NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Biol Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 October 27.

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Biol Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 October 27."

Transcription

1 NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry September 1; 62(5): doi: /j.biopsych Configural Cue Performance in Identical Twins Discordant for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Theoretical Implications for the Role of Hippocampal Function Mark W. Gilbertson, Stephanie K. Williston, Lynn A. Paulus, Natasha B. Lasko, Tamara V. Gurvits, Martha E. Shenton, Roger K. Pitman, and Scott P. Orr Manchester VA Medical Center (MWG, SKW, LAP, NBL, TVG, SPO), Manchester, New Hampshire; Harvard Medical School (MWG, NBL, TVG, MES, RKP, SPO); Massachusetts General Hospital (RKP), Boston; and the VA Boston Healthcare System (MES), Brockton, Massachusetts. Abstract Background A significant subgroup of individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) exhibits chronic, unremitting symptomatology that has also been associated with smaller hippocampal volume. The hippocampus plays a significant role in configural processing of contextual cues that facilitates context-appropriate extinction of conditioned fear. We test the hypothesis that hippocampus-based configural processing deficits are a pre-existing vulnerability factor for unremitting forms of PTSD. Methods Participants included male monozygotic twin pairs who were discordant for combat trauma. In 18 twin pairs the combat-exposed brother developed unremitting PTSD, whereas in 23 pairs the combat-exposed brother never developed PTSD. Participants were compared in the capacity to solve allocentric spatial processing tasks, and this performance was examined for its relationship to the severity of PTSD symptomatology and hippocampal volume. Results Although not completely differentiated from overall IQ, PTSD combat veterans demonstrated significantly impaired performance in configural processing relative to non-ptsd combat veterans. Despite having neither combat-exposure nor PTSD, the unexposed co-twins of combat veterans with PTSD displayed the same decrements as their brothers. Deficits were significantly related to PTSD severity and hippocampal volume. Conclusions The current study provides the first evidence that the relevance of the hippocampus in PTSD might be related to pre-existing configural cue processing deficits that predispose individuals to develop unremitting forms of the disorder. Keywords Configural; contextual; hippocampus; neuropsychology; PTSD; twins Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) includes a constellation of disabling behavioral and emotional symptoms that occur in a proportion of individuals exposed to severe psychological trauma. Although many individuals with PTSD show remission within the first year after traumatic exposure, a significant subgroup of PTSD patients perhaps as high as 30% 40% (Kessler et al. 1995) demonstrates an unremitting course of illness that might persist for many 2007 Society of Biological Psychiatry Address reprint requests to Mark W. Gilbertson, Ph.D., Research Service 151, Manchester VA Medical Center, 718 Smyth Road, Manchester, NH 03104; mark.gilbertson@med.va.gov..

2 Gilbertson et al. Page 2 years or decades. Furthermore, individuals diagnosed with chronic PTSD have shown the ability to maintain conditioned fear responses to traumatic stimuli years after an event (Orr et al. 1993). A number of factors might impact the development of chronic PTSD, including severity of trauma, level of social support, and the presence of anger or depression (Koenen et al. 2003). It is unclear to what degree biological factors might also influence the course or chronicity of PTSD-related emotional responses. Smaller hippocampal volume has been a frequently replicated finding in the PTSD literature. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies of hippocampal volume in human subjects have established a relatively consistent pattern showing smaller hippocampi in individuals diagnosed with PTSD (Bremner et al. 1995a, 1997; Gilbertson et al. 2002; Gurvits et al. 1996; Stein et al. 1997; Villarreal et al. 2002). These imaging studies have largely employed subjects with severe and unremitting courses of illness (i.e., combat veterans or childhood abuse survivors who continue to report significant symptomatology for many years). We have specifically reported (Gilbertson et al. 2002) that hippocampal volume differences in PTSD might be most relevant to those individuals who maintain severity scores > 65 on the Clinician- Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) for many years after the traumatic event. Studies failing to find hippocampal differences in PTSD have often employed less severe and/or less chronic populations of subjects (Bonne et al. 2001; Carrion et al. 2001; De Bellis et al. 1999, 2001). These findings suggest the possibility that hippocampal volume might be more relevant to the course of PTSD than to the development of PTSD per se (i.e., smaller hippocampal volume might be associated with a more severe and unremitting course of illness). An understanding of the mechanism through which smaller hippocampal volume might influence the development and course of PTSD remains unclear. In humans, the hippocampus has been frequently linked to declarative memory function (Squire 1992; Zola-Morgan and Squire 1993). Verbal memory impairments have often been reported in PTSD (Beers and De Bellis 2002; Bremner et al. 1993, 1995b; Gilbertson et al. 1997, 2001; Golier et al. 2002; Horner and Hamner 2002; Vasterling et al. 1998, 2002; Yehuda et al. 1995). Such impairments, however, do not readily lend themselves to an explanation of chronicity or the disorder's core clinical phenomenology, which includes excessive memory retrieval and intrusive, context-inappropriate emotional responding. Studies of patients with chronic PTSD have often failed to find a specific relationship between declarative memory and hippocampal volume (Bremner et al. 1997; Gurvits et al. 1996; Stein et al. 1997) or have instead suggested that these deficits might represent prefrontal lobe contributions (Vasterling et al. 1998; 2002). There is an emerging picture in the animal literature of the hippocampus as a critical site for the processing of contextual information, particularly as it relates to fear-mediated learning (Corcoran and Maren 2001; McDonald et al. 2002; Moses et al. 2002; Phillips and LeDoux 1992; Selden et al. 1991). Animals acquire conditioned responses not only to a discrete conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., tone) that is paired with an unconditioned stimulus (US; e.g., foot shock) but also to more complex contextual cues in the external environment that surround the CS US pairing (Holland and Bouton 1999; Maren et al. 1997; Phillips and LeDoux 1992; Selden et al. 1991). The hippocampus has been shown to be especially relevant to the mediation of spatial cues that have the quality of being allocentric (i.e., processing of configural relationships among distal environmental features) as opposed to egocentric (i.e., processing merely in reference to the organism's midline axis) (Astur et al. 2002; Burgess et al. 1999; Driscoll et al. 2003; Incisa della Rocchetta et al. 2004). Complex arrays of distal contextual cues might serve as configural signals for identifying when the environment is related to the original conditioned fear response (i.e., has a dangerous or safe context) and hence whether a fear response should be emitted. Animal studies have demonstrated that configural processing of environmental cues might play a significant role in discriminative fear conditioning and

3 Gilbertson et al. Page 3 extinction, with impairments in this type of contextual skill having been shown to impede the extinction of conditioned fear responses (Frankland et al. 1998; Maren, 2001). If individuals with PTSD exhibit basic deficits in allocentric processing capacity, this might provide preliminary evidence for a potential model of hippocampal function in PTSD that can improve our understanding of the chronic and unremitting emotional responses observed in the disorder. Most neuropsychological studies of PTSD have not employed allocentric-based spatial tasks. Standard visuospatial tasks such as recognition or recall of complex shapes are primarily egocentric in nature (i.e., encoded in reference to the midline axis of the participant's body). Cognitive studies have not consistently found PTSD-related deficits in such standardized tests of visuospatial function (Gilbertson et al. 2001, 2006; Horner and Hamner 2002; Vasterling et al. 1998, 2002). Some findings have seemed suggestive of allocentric-like spatial difficulties, including impairments in the copying of three-dimensional figures (Gurvits et al. 2000, 2002) or single block rotation errors on Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised (WAIS- R) Block Design (Vasterling et al. 2000). However, such tasks can be solved with egocentric processing alone and were not designed as primary allocentric measures. Employing a sample of monozygotic twin pairs discordant for combat exposure (Gilbertson, et al. 2002, 2006), we examined a task that emphasizes allocentric spatial processing. We attempted to test two hypotheses: 1) do individuals with PTSD demonstrate hippocampusbased impairment in the allocentric encoding of configural relationships among cues; and 2) if such a deficit exists, does it serve as a vulnerability factor for the development of more chronic forms of PTSD? Methods and Materials Participants Rationale Participants included male monozygotic twin pairs in which one brother served in combat during the Vietnam War (exposed, Ex) and his identical co-twin did not (unexposed, Ux). On the basis of the diagnosis of the combat-exposed brother, twin pairs were classified into two groups: chronic PTSD (P+) and non-ptsd (i.e., never had) (P ). The P+ or P designation refers to the combat-related PTSD status of the exposed twin (no unexposed cotwin in this study had non combat-related PTSD). Combat-unexposed brothers will also be referred to as high-risk (UxP+) versus low-risk (UxP ) co-twins on the basis of whether or not their combat-exposed brother developed PTSD. Differences in cognitive performance between brothers might be interpreted as evidence for unique environmental effects (e.g., stress-induced toxicity related to combat). Differences in performance between the (high-risk) unexposed brothers of PTSD combat veterans and the (low-risk) unexposed brothers of non-ptsd combat veterans provide evidence for familial, probably pre-existing, traits. Sample Recruitment Participants were recruited with the assistance of the Vietnam Era Twin (VET) Registry, which determined zygosity (Eisen et al. 1989) and combat status (via military records) of each twin pair. Combat severity in the Ex twin was assessed with a standard 18-item combat exposure measure (Janes et al. 1991). A full description of the recruitment strategy appears elsewhere (Gilbertson et al. 2002, 2006; Orr et al. 2003). The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Veterans Affairs Medical Center (Manchester, New Hampshire) Institutional Review Board and Human Subjects Subcommittee. After a full explanation of the procedures, all individuals provided written informed consent before participation. The PTSD diagnostic status of combat-exposed twins and their overall PTSD symptom severity were determined by an experienced doctoral-level psychologist (NBL) with CAPS (Blake et al. 1995). All participants completed a Stressful Life Events Checklist (available upon request) designed to quantify the lifetime number of non-

