Functional Anatomy of Biological Motion Perception in Posterior Temporal Cortex: An fmri Study of Eye, Mouth and Hand Movements

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Functional Anatomy of Biological Motion Perception in Posterior Temporal Cortex: An fmri Study of Eye, Mouth and Hand Movements"

Transcription

1 Cerebral Cortex December 2005;15: doi: /cercor/bhi064 Advance Access publication March 2, 2005 Functional Anatomy of Biological Motion Perception in Posterior Temporal Cortex: An fmri Study of Eye, Mouth and Hand Movements Kevin A. Pelphrey 1,2, James P. Morris 1, Charles R. Michelich 1, Truett Allison 3 and Gregory McCarthy 1,4 1 Duke-UNC Brain Imaging and Analysis Center, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA, 2 Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill School of Medicine, Chapel Hill, NC, USA, 3 Department of Neurology, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA and 4 Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Durham, NC, USA Passive viewing of biological motion engages extensive regions of the posterior temporal-occipital cortex in humans, particularly within and nearby the superior temporal sulcus (STS). Relatively little is known about the functional specificity of this area. Some recent studies have emphasized the perceived intentionality of the motion as a potential organizing principle, while others have suggested the existence of a somatotopy based upon the limb perceived in motion. Here we conducted an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment to compare activity elicited by movement of the eyes, mouth or hand. Each motion evoked robust activation in the right posterior temporal-occipital cortex. While there was substantial overlap of the activation maps in this region, the spatial distribution of hemodynamic response amplitudes differentiated the movements. Mouth movements elicited activity along the mid-posterior STS while eye movements elicited activity in more superior and posterior portions of the right posterior STS region. Hand movements activated more inferior and posterior portions of the STS region within the posterior continuing branch of the STS. Hand-evoked activity also extended into the inferior temporal, middle occipital and lingual gyri. This topography may, in part, reflect the role of particular body motions in different functional activities. Keywords: biological motion, fmri, social cognition, social perception, superior temporal sulcus Introduction The perception of biological motion selectively engages several distinct regions in the brain of the observer. Rizzolatti et al. (1996) first described activity in mirror neurons located in premotor cortex of monkeys perceiving the motions of others. Subsequent work revealed the existence of a somatotopic organization for perceived motion in several frontal brain regions in both humans and monkeys (Gallese et al., 1996; Buccino et al., 2001). Regions of the posterior lateral temporaloccipital cortex, particularly within and nearby the posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS) also respond to the perception of biological motion (reviewed in Decety and Gre` zes, 1999; Allison et al., 2000). Meta-analyses of the extant literature have suggested the existence of a coarse motion-based topography within this region. For example, Allison et al. (2000) noted in their review that in the STS region, the most posterior activation was evoked by the perception of hand action (Bonda et al., 1996), whereas the most anterior activation was evoked by the perception of mouth movement (Puce et al., 1998). This suggested a somatotopic organization roughly similar to that of the supplementary motor area, in which the hand area is posterior to the face area (Fried et al., 1991; Allison et al., 1996). More recent studies have examined activations evoked by different perceived motions using within-subjects designs. Wheaton et al. (2004) reported that within the right posterior STS, activity in response to viewing perceived facial feature movements was localized more anterior and lateral to activity evoked by observation of leg movements. Dubeau et al. (2001) reported in an abstract that activity in the STS to finger movements was localized most dorsally, whereas activity to mouth movements was localized anterior to eye movement activity in the STS. Other studies from our laboratory have demonstrated that the posterior lateral temporal-occipital region is sensitive to violations of the apparent goal of the observed motion, and have suggested that it participates in social perception via the analysis of the intentions conveyed by others actions. For example, the posterior STS responds more strongly when subjects observe characters making erroneous compared to correct gaze shifts (Pelphrey et al., 2003) or reaching-to-grasp actions (Pelphrey et al., 2004a). Furthermore, this area responds more strongly to mutual than to averted gaze during virtual social interactions (Pelphrey et al., 2004b). Others have reported differential activation in this region using non-biological stimuli. For example, Castelli et al. (2000) presented healthy control subjects with two triangles that produced motion in three different conditions: intentional (e.g. persuading ), goal-directed (e.g. chasing ) or random. STS activity was stronger to intentional and goal-directed motion compared to random motion. In addition, other functional neuroimaging studies (e.g. Fletcher et al., 1995; Brunet et al., 2000; Gallagher et al., 2003; Vogeley et al., 2001; Gre` zes et al., 2004; Saxe and Kanwisher, 2004) have reported activity in the STS region during various tasks that tap aspects of theory of mind (i.e. the ability to make inferences about the mental states of others and to make predictions about the behavior of others based on those inferences) (Premack and Woodruff, 1978). Taken together, these findings suggest that the functional organization of the lateral temporal-occipital region may be based upon yet-unknown social criteria such as motions that convey approach or withdrawal or intentional versus non-intentional motions. Here we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) experiment to determine whether a topography exists in the lateral posterior temporal-occipital cortex for three kinds of biological motion eyes, mouth, and hand movements made by animated characters. Materials and Methods Subjects Fifteen right-handed healthy young adults (eight females, seven males), age range years (mean = 24 years), provided written informed consent to participate in this study, which was approved by the Duke Ó The Author Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please journals.permissions@oupjournals.org

2 University Medical Center Institutional Review Board. Subjects were paid for participating. Experimental Design In this event-related design, subjects passively viewed three kinds of biological motion (eye, hand and mouth movements) at XGA resolution using MRI-compatible LCD virtual reality display goggles and the CIGAL stimulus presentation program (Voyvodic, 1999). The three experimental conditions were created using the Poser 4.0 Ò software program (Curious Labs Inc., Santa Cruz, USA) (Fig. 1). In each run, one of two animated characters (one male, one female) was presented from the midsection up with eyes forward and the left or right hand extended in front of the body with palm facing outward (Fig. 1, Still). This character remained onscreen in this posture throughout the scanning run, with experimental conditions differentiated by brief discrete movements. In the Mouth condition, the character opened and closed his or her mouth (Fig. 1, Mouth). In the Eyes condition, the character shifted his or her gaze to the left or the right and back to midline (Fig. 1, Eyes). In the Hand condition, the character moved his or her hand closing the palm to form a fist and then back to an open palm (Fig. 1, Hand). Each movement lasted 1 s. Each condition appeared (in random order) 64 times across 192 trials separated by a 17 s intertrial interval. An imaging session consisted of eight runs lasting 7.2 min each (24 trials per run). For the male and female characters, the hand that moved (left or right), and the direction of the eye movement (to the right or left) were counterbalanced across runs. Participants were instructed only to attend to the screen at all times. Figure 1. Experimental conditions. There were three experimental conditions: Eyes, Mouth and Hand. The animated character was always present. Trials were separated by a 17 s inter-trial interval, during which the figure was present on the screen (Still) but not moving. Imaging Scanning was performed on a General Electric (Waukesha, WI) 4 T LX NVi MRI scanner system equipped with 41 mt/m gradients. A quadrature birdcage radio frequency (RF) head coil was used for RF transmission and reception (General Electric, Waukesha, Wisconsin, USA). The subject s head was immobilized using a vacuum cushion and tape. Sixty-eight axial images were acquired using a three-dimensional fast SPGR pulse sequence (T R = 500 ms; T E = 20 ms; FOV = 24 cm; image matrix = ; voxel size = mm). These high resolution structural images were used for coregistration with the functional data. Functional images were acquired using a gradientrecalled inward spiral pulse sequence (Glover and Law, 2001; Guo and Song, 2003) sensitive to blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) contrast that allowed whole-brain coverage (T R = 1500 ms; T E = 35 ms; FOV = 24 cm; image matrix = 64 2 ; a = 62 ; voxel size = mm; 34 axial slices). The anterior and posterior commissures were identified in the mid-sagittal slice and used as landmarks for the prescription of BOLD contrast images. A semi-automated high-order shimming program optimized global field homogeneity. Runs consisted of 306 image volumes and began with four discarded RF excitations to allow for steady-state equilibrium. Image Preprocessing Image preprocessing was performed using SPM 99 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK) and custom MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA) scripts. Motion was detected by center of mass measurements. No volunteer had greater than a 3 mm deviation in the center of mass in the x-, y- or z-dimensions. Images were temporally adjusted to compensate for slice acquisition order and realigned to the tenth image to correct for head movements between scans. The realigned scans were then normalized to the Montre al Neurologic Institute Template found in SPM 99. This template conforms to the Montréal Neurologic Institute s (MNI) standardized brain space and closely approximates Talairach and Tournoux s (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) stereotaxic atlas. The functional data were high-pass filtered and spatially smoothed with an 8 mm isotropic Gaussian kernel prior to statistical analysis. These normalized and smoothed data were used in the analysis procedures described below. Data Analysis First, epochs synchronized to the movement onsets and containing two images preceding and nine images following the stimulus events were extracted from the continuous time series of image volumes. Epochs were segregated and averaged by movement condition (Hand, Eye or Mouth). The average BOLD intensity values were then converted to percent signal change relative to the 3 s prestimulus baseline period during which the figure was present but no movement was occurring. Second, voxel-based analyses identified activity associated with perception of each of the three kinds of biological motion through correlational analyses with a canonical reference hemodynamic response (HDR) waveform. On a subject-by-subject basis and for each of the three motion conditions, the average time courses for each condition from each voxel were correlated with the reference waveform and t- statistics were calculated from the correlation coefficients. This process provided three whole-brain normalized t-maps for each subject (one t-map for each stimulus condition). Third, across-subjects functional time course volumes and average t-statistic activation maps were computed for each of the three stimulus conditions, combining data from all 15 subjects. We created the average time courses by taking the arithmetic mean of the 15 subjects. The group-average t-maps were created by the average T method as described by Lazar et al. (2001). Next, using the group average time courses, we created greatest response topography maps in which each voxel that evinced a significant hemodynamic response to any one of the three conditions (i.e. the union of Eyes, Mouth and Hand) was color-coded by the condition where the greatest response was obtained (measured by the peak amplitude of the HDR waveform to each condition). These greatest response topography maps were masked by the results of the average t- statistic activation maps described, so that the greatest responses were only mapped for voxels that had a significant hemodynamic waveform in any of the three conditions. The threshold for significance set at t > 6.0 and a cluster size of three contiguous functional voxels. We also computed, for each subject, a t-statistic for each voxel on pair-wise comparisons between each of the three conditions. This analysis was based on time-course data for each condition (averaged over single trial repetitions of each condition) and was performed for each time point and computed across the single trial epochs. This provided a set of six four-dimensional images for each subject providing an estimate of the effect size (i.e. a t value) of the difference between each pair of conditions at each time point (e.g. Eyes versus Hand, Mouth versus Eyes, etc.). Then, on a subject-by-subject basis, we averaged the t values at each voxel for the time points s after stimulus onset (four time points) for each condition. This resulted in a measure of the Cerebral Cortex December 2005, V 15 N

