University of Groningen

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "University of Groningen"

Transcription

1 University of Groningen Ultraviolet crown colouration affects contest outcomes among male blue tits, but only in the absence of prior encounters Vedder, Oscar; Schut, Elske; Magrath, Michael J. L.; Komdeur, Jan; McGraw, Kevin Published in: Functional Ecology DOI: /j x IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2010 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Vedder, O., Schut, E., Magrath, M. J. L., Komdeur, J., & McGraw, K. (Ed.) (2010). Ultraviolet crown colouration affects contest outcomes among male blue tits, but only in the absence of prior encounters. Functional Ecology, 24(2), DOI: /j x Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum. Download date:

2 Functional Ecology 2010, 24, doi: /j x Ultraviolet crown colouration affects contest outcomes among male blue tits, but only in the absence of prior encounters Oscar Vedder*,1, Elske Schut 1, Michael J. L. Magrath 1,2 and Jan Komdeur 1 1 Animal Ecology Group, Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Studies, University of Groningen, P.O. Box 14, 9750 AA Haren, The Netherlands; and 2 Department of Zoology, University of Melbourne, Vic. 3010, Australia Summary 1. Static animal colour patches may function among competitors to minimize conflict escalation over resources, by serving as a signal of resource holding potential or aggressiveness. Empirical evidence for the use of colour patches in conflict resolution is largely restricted to pigment-based colours (melanins and carotenoids) and rarely defines the context in which the signals are used. 2. Here we test whether structural-based ultraviolet (UV) crown colouration functions in conflict resolution among dyads of first-year male blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus in captivity, in a context that is known to favour aggressive individuals. 3. We found that on first encounter in a pairwise context, experimentally UV-reduced males were significantly more likely to lose to control-treated opponents than expected by chance. However, this disparity was less pronounced when conflicts were settled with physical fighting or when the opponent was considerably smaller in size. 4. When the same dyads were tested again several weeks later, but with the UV treatment reversed among opponents, none of the effects remained significant, but instead the winner was most likely to be the individual that won at their first encounter. 5. Our results suggest that structural-based UV colouration can affect the outcome of an interaction, but that size differences and the outcome of initial interactions between opponents can override the influence of this signal in conflict resolution. Whether there is a functional basis to maintain a link between aggressiveness and colouration may thus be highly dependent on the general context under which individuals compete in the wild. Key-words: aggression, armament, male male competition, personality, social dominance, status signal, structural colouration Introduction Successful reproduction in animals requires the acquisition of sufficient resources, like food, nest sites, territories and mates. As these are often limited in availability, natural selection will strongly favour traits that enhance an individual s ability to obtain such resources, often at the expense of others. When two individuals compete over a resource at least three factors come into play. (i) The subjective resource value (Maynard Smith 1982). If there is an asymmetry in the perceived value of the contested resource between competitors, the individual that has more to gain (or lose) will be more prone to escalate the conflict towards actual fighting and consequently more likely to win, as its *Correspondence author. o.h.vedder@rug.nl opponent may favour avoiding the potential costs of fighting, like the risk of injury, and thus retreat. Such a scenario applies, for example, when there are differences in hunger level or prior residency between opponents (e.g. Sandell & Smith 1991; Lemel & Wallin 1993). (ii) The resource holding potential (RHP) of both competitors, defined as the ability to win an all out physical contest (Parker 1974; Maynard Smith 1982). When both competitors are highly motivated, conflicts are more likely to escalate to fighting, thus favouring the individual with the higher RHP. (iii) The level of aggressiveness of both competitors. Apart from contest-specific differences in motivation, individuals may also inherently differ in their tendency to resort to costly fighting. Herein aggressive individuals are defined as individuals that possess a higher propensity to escalate contests, independent of their RHP (Barlow, Rogers & Fraley 1986; Ó 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation Ó 2009 British Ecological Society

3 418 O. Vedder et al. Maynard Smith & Harper 1988). This strategy may be particularly favourable when both competitors judge a resource to be low in value (Maynard Smith & Harper 1988). With the recent renewed interest in animal personality traits, it has become apparent that aggressiveness is an important component of a wider behavioural syndrome, variously characterized as risk-prone, unresponsive, pro-active, bold or fast exploring that has an underlying genetic basis (rev. by Sih et al. 2004). Originally Rohwer (1975, 1982) hypothesized that it would be adaptive for both individuals in a contest to exhibit their status, so as to assess each other s competitive abilities without wasting time, energy or risking injury in physical fights. Empirical studies have since attributed such status signalling functions to a wide array of colourful patches exhibited by a diverse range of animals, but especially in birds (Senar 1999) and lizards (Whiting, Nagy & Bateman 2003). However, it has become less clear which factor in determining social status (from the three discussed above) is actually signalled. Maynard Smith & Harper (1988) argued that apparently arbitrary or conventional signals (i.e. signals that are not necessarily physically or physiologically connected to RHP) can signal aggressiveness. These signals would only predict the outcome of conflicts over low-value resources, as conflicts over highvalue resources will always be settled by differences in RHP, either via corresponding signals or directly by fighting. The authors support this prediction by presenting some previously unpublished empirical results on three bird species, showing that status signals only predict contest outcome at feeding sites with a low food intake rate. Recently, empirical evidence from paper wasps Polistes dominules also suggests that signal use may be limited to conflicts in which the benefit of winning is low (Tibbetts 2008). In birds, melanin-based signals, like the black throat patch in male house sparrows (Passer domesticus; Moller 1987; Gonzalez et al. 2002; Nakagawa et al. 2007) and the black ventral breast stripe in great tits (Parus major; Wilson 1992; Lemel & Wallin 1993), may best fit the pattern expected for a signal of aggressiveness, because these generally only correlate with status when resource values are low (see Lemel & Wallin 1993 for a detailed example). The correlation with social status may be mediated by testosterone, promoting both melanin expression (Evans, Goldsmith & Norris 2000; Buchanan et al. 2001; Gonzalez et al. 2001) and aggressive behaviour (rev. by Wingfield et al. 1987). In contrast, colour patches based on carotenoid pigments, like those in widowbirds (Euplectes; Pryke et al. 2002; Pryke & Andersson 2003), red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus; Smith 1972; Peek 1972) and scarlet-tufted malachite sunbirds (Nectarinia johnstoni; Evans & Hatchwell 1992), appear to signal social status in contests over high-value resources like territories. As carotenoids can not be synthesized de novo by animals (Olson & Owens 1998), the reliability of these signals to reveal genuine RHP may be maintained by the difficulty of acquiring sufficient carotenoids in their diet (Pryke et al. 2002). Hence, carotenoid signals are most likely to reveal RHP, instead of aggressiveness, and individuals should respect these signals also when the risk of fighting is high. In addition to pigmentation, colours can also arise from the selective reflectance of light from surface structures (Land 1972). Such structures mainly reflect the green, blue, violet and the ultraviolet (UV) part of the colour spectrum (Andersson 1999; Shawkey et al. 2003; Prum 2006). There is some indication that UV-blue structural colouration may be condition dependent (Keyser & Hill 1999; McGraw et al. 2002; Siefferman & Hill 2005a) and a recent experiment revealed a positive effect of testosterone on UV reflectance in first-year male blue tits (Fig. 1; Cyanistes caeruleus; Roberts, Ras & Peters 2009). Regarding a role in conflict resolution, there is experimental evidence that UV reflectance signals status in Augrabies flat lizards (Platysaurus broadleyi; Stapley & Whiting 2006), and positive correlations between measures of social dominance and UV reflectance have been reported in birds (Keyser & Hill 2000; Siefferman & Hill 2005b; Pryke & Griffith 2006). The only experimental support for a function of UV reflectance in competition in birds comes from a study in which territorial male blue tits with fertile females acted less aggressively towards taxidermic mounts with reduced crown UV-reflectance, compared to control males (Alonso-Alvarez, Doutrelant & Sorci 2004). However, these results were not repeatable in a different population when UV-manipulated mounts were presented simultaneously instead of sequentially (Korsten, Dijkstra & Komdeur 2007). Furthermore, they can also be interpreted in a mate choice context if, for example, a UV-reduced male poses less threat to a focal male s paternity (see Delhey et al. 2007), especially as status signalling studies generally report greater aggression towards individuals with reduced signal expression (e.g. Jarvi & Bakken 1984; Pryke et al. 2002; Pryke & Andersson 2003). Nevertheless, blue tits are likely candidates for UV status signalling as they are known to display their UV reflective crown feathers in competitive interactions (Stokes 1962; Scott & Deag 1998). We tested this possibility in a previous study where the crown UV reflectance of free-living blue tits was Fig. 1. Blue tit male (photo by Sandra Bouwhuis).

