Attention Factors in Temopral Distortion: The Effects of Food Availability on Responses within the Interval Bisection Task

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Attention Factors in Temopral Distortion: The Effects of Food Availability on Responses within the Interval Bisection Task"

Transcription

1 Utah State University All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies Attention Factors in Temopral Distortion: The Effects of Food Availability on Responses within the Interval Bisection Task Robert N.. Johnson Utah State University Follow this and additional works at: Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Johnson, Robert N.., "Attention Factors in Temopral Distortion: The Effects of Food Availability on Responses within the Interval Bisection Task" (2013). All Graduate Theses and Dissertations This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate Studies at It has been accepted for inclusion in All Graduate Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of For more information, please contact

2 ATTENTIONAL FACTORS IN TEMPORAL DISTORTION: THE EFFECTS OF FOOD AVAILABILITY ON RESPONSES WITHIN THE INTERVAL BISECTION TASK by Robert N. Johnson A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE in Psychology Approved: Amy L. Odum, Ph.D. Major Professor Timothy A. Shahan, Ph.D. Committee Member Donal G. Sinex, Ph.D. Committee Member Mark R. McLellan, Ph.D. Vice Preside for Research and Dean of the School of Graduate Studies UTAH STATE UNIVERSITY Logan, Utah 2013

3 ii Copyright Robert N. Johnson 2013 All Rights Reserved

4 iii ABSTRACT Attentional Factors in Temporal Distortion: The Effects of Food Availability on Responses Within the Interval Bisection Task by Robert N. Johnson, Master of Science Utah State University, 2013 Major Professor: Amy L. Odum, Ph.D. Department: Psychology There are differences within the timing literature regarding the effects of distracter stimulus presentation within timing tasks. Whereas some researchers have found underestimation (changes in the degree of temporal stimulus control), others have found generalized disruption of timing responses. The purpose of this thesis was to determine the importance of food availability on responses within a time estimation task, using pigeons as subjects. Specifically, it was hypothesized that presenting food access following timing responses after a distracter task would produce underestimation of the target interval, relative to control conditions. Using a 2-parameter function fit to proportion long data from the interval bisection task, data revealed a generalized disruption effect of the distracter on timing behavior. Further analysis revealed that presentation of the food following timing responses after the distracter task reduced stimulus control within the timing task, revealing underestimation of the target interval.

5 iv These findings suggest that the causes of the differences within the timing literature may be based upon differences in procedure. (41 pages)

6 v PUBLIC ABSTRACT Attentional Factors in Temporal Distortion: The Effects of Food Availability on Responses Within the Interval Bisection Task by Robert N. Johnson, Master of Science Utah State University, 2013 Major Professor: Amy L. Odum, Ph.D. Department: Psychology Time estimation is a process that underlies many complex behaviors. Accurately timed performance depends upon some biological mechanism, which may be revealed by experimental methods. There have been several proposals on the subject of the function of the timing mechanism, some of which have revealed contradictory findings regarding the effect of distraction on timing. Whereas some studies have revealed underestimation (reports of time moving more slowly than it actually is) of the target interval following distracter presentation, others have revealed generalized disruption (no meaningful evidence of timing). The purpose of the present study was to determine whether food presentation for timing responses following exposure to a distracter task would produce underestimation relative to control conditions. Pigeons were used as subjects on an interval timing task, wherein stimuli were presented for certain brief intervals, followed by presentation of side key lights, which produced food when pecked. After establishing a baseline, the task was modified to include brief access to a key light that had previously been associated with food presented during the interval to be timed. Following the interval, responses to the correct side-key produced food in one condition, and resulted in no food in the other condition. Analysis of the data revealed an overall generalized disruption in both distracter conditions, though a data-correction procedure revealed underestimation in the food-available distracter condition, relative to the no-food condition. These findings suggest that the causes of the differences between studies on timing may be based upon differences in procedure.

7 vi ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to acknowledge the assistance of all of those who made this project possible. Special thanks to Amy Odum for her help throughout the project. I would also like to thank Timothy Shahan and Donal Sinex for critical assistance on the analysis of these results and aid with overall presentation. Additional thanks to Heather Anderson, Scott Barret, Meredith Berry, Kelli Colston, Carrie Hales, Daniel Hutchison, Laura Hernandez, Corina Jiminez-Gomez, Brandon Norlund, Adam Kynaston, Chris Podlesnik, Adam Pyszczynski, Stacey Quick, Andy Rodewald, Michael Sirrine, Maggie Sweeney, Wes Thomas, Eric Thrailkill, and Ryan Ward for laboratory assistance. Finally, thanks to Susan Johnson for her patience and understanding. Robert N. Johnson

8 vii CONTENTS Page ABSTRACT... PUBLIC ABSTRACT... ACKNOWLEDGMENTS... iii v vi LIST OF TABLES... viii LIST OF FIGURES... ix CHAPTER I. INTRODUCTION... 1 II. LITERATURE REVIEW... 3 The Interval Bisection Task... 4 Procedural Factors... 6 Statement of the Problem... 9 III. METHODS Subjects Apparatus Procedure Dependent Measures Data Analysis IV. RESULTS Primary Measures and Model Fit Nonnormalized Measures and Parameters Normalized Measures and Parameters Latencies Response and Food Presentation Rates V. DISCUSSION REFERENCES... 31

9 viii LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Counterbalancing Key Light Color Assignments and Disrupter Order Descriptive Statistics for Nonnormalized and Normalized T 50, ε, and Range by Condition Descriptive Statistics for Nonnormalized and Normalized Goodness of Fit by Condition Medians and 25th and 75th Quartiles of Latency to Peck Trial-Ready or Choice Stimuli by Condition... 24

10 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Hypothetical distribution of the proportion of long responses as a function of sample duration fitted with a Gaussian cumulative function showing placement of the range, slope, and mean for the proportion of long responses as a function of sample duration Mean proportion of long responses as a function of sample duration for trials with food available (top panel) and not available (bottom panel) following disrupter presentation... 19

