Size invariance in curve tracing

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Size invariance in curve tracing"

Transcription

1 Memory & Cognition Size invariance in curve tracing PIERRE JOLICOEUR and MARGARET INGLETON University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada Subjects decided hether to dots ere on the same curve or on different curves in patterns consisting of to curves and to dots in displays that had an exposure duration of 200 msec or that remained in vie until the subjects' response. The overall size of the patterns as varied by a factor of to. Furthermore, across experiments, e manipulated the predictability of the size of the pattern on a particular trial. On halfof the trials, the to dots ere on the same curve; across these trials, the distance beteen the dots, along the curve, as manipulated systematically hile the Euclidean distance beteen the dots as held constant. On the other half of the trials, the to dots ere on different curves. The time to respond same increased monotonically as curve distance beteen the dots increased, suggesting that subjects mentally traced the curve in order to perform the task. The absolute size of the pattern had little or no effect on the response times, indicating that it as curve distance relative to the overall pattern size, rather than absolute distance, that controlled response times. Furthermore, expectancies about pattern size had essentially no effect on performance. Taken together, the results suggest that the rate oftracing is determined by various stimulus properties that covary ith the overall size ofthe pattern on hich tracing takes place, such as the distance beteen the traced curve and nearby distractor curves, or the curvature of the traced curve. Contours, outlines, and boundaries playa fundamental role in human vision (e.g., Biederman, 1985, 1988; Cavanagh, 1987; Hubel & Wiesel, 1968; Treisman & Gelade, 1980; Ullman, 1984). In this paper, e investigate a possible process, curve tracing, that could be involved in the processing of information about contours and curves in visual displays. Curve tracing is thought to be a rapid visual process that can track along a contour internally (ithout eye movements). The empirical signature of curve tracing is that the time to perform the task should increase as the distance along the curve separating the target information increases, assuming that other relevant potential contributors to response time have been controlled (Jolicoeur, 1988; Jolicoeur, Ullman, & Mackay, 1986, 1990; McCormick & Jolicoeur, 1990a; Pringle & Egeth, 1988). The Experimental Task In this section, e revie briefly the tracing paradigm developed by Jolicoeur et al. (1986). Subjects decided hether to Xs ere on the same curve or on to different curves as quickly as possible hile keeping errors to a minimum. Each display consisted of to nonintersecting curves and to Xs. The displays in the Jolicoeuret ai. (1986) paper ere similar to those shon in Figure 1, We thank James Chumbley, Alinda Friedman, and to anonymous revieers for comments on an earlier version of this article. We also thank Keith McGoan and Richard Crispin for technical assistance. This research as supported by Grant 00P from the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council ofcanada aarded to Pierre Jolicoeur. Correspondence may be sent to Pierre Jolicoeur, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3Gl, Canada. except that Xs ere used instead of dots (dots ere used in the present study). Across trials, the to Xs could either be on the same curve or on different curves. One of the Xs, the central X, alays appeared on the central curve at the location formerly occupied by the fixation point. A second, noncentral, X could appear at one of four distances along the same curve or at one of four locations on the other curve. In same-eurve trials, distance as measured as the distance required to travel along the curve from the central X to the noncentral X and as expressed in degrees ofvisual angle. The Euclidean distance beteen the to Xs as held constant at of visual angle by placing the noncentral X at locations here the curves intersected an imaginary circle concentric on the fixation point. The curves displayed in Figure 1 sho this distance as on the small pattern and on the large pattern, hich ere the distances used in the present study. After either restricted (250 msec) or unlimited (2,500 msec) stimulus exposure, the time to respond that the to Xs ere on the same curve increased monotonically as the distance separating the Xs along the curve increased. The time to respond that the to Xs ere on different curves as generally sloer than as the time to respond that the to Xs ere on the same curve. Curve distance is not defined for different trials. Furthermore, the apparent rate of curve tracing as not affected by exposure-duration conditions, hich suggests that eye movements ere not necessary to perform the task and ere not the cause of the increased response time ith increasing curve distance. This, in turn, suggests that curve tracing is an "internal" perceptual mechanism that can operate quickly on the information gleaned from a single glance at a visual display. 21 Copyright 1991 Psychonomic Society, Inc.

2 22 JOLICOEUR AND INGLETON Figure 1. Example stimuli ith (top figure) and ithout (bottom figure) construction constraints shon. The top pattern shos the four possible locations ofthe noncentral dot that yield a S/J1IIe trial. The distance beteen the dots, along the curve, as 4 in large patterns and 2 in small patterns. The dotted circle, not present in stimuli shon to subjects, as used to construct displays in hich the Euclidean distance beteen the clot at fixation and the noncentral dot as constant for all displays and all curvedistances (3.0 in large patterns and 1.5 in small ones). The bottom (i.e., small) pattern shos one of the four possible locations of the dot in a different trial. All dimensions on small patterns ere exactly half those shon for large patterns. Curve Tracing and Size Invariance The main focus in the present paper concerns the scale at hich tracing takes place. By scale e mean the spatial size or resolution ofthe operator(s) that support curve tracing. One interpretation of the results of previous studies of curve tracing is that the to-be-traced curve is trackedby a local operator and that the rate athich this operator can follo the curve is limited. It follos from these assumptions that tracing a longer piece of curve ould, all else being equal, take longer than tracing a shorter piece ofcurve. The main task ofthe local operator is to ensure that the tracing process remains "on curve," so that the process ill not accidently begin to track the rong curve and produce the rong anser. One ay to achieve this goal is to require that only a single curve be contained ithin the processing scope of the operator (see Jolicoeur et al., 1990; Mahoney & Ullman, 1988). Jolicoeur et al. (1990) hypothesized that the rate oftracing should be proportional to the size of the local tracing operator. This constraint falls out of the folloing considerations: Suppose that a local operator shifts along the curve in discrete steps. The maximum step size that the operator can take ould be limited by the size of the operator, otherise it could jump to the rong curve. We assume that the general relationship beteen operator size and tracing rate ould apply even ifthe operator tracked a curve in a smooth and continuous fashion, but this is clearly an assumption. These suggestions concerning the nature of the tracing mechanisms bear some similarity to the notion of a "beam" of attention, hich has been proposed or assumed by a number of researchers (e.g., Eriksen & Hoffman, 1972; Posner, 1980; Remington & Pierce, 1984; Shulman, Remington, & Mclean, 1979; Tsal, 1983; but see Eriksen & Murphy, 1987; Yantis, 1988). For example, Humphreys (1981) suggested that the idth of the "attentional focus," or in his ords, the "span of visual attention," can vary under some conditions (see also LaBerge, 1983). In our case, under the appropriate stimulus conditions, such as spatial isolation of the target curve (Jolicoeur et al., 1990), a ider beam or operator could be used, hich could result in faster tracing. Jolicoeur et al. (1990) found some evidence consistent ith the hypothesis that tracing is supported by a local operator ith variable size and that tracing speed varies ith the size of the operator. They asked subjects to decide hether to dots ere on the same straight line or on to different lines in displays that ere covered by a large number ofequally spaced parallel lines. Response time increased as the distance beteen the dots increased; this effect as magnified as the separation beteen the lines as decreased. That is, the effects ofcurve distance became larger hen distractor curves ere brought into greater proximity to a target curve. This pattern ofresults ould be expected ifthe proximal distractor curves caused a local operatorto become smaller (possibly so as to contain only a single curve) and if the rate of tracing as sloer hen the operator had a smaller scope. The suggestion is that, in general, the rate of tracing is not constant nor is it fixed entirely by lo-level properties of the visual system. Rather, the rate of tracing ould appear to depend on a number of properties of the curve to be traced and ofthe visual context in hich that curve is embedded (see also McCormick & Jolicoeur, 199Ob). The present experiments ere designed to provide converging evidence consistent ith the model of tracing discussed above and proposed by Jolicoeur et al. (1990). We varied the overall size ofthe visual displays, hich ere similar to those used by Jolicoeur et al. (1986). Increasing the size of the stimulus, hile keeping the thickness of lines and the size of targets the same, systematically

