GENERALIZATION OF A CONDITIONED INHIBITOR OF RESPONSE SUPPRESSION IN RATS

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "GENERALIZATION OF A CONDITIONED INHIBITOR OF RESPONSE SUPPRESSION IN RATS"

Transcription

1 ACTA NEUROBOL. EXP. 1988, 46: GENERALZATON OF A CONDTONED NHBTOR OF RESPONSE SUPPRESSON N RATS Malgorzata WKSERSKA, James F. BRENNAN and Kazimierz ZELNSK Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki nstitute of Experimental Biology Pasteura 3, Warsaw, Poland Key words: stimulus control, conditioned inhibitor, conditioned suppression, *generalization, rat Abstract. Following acquisition of conditioned suppression of barpresses to a visual conditiondd stimulus (CS), rats were subjected to a particular variety of differentiation training procedures. An auditory conditioned inhibitor (C), which preceded and accompanied the CS on some trials, signaled that inescapable shock will not be given on that trial. When the C acquired the capacity to attenuate markedly the suppressive effects of the CS presented within the C + CS compound, the frequency dimension of the C was varied during generalization tests. Reliable within-subjects gradients of barpress enhancement were obtained when tone test frequencies were presented alone. Frequency gradients obtained from C variation in compound with the CS were rather flat anld reflected some residual suppression of the on-going barpressing. NTRODUCTON Stimulus control of behavior may be achieved and demonstrated in different ways. One of the most persuasive is the procedure by which one discriminative stimulus, termed the conditioned inhibitor (C), acquires the capacity to inhibit the excitatory properties of another discriminative stimulus, termed the conditioned stimulus (CS), otherwise paired with a biologically important event (the unconditioned stimulus, US). The acquisition of conditioned inhibitory properties is due to differentiation

2 training (28) in which two kinds of trials are presented. The CS is a common element of 'both types of trials. On some trials, the CS is paired with the US (e.g., inescapable shock) becoming a signal for the presentation of this biologically important event, and this acquired property is demonstrated by elicitation of a specific conditioned response (CR). On other trials, the same CS is preceded andlor accompanied by the C and never paired with the US. After some training, the CS partially or completely loses its ability to evoke the CR when presented with the backgmwd of the C, but retains this evocation property when presented without the C. Thus, a C exerts stimulus control of behavior by indirectly modifying the signaling value of the CS. Such a mechanism of conditioned inhibitory action, cmsi~sting of the acute cancellation of the signaling properties of the CS otherwise retained, was proposed by Pavlov (19, p. 87) and shared by his students (18, p ). This belief had consequences for designing experiments in which conditioned inhibitory properties were tested. No attention has been paid to direct effects from a C on behaviors not elicited by the CS. The method of conditioned inhibition of conditioned suppression (24-26) provides an opportunity to study the direct effects exerted by a C on behavior. First, the typical Estes-Skinner paradigm for training the conditioned emotional response (CER) was used (7). Rats initially learned to produce stable barpressing rate for food were then exposed to a CS signaling a brief inescapable shock. After several CS-US pairings, the CS acquired the property to suppress the on-going barpressing. Then another kind of trial was introduced in which the same CS was preceded and accompanied by a discriminative stimulus of another modality, the C, and never paired with shock. After brief training the CS lost most of its suppressive properties when presented in compound with the C and suppressed the barpressing when presented without C. The well trained C presented either alone or in compound with the CS accelerated the banpressing rate (24). This enhancing effect of the C on the on-going barpressing behavior was independent of the emotional response elicited by the CS, since neither extinction nor reinstatement of the suppressive properties of the CS changed the behavioral effects of the C (25, 26). Taking these results into account, the capacity of the C to accelerate the barpressing rate may be used as an index for studying the direct effects of a C on behavior. Moreover, the time parameters used in our studies allowed independent measures of the effects of the G presented alone or in mip pound with the CS. The aim of the present study ms to investigate generalization functions by changing one of the dimensions of the stimulus used as the C. Since the C is of another modality than the CS that has inhibited exci-

3 tatory properties, the shape of the generalization functions of the C presented alone should not be erased by the excitatory tendencies generalized from the CS. n this respect the shape of the generalization function of the C presented in compound with the CS is also of interest. MATERAL AND METHODS The experiment was conducted on 16 experimentally naive, male hooded rats, approximately 3 months old at the beginning of training. Since one subjects data were lost accidentally at the final stage of experiment, anly 15 rats completed the generalization tests. Training was conducted in four identical sound insulated operant chambers, each having an electrifiable grid floor, a single bar on one of the walls and a fornod-tray under it. A pilot light centered on the top of the back chamber wall provided illumination in the vicinity of the bar equal to 205 $. 5 lx. Tonal frequencies were generated to identical permanent magnet speakers located below the grid floor of each chamber positioned 'on the same wall as the bar and the food-tray. Equipment for automatic programming and recording of the experiment was located in an adjoining room. Experimental sessions were conducted daily at the same time for each subject. Prior to any training the rats were reduced to of their ad lib. body weight anld maintained at that weight on a 24 h feeding rhythm during all stages of the expeliiment. TW first day of barpress training consisted of initial presentations of 40 "free" 45 mg food pellets on a 1-min V schedule followed immediately by a period with a continuous reinforcement schedule until 120 food pellets were delivered in a single session. After this training, all consecutive sessions lasted 2 h, and the barpresses were reinforced according to 2.5-min V food reinforcement schedule both during and between trials. The sixth and seventh days of instrumental barpressing training constituted two Pretest days (P-days), during which an unconditioned effect of the to-be-cs, a 3 min period of darkness provided by offset of the pilot light, was tested. Four 3 min periods of darkness, starting at 14, 24, 49, and 72 min after the beginning of the session, were presented during each P-day. Training of the CER followed in six successive sessions, when each 3-min presentation of the CS terminated with 1 s of scrambled electric foot shock of 2-mA intensity. Onsets of the CS were at the same time as during P-days. During six subsequent C training sessions, constituting the next stage of experiment, the number of CS presentations was increased to eight with additional CS onsets at 38, 56, 79, and 101 min after