11 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Attention to temporal cues is an important factor for many complex behaviors. Driving, cooking, and other behaviors would be more dangerous and difficult without attention to stimulus events occurring in the range of seconds to minutes. Interval-timing processes, which determine temporal behavior in the range of seconds to minutes, differ from circadian processes in several ways (Hinton & Meck, 1997). Interval timing is not necessarily linked to external events, such as tidal cycles. Also, interval timing is highly flexible, and the timing process may be stopped, restarted, or reset upon command. Finally, the interval timing process exhibits scalar variance, whereby greater intervals produce relatively greater response variability. Divided attention and changes in neuropharmacology can distort timing. Distorted timing typically takes the form of over- or underestimation, in which the timed response occurs early or late, or reported time passage is slower or faster than objective time. Sutton and Roberts (2002) defined timing with divided attention as timing while simultaneously engaged in another information-processing task (p. 124). Divided attention is typically associated with delay of the timed response, or underestimation of the interval-to-be-timed. Distorted temporal responding as a function of divided attention has not been a consistent finding, however. Whereas underestimation of the target interval (i.e., responses occur too late) is sometimes reported (e.g., Lejeune, Macar, & Zakay, 1999), a loss of temporal discriminability (i.e., responses are unrelated to the temporal aspect of the task) has also been found (e.g., Sutton & Roberts, 2002; Ward &

12 2 Odum, 2007). Temporal discriminability is a term for stimulus control of temporal stimuli over responses. These latter findings suggest that distraction within the target interval leads to a reduction in stimulus control of temporal responses. Within the timing literature, there are several procedural differences between the experiments that have resulted in systematic temporal distortions and those that have not. For example, using pigeons as subjects, Lejeune and colleagues (1999) found systematic temporal distortions when distracter probe trials sometimes resulted in food. Other experimental manipulations have included flashing stimulus lights as distracters (Ward & Odum, 2007), and probe trials that never resulted in food (Sutton & Roberts, 2002), and failed to find systematic temporal distortions. These findings suggest that the properties of the distracting stimuli and the probe trials strongly influence the probability of temporal distortions. The purpose of this study was to determine whether distortion of temporal performance by distraction is dependent upon the reinforcing properties of the temporal task. I expected to find underestimation of the interval for trials resulting in food (cf., Lejeune et al., 1999), and loss of temporal discriminability for trials that did not result in food (cf., Sutton & Roberts, 2002).

13 3 CHAPTER II LITERATURE REVIEW The ability of organisms to accurately estimate short intervals the passage of time between a few seconds and several hours is a critical adaptation. The foraging literature reveals numerous cases of how the behaviors associated with feeding may be dependent upon adequate interval time estimation. For example, Daan and Koene (1981) found that oystercatchers in the Netherlands departed roosts at a particular time of day in order to reach feeding grounds when food access was optimal. Because lunar and tidal cycles were different for each instance of the behavior, circadian timing explanations were found to be inadequate. Similarly, humans use interval timing to accomplish many tasks that are not clearly associated with circadian processes. For example, a driver s decision to brake or not when approaching a yellow traffic light is related to the amount of time the yield signal has been visible. Estimates of the seconds between the occurrence of the yellow light and the red stop signal that follows cannot reasonably be based on circadian processes. Some interval timing process that allows for estimation at the seconds-tominutes level must surely exist. Temporal estimation may be measurably disrupted in terms of overestimation, underestimation, or loss of stimulus control. Overestimation describes a tendency to report time as moving at a faster rate than it really is. Although overestimation has been found with some verbal reporting methods with human subjects (Johnson & Hutchens, 2008; Rai, 1973), and neuropharmacological manipulations with rats (methamphetamine Matell, Bateson, & Meck, 2006; ethanol Meck, 2007), it is

14 4 not typically found when intervals are disrupted by distracting stimuli. Underestimation describes a tendency to report time as moving at a slower rate than it really is, and is a frequently reported effect of haloperidol administration (see Cheng, Hakak, & Meck, 2007, for a comprehensive review), or disruption by distracting stimuli for rats (Aum, Brown, & Hemmes, 2004, 2007; Brown, Richer, & Doyère, 2007; Buhusi & Meck, 2006) or pigeons (Buhusi, Paskalis, & Cerutti, 2006; Lejeune et al., 1999). Finally, a loss of temporal discriminability may occur when disrupted temporal responses do not clearly represent over- or underestimation, and display decreased stimulus control. Loss of temporal discriminability has been reported with disruption by distracting stimuli (Sutton & Roberts, 2002; Ward & Odum, 2007) and neuropharmacological effects (e.g., morphine: Odum & Schaal, 2000; d-amphetamine: Odum & Ward, 2007) for pigeons. Although the influence of distracting stimuli on timing has been examined in several studies, a clear picture of the effect of distraction on time estimation has not been forthcoming. Whereas some studies have found specific temporal distortions (Aum et al., 2004; Buhusi & Meck, 2006; Lejeune et al., 1999), other studies have found a loss of temporal discriminability as a function of stimulus presentation (Sutton & Roberts, 2002; Ward & Odum, 2007). These inconsistent findings pose a problem for general statements regarding timing, as the effect of divided attention on time estimation should be predictable. The Interval Bisection Task The interval bisection task is a classic temporal estimation procedure, related to

15 5 the matching-to-sample paradigm. Relatively long or short sample intervals are presented, and following the sample, a response to a corresponding long or short key or lever (side or color differentiated) results in food. For example, Stubbs (1968) instituted a task whereby pigeons would receive food for responses to keys corresponding to 1- or 10-s sample presentations. Following the 1-s sample, pecking the green key light ( short response) resulted in 5-s access to food, whereas pecking the red key light ( long response) resulted in 60-s blackout. Likewise, following the 10-s sample, the long response resulted in food, and the short response resulted in blackout. When Stubbs introduced intermediate samples of 2- to 9-s in addition to the 1- and 10-s endpoints, the proportion of long responses resembled a sigmoidal generalization gradient, similar to that shown in Figure 1; with the lowest percentage of long responses following 1-s samples, and the greatest percentage of long responses following 10-s samples. The psychometric function produced by the proportion of long responses may be analyzed to determine sources of error and the possibility of temporal distortion. Blough (1996) manipulated sample color in a symbolic matching-to-sample task, in which pigeons pecked comparison keys according to whether the sample resembled endpoints on a spectrum. Blough found that manipulation of stimulus duration resulted in changes in slope, or variance, while manipulation of the retention interval between sample and comparison presentations resulted in lowered range of the psychometric function. These effects may be equated to changes in stimulus sensitivity and control, respectively. Blough further suggested that alterations in the mean of the distribution result from bias,