3 Pelphrey difference between two movement conditions and provided six threedimensional statistical parametric maps (e.g. Eyes versus Hand, Mouth versus Eyes, etc.) for each subject. For each map, the t-statistic value at a given voxel represented an estimate of the effect size of the difference between the epoch average HDR waveforms within a time-window encompassing the expected peaks of the hemodynamic response. The individual subject t-statistic maps were then used in a randomeffects analysis across subjects. For each voxel in the MNI common space, each group of t values (six derived from each subject) was tested for a significant difference from zero using a one-sample t-test. This process provided six whole-brain normalized maps (one for each comparison: Eyes versus Mouth, Hand versus Eyes, etc.) of significance values from the random-effects analysis. Lower P-values indicated a larger difference between a voxel s waveform at peak for any two conditions. The threshold for significance was set at a voxelwise uncorrected P < 0.05 (two-tailed) and a spatial extent of three functional voxels (Forman et al., 1995; Xiong et al., 1995). Using logical and statements, we then created three normalized maps of significance values for voxels representing the intersection where one condition was greater than both of the other two conditions (e.g. Eyes > Mouth and Eyes > Hand). This provided binary (0 or 1) maps where ones represented voxels where one condition was significantly greater than both of the other two conditions with probability level P < (the product of the two prior probabilities) and a spatial extent of three voxels. We also used logical and/or statements to create binary maps of voxels that responded to both facial features (eyes and mouth), but not to hand movements. These face-dominant voxels were identified as those for which both Eyes and Mouth evoked a response that was significantly larger than the response to Hand, but for which the response evoked by Eye and Mouth did not differ significantly. Note that because of the way we defined these voxels (i.e. a significant response to Eyes and Hand) they could not overlap with voxels that responded most strongly to Eyes or with those that responded most strongly to Mouth. All of these maps were masked by the results of the fixed-effects correlational analysis (i.e. the union of the three stimulus conditions) described above to limit regions of activations to those shown to be significantly activated by at least one of the stimulus conditions with the threshold for significance set at t > 6.0 and a cluster size of three contiguous functional voxels. We selected this threshold for masking to guard against Type II errors at this stage of the analysis. Results All three motions activated posterior lateral temporal-occipital cortex, with prominent activation occurring in the right hemisphere where the STS bifurcates into the posterior portion of the main branch and its ascending limb. The location of the STS activity observed in this study (e.g. Fig. 2a,b), is consistent with the area of STS identified in prior reports by our laboratory of activity evoked by observed mouth (x = 50, y = 49, z = 3) and eye (x = 49, y = 49, z = 3) movements (see Puce et al., 1998: Fig. 9) and by observed reaching-to-grasp motions of the hand and arm (Pelphrey et al., 2004a; x = 55, y = 45, z = 12). This region is also consistent with that reported by Iacoboni et al. (2001) in their study of observed and/or imitated finger movements (see their Fig. 1, x = 57, y = 50, z = 16). Figure 2c,d shows greatest response topographies overlaid upon sagittal and coronal slices of the template brain image, and these maps are displayed on a flattened and inflated template brain surface in Figure 3. Red voxels exhibited the greatest response to Eyes, blue voxels to Mouth, and green voxels to Hand. If the spatial distribution of activation for the three movement conditions was the same, we would expect a random distribution of colors representing noise variation in activation amplitudes. As can be seen, the spatial distribution of amplitude differentiated activation evoked by the three kinds of motion revealed a topography along right lateral temporal-occipital cortex for the type of biological motion observed (Fig. 3). Along the right lateral temporal cortex and extending dorsally into parietal cortex and posteriorly into occipital cortex were large expanses of voxels in which one condition dominated. Activity evoked by Eye movement was greatest in more superior and posterior portions of the right posterior STS region. Activity evoked by Mouth movement was greatest in more anterior regions of the STS and superior and middle temporal gyri (STG and MTG). Activity evoked by Hand movement was localized to more inferior and posterior portions of the right STS region within the posterior continuing branch of the STS and extending into portions of occipital cortex, particularly the middle occipital gyrus (MOG) and the lingual gyrus (LG). Moreover, in the left STS region (Fig. 3, Fig. 2i,f), a cluster of Mouth-dominant voxels was observed. As illustrated by comparing the right and left hemisphere images of displayed in Figure 3, Eyes- and Handdominant voxels were notably absent in the left STS region and mouth voxels dominated activity in the left hemisphere STS. We examined the across-subject variability in the distribution of activations by examining each subject s greatest response topography. Greatest responses topographies in right lateral temporal-occipital cortex from six individuals are displayed in Figure 4. A majority (9 of 15) of subjects clearly showed the distribution of Eye, Hand and Mouth activations along the lateral temporal region observed in the group average, though individual variability was also apparent. We examined the individual data both in native and common space and the number of subjects exhibiting the reported pattern did not differ across these two approaches. Categorizing each voxel by the movement condition yielding the greatest activation allowed us to visualize a distribution of activity along the STS region, but this descriptive technique does not test the statistical significance of this distribution. We therefore conducted a random-effects analysis across the 15 subjects through which we identified clusters of voxels that responded significantly more to one condition than to the other two conditions (e.g. Eyes > Mouth, and Eyes > Hand). Table 1 presents the MNI stereotaxic coordinates of the centroids of activation within each cluster of voxels in the STS region identified in this random-effects analysis. These coordinates are presented along with the centers of activations identified in other functional neuroimaging studies of eye, hand and mouth movements. Voxels that responded most strongly to Eyes (red voxels) were localized to the right posterior STS region (Fig. 2e,f, x = 46, y = 58, z = 11). HDR waveforms from this region are plotted by condition in Figure 5a. Note that while Eyes evoked the strongest response from this region, significant responses to Hand and Mouth were also observed, and these responses did not differ significantly. A cluster of activity in response to Mouth (blue voxels) was localized anterior and inferior to the Eyes cluster in the right STS region (Fig. 2e,f, x = 53, y = 47, z = 4). The average responses from this region are plotted by condition in Figure 5b. In addition to the right STS activation to Mouth, a second cluster of voxels that responded most strongly to Mouth was localized to the left STS region (Fig. 2f,i, x = 60, y = 44, z = 7). The average responses from this region are plotted by condition in Figure 7b. Finally, a cluster of voxels active in response to Hand (green voxels) was localized to the right posterior inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) and extending into right occipital cortex (Fig. 2e, x = 49, y = 68, z = 4). The average time courses from this cluster are given in Figure 5c. Hand activity was localized posterior and inferior to Eye and Mouth activity Spatial Differentiation of Biological Motion d et al.