4 UV status signalling in blue tits 419 reduced experimentally, but our findings failed to support a status signalling function for UV colouration, because neither status nor conflict escalation rate was affected by the manipulation (Vedder et al. 2008). However, such effects may be very difficult to detect in wild populations because of the strong site-dependent dominance structure that resident blue tits exhibit when moving around in the vicinity of their territories in winter (Colquhoun 1942; Hansen & Slagsvold 2004; Korsten et al. 2007). Consequently, the outcome of interactions may be much more significant than suggested by the value of the contested resource, as all interactions reaffirm the local territorial structure. Hence, although these previous findings can exclude UV reflectance signalling RHP, it may still act as a signal of aggressiveness. By definition, a static signal like an inconcealable plumage patch can not signal contest-specific motivation. Here we specifically test whether the UV crown plumage of blue tits serves as a signal of aggressiveness in first-year male blue tits. We experimentally manipulate the UV crown plumage to test whether UV reflectance is used as a cue to estimate an opponent s aggressiveness, and not merely a correlate of social dominance. We let the birds compete in pairwise trials over food, as such a context has been shown to favour aggressive, fast exploring, individuals in the closely related great tit (Verbeek, Boon & Drent 1996) and signal manipulations in great tits only appear to have an effect in this context (Lemel & Wallin 1993). To explore the consistency of signal use in dominance trials, we repeated all trials with reversed colour manipulations. We predict that if UV reflectance is used as a signal of aggressiveness, opponents of UV-reduced individuals would behave more aggressively towards these individuals, such that conflicts not resolved by fighting will be more often lost by the UV-reduced individuals. Methods ORIGIN AND HOUSING OF BIRDS In spring 2007 and 2008, wild nestling blue tits were taken into captivity and their sex determined from a blood sample using molecular sexing (following Griffiths et al. 1998). For the experiment, we used 22 male nestlings in 2007 and an additional 10 in In 2007, these nestlings were collected from 12 different 10-day-old broods from a wild nestbox population at the Hoge Veluwe National Park in the Netherlands (52 02 N, E) and hand reared in captivity (following Verbeek, Drent & Wiepkema 1994). In brief, the birds were placed in artificial nests in incubators and fed on a mixed diet including wax moth larvae and minced beef heart. Nestlings remained in incubators until fledged and were then placed in small cages (0Æ6 0Æ4 0Æ4 m) in groups of three to four individuals. Hand-feeding continued until they were observed to eat independently. Approximately 1 month after hatching, they were transferred to outdoor aviaries (3 2 2 m) at the Biological Centre, University of Groningen. The males were kept in groups of up to eight individuals, until we started the dominance trials. Between trials, males were housed in the same aviary with the same group of individuals. In 2008, the 10 male nestlings were taken into captivity from a wild nestbox population on the grounds of the Biological Centre, University of Groningen (53 10 N, E). Nestlings were swapped between four different nests, to create two broods that were taken into captivity. Just before fledging the two broods were placed in new nest boxes in outdoor aviaries (3 2 2 m) together with one of the parents (that was caught at the nestbox). The parents were ad libitum provided with mealworms and wax moth larvae, and supplemented with caterpillars from the wild. In both cases, the parent (one male and one female) continued feeding the nestlings in captivity. From 4 days after fledging, the parents were separated from their brood for part of the day, to stimulate the fledglings to forage independently. Initially separation lasted for 2 h but this period was increased progressively until the fledglings were completely independent (24 days after fledging). At this time, the parents were released and the male fledglings kept under the same conditions as described above for the 2007 cohort. PAIRWISE DOMINANCE TRIALS The first rounds of pairwise dominance trials took place in early November (in both 2007 and 2008). During each round of trials, half of all first-year males were randomly assigned to a UV-reduction treatment, whereas the other half was control treated (see below). The UV treatments of males were evenly distributed over the outdoor aviaries where they were kept before and between the rounds of trials, and randomly distributed with regard to their nest of origin in the wild. Pairwise dominance trials were staged such that, during each round, every individual competed with at least two individuals from a different aviary (i.e. unfamiliar) and two individuals from the same aviary (i.e. familiar), one each day, and always against an opponent of the opposite treatment. Hence, in one round all first-year males competed at least four times, against four different opponents of the opposite treatment. In November 2007, this was achieved by performing 47 trials involving the 22 males that hatched in spring In November 2008, we performed 25 trials involving the 10 males that hatched in spring In both winters, the trials were repeated in early January (in after 51 days, in after 49 days) with the exact same combinations of opponents (hereafter referred to as dyads ), but with the UV-reduction and control treatments reversed for all individuals. This allowed sufficient time for the original treatments in November to wear off. One day prior to each round of trials, all males were removed from their aviaries, measured and treated as described below, and placed in one compartment of a cage that consisted of two compartments equally divided by a sliding wooden panel. Compartments (0Æ4 0Æ4 0Æ4 m, following Verbeek, Boon & Drent 1996) were fitted with a perch and birds had ad libitum access to food and water, and were visually isolated from each other. These cages were placed outside near their original aviaries, such that the light dark cycle was unchanged and the birds could observe each other under natural light conditions during the trials. The following morning dominance was assessed by removing the slide between the compartments for 20 min. During this period all food was removed, except for a small bowl of commercially available dried insect food (Witte Molen, Meeuwen, the Netherlands), placed in the centre of the cage. Hence, the contested resource was of only low value to both individuals, which should favour the individuals that perceive their opponent as risk-averse or non-aggressive. In the 20 min. period, we scored the outcome of all competitive interactions, either over food or space, and noted whether they involved physical fighting. It was impossible to clearly separate interactions over food from interactions over personal space, since they generally did not tolerate each other in close proximity

5 420 O. Vedder et al. (<10 cm, O. Vedder & E. Schut, personal observations). In most cases at least one individual acted aggressively (displaying, chasing), after which the opponent responded submissively (fleeing, crouching). Interactions occasionally escalated to a few brief physical fights (on average 1Æ38 ± 0Æ31 SE fights, in trials where fighting occurred) after which one individual retreated and behaved submissively. Individuals were recognizable by colourbands and a single interaction was won if the opponent fled or crouched. All observations were made from a distance of about 3 m. After 20 min, the slide was replaced and in the afternoon the individuals were relocated to compartments adjacent to their next opponent in preparation for the following trial. Reflectance (%) MORPHOLOGICAL AND COLOUR MEASUREMENTS In both winters, the males were measured 1 day before the start of each round of trials (November and January). Body mass was measured to the nearest 0Æ1 g with a spring balance (Pesola, Baar, Switzerland). Tarsus-length was measured to the nearest 0Æ1 mmwith a sliding calliper, and length of the third (outermost) primary feather (hereafter referred to as P-3 ) to the nearest 0Æ5 mm with a ruler. The spectral reflectance of the crown feathers was measured with an USB-2000 spectrophotometer with illumination from a DH-2000 deuterium-halogen light source (both Avantes, Eerbeek, the Netherlands). The measuring probe was held at a right angle against the plumage, i.e. both illumination and recording were at an angle of 90 to the feathers. Five replicate readings were taken from the centre of the crown for each individual, by lifting and replacing the probe between each reading. These five readings were smoothed by calculating the running mean over 10 nm, and the average of the most commonly used colour index (i.e. UV chroma ) in UV colour signalling studies calculated, by dividing the sum of reflectance between 320 and 400 nm by the sum of reflectance between 320 and 700 nm (R R ). All second measurements (in January) of body mass, tarsus-length, P-3-length and UV chroma were highly repeatable, within individuals, with the initial measurements in November (Table 1). The UV-reduction treatment (following Johnsen et al. 2005) consisted of painting the UV-blue crown feathers with a dark blue (colour code 003) Edding 4500 T-shirt marker pen (Ahrensburg, Germany). The control treatment consisted of the same procedure, but with a light blue pen (colour code 010). To confirm the direct effect of both treatments, three replicate crown reflectance readings were taken directly after the treatment, following the protocol described above. The UV-reduction treatment decreased the UV chroma index by an average ± SE of 10Æ4 ± 0Æ94% (Fig. 2), which was highly significant 5 Original Control UV reduced Wavelength (nm) Fig. 2. Reflectance plotted against wavelength of the original crown colouration of 32 males (30 measured twice and two measured once) and their colouration after control treatment (32 males measured once) and after the UV-reduction treatment (31 males measured once). Standard errors are presented at 20-nm intervals. The dotted reference line at 400 nm represents the distinction between the human ( nm) and bird ( nm) visible range in colour spectrum. (t paired =10Æ82, n =30, P <0Æ001). The control treatment increased UV chroma by an average ± SE of only 0Æ3 ±0Æ59% (Fig. 2), which was not significant (t paired = )0Æ58, n =31, P = 0Æ568). The treatments were re-applied to all individuals every afternoon before the next dominance trial. Random sampling of some individuals some weeks later suggested that the treatment lasted a few weeks, confirming that UV reflectance was reduced during the trials (O. Vedder, personal observations). There were no significant initial differences between the group of males that was UV reduced and the group that was control treated in any of the traits (tarsus-length, P-3- length, body mass, original UV chroma) measured before the first round of dominance trials (Table 2). As suggested by the high repeatability of these traits, there were also no significant differences in these traits between treatment groups before the second round of trials (all P >0Æ40). Table 1. Estimates and test statistics for within-individual repeatability of trait measurements before the first (November) and second (January) experimental round Dependent variable Repeatability (±SE) n (measured twice) F P Tarsus-length 0Æ889 (±0Æ04) 31 19Æ98 <0Æ001 P-3-length 0Æ968 (±0Æ01) 31 61Æ82 <0Æ001 Body mass 0Æ676 (±0Æ10) 31 5Æ17 <0Æ001 UV chroma 0Æ659 (±0Æ10) 30 4Æ87 <0Æ001 Repeatability estimates were derived according to Lessells & Boag (1987) and the standard errors according to Becker (1984). Table 2. Summary of independent t-tests for pre-manipulation differences in morphological traits between the group of males that were UV reduced and the group of males that were control treated in the first round Dependent variable Mean Control n UV reduced Control UV reduced Tarsus-length (mm) 17Æ38 17Æ Æ48 0Æ636 P-3-length (mm) 51Æ63 51Æ Æ35 0Æ727 Body mass (g) 11Æ49 11Æ )0Æ66 0Æ512 UV chroma (%) 29Æ77 30Æ )0Æ38 0Æ709 t P