16 6 Figure 1. Hypothetical distribution of the proportion of long responses as a function of sample duration fitted with a Gaussian cumulative function showing placement of the range, slope, and mean for the proportion of long responses as a function of sample duration. or the report of a substantially greater number of samples resembling one endpoint relative to the other. Figure 1 shows how changes in slope, range, and mean of the function relate to the psychophysical distribution. McClure, Saulsgiver, and Wynne (2005) note that Blough s analysis may be applied to interval bisection data, providing an interpretation of changes in mean in terms of the bisection point at which a particular interval may be categorized as long half of the time. Directional changes in the bisection point, then, may be equated to over- or underestimation of the sample interval. Procedural Factors One reason for the inconsistency of findings when temporal responses undergo disruption may be procedural. Lejeune and colleagues (1999) found underestimation of

17 7 the target interval as a function of disruptor task presentation. Using three pigeons as subjects, baseline was established with interval endpoints of 10-s (short) and 40-s (long). Intermediate samples of 20- and 25-s durations were included following training on the 10- and 40-s endpoints. In separate sessions, a variable ratio (VR; Ferster & Skinner, 1957) schedule of reinforcement was implemented, which provided food for every 5 pecks on average to a blue key light. Over the course of ten 100-trial test sessions, the VR 5 task was available during the interval-to-be-timed 3-s after sample onset (early condition) for approximately half of the trials. Food was available for appropriate temporal responses following VR presentation 50% of the time. Probe trials produced modest underestimation relative to baseline. Temporal stimulus control may be lost when a stimulus within the interval-to-betimed comes to signal that responses during that trial will not result in food, as may be the case when non-reinforced probe trials are used. For example, Sutton and Roberts (2002) used distracter probe trials within the sample interval as part of an investigation into information processing effects on timing. In Experiment 3, the experimenters inserted probe trials in which food was not available for responses corresponding to sample length into one of every four blocks of trials, and a house light was on. As a distracting stimulus, the house lights failed to produce underestimation relative to baseline, and resulted in a decreased range of the psychophysical function. The experimenters attributed their findings to the possibility that the house light came to signal that no food was available for appropriate responses, resulting in less sample stimulus control, but no directional effect on estimation.

18 8 The experiments by Lejeune and colleagues (1999) and Sutton and Roberts (2002) were different in several ways. For example, Lejeune and colleagues used four sample durations, the longest differing from the shortest by 30 s. Sutton and Roberts used relatively briefer sample durations (2, 3, 4.5, 6.7 and 10 s). Lejeune and colleagues used white noise as the sample duration, and Sutton and Roberts used the house light. Finally, Lejeune and colleagues implemented probe trials in which food was sometimes available for appropriate timing responses following the VR task presentation. Food was never available for appropriate timing responses following disrupter presentation within Sutton and Roberts preparation. As yet, however, no studies have specifically investigated whether a disruptive stimulus, as opposed to a distracting task, can result in underestimation when food is available. As the availability of food is a clear difference between studies that have reported distortions and those that have not, food availability was manipulated in the current experiment. Given previous findings, it was expected that the use of a disruptive task during the interval-to-be-timed would decrease attention to the interval bisection task and result in underestimation (e.g., Lejeune et al., 1999). Furthermore, distracter trials that resulted in food for accurate performance were expected to result in underestimation relative to trials that did not result in food (e.g., Sutton & Roberts, 2002). Additionally, it was expected that presentation of a disrupter stimulus would reduce temporal stimulus control relative to baseline when food was not available for correct responses, due to reduced attention to choice keys within the interval bisection preparation.

19 9 Statement of the Problem Manipulation of distracter presentation within time estimation tasks has had differing results. Whereas some studies have found evidence for underestimation as a function of distraction, others have found loss of stimulus control. Alterations of a single variable, such as attention to the temporal estimation task, should have consistent effects on response patterns. That such patterns are not consistent over several studies suggests that procedural differences may play a role in observed outcomes. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to manipulate food availability for probe trials to determine the effects of these procedural differences on the attentional properties of temporal responding.

20 10 CHAPTER III METHODS Subjects Eight homing pigeons served as subjects. All eight had prior experience in unrelated operant procedures. Subjects were housed in a temperature- and light- (12 light hours: 12 dark hours) controlled colony and maintained at approximately 80% ad libitum weight. All experimental procedures were conducted at approximately the same time every day. Apparatus Eight BRS/LVE operant chambers housed within sound-attenuating chambers were used. The dimensions of these chambers were 30.7-cm long, 35-cm wide, and cm high. The front panel in each chamber featured three translucent keys, and each key was 2.6 cm in diameter, 24.6 cm from the floor, and could be lit from behind with white, turquoise, red, green, blue, and yellow light. Each key required a force of 0.10 N or more to record responses. The house light (28 V, 1.1 W) was centered at the top of the chamber, 4.4 cm above the center key. Pigeon chow pellets were obtained at a rectangular opening 9 cm below the center light, when the hopper was presented and lit with white light. House and stimulus lights were off during hopper presentations.

21 11 Procedure Shaping to peck lit keys was not necessary, as these pigeons had already been exposed to operant procedures. Training and Baseline Phases Every other day, for 52 days, four pigeons were trained on the interval bisection task with 10- and 31-s endpoints. The session began with the house light lit for 5-s, followed by the trial-ready center key being lit turquoise. A single peck to the turquoise key light turned off the turquoise center key, and turned on the center sample key light (see Table 1 for counterbalancing assignments). The sample was presented for 10- or 31- s, following which, the center key was turned off, and the side choice keys were lit. Pecking on the left key turned off the side key lights and resulted a 2-s hopper presentation following 10-s sample duration presentations, and pecking the right key turned off the side key lights and resulted in a 2-s hopper presentation following 31-s sample duration presentations. The house light was lit for a 5-s period following food presentation, after which the house light was turned off and the turquoise center trialready key light was lit again. Pecking on the left key following the 31-s sample duration presentation, or pecking the right key following the 10-s sample duration presentation lit the house light for 7 s, after which the house light was turned off and the turquoise center trial-ready key light was lit again. The period between the lighting of the trial-ready key light and the end of the 5- or 7-s house light presentation following a choice represented 1 trial. A session was comprised of 32 interval bisection trials.