4 the beginning of the'2-h session. Four presentations of the CS-terminated with a 1, s, 2-mA shock as in the CER acquisition stage (CS+ trials), whereas four others were preceded by onset of a 3,000 Hz tone of 75 db (re: 20 FN/m2) intensity one min before the CS onset and coterminated without shock (C 4- CS trials), The order of CS+ anid C 4- CS trials was changed each day in predetermined arrangements (see 14) in which the same kind of trials was never given more than twice in a row. The last stage of the experiment, also lasting six sessions, involved testing of the generalization of conditioned inhibition. Essentially, a test sampling procedure was used wherein five frequency generalization values were varied on C + CS trials within the context of five regular CSf trials. That is, each session of this stage consisted of five CSf trials (3 min darkness, terminating with 1 s of foot shock) and five C + CS trials (4 min tone, together with the last 3 min of darkness). On each C + CS trial different tonal frequencies were used: 1,000, 2,000, 3,000 (the original frequency), 4,000, and 5,000 Hz. Each tone was of the same intensity as that used in original C training. Additional trial onsets were at 93 and 115 min after the beginning of the sessions. The order of CS+ and C + CS trials was changed each day and presented according to Gellermans series (9) in which the same kind of trial was never given more than twice in a row. The order of presentation of tonal frequencies in C + CS trials was also changed each day and partially contrabalanced over consecutive days and subgroups of 4 subjects each. During all experimental sessions the numbers of barpresses emitted in consecutive 30 s periods were collected and served as the main measure of behavior. The magnitude of changes in barpressing rate during the CS+ trials was measured by computing the "suppression ratio", BL(A + B), where B represents the number of barpresses emitted during the 3 min action of the CS and A is the number of responses during the 3min period immediately before the CS onset. For C + CS trials two ratios were computed, and in both cases A was the number of responses during the 3-min period immediately before the C onset. The effect of the tone alone, the C, was measured by the ratio 3B,/(A + 3B1), where B, represents the number of barpresses emitted during the one minute from tone onset to the darkness, the CS onset. The joint action ofl the C + CS was measured by the B/(A + B) ratio, similar to CS+ trial measure. Ratios were computed independently for each subject anld trial. Additionally, daily ratios were calculated by summing the numbers of responses emitted during corresponding periods of time for all CS+ or C + CS trials. The measures used in CER studies are not always normally distribu-

5 ted. Thus nonparametric tests were always used parallel to ANOVA tests.. For the same reason, medians were preferred over means for the illustration of the group data. RESULTS Pretest days. The initial presentations of the house light offset interfered with barpressing and decreased the rate of the on-going behavior. This effect was statistically significant during the first and the second presentations of darkness (P < 0.05, Wilcoxon matched-pairs, two-tailed test). Thus, two P-days were employed in this experiment to allow full habituation of the to-be-cs. Data presented in the Table indicate that during the 2nd P-day, changes in the barpress rate during light offset periods were unsystematic. Median differences in barpressing rate between the 3-min period before and 3-min period during the to-be-cs (darkness) action on the Pretest Days (A-B index) Consecutive vresentations CER acquisition. As shown in the CER training portion of Fig. 1, acquisition of conditioned suppression was rapid. Comparisons of the numbers of barpresses emitted in A and B periods independently showed that initially, during trials of the first day of CER training, the CS enhanced the barpressing rate, although this effect was statistically nonsignificant. The CS evoked a significant decrease of barpressing after four pairings with shock (i.e., the 1st trial of the 2nd CER training day, P < 0.05, Wilcoxon test) and this suppression was observed on all subsequent trials of the 2nd trough the 6th days of training (P's < 0.01 in all comparisons). Comparisons of daily scores for A and B periods showed that during the 1st CER training session, the CS enhanced (P < 0.05, Wilcoxon test) and on subsequent, 2nd-6th CER training sessions, decreased the barpress rate (P's < 0.01, the same tests). An analysis of variance confirmed these results showing a significant day effect on daily suppression ratios

6 C 0.- v , 1 l l 1 Sessions P 1 CER -training C - training Fig. 1. Median daily suppression ratios during P-days, CER acquisition and C acquisition. Filled circles denote the light offset CS presented with the shock US, while open circles denote the same stimulus presented without the shock US, and triangles indicate the 3,000 Hz tone. The effects of 1 min C action alone (triangles) were measured by the ratio, 3Bl/(A + 3B1), and the effects of 3 min CS+ (circles) and C+ CS (circles with triangle) presentations were measured by the ratio, B/(h + B). Crasses below the marks for daily ratios denote suppression and crosses abo:re the marks denfote enhancement significant at P's < 0.05 or better, as estimated by the A-B comparisons (Wilcoxon tests). (FEv5 = 25.11, P < 0.001). Further, Duncan tests showed differences between the first anld all others days of CER training (P < 0.001), between the second anld the subsequent days (P < for the 3rd and 4th days, P < 0.01 for the 5th day, and P < 0.05 for the 6th day) and no systematic differences between the third and subsequent days. Thus, the CER was acquired during the first two days of training. C acquisition. As seen from Fig. 2 the first presentation of the 3,000 Hz tone decreased the rate of the on-going barpressing behavior. This effect was significant as shown by the comparison of the number of barpresses emitted before and during the 1 min action of the C (P < 0.01, Wilcoxon test). n next trials the C rather increased the barpressing rate and such increase was significant in four trials (on the 2nd, 3rd and 6th days of the C training, P < 0.05, the same test). As seen from Fig. 2. at the beginning of training the suppressing effects of and of the C f CS compound were similar in magnitude. The A-B comparisons showed that during the first day.of C training each presentation of the C + CS compound resulted in significant suppression of barpressing similarly as observed during presentation of the CS alone (P < 0.01 in each comparison). Similar comparisons for the 2nd-6th days of C training indicate that the supressing effect of the CS was maintained (all but one A-B comparisons showed differences significant at