3 CURVE TRACING 23 increases the absolute length of the curve to be traced in order to travel from one target to the other (e.g., from one X to the other in the Jolicoeur et al., 1986, experiments). Note, hoever, that although the absolute distance beteen targets varies, the distance, relative to the overall pattern size, does not. Suppose that the mechanisms that support our ability to mentally trace curves does not have the ability to adjust the size or scope ofthe processor that tracks the curve. In this case, e ould expect that the scope of the operator ould be relatively small, hich ould enable the system to trace curves even in a cluttered display (ith many nearby distractor curves). Ifso, e ould expect that tracing ould proceed at a fixed rate by shifting a finely focused beam along the length of curve beteen targets. Increasing the size of the pattern, and thus the absolute distance to be traced, ould increase the amount of time required in a directly proportional ay. On the other hand, if the size of the operator can be varied, and if the rate of tracing is variable, then absolute distance could increase ithout necessarily being accompanied by an increase in tracing time. At least to properties ofthe displays used in the present experiments that covary ith display size could be responsible for such a change in tracing rate (measured in absolute visual angle): (1) the distance beteen the curve to be traced and other elements in the display, and (2) the curvature ofcontinuous curves in the display. There is some evidence that both ofthese properties may have an effect on tracing rate. Jolicoeur et al. (1990) report results suggesting that the rate of tracing is sloer for trajectories ith higher curvature (see also Pringle & Egeth, 1988). The increased distance beteen the target curve and distractors could also allo the tracing operator postulated by Jolicoeur et al. (1990) to enlarge its processing scope, hich ould be accompanied by a corresponding increase in tracing speed. Thus, both of these factors could allo tracing to proceed at an increased rate, hich could compensate for the greater distance that needs to be traced on a larger pattern. Of course, it remains to be seen hether one ould obtain any compensation, partial compensation, total compensation, or even overcompensation in tracing rate for a given change in pattern size. Perhaps the most interesting result ould be essentially perfect compensation, hich ould beadvantageous from the point ofvie of maintaining a certain degree of''processing constancy" as pattern size varies (ith vieing distance, for example). The Experiments To discover hether or not the visual system can adjust the rate of tracing for different pattern sizes, the present studies used a modified version of the "to-xs-on-acurve" paradigm investigated by Jolicoeur et al. (1986). The most important manipulation as that the size of the curve displays as varied over a 2: 1 ratio. Thus, in terms of absolute length, one unit of curve distance on a large pattern (i.e., the distance beteen to target symbols) as exactly tice as long as one unit on a small pattern. Although the displays varied in overall size, the thickness ofthe curves and the size ofthe target symbols ere constant across displays ith different sizes and, thus, could not produce differences across size conditions (e.g., by making the symbols less visible and more difficult to find in smaller displays). The symbols in the present experiments ere yello dots rather than Xs. There as one difference beteen the displays that e did not control: the eccentricity of the curves and dots, hich as greater for the larger patterns. As it turns out, hoever, the results sho that this difference as not important, unless greater eccentricity made the curves and dots easier to process, hich is the opposite ofhat e ould expect. In the folloing paragraphs, e consider a number of potential outcomes from the experiments that ould bear directly on the conceptions of tracing outlined above. Suppose that tracing mechanisms operate at a fixed rate. If so, absolute distance in visual angle should determine tracing time. The time taken to trace one unit on a large pattern, therefore, should equal the time taken to trace to units of a small pattern. Plotting the expected same response times for large versus small patterns against the number of units traced ould produce an interaction beteen distance and pattern size in hich the slope of the effect for large patterns is tice that for small patterns. This predicted pattern of results is shon in Figure 2, Panel A. The 2: 1 slope difference in functions ould reflect an equivalent rate of tracing for the to pattern sizes and, thus, complete size dependence. Alternatively, in a more flexible system in hich tracing rate is adjustable, there is no reason to predict a 2: 1 difference in time to trace one 'unit' on a large pattern relative to a small one. Distances that are tice as long need not take tice as long to trace. One possibility is that the 2: 1 size difference is compensated perfectly (e.g., by adjusting the size of the tracing operator), effectively adjusting the speed of shifting the beam. If so, it should take as long to trace one unit of a large pattern as it does one unit of a small pattern. Graphically, this particular hypothesis ould predict overlapping and identical functions for the large and small patterns hen response times are plotted against relative distance. This predicted pattern of results is shon in Figure 2, Panel B. Note here that the equivalence of functions ould reflect a 2: 1 difference in absolute tracing rates (i.e., in terms of visual angle traversed) across the to pattern sizes and, thus, complete size invariance in terms of observed response times. There are other possibilities in addition to the above to. It is possible also that e may find incomplete compensation or perhaps even overcompensation for changes in pattern size. Also, e could find main-effect differences across conditions; these differences could reflect adjustments made in response to the overall pattern size

4 24 JOLICOEUR AND INGLETON PREDICTIONS A: Size Dependence ::!E F (f) z o Q. (f) W a: > a: LARGE small B: Size Invariance ::!E F (f) Z o Q. (f) W a: > a: LARGE small RELATIVE DISTANCE (UNITS) - SAME small: LARGE ABSOLUTE DISTANCE 8 16 (DEGREES) - SAME Figure 2. Predicted results for the present studies for the to classes ofmodels. Panel A depicts a difference in the functions relating response times to distance, reflecting equivalent tracing rates, as predicted from models incorporating size dept:ndence. Panel B depicts equivalent functions, reflecting rate differences that are proportional to size differences, as predicted by models incorporating size invariance. (e.g., the time to adjust the scope ofa local operatorcould be different for the to pattern sizes). EXPERIMENT 1 In Experiment I, the size of stimulus patterns as changed from block to block. Presenting the trials blocked by size should give subjects the opportunity to display complete size invariance, if this is possible, even if it takes some time to make "global" adjustments to various possible parameters of mechanisms used during tracing. There ere also to exposure-duration conditions: limited (200 msec) and unlimited (until the subject's response).1 The limited condition as included to limit the potential usefulness ofeye movements. Evidence for tracing under these conditions suggests that the processes that support tracing are rapid internal perceptual mechanisms. This desirable outcome as compromised by the fact that briefexposures tend to be associated ith higher error rates, hich can complicate the interpretation of response times. Thus, e also included the unlimited condition, for hich e expected loer error rates. Method Subjects. The subjects ere 32 University of Waterloo undergraduates beteen the ages of 18 and 44 years. They ere paid to participate in this study. Halfof the subjects ere in the limitedexposure group and half ere in the unlimited-exposure group. There ere 8 males and 8 females in each group. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None had participated previously in a curve-tracing equipment. Stimuli. A stimulus consisted of a pattern of to nonintersecting hite curves and to yello dots that could lie either on the

5 CURVE TRACING 25 same curve or on different curves. An example of a pattern is depicted in Figure I. One of the dots in every display, the central dot, appeared at the center of the display at the location previously occupied by the fixation point. This location in the display as also the center of an imaginary circle used to constrain the curves. The circle, represented by a dotted line in Figure I, as never present in an actual display. The central curve passed through the central dot and then meandered through the display, intersecting the circle in at least four locations. The other, different, curve also intersected the circle at four locations. The four points of intersection of each of the to curves ith the circle ere the eight possible locations of the second, noncentral, dot. Therefore, in each size condition, the retinal eccentricity of the noncentral dot and the Euclidean distance beteen the to dots as constant across all displays. In same trials, curve distance as defined as the distance traveled along the curve separating the central dot and the noncentral dot. This distance can be expressed in absolute terms, using visual angie as the measure, or in relative terms, using the distance beteen the central dot and the first noncentral dot location as the basic unit of distance. The results shon in Figures 4-8 are plotted against relative distance, ith the absolute distance corresponding ith small and large patterns also listed belo the abscissa. The curves ere constructed such that the distance along the curve separating consecutive dot locations as a constant and as equal to one unit of relative curve distance. There is no clear definition of curve distance in the case of different trials. Nine patterns, eight experimental and one practice, differing only in the shapes of the curves, ere dran on an Amiga 1000 computer in high-resolution interlaced mode, using antialiased lines (Field, 1984; Tanner, Jolicoeur, Coan, Booth, & Fishman, 1988) that produced smooth curves that ere not perceivably jaggy. These curves had previously been composed by hand so as to satisfy all the design criteria and ere then encoded on the computer by copying the curves onto a transparency that as taped to the screen and using a dra program to dra the curves ith a series of straight line segments. The program stored only the coordinates ofthe end points of these line segments. The nine pairs of curves are displayed in Figure 3, ith a central dot and the eight possible noncentral dots. Each of the nine basic patterns could be presented at one of to sizes (see Figure I). The large size as double the small size. For patterns displayed at the small size, the average size as 65 mm x 65 mm. The vieing distance as 930 mm, and, thus, the small patterns subtended visual angles of 4 and the large patterns subtended 8. One unit of curve distance as equivalent to 2 on a small pattern and 4 on a large pattern. Thus, the four possible curve distances ere equivalent to 2, 4,6, and 8 for small patterns, and 4, go, 12, and 16 for large patterns. The retinal eccentricity of the noncentral dot from the central dot as 1.5 for small patterns and 3 for large patterns. Dots ere 0.3 in diameter and curves ere 0.06 thick for both pattern sizes. Finally, each pattern as presented in to orientations, upright or inverted (rotated by 180 ), hich as done to reduce the chance that subjects ould recognize particular patterns and dot configurations and use their memory to retrieve a response rather than relying on perception. Presenting the patterns upside don also ensured that the direction of tracing into the left or right visual field ould be equated across all conditions. Procedure. The SUbjects ere seated in a ell-lit room, facing the monitor, using a chin rest to maintain a constant vieing distance. The task as to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible, hile keeping errors to a minimum, by pressing a button ith the index finger of one hand ifthe to dots ere on the same curve or by pressing another button ith the other hand if the to dots ere on different curves. Hand of response as counterbalanced over subjects; half of the subjects of each gender in each group responded same ith the dominant hand and different ith the nondominant hand, hich as reversed for the other half of the subjects.»2 (1) (21 (3) (4) (5) (9) (7) (8) (9) Figure 3. The nine pairs of curves used in the experiments (the bottom right pair as used in practice trials only). In this figure, all eight noncentral dots are shon for each pair of curves; there ere only to dots in the displays shon to subjects. A trial began ith the presentation of a central, yello, fixation dot. After 750 msec, a stimulus pattern as presented such that the fixation dot no served as the central dot of the display. In the limited-exposure condition, the target stimulus as exposed for 200 msec and its offset as folloed by a blank screen until a response as made. In the unlimited-exposure condition, the target stimulus remained in vie until a response as made. A millisecond clock as started at the onset of the target stimulus and as stopped hen a response as registered. The screen remained blank for 3 sec folloing the response, hile the next stimulus as accessed and readied for presentation. After instructions ere given, the subject initiated practice trials and experimental trials by pressing a key on the computer keyboard hen he/she as ready to start a block of trials. Within blocks, trials proceeded at a fixed response-trial interval of 3 sec. The subjects' responses, same or different, and response times to the nearest millisecond ere recorded for each experimental trial. Each subject responded to to full sets of combinations of the eight patterns, the to orientations, the to sizes, the four dot 10 eations, and the to responses, for a total of512 experimental trials, The 512 trials ere presented to the subjects in four blocks of 128, ith a brief rest period beteen blocks. Large and small patterns ere presented in separate blocks. The subjects ere presented to blocks ofsmall patterns and to blocks of large patterns in an ABBA counterbalanced design, ith half of the subjects receiving blocks ordered small-iarge-iarge-small (SLLS) and the other half of the subjects receiving blocks ordered large-small-small-iarge (LSSL). The subjects ere informed that size ould change beteen blocks but that, ithin any given block, size ould remain constant. A block consisted of eight subblocks of 16 trials, in hich each pattern occurred once at each orientation. On halfof these 16 trials, the noncentral dot as displayed on the central curve (same trials), tice at each of the four possible curve distances; on the remaining half, it as displayed on the noncentral curve (different trials),