22 12 Table 1 Counterbalancing Key Light Color Assignments and Disrupter Order Bird number Variable 367, , , , 1821 Key assignment Trial ready Turquoise Turquoise White White Sample Red Blue Red Blue Choice Yellow Green Yellow Green RR Green Yellow Green Yellow Disruptor order With food first Without food first After 40 sessions of 10- and 31-s endpoint sample duration training, performance remained at chance for the majority of the birds, so the sample duration endpoints were reduced to 4- and 10-s and correction procedures were implemented. Under correction procedures, an incorrect choice (pecking the short key following a 10-s sample duration, or the long following a 4-s sample duration) resulted in the repetition of the previous trial until the correct response was made. Intermediate sample durations were inserted in the following individual pairs: 5- and 9-s, 6- and 8-s, and 6.5- and 7.5-s, every 20 sessions until all baseline sample durations were included. After 20 sessions with all baseline sample durations included on correction, correction was removed. Twenty sessions following the removal of correction, the proportion of food availability associated with intermediate sample durations was reduced to 0.8, 0.6, and 0.5 over the course of 45 sessions.

23 13 During the course of establishing baseline, two birds died and were replaced, and four more birds were added to the experiment. The six new birds also had previous experience with operant procedures, so no pretraining was necessary. Training for the new six birds was briefer, as they experienced only the 4- and 10-s sample duration endpoints. Each of the new six birds experienced 35 sessions of endpoint training prior to insertion of intermediate sample durations. Intermediate sample durations were inserted as above, but a pair was inserted every five days instead of every 20. After the insertion of the final pair of intermediate sample durations, the probability of food presentation following accurate responses during intermediate sample duration trials was reduced from 1.0 to.8,.6, and.5, as above, over the course of 15 sessions. For all eight birds, once the probability for food presentation was reduced to.5, the number of trials was increased to 64 per session and measurement of baseline temporal responding began. The baseline phase consisted of 20 sessions of 64 trials prior to each disrupter phase. Nineteen sessions of correction were required for one subject following a disrupter phase, due to severe side bias, but was not required for any of the other subjects throughout the remainder of the experiment. RR Training Beginning the day after the first session with the interval bisection task, and every other day throughout 30 to 54 of the initial training days of the experiment, pigeons responded on a task that was later introduced as a disrupter. Pigeons pecked a key for a.5 probability of 2-s access to food (random ratio [RR] 2; Lattal, 1991; see Table 1 for key color counterbalancing). The location of the lit key (right or left) varied randomly across

24 14 trials during these sessions. The house light remained extinguished throughout these sessions. Sessions finished after 40 trials. After 30 to 54 days, RR training sessions occurred every 5 days, except the day prior to, during, and following disrupter phases. Experimental Manipulations Two experimental conditions were implemented to determine the effects of food availability on performance within the temporal estimation task. These conditions included probe trials in which food became available for responses corresponding with sample length (with food) and probe trials in which food is unavailable regardless of responses following the sample (without food). Previous findings suggest that temporal responses may become habituated to the disrupter task (Lejeune et al., 1999), so the number of probe trials was limited to eight per session. Furthermore, each experimental manipulation was in effect for 10 sessions, and was separated from other experimental manipulations by 20 sessions of the baseline interval bisection task and three sessions of RR2 training. Order of manipulation presentation was counterbalanced across pigeon as shown in Table 1. Disruption with Food To determine the effect of food availability following distracter probe trials on temporal distortion or stimulus control within the interval bisection task, 10 interval bisection sessions were modified to include eight probe distracter trials in each session. Each probe trial replaced one equivalent sample in each 8-trial block outlined above. Therefore, each sample duration was presented in seven normal trials and one probe trial.

25 15 During probe trials, a single side key light, color-associated with the previously trained RR 2, acted as a distracter stimulus. Responses to the distracter resulted in food access as determined by the RR 2 contingency outlined above, though only responses within the first 1-s of presentation resulted in 2-s access to food with a.5 probability. This restriction was meant to ensure that food access, if obtained, did not extend beyond the sample duration. Which side key was lit was randomly determined from trial to trial. The color of the key light when activated corresponded with the color of the stimulus light associated with the previously trained RR 2 contingency outlined above. Disruption Without Food Disrupter sessions without food were as disrupter sessions with food, except that food was never available for responses following a disrupter trial. Model Fitting The proportion of long responses during baseline and disruption performance were analyzed in Graphpad Prism 5.0a using a 2-parameter function (e.g., Odum, 2002); Proportion long =, where the proportion of long responses is plotted as a function of sample duration t; T 50 is the point at which the proportion of long responses = 0.5, or the mean of the function; and ε is the standard deviation, or slope, which indexes the sensitivity (precision) of timing. Changes in the T 50 are theoretically related to the subjective passage of time. A decrease in T 50 relative to baseline would indicate overestimation, and an increase in T 50 relative to baseline would indicate underestimation. Equation 1 was used to fit data for

26 16 individual and group performance. The range was computed by subtracting the proportion of long responses at the 4-s sample duration from the proportion of long responses at the 10-s sample duration for each set of baseline and disruption trials analyzed. Normalization of Proportion Long Data Previous research (McClure et al., 2005) has shown that changes in the range of proportion of long responses can result in artifactual changes in the T 50 and ε of the function. These artifactual changes to the T 50 and ε may influence interpretation of the effects of a manipulation, indicating under- or overestimation of the timed interval or a change in the sensitivity instead of a reduction in stimulus control. An established normalization procedure removes the effect of range on T 50 and ε (e.g., Odum & Ward, 2007). To eliminate any artifactual effect of the range on T 50 and ε, the proportion of long responses were normalized so that the proportion of long response at the 4-s sample duration was subtracted from the obtained proportion of long responses for each of the short sample durations (i.e., 5-, 6-, and 6.5-s), with the constraint that the result could not be less than 0. The difference between the proportion of long responses at the 10-s sample duration and 1.0 was added to the obtained proportion of long responses for each of the long sample durations (i.e., 7.5-, 8-, and 9-s), with the constraint that the result could not be greater than 1.0. Dependent Measures The proportion of long responses for baseline and disrupter sessions was