7 P < 0.05 or better), whereas the suppressive effect of the C +CS compound was less systematic. However, in this experiment the C + CS evoked significant suppression of barpressing on some trials even toward the end of training (on the 2nd day, three trials with significant suppression; 3rd day, one trial; '4th day, one trial; 5th day, two trials; and the 6th day, one trial). The slow extinction of the suppressive properties of the C + CS compound observed in this experiment was presumably related to thec modalities of stimuli used as CS and C, which will be discussed later. Fig. 2. Median numbers of barpress responses emitted in consecutive 30 s periods before, during, and after each presentation of the CS+ and the C+ CS during the first day of C acquisition. Numbers in the circles denote consecutive trials within the session. The introduction of the C + CS trials resulted in the attenuation of the suppressive effect of the CS+ trials clearly observed in Fig. 1. The differences between the daily suppression ratios estimated for the CS+ 2 - Aeta Neurobiologiae Exp. 1/86

8 and C + CS appeared on the 3rd day of the C training and were also observed on the 4th and 5th days of training (Table 11). The course of inhibitory training was additionally tested using a twoway analysis of variance of the daily suppression ratios. This analysis revealed significant main effects from stimuli used (F,,,, = 28.57, P < 0.001), and days of training (F5,75 = 5.95, P < 0.001) but showed Median daily suppression ratios during the CtCS training stage of the experiment calculated for the CS+, C, and CSCS periods independently CS+ C c+cs CS+ vs CtCS comparisons (P<) ( Days of C+CS training - 1st 2nd l d l? h l 5th 6th *** *** 0:394*** *** 0.402'"~ 0.470' NS NS 0.01 no interaction. Further, Duncan test showed that the greatest suppressive effect of the C was observed on the first day and it differed from all consecutive days (P's < 0.01 or better). Similarly, h e suppressive effect of the C +CS was the greatest during the first day and differed from all other days (P's < 0.001). Daily suppression ratios for the CSf trials were also lowest during the first C training day and differed from others days (P's < 0.05 or better) except for the fifth day of training. Generalization tests. This stage of the experiment was in some respect a continuation of the C training stage, since the CS+ trials (darkness with shock) were presented as before and only the frequency of the tone used as the C was changed each trial. This change in the procedure had no effect on the daily suppression ratios on the CS trials as shown by an ANOVA test in which CSf daily suppression ratios from all sessions of C acquisition and generalization test stages were compared. Similarly no changes between the daily A scores between these two stages of the experiment were observed. As seen from Fig. 3 the typical drop in the on-going barpressing rate at the beginning of the CER training followed by gradual recovery was observed in this experiment. An ANOVA of the mean rate of barpresses in A scores for all of the sessions showed a significant effect of successive experimental sessions (F11/238 = 8.35, P < 0.001). However, there were only small fluctuations of the "baseline" during the Cl training and the generalization test stages of the ex-

9 periment. Duncan tests showed significant differences between scores involving the CER training sessions, however, they are of no importance here. Nevertheless, the same tests indicated that only during the second day of generalization testing was the baseline responding lower than during the C training sessions and some of the other generalization testing sessions (P's < 0.01). The stability of performance during the generalization tests was additionally supported by an ANOVA of the suppression ratios for each CSf trial during the last stage of experiment. No effects from testing days, trials, or their interaction were obtained. Thus, it may be concluded that the on-going barpressing rate was rather stable over days in which generalization tests were conducted. Similarly, the suppression evoked by CS+ did not change significantly beween trials or days. 1 D P CER -training! C -training Generalization tests Fig. 3. Median numbers of barpress responses per min emitted in pre-cs periods during all consecutive stages of the experiment. The generalization functions may be seen in Fig. 4, which indicated that all C and C + CS values, independent of tonal frequency variation, were above the suppression ratio from the reference point, the third CS+ trial of the test sessions ). Similar to previous sessions, the C presented alone tended to enhance the barpressing rate, whereas the C presented in compound with CS only attenuated suppressin~g properties of The level of responding to the CS+ during the generalization tests was characterize'd by only one trial, the third trial within a given session. The seasonn for this strategy was an attempt to characterize performance during the generalization test sessions by figures based on the same number of raw data an4 having similar variability.

10 the CS. Of the two measures, generalization of the C value alone tended to be sharper than generalization of the C + CS compound. The overall analysis of the generalization of the C value based on "suppression" ratios indicated effects from tonal frequency variation (F4/,, = 7.36, P<0.001) and the interaction between frequency change and testing days (F = = 2.32, P < 0.001), but the main effect of days was not significant. Duncan comparisons revealed that response enhancement at the original C training value of 3,000 Hz was significantly different from all other testing frequencies except for the 5,000 Hz value (all P's < 0.01). The lowest suppression ratios were observed for the 4,000 Hz value resulting in distortion of the graldient. A similar overall analysis of the generalization of the C + CS value showed no significant effects of tonal frequencies, successive testing days, or their interaction x 1 0 ~ HZ Fig. 4. Generalization gradients based on suppression ratios estimated for the 1 min periods of the C presented alone (triangles) and for the 3 min periods of C given in compound with the CS (open circles with triangles). The mean suppression ratio for the thiad CSf trial of each testing session (filled circle) is given as a reference point. Each point in the Figure is a mean from six medians, thus representing data collected on one particular trial of a given session from 15 subjects. To explore further the change in generalization of the C across sessions, indicated by the interaction of sessions X test frequency in the overall analysis, separate analyses were conducted for each testing session and are summarized in Table 111. The a posteriori comparisons of the specific analyses supported the results of the overall analysis. n

11 Results of analyses of each generalization testing session for the variation of the C and C+CS values C generalization C+ CS generalization session PC P< three sessions significant effects of C frequency variation were obtained. The most symmetrical gradient was observed in Session 2 with the 3,000 Hz value similar to 2,000 Hz and 4,000 Hz values and different from 1,000 Hz and 5,000 Hz values (P < 0.05, Duncan test). On session 4 responding to the 4,000 Hz value was the lowest and differed from response levels of all other test stimuli (P < 0.005). On Session 6 responding to the 3,000 Hz value differed only from that to 4,000 Hz and 5,000 Hz (P < 0.05). Similar separate analyses were conjducted for the C + CS data and also presented in Table 111. As seen, in two sessions significant effects of C frequency variation on the C + CS data were obtained. On session 4 responding to 4,000 Hz C given in compound with CS was the highest and differed from responding to all other frequencies (P's < 0.05). On session 6 the highest level of responding was observed to thq 2,000 Hz frequency, but it differed.only from the 5,000 Hz frequency evoking the lowest level of barpressing (P < 0.01). DSCUSSON The major result of this study was the finding of reliable, withinsubjects gradients of barpressing enhancement as a result of variation in tonal frequency used as the conditioned inhibitor of conditioned barpress suppression. The effects of the C were tested in two situations: when presented alone during one min periods and -during the next three min intervals when presented in compound with the CS. The behavioral effects of the C in the two situations were different. The C presented alone was easily discriminated from the CS and after a few presentations ceased to suppress on-going banpressing. On the contrary, more than six days of training were necessary for the C to acquire the property of in-