6 26 JOLICOEUR AND INGLETON tice at each of the four possible locations on the noncentral curve. Within and beteen subblocks, trials ere randomly presented, ith the constraints that a pattern at either orientation ould not be repeated on successive trials and that the same response (either same or different) ould not be repeated on more than four successive trials. A ne randomization oftrials fulfilling all ofthe above constraints as generated for each subject. Each experimental block as preceded by at least one block of 16 practice trials in hich the pattern size as the same as in the upcoming experimental trials. To practice blocks, one for each size, ere created and used for every subject. These trials consisted of a randomly ordered sequence of the practice pattern at the to orientations, ith the four dot locations and the to responses, the latter being constrained to appear in no more than four consecutive trials. An accuracy rate of 94% (15 correct out of 16) in the practice trials as required in order to proceed to the experimental trials. If, after 16 practice trials, this accuracy criterion as not reached, another block of 16 practice trials as presented automatically. This procedure could be repeated four times. All subjects reached 94% accuracy on or before the fourth block of practice trials. The subjects ere given no performance feedback during the experimental trials. Each subject as self-paced, ith respect to length of rest periods, but as encouraged to take at least a short break beteen each block of 128 trials. On average, the subjects took about 1 h to complete the experimental session. Results Response times. Response times in trials ith a correct response ere sorted into 32 cells for each subject. The cells corresponded ith the factorial combination of responses (same/different) x block (to at each size) x size (small/large) x distance (4; this as a dummy variable for different trials). Thus, the data ere pooled across orientations and the eight sets of curves for the analyses described here. The data in a cell ere screened for outliers using the folloing algorithm. The mean response time and the standard deviation ere computed ith the smallest and largest observation in that cell temporarily excluded. Then, the smallest and largest observations ere compared against the temporary mean plus or minus four times the standard deviation (M±4SD). If either the smallest or the largest response time (or both) ere outside ofthe limits, it as declared an outlier and excluded from further analysis, and the algorithm as applied to the remaining numbers recursively, until all the remaining values ere ithin limits. At that point, the final mean as computed, including the smallest and largest numbers that had been temporarily excluded in the last iteration. This procedure resulted in the exclusion of 2.54% of the correct responses. 2 The results for different trials ere examined to ensure that the subjects ere able to perform the task. To do so, the means from each cell ere subjected to an analysis of variance, such as the one described belo for same responses only, butthepresentanalysis included response as an additional ithin-subjects factor. The mean response time for same responses as 837 msec and the mean for different responses as 1,060 msec [F(l,28) = 47.38, P <.0001]. Errors ere defined as trials in hich the subject made the rong response (these did not include trials for hich the response time as an outlier). The error rate as 6.4% for same responses and 3.1 % for different responses [F(l,28) = 14.64, P <.0007]. The means of the correct same trials, averaged over patterns and orientations, ere submitted to a mixedmodel analysis of variance in hich exposure duration and blockorder (SLLS/LSSL) ere beteen-subjects factors and size (small/large), block (first vs. second, at a given size), and distance ere ithin-subjects factors. The mean of these means, averaged across subjects, for each distance, each exposure duration, and each size can be seen in Figure 4. In the analysis of variance and in Figure 4, curve distance is expressed in units relative to the overall pattern size. There are a number of results that are immediately apparent upon looking at Figure 4. First, the time to respond that the to dots ere on the same curve increased monotonically ith increasing curve distance [F(3,84) = 40.29, P <.0001], suggesting that the subjects traced the curves to perform the task. Second, there ere little or no effects of changing the size of the patterns. The main effect of size as not significant [F(l,28) = 1.24, P >.27], and size did not interact significantly ith exposure-duration conditions [F(l,28) = 1.97, P >.17]. Perhaps even more importantly, the effects ofdistance on response times ere essentially identical across small and large patterns (F < 1) and across combinations of size and exposureduration conditions (F < 1). These results are hat e ould expect ifthere as complete compensation by tracing mechanisms for the increased size of the patterns. Third, response times ere faster in the limited-exposure condition than in the unlimited-condition, although this effect as only marginal in the analysis of variance [F(l,28) = 3.76, P <.063]. Block order effects. We also examined the various counterbalancing factors to discover hether the effects described above changed in important ays depending on testing order. For any given subject, the four blocks of trials ere coded as the 2 x 2 combination ofpattern size (large or small) and hether a block as the first or second block ithin each size. In general, the effects that ere significant in the analysis of variance appear to be due to practice. For completeness, e list here all the effects that ere significant. The subjects ere faster on the second block (798 msec) at a particular size than on the first block (876 msec)[f(l,28) = 41.30, P <.0001]; the effects of distance ere slightly larger on the first block than on the second [F(3,84) = 2.87, P <.042]. Both of these significant effects probably reflect nothing more than practice. Unexpectedly, the SLLS group (753 msec) responded faster overall than did the LSSL group (921 msec) [F(l,28) = 5.80, P <.023]. We have no ready explanation for this difference. The group factor also interacted ith pattern size (for the SLLS group, small = 787 msec, large = 719 msec; for the LSSL group, small = 902 msec, large = 938 msec) [F(l,28) = 15.01, P <.0006]. A likely explanation for this difference is that practice effects tend to be larger early in the course of learning

7 CURVE TRACING 27 Figure 4. Mean response time (in milliseconds) and percent error rate for JIIIM trials in Experiment 1 for each exposure condition, each dbaoce, and each pattern size. Circles denote JIIIM trials. Also depicted is the mean response time for dqfermt trials for each exposure condition and each pattern size, couapsed across distadce. Squares denote dqfermt trials. Open symbols denote the limited-exposure condition. Filled symbols denote the unlimited-exposure condition. than later on. Thus, for the group that started ith large patterns, e ould expect longer responses to large patterns than to small ones, and for the group that started ith small patterns, e ould expect longer responses to small patterns than to large ones, as e found. Neither of the above effects entered into significant interactions ith the distance factor, suggesting that the rates of tracing ere not affected strongly by these group and block effects [for the block order (SLLS/LSSL) X distance interaction, F(3,84) = 1.41, P >.24; for the block order X size X distance interaction, F(3, 84) = 1. 31, P >.27]. There ere no other significant effects in the analysis of variance. Errors. The mean percent error rate for same trials, averaged across subjects, for each distance, each exposure duration, and each size, is shon at the bottom of Figure 4. Curve distance is plotted in units relative to the overall pattern size. The means ere submitted to the same analyses used for response times. As in the response-time results, there are a number of results that are immediately apparent upon looking at Figure 4. First, there ere more errors in the limited condition than in the unlimited condition [F(l,28) = 13.73, P <.001]. Second, there ere more errors as the distance beteen the dots along the curve increased [F(3,84) = 18.11, P <.0001], hich mirrored the pattern of results for the response times. Third, the effect of distance on error rates as larger in the limitedexposure group than in the unlimited-exposure group [F(3,84) = 5.51, P <.002]. Given the brief exposure duration in the limited group, it is possible that these subjectsere processing a fading representation ofthe stimulus. This representation could have become increasingly

8 28 JOLICOEUR AND INGLETON more difficult to use as the distance to be traced increased, given that more time as required to complete processing in that condition. There as no overall difference in the error rates for small versus large patterns [F(l,28) = 2.40, p >.13]. Hoever, the difference in error rates across pattern size as different across the to exposure-duration conditions [F(1,28) = 6.50,p <.017]. As can be seen in Figure 4, small patterns ere associated ith higher error rates than ere larger patterns in the limited condition, hereas there as virtually no difference in the unlimited condition. Separate analyses on the results for each exposure condition shoed that the small-large difference as significant in the limited condition [F(l,14) = 4.86, P <.045], but not in the unlimited condition [F(l, 14) = 1.84, P >.19]. It is possible that some of these effects ere due to speed-accuracy tradeoffs (e.g., a tendency for error rates to increase more across distance for small patterns than for large ones in the limited condition, hich may have masked an advantage for large patterns overall). Hoever, hat e ant to emphasize is that the direction of these effects is such that our main conclusion is unaffected-the slope of the distance function as not steeper for larger patterns than it as for small ones for either errors or response times. Block order effects. We also examined the various counterbalancing factors to discover hether or not the effects described above changed in important ays. There ere many significant effects involving block order. In general, as in the response-time results, they seemed to reflect simple practice effects. There as one effect, hoever, that e cannot describe simply: the five-ay exposure duration X block order X size X block (first or second, at a given size) x distance interaction [F(3,84) = 5.16, P <.0026]. The relevant means are presented in Table 1. In the main, the effects of pattern size tended to be small, and it seems reasonably safe to interpret the response-time results ithout strong concern for speedaccuracy tradeoffs for all but the tradeoff in overall response time and errors across the unlimited and the limited conditions. Discussion Overall, the results ere more consistent ith complete compensation for changes in pattern size than ith any of the other alternatives mentioned in the introduction. The effects of distance on response times ere equivalent across patterns of different size hen size as expressed in units relative to the overall pattern size (see Figure 4). Another ay to think about this result is that, in order to produce the observed pattern of means, the absolute rate oftracing in terms of visual angle traversed per unit time ould have to have been roughly tice as fast in blocks of trials involving large patterns than as it ould have to have been in blocks ith small patterns. These results ill be discussed in more detail in the General Discussion. An interpretation of the error-rate results as complicated by a significant higher order interaction involving the block-order counterbalancing variables. When collapsed across all these variables, hoever, the results seem more consistent ith complete size invariance than ith any otheralternative, as is evident in Figure 4. This slight complication in the present experiment can be taken as an incentive to seek converging evidence for hat otherise appeared to be a clear-cut pattern of results in favor of size invariance. We sought that converging evidence in Experiments 2 and 3. EXPERIMENT 2 The results of Experiment 1 suggest that the rate of curve tracing varies proportionally ith pattern size. Hoever, it is important to determine not only ifblocking the trials by the size of patterns maximized rate differences, as as expected, but ifin fact it produced rate differences that ould not be observed ithout blocking. For example, it could be that the size invariance demonstrated in Experiment 1 is possible only under restricted conditions in hich the scale at hich tracing takes place is roughly constant across trials. This could have been the case if, for example, the blocking procedure permitted a difficult and slo global adjustment to some mechanism involved Table 1 Mean Percent Error Rate for Each Exposure Duration, Each Block Order (SLLS/LSSL), Each Pattern Size, Each Block (First or Second at a Given Size), and Each Distance in Experiment 1 Small SLLS Large Distance First Second First Second Small LSSL Large First Second First Second Unlimited Exposure Limited Exposure