4 Gilbertson et al. Page 4 combat events that potentially met DSM-IV PTSD A (stressor) criterion. All participants were interviewed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; First et al. 1994) to determine the presence of other Axis I mental disorders. The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (MAST; Selzer 1971) was employed as a measure of prior alcohol abuse. Combat veterans with past but not current PTSD were excluded. No participants met DSM-IV criteria for a psychotic or bipolar disorder or non combat-related PTSD. Owing to high comorbidity of major depression and substance use disorders with PTSD, these disorders were not exclusionary. Likewise, individuals reporting current psychotropic medication usage were not excluded from participation. Issues of comorbidity and medication use have been addressed statistically in this sample and reported in detail previously (Gilbertson et al. 2006). History of major neurologic disorder or significant abnormality discovered upon MRI scan (e.g., tumor) resulted in exclusion of two participants. Owing to a scheduling conflict and an acute medical condition, two participants were unable to complete neuropsychological testing. Data from both members of a twin pair were excluded from the analyses if either member was excluded. The final sample comprised 18 PTSD twin pairs and 23 non-ptsd twin pairs. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample appear in Table 1. Configural Cue Processing Patterned after a task described by Halpern (1986) and French et al. (1963), a Cube and Paper Test was constructed by the first author in an effort to assess the ability to mentally manipulate allocentric spatial cues. The Cube and Paper Test provides an untimed measure of visuospatial function in a multiple-choice format without a praxic component. The task is allocentric in nature (i.e., dependent upon configural relationships to neighboring stimuli and cannot be solved by merely referencing the midline axis of the participant). It is composed of 20 items of two types (see sample items in Figure 1). In one set of items, subjects are asked to imagine a piece of paper being folded and a series of holes being punched through all layers of the folded paper. Subjects are then asked to select the correct configural pattern of holes when the paper is unfolded. The second set of items is composed of cubes that contain markings (e.g., letters, numbers, geometric shapes) on each face, with the restriction that no item appears on more than one face of a given cube. Subjects are asked to examine a target cube and then select the cube among several alternatives that could be a different (rotated) view of the target cube. The two item types represent visuospatial constructs that are considered differentiable (McGee 1979) (i.e., orientation [cube rotation] and visualization [paper folding]), but the historic and empiric description of these two constructs both fall within the concept of allocentric processing as described here. We found the two item types to be highly and significantly intercorrelated within our sample, and the major analyses conducted in the current study found no differences dependent upon item type. Thus, the items were collapsed into a single measure with the total number of errors in the 20-item task serving as the dependent variable. The Cube and Paper Test was administered as part of a larger cognitive assessment battery employing more standard measures of neuropsychological function (Gilbertson et al. 2006). It was therefore possible to compare performance on the Cube and Paper Test with more standard measures of visuospatial function that are not allocentric in nature. These measures included the Block Design from the WAIS-R (Wechsler 1987), the Benton Facial Recognition Task (FRT; Benton and Van Allen 1973), and the Visual Object and Spatial Perception test (VOSP; Warrington and James 1991). Results of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; Delis et al. 1987), a measure of verbal declarative memory, were also available for comparison, as was an overall measure of estimated IQ (Gilbertson et al. 2006).

5 Gilbertson et al. Page 5 Hippocampus Volume: MRI Image Acquisition and Volumetric Analyses Statistical Analysis Results Results of volumetric analyses of the twin sample studied here have been previously reported (Gilbertson et al. 2002), as have the specific imaging techniques employed (Gurvits et al. 1996; Shenton et al. 1992). Total hippocampal volume was therefore available for correlation with performance on the Cube and Paper Test. Demographic and psychometric data were analyzed by means of a mixed model that treated PTSD Diagnosis (i.e., chronic combat-related PTSD vs. non-ptsd in the combat-exposed twin) as a between-pairs fixed effect, Combat Exposure (presence or absence) as a within-pair fixed effect, and twin pairs as a random effect (Little et al. 1996). The analysis yields a t statistic. If a dependent variable represents a PTSD risk factor, the model predicts a significant Diagnosis main effect in the absence of a Diagnosis Exposure interaction. If a dependent variable represents a traumatically-acquired PTSD sign, the model predicts a significant Diagnosis Exposure interaction. Follow-up contrasts were performed by means of independent t tests between combat-exposed participants with versus without PTSD and separately for respective (high-risk) unex-posed co-twins of combat-exposed twins with PTSD versus (low-risk) unexposed co-twins of combat-exposed twins without PTSD. Pearson correlations were calculated between the total CAPS scores (PTSD severity) of combat-exposed participants and their own as well as their unexposed brothers performances on the Cube and Paper Test. Two-tailed p values are reported for all tests. Finally, a stepwise regression analysis was performed in which hippocampal volume was regressed against IQ, two CVLT measures of verbal declarative memory (General Learning, Response Discrimination) found to differentiate PTSD and non-ptsd subjects (Gilbertson et al. 2006), and the Cube and Paper Test. A significance level of.15 was employed as the criterion for model entry. Demographics and Clinical Characteristics Group mean demographic data and statistical comparisons appear in Table 1. No significant main effects or interactions were found for age or education. Combat-exposed veterans with PTSD reported significantly higher levels of combat exposure and PTSD symptom severity (total CAPS score), relative to combat-exposed veterans with no history of PTSD. Lifetime alcohol abuse symptoms (MAST) showed a strong Diagnosis Exposure interaction, revealing that combat-exposed veterans with PTSD reported more severe alcohol histories than both their unexposed brothers and the non-ptsd twin pair members (both exposed and unexposed). Although the Diagnosis Exposure interaction failed to reach statistical significance for number of traumatic lifetime events (non-combat related), significant main effects for both Diagnosis and Exposure were found. The PTSD combat veterans reported more of these than non-ptsd combat veterans [ExP+ > ExP : t(35) = 2.6, p =.013] and more events than their own unexposed co-twins [ExP+ > UxP+: t(14) = 2.5, p =.028]. No significant difference was found between UxP+ and UxP groups [t(36) = 1.24, p =.224]. We have previously reported in this sample (Gilbertson et al. 2006) that the combat veterans with PTSD demonstrated significantly elevated histories (p <.05) of alcohol and drug use disorders, major depression, and psychotropic medication use relative to both their own twin brothers as well as combat veterans without PTSD (and their twin brothers). Furthermore, the unexposed brothers of combat veterans with versus without PTSD did not differ on any of these comorbidities (p >. 50). We examined the history of head injuries and found the same pattern. The PTSD combat