12 hibiting the suppressive effects of the CS (24). Thus, at early stages of inhibitory training, the suppression ratios estimated for the C were close to the 0.5 value reflecting no change in on-going barpressing, whereas the suppression ratios for the C + CS compound were lower than 0.5 value denoting suppression of the on-going barpressing rate. With prolonged training, both the C and C $ CS acquired a tendency to enhance barpressmg rate (24-26). The present experiment showed that the enhancement effect was more readily acquired by the C presented alone than given in compound with the CS. The obtained generalization gradient for the C indicated that the strongest enhancing effect was evoked by the tonal frequency used as the C in the original training. Among all frequencies tested only the 4,000 Hz tone suppressed the on-going barpressing. The finding of suppression to the 4,000 Hz value, in conjunction with response enhancement to the 5,000 Hz value may be considered as an evidence of a contrast effect in the discrimination of the test values (22). This contrast effect in an aversively motivated context is similar to results reported by Hoffman (12) in a study of conditioned suppression of key pecking in pigeons and by Brennan and Riccio (3) in discriminative avoidance behavior in rats. The generalization gradient for the C + CS was rather flat and the overall analysis did not show a significant effect from variation of tonal frequency. Separate analyses did reveal an effect on two test sessions, although in neither case was the 3,000 Hz the preferred frequency. Broad generalization for the C + CS should be expected, since this stimulus situation had a mixed character, in that one element of the inhibitory compound was the excitatory CS. Earlier experiments on stimulus generalization of conditioned suppression, in which some parameters of the CS paired with inescapable shock were changed, yielded broad generalization gradients (4, 8, 10-13, 20, 29). These gradients sharpened with prolonged testing in extinction, when values for away from the CS+ eventually began to enhance the on-going barpressing. However, in the case of generalization around a C + CS oompound, an opposite process may be expected. A sharpening of the C + CS gradient would be related rather to prolonged training, when the properties of the C gradually became dominant within the C + CS compound. Significant effects of tonal frequencies on the C 4- CS generalization functions, which were \ observed only during the final testing sessions, support this supposition. The findings of the present experiment are related to the traditional discussion concerning the nature of conditioned inhibition. Pavlov distinguished two basic situations in which a CS ceased to evoke an acquired CR. One, occurring upon systematic presentations of the CS without

13 pairing with the US, produces the process of conditioned inhibition that overcomes the previously acquired conditioned excitation. The second, occuring when concurently with the CS another stimulus is presented evoking such strong excitation that the organism engages in another kind of activity. The first mechanism was termed internal inhibition, and the second - external inhibition. nternal inhibition, a synonymous with conditioned inhibition, reflected a notion that inhibition developed within the reflex arc of the CR under consideration. The term external inhibition denotes that lack of the CR or its diminution was not due to some changes in the reflex arc of this particular response, but rather related to influences from reflex arc of another response (19). This distincticon between the two mechanisms is not only of historical importance. n modern interpretations, this distinction was reflected in the two opposite definitions of conditioned inhibitory stimuli. Jenkins defined a conditioned inhibitory stimulus as "a signal for not responding" (15, p. 35), whereas Rescorla considered it as a stimulus controlling "a tendency opposite to that of the conditioned excitor" (21, p. 78). Properties of the conditioned inhibitor of conditioned suppression fit much better with Rescorla's than with Jenkin's definition. nhibition of conditioned suppression lealds to the enhancement of the on-going barpressing behavior. A previously published review (24) of experimental data showed that not only training of the conditioned inhibitor but also bther Pavlovian procedures, such as experimental extinction, differentiation learning and inhibition of delay training, produce stimuli that inhibit conditioned suppression and evoke enhancement of the on-going behavior. Thus, it may be concluded that in situations in which an organism is under the influence of different motivations to which specific response patterns are linked, inhibition of one response pattern elicits the opposite response. n consequence an important question may be asked, whether stimuli acquiring inhibitory properties for one response pattern at the same time acquire exeit'atory properties for the apposite response. There is no firm answer to this question (5). Stimuli acquire inhibitory properties in the course of leaning involving gradual elimination of other response tendencies. According to Terrace only learning with errors produce inhibitory stimuli; and "after discrimination is learned without errors, S- appears to function as a neutral stimulus" (23, p. 1678). The present experiment showed that the C presented alone acquired inhibitory proper'ties very easily, without, or with very few, "errors". Moreover, reliable generalization gradient of enhancement elicited by the C indicate that the conditioned inhibitor cannot be considered as a "neutral stimulus" but exerted specific control over conditioned enhancement - the behavior opposite to the suppres-