27 17 normalized. As described above, 20 baseline sessions were followed by 10 daily disrupter sessions. Disrupter sessions consisted of a single RR2 presentation during a stimulus duration presentation per block of eight interval bisection trials. For purposes of meaningful comparison between baseline and disrupter sessions, eight trials from each of 10 baseline sessions for each subject were selected according to a list generated by MED PC in a manner identical to that used to select trials for disrupter presentation. Responses per minute during disrupter presentation, rate of hopper presentations during the disrupter, and latency to peck trial-ready and choice keys for selected baseline and disrupter trials were also analyzed. Responses per minute were computed by dividing the total number of RR key responses in the session by the total time the RR stimulus was presented, minus feeder time. Latency to peck the trial-ready and choice keys was defined as the number of seconds between the presentation of the trial-ready or choice key lights and the first peck. Data Analysis Two-way repeated measures ANOVAs (food availability x disrupter presentation) were performed on T 50, ε, and the Range for the selected baseline data and disruption data. Planned t-test comparisons were performed for mean T 50, ε, and the range between baseline and with and without food disrupter conditions. Responses per minute to the RR key, and the total number of hopper presentations during RR presentation for with and Without Food disrupter presentations were also compared using paired-samples t test. Friedman s nonparametric analyses of the baseline and with and without food disrupter

28 18 condition medians of latency to peck trial-ready and choice keys were performed. Baseline and with and without food disrupter condition goodness of fit (R 2 ) for Equation 1 medians were also analyzed using Friedman s nonparametric analysis. Alpha =.05 was used to determine significance for all inferential tests.

29 19 CHAPTER IV RESULTS Primary Measures and Model Fit Figure 2 shows the mean proportion of long responses as a function of sample duration and trial type (with food presentation at top and without food presentation on the bottom) with normalized performance on the left and nonnormalized performance on the right. For normalized and nonnormalized baseline data (unfilled circles), there is a Figure 2. Mean proportion of long responses as a function of sample duration for trials with food available (top panel) and not available (bottom panel) following disrupter presentation. Filled circles represent responses following disrupter presentation and unfilled circles represent performance during baseline. The left panel shows normalized data (with the endpoint range set to 1.0) and the right panel shows nonnormalized data, with freely varying endpoint range.

30 20 smooth, sigmoidal increase in the proportion of long responses as a function of sample duration, so that the highest proportion of long responses tend to occur after samples longer than the 7-s arithmetic mean, and the lowest proportion tends to occur after samples shorter than 7-s. However, performance during non-normalized disruption performance is visibly different, with a clear reduction in the range (distance between proportion of long responses at the endpoints; see Figure 2, right side) relative to baseline. Nonnormalized Measures and Parameters The mean T 50, ε, and range for nonnormalized proportion of long responses are shown in Table 2. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA did not reveal significant differences between disrupter types or baseline for T 50, which shows that directional shifts were not detected in the non-normalized data set. There was no main effect of disrupter presentation on T 50, F (1,7) = 0.26, p =.62. There was no main effect of disrupter type on T 50, F (1,7) = 2.80, p =.14. Finally, the interaction of disrupter type and disrupter presentation, F (1,7) = 0.87, p =.38, was not significant, which shows that T 50 did not vary differentially by the two factors. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA for ε revealed no main effect of disrupter presentation, F(1,7) = 0.39, p =.55, which shows that sensitivity was not affected by introduction of disrupter trials. There was no main effect of disrupter type on ε, F(1,7) = 0.02, p =.89, which shows that sensitivity of timing was not affected by food availability. Finally, the interaction of disrupter type and disrupter presentation was not significant,

31 21 Table 2 Descriptive Statistics for Nonnormalized and Normalized T 50, ε, and Range by Condition Condition With food parameters and range Nonnormalized Baseline Disruption Normalized Baseline Disruption M SEM M SEM M SEM M SEM T ε Range Without food parameters and range T ε Range F(1,7) = 0.19, p =.67), which shows that sensitivity did not vary differentially by the two factors. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA for the range revealed a significant main effect of disrupter presentation, F(1,7) = 14.38, p <.01, which shows that stimulus control for timing behavior was affected by disrupter presentations. There was no main effect of disrupter type on the range, F(1,7) = 0.08, p =.93, though, showing that there was no differential effect of food availability on stimulus control. Finally, the interaction for disrupter presentation and disrupter type was also not significant, F(1,7) = 1.73, p =.23), which shows that stimulus control did not very differentially by the two factors. Paired t tests revealed significant differences between mean range for baseline and

32 22 disruption with food: t(7) = 2.78, p =.03; and Without Food: t(7) = 4.27, p <.01; but differences between baselines, t(7) = 1.00, p =.35, and disruption types, t(7) = 0.59, p =.57, were not significant. These findings show that disrupter presentation reduced stimulus control relative to baseline. Medians and 25 th and 75 th quartiles for goodness of fit (R 2 ) are shown in Table 3. A Friedman nonparametric ANOVA revealed a significant effect of median goodness of fit (R 2 ) between the baseline and disrupter conditions, χ 2 (3) = 12.75, p <.01. Wilcoxon analyses revealed this to be an effect of reduced R 2 in disruption relative to baseline performance, with significant differences between baseline and disruption With Food R 2 (z = 2.52, p =.01), and Without Food: (z = 2.10, p =.04). There were no significant differences for the goodness of fit for Equation 1 between baselines (z = 0.84, p =.40) or disruption types (z = 0.98, p =.40). Normalized Measures and Parameters The mean T 50, ε, and range for normalized proportion of long responses are shown in Table 2. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant differences Table 3 Descriptive Statistics for Nonnormalized and Normalized Goodness of Fit by Condition Nonnormalized Baseline Disruption Normalized Baseline Disruption Goodness of fit Med 25 th /75th Med 25 th /75th Med 25 th /75th Med 25 th /75th With food R / / / /0.63 Without food R / / / /0.62

33 23 between disrupter types and baseline for T 50. There was a significant main effect for disrupter presentation on T 50, F (1,7) = 21.17, p <.01, which means that the disrupter presentation resulted in a rightward shift of the timing function. There was also a significant main effect for disrupter type on T 50, F (1,7) = 6.56, p =.04, showing that manipulation of food availability produced a rightward shift in the function relative to the without food disrupter condition. Finally, the interaction of disrupter type and disrupter presentation on T 50, F (1,7) = 1.16, p =.32, was not significant, which shows that T 50 did not vary differentially by the two factors. Planned t test comparisons on T 50 for the normalized data showed that the means for baseline T 50 differed from disruption in the with food condition, t(7) = 3.82, p <.01. Comparisons of T 50 for baseline and disruption without food, t(7) = 1.53, p =.17; baselines, t(7) = 2.08, p =.07; and disrupter types, t(7) = 1.31, p =.23, were not significantly different. Overall, these findings show that food availability in a context of disruption caused underestimation of the interval to be timed to occur. A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA for ε revealed no main effect of disrupter presentation, F(1,7) = 3.01, p =.13, which shows that sensitivity was not affected by introduction of disrupter trials. There was no main effect of disrupter type on ε, F(1,7) = 0.17, p =.69, which shows that sensitivity of timing was not affected by food availability. Finally, the interaction of disrupter type and disrupter presentation on ε, was not significant F(1,7) = 0.09, p =.77, which shows that sensitivity did not vary differentially by the two factors. Medians and 25 th and 75 th quartiles for goodness of fit (R 2 ) are shown in Table 4.