6 UV status signalling in blue tits 421 DATA AND ANALYSES In total, we performed 72 trials in the first round (both years combined), of which 68 dyads were repeated with reversed treatments in the second round (one male from 2007 died between rounds so we could not repeat four dyads). An individual was assigned as the winner of the trial if it won significantly more interactions than its opponent, based on a binomial probability test of all interactions within the 20 min trial period. In 21 trials (11 in the first round and 10 in the second round) no significant winner could be assigned, and these trials were scored as draws and omitted from the analyses. Draws resulted mainly from the lack of aggression from both opponents, as an average of only 12Æ7 (range: 0 46) interactions were observed in these trials, compared with an average of 24Æ5 (range: 5 87) interactions in trials with a clear winner. In trials with a clear outcome, on average 23Æ9 ±1Æ56 SE of interactions were won by the winner and 0Æ6 ±0Æ15 SE by the loser. We tested if the probability of UV-reduced individuals winning trials deviated from random (0Æ50) using generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) with a binomial error distribution and a logit link function. As the probability of an individual winning an interaction is not independent of the chance that the opponent loses, we only analysed interactions from the perspective of the UV-reduced individuals. In this way we avoided pseudo-replication, as each interaction was only counted as a single datapoint, and the outcome of an interaction can be analysed as a binary response (i.e. win or lose). To account for repeated interactions of UV-reduced individuals and opponents, we included both the identity of all UV-reduced males and their identity when they acted as opponents (control treated) as cross-classified random effects. Categorical fixed effects added to the initial model were year (the winter in which the trials were performed), familiarity (yes, if they came from the same aviary), round (first or second occasion the dyad was tested) and fighting (yes, if physical fighting took place within the trial period). We also included the interaction between familiarity and round as the effect of familiarity may be different in the second round because all males had already competed with their opponents in the first round. Covariates added to the initial model were the difference in tarsuslength, the difference in P-3-length, the difference in body mass and the difference in original UV chroma between the UV-reduced individual and its control-treated opponent. In all cases, positive values indicate a greater absolute value for the UV-reduced individual. We chose to use differences instead of absolute values as the difference between opponents is more biologically meaningful and this way the model constant reflects the probability of winning for an UVreduced individual when there are no other differences with its opponent. We failed to measure the spectral reflectance of one male s crown before treatment in the first round, so models including this term are based on four fewer trials. To account for intercorrelatedness between the size-variables (i.e. differences in tarsus-length, P-3-length and bodymass), we included only one of these covariates at a time in three initial models. The final model that included the most significant of these size variables was subsequently expanded with the other two size variables, following the same procedure, to test for possible independent effects of these variables. Final models were obtained by backward stepwise removal of nonsignificant terms. Significance (P < 0Æ05, two-tailed) was assessed using the Wald statistic. Non-significance was confirmed by re-entering the term in the final model. The GLMMs were fitted using the Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) procedure in MlwiN 2.02 (Rasbash et al. 2004). All other analyses were performed in SPSS 14.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Illinois, USA). Results Analyses of both rounds combined showed that UV-reduced individuals in the first round were significantly less likely to win than the UV-reduced individuals in the second round (see the effect of round in Table 3). As the constant represents the first round probability of winning for the UV-reduced individuals, this probability was significantly less than the random expectation of 0Æ50 (Table 3). However, this probability significantly increased in cases (n = 12) where physical fighting occurred during conflict resolution, and when the UV-reduced individual was considerably larger (as represented by a greater tarsus-length) than its opponent (Table 3). Year and familiarity did not affect the probability of an UV-reduced individual winning, nor did the difference in P-3-length, body mass or original UV chroma (Table 3). When entered as the only size-variable in the model (i.e. without difference in tarsus-length), neither difference in P-3-length or difference in bodymass was significant (P >0Æ15 for both). For closer examination, we subsequently analysed each round separately in initial models that included only the significant terms from the overall model presented above (i.e. difference in tarsus-length and occurrence of physical fighting). The final model for the first round confirmed that UV-reduced individuals had a lower than random chance of winning (24Æ6% of conflicts were won by the UV-reduced individual (n = 61); constant ± SE = )1Æ550±0Æ77, v 2 =4Æ10, d.f. = 1, P =0Æ043) and the positive effect of a greater tarsus-length (coefficient ± SE = 2Æ395 ± 1Æ15, v 2 =4Æ33, d.f. = 1, P =0Æ037) (see Fig. 3 for visualization of these logistic effects). Although the occurrence of physical fighting was rejected from the model, inclusion of this term in the final model did suggest that the probability of UV-reduced individuals winning tended to increase if physical fighting was involved in conflict resolution (coefficient ± SE = 3Æ213 ± 1Æ85, v 2 =3Æ02, d.f. = 1, P =0Æ082). The final model for the second round yielded no significant effects (all P > 0Æ10). However, the outcomes in the second round were very much Table 3. Model summary of generalized linear mixed model testing for effects on the probability of an UV-reduced individual winning a 20 min dominance trial against a control treated opponent Explanatory variable Coefficient (±SE) v2 Dd.f. P Accepted Constant )1Æ982 (±0Æ73) 7Æ35 1 0Æ007 Round (first = ref) 3Æ047 (±1Æ04) 8Æ61 1 0Æ003 Fighting (no = ref) 2Æ224 (±1Æ06) 4Æ39 1 0Æ036 Diff. in tarsus-length 1Æ646 (±0Æ73) 5Æ09 1 0Æ024 Rejected Year )0Æ506 (±0Æ99) 0Æ26 1 0Æ609 Familiarity (no = ref) 0Æ751 (±0Æ56) 1Æ78 1 0Æ182 Familiarity round 0Æ207 (±1Æ12) 0Æ03 1 0Æ854 Diff. in P-3-length 0Æ293 (±0Æ25) 1Æ38 1 0Æ240 Diff. in body mass 0Æ190 (±0Æ48) 0Æ16 1 0Æ689 Diff. in UV chroma 0Æ021 (±0Æ12) 0Æ03 1 0Æ862