14 sion. Large amount of experimental data supported Terrace's state- ment. Thus, it has to be concluded that inhibitory properties of the C were acquired due to numerous "errors" commited when the C was given in compound with the CS. The proportion of trials in which the C 4- CS compound evokes signlficant suppression of the on-going behavior may be considered as an index of the amount of generalization from the CS. Comparison of the data from several experiments in which white -noise was used as the CS and darkness as the C (24, 25) showed that the more intense the CS the greater the proportion of C + CS presentations that produce significant suppression of barpressing during,the six days of inhibitory training: 42'O/o with an 50 db CS, 55O/o with an 70 db CS, and 75Q/d with an 80 db CS. n the present experiment the C + CS compound produced significant suppression in 50 /o of the trials. This showed that darkness may be considered as a weak CS for conditioned suppression, which is in agreement with other CER studies in which illumination of different intensities or flashing lights of differeht colours were used as CSi signaling inescapable shock (1, 2, 6, 16, 17, 27). This investigation was supported by Project of the Polish Academy of Science. Partkirpatim in this experimenit by the second author was made possible by a fellowship grant from the USA National Academy of Sciences. REFERENCES 1. BLACKMAN, D. E. and SCRUTON, P Conditioned suppression and discriminative control of behavior. Anim. Learn. Behav. 1: BRAHLEK, J. A Conditioned suppression as a function of the number of stimuli that precede shock. Psychoa. Sci. 12: BRENNAN, J. F. and RCCO, D. C Sitimulus control of avoidance behavior in rahs following differential or nondifferential Pavlovian training along dimensions of the conditioned stimulus. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 85: BRENNAN, J. F. and RCCO, D. C Stimulus generalization of suppression in rats following aversively motivated instrumental or Pavlovian training. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 88: DCKNSON, A Appetitive-aversion interactions: Facilitation of aversive conditioning by prior appetitive training in the rat. Anim. Learn. Behav. 4: DONEGAN, N. H., WHTLOW, J. W. Jr. and WAGNER, A. R Posttrial reinstatement of the CS in Pavlovian conditionilng: Facilitation or impairment of acquisition as a funection of individual differences in responsiveness to the CS. J. Exp. Psychol.: Anim. Behav. Proc. 3: ESTES, W. K. and SKNNER,'B. F Some quantitative properties of anxiety. J. Exp. Psychol. 29:

15 8. FLESHLER, M. and HOFFMAN, H. S Stimulus generalization of conditioned suppression. Science 133: ,GELLERMAN, L. N Chance orders of alternating stimuli in visual discrimination experiments. J. Gen. Psychol. 42: HOFFMAN, H. S The stimulus generalization of conditioned suppression. n D.. Mostofsky (ed.), Stimulus generalization. Stanford Univ. Press, Stanford, p HOFFMAN, H. S Stimulus generalizatian versus discrimination failure in conditioned suppression. n R. M. Gilbert and N. S. Sutherland (ed.), Animal discrimination learning. Academic Press, London, p HOFFMAN, H. S Stimulus factors in conditioned suppression. n B. A. Campbelt and R. 'M. Church (ed.), Punishment and aversive behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, p HOFFMAN, H. S. and FLESHLER, M Stimulus factors in aversive control: the generalization of conditioned suppression. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 4: JAKUBOWSKA, E. and ZELNSK, K Differentiation learning as a function of stimulus intensity and previous experience with the CS+. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 36: JENKNS, H. M Generalization gradients and the concept of inhibition. n D.. Mostofsky (ed.), Stimulus generalization. Stanford Univ. Press, Stanford, p KAMN, L. J Temporal and intensity characteristitcs of the conditioned stimulus, n W. F. Prokasy (ed.), Classical conditioning. Appleton-Century- Crofts, New Pork, p KAMN, L. J "Attention-like" processes in classical conditioning. n M. R. Janes (ed.). Miami symposium on the prediction of behavior. Univ. Miami Press, Coral Gables, p KONORSK, J Conditioned reflexes and neuron organization. Univ. Cambridge Press, Cambridge, 267 p. 19. PAVLOV,. P Conditiloned reflexes, an investigation of the higher nervous activity of the cerebral cortex (Transl. by G. V. Anrep) OxfordUniv. Press, Oxford, 430 p. 20. RAY, 0. S. and STEN, L Generalization of conditioned suppression. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2: RESCORLA, R. A Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychol. Bull. 72: : SPENCE, K. W The differential response in animals to stimuli varying within a single dimension. Pspchol. Rev. 44: TERRACE, H. S Discrimination learning and inhibition. Science 154: WgSERSKA, M. and ZELNSK, K Enhancement of bar-pressing rate in rats by the conditioned inhibitor of the CER. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 40: WqSERSKA, M. and ZELRSK, K Extinction, reinstatement and spontaneous recovery of conditioned suppression in rats and the stability of conditioned inhibition performance. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 45: WgSERSKA, M. and ZELRSK, K Conditioned inhibitor of the CER in rats. Physiol. Bohemoslov. (in press).

16 27. YOSHDA, T., KA, M. and MADA, H A methodological study of CER in rats with "licking" as the criterion response. Jap. Psychol. Res. 11: ZLELNSK, K Extinction, inhibition, and differentiation learning. n Dickinson, A. and Boakes, R. A. (ed.), Mechanisms of learning and rnoiivation. L. Erlbaum Ass. Hillsdale, p ZELNSK, K. and JAKUBOWSKA, E, Auditory intensity generalization after CER differentiation training. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 37: James F. BRENNAN. The Commonwealth of Messachusetts, University of Massachusetts-Boston, Harbor Campus, Boston, Massachusetts 02125, USA.

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Habituation of the orienting response to auditory and visual stimuli in rats trained to press a bar

Habituation of the orienting response to auditory and visual stimuli in rats trained to press a bar Habituation of the orienting response to auditory and visual stimuli in rats trained to press a bar Grazyna Walasek, Malgorzata Wqsierska and Kazimierz Zielinski Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

DIFFERENTIATION LEARNING AND AUDITORY GENERALIZATION IN NORMAL AND PREFRONTAL DOGS AFTER EXTENSIVE AVOIDANCE TRAINING

DIFFERENTIATION LEARNING AND AUDITORY GENERALIZATION IN NORMAL AND PREFRONTAL DOGS AFTER EXTENSIVE AVOIDANCE TRAINING ACTA NEUROBIOL. EXP. 1981, 41: 403-422 DIFFERENTIATION LEARNING AND AUDITORY GENERALIZATION IN NORMAL AND PREFRONTAL DOGS AFTER EXTENSIVE AVOIDANCE TRAINING Danuta KOWALSKA, Kazimierz ZIELINSKI and James

More information

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction PUNISHMENT A ND RECO VER Y D URING FIXED-RA TIO PERFORMA NCE' NATHAN H. AZRIN2 ANNA STATE HOSPITAL When a reinforcement is delivered according to a fixed-ratio schedule, it has been found that responding

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback acquisition associative learning in classical conditioning the initial stage when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation.