4 Figure 2. (a, b) Average t-statistic maps showing the results of a fixed effects analysis of the data thresholded at t [6.0 and clustered at three contiguous functional voxels. The dark to light color gradient indicates the strength (from weaker to stronger) of the correlation between the average hemodynamic response from each voxel and a canonical waveform. (c, d) Activation maps indicating the condition exhibiting the greatest response at each voxel. Red voxels exhibited the greatest response to Eyes, blue voxels to Mouth, and green voxels to Hand. Note that if an anatomical region did not exhibit a topography we would expect to see patches of interdigitated colors. The lateral temporal-occipital region exhibited a clear anterior-to-posterior distribution for mouth and eye activity. (e, f) Results of a random-effects analysis identifying voxels that responded significantly more to one condition than to the other two conditions (e.g. Eyes [ Mouth; Eyes [ Hand). Red voxels exhibited the greatest response to Eyes, blue voxels to Mouth, and green voxels to Hand. (g, h) Face voxels responded more strongly to Eyes and Mouth than to Hand and did not significantly differ in their response to Eye and Mouth. (i) A cluster of voxels that responded significantly more to Mouth than to Eyes and Hand was localized to the left STS region. The gold lines in panels f and h indicate the fundus of the STS. Cerebral Cortex December 2005, V 15 N

5 Pelphrey Figure 3. Activation maps combining data from all 15 subjects indicating the condition exhibiting the greatest response at each voxel overlaid on the right and left hemisphere flattened and inflated lateral brain surface. Surface models of a template brain were created and inflated. The gray-to-white shades illustrate curvature of the folded brain (dark gray 5 depths of the sulcus, white 5 crowns of the gyrus). Red voxels exhibited the greatest response to Eyes, blue voxels to Mouth, and green voxels to Hand. Figure 4. Greatest response topography maps from six individuals. The individual data are masked by the group average activation t-statistic map (t [ 6.0; three contiguous functional voxels). Red voxels exhibited the greatest response to Eyes, blue voxels to Mouth, and green voxels to Hand. In summary, a random-effects analysis revealed a statistically reliable topography in the right lateral temporal region for the type of biological motion observed. Activity elicited by eye movement was greatest in more superior and posterior portions of the right posterior STS region. Activity evoked by mouth movements was greatest in more anterior regions of the STS. Hand movements activated more inferior and posterior portions of the STS region. We conducted a secondary analysis to determine whether some voxels responded equivalently to the movement of face features (eyes or mouth), but not to hand movements. These face-dominant voxels were identified as those for which both 1870 Spatial Differentiation of Biological Motion d et al.

6 Table 1 Description of activation centers Movement/study Right Left x y z Location/BA x y z Location/BA Eyes Current study 46 ÿ58 11 STS/22 Dubeau et al. (2001) 58 ÿ52 08 MTG/21 ÿ56 ÿ48 08 MTG/21 Hoffman and Haxby (2000) 50 ÿ63 04 MTG/37 ÿ45 ÿ56 11 MTG/39 Pelphrey et al. (2004) 55 ÿ45 12 STS/22 Puce et al. (1998) 49 ÿ49 03 STS/22 ÿ46 ÿ MTG/39 Puce et al. (1998) 47 ÿ53 07 STS/22 ÿ49 ÿ MTG/21 Wicker et al. (1998) 52 ÿ ITG/19 ÿ44 ÿ68 04 MOG/37 Mean of prior studies 52 ÿ54 05 MTG/39 ÿ48 ÿ55 06 MTG/39 Mouth Current study 53 ÿ37 04 STS/22 ÿ60 ÿ44 07 STS/22 Calvert and Campbell (2003) 54 ÿ50 04 STS/22 ÿ48 ÿ46 04 STS/21 Calvert and Campbell (2003) 58 ÿ34 12 STS/22 ÿ50 ÿ38 12 STS/22 Calvert et al. (1997) 55 ÿ25 08 STS/41 ÿ52 ÿ STS/41 Calvert et al. (1997) 61 ÿ17 03 STS/22 ÿ61 ÿ28 13 STS/22 Dubeau et al. (2001) 58 ÿ42 06 STS/22 ÿ62 ÿ38 00 STS/21 Puce et al. (1998) 50 ÿ49 03 STS/22 ÿ47 ÿ50 02 MTG/37 Santi et al. (2003) 56 ÿ29 09 STS/41 ÿ STS/22 Mean of prior studies 56 ÿ35 06 STS/22 ÿ54 ÿ33 05 STS/22 Hand Current study 49 ÿ ITG/19 Bonda et al. (1996) ÿ48 ÿ61 17 MTG/19 Dubeau et al. (2001) 60 ÿ48 16 STS/22 ÿ52 ÿ50 14 STS/22 Grèzes et al. (1999) 46 ÿ STS/22 ÿ48 ÿ68 34 AG/39 Iacoboni et al. (2001) 57 ÿ STS/22 ÿ52 ÿ50 14 STS/22 Pelphrey et al. (2004a) 56 ÿ STS/22 Wheaton et al. (2004) 48 ÿ ITG/19 Mean of prior studies 53 ÿ53 07 STS/39 ÿ50 ÿ57 20 STS/39 The centers of STS region activation in this study are compared with those of other functional neuroimaging studies of biological motion perception. x, y and z refer to the MNI stereotaxic coordinates of the center activation within an ROI. BA, Broadmann s area; STS, superior temporal sulcus region (including the STS and the superior temporal gyrus); AG, angular gyrus; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; MOG, middle occipital gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus. Eyes and Mouth evoked a response that was significantly larger than to Hand, but for which the response evoked by Eye and Mouth did not differ significantly. A cluster of these voxels was localized to the right MTG (Fig. 2g,h, x= 62, y = 40, z = 4). Waveforms from this cluster are presented in Figure 5d. Although this study focused on lateral temporal cortex, several other regions were activated by the biological motion stimuli. A broad region of activation centered in the right middle frontal gyrus (MFG; x = 4, y = 30, z = 39) and extending into right anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and laterally into the left MFG and ACC responded to Eye and Mouth but not to Hand (Fig. 6a,f). The average time courses from this cluster are given in Figure 7a. A cluster of Mouth activation was localized to the left fusiform gyrus (FFG; x = 35, y = 51, z = 25) (Fig. 6b,c). Along ventral temporal-occipital cortex, Mouth activity was localized anterior to Hand activity. A large cluster of Hand activity was observed bilaterally but centered in the right lingual gyrus (LG; x = 7, y = 79, z = 0) (Fig. 6b,d,e). Waveforms from this LG Hand activation are presented in Figure 7c. A second cluster of Hand activity was centered in the left middle occipital gyrus (MOG; x = 46, y = 79, z = 0) and extended into the left middle and ITG (Fig. 6d). This region responded most strongly to Hand, but also responded robustly to Mouth and Eyes (Fig. 7d). In addition to the MFG activation to facial feature movements, other medial brain regions responded to three movement conditions (Fig. 6f) including the thalamus and colliculi (Fig. 7f). As illustrated in Figure 6f,g, activations to Eyes and Mouth movements dominated in anterior brain regions and Hand activations were most prominent in posterior brain regions. We also observed activations in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), insula and superior parietal lobule. Within the insula and IFG activations, Mouth activity was anterior to Eyes activity (Fig. 6g). The activation centroids reported in prior studies of eye, mouth and hand movements and the present study are very similar, although some variability is apparent (Table 1). Notably, in the right hemisphere, the average center for Eyes (x = 52, y = 54, z = 5) was located 19 mm posterior from the average center for Mouth (x = 56, y = 35, z = 6). In Figure 8, the centers from the current study, eight prior studies, and the means of the prior studies are shown projected (in the lateral--medial direction) onto the right lateral brain surface. We use the term centers to refer to the coordinates of the centroid of an area of activation or to the coordinates of the highest t or z value within an area of activation, depending on how the original authors reported their activation coordinates. Eye activations (red diamonds) were localized posterior to mouth activations (blue circles). This informal meta-analysis is thus consistent with the present results in suggesting an anterior--inferior to posterior--superior gradient for mouth and eye activations. We note that the lefthemisphere focus of Mouth activity observed in the current study is consistent with a trend towards Mouth-dominant activity in the left hemisphere and Eye-dominant activity in the right hemisphere that we observed during analysis of the neuroimaging data reported by Puce et al. (1998). Discussion We compared activity elicited by passive viewing of eyes, mouth and hand movements. Prominent activations for each movement were localized to right posterior temporal-occipital cortex. Eye activity was greatest in more superior and posterior Cerebral Cortex December 2005, V 15 N