7 422 O. Vedder et al. Probability UV-reduced winning Difference in tarsus-length Fig. 3. Probability of UV-reduced males winning dominance trials against control-treated opponents in relation to the difference in their tarsus-length (positive values indicate a greater tarsus for the UVreduced male) in the first round of trials. Logistic model prediction (solid line) is based on real conflict outcomes (binary datapoints). Overlapping datapoints are placed on the top of each other. Dotted reference lines indicate the random winning probability (0Æ5 ony-axis) and the estimated probability when there is no difference in tarsuslength (0Æ0 onx-axis). predicted by the outcomes in the first round, as the same male won in 46 of 52 cases (88Æ5%) where there was a clear winner in the same dyad in both rounds. The significantly greater probability of winning for controltreated individuals in the first round, combined with the high repeatability of winner identity between rounds, would suggest that UV-reduced males should have been more likely to win contests in the second round, as the second round consisted of the same dyads as in the fist round, with only the UV treatment reversed among opponents. However, because this was not the case [67Æ2% of conflicts were won by the UV-reduced individual (n = 58); constant ± SE = 1Æ175 ± 0Æ78, v 2 =2Æ28, d.f. = 1, P =0Æ131], most of the few changes in winner identity between rounds must have been in favour of the individual that was control treated in the second round. Indeed, in 83Æ3% (n = 6) of dyads where the loser of the first round became the winner in the second round, the individual lost when UV reduced, but won when control treated. In 53Æ8% (n =13)of dyads in which there was a draw in one round, the winner of the other round was control treated. Three dyads ended in a draw in both rounds, and four dyads were only performed once (two wins of control and two draws), due to the death of one male between rounds. Discussion Here we show that on first encounter in a pairwise setting, first-year male blue tits with reduced crown UV reflectance lose significantly more interactions to control treated males than predicted by chance. However, when the UV-reduced male was of larger size (as indicated by a greater tarsuslength) than its opponent, or when conflicts were settled by physical fighting, its probability of winning increased significantly. When the same combinations of opponents interacted again in the same pairwise setting c. 50 days later, but with the UV treatment reversed, none of the above mentioned effects remained significant. Instead, the outcome in the first round appeared to be a strong predictor of the outcome in the second round. Together, these results suggest that male blue tits use multiple cues and strategies in conflict resolution. Across various taxa, body size is generally an important determinant of RHP (Kokko, Lopez-Sepulcre & Morrell 2006). A difference in size that can be easily observed by an opponent may override all other cues in conflict settlement, because actual fighting against a considerably larger opponent would risk injury with only a low probability of winning. That the effect of our signal manipulation was weaker in conflicts settled with fighting is consistent with prediction, as we did not manipulate true RHP. Thus, the role of UV crown reflectance in conflict resolution may be limited to situations where the opponents have not previously interacted, there is little difference in true RHP, and conflicts have a low probability to escalate to actual fighting. This situation would seem uncommon among territorial blue tits in the wild and thus may account for the absence of an effect of UV manipulation on social status in a previous study of free-living blue tits (Vedder et al. 2008). Instead, our results suggest that crown UV reflectance may be used as a signal of aggressiveness. Although there is debate over whether such signals are evolutionary stable (Owens & Hartley 1991; Johnstone & Norris 1993; Hurd 2006), a signal that is only respected when the value of the contested resource is low conforms to expectations of signals for aggressiveness (Maynard Smith & Harper 1988). Respecting such signals will reduce the risk of injury in conflicts over low value resources, whereas misleading signalling of aggressiveness will only increase the probability of conflict escalation for risk-averse individuals. Aggressive or risk-prone behaviour may yield benefits in the short term, but may compromise future returns, leading to the stable coexistence of different behavioural types in a population (Wolf et al. 2007). This would explain the occurrence of functional variation in signals that lack clear production costs, like those resulting from the presence absence of melanin pigments (e.g. Lemel & Wallin 1993; Qvarnstrom 1997; Gonzalez et al. 2002; Tibbetts & Dale 2004). Empirically this is nicely demonstrated by the observation that male house sparrows with initially large black throat patches showed increased winter survival after signal enlargement, whereas the reverse occurred among initially smallpatched males after signal enlargement (Nakagawa et al. 2008). Hence, the lack of any benefit of increased signal expression for risk-averse individuals, instead of high production costs, can functionally explain the stability of signalling aggressiveness (see also Folstad & Karter 1992 for additional testosterone-mediated trade-offs in life-history strategy).

8 UV status signalling in blue tits 423 Mechanistically, for melanin-based signals this link with aggressive, risk-prone behaviour is thought to be mediated by testosterone (Evans, Goldsmith & Norris 2000; Buchanan et al. 2001; Gonzalez et al. 2001). Interestingly, it was recently reported that testosterone stimulates the expression of crown UV reflectance in first year male blue tits, via preening behaviour (Roberts, Ras & Peters 2009), adding to previous research that showed UV reflectance to correlate positively with testosterone in first-year, but not so in older males (Peters et al. 2006). Perhaps, more UV reflective individuals receive more social stimulation, at least among recently established first-year males, because they are signalling an aggressive, risk-prone strategy. As such, the signal may also provide positive feedback for the stimulation of testosterone and thus aggressiveness (cf. McGraw, Dale & Mackillop 2003; Safran et al. 2008), further contributing to within individual behavioural consistency across contexts and time (Wolf, van Doorn & Weissing 2008). The stable site-dependent dominance structure among territorial males, as an alternative way of resolving conflicts between individuals, and the concomitant lack of function of UV reflectance in conflict settlement among wintering territorial males (Korsten et al. 2007; Vedder et al. 2008), may break down the cycle of positive feedback that signal expression can have on testosterone levels via social stimulation (Safran et al. 2008) and explain the absence of a positive association between testosterone and UV signalling in older more established males. This leads us to the absence of an effect of our UV manipulation during the second round of trials. Experimental studies of status signalling using a reciprocal design are rare, as we know of only two previous studies that applied reversed colour treatments in the same dyads of individuals (Pryke et al. 2002; Pryke & Andersson 2003). In contrast to our findings, these studies, performed in red-collared widowbirds Euplectes ardens and red-shouldered widowbirds Euplectes axillaris, did show that conflict outcomes could be reversed by reversing the treatment. However, as these signals are carotenoidbased and likely to signal true RHP, instead of aggressiveness, the cost of disregarding these signals may be too high to risk using alternative cues in conflict resolution. Capability of long-term social memory has been suggested for species in a range of taxa varying from paper wasps (Sheehan & Tibbetts 2008) to African elephants (Loxodonta africana; Hart, Hart & Pinter-Wollman 2008), and also in birds (Godard 1991). Possibly the information gained from the first encounter about the opponents aggressiveness (based on the UV signal) is retained but in subsequent encounters associated with the opponent s identity and no longer the signal per se, potentially explaining the high repeatability of conflict outcome within dyads between rounds. This would also explain the observation in wild, territorial blue tits that conflict outcomes are almost solely determined by the difference between opponents in distance from territory to the site where the conflict takes place, without obvious signs of aggression (Colquhoun 1942; Korsten et al. 2007; Vedder et al. 2008). Only individual recognition combined with sense of place would allow for such conventions to be stable, after initial settlement, without signalling of contest-specific motivation. Social information must be context-specific for this line of reasoning to hold, as otherwise the absence of an effect of familiarity (i.e. coming from the same aviary) cannot be explained. In great tits, the effect of personality on the probability of winning differs between contexts. Whereas fastexploring, more aggressive, males are favoured in a pairwise setting (Verbeek, Boon & Drent 1996), slow but thorough explorers perform better in stable hierarchies in aviaries (Verbeek et al. 1999). Similarly, the size of the black ventral breast stripe determines conflict outcome in great tit males in pairwise settings, but not in aviaries (Lemel & Wallin 1993), which was also shown for the black bib in coal tits (Periparus ater; Brotons 1998). Apparently, the kind of information that is used to settle conflicts varies depending on the social context. As such, it is possible that individuals disregard information gathered in a different context when confronted with the same opponent. Alternatively, the relatively slow recovery to original UV reflectance after the reduction treatment may have led to loser-effects (Hsu, Earley & Wolf 2006) occurring within the aviaries where the males were housed in between rounds. If such loser-effects carried through to the second round of trials, it would also explain the high repeatability in outcomes and absence of a treatment effect in the second round. However, this scenario appears unlikely because in the second winter, we did not observe any effect of the first round UVreduction treatment on dominance status within the aviaries, in between rounds (O. Vedder and Y. Roelofs, unpublished data). In conclusion, we provide evidence in birds (for lizards see: Stapley & Whiting 2006) that structural-based UV plumage is used in the settlement of conflicts, but only under specific social conditions. As the effect of our manipulation overruled differences in UV reflectance prior to manipulation, our results imply that male blue tits exhibit the behavioural plasticity to adjust their aggressive response to an opponents signal. The mechanistic link between UV reflectance and testosterone in first-year male blue tits (Peters et al. 2006; Roberts, Ras & Peters 2009) would provide a basis for some degree of within individual consistency in aggressiveness linked to signal expression, necessary for a static signal to provide information on an individuals inherent aggressiveness. However, in the wild, as individuals become locally established, individual recognition may replace the function of UV reflectance as a means to resolve conflicts, which may cause both the functional and mechanistic basis for the link between aggressiveness and colouration to deteriorate with age. Future work should aim to more accurately determine the social context in which colour signals are displayed and received. Acknowledgements We would like to thank Peter Santema and Yvonne Roelofs for practical assistance with the performance of the dominance trials. Piet Drent and Kees van