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation. Unit 06 - Overview How We Learn and Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Operant Conditioning s Applications, and Comparison to Classical Conditioning Biology, Cognition, and Learning Learning By

More information

Chapter 7. Learning From Experience

Chapter 7. Learning From Experience Learning From Experience Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne What s It For? Learning From Experience Noticing and Ignoring Learning What Events Signal Learning About the Consequences of Our Behavior

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

Conditioning of fear and conditioning of safety in rats

Conditioning of fear and conditioning of safety in rats Conditioning of fear and conditioning of safety in rats Grazyna Walasek, Malgorzata Wqsierska and Kazimierz Zielinski Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, 3 Pasteur

More information

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives Theoretical Perspectives REBT (1955) Albert Ellis Action & Result Oriented Teaches how to identify self-defeating thoughts Replaces thoughts w/ life enhancing ones 1 A B C s of personality formation: A

More information

Learning. Learning. Habituation. Sensitization. Habituation and Sensitization

Learning. Learning. Habituation. Sensitization. Habituation and Sensitization Learning Learning Adaptive change in behavior that results from past experiences Nonassociative Learning Habituation Sensitization Classical Conditioning Instrumental Learning/Operant Conditioning Ability

More information

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn?

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Defining Learning A relatively permanent change in behavior or the potential for behavior that results from experience Results from many life experiences, not just structured

More information

What is Learned? Lecture 9

What is Learned? Lecture 9 What is Learned? Lecture 9 1 Classical and Instrumental Conditioning Compared Classical Reinforcement Not Contingent on Behavior Behavior Elicited by US Involuntary Response (Reflex) Few Conditionable

More information

I. Classical Conditioning

I. Classical Conditioning Learning Chapter 8 Learning A relatively permanent change in an organism that occur because of prior experience Psychologists must study overt behavior or physical changes to study learning Learning I.

More information

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher.

1. A type of learning in which behavior is strengthened if followed by a reinforcer or diminished if followed by a punisher. 1. A stimulus change that increases the future frequency of behavior that immediately precedes it. 2. In operant conditioning, a reinforcement schedule that reinforces a response only after a specified

More information

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Chapter 6/9: Learning Chapter 6/9: Learning Learning A relatively durable change in behavior or knowledge that is due to experience. The acquisition of knowledge, skills, and behavior through reinforcement, modeling and natural

More information

Classical Conditioning. Learning. Classical conditioning terms. Classical Conditioning Procedure. Procedure, cont. Important concepts

Classical Conditioning. Learning. Classical conditioning terms. Classical Conditioning Procedure. Procedure, cont. Important concepts Learning Classical Conditioning Pavlov study of digestion dogs salivate before getting food learning as signal detection: emphasis on what happens before a given behavior Classical conditioning terms Stimulus:

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (3), 230-245 doi:10.3758/lb.37.3.230 Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning JUSTIN A. HARRIS, SABA A GHARA EI, AND CLINTON A. MOORE

More information

Classical and Instrumental Conditioning. Lecture 8

Classical and Instrumental Conditioning. Lecture 8 Classical and Instrumental Conditioning Lecture 8 1 Basic Procedure for Classical Conditioning CS (Bell) US (Meat Powder) CR (Salivation) UR (Salivation) 2 Acquisition Major Phenomena of Classical Conditioning

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule*

A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2 (3),168-172. A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule* JOHN HARSH and PETRO BADA Bowling Green State University,

More information

Associative Learning

Associative Learning Learning Learning Associative Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Biological Components of Learning Cognitive Components of Learning Behavioral Therapies Associative

More information

Psychology in Your Life

Psychology in Your Life Sarah Grison Todd Heatherton Michael Gazzaniga Psychology in Your Life FIRST EDITION Chapter 6 Learning 2014 W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. Section 6.1 How Do the Parts of Our Brains Function? 6.1 What Are

More information

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning

Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(2),199-203 Effects of compound or element preexposure on compound flavor aversion conditioning PETER C. HOLLAND and DESWELL T. FORBES University ofpittsburgh, Pittsburgh,

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events acquisition associative learning In classical conditioning, the initial stage, when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

Chapter 7 - Learning

Chapter 7 - Learning Chapter 7 - Learning How Do We Learn Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Defining Learning Learning a relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience.

More information

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1965, Vol. 59, No. 1, 13-17 AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 GORDON BOWER, RONALD STARR, AND LEAH LAZAROVITZ Stanford

More information

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events.

Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events. Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning - a type of learning in which one learns to link two stimuli and anticipate events. behaviorism - the view that psychology (1) should be an objective science

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING JOURNL OF THE EXPERMENTL NLYSS OF BEHVOR 1979, 32, 121-127 NUMBER (JULY) CQUSTON OF STMULUS CONTROL WHLE NTRODUCNG NEW STMUL N FDNG LNNY FELDS THE COLLEGE OF STTEN SLND fter establishing a discrimination

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning) Classical vs. Instrumental The modern view is that these two types of learning involve similar

More information

Psychological Hodgepodge. Mr. Mattingly Psychology

Psychological Hodgepodge. Mr. Mattingly Psychology Psychological Hodgepodge Mr. Mattingly Psychology The Number: Eight What is conditioning? Conditioning = learned or trained Classical Conditioning = learning procedure where associations are made Usually

More information

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 5, NUMBF- 4 OCITOBER, 1 962 THE EFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES OF PRIMARY AND CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT JOAN G. STEVENSON1 AND T. W. REESE MOUNT HOLYOKE

More information

Spontaneous recovery. Module 18. Processes of Conditioning. Classical Conditioning (cont d)

Spontaneous recovery. Module 18. Processes of Conditioning. Classical Conditioning (cont d) Module 18 Classical Conditioning (cont d) Processes of Conditioning Acquisition Extinction Spontaneous Recovery Stimulus Generalization/ Discrimination Spontaneous recovery The re-emergence of an extinguished

More information

Learning. Association. Association. Unit 6: Learning. Learning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Different Types of Learning

Learning. Association. Association. Unit 6: Learning. Learning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Different Types of Learning Unit 6: Learning Learning Learning relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience experience (nurture) is the key to learning Different Types of Learning Classical -learn by association