7 Pelphrey Figure 5. Response properties of activated clusters. Average BOLD signal change time courses from the activated voxels from clusters in the STS region that responded most strongly to (a) Eyes, (b) Mouth, (c) Hand, and (d) Face. portions of the right STS region. Mouth activity was greatest in anterior regions of the STS, relative to eye activity. Hand activity was observed in more inferior and posterior portions of the lateral temporal-occipital region. Our results support the thesis that biological motion perception in posterior lateral temporal-occipital cortex is organized along a coarse somatotopy. How then, can these results be reconciled with recent studies from our laboratory and others that have implicated these same regions in the analysis of the goal-directedness, social meaning and intentionality of biological motion? One possibility is that these brain regions are initially activated in an obligatory manner and somatotopically, and that this information is fed forward to higher systems that analyze the goal-directed and intentional components of these motions. These higher systems may engage and maintain activation in the posterior lateral regions and thus this higherlevel processing is reflected in the activation patterns of these lower-level systems. Pelphrey et al. (2003) found that viewing apparent errors in eye gaze shifts with respect to target locations resulted in temporally extended activation of the right STS, as measured by the fmri hemodynamic response waveform. The locations of these putative higher systems within this model are unspecified and may be subsumed in the broad overlap of activation in the posterior STS i.e. a polysomatic integration area. It may also include the prefrontal regions activated in this study. Indeed, Blakemore and Decety (2001) posited that analysis of intentionality or theory of mind is, in part, a prefrontal function. An alternative model admits the empirical somatotopy reported here, but relegates it to an artifact related to the confounding of specific motions and likely intentions. This model proposes that the posterior STS is organized (as its primary function) to perceive the intentions of others actions. However, presentation of different biological motions (i.e. eye, hand, mouth movements) in isolation reveals a somatotopic distribution that is driven by each motion s specific recruitment of nearby and relatively distinct regions of posterior lateral temporal-occipital cortex in the service of action perception. Thus, the anterior-to-posterior distribution of mouth-to-eye activity presented here might reflect the differential involvement of the STS region in directing spatial attention and in social communication. The posterior/superior focus of eye movement evoked activity is well-positioned in relation to the frontoparietal network thought to be important for spatially directed attention (Corbetta et al., 1993; Nobre et al., 1997; Culham et al., 1998; reviewed in Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000). Similarly, the more anterior focus of mouth activity is well positioned near polysensory regions of the left and right STS and STG, which are known to be involved in the integration of audio and visual components of speech (Calvert et al., 1997, 2001; Calvert and Campbell, 2003; Wright et al., 2003). Consistent with this interpretation, the left STS focus of mouth activity is also well positioned in relation to classical left hemisphere language comprehension areas (Binder and Price, 2001; Dronkers et al., 2004; for reviews see also Mesulam, 1990; Price, 1998). The more inferior and posterior hand activity is localized close to the extrastriate body area (Downing et al., 2001), which is known to be involved in the visual perception of the human body and body parts (Grossman and Blake, 2002) and has recently been shown to respond to the performance (by the subject) of hand motor actions (Astafiev et al., 2004), even in the absence of visual feedback. Finally, Beauchamp et al. (2003) 1872 Spatial Differentiation of Biological Motion d et al.

8 Figure 6. Activation maps indicating areas of activation outside of the STS region. In addition to activity in the STS region, other brain regions were activated by the viewing the three kinds of biological motion. These included (a, f) the right and left medial frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex, (b, c, d) the fusiform and lingual gyri, (f) the thalamus and colliculi, and (g) inferior frontal gyrus, insula and superior parietal lobule. demonstrated that regions of posterior lateral temporal cortex respond to the movements of man-made manipulable objects (tools) moving with their characteristic natural motion. Tool movement responsive regions are localized inferior and posterior (within the posterior MTG and inferior temporal sulcus) to those that respond to biological motion (localized to the STS). It is possible that the motion of a hand alone (the primary effector involved in tool use) engages cortical regions that are also engaged by the movement of tools. While the theoretical impetus for our current methodology was to test an a priori hypothesis regarding the existence of a distribution along the STS region for the type of biological motion observed, the fact that we acquired whole-brain fmri data allowed us to examine activation in other brain regions to three kinds of biological motion. The activity in regions of frontal and parietal cortex evoked by observation of an actor s movements is consistent with prior work by Rizzolatti et al. (1996) concerning neurons (in monkeys) and brain regions (in humans) that respond when subjects both execute an action and when they view an agent performing the same action (Iacoboni et al., 1999; Buccino et al., 2001; Gre` zes and Decety, 2002). These mirror neurons have been identified in area F5 of the monkey (Rizzolatti et al., 1996) and mirror regions have been localized to premotor, parietal and posterior temporal areas in humans (e.g. Iacoboni et al., 1999, 2001; Gre` zes and Decety, 2002). In addition, previously unreported regions including portions of medial frontal cortex, the thalamus and colliculi were activated. It remains to be determined if these Cerebral Cortex December 2005, V 15 N

9 Pelphrey Figure 7. BOLD signal time courses. Average BOLD signal change time courses from the activated voxels from clusters that responded most strongly to (a) Face in medial frontal gyrus, (b) Mouth in fusiform gyrus, (c) Hand in lingual gyrus, and (d) Hand in middle occipital gyrus. Figure 8. Centers from the current study, prior functional neuroimaging studies of observed eye and mouth movements, and the means of the prior studies are shown projected (in the lateral--medial direction) onto the right lateral brain surface. Eye activations (red diamonds) were localized posterior to mouth activations (blue circles). This informal meta-analysis indicates an anterior--inferior to posterior--superior gradient for mouth and eye activations. Centers from the present study are indicated by open gold circles. Mean centers are indicated by gold diamonds. Centers of activation are taken from: Calvert et al., 1997; Puce et al., 1998; Wicker et al., 1998; Hoffman and Haxby, 2000; Dubeau et al., 2001; Pelphrey et al., 2004a; Calvert and Campbell, 2003; Santi et al., regions share other aspects of functioning with better-characterized mirror regions. Buccino et al. (2001) reported a somatotopic organization in premotor and parietal cortical regions consistent with the classical motor homunculus (Penfield and Rasmussen, 1950). Mouth was localized most inferior, followed by hand, and foot most superior (see their Fig. 4). Wheaton et al. (2004) reported a similar somatotopy in right ventral premotor cortex for viewing face, hand and leg motions. We did not observe a somatotopy in premotor cortex, although within the IFG and right insula there was a tendency for mouth activity to be located anterior to eye activity. Our use of eye movements is one factor that could account for the failure to replicate these prior studies. In more anterior brain regions, including premotor cortex, eye activity dominated the other two conditions (e.g. see Fig. 3). This robust activity might have obscured possible somatotopic patterns of activation in these frontal regions. The location of the MOG activation in the left hemisphere is consistent with previous reports of activation in the more general motion-sensitive region area MT or V5 (MT/V5; Zeki, 1991; Watson et al., 1993; McCarthy et al., 1995; Tootell et al., 1995). Furthermore, if this were MT/V5 we might expect to see response amplitudes covary with the size of the movement. This is consistent with the pattern of responses presented for this region in Figure 7d. This present study has some limitations. First, only one version of each movement was used in each condition (i.e. one eye movement, one hand movement and one mouth movement). Therefore, we cannot generalize our results to all eye, hand and mouth movements. The present data do not exclude the possibility that areas within the STS region respond to movements of a certain form, which are more often associated with, for example, eye movements, rather than responding to any eye movement per se (even eye movements not frequently encountered). Second, the movements we 1874 Spatial Differentiation of Biological Motion d et al.