6 Gilbertson et al. Page 6 veterans reported more head injury than their identical twins (61% vs. 27%; McNemar's Test: p <.05) and non-ptsd combat veterans (61% vs. 18%; Fischer's Exact Test: p <.01). Unexposed brothers of combat veterans with versus without PTSD did not differ in head-injury history (27% vs. 22%; Fisher's Exact Test: p >.70). Seventy-two percent of reported head injuries involved loss of consciousness (no significant differences between groups), 90% of which were a few seconds to few minutes in duration. Contextual Cue Processing Group Comparisons On the Cube and Paper Test, a significant Diagnosis main effect [t (39) = 2.78, p =.008] was found in the absence of either a Combat Exposure main effect [t (40) = 1.13, p =.263] or a Diagnosis Exposure interaction [t(39) =.26, p =.797]. As shown in Figure 2, PTSD combat veterans (M = 8.7 errors, SD = 4.7) and their unexposed brothers (M = 8.0 errors, SD = 6.1) demonstrated more errors in configural cue processing than non- PTSD combat veterans (M = 5.3 errors, SD = 4.1) and their unexposed brothers (M = 4.2 errors, SD = 4.4). The effect size (Cohen's d) for the Diagnosis main effect was.89, compared with. 36 and.08 for Exposure main effect and Diagnosis Exposure interaction, respectively. Follow-up contrasts revealed that PTSD combat veterans made more errors than non-ptsd combat veterans [ExP+>ExP : t(39) = 2.43, p =.020], and a comparable relationship existed between their respective high-risk and low-risk co-twins [UxP+ > UxP : t(39) = 2.30, p =. 027]. The Diagnosis main effect remained significant after controlling for lifetime alcohol abuse [MAST; t(45.5) = 2.31, p =.026]. The diagnosis main effect also remained marginally significant after controlling for an IQ-based measure of visuospatial functioning [WAIS-R Block Design; t(38.9) = 1.98, p =.054] as well as in a model simultaneously controlling for all available visuospatial measures [FRT, VOSP, Block Design; t(39) = 2.02, p =.050]. The diagnosis main effect failed to maintain significance when controlling for general verbal functioning [WAIS-R Vocabulary; t(40.9) = 1.35, p =.186] or overall estimated IQ [t(41.4) = 1.00, p =.325]. Correlations With PTSD Symptom Severity and Combat Severity Severity of PTSD symptomatology (total CAPS score) in combat-exposed veterans was positively correlated with both their own Cube and Paper Test errors [r(41) =.40, p =.010] as well as those of their combat-unexposed brothers [r(41) =.31, p =.046]. Errors in the Cube and Paper Test by combat-exposed veterans were positively correlated with amount of combat exposure [r(41) =.33, p =.036]. The relationship between Cube and Paper Test performance and PTSD severity fell to a marginal level after controlling for amount of combat exposure [partial; r(41) =.26, p =.104]. However, interpretation of the relationship is complicated by the highly confounded relationship between PTSD severity and amount of combat exposure [r(41) =.64, p <.0001]. Regression Analysis of Hippocampal Volume and Cognitive Measures A stepwise regression analysis was performed regressing total MRI hippocampal volume against estimated IQ, two measures of CVLT verbal memory function (General Learning, Response Discrimination), and the Cube and Paper Test. The Cube and Paper Test performance was found to be the only predictor of total hippocampal volume [F = 6.23, p =.015]; no other variable entered into the model at a significance level < p =.15. Furthermore, when IQ and memory measures were forced first into the regression model, the addition of Cube and Paper Test performance was significant [Cube and Paper: F = 3.98, p =.050; IQ: F =.02, p =.901; General Learning: F =.04, p =.848; Response Discrimination: F =.60, p =.439]. Configural cue processing alone accounted for 8.0% of total hippocampal volume variance compared with 3.6% of hippocampal volume variance explained by the combined variance of IQ and

7 Gilbertson et al. Page 7 Discussion declarative memory. The full model containing all four cognitive measures accounted for 8.9% of total hippocampal volume variance. Finally, when alcohol history and either total grey matter volume or total amygdala volume were also added simultaneously to the model, Cube and Paper Test performance continued to be the only significant predictor of hippocampal volume (however, in the case of adding total grey matter volume to the model, both Cube and Paper Test and total grey matter volume predicted hippocampal volume, with Cube and Paper Test somewhat attenuated [i.e., p =.068]). The current study provides evidence supporting the presence of allocentric spatial processing deficits in chronic PTSD. Although previous studies have not consistently documented general visuospatial impairment in PTSD, prior investigations have largely employed egocentric (in reference to body-midline) rather than allocentric (related to configural relationships among distal stimuli) tasks. The specific nature of this spatial deficit was demonstrated in the current study by the fact that differences in allocentric performance were maintained even after controlling for multiple measures of general visuospatial ability. Of additional interest is the fact that the difference observed in allocentric spatial processing between PTSD and non-ptsd combat veterans in this study seems to represent a pre-existing familial risk factor as evidenced by the presence of a comparable deficit in their identical cotwin brothers who had neither combat-exposure nor PTSD. These high risk co-twins demonstrated significantly impaired performance relative to the non-ptsd combat veterans and their low risk unexposed co-twins. Furthermore, the severity of PTSD symptomatology in combat-exposed participants was predicted not only by their own performance on the Cube and Paper Test but also by the performance of their unexposed identical co-twins. We have previously demonstrated in this sample (Gilbertson et al. 2002, 2006) that the deficits observed in the familial vulnerability pattern cannot be explained by comorbidity confounds (e.g., alcohol/substance abuse, depression) or medication use. In the current study, we also extend this finding to history of head injury. The ExP+ twins and their UxP+ co-twins differ in these comorbid conditions (i.e., UxP+ co-twins do not show elevated comorbid conditions, history of head injury, or medication use), yet the ExP+ and UxP+ co-twins show similar performance in the dependent variable of interest, in this case allocentric spatial processing. When placed in a prediction model alongside measures of IQ and verbal declarative memory, Cube and Paper Test performance was the best (and only) predictor of hippocampal volume in study participants. We found no significant relationship between hippocampal volume and the measures of verbal declarative memory and IQ that differentiated PTSD and non-ptsd veterans in this sample (Gilbertson et al. 2006). This is the first study to provide evidence that the role of hippocampal morphology in PTSD might be related to allocentric spatial processing. It should be noted, however, that our findings do not clearly establish the overall specificity of the hippocampus in mediating this task, and non-hippocampal regions (e.g., posterior parietal, orbitofrontal) might be expected to impact performance as well. In animal studies, Corwin et al. (1994) demonstrated the potential relevance of orbitofrontal cortex to allocentric processing, although human studies have been less compelling (Galati et al. 2000; Neggers et al. 2006). The fact that performance on the Cube and Paper Test continued to predict hippocampal volume even after adding amygdala volume or total grey matter volume to the regression model is suggestive of a prominent role for the hippocampus. However, it would be premature to rule out the impact of other brain regions, and more sophisticated allocentric tasks should be employed in future studies. Configural cue processing deficits provide support, albeit of a preliminary nature, for what we believe to be an intuitively appealing, alternative model of hippocampal function in PTSD.

8 Gilbertson et al. Page 8 This model, which has been broadly suggested by Davidson et al. (Davidson 2000; Davidson et al. 2000), asserts that the role of the hippocampus in psychopathological disorders such as PTSD might involve deficits in the ability to encode context (e.g., patterns of environmental cues) to regulate emotional responses. Building on the general role attributed to the hippocampus in the spatial processing of environmental cues (e.g., spatial map theory of O'Keefe and Nadel [1978]), numerous studies have begun to more specifically delineate the contribution of the hippocampus in shaping fear acquisition and extinction via contextual processing (Anagnostaras et al. 1995; Corcoran and Maren 2001; Frankland et al. 1998; Phillips and LeDoux 1994; Winocur 1997). Background environmental cues might be important in setting the scene for interpreting the fear relevance of a given CS (Schmajuk and Buhusi 1997; i.e., context-appropriate fear responding). Whereas the amygdala has been shown to play a critical role in the mediation of discrete CS CR pairings, animal studies have demonstrated that lesions of the hippocampus (particularly the dorsal hippocampus) prevent or alter the acquisition and expression of many forms of contextual fear conditioning. In the presence of compromised hippocampal capacity, animals might be less proficient in their ability to encode complex background environment to suppress context-inappropriate conditioned responses. As noted previously, the hippocampus has been shown to be especially relevant to the mediation of allocentric as opposed to egocentric spatial cues (Burgess et al. 1999), a function that makes it ideal for complex, configural contextual processing. Other nonhippocampal brain regions can support the limited processing of context at a more elemental or individual cue level. However, an intact hippocampus seems to retain a relatively unique capacity for integrating multiple individual elements into a relational whole while at the same time suppressing more elemental cue processing by other brain regions (Driscoll et al. 2005; Maren 2001; Sutherland et al. 1989). Reduced hippocampal capacity would therefore be expected to result in a relative over-reliance on contextual conditioning to simple, elemental cues mediated by non-hippocampal brain regions. A larger or more functional hippocampus, which monitors the presence and configural relationship of multiple cues within an environment and not merely the presence of individual or isolated cues, might better discriminate whether a particular contextual environment appropriately signals the relevance of a conditioned fear response. The potential significance of hippocampus-based, allocentric processing of contextual cues for the pathophysiology of PTSD is perhaps best made evident in animal studies of extinction (i.e., loss of conditioned fear response when a CS is repeatedly presented in the absence of the US). It is generally accepted that stimulus-response conditioning has relative permanency (i.e., extinction does not destroy or cause an unlearning of the original conditioned response) (Bouton 2002; Rescorla 2001). Rather, inhibitory processes can temporarily or situationally suppress expression of the CR, resulting in extinction. One such inhibitory process contributing to extinction is environmental context (Bouton 1993, 2000). In an important demonstration of this process in relation to hippocampal morphology, Frankland et al. (1998) found that animals that received hippocampal lesions before fear conditioning were unable to fully differentiate aversive versus non-aversive contextual environments and failed to effectively extinguish conditioned fear responses in a new chamber that had no history of aversive conditioning. The mere presence of isolated cues associated with the original aversive chamber seemed capable of signaling the relevance of a fear response in the safe chamber. Frankland et al. replicated this result in mice with genetic mutations associated with compromised hippocampal function. We propose that, consistent with this formulation, smaller hippocampal volume before trauma exposure might lead to failures in context-appropriate extinction of conditioned emotional responses, resulting in unremitting PTSD symptomatology. In the clinical literature, the term contextualization has been coined to describe a patient's capacity to discriminate between then (original traumatic environment) and now (current