9 CURVE TRACING 29 in tracing that might not have been possible ifpattern size as changed from trial to trial. In Experiment 2, the size of patterns varied randomly from trial to trial so that the subjects could not anticipate hich of the to sizes ould appear next. Method Subjects. The subjects ere 32 University of Waterloo undergraduates beteen the ages of 17 and 35 years. They ere paid to participate in this study. Halfof the subjects ere in the limitedexposure group and half ere in the unlimited-exposure group. There ere 8 males and 8 females in each group. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None had participated previously in a curve-tracing experiment. Stimuli and Procedure. The stimuli ere those used in Experiment I. Pattern size varied randomly from trial to trial, ith the constraint that a particular size could not be repeated on more than four consecutive trials. The other blocking constraints employed in Experiment I ere also met. For each subject, subblocks of 16 trials ithin the first to and last to blocks of trials used in Experiment I ere combined into ne subblocksof 32 trials, herein each pattern in each size format occurred tice, once upright and once inverted. The 512 trials ere presented in four blocks of 128, ith a rest period beteen blocks. Also, practice trials of randomly mixed size preceded each experimental block. The subjects ere informed that pattern sizes ould change randomly from trial to trial. Otherise, the task, instructions, and procedure ere the same as those in Experiment I. The subjects took approximately I h to complete the experimental session. Results Response times. The response times ere first screened for outliers using the procedure detailed in Experiment I, hich resulted in the rejection of 1.88% of the correct response times. Folloing the outlier-identification procedure, the mean response time as computed for each subject, each exposure-duration condition, each pattern size, each response, and each distance by averaging across patterns and orientations. The relevant means are displayed in Figure 5. Correct response times and percent error rates ere initially analyzed, including the factors of block, response, size, and distance. We found the expected practice effects across blocks, but there ere no differences in the trends over distance beteen blocks for the to sizes. For this reason, the block factor as excluded from further consideration. As in Experiment I, the results for different trials ere examined to ensure that the subjects ere able to perform the task. To do so, the means from each cell ere subjected to an analysis of variance like the one described belo for same responses only, but the present analysis included response as an additional ithin-subjects factor. The mean response time for same responses as 806 msec and the mean for different responses as 1,019 msec [F(I,30) ::: ,p <.0001]. The error rate as 5.6% for same responses and 3.6% for different responses [F(l,30) = 10.47, P <.003]. The means of the correct same trials ere submitted to a mixed-model analysis of variance in hich exposure duration as a beteen-subjects factor and in hich size (small/large) and distance ere ithin-subjects factors. As in Experiment I, on average, response time increased monotonically ith increasing curve distance [F(3,90) ::: 60.04, p <.0001], suggesting that the subjects traced the curves to perform the task. Unlike in Experiment I, hoever, there ere a number ofsignificant effects involving pattern size. Responses ere faster for large patterns than for small ones [F(l,30) ::: 18.46, p <.0002]. Furthermore, the effects of distance depended significantly on the joint effects of pattern size and exposure duration [F(3,90) ::: 4.00, p <.011]. We examined this interaction in more detail by performing separate analyses on the results from each exposure duration. The interaction beteen size and distance as not significant in the limited condition [F(3,45) ::: 1.71,p >.17] but as significant in the unlimited condition [F(3,45) = 3.35, P <.028]. As can be seen in Figure 5, the magnitude of the distance effect as smaller for large patterns than for small patterns, hich suggests, if anything, some degree of overcompensation for pattern size. There ere no other significant effects in the analysis of same trials. Errors. The mean percent error rate is graphed for each distance, each exposure duration, and each pattern size in the bottom panel of Figure 5. These error rates ere submitted to the same type of analysis as that used for the response times. There ere more errors in the limitedexposure condition than in the unlimited-exposure condition [F(l,30) = 23.88, p <.0001]. The error rate increased ith increasing curve distance [F(3,90) ::: 15.75, p <.0001], and this increase as larger in the limited condition than in the unlimited condition [F(3,90) ::: 3.86, p <.012]. As in Experiment I, the larger error rate in the limited condition as accompanied by faster response times (although the response time difference as not significant) than in the unlimited condition. For the other effects, the pattern of error rates did not suggest speedaccuracy tradeoffs. Sequential analysis. In addition to the above analyses, e also subjected the results for same trials to an analysis in hich e conditionalized trials depending not only on the size of the pattern on that trial, but also on the size of the pattern on the preceding trial. We called a trial congruent for size if the present size matched that ofthe preceding trial. The analysis as otherise identical to the one described above for response times. The means for each exposure duration, each distance, and each size condition (congruent-small, congruent-large, incongruent-small, and incongruent-large) are shon in Figure 6. As is evident in Figure 6, conditionalizing trials on the size of the preceding trial did not modulate the magnitude of the distance effect, as suggested by the nonsignificant interaction beteen preceding size, present size, and distance [F(3,90) ::: 1.04, p >.37], and this effect did not depend on exposure duration (F < I). These

10 30 JOLICOEUR AND INGLETON 1100 small LARGE f= C/) z C/) 850 IT: z <l: UNLIMITED EXPOSURE small c small LARGE 0 LARGE amall LARGE LIMITED EXPOSURE 700 r ;e e IT: 15.0 IT: IT: IT: 0.0 W small: LARGE: LARGE LIMITED small /' EXPOSURE small ;===e l"j 8E UNLIMITED t -- LAE II -=. GEXPOSURE RELATIVE DISTANCE (UNITS) SAME I IJ//---tl;.;.;:.: ABSOLUTE DISTANCE (DEGREES) - SAME.. DIFFERENT Figure S. Mean response time (in milliseconds) and percent error rate for IIIJIM trials inexperiment 2 for eachexposure condition, eachdistance, and each patternsize. Circles denote IIIJIM trials. Also depleted is the mean response time for tjqferat trials for each exposure condition and each pattern size. Squares denote difftrmt trials. Open symbols denote the Umited-exposure condition. Filled symbols denote the unlimited-exposure condition. results suggest that the rate of tracing as not affected by the congruence or incongruence ofthe size of the pattern on a given trial ith the size on the preceding trial. The interaction beteen preceding size and present size did reach significance, hoever [F(l,30) = 4.44, P <.044]. The means are shon in Table 2. There appears to have been essentially no effect of preceding size on response times hen the present size as large. In contrast, hen the preceding size as small and the present size as small, responses ere sloer than hen the preceding size as large and the present size as small. That is, for small patterns, the subjects ere slightly faster hen the size as incongruent than hen it as congruent. The reason for this effect is not clear, and it did not replicate in Experiment 3. We therefore do not consider it further. Discussion As in Experiment 1, the results ere more consistent ith complete compensation in tracing rate for pattern size than ith any ofthe alternatives discussed in the introduction. Overall, the effects of relative distance ere very similar across patterns ofdifferent size, suggesting that, ifthe curves ere traced, tracing mechanisms could have been adjusted to the particular characteristics ofthe stimuli on a trial-by-trial basis. This suggestion is also supported by the results of the sequential analysis, in hich there ere no effects from the size of the pattern on Trial n on the time to respond on Trial n+1 on the apparent rate oftracing. The lack ofmeasurable sequential effects could have resulted from the 3-sec intertrial interval, although Larsen and Bundesen (1978) found substantial sequential effects in a size-scaling paradigm ith 2-sec intervals.