10 employed here were not goal-directed. Previous studies have reported activation difference between object-directed and non-object-directed actions (Pelphrey et al., 2003, 2004a; for a review see Gre` zes and Decety, 2001). Object-directed actions generally appear more intentional than do non-object-directed actions, which can appear to be accidental or at least appear less intentional. Given the known role of the STS in the perceiving the intentions of others actions (for reviews see Allison et al., 2000; Blakemore and Decety, 2001) the distributions of activity might differ for object-directed compared to non-object-directed actions. Finally, we note that we used a virtual actor to create our stimuli. This has the advantage of affording precise control over the movements of the actor as well as some potentially confounding variables such as background color and lighting. However, some reports indicate that activation within the STS region may differ depending upon whether the actions of virtual or real-life actors are viewed (Perani et al., 2001). We conclude that within the right posterior lateral temporaloccipital region there is a topography for the type of biological motion observed. This topography follows an anterior-toposterior distribution such that eye activity is greatest in more superior and posterior portions of the STS region and mouth activity is greatest in more anterior regions of the STS, relative to eye activity. Hand activity is greatest in more inferior and posterior portions of the lateral temporal-occipital region. This anterior to posterior distribution probably reflects the differential involvement of the STS region in the perception of other s attentional focus (the eye region) and in social communication (the mouth region). The more inferior and posterior focus of the hand movement evoked activity focus could reflect the role of manual gestures in symbolic communication and/or the specialization of regions of lateral temporal cortex for the perception of tools. Notes We thank J. Voyvodic, B. Mack, R. Viola, J. Atkin and Dr A. Song for assistance with several aspects of this research. We thank Dr Gary Glover for providing source code for the spiral pulse sequence. This research was supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs and NIH grant MH K.A.P. was supported by NICHD 1-T32-HD G.M. is a VA Senior Research Career Scientist. These results were reported in preliminary form at the 10 th Annual Cognitive Neuroscience Society Meeting, New York. Address correspondence to Gregory McCarthy, Duke-UNC Brain Imaging and Analysis Center, 163 Bell Building, Box 3918, Durham, NC 27710, USA. gregory.mccarthy@duke.edu. References Allison T, McCarthy G, Luby M, Puce A, Spencer DD (1996) Localization of functional regions of human mesial cortex by somatosensory evoked potential recording and by cortical stimulation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 100: Allison T, Puce A, McCarthy G (2000) Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn Sci 4: Asafiev SV, Stanley CM, Shulman GL, Corbetta M (2004) Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions. Nat Neurosci 5: Beauchamp MS, Lee Ke, Haxby JV, Martin A (2003) FMRI responses to video and point-light displays of moving humans and manipulable objects. J Cogn Neurosci 15: Binder JR, Price C (2001) Functional neuroimaging of language. In: Handbook of functional neuroimaging of cognition (Cabeza R, Kingstone A, eds), pp Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Blakemore SJ, Decety, J (2001) From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: Bonda E, Petrides M, Ostry D, Evans A (1996) Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion. J Neurosci 16: Brunet E, Sarfati Y, Hardy-Bayle MC, Decety J (2000) A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. Neuroimage 11: Buccino G, Binkofski F, Fink GR, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Seitz RJ, Zilles K, Rizzolatti G, Freund HJ (2001) Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fmri study. Eur J Neurosci 13: Calvert GA, Campbell R (2003) Reading speech from still and moving faces: the neural substrates of visible speech. J Cogn Neurosci 15: Calvert GA, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Campbell R, Williams SC, McGuire PK, Woodruff PW, Iversen SD, David AS (1997) Activation of auditory cortex during silent lipreading. Science 276: Calvert GA, Hansen PC, Iversen SD, Brammer MJ (2001) Detection of audio-visual integration sites in humans by application of electrophysiological criteria to the BOLD effect. Neuroimage 14: Castelli F, Happe F, Frith U, Frith C (2000) Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perceptions and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage 12: Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Shulman GL, Petersen SE (1993) A PET study of visuospatial attention. J Neurosci 13: Culham JC, Brandt SA, Cavanagh P, Kanwisher NG, Dale AM, Tootell RB (1998) Cortical fmri activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. J Neurophysiol 80: Decety J, Greze s J (1999) Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions. Trends Cogn Sci 3: Downing PE, Jiang Y, Shuman M, Kanwisher N (2001) A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science 293: Dronkers NF, Wilkins, DP, Van Valin RD, Redfern BB, Jaeger JJ (2004) Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition 92: Dubeau MC, Iacoboni M, Koski LM, Markovac J, Mazziotta, JC (2001) Topography for body parts motion in the STS region. Soc Neurosci Abs 27. Fletcher PC, Happe F, Frith U, Baker SC, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD (1995) Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of theory of mind in story comprehension. Cognition 57: Forman SD, Cohen JD, Fitzgerald M, Eddy WF, Mintun MA, Noll DC (1995) Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn Res Med 33: Fried I, Katz A, McCarthy G, Sass KJ, Williamson P, Spencer SS, Spencer DD (1991) Functional organization of human supplementary motor cortex studied by electrical stimulation. J Neurosci 11: Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2003) Functional imaging of theory of mind. Trends Cogn Sci 7: Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G (1996) Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain. 119: Glover GH, Law CS (2001) Spiral-in/out BOLD fmri for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifacts. Magn Res Med 46: Grezes J, Costes N, Decety J (1999) The effects of learning and intention on the neural network involved in the perception of meaningless actions. Brain 122: Gre` zes J, Decety J (2001) Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a metaanalysis. Human Brain Mapp 12: Gre` zes J, Decety J (2002) Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia 40: Gre` zes J, Frith CD, Passingham RE (2004) Inferring false beliefs from the actions of oneself and others: an fmri study. Neuroimage 21: Grossman ED, Blake R (2002) Brain areas active during visual perception of biological motion. Neuron 35: Guo H, Song AW (2003) Single-shot spiral image acquisition with embedded z-shimming for susceptibility signal recovery. J Magn Reson Imaging 18: Cerebral Cortex December 2005, V 15 N

11 Pelphrey Hoffman EA, Haxby JV (2000) Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat Neurosci 3: Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M, Bekkering H, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (1999) Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286: Iacoboni M, Koski LM, Brass M, Bekkering H, Woods RP, Dubeau MC, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (2001) Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: Kastner S, Ungerleider LG (2000) Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23: Lazar NA, Luna B, Sweeney JA, Eddy WE (2001) Combining brains: a survey of methods for statistical pooling of information. Neuroimage 16: McCarthy G, Spicer M, Adrignolo A, Luby M, Gore J, Allison T (1995) Brain activation associated with visual motion studied by functional magnetic resonance imaging in humans. Hum Brain Mapp 2: Mesulam MM (1990). Large-scale neurocognitve networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Ann Neurol 28: Nobre AC, Sebestyen GN, Gitelman DR, Mesulam MM, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD (1997) Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. Brain 120: Pelphrey KA, Singerman JD, Allison T, McCarthy G (2003) Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: the influence of context. Neuropsychologia 41: Pelphrey KA, Morris JP, McCarthy G (2004a) Grasping the intentions of others: The perceived intention of an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception. J Cogn Neurosci 16: Pelphrey KA, Viola RJ, McCarthy G (2004b) When strangers pass: processing of mutual and averted gaze in the superior temporal sulcus. Psychol Sci 15: Penfield W, Rasmussen T (1950) The cerebral cortex of man. Macmillan: New York. Perani D, Fazio F, Borghese NA, Tettamanti M, Ferrari S, Decety J, Gilardi MC (2001) Different brain correlates for watching real and virtual hand actions. Neuroimage 14: Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 4: Price CJ (1998) The functional anatomy of word comprehension and production. Trends Cogn Sci 2: Puce A, Allison T, Bentin S, Gore JC, McCarthy G (1998) Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth movements. J Neurosci 18: Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 3: Santi A, Servos P, Vatikiotis-Bateson E, Kuratate T, Munhall K (2003) Perceiving biological motion: Dissociating visible speech from walking. J Cogn Neurosci 15: Saxe R, Kanwisher N (2003) People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in theory of mind. Neuro image 19: Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: Thieme. Tootell RB, Reppas JB, Dale AM, Look RB, Sereno MI, Brady TJ, Rosen BR (1995) Visual motion aftereffect in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature 375: Vogeley K, Bussfeld P, Newen A, Herrmann S, Happe F, Falkai P, Maier W, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Zilles K (2001) Mind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. Neuroimage 14: Voyvodic JT (1999) Real-time fmri integrating paradigm control, physiology, behavior, and on-line statistical analysis. Neuroimage 10: Watson JD, Myers R, Frackowiak RS, Hajnal JV, Woods RP, Mazziotta JC, Shipp S, Zeki S (1993) Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex 3: Wheaton KJ, Thompson JC, Syngeniotis A, Abbott DF, Puce A (2004) Viewing the motion of human body parts activates different regions of premotor, temporal, and parietal cortex. Neuroimage 22: Wicker B, Michel F, Henaff MA, Decety J (1998) Brain regions involved in the perception of gaze: a PET study. Neuroimage 8: Wright TM, Pelphrey KA, Allison T, McKeown MJ, McCarthy G (2003). Polysensory interactions along lateral temporal regions evoked by audiovisual speech. Cereb Cortex 13: Xiong J, Gao J, Lancaster JL, Fox PT (1995) Clustered pixels analysis for functional MRI activation studies of the human brain, Human Brain Mapp Zeki S (1991) A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. J Neurosci 11: Spatial Differentiation of Biological Motion d et al.