34 24 Table 4 Medians and 25th and 75th Quartiles of Latency to Peck Trial-Ready or Choice Stimuli by Condition With food Without food Condition Trial-ready Choice Trial-ready Choice Baseline Median th /75 th 1.07/ / / /4.20 Disruption Median th /75 th 1.21/ / / /4.03 A Friedman nonparametric ANOVA did not reveal significant differences between median goodness of fit (R 2 ) for Equation 1 for the baseline and disrupter conditions (χ 2 (3) = 2.55, p =.47). This shows that normalization improved the degree of fit of Equation 1 to the proportion of long response data. Latencies Table 4 shows median seconds between stimulus presentation and pecking for trial-ready and comparison keys. Latencies were fairly stable across conditions. Friedman ANOVAs for latencies did not reveal significant differences between baseline and disrupter sessions latencies to peck (trial-ready: χ 2 (3) = 4.42, p =.22, choice: χ 2 (3) = 6.13, p =.10).

35 25 Response and Food Presentation Rates Responses per minute during RR disrupter presentation for each disrupter condition was fairly stable across the with food (M = 63.80, SEM = 8.59) and without food (M = 55.83, SEM = 6.40) conditions; a paired t test did not show significant differences between disrupter conditions on response rates during RR stimulus presentation, t(18) = 1.14, p =.27. Total number of hopper presentations per session during RR disrupter presentation for each disrupter condition was fairly stable across the with food (M = 0.72, SEM =.09) and without food (M = 0.69, SEM = 0.12) conditions; a paired t test did not show significant differences between disrupter conditions on number of hopper presentations, t(9) = 0.23, p =.82.

36 26 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to determine whether the availability of food following the presentation of a disrupter stimulus affects the performance of pigeons in the interval bisection procedure. The lighting of food-associated disrupter keys during the sample duration of probe sessions reduced the accuracy of responses during the choice phase, so that the range of the psychophysical function was reduced. Normalization of the data to account for a reduction in range revealed a shift in the mean of the function relative to baseline for the condition in which food was available for accurate performance following disrupter presentation. This shift in the mean of the function was not present in the condition where food was not available following disrupter presentation. These findings show that pigeons underestimated the passage of time when food was available for accurate responses, but stimulus control of timing behavior was reduced in both the with food and without food disrupter conditions. In the timing literature, reports of underestimation of the interval-to-be-timed as a function of disrupter presentation have been countered by reports of loss of temporal stimulus control. For example, Lejeune and colleagues (1999) disrupted temporal performance in an interval bisection task by presenting the opportunity to respond on a variable-ratio schedule of reinforcement during the interval-to-be-timed. They found that disrupter presentation caused underestimation of the interval-to-be-timed relative to baseline measurements. Sutton and Roberts (2002) examined the effect of the presentation of a house light during the interval-to-be-timed and found loss of temporal

37 27 stimulus control. The difference between studies that report underestimation as a function of disrupter presentation and those that report loss of stimulus control may be procedural, in the availability of food for appropriate responses within the disrupter trial. For example, Lejeune and colleagues sometimes made food available for correct temporal responses following disrupter presentation, whereas food was never available in the disrupter trials reported by Sutton and Roberts. When food is available for correct timing responses within an interval bisection preparation, attention to the comparisons may be increased, thereby increasing the likelihood that a temporal distortion, if present, would produce measurably different behavior. The purpose of this study was to manipulate food availability for appropriate responses following disrupter presentations. Presentation of the disrupter stimulus was expected to cause underestimation of the interval-to-be-timed in the with food condition. When food was available for accurate responses following the presentation of the RR stimulus, temporal stimulus control was generally lost. Loss of temporal stimulus control obscured the presence of a rightward shift of the timing function, however, as normalization of the proportion of long responses for during disruption with food revealed a modest rightward shift of T 50 relative to baseline. Presentation of the disrupter stimulus was expected to cause a loss of temporal stimulus control in the without food disrupter condition. Disrupter presentation resulted in loss of stimulus control in both with and without food disrupter conditions.

38 28 Normalization of the proportion of long responses for the without food disrupter condition did not reveal any shift of T 50 relative to baseline, which supports the hypothesis that food unavailability following disrupter presentation would reduce stimulus control without accompanying distortion of the timing process. Presentation of the disrupter stimulus reduced the range of the proportion of long responses relative to baseline. Reduction of the range as a function of disrupter presentation has been reported previously in similar circumstances (Sutton & Roberts, 2002; Ward & Odum, 2007) and is generally considered loss of temporal stimulus control. Both with food and without food disrupter sessions produced relatively equal reductions in the range. Reductions in the range can affect interpretation of timing, so data were normalized. The normalized proportion of long responses revealed that the availability of food in disrupter sessions produced a slight rightward shift relative to baseline. Shifts in the proportion of long response data as a function of disrupter stimulus presentation have been previously reported (Lejeune et al., 1999), and are considered examples of underestimation. Underestimation in the current experiment only occurred during the disrupter sessions in which food was available for accurate responses following the disrupter presentation, as predicted by the hypothesis. Disrupter presentations may stop, reset, or restart timing processes, but the interval bisection procedure allows separation of attention to the temporal cue from attention to the choice, which is necessary for that distortion to be measured. In this experiment, attention to the choice keys was manipulated by making food available for correct responses following disrupter