9 424 O. Vedder et al. Oers assisted in hand-rearing the nestlings in Caroline Isaksson gave advice on how to take the nestlings into captivity in Sandra Bouwhuis and two anonymous referees provided valuable comments on earlier versions of the manuscript. Permission for all procedures involved in the experiment was granted by the Animal Experiments Committee (DEC) of the University of Groningen. The research was financially supported by Grant No from the European community s sixth framework programme (FP ) to J.K. References Alonso-Alvarez, C., Doutrelant, C. & Sorci, G. (2004) Ultraviolet reflectance affects male-male interactions in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus ultramarinus). Behavioral Ecology, 15, Andersson, S. (1999) Morphology of UV reflectance in a whistling-thrush: implications for the study of structural colour signalling in birds. Journal of Avian Biology, 30, Barlow, G.W., Rogers, W. & Fraley, N. (1986) Do Midas Cichlids win through prowess or daring it depends. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 19, 1 8. Becker, W.A. (1984). A Manual of Quantitative Genetics. Academic Enterprises, Washington. Brotons, L. (1998) Status signalling in the Coal Tit (Parus ater): the role of previous knowledge of individuals. Etologia, 6, Buchanan, K.L., Evans, M.R., Goldsmith, A.R., Bryant, D.M. & Rowe, L.V. (2001) Testosterone influences basal metabolic rate in male house sparrows: a new cost of dominance signalling? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 268, Colquhoun, M.K. (1942) Notes on the social behaviour of blue tits. British Birds, 35, Delhey, K., Peters, A., Johnsen, A. & Kempenaers, B. (2007) Fertilization success and UV ornamentation in blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus: correlational and experimental evidence. Behavioral Ecology, 18, Evans, M.R., Goldsmith, A.R. & Norris, S.R.A. (2000) The effects of testosterone on antibody production and plumage coloration in male house sparrows (Passer domesticus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 47, Evans, M.R. & Hatchwell, B.J. (1992) An experimental-study of male adornment in the scarlet-tufted malachite sunbird: 1. The role of pectoral tufts in territorial defense. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 29, Folstad, I. & Karter, A.J. (1992) Parasites, bright males, and the immunocompetence handicap. American Naturalist, 139, Godard, R. (1991) Long-term-memory of individual neighbors in a migratory songbird. Nature, 350, Gonzalez, G., Sorci, G., Smith, L.C. & de Lope, F. (2001) Testosterone and sexual signalling in male house sparrows (Passer domesticus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50, Gonzalez, G., Sorci, G., Smith, L.C. & de Lope, F. (2002) Social control and physiological cost of cheating in status signalling male house sparrows (Passer domesticus). Ethology, 108, Griffiths, R., Double, M.C., Orr, K. & Dawson, R.J.G. (1998) A DNA test to sex most birds. Molecular Ecology, 7, Hansen, B.T. & Slagsvold, T. (2004) Early learning affects social dominance: interspecifically cross-fostered tits become subdominant. Behavioral Ecology, 15, Hart, B.L., Hart, L.A. & Pinter-Wollman, N. (2008) Large brains and cognition: where do elephants fit in? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, Hsu, Y.Y., Earley, R.L. & Wolf, L.L. (2006) Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: mechanisms and contest outcomes. Biological Reviews, 81, Hurd, P.L. (2006) Resource holding potential, subjective resource value, and game theoretical models of aggressiveness signalling. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 241, Jarvi, T. & Bakken, M. (1984) The function of the variation in the breast stripe of the great tit (Parus major). Animal Behaviour, 32, Johnsen, A., Delhey, K., Schlicht, E., Peters, A. & Kempenaers, B. (2005) Male sexual attractiveness and parental effort in blue tits: a test of the differential allocation hypothesis. Animal Behaviour, 70, Johnstone, R.A. & Norris, K. (1993) Badges of status and the cost of aggression. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 32, Keyser, A.J. & Hill, G.E. (1999) Condition-dependent variation in the blueultraviolet coloration of a structurally based plumage ornament. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 266, Keyser, A.J. & Hill, G.E. (2000) Structurally based plumage coloration is an honest signal of quality in male blue grosbeaks. Behavioral Ecology, 11, Kokko, H., Lopez-Sepulcre, A. & Morrell, L.J. (2006) From hawks and doves to self-consistent games of territorial behavior. American Naturalist, 167, Korsten, P., Dijkstra, T.H. & Komdeur, J. (2007) Is UV signalling involved in male-male territorial conflict in the blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus)? A new experimental approach Behaviour, 144, Korsten, P., Vedder, O., Szentirmai, I. & Komdeur, J. (2007) Absence of status signalling by structurally based ultraviolet plumage in wintering blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61, Land, M.F. (1972) The physics and biology of animal reflectors. Progress in Biophysics and Molecular Biology, 24, Lemel, J. & Wallin, K. (1993) Status signaling, motivational condition and dominance an experimental-study in the great tit, Parus major L. Animal Behaviour, 45, Lessells, C.M. & Boag, P.T. (1987) Unrepeatable repeatabilities a common mistake. Auk, 104, Maynard Smith, J Evolution and the Theory of Games. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Maynard Smith, J. & Harper, D.G.C. (1988) The evolution of aggression: can selection generate variability? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 319, McGraw, K.J., Dale, J. & Mackillop, E.A. (2003) Social environment during molt and the expression of melanin-based plumage pigmentation in male house sparrows (Passer domesticus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 53, McGraw, K.J., Mackillop, E.A., Dale, J. & Hauber, M.E. (2002) Different colors reveal different information: how nutritional stress affects the expression of melanin- and structurally based ornamental plumage. Journal of Experimental Biology, 205, Moller, A.P. (1987) Variation in badge size in male house sparrows passerdomesticus evidence for status signaling. Animal Behaviour, 35, Nakagawa, S., Ockendon, N., Gillespie, D.O.S., Hatchwell, B.J. & Burke, T. (2007) Assessing the function of house sparrows bib size using a flexible meta-analysis method. Behavioral Ecology, 18, Nakagawa, S., Lee, J.W., Woodward, B.K., Hatchwell, B.J. & Burke, T. (2008) Differential selection according to the degree of cheating in a status signal. Biology Letters, 4, Olson, V.A. & Owens, I.P.F. (1998) Costly sexual signals: are carotenoids rare, risky or required? Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 13, Owens, I.P.F. & Hartley, I.R. (1991) Trojan sparrows evolutionary consequences of dishonest invasion for the badges-of-status model. American Naturalist, 138, Parker, G.A. (1974) Assessment strategy and the evolution of animal conflicts. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 47, Peek, F.W. (1972) Experimental study of territorial function of vocal and visual display in the male red-winged blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus). Animal Behaviour, 20, Peters, A., Delhey, K., Goymann, W. & Kempenaers, B. (2006) Age-dependent association between testosterone and crown UV coloration in male blue tits (Parus caeruleus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 59, Prum, R.O. (2006) Physics and evolution of structural colors. Bird Coloration, Vol. 1: Mechanisms and Measurements (eds G.E. Hill & K.J. McGraw), pp Harvard University Press, Cambridge. Pryke, S.R. & Andersson, S. (2003) Carotenoid-based status signalling in redshouldered widowbirds (Euplectes axillaris): epaulet size and redness affect captive and territorial competition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 53, Pryke, S.R. & Griffith, S.C. (2006) Red dominates black: agonistic signalling among head morphs in the colour polymorphic Gouldian finch. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 273, Pryke, S.R., Andersson, S., Lawes, M.J. & Piper, S.E. (2002) Carotenoid status signaling in captive and wild red-collared widowbirds: independent effects of badge size and color. Behavioral Ecology, 13, Qvarnstrom, A. (1997) Experimentally increased badge size increases male competition and reduces male parental care in the collared flycatcher. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 264, Rasbash, J., Steele, F., Browne, W. & Prosser, B A User s Guide to MLwiN, Version 2.0. Institute of Education, London. Roberts, M.L., Ras, E. & Peters, A. (2009) Testosterone increases UV reflectance of sexually selected crown plumage in male blue tits. Behavioral Ecology, 20,

The role of social interactions on the development and honesty of a signal of status

The role of social interactions on the development and honesty of a signal of status University of Kentucky UKnowledge Theses and Dissertations--Biology Biology 2012 The role of social interactions on the development and honesty of a signal of status Sanh K. Diep University of Kentucky,

More information

Territorial Contests. Chapter 22: Territorial Signals. Games again. Territories. Ownership asymmetry