When Strangers Pass. Processing of Mutual and Averted Social Gaze in the Superior Temporal Sulcus. Research Article

When Strangers Pass. Processing of Mutual and Averted Social Gaze in the Superior Temporal Sulcus. Research Article PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article When Strangers Pass Processing of Mutual and Averted Social Gaze in the Superior Temporal Sulcus Kevin A. Pelphrey, 1,2 Ronald J. Viola, 1 and Gregory McCarthy 1,3

More information

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 SPECIAL ISSUE WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TE US ABOUT AND DIS? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1 By: Angelika Dimoka Fox School of Business Temple University 1801 Liacouras Walk Philadelphia, PA

More information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Supplementary Methods Materials & Methods Subjects Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the Dartmouth community. All subjects were native speakers of English,

More information

HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR

HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR Applicants Principal Investigator Student ID 4039921 Collaborators Name(s) Institution(s) Title of project: Neural basis of verbal and non-verbal false

More information

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri Human Brain Mapping 5:293 297(1997) Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri Vincent P. Clark, 1 * Raja Parasuraman, 2 Katrina Keil, 1 Rachel Kulansky, 1 Sean Fannon, 2 Jose Ma.

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) 743 748 WCPCG 2014 Differences in Visuospatial Cognition Performance and Regional Brain Activation

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Lingnau et al. 10.1073/pnas.0902262106 Fig. S1. Material presented during motor act observation (A) and execution (B). Each row shows one of the 8 different motor acts. Columns in

More information

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention Goldsmiths Research Online. The Authors. Originally published: Science vol.291 2 March 2001 1803-1806. http://www.sciencemag.org. 11 October 2000; accepted 17 January 2001 The Role of Working Memory in

More information

Mirror Neuron System Differentially Activated by Facial Expressions and Social Hand Gestures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study

Mirror Neuron System Differentially Activated by Facial Expressions and Social Hand Gestures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study Mirror Neuron System Differentially Activated by Facial Expressions and Social Hand Gestures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study Kimberly J. Montgomery and James V. Haxby Abstract & Facial expressions

More information

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces?

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces? Cognitive Neuroscience and Neurophysiology 10, 2945±2950 (1999) TO examine the speci city of face-responsive regions for face processing, we used fmri to measure the response of the fusiform gyrus and

More information

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Available online at www.sciencedirect.com R NeuroImage 18 (2003) 806 810 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Technical Note Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri Andrea Mechelli,

More information

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis (OA). All subjects provided informed consent to procedures

More information

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus FRONTAL LOBE Central Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Cingulate Lateral Lateral View Medial View Motor execution and higher cognitive functions (e.g., language production, impulse inhibition, reasoning

More information

The Change of Mu Rhythm during Action Observation in People with Stroke. Tae-won Yun, PT, MSc, Moon-Kyu Lee, PT, PhD

The Change of Mu Rhythm during Action Observation in People with Stroke. Tae-won Yun, PT, MSc, Moon-Kyu Lee, PT, PhD 1) 동작관찰시뇌졸중환자의뮤리듬변화 The Change of Mu Rhythm during Action Observation in People with Stroke Tae-won Yun PT MSc Moon-Kyu Lee PT PhD Department of Rehab Center Gwangju City Rehabilitation Hospital

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism

Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism doi:10.1093/brain/awh404 Brain (2005), 128, 1038 1048 Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism Kevin A. Pelphrey, 1,3 James P. Morris 1 and Gregory McCarthy 1,2 1 Duke-UNC Brain Imaging and

More information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information Supplementary Information The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice Joseph W. Kable and Paul W. Glimcher a 10 0 b 10 0 10 1 10 1 Discount rate k 10 2 Discount rate k 10 2 10

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/324/5927/646/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves Modulation of the vmpfc Valuation System Todd A. Hare,* Colin F. Camerer, Antonio

More information

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of Placebo effects in fmri Supporting online material 1 Supporting online material Materials and Methods Study 1 Procedure and behavioral data We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was

More information

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition Outline Functional MRI Mapping Cognition Michael A. Yassa, B.A. Division of Psychiatric Neuro-imaging Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Why fmri? fmri - How it works Research

More information

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Human Brain Mapping 6:373 377(1998) Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Randy L. Buckner 1,2,3 * 1 Departments of Psychology, Anatomy and Neurobiology, and Radiology, Washington University,

More information

The effects of single-trial averaging upon the spatial extent of fmri activation

The effects of single-trial averaging upon the spatial extent of fmri activation BRAIN IMAGING NEUROREPORT The effects of single-trial averaging upon the spatial extent of fmri activation Scott A. Huettel,CA and Gregory McCarthy, Brain Imaging and Analysis Center, Duke University Medical

More information

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Neuropsychologia 41 (2003) 341 356 Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory Joseph B. Sala a,, Pia Rämä a,c,d, Susan M.

More information

Mirror neurons. Romana Umrianova

Mirror neurons. Romana Umrianova Mirror neurons Romana Umrianova The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations Giacomo Rizzolatti and Corrado Sinigaglia Mechanism that unifies action

More information

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Author's response to reviews Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm Authors: Julia Miro (juliamirollado@gmail.com) Pablo Ripollès (pablo.ripolles.vidal@gmail.com)

More information

When Gaze Turns into Grasp

When Gaze Turns into Grasp When Gaze Turns into Grasp Andrea C. Pierno 1, Cristina Becchio 2, Matthew B. Wall 3, Andrew T. Smith 3, Luca Turella 1, and Umberto Castiello 1,3 Abstract & Previous research has provided evidence for

More information

The Critical Relationship between the Timing of Stimulus Presentation and Data Acquisition in Blocked Designs with fmri

The Critical Relationship between the Timing of Stimulus Presentation and Data Acquisition in Blocked Designs with fmri NeuroImage 10, 36 44 (1999) Article ID nimg.1999.0447, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on The Critical Relationship between the Timing of Stimulus Presentation and Data Acquisition in Blocked

More information

Chantal E. Stern,*, Adrian M. Owen, Irene Tracey,*, Rodney B. Look,* Bruce R. Rosen,* and Michael Petrides

Chantal E. Stern,*, Adrian M. Owen, Irene Tracey,*, Rodney B. Look,* Bruce R. Rosen,* and Michael Petrides NeuroImage 11, 392 399 (2000) doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0569, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Activity in Ventrolateral and Mid-Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex during Nonspatial Visual Working

More information

Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla

Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla Atsushi Miki*, Grant T. Liu*, Sarah A. Englander, Jonathan Raz, Theo G. M. van Erp,

More information

Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain

Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain Motivating Questions Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D. Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory COGS1 class How do our brains perceive the mental states of others

More information

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions NeuroImage 10, 642 657 (1999) Article ID nimg.1999.0500, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential

More information

Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex

Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 95, pp. 7721 7726, June 1998 Neurobiology Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex ADRIAN M. OWEN*,

More information

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri Magnetic Resonance Imaging 24 (2006) 181 185 Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri Birgit Abler a, 4, Alard Roebroeck b, Rainer Goebel b, Anett

More information

Modulation of Cortical Activity During Different Imitative Behaviors

Modulation of Cortical Activity During Different Imitative Behaviors J Neurophysiol 89: 460 471, 2003; 10.1152/jn.00248.2002. Modulation of Cortical Activity During Different Imitative Behaviors LISA KOSKI, 1,2 MARCO IACOBONI, 1,3,6 MARIE-CHARLOTTE DUBEAU, 1,3 ROGER P.