More information

CUEING PROPERTIES OF THE DECREASE OF WHITE NOISE INTENSITY FOR AVOIDANCE CONDITIONING IN CATS

CUEING PROPERTIES OF THE DECREASE OF WHITE NOISE INTENSITY FOR AVOIDANCE CONDITIONING IN CATS ACTA NEUROBIOL. EXP. 1979, 39: 263-283 CUEING PROPERTIES OF THE DECREASE OF WHITE NOISE INTENSITY FOR AVOIDANCE CONDITIONING IN CATS Kazimierz ZIELINSKI Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Insti'tute

More information

Myers PSYCHOLOGY. (7th Ed) Chapter 8. Learning. James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University. Worth Publishers

Myers PSYCHOLOGY. (7th Ed) Chapter 8. Learning. James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University. Worth Publishers Myers PSYCHOLOGY (7th Ed) Chapter 8 Learning James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University Worth Publishers Learning Learning relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience Association

More information

Psychology, Ch. 6. Learning Part 1

Psychology, Ch. 6. Learning Part 1 Psychology, Ch. 6 Learning Part 1 Two Main Types of Learning Associative learning- learning that certain events occur together Cognitive learning- acquisition of mental information, by observing or listening

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Classical Conditioning Learning through Association Ivan Pavlov discovered the form of learning called Classical

More information

Association. Operant Conditioning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Learning to associate two events. We learn to. associate two stimuli

Association. Operant Conditioning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Learning to associate two events. We learn to. associate two stimuli Myers PSYCHOLOGY (7th Ed) Chapter 8 Learning James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University Worth Publishers Learning Learning relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience Association

More information

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning Chapter 6: Learning 1 Learning 1. In perception we studied that we are responsive to stimuli in the external world. Although some of these stimulus-response associations are innate many are learnt. 2.

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice. What is Learning? Learning is the process that allows us to adapt (be flexible) to the changing conditions

More information

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's JOURNAt, OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1968, 1 1, 1-8 NUMBER I (JANUARY) A UTO-SHAPING OF THE PIGEON'S KEY-PECK' PAUL L. BROWN AND HERBERT M. JENKINS MCMASTER UNIVERSITY Reliable acquistion

More information

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience.

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience. Chapter 6: Learning Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience. Conditioning = a process in which environmental stimuli and behavioral processes become connected Two types of

More information

in two-way avoidance learning

in two-way avoidance learning Intertrial responses of rats in two-way avoidance learning to visual and auditory stimuli Kazimierz Zielin ski, Tomasz Werka and Eugeniusz Nikolaev Department of Neurophysiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental

More information

AVOIDANCE LEARNING IN SHUTTLING AND NONSHUTTLING SITUATIONS, WITH AND WITHOUT A BARRIER. Kobe Juvenile Detention and Classification Home

AVOIDANCE LEARNING IN SHUTTLING AND NONSHUTTLING SITUATIONS, WITH AND WITHOUT A BARRIER. Kobe Juvenile Detention and Classification Home Japanese Psychological Research 1964, Vol.6, No.3, 129-135 AVOIDANCE LEARNING IN SHUTTLING AND NONSHUTTLING SITUATIONS, WITH AND WITHOUT A BARRIER EIJI KUNITOMI, Kobe Juvenile Detention and Classification

More information

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1985 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/85/$00.75 1985, VOl. 11, No. 4, 548-564 Predictive Accuracy and the

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

Classical Conditioning & Operant Conditioning

Classical Conditioning & Operant Conditioning Classical Conditioning & Operant Conditioning What is Classical Conditioning? Learning Objective: Students will be able to describe the difference between Classical and Operant Conditioning. How Do We

More information

Acquired Equivalence and Distinctiveness of Cues

Acquired Equivalence and Distinctiveness of Cues Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1989 by lhe American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-740389$00.75 1989, Vol. 15, No. 4, 338-346 Acquired Equivalence and Distinctiveness

More information

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 322 328 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.322

More information

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010 Notes Textbook used Week 1-3: Bouton, M.E. (2016). Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis. 2nd Ed. Sinauer Week 4-6: Rieger, E. (Ed.) (2014) Abnormal Psychology:

More information

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1

The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Computational Models of Neural Systems Lecture 5.1 The Rescorla Wagner Learning Model (and one of its descendants) Lecture 5.1 David S. Touretzky Based on notes by Lisa M. Saksida November, 2015 Outline Classical and instrumental conditioning The Rescorla

More information

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition Denniston, James C., Robert P. Cole, and Ralph R. Miller. (1998). "The role of temporal relationships in the transfer of conditioned inhibition." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

More information

Study Plan: Session 1

Study Plan: Session 1 Study Plan: Session 1 6. Practice learning the vocabulary. Use the electronic flashcards from the Classical The Development of Classical : The Basic Principles of Classical Conditioned Emotional Reponses:

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

STUDY GUIDE ANSWERS 6: Learning Introduction and How Do We Learn? Operant Conditioning Classical Conditioning

STUDY GUIDE ANSWERS 6: Learning Introduction and How Do We Learn? Operant Conditioning Classical Conditioning STUDY GUIDE ANSWERS 6: Learning Introduction and How Do We Learn? 1. learning 2. associate; associations; associative learning; habituates 3. classical 4. operant 5. observing Classical Conditioning 1.

More information

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction 1 2 3 4 5 Myers Psychology for AP* Unit 6: Learning Unit Overview How Do We Learn? Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Learning by Observation How Do We Learn? Introduction Learning Habituation

More information

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Jennifer A. Kaminski (kaminski.16@osu.edu) Center for Cognitive Science, Ohio State University 10A

More information

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson, H Djafari Marbini, AJ Toner, TW Robbins, BJ Everitt Departments of

More information

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5 Learning AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5 Learning Learning is a lasting change in behavior or mental process as the result of an experience. There are two important parts: a lasting change a simple reflexive reaction

More information

Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats

Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1980, 8 (1), 67-74 Systematic manipulation of CS-US pairings in negative CS-US correlation procedures in rats ELIZABETH S. WITCHER and JOHN J. B. AYRES University ofmassachusetts,

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION Except where reference is made to the works of others, the work described in this dissertation is my own or was done in collaboration

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Unit 6 Learning.