11 CURVE TRACING UNLIMITED EXPOSURE LIMITED EXPOSURE 1050 CONGRUENT LARGE 1000 amall W INCONGRUENT ;::; 950.& LARGE I-... amall W (f) z (f) a: 850 z «800,,, 750,'jA.- l' /1 "..&,' 700.,--;(, " 'e I I I I small: 2 LARGE: r I CONGRUENT o LARGE o amall INCONGRUENT l!. LARGE... amall " 0' ---- RELATIVE DISTANCE (UNITS) SAME ABSOLUTE DISTANCE 2 (DEGREES) SAME 3 I, I d I I, I J:'l!. ", Figure 6. Mean response times (in milliseconds) for!jtjim trials in each exposure condition of Experiment 2 as a function ofdistance, pattern size in the present trial, and hether the present size is congruent or incongruent ith the size ofthe pattern on the preceding trial. Circles denote size congruent trials. Triangles denote size incongruent trials. Solid lines denotesmall patterns. Dotted lines denote large patterns. 6 12, " I I " I, II ' EXPERIMENT 3 In Experiment 2, the analysis considering possible carryover effects from the size of the pattern on one trial to the next revealed no hint ofsuch effects. In Experiment 3, size as varied from trial to trial ith a probability manipulation, such that there as an 80% chance that the size ould be the same as on the preceding trial and only a 20% chance that the size ould change. This manipulation as intended to induce an expectancy for patterns at a particular size; e hoped to see hether or not this expectancy had any effect by looking for differences in performance across trials for hich the expectancy as met and those for hich the expectancy as violated. Method Subjects. The subjects ere 32 University of Waterloo undergraduates beteen the ages of 18 and 31 years. They ere paid to participate in this study. Halfofthe subjects ere in the limitedexposure group and half ere in the unlimited-exposure group. There ere 8 males and 8 females in each group. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None had participated previously in a curve-tracing experiment. Stimuli and Procedure. The stimuli ere the same as those in Experiments 1 and 2. The trials ere ordered such that, on each trial, there as an 80% probability that the present stimulus size ould be the same as on the previous trial and a 20% probability that it ould be different. The trial orders ere created by using to ofthe blocks used for each subject in Experiment 1 (one block of each size) and selecting trials from these blocks according to a schedule that had a.8 probability of selecting from the present block and a.2 probability of sitching to the other block. Blocks I and 2 again ere randomized for each subject ith the original constraints that there be no repeated patterns at either orientation and no more than four consecutive trials ith the same response. Because ofthis probabilistic arrangement ofpattern size, there ere unequal numbers of small and large patterns in the first to blocks. To ensure that equal numbers of the to sizes occurred over the 512 trials, Blocks 3 and 4 ere generated from Blocks 1 and 2 by exchanging the pattern size values. That is, patterns that ere small in Blocks I and 2 ere large in Blocks 3 and 4. Conversely, patterns that ere large in Blocks I and 2 ere small in Blocks 3 and 4. Practice trials ere generated ith the same probability manipulation as in the experimental trials. The subjects ere informed that the pattern size could change on a trial-to-trial basis, but they ere not informed explicitly in the instructions that there as a greater chance that it ould stay the same. We assumed that they ould soon discover this aspect ofthe procedure during the practice trials and in the first portion ofthe experimental trials. In fact, hen questioned after the experiment, most subjects claimed to have noticed the probability manipulation. Apart from the above changes, the task and procedure ere the same as in Experiment 2. The subjects completed the experimental session in about 1 h.

12 32 JOLICOEUR AND INGLETON Table 2 Mean Response Time (in milliseconds) for Each Pattern Size, Depending on the Size of the Previous Trial, in Experiment 2 Present Size Small Large Small Preceding Size Large Results Response times. The data ere analyzed as in Experiment 2. The correct response times ere first screened for outliers using the algorithm described in Experiment I, hich resulted in the rejection of 1.39% of the data. Correct response times and percent error rates ere initially analyzed, including the factors ofblock, response, size, and distance. Again, e found the expected practice effects across blocks, but there ere no differences in the trends over distance beteen blocks for the to sizes. Forthis reason, the block factor as excluded from further analysis. Responses ere faster overall for same trials (831 msec) than for different trials (993 msec) [F(1,30) = 44.73, p <.0001]. Hoever, there ere more errors on same trials (6.8%) than on different trials (3.8%) [F(1,30) = 14.32, p <.0007]. This patternof results as essentially the same as in Experiments I and 2. The mean response time for each exposure duration, each response, each pattern size, and each distance is plotted in Figure 7. The response times increased monotonically ith increasing curve distance [F(3,90) = 33.58, p <.0001]. The distance factor did not interact ith any other effect in the analysis (p >.11, in every case). The only other significant effect reflected the fact that responses ere slightly faster for large patterns (821 msec) than for small patterns (841 msec) [F(l,30) = 5.38, P <.028], as as found in Experiment 2. Errors. The mean percenterror rate for each curve distance, each exposure duration, and each pattern size is also shon in Figure 7. These error rates ere analyzed using the same analysis as for the response times. As ould be expected upon inspection of Figure 7, the only to significant effects ere due to exposure duration [F(1,30) = 11.73, p <.0018] and distance [F(3,90) = 20.30, p <.0001]. Apart from the possible tradeoff across the to exposure-durationconditions, it seems safe to say that the pattern of response times does not appear to be due to speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Sequential analysis. As in Experiment 2, the data from same trials ere also subjected to an analysis in hich e conditionalized trials depending on the size ofthe pattern on that trial and on the size of the pattern on the preceding trial. The means are shon in Figure 8. Using the same analysis-of-variance model as for the sequential analysis in Experiment 2, e found that none ofthe effects involving the size of the preceding trial and the size of the present trial (hich ould have reflected effects of size congruency) ere significant (p >.11, in all cases). Discussion The results ere consistent ith complete size invariance: There as no difference in the magnitude of the distance effects (in relative units) across the to pattern sizes. Also, there ere no sequential effects across trials despite a probability manipulation that should have encouraged the subjects to expecta particularsize atthe onset of a trial. These results suggest that the mechanisms supporting performance in this paradigm can adjust to some characteristics of the stimuli rapidly from information encoded at the onset of a trial. GENERAL DISCUSSION The three experiments reported in the present paper investigated the possible effects ofoverall pattern size on the time to decide hether to dots are on the same curve or on to different curves. Response time increased sharply and monotonically as the length ofthe curve joining the dots as increased, suggesting that curve tracing as used to solve the experimental task (see Jolicoeur, 1988; Jolicoeur et al., 1986, 1990; McCormick & Jolicoeur, 1990a, 1990b; Pringle & Egeth' 1988). On the assumption that the magnitude of the effects of distance reflects the rate at hich an underlying mechanism is able to trace the curve, the results clearly indicate that the rate of tracing, in absolute terms, changed proportionally ith pattern size for the patterns used in the present study. An alternative ay ofexpressing the findings is that the rate of tracing remained constant, relative to the overall pattern size (ith distance expressed in units proportional to pattern size). In the latter terms, tracing exhibited complete size invariance, hich e could also refer to as a type of object constancy. Recently, Jolicoeur et al. (1990) examined the role of the curvature ofthe to-be-traced curve on the rate oftracing (see also Jolicoeur, 1988). Tracing as sloer, in absolute terms, hen the curves had higher curvature. That is, tracing along a curve for, say, 2 0 of visual angle took longer hen the curve as bent along a smaller radius of curvature than hen the curvature as smaller. This finding is similar to one reported by Pringle and Egeth (1988). Their stimuli can be described as a circle ith portions removed on either side ofan imaginary diameter line. The remaining stimulus is a circle ith to gaps, the gaps being on opposite "sides" ofthe circle. The task as to decide hether to Xs ere on the same arc or on to different arcs of the circle-ith-gaps figure. Increasing the distance beteen the Xs along the same arc

13 CURVE TRACING LIMITED :2 950 EXPOSURE i= 00 z C- oo a: 850 z «800 :2 amall Imall LARGE LARGE Imall LARGE EJ t.r1fe UNLIMITED EXPOSURE r 25.0 W 20.0 a: 15.0 a: a: a: 0.0 : RELATIVE DISTANCE (UNITS) SAME small LARGE: ABSOLUTE DISTANCE (DEGREES) - SAME I I'; LIMITED EXPOSURE / UNLIMITED 1:1?iEEXPOSURE.Imall""'- LARGE DIFFERENT Figure 7. Mean response time (in milliseconds) and percent error rate for same trials in Experiment 3 for each exposure condition, each distance, and each pattern size. Circles denote same trials. Also depleted is the mean response time for tlufn'mt trlais for each exposure condition and each pattern size. Squares denote tlufn'mt trials. Open symbols denote the limited-exposure condition. Filled symbols denote the unlimited-exposure condition. increased the time it took for subjects to respond same, suggesting that subjects ere tracing the arc joining the to Xs in order to perform the task. Across their Experiments 1 and 2, Pringle and Egeth varied the size of their circ1e-ith-gaps figure by a factor of to. Their results ere similar to ours in that the time taken to trace one unit ofrelative distance as the same across the to sizes, even though there as effectively tice as much curve to be traced in the larger stimulus, as as found in the present experiments. These results are also consistent ith the curvature effects reported by Jolicoeur et al. (1990) in that their larger stimulus also had a smaller curvature, hich should allo tracing to proceed at a faster rate. Thus, the Pringle and Egeth (1988) results agree quite ell ith those of Jolicoeur et al. (1990) and ith the results of the present experiments. As mentioned earlier, Jolicoeur et al. (1990) found evidence suggesting that increasing the proximity of distractor curves slos don the rate oftracing. They suggested that proximal distractor curves ould force the size of a tracing operator to shrink hereas distant distractor curves ould allo it to expand and that the rate of tracing should be proportional to the size ofthe operator. The results of the present study provide converging evidence for these hypotheses. In the present experiments, the absolute rate of tracing doubled hen the patterns doubled in overall size. Increasing the size of the patterns, hoever, also reduced the curvature of the target curves and doubled the distance beteen the target curves and distractors. Both ofthese factors, e argue, ould allo the rate of tracing to increase. What is interesting about the present set of experiments is that the increase in the rate