Territorial Contests. Chapter 22: Territorial Signals. Games again. Territories. Ownership asymmetry Territorial Contests Chapter 22: Territorial Signals Ownership asymmetry New twist: what significance? Previous models have only considered asymmetries in resource value and cost of conflict (fighting

More information

Absolute and relative signals: a comparison between melanin- and carotenoid-based patches

Absolute and relative signals: a comparison between melanin- and carotenoid-based patches Absolute and relative signals: a comparison between melanin- and carotenoid-based patches J.C. Senar 1) & J. Quesada (Behavioural and Evolutionary Ecology Associate Research Unit (CSIC), Museu de Ciències

More information

Bird Mating Systems/Sexual Selection

Bird Mating Systems/Sexual Selection Bird Mating Systems/Sexual Selection Why study mating systems? What are the ecological conditions under which different mating systems arise? Implications for evolution mating is where genes are passed

More information

Adaptation and Optimality Theory

Adaptation and Optimality Theory Adaptation and Optimality Theory Prisoner s Dilemma game: An optimality model What is the optimal strategy when you are playing with different people of unknown reputation? Defect: We call this the evolutionarily

More information

The evolution of cooperative turn-taking in animal conflict

The evolution of cooperative turn-taking in animal conflict RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access The evolution of cooperative turn-taking in animal conflict Mathias Franz 1*, Daniel van der Post 1,2,3, Oliver Schülke 1 and Julia Ostner 1 Abstract Background: A fundamental

More information

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30)

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30) FILM SESSIONS Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30) 1. Talking to Strangers (60 min.) 1 This film surveys some of the general communication signals used by animals from diverse taxa,

More information

Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences

Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences Males and females have the same genes. Why do the sexes often look and act so differently? Why is the male often insanely ornamented? (Or simply insane?)

More information

Is Signalling of Fighting Ability Costlier for Weaker Individuals?

Is Signalling of Fighting Ability Costlier for Weaker Individuals? J. theor. Biol. (997) 84, 83 88 Is Signalling of Fighting Ability Costlier for Weaker Individuals? PETER L. HURD Division of Ethology, Department of Zoology, University of Stockholm, S-06 9 Stockholm,

More information

(A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above

(A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above CLEP Biology - Problem Drill 24: Behavior No. 1 of 10 1. Habituation is a type of. (A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above Having a certain drive

More information

Female mating preferences for colourful males in a population of guppies subject to high predation

Female mating preferences for colourful males in a population of guppies subject to high predation Journal of Fish Biology (2004) 65, 1154 1159 doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2004.00502.x, available online at http://www.blackwell-synergy.com Female mating preferences for colourful males in a population of

More information

Sexual selection. Intrasexual selection mating success determined by within-sex interactions e.g., male-male combat

Sexual selection. Intrasexual selection mating success determined by within-sex interactions e.g., male-male combat Sexual dimorphism Sexual selection Is sexual selection different from natural selection? Darwin saw them as distinct - only sexual selection could produce traits that compromise survival The basic principle

More information

University of Groningen. Avian personalities Groothuis, Ton; Carere, C; Groothuis, Ton. Published in: Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

University of Groningen. Avian personalities Groothuis, Ton; Carere, C; Groothuis, Ton. Published in: Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews University of Groningen Avian personalities Groothuis, Ton; Carere, C; Groothuis, Ton Published in: Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.010 IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised

More information

Female coloration indicates female reproductive capacity in blue tits

Female coloration indicates female reproductive capacity in blue tits doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01451.x Female coloration indicates female reproductive capacity in blue tits C. DOUTRELANT,*A.GRÉGOIRE,* N. GRNAC,* D. GOMEZ, M. M. LAMBRECHTS* & P. PERRET* *CEFE CNRS, 1919

More information

Condition-dependent sexual traits and social dominance in the house finch

Condition-dependent sexual traits and social dominance in the house finch Condition-dependent sexual traits and social dominance in the house finch Renée A. Duckworth, a Mary T. Mendonça, b and Geoffrey E. Hill b a Biology Department, Box 90338, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708,

More information

University of Groningen

University of Groningen University of Groningen Mutual ornamentation, age, and reproductive performance in the European starling Komdeur, Jan; Oorebeek, M; van Overveld, T; Cuthill, IC; Cuthill, Innes C. Published in: Behavioral

More information

The reproductive choices of eavesdropping female black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus

The reproductive choices of eavesdropping female black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus Naturwissenschaften (2003) 90:577-582 DOI 10.1007/s00114-003-0479-3 SHORT COMMUNICATION Daniel J. Mennill Peter T. Boag Laurene M. Ratcliffe The reproductive choices of eavesdropping female black-capped

More information

Agonistic interactions in crayfish: what factors determine who wins? Introduction

Agonistic interactions in crayfish: what factors determine who wins? Introduction Agonistic interactions in crayfish: what factors determine who wins? Introduction When important resources, such as food, mates or shelter, are limited, individuals of the same species will compete for

More information

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective Academic Leadership Journal in Student Research Volume 4 Spring 2016 Article 4 2016 Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective Kevin L. Kenney Fort Hays State University Follow this

More information

Chapter 3A: ESS in two easy lessons

Chapter 3A: ESS in two easy lessons Chapter 3 Chapter 3A: ESS in two easy lessons The purpose of this chapter is to review some classical examples of evolutionary game theory models for behavior and life-history traits, and give an indication

More information

Biology 352, Spring 2018 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam Part 1 (short answer worth 21 % of grade)

Biology 352, Spring 2018 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam Part 1 (short answer worth 21 % of grade) Part 1 (short answer worth 21 % of grade) 1-1) Match the following terms used in behavioral ecology with the appropriate topics of behavioral study listed to the right (you may apply more than one topic

More information

Biology 352, Spring 2017 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam

Biology 352, Spring 2017 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam Biology 352, Spring 2017 Exam Number KEY Exam Instructions: 1) Write your NAME here: 2) Write your exam number (given above, right) at the top of the other pages. 3) Look over the exam to verify that you

More information

Ultraviolet reflectance affects male-male interactions in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus ultramarinus)

Ultraviolet reflectance affects male-male interactions in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus ultramarinus) Ultraviolet reflectance affects male-male interactions in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus ultramarinus) Behavioral Ecology Vol. 15 No. 5: 805 809 doi:10.1093/beheco/arh083 Advance Access publication on June

More information

PRODUCTION OF MELANIN-BASED PLUMAGE TRAITS IN MALE HOUSE SPARROWS: NO EFFECT OF DIETARY COPPER ABSTRACT

PRODUCTION OF MELANIN-BASED PLUMAGE TRAITS IN MALE HOUSE SPARROWS: NO EFFECT OF DIETARY COPPER ABSTRACT Intern. Stud. Sparrows 2011, 35: 4-15 Ian R.K. STEWART* 1 and David F. WESTNEAT Department of Biology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky, 40506, USA 1 Current address: Department of Biology,

More information

Group fusion: the impact of winner, loser, and bystander effects on hierarchy formation in large groups

Group fusion: the impact of winner, loser, and bystander effects on hierarchy formation in large groups Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 3: 367 373 Group fusion: the impact of winner, loser, and bystander effects on hierarchy formation in large groups Lee Alan Dugatkin and Ryan L. Earley Department of Biology,

More information

UV-COLOURATION: SEXUAL SELECTION AND

UV-COLOURATION: SEXUAL SELECTION AND Theses of the dissertation UV-COLOURATION: SEXUAL SELECTION AND MATERNAL INVESTMENT IN EUROPEAN GREEN LIZARD (LACERTA VIRIDIS) Katalin Bajer Department of Systematic Zoology and Ecology Eötvös Loránd University

More information

Supplementary Figure 1. Funnel plots of meta-analytic residuals. Effect size (Zr) plotted against (a)

Supplementary Figure 1. Funnel plots of meta-analytic residuals. Effect size (Zr) plotted against (a) a b Supplementary Figure 1. Funnel plots of meta-analytic residuals. Effect size (Zr) plotted against (a) standard error and (b) precision (inverse of the standard error) n = 11 effect sizes. Screening

More information

Kin Selection and Family Conflicts

Kin Selection and Family Conflicts The Evolution of Sex and its Consequences HS 2011 Part II: Consequences Kin Selection and Family Conflicts Kin Selection: Altruism and Conflict Blue tit (Parus caeruleus) Burying beetle (Nicrophorus vespilloides)

More information

Rafał Martyka 1*, Ewa B. Śliwińska 1, Mirosław Martyka 1, Mariusz Cichoń 2 and Piotr Tryjanowski 3