9 Gilbertson et al. Page 9 environment), contributing to the persistence of PTSD-related symptoms (Ehlers and Clark 2000; Foa and Rothbaum 1998). The hippocampus might represent a biological substrate for this process, and the inability to adequately engage allocentric processing of contextual information might provide a model for understanding the persistent nature of conditioned emotional responses in chronic PTSD patients after removal from the original traumatic environment. The well-described re-experiencing symptoms of PTSD as documented in the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association 1994; e.g., physiological reactivity on exposure to internal or external cues [criterion B(5)]) would be predicted by impaired hippocampal capacity to decode configural arrays of cues that would normally suppress inappropriate fear responses to the mere presence of isolated individual cues. Thus, for example, a combat veteran with unremitting PTSD who is no longer in the combat field might experience significant conditioned physiological and emotional responses when exposed to a single cue (e.g., sound of a helicopter, smell of gun powder, sight of a person who shares physical features with the former enemy, etc.) even though this cue exists in an array of complex cues that otherwise defines the context as safe. A number of limitations to the current study should be acknowledged. We employed a rather simple paper-and-pencil task in our assessment of allocentric spatial processing. More complex procedures for measuring allocentric spatial processing with three-dimensional space or virtual reality (Astur et al. 2002; Driscoll et al. 2003) should be employed in future studies. Future studies employing psychophysiological conditioning and extinction protocols with human participants diagnosed with PTSD will also be required to more fully validate the model proposed here. Future studies will also require the inclusion of both remitted and unremitted populations of PTSD patients to more clearly establish a relationship between the course or chronicity of PTSD and allocentric processing. An additional caution should be raised regarding the potentially confounding factor of overall intelligence. Cube and Paper Test differences did not maintain statistical significance when the covariates of verbal and overall IQ were taken into account. As described by Miller and Chapman (2001), the interpretation of covariance analyses must be cautious when the covariate (i.e., IQ) differs between experimental groups (i.e., PTSD vs. non-ptsd). Controlling for differences in IQ under these circumstances can statistically underestimate the true difference and significance level of the dependent measure. Nonetheless, our findings can not definitively rule out the possibility that Cube and Paper performance reflects overall poorer cognitive functioning. A number of factors potentially mitigate this concern. First, the viewpoint that Cube and Paper performance is not simply synonymous with IQ is supported by the observation that the Cube and Paper task was predictive of hippocampal volume whereas IQ was not (likewise, Cube and Paper performance was predictive of hippocampal volume even when IQ was accounted for). Furthermore, not all aspects of IQ significantly attenuated the effects of Cube and Paper test performance (i.e., it remained significant when controlling for nonverbal IQ). Finally, the finding that other cognitive tasks were highly correlated with IQ in our sample (e.g., VOSP, FRT) yet did not differentiate PTSD and non-ptsd subjects (Gilbertson et al. 2006) again mitigates against the notion that IQ alone is the relevant factor. In recent years, neuroimaging studies have yielded an impressive array of findings uncovering significant brain-based differences associated with PTSD. These exciting developments have usefulness to the degree that coherent links can be established to the specific clinical phenomenology of interest. Findings of the current study suggest specific mechanisms by which hippocampal morphology and function might mediate particular clinical features of chronic PTSD. These findings further point to the potential importance of examining preexisting, brain-based cognitive differences that might impact how a traumatic event is processed and therefore affect the severity and/or longevity of associated symptoms. Ultimately, the elucidation of these brain-based cognitive processes might guide the

10 Gilbertson et al. Page 10 development of new behavioral treatment strategies (e.g., remediation of contextual cue deficits) that might improve the treatment of intractable or treatment-resistant PTSD. Acknowledgments References This work was supported by Department of Veterans Affairs Merit Review Grants (MWG, SPO), USPHS Grant R01- MH54636 (RKP), and USPHS Grant K02-MH01110 (MES). We would like to thank M. Macklin, K. Sheldon, H. Croteau, and the VA Cooperative Studies VET Registry (M.E. Vitek, K. Bukowski, R. Havlicek, T. Colton, W.E. Nance, R.S. Paffenbarger Jr., M.M. Weissman, and R.R. Williams) for their assistance in this project. We gratefully acknowledge the participation of the veterans and their twin brothers. Without their contribution this research would not have been possible. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Vol. 4th ed.. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: Anagnostaras SG, Maren S, Fanselow MS. Scopolamine selectively disrupts the acquisition of contextual fear conditioning in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1995;64: [PubMed: ] Astur RS, Taylor LB, Mamelak AN, Philpott L, Sutherland RJ. Humans with hippocampus damage display severe spatial memory impairments in a virtual Morris water task. Behav Brain Res 2002;132: [PubMed: ] Beers SR, De Bellis MD. Neuropsychological function in children with maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2002;159: [PubMed: ] Benton, AL.; Van Allen, MW. Test of Facial Recognition: Neurosensory Center Publication No University of Iowa; Iowa City, Iowa: Blake DD, Weathers FW, Nagy LM, Kaloupek DG, Gusman FD, Charney DS, Keane TM. The development of a Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale. J Trauma Stress 1995;8: [PubMed: ] Bonne O, Brandes D, Gilboa A, Gomori JM, Shenton ME, Pitman RK, Shalev AY. Longitudinal MRI study of hippocampal volume in trauma survivors with PTSD. Am J Psychiatry 2001;158: [PubMed: ] Bouton ME. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychol Bull 1993;114: [PubMed: ] Bouton ME. A learning theory perspective on lapse, relapse, and the maintenance of behavior change. Health Psychol 2000;19: [PubMed: ] Bouton ME. Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biol Psychiatry 2002;52: [PubMed: ] Bremner JD, Randall P, Scott TM, Bronen RA, Seibyl JP, Southwick SM, et al. MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1995a;152: [PubMed: ] Bremner JD, Randall P, Scott TM, Capelli S, Delaney R, McCarthy G, Charney DS. Deficits in shortterm memory in adult survivors of childhood abuse. Psychiatry Res 1995b;59: [PubMed: ] Bremner JD, Randall P, Vermetten E, Staib L, Bronen RA, Mazure C, et al. Magnetic resonance imagingbased measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse a preliminary report. Biol Psychiatry 1997;41: [PubMed: ] Bremner JD, Scott TM, Delaney RC, Southwick SM, Mason JW, Johnson DR, et al. Deficits in shortterm memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1993;150: [PubMed: ] Burgess, N.; Jeffrey, K.; O'Keefe, J. Integrating hippocampal and parietal functions: A spatial point of view.. In: Burgess, N.; Jeffrey, K.; O'Keefe, J., editors. The Hippocampal and Parietal Foundations of Spatial Cognition. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 1999.

11 Gilbertson et al. Page 11 Carrion VG, Weems CF, Eliez S, Patwardhan A, Brown W, Ray RD, Reiss AL. Attenuation of frontal asymmetry in pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2001;50: [PubMed: ] Corcoran KA, Maren S. Hippocampal inactivation disrupts contextual retrieval of fear memory after extinction. J Neurosci 2001;21: [PubMed: ] Corwin JV, Fussinger M, Meyer RC, King VR, Reep RL. Bilateral destruction of the ventrolateral orbital cortex produces allocentric but not egocentric spatial deficits in rats. Behav Brain Res 1994;61: [PubMed: ] Davidson RJ. Cognitive neuroscience needs affective neuroscience (and vice versa). Brain Cogn 2000;42: [PubMed: ] Davidson RJ, Jackson DC, Kalin NH. Emotion, plasticity, context, and regulation: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Psychol Bull 2000;126: [PubMed: ] De Bellis MD, Hall J, Boring AM, Frustaci K, Moritz G. A pilot longitudinal study of hippocampal volumes in pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2001;50: [PubMed: ] De Bellis MD, Keshavan MS, Clark DB, Casey BJ, Giedd JN, Boring AM, et al. A.E. Bennett Research Award. Developmental traumatology. Part II: Brain development. Biol Psychiatry 1999;45: [PubMed: ] Delis, DC.; Kramer, JH.; Kaplan, E.; Ober, BA. California Verbal Learning Test Adult Version. The Psychological Corporation; New York: Driscoll I, Hamilton DA, Petropoulos H, Yeo RA, Brooks WM, Baumgartner RN, Sutherland RJ. The aging hippocampus: Cognitive, biochemical and structural findings. Cereb Cortex 2003;13: [PubMed: ] Driscoll I, Howard SR, Prusky GT, Rudy JW, Sutherland RJ. Seahorse wins all races: Hippocampus participates in both linear and non-linear visual discrimination learning. Behav Brain Res 2005;164: [PubMed: ] Ehlers A, Clark DM. A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther 2000;38: [PubMed: ] Eisen S, Neuman R, Goldberg J, Rice J, True W. Determining zygosity in the Vietnam Era Twin Registry: An approach using questionnaires. Clin Genet 1989;35: [PubMed: ] First, MB.; Spitzer, RL.; Gibbon, M.; Williams, JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM-IV Disorders, Version 2.0. Biometrics Research Department; New York: Foa, EB.; Rothbaum, BO. Cognitive-Behavior Therapy for PTSD. The Guilford Press; New York: Treating the Trauma of Rape.. Frankland PW, Cestari V, Filipkowski RK, McDonald RJ, Silva AJ. The dorsal hippocampus is essential for context discrimination but not for contextual conditioning. Behav Neurosci 1998;112: [PubMed: ] French, JW.; Ekstrom, RT.; Price, LA. Kit of Reference Tests for Cognitive Factors. Educational Testing Service; Princeton, New Jersey: Galati G, Lobel E, Guiseppe V, Berthoz A, Pizzamiglio L, Le Bihan D. The neural basis of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in humans: A functional magnetic resonance study. Exp Brain Res 2000;133: [PubMed: ] Gilbertson MW, Gurvits TV, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK. Multivariate assessment of explicit memory function in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress 2001;14: [PubMed: ] Gilbertson MW, Gurvits TV, Lasko NB, Pitman RK. Neuropsychological assessment of Vietnam combat veterans with and without PTSD. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1997;821: [PubMed: ] Gilbertson MW, Paulus LA, Williston SK, Gurvits TV, Lasko NB, Shenton ME, et al. Neurocognitive function in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: Relationship to posttraumatic stress disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 2006;115: [PubMed: ] Gilbertson MW, Shenton ME, Ciszewski A, Kasai K, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK. Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nat Neurosci 2002;5: [PubMed: ]

