cowen a, John D. Porter

More information

Modèles mathématiques et corrélats anatomiques du mouvement (2/2)

Modèles mathématiques et corrélats anatomiques du mouvement (2/2) 17 mars 2016 Modèles mathématiques et corrélats anatomiques du mouvement (2/2) Emmanuel Guigon Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique Université Pierre et Marie Curie CNRS / UMR 7222 Paris,

More information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 BIPN 140 Problem Set 6 1) Hippocampus is a cortical structure in the medial portion of the temporal lobe (medial temporal lobe in primates. a) What is the main function of the hippocampus? The hippocampus

More information

Functional Distinctions

Functional Distinctions Functional Distinctions FUNCTION COMPONENT DEFICITS Start Basal Ganglia Spontaneous Movements Move UMN/LMN Cerebral Cortex Brainstem, Spinal cord Roots/peripheral nerves Plan Cerebellum Ataxia Adjust Cerebellum

More information

Cerebellum: little brain. Cerebellum. gross divisions

Cerebellum: little brain. Cerebellum. gross divisions Cerebellum The anatomy of the cerebellum and its gross divisions Its principal input and output pathways The organization of the cerebellar cortex Role of climbing vs. mossy fibre input The parallel-fibre/

More information

Latent Acquisition of Timed Responses in Cerebellar Cortex

Latent Acquisition of Timed Responses in Cerebellar Cortex The Journal of Neuroscience, January 15, 2001, 21(2):682 690 Latent Acquisition of Timed Responses in Cerebellar Cortex Tatsuya Ohyama and Michael D. Mauk Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, University

More information

CENTRAL CONTROL OF AN INSECT SENSORY INTERNEURONE

CENTRAL CONTROL OF AN INSECT SENSORY INTERNEURONE J. Exp. Biol. (1970), S3, 137-145 With 4 text-figures Printed in Great Britain CENTRAL CONTROL OF AN INSECT SENSORY INTERNEURONE BY J. M. MCKAY* Department of Zoology, Makerere University College, Kampala,

More information

Systems Neurobiology: Plasticity in the Auditory System. Jason Middleton -

Systems Neurobiology: Plasticity in the Auditory System. Jason Middleton - Systems Neurobiology: Plasticity in the Auditory System Jason Middleton - jmiddlet@pitt.edu Auditory plasticity Plasticity Early development and critical period Adult plasticity and neuromodulation Brainstem

More information

Final Report. Title of Project: Quantifying and measuring cortical reorganisation and excitability with post-stroke Wii-based Movement Therapy

Final Report. Title of Project: Quantifying and measuring cortical reorganisation and excitability with post-stroke Wii-based Movement Therapy Final Report Author: Dr Penelope McNulty Qualification: PhD Institution: Neuroscience Research Australia Date: 26 th August, 2015 Title of Project: Quantifying and measuring cortical reorganisation and

More information

Intracranial Studies Of Human Epilepsy In A Surgical Setting

Intracranial Studies Of Human Epilepsy In A Surgical Setting Intracranial Studies Of Human Epilepsy In A Surgical Setting Department of Neurology David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA Presentation Goals Epilepsy and seizures Basics of the electroencephalogram

More information

The jaw re exes. Giorgio Cruccu a, * and Bram W. Ongerboer de Visser b. Chapter 6.1. Physiological background

The jaw re exes. Giorgio Cruccu a, * and Bram W. Ongerboer de Visser b. Chapter 6.1. Physiological background Recommendations for the Practice of Clinical Neurophysiology: Guidelines of the International Federation of Clinical Physiology (EEG Suppl. 52) Editors: G. Deuschl and A. Eisen q 1999 International Federation

More information

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not Rony Paz Rony.paz@weizmann.ac.il Thoughts What is a reward? Learning is best motivated by threats to survival Threats are much better reinforcers Fear is a prime

More information

Synaptic inhibition of Purkinje cells mediates consolidation of. vestibulo-cerebellar motor learning