Rafał Martyka 1*, Ewa B. Śliwińska 1, Mirosław Martyka 1, Mariusz Cichoń 2 and Piotr Tryjanowski 3 Martyka et al. Frontiers in Zoology (2018) 15:25 https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-018-0272-y RESEARCH The effect of pre-laying maternal immunization on offspring growth and immunity differs across experimentally

More information

Multiple sexual ornaments in satin bowerbirds: ultraviolet plumage and bowers signal different aspects of male quality

Multiple sexual ornaments in satin bowerbirds: ultraviolet plumage and bowers signal different aspects of male quality Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 4: 503 509 Multiple sexual ornaments in satin bowerbirds: ultraviolet plumage and bowers signal different aspects of male quality Stéphanie M. Doucet and Robert Montgomerie

More information

POST-ALLOGROOMING REDUCTIONS IN SELF-DIRECTED BEHAVIOUR ARE AFFECTED BY ROLE AND STATUS IN THE GREEN WOODHOOPOE

POST-ALLOGROOMING REDUCTIONS IN SELF-DIRECTED BEHAVIOUR ARE AFFECTED BY ROLE AND STATUS IN THE GREEN WOODHOOPOE 2 3 4 5 6 POST-ALLOGROOMING REDUCTIONS IN SELF-DIRECTED BEHAVIOUR ARE AFFECTED BY ROLE AND STATUS IN THE GREEN WOODHOOPOE Andrew N. Radford Electronic Supplementary Material 8 9 0 2 3 4 5 6 8 9 Study Species

More information

Spatial patterns of extra-pair paternity: beyond paternity gains and losses

Spatial patterns of extra-pair paternity: beyond paternity gains and losses Journal of Animal Ecology 2015, 84, 518 531 doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12293 Spatial patterns of extra-pair paternity: beyond paternity gains and losses Lotte Schlicht*, Mihai Valcu and Bart Kempenaers Department

More information

Reproduction. Chapter 7

Reproduction. Chapter 7 Reproduction Chapter 7 Sexual Selection Darwin's theory to explain traits that aren't obviously advantageous https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ll30qttsz9u Bower Birds Bower Birds She builds the nest, cares

More information

University of Groningen. Maternal phenotypic engineering Müller, Wendt

University of Groningen. Maternal phenotypic engineering Müller, Wendt University of Groningen Maternal phenotypic engineering Müller, Wendt IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the

More information

Principles of publishing

Principles of publishing Principles of publishing Issues of authorship, duplicate publication and plagiarism in scientific journal papers can cause considerable conflict among members of research teams and embarrassment for both

More information

The prevailing view in communication research is that hormones,

The prevailing view in communication research is that hormones, Socially selected ornaments influence hormone titers of signalers and receivers Elizabeth A. Tibbetts a,1, Katherine Crocker a, and Zachary Y. Huang b a Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of

More information

Beetle Battles Featured scientist: Erin McCullough from the University of Western Australia

Beetle Battles Featured scientist: Erin McCullough from the University of Western Australia Beetle Battles Featured scientist: Erin McCullough from the University of Western Australia Research Background: Male animals spend a lot of time and energy trying to attract females. In some species,

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line in evolution is reproductive success reproductive success:

More information

The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories

The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories Wai Keen Vong (waikeen.vong@adelaide.edu.au) Andrew T. Hendrickson (drew.hendrickson@adelaide.edu.au) Amy Perfors (amy.perfors@adelaide.edu.au)

More information

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection Intrasexual Natural Selection Sexual Selection Survival Mating Success Parental Care Intrasexual

More information

University of Groningen. An ethological approach of interpersonal theories of depression Geerts, Erwin Adrianus Henricus Maria

University of Groningen. An ethological approach of interpersonal theories of depression Geerts, Erwin Adrianus Henricus Maria University of Groningen An ethological approach of interpersonal theories of depression Geerts, Erwin Adrianus Henricus Maria IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's

More information

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887 The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887 1 Outline causes sexual selection vs. natural selection sexual selection during courtship male-male

More information

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES Correlational Research Correlational Designs Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are

More information

Color Cues and Viscosity in. Iterated Prisoner s Dilemma

Color Cues and Viscosity in. Iterated Prisoner s Dilemma Color Cues and Viscosity in Iterated Prisoner s Dilemma Michael Joya University of British Columbia Abstract The presence of color tag cues and a viscous environment have each been shown to foster conditional

More information

Experimental and natural changes in the peacock's (Pavo cristatus} train can affect mating success

Experimental and natural changes in the peacock's (Pavo cristatus} train can affect mating success Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1994) 35:213-217 Springer-Verlag 1994 Marion Petrie - Tim Halliday Experimental and natural changes in the peacock's (Pavo cristatus} train can affect mating success Received: 18

More information

Animal Behavior. Thur March 24: Animal Behavior: outline. Questions: What, How, and Why. Behavior is shaped partly by inheritance

Animal Behavior. Thur March 24: Animal Behavior: outline. Questions: What, How, and Why. Behavior is shaped partly by inheritance Thur March 24: Animal Behavior Animal Behavior: outline Animal Behavior Questions Behavior is partly shaped by Inheritance Behavior is partly shaped by Learning Hormones and Behavior The Genetics of Behavior

More information

University of Groningen. The illusion of monogamy Bouwman, Karen Marian

University of Groningen. The illusion of monogamy Bouwman, Karen Marian University of Groningen The illusion of monogamy Bouwman, Karen Marian IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the

More information

Peer review on manuscript "Multiple cues favor... female preference in..." by Peer 407

Peer review on manuscript Multiple cues favor... female preference in... by Peer 407 Peer review on manuscript "Multiple cues favor... female preference in..." by Peer 407 ADDED INFO ABOUT FEATURED PEER REVIEW This peer review is written by an anonymous Peer PEQ = 4.6 / 5 Peer reviewed

More information

House sparrows (Passer domesticus) behaviour in a novel environment is modulated by social context and familiarity in a sex-specific manner

House sparrows (Passer domesticus) behaviour in a novel environment is modulated by social context and familiarity in a sex-specific manner Tuliozi et al. Frontiers in Zoology (2018) 15:16 https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-018-0267-8 RESEARCH House sparrows (Passer domesticus) behaviour in a novel environment is modulated by social context and

More information

Reproductive success and survival during the breeding season in relation to individual behaviour in the great tit, Parus major

Reproductive success and survival during the breeding season in relation to individual behaviour in the great tit, Parus major Reproductive success and survival during the breeding season in relation to individual behaviour in the great tit, Parus major Camilla Thorsteinsen Master of Science Thesis Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary

More information

Sexual Selection. Male and female. Lukas Schärer. Evolutionary Biology Zoological Institute University of Basel

Sexual Selection. Male and female. Lukas Schärer. Evolutionary Biology Zoological Institute University of Basel Sexual Selection Lukas Schärer! Evolutionary Biology Zoological Institute University of Basel 1 8.11.2017 Advanced-level Evolutionary Biology Male and female 2 Summary: Sexual Selection defining sexual

More information

April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice. Female choice

April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice. Female choice April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice Female choice In general, females are expected to be choosier than males because they have more to lose by making bad mating decisions Costs and Benefits: Possible

More information

Developing an Experimental Protocol: Agonistic Behavior in Betta splendens by Dana Krempels and Adrienne DuBois

Developing an Experimental Protocol: Agonistic Behavior in Betta splendens by Dana Krempels and Adrienne DuBois Developing an Experimental Protocol: Agonistic Behavior in Betta splendens by Dana Krempels and Adrienne DuBois You and your teammates should now be familiar with the natural history and basic behaviors

More information

Manipulating the perceived opportunity to cheat: an experimental test of the active roles of male and female zebra finches in mate guarding behavior

Manipulating the perceived opportunity to cheat: an experimental test of the active roles of male and female zebra finches in mate guarding behavior DOI 10.1007/s00265-013-1532-7 ORIGINAL PAPER Manipulating the perceived opportunity to cheat: an experimental test of the active roles of male and female zebra finches in mate guarding behavior Leah C.

More information

Territoriality. Environmental factors and territory size in kites. Environmental factors and territory size in kites

Territoriality. Environmental factors and territory size in kites. Environmental factors and territory size in kites Territoriality, Habitat Selection, and Aggression Territoriality When is it beneficial? 1. When limited resources are clumped and ephemeral? 2. When limited resources are uniformly distributed in time

More information

Evolutionary quantitative genetics of behavioral responses to handling in a wild passerine

Evolutionary quantitative genetics of behavioral responses to handling in a wild passerine https://helda.helsinki.fi Evolutionary quantitative genetics of behavioral responses to handling in a wild passerine Class, Barbara 2014-02 Class, B, Kluen, E & Brommer, J E 2014, ' Evolutionary quantitative

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons behind the lifestyles of our non-human primate relatives

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Ebbes, P. (2004). Latent instrumental variables: a new approach to solve for endogeneity s.n.