39 29 presentation. The results of the present experiment imply that the choice of pecking long or short is dependent upon an attentional process, whereby a temporal response is based upon whether it has been associated with a biologically salient event (i.e., food). If food is not paired with accurate performance in a context of disruption, loss of stimulus control is a likely outcome. However, if food is associated with accurate performance in a context of disruption, the duration of the stimulus presentation may inform a temporal decision more clearly. Due to potentially artifactual effects of range on T 50 and ε, a normalization procedure was implemented on the proportion long response data. McClure and colleagues (2005) studied the effects of d-amphetamine on pigeon behavior within the interval bisection task. Using a four-parameter model, they found dose-dependent reductions in the range of the proportion of short and long responses, but no systematic effect on the mean of the function. Two-parameter analyses of proportion long data, however, revealed an apparent leftward shift as a function of dose, due to an inability to account for the effect of range. To reduce any artifactual effects on T 50 and ε, Odum and Ward (2007) instituted the normalization procedure described in the present experiment when examining the effects of d-amphetamine administration on pigeons within two versions of the interval bisection task. A normalization procedure to reduce artifactual effects of range on T 50 and ε was not performed by Lejeune and colleagues (1999) or Sutton and Roberts (2002). Lejeune and colleagues found that the placement of a VR task within probe sample intervals produced underestimation relative to baseline. The Wilcoxon test was used to compare

40 30 baseline and disruption percentage long response data for each individual bird. Wilcoxon analysis revealed a tendency to peck short where differences between baseline and disruption conditions were significant at the 10-, 20-, and 25-s sample intervals (the three shortest), but not at the longest (40 s). Sutton and Roberts (2002) found that house light presentation reduced the range of the proportion of long responses during probe trials. An ANOVA revealed an interaction between sample duration and condition,and a t test on the slope of a regression revealed a significant difference. Limitations of the current study include the lack of the manipulation of disrupter placement and longer disrupter presentation and food availability during disruption. Lejeune and colleagues (1999) manipulated disrupter placement within probe trials, with the hypothesis that relatively late disrupter presentation produced more shifts in attention relative to early presentation, causing greater timing distortion. Manipulation of placement was not included in the current experiment due to the necessity of relatively brief sample durations and food availability. Training longer sample durations could facilitate manipulation of disrupter placement, which would be informative regarding the effect of compounding shifts of attention on timing.

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 8-2012 Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Meredith

More information

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2008 Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

The perception of empty and filled time intervals by pigeons

The perception of empty and filled time intervals by pigeons pqjb04b-06.qxd 12/29/04 11:00 AM Page 31 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2005, 58B (1), 31 45 The perception of empty and filled time intervals by pigeons Andrew Miki and Angelo Santi

More information

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-009 The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Wesley Paul Thomas Utah

More information

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a Effects of Food Deprivation on Memory Performance. Jacob. L. Kerewaro Abstract The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Learn Behav. 2009 November ; 37(4): 357 364. doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.357. Behavioral Momentum and Relapse of Extinguished Operant Responding

More information

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 348 370 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Chris Hope and Angelo Santi Wilfrid

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

Instrumental Conditioning I

Instrumental Conditioning I Instrumental Conditioning I Basic Procedures and Processes Instrumental or Operant Conditioning? These terms both refer to learned changes in behavior that occur as a result of the consequences of the

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, & JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR1 1982, 38, 71-85 NUMBER I (JULY) MEMORY FOR RECENT BEHAVIOR IN THE PIGEON STEPHEN P. KRAMER CORRECTIONS DIVISION, DRAPER, UTAH Variations of the symbolic

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand).

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). http://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/ Research Commons at the University of Waikato Copyright Statement: The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). The thesis

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 122 2001 Participating Faculty Professor James Dickson (dickson@stolaf.edu) Professor Dana Gross (grossd@stolaf.edu)

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December 2008 APPROVED:

More information

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Summer 2005 Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli William

More information

Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers

Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2008 Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers Christopher Aaron Podlesnik

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE Isabelle Guay and Simon Grondin Université Laval, Québec, Canada Email: simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca ABSTRACT

More information

Psychology Research Process

Psychology Research Process Psychology Research Process Logical Processes Induction Observation/Association/Using Correlation Trying to assess, through observation of a large group/sample, what is associated with what? Examples:

More information

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Elizabeth M. Brannon*, Courtney J. Wusthoff #, C.R. Gallistel^, and John Gibbon @ * Center for Cognitive Neuroscience

More information

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(4),621-629 Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives THOMAS R. ZENTALL University ofkentucky, Lexington,

More information

Examining the Effects of Reinforcement Context on Relapse of Observing

Examining the Effects of Reinforcement Context on Relapse of Observing Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2011 Examining the Effects of Reinforcement Context on Relapse of Observing Eric A. Thrailkill Utah State

More information

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A.

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 89 (2012) 212 218 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes j o ur nal homep age : www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Providing a reinforcement history

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action Categorization and Discrimination Animals respond to stimuli in ways that suggest they form categories. Pigeons

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 261-268 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) CONCURRENT SCHEULES OF PRIMARY AN CONITIONE REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' ONAL W. ZIMMERMAN CARLETON UNIVERSITY Rats responded

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Learning and Motivation 39 (2008) 71 94 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Luís Oliveira,

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement Learning & Behavior 2005, 33 (3), 309-323 Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement GLENN S. BROWN and K. GEOFFREY WHITE University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand In two experiments, pigeons responding

More information

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 11, 79-91 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2010) 79 Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats Rogelio Escobar National Autonomous University of Mexico

More information

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 74-84 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.74 Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure PATRICK VAN ROOYEN AND ANGELO

More information

Extension of Behavioral Momentum Theory to Conditions with Changing Reinforcer Rates

Extension of Behavioral Momentum Theory to Conditions with Changing Reinforcer Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2017 Extension of Behavioral Momentum Theory to Conditions with Changing Reinforcer Rates Andrew R. Craig

More information

Designing Psychology Experiments: Data Analysis and Presentation

Designing Psychology Experiments: Data Analysis and Presentation Data Analysis and Presentation Review of Chapter 4: Designing Experiments Develop Hypothesis (or Hypotheses) from Theory Independent Variable(s) and Dependent Variable(s) Operational Definitions of each

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION METHOD ON ACQUISITION SPEED OF AN IDENTITY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASK

EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION METHOD ON ACQUISITION SPEED OF AN IDENTITY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASK EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION METHOD ON ACQUISITION SPEED OF AN IDENTITY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASK A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Stanislaus In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

More information

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 3-2016 Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Andrew R. Craig Utah State University

More information

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 12-1991 Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio

More information

Designing Psychology Experiments: Data Analysis and Presentation

Designing Psychology Experiments: Data Analysis and Presentation Data Analysis and Presentation Review of Chapter 4: Designing Experiments Develop Hypothesis (or Hypotheses) from Theory Independent Variable(s) and Dependent Variable(s) Operational Definitions of each