14 34 JOLICOEUR AND INGLETON 1100 UNLIMITED EXPOSURE F 950 C/) z 0 a C/) II: 850 Z « CONGRUENT LARGE amall INCONGRUENT.. LARGE.. amall,,,,,,.-...," ---/,, ' LIMITED EXPOSURE CONGRUENT o LARGE amall INCONGRUENT Ii. LARGE.. 8mall I I I RELATIVE DISTANCE (UNITS). SAME small: LARGE: ABSOLUTE DISTANCE (DEGREES) SAME Figure 8. Mean response times (in milliseconds) for SlUM trials in each exposure condition of Experiment 3 as a function of distance, pattern size in the present trial, and hether the present size as congruent or incongruent ith the size of the pattern on the preceding trial. Circles denote size congruent trials. Triangles denote size incongruent trials. Solid lines denote small patterns. Dotted lines denote large patterns. of tracing appears to compensate exactly for the change in pattern size. Thus, effectively, tracing on these stimuli as size invariant. Advance knoledge of the size of the pattern to be traced seemed to convey no advantage to curve-tracing operations, hich as shon in the sequential analyses in Experiments 2 and 3. One might be concerned by the relatively long time interval beteen stimuli in these experiments, hich could have attenuated sequential effects. Hoever, Larsen and Bundesen (1978) found substantial sequential ef(ects in a size-scaling paradigm ith intervals of 2-sec beteen stimuli. Furthermore, there as no interaction beteen experiments and distance (F < 1) in an omnibus analysis of variance in hich the three experiments in the present paper ere included as a beteensubjects factor. Thus, blocking the trials by size in Experiment 1 did not change the apparent rate of tracing compared ith that in Experiments 2 and 3, contrary to hat ould be expected if knoledge of the size of the upcoming pattern alloed the subjects to calibrate various parameters in the visual system so as to optimize performance. These results suggest that the processes that support curve tracing can adapt rapidly to the characteristics of the stimuli, hich is also suggested by the very similar results obtained in the limited-exposure condition (200 msec) and in the unlimited-exposure condition. Nonlinear Effects of Distance One aspect ofthe present results and those ofjolicoeur et al. (1986) that may seem problematic for our model of tracing is the nonlinear pattern of response times as a function ofcurve distance. Recent ork by McCormick and Jolicoeur (l990b), hoever, suggests that the problem is more apparent than real. In fact, one of our central claims concerning tracing operations is that the rate of tracing is not constant, but rather it depends on a number of properties of the displays-in particular, on the proximity ofdistractor curves to the target curve. McCormick and Jolicoeur performed a number ofpost hoc item analyses using the data from the present study. Some of the displays (Pairs 1, 5, 6, and 7) in Figure 3 seemed to produce quite linear results, hereas the remaining displays (Pairs 2, 3,4, and 8) seemed to produce markedly less linear results than those reported in the present article, hich ere produced by averaging the results across these groups of items. Pairs I, 5, 6, and 7 share the

15 CURVE TRACING 35 property that one end of the distractor curve enters the first "lobe" of the target curve (i.e., that part of the target curve near Target-Dot Locations 1 and 2) from the inside. We call these pairs in pairs. The remaining pairs do not have this property; the distractor curve, although at the same distance from Dot Locations 1 and 2, is on the outside of the lobe. We call these pairs out pairs. According to our model, the presence of the distractor curve ithin the first lobe should force the scope of the tracing operator to become smaller than it ould be for patterns in hich the distractor curve does not enter the lobe from the inside (see McCormick & Jolicoeur, 1990b, for a more detailed account). The smaller scope of the operator should result in a sloer tracing speed, hich ould result in a more linear pattern of results hen compared ith the tracing speed possible in out patterns. McCormick and Jolicoeur tested this hypothesis directly in to experiments in hich each target curve as paired ith an in distractor curve and ith an out distractor curve equally often. The results ofthese experiments ere consistent ith our expectations: In distractors produced linear distance effects, hereas out distractors produced nonlinear effects (see McCormick & Jolicoeur, 1990b, for more details). Alternative Accounts In this final section e consider other possible accounts of our results. Size scaling. Perhaps the subjects traced curves to solve the task and perhaps the tracing had a fixed speed, but the subjects scaled the size ofthe representation on hich tracing took place prior to tracing (Besner, 1983; Besner & Coltheart, 1975, 1976; Bundesen & Larsen, 1975; Bundesen, Larsen, & Farrell, 1981; Jolicoeur & Besner, 1987; Larsen, 1985; Larsen & Bundesen, 1978; Sekuler & Nash, 1972). We cannot rule out this account entirely. Hoever, e believe that it is not as parsimonious or coherent as the account e provided above. For example. the fact that the response times for large patterns tended to be smaller than they ere for small patterns ould suggest, on a scaling account, that small patterns ere scaled up and large patterns ere either not scaled or scaled don but to a lesser extent. Hoever, that ould increase the distance to be traced for scaled-up small patterns, hich seems counterproductive because the distance to be traced ould be increased. The main alternative is to consider the possibility that the rate of tracing is not constant but that size scaling also takes place. For example, perhaps small patterns ere scaled up so as to increase the distance beteen the target curve and the distractor curve in order to facilitate subsequent tracing. Hoever, this account concedes that the rate of tracing depends on various properties ofthe displays, such as curvature and proximity ofdistractors, hich seems sufficient to account for the results. By Occam's razor, e should invoke size scaling only ifcompelledto do so by some aspect ofthe results that could not be accounted for by the notion that the rate of tracing is variable. Fitts'la. Pringle and Egeth (1988) suggested that the size invariance they found in their tracing task could potentially be explained by appeal to Fitts' La (e.g., Fitts, 1954). The observation is that, in the case of motor control, there is an inverse relation beteen the speed of a movement and the accuracy of that movement. The analogy is that, for the small patterns in our study, a tracing process ould require finer control over the positioning of the processing locus in order to remain "on curve." It remains to be seen hether or not this analogy beteen motor control and vision is correct, but it seems orthy of further thought. Attention. Some readers may consider the possibility that the effects ofdistance in the present paradigm might be due to more general attentional mechanisms than the ones e are proposing, such as feature integration (Treisman & Gelade, 1980). In this vie, the distance effects e observe may not be due to tracing at all but to some other property of the displays that increases the difficulty of the task as curve distance increases. Given the present paradigm, hoever, e believe that such possibilities are unlikely. At any particular display size, the Euclidean distance beteen the dots and the retinal eccentricity does not vary as curve distance varies. Furthermore, if feature integration as at the root of the present distance effects, one ould expect a much greater likelihood ofconjunction errors in the limited-exposure condition than in the unlimited-exposure condition, hich in tum ould lead us to expect larger distance effects in the limitedexposure condition than in the unlimited-exposure condition. Hoever, the effects of distance and exposure duration ere additive (Sternberg, 1969), suggesting that feature integration does not playa causal role in producing effects of curve distance in the present paradigm. In addition, the greater eccentricity ofthe dots and the greater distances over hich a nontracing attentional mechanism ould have to operate on a large pattern, as ell as the greater retinal eccentricity, relative to small patterns ould lead one to expect sloer response times and probably steeper slopes for large patterns than for small ones, hich is at variance ith hat e found. Another possibility is that tracing mechanisms are a subset of a more general set of attentional mechanisms. What e refer to as a tracing operator may be part ofthis more general spatial attentional system (e.g., Eriksen & Hoffman, 1972; Eriksen & Murphy, 1987; Humphreys, 1981; LaBerge, 1983; Posner, 1980; Remington & Pierce, 1984; Shulman, Remington, & Mclean, 1979; Tsal, 1983; Yantis, 1988). McCormick and Jolicoeur (1990a) have begun a systematic investigation of this possibility. We have no difficulty ith this vie. Hoever, e consider it an open question orthy of independent empirical investigation. If tracing mechanisms are the same ones that are involved in other attentional tasks, then the present paradigm ould contribute important ne insights and techniques for our investigation of these attentional mechanisms. For example, it ould be clear that, under cer-