Synaptic inhibition of Purkinje cells mediates consolidation of. vestibulo-cerebellar motor learning Synaptic inhibition of Purkinje cells mediates consolidation of vestibulo-cerebellar motor learning Peer Wulff, Martijn Schonewille, Massimiliano Renzi, Laura Viltono, Marco Sassoè-Pognetto, Aleksandra

More information

Cerebellar Substrates for Error Correction in Motor Conditioning

Cerebellar Substrates for Error Correction in Motor Conditioning Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 76, 314 341 (2001) doi:10.1006/nlme.2001.4031, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Cerebellar Substrates for Error Correction in Motor Conditioning Mark

More information

In review. Editorial: Eyeblink Classical Conditioning in Psychiatric Conditions: Novel Uses for a Classic Paradigm

In review. Editorial: Eyeblink Classical Conditioning in Psychiatric Conditions: Novel Uses for a Classic Paradigm Editorial: Eyeblink Classical Conditioning in Psychiatric Conditions: Novel Uses for a Classic Paradigm Tracy L. Greer 1*, Lucien T. Thompson 2* 1 Department of Psychiatry, UT Southwestern Medical Center,

More information

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch!

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch! Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch! How do we perceive objects held in the hand?! Touch receptors deconstruct objects to detect local features! Information is transmitted in parallel

More information

Temporal Encoding in Fear Conditioning Revealed Through Associative Reflex Facilitation

Temporal Encoding in Fear Conditioning Revealed Through Associative Reflex Facilitation Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 118, No. 2, 395 402 0735-7044/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.395 Temporal Encoding in Fear Conditioning

More information

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories? CASE 49 A 43-year-old woman is brought to her primary care physician by her family because of concerns about her forgetfulness. The patient has a history of Down syndrome but no other medical problems.

More information

Involvement of the human medial cerebellum in long-term habituation of the acoustic startle response

Involvement of the human medial cerebellum in long-term habituation of the acoustic startle response Exp Brain Res (2000) 133:359 367 Digital Object Identifier (DOI) 10.1007/s002210000417 RESEARCH ARTICLE Matthias Maschke Johannes Drepper Kerstin Kindsvater Florian P. Kolb Hans-Christoph Diener Dagmar

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Paired-Pulse TMS to one Brain Region. Joyce Gomes-Osman Research Fellow Berenson-Allen Center for Non-Invasive Stimulation LEASE DO NOT COPY

Paired-Pulse TMS to one Brain Region. Joyce Gomes-Osman Research Fellow Berenson-Allen Center for Non-Invasive Stimulation LEASE DO NOT COPY Paired-Pulse TMS to one Brain Region Joyce Gomes-Osman Research Fellow Berenson-Allen Center for Non-Invasive Stimulation Paired-Pulse Paradigms Sequential pulses applied to the same cortical region Variable

More information

Shadowing and Blocking as Learning Interference Models

Shadowing and Blocking as Learning Interference Models Shadowing and Blocking as Learning Interference Models Espoir Kyubwa Dilip Sunder Raj Department of Bioengineering Department of Neuroscience University of California San Diego University of California

More information

Bilateral disruption of conditioned responses after unilateral blockade of cerebellar output in the decerebrate ferret

Bilateral disruption of conditioned responses after unilateral blockade of cerebellar output in the decerebrate ferret Keywords: Cerebellum, 5977 Journal of Physiology (1997), 502.1, pp. 189 201 189 Bilateral disruption of conditioned responses after unilateral blockade of cerebellar output in the decerebrate ferret M.

More information

Impaired Motor Learning in the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Mice with Multiple Climbing Fiber Input to Cerebellar Purkinje Cells

Impaired Motor Learning in the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Mice with Multiple Climbing Fiber Input to Cerebellar Purkinje Cells 5672 The Journal of Neuroscience, May 23, 2007 27(21):5672 5682 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Impaired Motor Learning in the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Mice with Multiple Climbing Fiber Input to Cerebellar

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1 SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Figure 1 The supralinear events evoked in CA3 pyramidal cells fulfill the criteria for NMDA spikes, exhibiting a threshold, sensitivity to NMDAR blockade, and all-or-none