Unit 6 Learning. Unit 6 Learning https://www.apstudynotes.org/psychology/outlines/chapter-6-learning/ 1. Overview 1. Learning 1. A long lasting change in behavior resulting from experience 2. Classical Conditioning 1.

More information

Chapter 5 Study Guide

Chapter 5 Study Guide Chapter 5 Study Guide Practice Exam Questions: Which of the following is not included in the definition of learning? It is demonstrated immediately Assuming you have eaten sour pickles before, imagine

More information

The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce

The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce Learning Chapter 7 Design a Brain The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce What characteristics are needed to behave adaptively and reproduce successfully? Consider: nearly

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Learning and Motivation 32, 349 366 (2001) doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1089, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Charlotte Bonardi

More information

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia AM. ZOOI.OCIST, 12:507-512 (1972). Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia PAUL DOWNEY AND BEHRUS JAHAN-PARWAR Biology Department, Clark University, Worcester, Massachusetts 01610 and Worcester Foundation

More information

Interval duration effects on blocking in appetitive conditioning

Interval duration effects on blocking in appetitive conditioning Behavioural Processes 71 (2006) 318 329 Interval duration effects on blocking in appetitive conditioning Dómhnall Jennings 1, Kimberly Kirkpatrick Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington,

More information

Classical & Operant Conditioning. Learning: Principles and Applications

Classical & Operant Conditioning. Learning: Principles and Applications Classical & Operant Conditioning Learning: Principles and Applications Which Pen Would You Choose? The researchers placed the participants in the room. In this room the participants first viewed purple

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

LACK OF INSTRUMENTAL CONDITIONING WITH INTRAGASTRIC REINFORCEMENT IN NORMAL EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURE

LACK OF INSTRUMENTAL CONDITIONING WITH INTRAGASTRIC REINFORCEMENT IN NORMAL EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURE ACTA NEUROBOL. EXP. 1972, 32: 767-772 LACK OF NSTRUMENTAL CONDTONNG WTH NTRAGASTRC RENFORCEMENT N NORMAL EXPERMENTAL PROCEDURE Jerzy CYTAWA, Danuta LUSZAWSKA, Ryszard SCHOENBORN and Marian ZAJqC Department

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

Computational Versus Associative Models of Simple Conditioning i

Computational Versus Associative Models of Simple Conditioning i Gallistel & Gibbon Page 1 In press Current Directions in Psychological Science Computational Versus Associative Models of Simple Conditioning i C. R. Gallistel University of California, Los Angeles John

More information

Dikran J. Martin Introduction to Psychology

Dikran J. Martin Introduction to Psychology Dikran J. Martin Introduction to Psychology Name: Date: Lecture Series: Chapter 7 Learning Pages: 32 TEXT: Lefton, Lester A. and Brannon, Linda (2003). PSYCHOLOGY. (Eighth Edition.) Needham Heights, MA:

More information

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning.

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning. In Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/xan/index.aspx 2013, vol. 39 (2), pp 107 116 2013 American Psychological Association DOI: 10.1037/a0031315

More information

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online

Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online Chapter Title Attention and Pavlovian Conditioning Copyright Year 2011 Copyright Holder Springer Science+Business Media, LLC Corresponding Author

More information

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting Animal Learning & Behavior 1989, 17 (3), 269-279 Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting PETER C. HOLLAND Duke University, Durham, North Carolina In three experiments, transfer effects

More information

Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training

Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 17, 243-268 (1986) Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training PETER C. HOLLAND University of Pittsburgh Three experiments with rat subjects examined the transfer

More information

Name: Period: Chapter 7: Learning. 5. What is the difference between classical and operant conditioning?

Name: Period: Chapter 7: Learning. 5. What is the difference between classical and operant conditioning? Name: Period: Chapter 7: Learning Introduction, How We Learn, & Classical Conditioning (pp. 291-304) 1. Learning: 2. What does it mean that we learn by association? 3. Habituation: 4. Associative Learning:

More information

that simple discrimination training to compound given a set to react -to one aspect of a stimulus a "set", a discrimination is "pretrained" along

that simple discrimination training to compound given a set to react -to one aspect of a stimulus a set, a discrimination is pretrained along JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1968, ll, 157-166 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) SOME DETERMINERS OF ATTENTION' DANIEL F. JOHNSON AND WILLIAM W. CUMMING VIRGINIA POLYTECHNIC INSTITUTE AND COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION Tnesis for the Degree of M. A. MiCHiGAN STATE UNWERSITY RODNEY CHARLES HOWARD 1977 f L13 1-;- R Y Michiga: State gr? Umw 1y ABSTRACT TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES

More information

DEFINITION. Learning is the process of acquiring knowledge (INFORMATIN ) and new responses. It is a change in behavior as a result of experience

DEFINITION. Learning is the process of acquiring knowledge (INFORMATIN ) and new responses. It is a change in behavior as a result of experience LEARNING DEFINITION Learning is the process of acquiring knowledge (INFORMATIN ) and new responses. It is a change in behavior as a result of experience WHAT DO WE LEARN? 1. Object :we learn objects with

More information

CONDITIONED EMOTIONAL RESPONSE AFTER DORSOHIPPOCAMPAL, VENTROHIPPOCAMPAL OR ENTORHINAL CORTICAL LESIONS IN RATS

CONDITIONED EMOTIONAL RESPONSE AFTER DORSOHIPPOCAMPAL, VENTROHIPPOCAMPAL OR ENTORHINAL CORTICAL LESIONS IN RATS ACTA NEUROBIOL. EXP. 1981, 41: 605-617 The Symposium "Brain and behavior" held in Jablonna near Warsaw Poster presentation May 1981 CONDITIONED EMOTIONAL RESPONSE AFTER DORSOHIPPOCAMPAL, VENTROHIPPOCAMPAL

More information

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli.

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli. Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli. Harry A. Mackay, Brooks Thompson, & William J. McIlvane

More information

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli. BACB 4 th Edition Task List s Content Area E: Specific Behavior-Change Procedures E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

More information