More information

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia Supplemental Information Table of Contents 2 Behavioral Data 2 Table S1. Participant demographics

More information

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism Justin H G Williams Morven McWhirr Gordon D Waiter Cambridge Sept 10 th 2010 Joint attention in autism Declarative and receptive aspects initiating

More information

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex in dual-task performance Fabienne Collette a,b, Laurence 01ivier b,c, Martial Van der Linden a,d, Steven Laureys b, Guy Delfiore b, André Luxen

More information

Neuroimaging methods vs. lesion studies FOCUSING ON LANGUAGE

Neuroimaging methods vs. lesion studies FOCUSING ON LANGUAGE Neuroimaging methods vs. lesion studies FOCUSING ON LANGUAGE Pioneers in lesion studies Their postmortem examination provided the basis for the linkage of the left hemisphere with language C. Wernicke

More information

Giacomo Rizzolatti - selected references

Giacomo Rizzolatti - selected references Giacomo Rizzolatti - selected references 1 Rizzolatti, G., Semi, A. A., & Fabbri-Destro, M. (2014). Linking psychoanalysis with neuroscience: the concept of ego. Neuropsychologia, 55, 143-148. Notes: Through

More information

Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network

Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network European Journal of Neuroscience European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 30, pp. 1383 1392, 2009 doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06941.x COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical

More information

performance of verbal working memory tasks

performance of verbal working memory tasks Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 90, pp. 878-882, February 1993 Neurobiology Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks MICHAEL PETRIDES, BESSIE

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

The BOLD fmri refractory effect is specific to stimulus attributes: evidence from a visual motion paradigm

The BOLD fmri refractory effect is specific to stimulus attributes: evidence from a visual motion paradigm Rapid Communication The BOLD fmri refractory effect is specific to stimulus attributes: evidence from a visual motion paradigm Scott A. Huettel, a,b,c,d, * Olufolajimi O. Obembe, a,e Allen W. Song, a,f,g

More information

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Online supplementary information for: Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning Joel L. Voss, Brian D. Gonsalves, Kara D. Federmeier,

More information

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Overview of the SIIPS1 development. The development of the SIIPS1 consisted of individual- and group-level analysis steps. 1) Individual-person

More information

Evidence for a Refractory Period in the Hemodynamic Response to Visual Stimuli as Measured by MRI

Evidence for a Refractory Period in the Hemodynamic Response to Visual Stimuli as Measured by MRI NeuroImage 11, 547 553 (2000) doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0553, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Evidence for a Refractory Period in the Hemodynamic Response to Visual Stimuli as Measured by

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16 SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16 women and 16 men. One female had to be excluded from brain data analyses because of strong movement

More information

Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe

Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe articles Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe V. Prabhakaran 1, K. Narayanan 2, Z. Zhao 2 and J. D. E. Gabrieli 1,2 1 Program in Neurosciences and 2 Dept. of Psychology,

More information

Activation of the Dorsal Premotor Cortex and Pre-Supplementary Motor Area of Humans During an Auditory Conditional Motor Task

Activation of the Dorsal Premotor Cortex and Pre-Supplementary Motor Area of Humans During an Auditory Conditional Motor Task RAPID COMMUNICATION Activation of the Dorsal Premotor Cortex and Pre-Supplementary Motor Area of Humans During an Auditory Conditional Motor Task KIYOSHI KURATA, 1 TOSHIAKI TSUJI, 3 SATOSHI NARAKI, 3 MORIO

More information

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information The Journal of Neuroscience, 2000, Vol. 20 RC108 1of5 Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information Charan Ranganath, 1 Marcia K. Johnson, 2 and Mark

More information

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits Brain electrical activity methods description and explanation of method state effects trait effects Positron emission tomography

More information

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach Attentional Control 1 Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach Barry Giesbrecht & George R. Mangun Center for Mind & Brain University of California, Davis

More information

Su Mei Lee, Tao Gao, and Gregory McCarthy. Department of Psychology. Yale University, New Haven, CT

Su Mei Lee, Tao Gao, and Gregory McCarthy. Department of Psychology. Yale University, New Haven, CT Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Advance Access published September 14, 2012 Intention attribution in the psts Running head: INTENTION ATTRIBUTION IN THE PSTS Attributing intentions to random

More information

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Human Visual Cortex during Face Matching: A Comparison with Positron Emission Tomography

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Human Visual Cortex during Face Matching: A Comparison with Positron Emission Tomography NEUROIMAGE 4, 1 15 (1996) ARTICLE NO. 0025 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Human Visual Cortex during Face Matching: A Comparison with Positron Emission Tomography V. P. CLARK, K. KEIL, J. MA.

More information

The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action

The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 260 267 Review The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action Sarah-Jayne Blakemore a,, Chris Frith b,1 a Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 Queen Square, London

More information

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2 Hillyard(1973) Dichotic Listening Task N1 component enhanced for attended stimuli Supports early selection Effects of Voluntary

More information

Dr. Mark Ashton Smith, Department of Psychology, Bilkent University

Dr. Mark Ashton Smith, Department of Psychology, Bilkent University UMAN CONSCIOUSNESS some leads based on findings in neuropsychology Dr. Mark Ashton Smith, Department of Psychology, Bilkent University nattentional Blindness Simons and Levin, 1998 Not Detected Detected

More information

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex PARIETAL LOBE The Parietal Lobes develop at about the age of 5 years. They function to give the individual perspective and to help them understand space, touch, and volume. The location of the parietal

More information

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso Control of visuo-spatial attention Emiliano Macaluso CB demo Attention Limited processing resources Overwhelming sensory input cannot be fully processed => SELECTIVE PROCESSING Selection via spatial orienting

More information

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition Gloria Wong 1,2, Sanda Dolcos 1,3, Ekaterina Denkova 1, Rajendra A. Morey 4,5, Lihong Wang 4,5, Nicholas Coupland 1, Gregory

More information

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Neuron, Volume 70 Supplemental Information Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion Luke J. Chang, Alec Smith, Martin Dufwenberg, and Alan G. Sanfey Supplemental Information

More information

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Supplementary Information Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex David V. Smith 1-3, Benjamin Y. Hayden 1,4, Trong-Kha Truong 2,5, Allen W. Song 2,5, Michael L.

More information

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018)

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) 1 / 22 Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018) Jérôme Dockès, ussel Poldrack, Demian Wassermann, Fabian Suchanek, Bertrand

More information

The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fmri Study

The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fmri Study NeuroImage 16, 449 453 (2002) doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1074, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fmri Study S. Durston,*, K. M.