12 Gilbertson et al. Page 12 Golier J, Yehuda R, Lupien SJ, Harvey PD, Grossman R, Elkin A. Memory performance in Holocaust survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2002;159: [PubMed: ] Gurvits TV, Gilbertson MW, Lasko NB, Tarhan AS, Simeon D, Macklin ML, et al. Neurologic soft signs in chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57: [PubMed: ] Gurvits TV, Lasko NB, Repak AL, Metzger LJ, Orr SP, Pitman RK. Performance on visuospatial copying tasks in individuals with chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res 2002;112: [PubMed: ] Gurvits TV, Shenton ME, Hokama H, Ohta H, Lasko NB, Gilbertson MW, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging study of hippocampal volume in chronic, combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1996;40: [PubMed: ] Halpern, DF. Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities. Lawrence Erlbaum; Hillsdale, New Jersey: Holland PC, Bouton ME. Hippocampus and context in classical conditioning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1999;9: [PubMed: ] Horner MD, Hamner MB. Neurocognitive functioning in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychol Rev 2002;12: [PubMed: ] Incisa della Rocchetta A, Samson S, Ehrle N, Denos M, Hasboun D, Baulac M. Memory for visuospatial location following selective hippocampal sclerosis: The use of different coordinate systems. Neuropsychology 2004;18: [PubMed: ] Janes GR, Goldberg J, Eisen SA, True WR. Reliability and validity of a combat exposure index for Vietnam era veterans. J Clin Psychol 1991;47: [PubMed: ] Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995;52: [PubMed: ] Koenen KC, Stellman JM, Stellman SD, Sommer JF Jr. Risk factors for course of posttraumatic stress disorder among Vietnam veterans: A 14-year follow-up of American Legionnaires. J Consult Clin Psychol 2003;71: [PubMed: ] Little, RC.; Milliken, GA.; Stroup, WW.; Wolfinger, RD. SAS System for Mixed Models. SAS Institute; Cary, North Carolina: Maren S. Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci 2001;24: [PubMed: ] Maren S, Aharonov G, Fanselow MS. Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Behav Brain Res 1997;88: [PubMed: ] McDonald RJ, Ko CH, Hong NS. Attenuation of context-specific inhibition on reversal learning of a stimulus-response task in rats with neurotoxic hippocampal damage. Behav Brain Res 2002;136: [PubMed: ] McGee MG. Human spatial abilities: Psychometric studies and environmental, genetic, hormonal, and neurological influences. Psychol Bull 1979;86: [PubMed: ] Miller GA, Chapman JP. Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. J Abnorm Psychol 2001;110: [PubMed: ] Moses SN, Sutherland RJ, McDonald RJ. Differential involvement of amygdala and hippocampus in responding to novel objects and contexts. Brain Res Bull 2002;58: [PubMed: ] Neggers SFW, Van der Lubbe RHJ, Ramsey NF, Postma A. Interactions between ego- and allocentric neuronal representations of space. Neuro-Image 2006;31: [PubMed: ] O'Keefe, J.; Nadel, L. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford University Press; Oxford: Orr SP, Metzger LJ, Lasko NB, Macklin ML, Hu FB, Shalev AY, Pitman RK. Physiologic responses to sudden, loud tones in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: Association with posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;60: [PubMed: ] Orr SP, Pitman RK, Lasko NB, Herz LR. Psychophysiological assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder imagery in World War II and Korean combat veterans. J Abnorm Psychol 1993;102: [PubMed: ] Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 1992;106: [PubMed: ]

13 Gilbertson et al. Page 13 Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Lesions of the dorsal hippocampal formation interfere with background but not foreground contextual fear conditioning. Learn Mem 1994;1: [PubMed: ] Rescorla RA. Retraining of extinguished Pavlovian stimuli. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2001;27: [PubMed: ] Schmajuk NA, Buhusi CV. Stimulus configuration, occasion setting, and the hippocampus. Behav Neurosci 1997;111: [PubMed: ]appendix 258 Selden NR, Everitt BJ, Jarrard LE, Robbins TW. Complementary roles for the amygdala and hippocampus in aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience 1991;42: [PubMed: ] Selzer ML. The Michigan alcoholism screening test: The quest for a new diagnostic instrument. Am J Psychiatry 1971;127: [PubMed: ] Shenton ME, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, Pollak SD, LeMay M, Wible CG, et al. Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe and thought disorder in schizophrenia. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. N Engl J Med 1992;327: [PubMed: ] Squire LR. Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev 1992;99: [PubMed: ] Stein MB, Koverola C, Hanna C, Torchia MG, McClarty B. Hippocampal volume in women victimized by childhood sexual abuse. Psychol Med 1997;27: [PubMed: ] Sutherland RJ, McDonald RJ, Hill CR, Rudy JW. Damage to the hippocampal formation in rats selectively impairs the ability to learn cue relationships. Behav Neural Biol 1989;52: [PubMed: ] Vasterling JJ, Brailey K, Constans JI, Sutker PB. Attention and memory dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychology 1998;12: [PubMed: ] Vasterling JJ, Brailey K, Sutker PB. Olfactory identification in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress 2000;13: [PubMed: ] Vasterling JJ, Duke LM, Brailey K, Constans JI, Allain AN Jr, Sutker PB. Attention, learning, and memory performances and intellectual resources in Vietnam veterans: PTSD and no disorder comparisons. Neuropsychology 2002;16:5 14. [PubMed: ] Villarreal G, Hamilton DA, Petropoulos H, Driscoll I, Rowland LM, Griego JA, et al. Reduced hippocampal volume and total white matter volume in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2002;52: [PubMed: ] Warrington, EK.; James, M. The Visual Object and Space Perception Battery. Thames Valley Test Company; Bury St. Edmunds, United Kingdom: Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. The Psychological Corporation; New York: Winocur G. Hippocampal lesions alter conditioning to conditional and contextual stimuli. Behav Brain Res 1997;88: [PubMed: ] Yehuda R, Keefe RS, Harvey PD, Levengood RA, Gerber DK, Geni J, Siever LJ. Learning and memory in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1995;152: [PubMed: ] Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR. Neuroanatomy of memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 1993;16: [PubMed: ]

14 Gilbertson et al. Page 14 Figure 1. Sample items from Cube & Paper Test.

15 Gilbertson et al. Page 15 Figure 2. Total errors on Cube & Paper Test.

Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma

Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma Mark W. Gilbertson 1,2, Martha E. Shenton 2 4, Aleksandra Ciszewski 4, Kiyoto Kasai 4, Natasha B. Lasko 1,2,5, Scott

More information

Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma

Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story

More information

Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD. Lei Zhang. Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center

Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD. Lei Zhang. Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center Running head: NEUROBEHAVIORAL MECHANISMS OF FEAR GENERALIZATION 1 Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD Lei Zhang Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center Running head:

More information

Longitudinal MRI Study of Hippocampal Volume in Trauma Survivors With PTSD

Longitudinal MRI Study of Hippocampal Volume in Trauma Survivors With PTSD Longitudinal MRI Study of Hippocampal Volume in Trauma Survivors With PTSD The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation

More information

IQ and Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms in Children Exposed to Interpersonal Violence

IQ and Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms in Children Exposed to Interpersonal Violence IQ and Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms in Children Exposed to Interpersonal Violence Kasey M. Saltzman, PhD Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Stanford University School of Medicine Carl F. Weems,

More information

STRESS, MEMORY AND BOSNIAN WAR VETERANS

STRESS, MEMORY AND BOSNIAN WAR VETERANS & STRESS, MEMORY AND BOSNIAN WAR VETERANS Aida Sarač Hadžihalilović ¹*, Amela Kulenović¹, Abdulah Kučukalić² ¹ Department for Anatomy, Faculty of Medicine, University of Sarajevo, Čekaluša 90, 71000 Sarajevo,

More information

Cavum septum pellucidum in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: relationship to posttraumatic stress disorder

Cavum septum pellucidum in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: relationship to posttraumatic stress disorder Cavum septum pellucidum in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: relationship to posttraumatic stress disorder The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how