More information

The Effect of Retention Interval on Temporal Control of Responding in Rats on the Peak Interval Procedure

The Effect of Retention Interval on Temporal Control of Responding in Rats on the Peak Interval Procedure City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Graduate Center 9-2015 The Effect of Retention Interval on Temporal Control of Responding in Rats on

More information

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l Journal oj Experimental Psychology 12, Vol. 63, No. 3, 289-293 REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l MAURICE HERSHENSON 2 Brooklyn College In measuring reaction time (RT) to simultaneously

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? Keyed. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 55B (1), 1 25 Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? J.H. Wearden, A. Parry, and L. Stamp University of

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching

Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (2), 189 196 Coding of hedonic and nonhedonic samples by pigeons in many-to-one delayed matching THOMAS R. ZENTALL and LOU M. SHERBURNE University of Kentucky, Lexington,

More information

Time Interval Estimation: Internal Clock or Attentional Mechanism?

Time Interval Estimation: Internal Clock or Attentional Mechanism? Time Interval Estimation: Internal Clock or Attentional Mechanism? Niels Taatgen 1,2 (taatgen@cmu.edu), John Anderson 1 (ja@cmu.edu), Daniel Dickison 1 (danieldickison@cmu.edu) and Hedderik van Rijn 2

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques C O N F I D E N T I A L Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques DATE This study was conducted under the terms of a Materials Transfer and Services Agreement between NeuroDetective International and

More information

Conditioned Reinforcement and the Value of Praise in Children with Autism

Conditioned Reinforcement and the Value of Praise in Children with Autism Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2014 Conditioned Reinforcement and the Value of Praise in Children with Autism Ben Beus Utah State University

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) 1 Outline Method of Adjustment Method of Limits Method of Constant Stimuli Probit Analysis 2 Method of Constant Stimuli A set of equally spaced levels of the stimulus

More information

UNIVERSITY OF IOWA AND SOUTHERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY AT CARBONDALE

UNIVERSITY OF IOWA AND SOUTHERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY AT CARBONDALE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2002, 78, 365 373 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) BRIEF PRESENTATIONS ARE SUFFICIENT FOR PIGEONS TO DISCRIMINATE ARRAYS OF SAME AND DIFFERENT STIMULI EDWARD A. WASSERMAN,

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez The Psychological Record, 28, 58, 57 528 Some Effects of Magnitude of Reinforcement on Persistence of Responding Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman The University of Minnesota Angel Jimenez The University

More information

Chapter 12 Behavioral Momentum Theory: Understanding Persistence and Improving Treatment

Chapter 12 Behavioral Momentum Theory: Understanding Persistence and Improving Treatment Chapter 12 Behavioral Momentum Theory: Understanding Persistence and Improving Treatment Christopher A. Podlesnik and Iser G. DeLeon 12.1 Introduction Translational research in behavior analysis aims both

More information

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2009, Vol. 35, No. 3, 293 311 2009 American Psychological Association 0097-7403/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013926 Resistance to Change Within

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION By JIN HO YOON A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY

More information

Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness

Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2013 Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness Kerin A. Weingarten University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Follow this

More information

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, 152 162 2329-8456/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000053 Memory Systems

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand).

The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). http://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/ Research Commons at the University of Waikato Copyright Statement: The digital copy of this thesis is protected by the Copyright Act 1994 (New Zealand). The thesis

More information

Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation

Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation Emily A. Williams (Emily.A.Williams@Manchester.ac.uk) Andrew J. Stewart (Andrew.J.Stewart@Manchester.ac.uk) Luke A. Jones (Luke.Jones@Manchester.ac.uk)

More information

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1

Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1 Japanese Psychological Research 1998, Volume 40, No. 2, 104 110 Short Report Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem

Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem Animal Learning & Behavior 1981,9 (4),581-586 Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem DAVID R. THOMAS, ALAN R. McKELVIE, MICHAEL RANNEY, and THOMAS B. MOYE University ofcolorado,

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific?

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Temporal Binding and Internal Clocks: Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Richard Fereday (FeredayR@cardiff.ac.uk), Marc J. Buehner (BuehnerM@cardiff.ac.uk) Cardiff University, School of Psychology,

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

Extinction. n Operant Extinction: n Ideally combined with DRO (esp. DRI) n No longer reinforcing operant behavioral response

Extinction. n Operant Extinction: n Ideally combined with DRO (esp. DRI) n No longer reinforcing operant behavioral response Extinction Extinction n Operant Extinction: n No longer reinforcing operant behavioral response n Ultimately reducing B s probability to zero n Ideally combined with DRO (esp. DRI) n Initial & Temporary

More information

Name Psychophysical Methods Laboratory

Name Psychophysical Methods Laboratory Name Psychophysical Methods Laboratory 1. Classical Methods of Psychophysics These exercises make use of a HyperCard stack developed by Hiroshi Ono. Open the PS 325 folder and then the Precision and Accuracy

More information

Journal of Comparative Psychology

Journal of Comparative Psychology Journal of Comparative Psychology Change Detection and Change Blindness in Pigeons (Columba livia) Walter T. Herbranson, Yvan T. Trinh, Patricia M. Xi, Mark P. Arand, Michael S. K. Barker, and Theodore

More information

Interval timing in genetically modified mice: a simple paradigm

Interval timing in genetically modified mice: a simple paradigm Genes, Brain and Behavior (7) # 7 The Authors Journal compilation # 7 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Techniques Interval timing in genetically modified mice: a simple paradigm F. Balci*,, E. B. Papachristos,

More information

Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach

Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach PQJE178995 TECHSET COMPOSITION LTD, SALISBURY, U.K. 6/16/2006 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 0000, 00 (0), 1 17 Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection

More information

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation The description and presentation of results involves a number of topics. These include scales of measurement, descriptive statistics used to summarize

More information

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented 1 Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample A Thesis Presented by Kimberly M. Walter The Department of Counseling and Applied

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Perception of the duration of auditory and visual stimuli in children and adults

Perception of the duration of auditory and visual stimuli in children and adults Q283 QJEP(A)42 / Jun, 4 (Tue)/ [22 pages 3 Tables 3 Figures 2 Footnotes Appendices].. Centre single caption. cf. [no comma]. disk edited RJ,. Graphics from word/xcel into cdraw. Please check THE QUARTERLY

More information