16 36 JOLICOEUR AND INGLETON tain circumstances, attention does seem to move across space at a limited rate (that depends on a number of display properties), hich ould be support for the spotlight metaphor for attention (e.g., Tsal, 1983). REFERENCES BESNER, D. (1983). Visual pattern recognition: Size preprocessing reexamined. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, BESNER, D., & COLTHEART, M. (1975). Same-different judgments ith ords and nonords: The differential effects of relative size. Memory & Cognition, 3, BESNER, D., & COLTHEART, M. (1976). Mental size scaling examined. Memory & Cognition, 4, BIEDERMAN, I. (1985). Human image understanding: Recent research and a theory. Computer Vision, Graphics, & Image Processing, 32, BIEDERMAN, I. (1988). Aspects and extensions of a theory of human image understanding. In Z. W. Pylyshyn (Ed.), Computational processes in human vision: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp ). Norood, NJ: Ablex. BUNDESEN, c., & LARSEN, A. (1975). Visual transformation of size. Journal ofexperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, I, BUNDESEN, C., LARSEN, A., & FARRELL, J. E. (1981). Mental transformation of size and orientation. In A. Baddeley & J. Long (Eds.), Attention & Performance IX (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. CAVANAGH, P. (1987). Reconstructing the third dimension: Interactions beteen color, texture, motion, binocular disparity, and shape. Computer Vision, Graphics, & Image Processing, 37, ERIKSEN, C. W., & HOFFMAN, J. E. (1972). Temporal and spatial characteristics of selective encoding from visual displays. Perception & Psychophysics, 12, ERIKSEN, C. W., & MURPHY, T. D. (1987). Movement ofattentional focus across the visual field: A critical look at the evidence. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, FIELD, D. (1984). To algorithms for draing anti-aliased lines. In Proceedings ofgraphics Interface '84 (pp ). Toronto: The Canadian Information Processing Society. FITTS, P. M. (1954). The information capacity ofthe human motor system in controlling the amplitude ofmovement. Journal ofexperimental Psychology, 47, HUBEL, D. H., & WIESEL, T. N. (1968). Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. Journal ofphysiology (London), 195, HUMPHREYS, G. W. (1981). On varying the span of visual attention: Evidence for to modes ofspatial attention. Quarterly Journal ofexperimental Psychology, 33A, JOLICOEUR, P. (1988). Curve tracing operations and the perception of spatial relations. In Z. W. Pylyshyn (Ed.), Computational processes in human vision: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp ). Norood, NJ: Ablex. JOLICOEUR, P., & BESNER, D. (1987). Additivity and interaction beteen size ratio and response category in the comparison of sizediscrepant shapes. Journal ofexperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 13, JOLICOEUR, P., ULLMAN, S., & MACKAY, M. (1986). Curve tracing: A possible basic operation in the perception ofspatial relations. Memory & Cognition, 14, JOLICOEUR, P., ULLMAN, S., & MACKAY, M. (1990). Curve tracing properties. Manuscript submitted for publication. LABERGE, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and ords. Journal ofexperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 9, LARSEN, A. (1985). Pattern matching: Effects ofsize ratio, angular difference in orientation, and familiarity. Perception & Psychophysics, 38, LARSEN, A., & BUNDESEN, C. (1978). Size scaling in visual pattern recognition. Journal ofexperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 4, MAHONEY, J. V., & ULLMAN, S. (1988). Image chunking: Defining spatial building blocks for scene analysis. In Z. W. Pylyshyn (Ed.), Computational processes in human vision: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp ). Norood, NJ: Ablex. McCORMICK, P. A., & JOLICOEUR, P. (l99oa). Capturing visual attention and the curve tracing operation. Manuscript submitted for publication. MCCORMICK, P. A., & JOLICOEUR, P. (l990b). Predicting linear and nonlinear effects ofdistance in curve tracing: Evidence for a zoomlens operator. Manuscript submitted for publication. POSNER, M. I. (1980). Orienting ofattention. Quarterly Journal ofexperimental Psychology, 32, PRINGLE, R., & EGETH, H. E. (1988). Mental curve tracing ith elementary stimuli. Journal ofexperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 14, REMINGTON, R., & PIERCE, L. (1984). Moving attention: Evidence for time-invariant shifts of visual selective attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 35, SEKULER, R., & NASH, D. (1972). Speed ofsize scaling in human vision. Psychonomic Science, 27, SHULMAN, G. L., REMINGTON, R. W., & McLEAN, 1. P. (1979). Moving attention through visual space. Journal ofexperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,S, STERNBERG, S. (1%9). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method. Acta Psychologica, 30, TANNER, P. P., JOLICOEUR, P., COWAN, W. B., BOOTH, K., & FISHMAN, F. D. (1988). Antialiasing: A technique for smoothing jagged lines on a computer graphics image-an implementation on the AMIGA. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 21, TREISMAN, A. M., & GELADE, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, TSAL, Y. (1983). Movements of attention across the visual field. Journal ofexperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 9, ULLMAN, S. (1984). Visual routines. Cognition, 18, YANTIS, S. (1988). On analog movements of visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 43, NOTES 1. The limited conditions in Experiments I, 2, and 3 ere carried out as separate experiments first. Several months later, three ne experiments (4, 5, and 6) implementing the unlimited conditions corresponding ith the first three experiments ere carried out. For expository purposes, Experiments I and 4, Experiments 2 and 5, and Experiments 3 and 6 are reported here as three experiments ith a beteen-subjects manipulation of exposure duration. 2. One revieer expressed some concern about this method for screening out outliers. We repeated the analyses using a similar algorithm but one that did not first temporarily exclude the smallest and largest observation and using three standard deviations to define the limits for all the experiments in the present paper. None of the analyses that e report changed significantly-all effects that ere significant remained significant, and all effects that ere not significant remained not significant. (Manuscript received January 22, 1990; revision accepted for publication May II, 1990.)

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SESORY COSTRAIS? TOWARD A UIFIED THEORY OF GEERAL TASK DIFFICULTY Hanna Benoni and Yehoshua Tsal Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University hannaben@post.tau.ac.il

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay*

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay* Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay* JACOB BECK and BRUCE AMBLER University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 other extreme, when no masking

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance VOL. I I, NO. 6 DECEMBER 1985 Separability and Integrality of Global and Local Levels of Hierarchical Patterns Ruth Kimchi University

More information

Genetic Variations. F1 Generation. Mechanisms of Genetics W W. STAAR Biology: Assessment Activities

Genetic Variations. F1 Generation. Mechanisms of Genetics W W. STAAR Biology: Assessment Activities male parent female parent sperm cells egg cells F1 Generation Mechanisms of Genetics 181 182 Mechanisms of Genetics Teacher Pages Purpose The purpose of this activity is to reinforce students understanding

More information

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Automatic detection - 1 Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1986, 24 (6), 431-434 SIU L. CHOW The University of Wollongong,

More information

J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY. The relative inability of the usual differential. to ask whether performance under DRL schedules

J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY. The relative inability of the usual differential. to ask whether performance under DRL schedules JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 27-38 NUMBER I (JANUARY) THE EFFECT OF INFORMATIVE FEEDBACK ON TEMPORAL TRACKING IN THE PIGEON' J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY Pigeons emitted

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

Scale-free tests ofan additive model for the size-weight illusion*

Scale-free tests ofan additive model for the size-weight illusion* Perception &Psychophysics 1974, Vol. 16 (2), 276-282 Scale-free tests ofan additive model for the size-eight illusion* MICHAEL H. BIRNBAUMt Kansas State University, Manhattan, Kansas 66506 and CLAIRICET.

More information

Image generation in a letter-classification task

Image generation in a letter-classification task Perception & Psychophysics 1976, Vol. 20 (3),215-219 Image generation in a letter-classification task THOMAS R. HERZOG Grand Valley State Colleges, Allandale, Michigan 49401 Subjects classified briefly

More information

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (4), 697-702 Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search JAN THEEUWES Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, the Netherlands and ARTHUR F. KRAMER and

More information

Discover Activity. Think It Over Inferring Do you think height in humans is controlled by a single gene, as it is in peas? Explain your answer.

Discover Activity. Think It Over Inferring Do you think height in humans is controlled by a single gene, as it is in peas? Explain your answer. Section Human Inheritance Reading Previe Key Concepts What are some patterns of inheritance in humans? What are the functions of the sex chromosomes? What is the relationship beteen genes and the environment?

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Paul E. Hemeren (paul.hemeren@his.se) University of Skövde, School of Humanities and Informatics

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Limitations of Object-Based Feature Encoding in Visual Short-Term Memory

Limitations of Object-Based Feature Encoding in Visual Short-Term Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 458 468 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.458

More information

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1 The attentional window configures to object boundaries University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 2 ABSTRACT When

More information

Measurement of visual memory span by means of the recall of dot-in-matrix patterns

Measurement of visual memory span by means of the recall of dot-in-matrix patterns Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation 1982, Vol. 14(3),39-313 Measurement of visual memory span by means of the recall of dot-in-matrix patterns SHIN ICHI ICHIKAWA University of Tokyo, Bunkyo-ku,

More information

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 BLL R. BROWN AND THOMAS J. REBBN2 UNlVERSTY OF LOUSVLLE This experiment required Ss to make Markov-generated histoforms that were

More information

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 J'OURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2, 446-452 (1963) Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 A visual configuration, or pattern, has qualities over and above those which can be specified

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks*

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks* The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks* ARNOLD D. WELLt University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 Multidimensional stimuli, which could vary on one, two, or all three

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction

More information

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set In press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set Timothy J. Wright 1, Walter

More information

Using Perceptual Grouping for Object Group Selection

Using Perceptual Grouping for Object Group Selection Using Perceptual Grouping for Object Group Selection Hoda Dehmeshki Department of Computer Science and Engineering, York University, 4700 Keele Street Toronto, Ontario, M3J 1P3 Canada hoda@cs.yorku.ca

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Masters of Science in Psychology By Quan Ying Gao University of Canterbury

More information

The edge complex: Implicit memory for figure assignment in shape perception

The edge complex: Implicit memory for figure assignment in shape perception Perception & Psychophysics 2005, 67 (4), 727-740 The edge complex: Implicit memory for figure assignment in shape perception MARY A. PETERSON University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona and JAMES T. ENNS University

More information

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON Journal of Experimental Psychology 1971, Vol. 89, No. 1, 147-151 JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION DOMINIC W. MASSARO» University of Wisconsin AND NORMAN H. ANDERSON University of California,

More information

Additivity and Interaction Between Size Ratio and Response Category in the Comparison of Size-Discrepant Shapes

Additivity and Interaction Between Size Ratio and Response Category in the Comparison of Size-Discrepant Shapes .lottrnal of Experimental Psychology:. ~ t 1987 by the American Ps3~hological A,~acialioo, Inc. Human Peftml~on and Performance 1987, Vol. 13, No. 3, 478-487 0096-1523/87/$00.75 Additivity and Interaction

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Finding a new target in an old display: Evidence for a memory recency effect in visual search

Finding a new target in an old display: Evidence for a memory recency effect in visual search Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 846-851 Finding a new target in an old display: Evidence for a memory recency effect in visual search CHRISTOF KÖRNER University of Graz, Graz, Austria AND IAIN

More information

Grouping does not require attention

Grouping does not require attention Perception & Psychophysics 2006, 68 (1), 17-31 Grouping does not require attention DOMINIQUE LAMY, HANNAH SEGAL, and LITAL RUDERMAN Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel Many theories of visual perception

More information

Flexibility of attention between stimulus dimensions

Flexibility of attention between stimulus dimensions Perception & Psychophysics 1981,30 (3), 291-302 Flexibility of attention between stimulus dimensions GLYN W. HUMPHREYS BirkbeckCollege, University oflondon, London WCIE 7HX, England Three experiments examined

More information

The impact of item clustering on visual search: It all depends on the nature of the visual search

The impact of item clustering on visual search: It all depends on the nature of the visual search Journal of Vision (2010) 10(14):24, 1 9 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/14/24 1 The impact of item clustering on visual search: It all depends on the nature of the visual search Yaoda Xu Department

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination Timothy N. Rubin (trubin@uci.edu) Michael D. Lee (mdlee@uci.edu) Charles F. Chubb (cchubb@uci.edu) Department of Cognitive

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 916-920 Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion JOHN PREDEBON University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia Two experiments

More information

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Memory & Cognition 1974, Vol. 2, No.3, 533 538 Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Ss were required to learn linear

More information

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments Perception, 2010, volume 39, pages 514 ^ 532 doi:10.1068/p6501 The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments Alexander Todorov, Valerie Loehr, Nikolaas N Oosterhof Department

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

ing the fixed-interval schedule-were observed during the interval of delay. Similarly, Ferster

ing the fixed-interval schedule-were observed during the interval of delay. Similarly, Ferster JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAIOR 1969, 12, 375-383 NUMBER 3 (MAY) DELA YED REINFORCEMENT ERSUS REINFORCEMENT AFTER A FIXED INTERAL' ALLEN J. NEURINGER FOUNDATION FOR RESEARCH ON THE NEROUS

More information

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* Perception & Psychophysics 1973. Vol. 13. So. 2.356-360 Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* ANGEL VASSILEV+ Institu te of Physiology. Bulgarian Academy of Sciences.