More information

Neuropsychological Functioning in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Alcohol Abuse

Neuropsychological Functioning in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Alcohol Abuse Neuropsychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 20, No. 6, 716 726 0894-4105/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.6.716 Neuropsychological Functioning in Posttraumatic

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Ann N Y Acad Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 October 29.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Ann N Y Acad Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 October 29. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006 July ; 1071: 242 254. doi:10.1196/annals.1364.019. Clarifying the Origin of Biological Abnormalities in PTSD

More information

Probing reward function in post-traumatic stress disorder with beautiful facial images

Probing reward function in post-traumatic stress disorder with beautiful facial images Psychiatry Research 135 (2005) 179 183 www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres Probing reward function in post-traumatic stress disorder with beautiful facial images Igor Elman a, *, Dan Ariely b, Nina Mazar

More information

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and dissociative disorder among abused children

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and dissociative disorder among abused children Blackwell Publishing AsiaMelbourne, AustraliaPCNPsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences1323-13162006 Folia Publishing Society2006604434438Original ArticleADHD and dissociation in abused childrent. Endo et

More information

In the early aftermath of the Holocaust, an array of neuropsychological

In the early aftermath of the Holocaust, an array of neuropsychological Article Memory Performance in With Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Julia A. Golier, M.D. Rachel Yehuda, Ph.D. Sonia J. Lupien, Ph.D. Philip D. Harvey, Ph.D. Robert Grossman, M.D. Abbie Elkin, M.A. Objective:

More information

Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates

Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates Wen Cai, MD, Ph.D. Chief Medical Officer--La Frontera Arizona Clinical Associate Professor--Psychiatry and Psychology University of Arizona College

More information

THE WAR WITHIN Neuroimaging studies in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

THE WAR WITHIN Neuroimaging studies in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder THE WAR WITHIN Neuroimaging studies in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder ELBERT GEUZE Introduction Since World War II, the concept of war has changed considerably (see van Creveld (1991)). Modern forms of

More information

PTSD and the brain: What clinicians need to know

PTSD and the brain: What clinicians need to know PTSD and the brain: What clinicians need to know Namik Kirlić, MA Elana Newman, PhD Underexplored Territories in Trauma Education: Charting Frontiers for Clinicians and Researchers The University of Tulsa

More information

Clinical Relevance of Biological Alterations in PTSD. Rachel Yehuda, PhD Mount Sinai School of Medicine New York, NY

Clinical Relevance of Biological Alterations in PTSD. Rachel Yehuda, PhD Mount Sinai School of Medicine New York, NY Clinical Relevance of Biological Alterations in PTSD Rachel Yehuda, PhD Mount Sinai School of Medicine New York, NY New developments in PTSD Conceptual shift New findings of prevalence, longitudinal course,

More information

THE ROLE OF COGNITIVE SCHEMATA IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER: RESULTS OF CROSS-SECTIONAL AND LONGITUDINAL STUDIES

THE ROLE OF COGNITIVE SCHEMATA IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER: RESULTS OF CROSS-SECTIONAL AND LONGITUDINAL STUDIES International Journal of Occupational Medicine and Environmental Health 2011;24(1):29 35 DOI 10.2478/s13382-011-0010-6 THE ROLE OF COGNITIVE SCHEMATA IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER:

More information

Understanding the role of Acute Stress Disorder in trauma

Understanding the role of Acute Stress Disorder in trauma Understanding the role of Acute Stress Disorder in trauma Dr. Trina Hall Police Psychologist Dallas Police Department Lessons Learned: Unfolding the story of PTSD NAMI 2014 Fall Conference Trauma and

More information

Individual Differences in Psychophysiological Reactivity in Adults with Childhood Abuse

Individual Differences in Psychophysiological Reactivity in Adults with Childhood Abuse Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 9, 7 76 () Individual Differences in Psychophysiological Reactivity in Adults with Childhood Abuse Christian G. Schmahl, Bernet M. Elzinga

More information

Reducing Risk and Preventing Violence, Trauma, and the Use of Seclusion and Restraint Neurobiological & Psychological Effects of Trauma

Reducing Risk and Preventing Violence, Trauma, and the Use of Seclusion and Restraint Neurobiological & Psychological Effects of Trauma Reducing Risk and Preventing Violence, Trauma, and the Use of Seclusion and Restraint Neurobiological & Psychological Effects of Trauma Module created by Glenn Saxe, MD: 2002 revised 2009, 2011, 2013,

More information

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition 1 Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition Intro Memory Empirical Studies Interf MR Scan LTM Codes DCT Imagery & Spatial Cognition Rel Org Principles ImplEnc SpatEq Neuro Imaging Critique StruEq Prop

More information

SHORT REPORT. Is Acute Stress Disorder the optimal means to identify child and adolescent trauma survivors. at risk for later PTSD?

SHORT REPORT. Is Acute Stress Disorder the optimal means to identify child and adolescent trauma survivors. at risk for later PTSD? SHORT REPORT Is Acute Stress Disorder the optimal means to identify child and adolescent trauma survivors at risk for later PTSD? Tim Dalgleish PhD, Richard Meiser-Stedman PhD, Nancy Kassam-Adams PhD,

More information

APNA 25th Annual Conference October 19, Session 1022

APNA 25th Annual Conference October 19, Session 1022 When Words Are Not Enough The Use of Sensory Modulation Techniques to Replace Self- Injurious Behaviors in Patients with Borderline Personality Disorder General Organization of the Brain Lita Sabonis,

More information

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety C82NAB Neuroscience and Behaviour Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 1 Outline Emotion I (first part) Studying brain substrates of

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Nat Neurosci. 2006 August ; 9(8): 1004 1006. Maternal presence serves as a switch between learning fear and attraction in infancy

More information

Fatty-acid amide hydrolase polymorphisms and posttraumatic stress disorder after penetrating brain injury

Fatty-acid amide hydrolase polymorphisms and posttraumatic stress disorder after penetrating brain injury Citation: Transl Psychiatry (2012) 2, e75, doi:10.108/tp.2012.1 & 2012 Macmillan Publishers Limited All rights reserved 2158-188/12 www.nature.com/tp Fatty-acid amide hydrolase polymorphisms and posttraumatic

More information

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Biol Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 March 24.

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Biol Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 March 24. Fear and Anxiety from Principle to Practice: Implications for When to Treat Youth with Anxiety Disorders Andrew T. Drysdale a,b,*, Catherine A. Hartley a, Siobhan S. Pattwell a,c, Erika J. Ruberry e, Leah

More information

Structural and Functional Neuroplasticity in Relation to Traumatic Stress Iris-Tatjana Kolassa and Thomas Elbert

Structural and Functional Neuroplasticity in Relation to Traumatic Stress Iris-Tatjana Kolassa and Thomas Elbert Structural and Functional Neuroplasticity in Relation to Traumatic Stress Iris-Tatjana Kolassa and Thomas Elbert University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany ABSTRACT The body s stress response is an essential

More information

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments Annual Review of Cybertherapy and Telemedicine 2017 123 An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments Thomas D. PARSONS a,*, Paul SCHERMERHORN b, Timothy MCMAHAN a,

More information

Manual Supplement. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist (PCL)

Manual Supplement. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist (PCL) Manual Supplement V OLUME 1, I SSUE 1 N OVEMBER 18, 2014 Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist (PCL) The Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist (PCL) is one of the most frequently used standardized

More information

Losing one's way in a digital world: VR studies on navigation

Losing one's way in a digital world: VR studies on navigation Losing one's way in a digital world: VR studies on navigation Albert Postma, Jessie Bullens and Ineke J. M. van der Ham Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, the Netherlands

More information

The influence of pre-deployment neurocognitive functioning on post-deployment PTSD symptom outcomes among Iraq-deployed Army soldiers

The influence of pre-deployment neurocognitive functioning on post-deployment PTSD symptom outcomes among Iraq-deployed Army soldiers Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2009), 15, 840 852. Copyright INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2009. doi:10.1017/s1355617709990488 SYMPOSIUM The influence of pre-deployment

More information

Literature Review: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder as a Physical Injury Dao 1. Literature Review: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder as a Physical Injury

Literature Review: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder as a Physical Injury Dao 1. Literature Review: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder as a Physical Injury Literature Review: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder as a Physical Injury Dao 1 Literature Review: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder as a Physical Injury Amanda Dao University of California, Davis Literature Review:

More information

ADHD & PTSD. ADHD Across the Lifespan March 18, 2017 Andrea E. Spencer, MD.