More information

Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements

Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements The Journal of Neuroscience, March 15, 1998, 18(6):2188 2199 Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements Aina Puce, 1,2 Truett Allison, 1,3 Shlomo Bentin, 5 John C. Gore, 4 and

More information

Perceptual Gain and Perceptual Loss: Distinct Neural Mechanisms of Audiovisual Interactions*

Perceptual Gain and Perceptual Loss: Distinct Neural Mechanisms of Audiovisual Interactions* ISSN 1749-8023 (print), 1749-8031 (online) International Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging Vol. 01, No. 01, 2007, pp. 003-014 Perceptual Gain and Perceptual Loss: Distinct Neural Mechanisms of Audiovisual

More information

RAPID COMMUNICATION A PET Exploration of the Neural Mechanisms Involved in Reciprocal Imitation

RAPID COMMUNICATION A PET Exploration of the Neural Mechanisms Involved in Reciprocal Imitation NeuroImage 15, 265 272 (2002) doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0938, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on RAPID COMMUNICATION A PET Exploration of the Neural Mechanisms Involved in Reciprocal Imitation

More information

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Brain and Cognition 54 (2004) 235 239 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri Kim Cornish, a,b, * Rachel

More information

Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing

Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 90, pp. 873-877, February 1993 Neurobiology Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing (working memory/conditional

More information

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Subjects The experiment included twelve subjects: ten sighted subjects and two blind. Five of the ten sighted subjects were expert users of a visual-to-auditory

More information

Face-specific resting functional connectivity between the fusiform gyrus and posterior superior temporal sulcus

Face-specific resting functional connectivity between the fusiform gyrus and posterior superior temporal sulcus HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE Original Research Article published: 24 September 2010 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00176 Face-specific resting functional connectivity between the fusiform gyrus and posterior superior temporal

More information

Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention

Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention NeuroImage 19 (2003) 496 512 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention B. Giesbrecht, a, * M.G. Woldorff, a,b A.W. Song, a,c and G.R. Mangun

More information

Stuttering Research. Vincent Gracco, PhD Haskins Laboratories

Stuttering Research. Vincent Gracco, PhD Haskins Laboratories Stuttering Research Vincent Gracco, PhD Haskins Laboratories Stuttering Developmental disorder occurs in 5% of children Spontaneous remission in approximately 70% of cases Approximately 1% of adults with

More information

Detection of Functional Connectivity Using Temporal Correlations in MR Images

Detection of Functional Connectivity Using Temporal Correlations in MR Images Human Brain Mapping 15:247 262(2002) DOI 10.1002/hbm.10022 Detection of Functional Connectivity Using Temporal Correlations in MR Images Michelle Hampson, 1,2 * Bradley S. Peterson, 2 Pawel Skudlarski,

More information

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight? How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? What happens to visual-devoted brain structure in individuals who are born deprived of sight?

More information

The frontal cortex comprises a third of

The frontal cortex comprises a third of REVIEW: NEUROSCIENCE REVIEW Storage and Executive Processes in the Frontal Lobes Edward E. Smith 1,2 * and John Jonides 1 The human frontal cortex helps mediate working memory, a system that is used for

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Impact of Early Deafness and Early Exposure to Sign Language on the Cerebral Organization for Motion Processing

Impact of Early Deafness and Early Exposure to Sign Language on the Cerebral Organization for Motion Processing The Journal of Neuroscience, November 15, 2001, 21(22):8931 8942 Impact of Early Deafness and Early Exposure to Sign Language on the Cerebral Organization for Motion Processing Daphne Bavelier, 1 Craig

More information

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis International Journal of Innovative Research in Computer Science & Technology (IJIRCST) ISSN: 2347-5552, Volume-2, Issue-6, November-2014 Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using

More information

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning Resistance to Forgetting 1 Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning Brice A. Kuhl, Arpeet T. Shah, Sarah DuBrow, & Anthony D. Wagner Resistance to

More information

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1 The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1 Psycholinguistics / Neurolinguistics Happy Hour!!! Tuesdays 7/11, 7/18, 7/25 5:30-6:30 PM @ the Boone Center

More information

Peripheral facial paralysis (right side). The patient is asked to close her eyes and to retract their mouth (From Heimer) Hemiplegia of the left side. Note the characteristic position of the arm with

More information

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Supplemental informations Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease Stéphanie Lefebvre, PhD^1,2, Guillaume Baille, MD^4, Renaud Jardri MD, PhD 1,2 Lucie Plomhause, PhD

More information

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner Cerebral Cortex 1 Sarah Heilbronner heilb028@umn.edu Want to meet? Coffee hour 10-11am Tuesday 11/27 Surdyk s Overview and organization of the cerebral cortex What is the cerebral cortex? Where is each

More information

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications. Dr. Peter J. Fiester November 14, 2012 Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications. Briefly discuss a few examples

More information

Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision touch synaesthesia

Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision touch synaesthesia doi:10.1093/brain/awh500 Brain (2005), 128, 1571 1583 Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision touch synaesthesia S.-J. Blakemore, 1 D. Bristow, 2 G. Bird, 1,3 C.

More information

Title motion: an event-related FMRI study. Author(s) Osaka, Naoyuki; Ikeda, Takashi; Osa.

Title motion: an event-related FMRI study. Author(s) Osaka, Naoyuki; Ikeda, Takashi; Osa. Title Effect of intentional bias on agenc motion: an event-related FMRI study Author(s) Osaka, Naoyuki; Ikeda, Takashi; Osa Citation PloS one (2012), 7(11) Issue Date 2012-11-14 URL http://hdl.handle.net/2433/163076

More information

FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE EVIDENCE OF CORTICAL ALTERATIONS IN A CASE OF REVERSIBLE CONGENITAL LYMPHEDEMA OF THE LOWER LIMB: A PILOT STUDY

FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE EVIDENCE OF CORTICAL ALTERATIONS IN A CASE OF REVERSIBLE CONGENITAL LYMPHEDEMA OF THE LOWER LIMB: A PILOT STUDY 19 Lymphology 40 (2007) 19-25 FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE EVIDENCE OF CORTICAL ALTERATIONS IN A CASE OF REVERSIBLE CONGENITAL LYMPHEDEMA OF THE LOWER LIMB: A PILOT STUDY M. Pardini, L. Bonzano, L. Roccatagliata,

More information

Chapter 10 Importance of Visual Cues in Hearing Restoration by Auditory Prosthesis

Chapter 10 Importance of Visual Cues in Hearing Restoration by Auditory Prosthesis Chapter 1 Importance of Visual Cues in Hearing Restoration by uditory Prosthesis Tetsuaki Kawase, Yoko Hori, Takenori Ogawa, Shuichi Sakamoto, Yôiti Suzuki, and Yukio Katori bstract uditory prostheses,

More information

The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy

The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Neuroethics Publications Center for Neuroscience & Society 1-1-2009 The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy Andrea L. Glenn University

More information

Visual areas involved in the perception of human movement from dynamic form analysis

Visual areas involved in the perception of human movement from dynamic form analysis BRAIN IMAGING Visual areas involved in the perception of human movement from dynamic form analysis Lars Michels, 1,CA Markus Lappe 1 and Lucia Maria Vaina 2,3 1 Psychologisches Institut II,WestfÌlische

More information

Mental rotation of anthropoid hands: a chronometric study

Mental rotation of anthropoid hands: a chronometric study Mental Brazilian rotation Journal of of anthropoid Medical and hands Biological Research (2007) 40: 377-381 ISSN 0100-879X Short Communication 377 Mental rotation of anthropoid hands: a chronometric study

More information

Neuroimaging. BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT. Human Brain Mapping

Neuroimaging. BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT. Human Brain Mapping 11/8/2013 Neuroimaging N i i BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT 2 Human Brain Mapping H Human m n brain br in m mapping ppin can nb

More information

Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks

Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks Jinglei Lv 1,2, Dajiang Zhu 2, Xintao Hu 1, Xin Zhang 1,2, Tuo Zhang 1,2, Junwei Han 1, Lei Guo 1,2, and Tianming Liu 2 1 School

More information

Supplemental Information. Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain. Disrupts Melody Processing

Supplemental Information. Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain. Disrupts Melody Processing Current Biology, Volume 27 Supplemental Information Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain Disrupts Melody Processing Frank E. Garcea, Benjamin L. Chernoff, Bram Diamond, Wesley Lewis, Maxwell

More information

Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus

Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus The Journal of Neuroscience, November 23, 2005 25(47):11055 11059 11055 Brief Communication Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus Rebecca F. Schwarzlose, 1,2 Chris I. Baker, 1,2 and

More information

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging

Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 1428 1442 Neural correlates of memory for object identity and object location: effects of aging Alessandra Schiavetto a, Stefan Köhler a, Cheryl L. Grady a, Gordon Winocur a,c,

More information

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material 1 Supplemental Material Golomb, J.D, and Kanwisher, N. (2012). Higher-level visual cortex represents retinotopic, not spatiotopic, object location. Cerebral Cortex. Contents: - Supplemental Figures S1-S3

More information

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview Investigations in Resting State Connectivity Scott FMRI Laboratory Overview Introduction Functional connectivity explorations Dynamic change (motor fatigue) Neurological change (Asperger s Disorder, depression)

More information