More information

SENSORY PLASTICITY. Sensory Plasticity

SENSORY PLASTICITY. Sensory Plasticity 801 Sensory Plasticity SENSORY PLASTICITY You may have the idea that the visual, auditory and somatosensory systems are static pathways (i.e., the neural wiring is in place and simply does its job). To

More information

Strick Lecture 3 March 22, 2017 Page 1

Strick Lecture 3 March 22, 2017 Page 1 Strick Lecture 3 March 22, 2017 Page 1 Cerebellum OUTLINE I. External structure- Inputs and Outputs Cerebellum - (summary diagram) 2 components (cortex and deep nuclei)- (diagram) 3 Sagittal zones (vermal,

More information

The metabolic topography of essential blepharospasm

The metabolic topography of essential blepharospasm The metabolic topography of essential blepharospasm A focal dystonia with general implications M. Hutchinson, MD; T. Nakamura, MD; J.R. Moeller, PhD; A. Antonini, MD; A. Belakhlef, PhD; V. Dhawan, PhD;

More information

The Marmoset Monkey as Model for Neurological Disorders

The Marmoset Monkey as Model for Neurological Disorders The Marmoset Monkey as Model for Neurological Disorders Jan Langermans and Ingrid Philippens From Laboratory to Clinic Disease models neuroscience: Parkinson, Sleep, Stress, Alzheimer, MS MS Models: rhmog

More information

Neurobiology: The nerve cell. Principle and task To use a nerve function model to study the following aspects of a nerve cell:

Neurobiology: The nerve cell. Principle and task To use a nerve function model to study the following aspects of a nerve cell: Principle and task To use a nerve function model to study the following aspects of a nerve cell: INTRACELLULAR POTENTIAL AND ACTION POTENTIAL Comparison between low and high threshold levels Comparison

More information

Extraocular Muscles and Ocular Motor Control of Eye Movements

Extraocular Muscles and Ocular Motor Control of Eye Movements Extraocular Muscles and Ocular Motor Control of Eye Movements Linda K. McLoon PhD mcloo001@umn.edu Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Neurosciences Your Eyes Are Constantly Moving. Yarbus, 1967 Eye

More information

Brain-computer interface to transform cortical activity to control signals for prosthetic arm

Brain-computer interface to transform cortical activity to control signals for prosthetic arm Brain-computer interface to transform cortical activity to control signals for prosthetic arm Artificial neural network Spinal cord challenge: getting appropriate control signals from cortical neurons

More information

Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Introduction to TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Lisa Koski, PhD, Clin Psy TMS Neurorehabilitation Lab Royal Victoria Hospital 2009-12-14 BIC Seminar, MNI Overview History, basic principles, instrumentation

More information

Cell transplantation in Parkinson s disease

Cell transplantation in Parkinson s disease Cell transplantation in Parkinson s disease Findings by SBU Alert Published September 18, 2001 Revised November 7, 2003 Version 2 Technology and target group: In Parkinsons disease, the brain cells that

More information

Oculomotor System George R. Leichnetz, Ph.D.

Oculomotor System George R. Leichnetz, Ph.D. Oculomotor System George R. Leichnetz, Ph.D. OBJECTIVES After studying the material of this lecture, the student should be able to: 1. Define different types of eye movement and their underlying neural

More information

Limbic system outline

Limbic system outline Limbic system outline 1 Introduction 4 The amygdala and emotion -history - theories of emotion - definition - fear and fear conditioning 2 Review of anatomy 5 The hippocampus - amygdaloid complex - septal

More information

PARALLELS BETWEEN CEREBELLUM- AND AMYGDALA-DEPENDENT CONDITIONING

PARALLELS BETWEEN CEREBELLUM- AND AMYGDALA-DEPENDENT CONDITIONING PARALLELS BETWEEN CEREBELLUM- AND AMYGDALA-DEPENDENT CONDITIONING Javier F. Medina, J. Christopher Repa, Michael D. Mauk and Joseph E. LeDoux Recent evidence from cerebellum-dependent motor learning and

More information