ADHD & PTSD. ADHD Across the Lifespan March 18, 2017 Andrea E. Spencer, MD. ADHD & PTSD ADHD Across the Lifespan March 18, 2017 Andrea E. Spencer, MD Disclosures Neither I nor my partner has a relevant financial relationship with a commercial interest to disclose. Acknowledgments

More information

Word priming in schizophrenia: Associational and semantic influences

Word priming in schizophrenia: Associational and semantic influences Word priming in schizophrenia: Associational and semantic influences The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters. Citation

More information

Childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventative help:

Childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventative help: Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families Childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventative help: Understanding the link between childhood maltreatment and long-term

More information

Review of Research on Post-Traumatic Stress and Current Treatments. published in The San Francisco Psychologist, June 2005 issue, pp 6-7

Review of Research on Post-Traumatic Stress and Current Treatments. published in The San Francisco Psychologist, June 2005 issue, pp 6-7 Review of Research on Post-Traumatic Stress and Current Treatments published in The San Francisco Psychologist, June 2005 issue, pp 6-7 By Shelley F. Diamond, Ph.D. On May 6 th, 2005, approximately 20

More information

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory Systems Neuroscience November 29, 2016 Memory Gabriela Michel http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html Forms of memory Different types of learning & memory rely on different brain structures

More information

Serial model. Amnesia. Amnesia. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras. Lecture 3: HM, the medial temporal lobe, and amnesia

Serial model. Amnesia. Amnesia. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras. Lecture 3: HM, the medial temporal lobe, and amnesia Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Serial model Memory terminology based on information processing models e.g., Serial Model Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras Lecture 3: HM, the medial temporal lobe, and amnesia

More information

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT Why delaying the onset of alcohol and other drug use during adolescence is so important Aaron White, PhD Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research National Institute

More information

Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) Among People Living with HIV

Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) Among People Living with HIV Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) Among People Living with HIV Milton L. Wainberg, M.D. Associate Clinical Professor of Psychiatry College of Physicians and Surgeons Columbia University mlw35@columbia.edu

More information

Auditory Startle Response in Trauma Survivors With Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Prospective Study

Auditory Startle Response in Trauma Survivors With Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Prospective Study Auditory Startle Response in Trauma Survivors With Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Prospective Study Arieh Y. Shalev, M.D., Tuvia Peri, Ph.D., Dalia Brandes, M.A., Sara Freedman, M.Sc., Scott P. Orr,

More information

Can Traumatic Stress Alter the Brain? Understanding the Implications of Early Trauma on Brain Development and Learning

Can Traumatic Stress Alter the Brain? Understanding the Implications of Early Trauma on Brain Development and Learning Journal of Adolescent Health 51 (2012) S23 S28 www.jahonline.org Review article Can Traumatic Stress Alter the Brain? Understanding the Implications of Early Trauma on Brain Development and Learning Victor

More information

Effect of Darkness on Acoustic Startle in Vietnam Veterans With PTSD

Effect of Darkness on Acoustic Startle in Vietnam Veterans With PTSD GRILLON, EFFECT Am J OF Psychiatry MORGAN, DARKNESS 155:6, DAVIS, ON June ACOUSTIC 1998 ET AL. STARTLE Effect of Darkness on Acoustic Startle in Vietnam Veterans With PTSD Christian Grillon, Ph.D., Charles

More information

Neuroscience of Consciousness II

Neuroscience of Consciousness II 1 C83MAB: Mind and Brain Neuroscience of Consciousness II Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Consciousness State of consciousness - Being awake/alert/attentive/responsive Contents

More information

Memory Functioning in Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Objective Findings versus Subjective Complaints

Memory Functioning in Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Objective Findings versus Subjective Complaints SHORT COMMUNICATION Memory Functioning in Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Objective Findings versus Subjective Complaints Noelle E. Carlozzi 1,2 *, Celinda Reese-Melancon 3 & David G. Thomas 3 1 University

More information

Recent advances in the study of biological alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD)

Recent advances in the study of biological alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) Psychiatr Clin N Am 25 (2002) xi xv Preface Recent advances in the study of biological alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) Rachel Yehuda, PhD Guest Editor This issue of The Psychiatric

More information

Dichotic listening in schizotypal personality disorder: Evidence for gender and laterality effects

Dichotic listening in schizotypal personality disorder: Evidence for gender and laterality effects Dichotic listening in schizotypal personality disorder: Evidence for gender and laterality effects The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you.

More information

Neuroimaging for Diagnosis of Psychiatric Disorders

Neuroimaging for Diagnosis of Psychiatric Disorders Psychiatric Disorder Neuroimaging for Diagnosis of Psychiatric Disorders JMAJ 45(12): 538 544, 2002 Yoshio HIRAYASU Associate Professor, Department of Neuropsychiatry Kyorin University School of Medicine

More information

An Objective Assessment of Psychological Injuries in the 21 st Century

An Objective Assessment of Psychological Injuries in the 21 st Century An Objective Assessment of Psychological Injuries in the 21 st Century Professor Cherrie Galletly Discipline of Psychiatry, University of Adelaide Professor Alexander C. McFarlane The Centre of Military

More information

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Behavioural Brain Research 110 (2000) 97 108 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Stephen Maren *, William Holt Department of Psychology

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Biol Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 28.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Biol Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 28. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry. 1996 December 1; 40(11): 1091 1099. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00229-6. Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Hippocampal

More information

Impaired Transverse Patterning in Human Amnesia Is a Special Case of Impaired Memory for Two-Choice Discrimination Tasks

Impaired Transverse Patterning in Human Amnesia Is a Special Case of Impaired Memory for Two-Choice Discrimination Tasks Behavioral Neuroscience 999, Vol. 3, No.,3-9 In the public domain Impaired Transverse Patterning in Human Amnesia Is a Special Case of Impaired Memory for Two-Choice Discrimination Tasks Jonathan M. Reed

More information

Meiser-Stedman, R., Yule, W., Smith, W., Glucksman, E. & Dalgleish, T. (2005). Acute

Meiser-Stedman, R., Yule, W., Smith, W., Glucksman, E. & Dalgleish, T. (2005). Acute Meiser-Stedman, R., Yule, W., Smith, W., Glucksman, E. & Dalgleish, T. (2005). Acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in children and adolescents involved in assaults and motor vehicle

More information

PREVALENCE OF POST TRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER AMONG BASRAH MEDICAL STUDENTS

PREVALENCE OF POST TRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER AMONG BASRAH MEDICAL STUDENTS THE MEDICAL JOURNAL OF BASRAH UNIVERSITY PREVALENCE OF POST TRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER AMONG BASRAH MEDICAL STUDENTS Asaad Q. Al-Yassen, Aqeel Ibrahim Salih ABSTRACT Background Post traumatic stress disorder

More information

Rebecca Campbell, Ph.D. The Neurobiology of Sexual Assault. Ahrens, C., Abeling, S., Ahmad, S., & Himman, J. (2010). Spirituality and well-being: The

Rebecca Campbell, Ph.D. The Neurobiology of Sexual Assault. Ahrens, C., Abeling, S., Ahmad, S., & Himman, J. (2010). Spirituality and well-being: The Rebecca Campbell, Ph.D. The Neurobiology of Sexual Assault Ahrens, C., Abeling, S., Ahmad, S., & Himman, J. (2010). Spirituality and well-being: The relationship between religious coping and recovery from

More information

STRONG STAR and the Consortium to Alleviate PTSD (CAP)

STRONG STAR and the Consortium to Alleviate PTSD (CAP) STRONG STAR and the Consortium to Alleviate PTSD (CAP) Presentation to: San Antonio Combat PTSD Conference, October 18-19, 2017 Presentation by: Sheila A.M. Rauch, Ph.D. Biological and Symptom Changes

More information

Catherine Fassbender, Ph.D.

Catherine Fassbender, Ph.D. Catherine Fassbender, Ph.D. Fassbender Catherine, Ph.D., Assistant Professional Researcher, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, School of Medicine Education B.A., Psychology, University College

More information

Memory for context becomes less specific with time

Memory for context becomes less specific with time Brief Communication Memory for context becomes less specific with time Brian J. Wiltgen 1 and Alcino J. Silva Departments of Neurobiology, Psychology, Psychiatry and the Brain Research Institute, UCLA,

More information

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Ronna Fried, Ed.D. Director of Neuropsychology in the Clinical and Research Programs in Pediatric Psychopharmacology and Adult ADHD, Massachusetts

More information

Key words children; maternal posttraumatic stress symptoms; pediatric injury; posttraumatic

Key words children; maternal posttraumatic stress symptoms; pediatric injury; posttraumatic Brief Report: The Impact of Maternal Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms and Child Gender on Risk for Persistent Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms in Child Trauma Victims Sarah A. Ostrowski, 1

More information

Gender Sensitive Factors in Girls Delinquency

Gender Sensitive Factors in Girls Delinquency Gender Sensitive Factors in Girls Delinquency Diana Fishbein, Ph.D. Research Triangle Institute Transdisciplinary Behavioral Science Program Shari Miller-Johnson, Ph.D. Duke University Center for Child

More information

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Outline Neuroanatomy of emotion Critical conceptual, experimental design, and interpretation issues in neuroimaging research Fear and anxiety Neuroimaging research

More information

How does adversity in childhood get under the skin

How does adversity in childhood get under the skin How does adversity in childhood get under the skin What can we learn from neuroscience and epigenetics? Eamon McCrory Professor of Developmental Neuroscience & Psychopathology, UCL Consultant Clinical

More information

Trauma Centrality and PTSD Symptom Severity in Adult Survivors of Childhood Sexual Abuse

Trauma Centrality and PTSD Symptom Severity in Adult Survivors of Childhood Sexual Abuse Trauma Centrality and PTSD Symptom Severity in Adult Survivors of Childhood Sexual Abuse The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story

More information

What Can the Brain Teach Us About Treating PTSD? Thomas C. Neylan, MD Norbert Schuff, PhD Charles R. Marmar, MD Michael W.