More information

Stroop Dilution Depends on the Nature of the Color Carrier but Not on Its Location

Stroop Dilution Depends on the Nature of the Color Carrier but Not on Its Location Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 4, 826 839 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.826

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

On perfect working-memory performance with large numbers of items

On perfect working-memory performance with large numbers of items Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:958 963 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0108-7 BRIEF REPORT On perfect working-memory performance with large numbers of items Jonathan E. Thiele & Michael S. Pratte & Jeffrey N. Rouder

More information

Voluntary Task Switching: Chasing the Elusive Homunculus

Voluntary Task Switching: Chasing the Elusive Homunculus Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2005, Vol. 31, No. 4, 683 702 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.4.683

More information

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (3), 504-513 Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks Christian Frings Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany and Peter Wühr Friedrich

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

Inhibition of return with rapid serial shifts of attention: Implications for memory and visual search

Inhibition of return with rapid serial shifts of attention: Implications for memory and visual search Perception & Psychophysics 2003, 65 (7), 1126-1135 Inhibition of return with rapid serial shifts of attention: Implications for memory and visual search MICHAEL D. DODD, ALAN D. CASTEL, and JAY PRATT University

More information

Finding Memory in Search: The Effect of Visual Working Memory Load on Visual Search. 1 Department of Psychology, University of Toronto

Finding Memory in Search: The Effect of Visual Working Memory Load on Visual Search. 1 Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Finding Memory 1 Running head: FINDING MEMORY IN SEARCH This is a preprint of an article submitted for consideration in the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 2010 Experimental Psychology Society;

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get IAT 355 Perception 1 Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get Why we need to understand perception The ability of viewers to interpret visual (graphical) encodings of information and

More information

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object Supplemental materials for: The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object David Whitney*, David A. Westwood, & Melvyn A. Goodale* *Group on Action and Perception, The

More information

The Ebbinghaus illusion modulates visual search for size-defined targets: Evidence for preattentive processing of apparent object size

The Ebbinghaus illusion modulates visual search for size-defined targets: Evidence for preattentive processing of apparent object size Perception & Psychophysics 2004, 66 (3), 475-495 The Ebbinghaus illusion modulates visual search for size-defined targets: Evidence for preattentive processing of apparent object size ASTRID BUSCH and

More information

Convergence Principles: Information in the Answer

Convergence Principles: Information in the Answer Convergence Principles: Information in the Answer Sets of Some Multiple-Choice Intelligence Tests A. P. White and J. E. Zammarelli University of Durham It is hypothesized that some common multiplechoice

More information

Effects of similarity and practice on speeded classification response times and accuracies: Further tests of an exemplar-retrieval model

Effects of similarity and practice on speeded classification response times and accuracies: Further tests of an exemplar-retrieval model Memory & Cognition 1999, 27 (1), 78-93 Effects of similarity and practice on speeded classification response times and accuracies: Further tests of an exemplar-retrieval model ROBERT M. NOSOFSKY and LEOLA

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 3, 580 584 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027885 OBSERVATION Templates

More information

Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty

Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty Perception & Psychophysics 2008, 70 (8), 1435-1443 doi: 10.3758/PP.70.8.1435 Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty ZHE CHEN University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences 2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences Stanislas Dehaene Chair of Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture n 5 Bayesian Decision-Making Lecture material translated

More information

Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z

Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 10, 522-527 (1971) Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z JAMES F. JUOLA AND R. C. ATKINSON Stanford University, Stanford, California 94305 Two groups

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

Discrete Resource Allocation in Visual Working Memory

Discrete Resource Allocation in Visual Working Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 5, 1359 1367 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015792 Discrete Resource

More information

Attentional Capture in Singleton-Detection and Feature-Search Modes

Attentional Capture in Singleton-Detection and Feature-Search Modes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2003, Vol. 29, No. 5, 1003 1020 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1003

More information

Bottom-Up Guidance in Visual Search for Conjunctions

Bottom-Up Guidance in Visual Search for Conjunctions Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2007, Vol. 33, No. 1, 48 56 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.1.48

More information

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Laboratory Equipment & Test Procedures. Motor learning and control historically has built its discipline through laboratory research. This has led to the

More information

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect VISUAL COGNITION, 2007, 15 (2), 133148 Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect Jay Pratt University of Toronto,

More information

Dynamics of Color Category Formation and Boundaries

Dynamics of Color Category Formation and Boundaries Dynamics of Color Category Formation and Boundaries Stephanie Huette* Department of Psychology, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN Definition Dynamics of color boundaries is broadly the area that characterizes

More information

Sawtooth Software. The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? RESEARCH PAPER SERIES

Sawtooth Software. The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? RESEARCH PAPER SERIES Sawtooth Software RESEARCH PAPER SERIES The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? Dick Wittink, Yale University Joel Huber, Duke University Peter Zandan,

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

Visual Working Memory Represents a Fixed Number of Items Regardless of Complexity Edward Awh, Brian Barton, and Edward K. Vogel

Visual Working Memory Represents a Fixed Number of Items Regardless of Complexity Edward Awh, Brian Barton, and Edward K. Vogel PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Visual Working Memory Represents a Fixed Number of Items Regardless of Complexity University of Oregon ABSTRACT Does visual working memory represent a fixed number

More information

Visual Transformation of Size

Visual Transformation of Size Journal ol Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1975, Vol. 1, No. 3, 214-220 Visual Transformation of Size Glaus Bundesen and Axel Larsen Copenhagen University, Denmark To investigate

More information

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (7), 1236-1247 doi: 10.3758/MC/36.7.1236 An action sequence held in memory can interfere with response selection of a target stimulus, but does not interfere with response activation

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

On the generality of optimal versus objective classifier feedback effects on decision criterion learning in perceptual categorization

On the generality of optimal versus objective classifier feedback effects on decision criterion learning in perceptual categorization Memory & Cognition 2003, 31 (2), 181-198 On the generality of optimal versus objective classifier feedback effects on decision criterion learning in perceptual categorization COREY J. BOHIL and W. TODD

More information

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH Laurence D. Smith University of Maine ldsmith@maine.maine.edu Ronald M. Pickett Marjan Trutschl Institute for Visualization and Perception

More information

Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection?

Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection? Learning and Motivation 33, 10 31 (2002) doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1097, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection? Lorraine G. Allan McMaster University,

More information

How Far Away Is That? It Depends on You: Perception Accounts for the Abilities of Others

How Far Away Is That? It Depends on You: Perception Accounts for the Abilities of Others Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2015, Vol. 41, No. 3, 000 2015 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000070 OBSERVATION

More information

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The number line effect reflects top-down control Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),862-868???-??? The number line effect reflects top-down control JELENA RISTIC University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 284 (2015) 167 178 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report How coordinate and categorical

More information

7 Grip aperture and target shape

7 Grip aperture and target shape 7 Grip aperture and target shape Based on: Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. The influence of target object shape on maximum grip aperture in human grasping movements. Exp Brain Res, In revision 103 Introduction

More information

LOG- LINEAR ANALYSIS OF FERTILITY USING CENSUS AND SURVEY DATA WITH AN EXAMPLE

LOG- LINEAR ANALYSIS OF FERTILITY USING CENSUS AND SURVEY DATA WITH AN EXAMPLE LOG- LIEAR AALYSIS OF FERTILITY USIG CESUS AD SURVEY DATA WITH A EXAMPLE I. Elaine Allen and Roger C. Avery, Cornell University The use of log -linear models is relatively ne to the field of demography,

More information

pain" counted as "two," and so forth. The sum of point, the hour point, and so forth. These data are

pain counted as two, and so forth. The sum of point, the hour point, and so forth. These data are FURTHER STUDIES ON THE "PHARMACOLOGY" OF PLACEBO ADMINISTRATION 1 BY LOUIS LASAGNA, VICTOR G. LATIES, AND J. LAWRENCE DOHAN (From the Department of Medicine (Division of Clinical Pharmacology), and the

More information

Attentional Window and Global/Local Processing

Attentional Window and Global/Local Processing University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 6-16-2016 Attentional Window and Global/Local Processing Steven Peter Schultz University of South Florida,

More information

Searching through subsets:

Searching through subsets: Searching through subsets: A test of the Visual Indexing Hypothesis Burkell, Jacquelyn A. and Pylyshyn, Zenon W. University of Western Ontario and Rutgers University [1] Short Title: Searching through

More information

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society Title Implicit learning: A demonstration and a revision to a novel SRT paradigm Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/5np49798

More information

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS BRUC W. HAMILL VISUAL PRCPTION OF STRUCTURD SYMBOLS A set of psychological experiments was conducted to explore the effects of stimulus structure on visual search processes. Results of the experiments,

More information

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion Perception & Psychophysics 1980.28 (5). 465-470 Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion GREGORY M. CORSO Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia 30332 A pair of pure-tone sine

More information

Enhanced discrimination in autism

Enhanced discrimination in autism THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2001, 54A (4), 961 979 Enhanced discrimination in autism Michelle O Riordan and Kate Plaisted University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Children with autism

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information