What Can the Brain Teach Us About Treating PTSD? Thomas C. Neylan, MD Norbert Schuff, PhD Charles R. Marmar, MD Michael W. What Can the Brain Teach Us About Treating PTSD? Thomas C. Neylan, MD Norbert Schuff, PhD Charles R. Marmar, MD Michael W. Weiner, MD Stress and Biological Sciences Abram Kardiner (1891-1981) Described

More information

3. Title: Within Fluid Cognition: Fluid Processing and Fluid Storage?

3. Title: Within Fluid Cognition: Fluid Processing and Fluid Storage? Cowan commentary on Blair, Page 1 1. Commentary on Clancy Blair target article 2. Word counts: Abstract 62 Main text 1,066 References 487 (435 excluding 2 references already in the target article) Total

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) FOCUSED QUESTION

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) FOCUSED QUESTION CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) FOCUSED QUESTION In military veterans, what is the effect of virtual reality exposure therapy (VRE) on posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depressive symptoms, as

More information

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and 77 5 Discussion The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and implicit learning systems. Chapter 2 described the effects of performing a working memory task

More information

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Chapter Title Attention and Pavlovian Conditioning Copyright Year 2011 Copyright Holder Springer Science+Business Media, LLC Corresponding Author

More information

Index. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Index. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Index Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. A Age as factor in selective mutism, 623 as factor in social phobia, 623 Agoraphobia, 593 600 described, 594 596 DSM-V changes related to,

More information

Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of

Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of Introduction Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of scientific inquiry for over 100 years. In particular, this topic has had significant interest in the past

More information

THANK YOU! Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) 1/30/2015

THANK YOU! Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) 1/30/2015 THANK YOU! Fear Potentiated Startle and Fear Extinction in a Sample of Undergraduate Women Exposed to a Campus Mass Shooting Susan M. Hannan, M.A. Antonia V. Seligowski, M.A. Holly K. Orcutt, Ph.D. Our

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Classical Conditioning Learning through Association Ivan Pavlov discovered the form of learning called Classical

More information

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman BIOMED 509 Executive Control Cambridge University Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan UNM SOM JL Brigman 4-7-17 Symptoms and Assays of Cognitive Disorders Symptoms of Cognitive Disorders Learning & Memory

More information

Limbic system outline

Limbic system outline Limbic system outline 1 Introduction 4 The amygdala and emotion -history - theories of emotion - definition - fear and fear conditioning 2 Review of anatomy 5 The hippocampus - amygdaloid complex - septal

More information

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:721 727 DOI 10.3758/s13423-013-0523-z BRIEF REPORT Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory Karen L. Siedlecki & Timothy A. Salthouse Published online:

More information

Editorial Comments: Complex Developmental Trauma

Editorial Comments: Complex Developmental Trauma Journal of Traumatic Stress, Vol. 18, No. 5, October 2005, pp. 385 388 ( C 2005) Editorial Comments: Complex Developmental Trauma The diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) was included in the

More information

Comorbidity With Substance Abuse P a g e 1

Comorbidity With Substance Abuse P a g e 1 Comorbidity With Substance Abuse P a g e 1 Comorbidity With Substance Abuse Introduction This interesting session provided an overview of recent findings in the diagnosis and treatment of several psychiatric

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information

The Use of Collateral Reports for Patients with Bipolar and Substance Use Disorders

The Use of Collateral Reports for Patients with Bipolar and Substance Use Disorders AM. J. DRUG ALCOHOL ABUSE, 26(3), pp. 369 378 (2000) The Use of Collateral Reports for Patients with Bipolar and Substance Use Disorders Roger D. Weiss, M.D.* Shelly F. Greenfield, M.D., M.P.H. Margaret

More information

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear Historical views that thought and emotion were processed separately in the brain Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: So far, fear is the best understood What is fear? Dictionary: A feeling of agitation and

More information

The Consistency of Combat Exposure Reporting and Course of PTSD in Vietnam War Veterans

The Consistency of Combat Exposure Reporting and Course of PTSD in Vietnam War Veterans Journal of Traumatic Stress, Vol. 20, No. 1, February 2007, pp. 3 13 ( C 2007) The Consistency of Combat Exposure Reporting and Course of PTSD in Vietnam War Veterans K. C. Koenen Departments of Society,

More information

Posttraumatic stress disorder; combat exposure; and nicotine dependence, alcohol dependence, and major depression in male twins

Posttraumatic stress disorder; combat exposure; and nicotine dependence, alcohol dependence, and major depression in male twins Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Comprehensive Psychiatry 49 (2008) 297 304 www.elsevier.com/locate/comppsych Posttraumatic stress disorder; combat exposure; and nicotine dependence, alcohol dependence,

More information

Assessment of Memory

Assessment of Memory Journal of the K. S. C. N. Vol. 2, No. 2 Assessment of Memory Juhwa Lee Department of Neurology, College of Medicine, Kaemyung University - Abstract - The characteristics of human memory structure and

More information

EXAMPLES OF TRAUMA AND DISASTER CONFRONT US EVERYDAY

EXAMPLES OF TRAUMA AND DISASTER CONFRONT US EVERYDAY Disclosure Virtually Better, Inc (VBI), part owner managed under Emory University's Conflict of Interest Policy Virtual Iraq created by Dr. Skip Rizzo at USC, not VBI, but VBI is selling virtual reality

More information

Using immersive virtual reality to test allocentric spatial memory impairment following unilateral temporal lobectomy

Using immersive virtual reality to test allocentric spatial memory impairment following unilateral temporal lobectomy Using immersive virtual reality to test allocentric spatial memory impairment following unilateral temporal lobectomy G Morris, D Parslow and M D ecce 3 Neuropsychology Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, De

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet University of Amsterdam Supplementary Figures and Legends Day 1 Day 2 Day 3

More information

REFERENCES. Hemispheric Processing Asymmetries: Implications for Memory

REFERENCES. Hemispheric Processing Asymmetries: Implications for Memory TENNET XI 135 Results Allocentric WM. There was no difference between lesioned and control rats; i.e., there was an equal proportion of rats from the two groups in the target quadrant (saline, 100%; lesioned,

More information

Substance use and perceived symptom improvement among patients with bipolar disorder and substance dependence

Substance use and perceived symptom improvement among patients with bipolar disorder and substance dependence Journal of Affective Disorders 79 (2004) 279 283 Brief report Substance use and perceived symptom improvement among patients with bipolar disorder and substance dependence Roger D. Weiss a,b, *, Monika

More information

BM (MM030134); Meiser-Stedman.doc. Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in Children

BM (MM030134); Meiser-Stedman.doc. Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in Children BM-04-07-1038 (MM030134); 2005-07 Meiser-Stedman.doc Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in Children and Adolescents Involved in Assaults or Motor Vehicle Accidents Richard Meiser-Stedman,

More information

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist M------- P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION Name: Date of Birth: Date of Evaluation: 05-28-2015 Tests Administered: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth

More information

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Assessment and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder For this month s topic, I am excited to share my recent experience using the fifth edition of the Diagnostic

More information

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues Edward E. Smith Columbia University Outline Background Maintenance: Modality specificity and buffers Interference resolution: Distraction and

More information

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective MILITARY PSYCHOLOGY, 21:(Suppl. 1)S108 S112, 2009 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0899-5605 print / 1532-7876 online DOI: 10.1080/08995600802554748 Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future:

More information

DVHIP. TBI: Clinical Issues, Controversies, and Learning from Patients. Defense and Veterans Head Injury Program. What is Neuropsychology?

DVHIP. TBI: Clinical Issues, Controversies, and Learning from Patients. Defense and Veterans Head Injury Program. What is Neuropsychology? TBI: Clinical Issues, Controversies, and Learning from Patients DVHIP Defense and Veterans Head Injury Program Richard A. Lanham, Jr., Ph.D. Assistant Professor Division of Medical Psychology Psychiatry

More information

The Role of Emotion Suppression in Reaction Time and Threat Response During Highly Intense Stress. Alan Neider Briarcliff High School

The Role of Emotion Suppression in Reaction Time and Threat Response During Highly Intense Stress. Alan Neider Briarcliff High School The Role of Emotion Suppression in Reaction Time and Threat Response During Highly Intense Stress Alan Neider Briarcliff High School Purpose/Introduction This experiments goal was to determine the role

More information

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety Outline Neuroanatomy of emotion Fear and anxiety Brain imaging research on anxiety Brain functional activation fmri Brain functional connectivity fmri Brain structural

More information

Trauma processing and the development of posttraumatic stress disorder

Trauma processing and the development of posttraumatic stress disorder Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 36 (2005) 69 76 www.elsevier.com/locate/jbtep Trauma processing and the development of posttraumatic stress disorder Merel Kindt a,, Iris M. Engelhard

More information