Citation for published version (APA): Ebbes, P. (2004). Latent instrumental variables: a new approach to solve for endogeneity s.n. University of Groningen Latent instrumental variables Ebbes, P. IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

Developing bipolar disorder. A study among children of patients with bipolar disorder Hillegers, Manon Hubertine Johanna

Developing bipolar disorder. A study among children of patients with bipolar disorder Hillegers, Manon Hubertine Johanna University of Groningen Developing bipolar disorder. A study among children of patients with bipolar disorder Hillegers, Manon Hubertine Johanna IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's

More information

University of Groningen

University of Groningen University of Groningen Primary sex ratio adjustment to experimentally reduced male UV attractiveness in blue tits Korsten, Peter; Lessells, CM; Mateman, AC; van der Velde, Marco; Komdeur, Jan Published

More information

Is there an evolutionary trade-off between quality signaling and social recognition?

Is there an evolutionary trade-off between quality signaling and social recognition? Behavioral Ecology The official journal of the ISBE International Society for Behavioral Ecology Behavioral Ecology (2016), 27(1), 2 13. doi:10.1093/beheco/arv109 Invited Review Is there an evolutionary

More information

Agonistic Behavior in Betta splendens: Developing an Experimental Protocol by Dana Krempels and Adrienne DuBois

Agonistic Behavior in Betta splendens: Developing an Experimental Protocol by Dana Krempels and Adrienne DuBois Agonistic Behavior in Betta splendens: Developing an Experimental Protocol by Dana Krempels and Adrienne DuBois You and your teammates should now be familiar with the natural history and basic behaviors

More information

Cost/benefit approach

Cost/benefit approach Cost/benefit approach Care FEMALE Abandon MALE Care F: wp 2 WP 1 M: wp 2 WP 1 Abandon F: wp 1 WP 0 M: wp 1 (1+p M ) WP 0 (1+p M ) P 0,1,2 = probability eggs survive given 0, 1, or 2 parents W, w = eggs

More information

The meaning of melanin, carotenoid, and pterin pigments in the bluefin killifish, Lucania goodei

The meaning of melanin, carotenoid, and pterin pigments in the bluefin killifish, Lucania goodei Behavioral Ecology Advance Access published September 30, 2014 Behavioral Ecology Behavioral Ecology (2014), 00(00), 1 10. doi:10.1093/beheco/aru164 The official journal of the ISBE International Society

More information

Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens. The following table shows the percentage of male fairy-wrens in various age and social status categories.

Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens. The following table shows the percentage of male fairy-wrens in various age and social status categories. Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens Superb fairy-wrens are small (10g) insectivorous birds found in woodlands and edge habitat throughout eastern Australia. They live in cooperative social groups composed

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Weert, E. V. (2007). Cancer rehabilitation: effects and mechanisms s.n.

Citation for published version (APA): Weert, E. V. (2007). Cancer rehabilitation: effects and mechanisms s.n. University of Groningen Cancer rehabilitation Weert, Ellen van IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

Animal Behaviour. Raoul Mulder. Signal reliability: what keeps signals honest?

Animal Behaviour. Raoul Mulder. Signal reliability: what keeps signals honest? Animal Behaviour Raoul Mulder Signal reliability: what keeps signals honest? The problem of signal reliability Roosevelt Elk If signaller and receiver interests conflict, each will be selected to pursue

More information

Evolutionarily stable strategies in competition for resource intake rate maximization

Evolutionarily stable strategies in competition for resource intake rate maximization Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1989) 25:201-205 Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 9 Springer-Verlag 1989 Evolutionarily stable strategies in competition for resource intake rate maximization II. Oviposition behavior

More information

Cheating as a mixed strategy in a simple model of aggressive communication

Cheating as a mixed strategy in a simple model of aggressive communication ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2000, 59, 221 230 Article No. anbe.1999.1293, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Cheating as a mixed strategy in a simple model of aggressive communication SZABOLCS SZAu

More information

Martin Rodriguez Insect Behavior Territorial Spiders and Other Species

Martin Rodriguez Insect Behavior Territorial Spiders and Other Species Territorial Spiders and Other Species It has been theorized that the environment will be separated amongst members of a species if an aggressive encounter is avoided. Avoiding an aggressive encounter generates

More information

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise...

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise... 1. Female choice of males that have exaggerated characters has been explained by female choice of males that have better genes. Explain how female choice of higher quality males might lead to exaggerated

More information

Pharmacoeconomic analysis of proton pump inhibitor therapy and interventions to control Helicobacter pylori infection Klok, Rogier Martijn

Pharmacoeconomic analysis of proton pump inhibitor therapy and interventions to control Helicobacter pylori infection Klok, Rogier Martijn University of Groningen Pharmacoeconomic analysis of proton pump inhibitor therapy and interventions to control Helicobacter pylori infection Klok, Rogier Martijn IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult

More information

University of Groningen

University of Groningen University of Groningen Effect of compensatory viewing strategies on practical fitness to drive in subjects with visual field defects caused by ocular pathology Coeckelbergh, Tanja Richard Maria IMPORTANT

More information

Urine marking and urination in Lemur catta: a comparison of design features

Urine marking and urination in Lemur catta: a comparison of design features Ann. Zool. Fennici 43: 28 284 ISSN 3-455X Helsinki 28 June 26 Finnish Zoological and Botanical Publishing Board 26 Urine marking and urination in Lemur catta: a comparison of design features Elisabetta

More information

13.17 Natural selection cannot fashion perfect organisms

13.17 Natural selection cannot fashion perfect organisms 13.17 Natural selection cannot fashion perfect organisms 1. Selection can only act on existing variation Natural selection cannot conjure up new beneficial alleles 2. Evolution is limited by historical

More information

Dominance relationships in female Herotilapia

Dominance relationships in female Herotilapia Animal Learning & Behavior 1981, 9 (2),164-168 Dominance relationships in female Herotilapia multispinosa (Pisces: Cichlidae) JANIS WILEY DRISCOLL and PHILIP R. WELANKO University ofcolorado, Denver, Colorado

More information

EnSt/Bio 295 Exam II This test is worth 100 points; you have approximately 50 minutes. Allocate your time accordingly.

EnSt/Bio 295 Exam II This test is worth 100 points; you have approximately 50 minutes. Allocate your time accordingly. Name: 1 NAME: EnSt/Bio 295 Exam II This test is worth 100 points; you have approximately 50 minutes. Allocate your time accordingly. 1) Describe the following concepts in a few sentences (2 points each)

More information

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons behind the lifestyles of our non-human primate relatives

More information

Mental health treatment provided by primary care psychologists in the Netherlands Verhaak, Petrus; Kamsma, H.; van der Niet, A.

Mental health treatment provided by primary care psychologists in the Netherlands Verhaak, Petrus; Kamsma, H.; van der Niet, A. University of Groningen Mental health treatment provided by primary care psychologists in the Netherlands Verhaak, Petrus; Kamsma, H.; van der Niet, A. Published in: Psychiatric Services IMPORTANT NOTE:

More information

No evidence for differential maternal allocation to offspring in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus)

No evidence for differential maternal allocation to offspring in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus) Behavioral Ecology Vol. 14 No. 3: 340 346 No evidence for differential maternal allocation to offspring in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus) Jérémy Mazuc, a,b Olivier Chastel, b and Gabriele Sorci

More information

Fighting rules. Value of resource. Assessment and possible escalation. Degree of escalation depends on resource value. Elephant seal Rep at age 9-12

Fighting rules. Value of resource. Assessment and possible escalation. Degree of escalation depends on resource value. Elephant seal Rep at age 9-12 Fighting rules Assessment and possible escalation Degree of escalation depends on resource value Value of resource Red deer Rep from ages 6-12 75% reach rep. age 2-4 calves sired per year Fights typically

More information

University of Groningen. Metabolic risk in people with psychotic disorders Bruins, Jojanneke

University of Groningen. Metabolic risk in people with psychotic disorders Bruins, Jojanneke University of Groningen Metabolic risk in people with psychotic disorders Bruins, Jojanneke IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from

More information

Winner and Loser Effects in Major League Baseball Double Headers

Winner and Loser Effects in Major League Baseball Double Headers Nova Southeastern University NSUWorks Biology Faculty Articles Department of Biological Sciences 1-1-2014 Winner and Loser Effects in Major League Baseball Double Headers Andrew C. Gallup State University

More information

r = intrinsic rate of natural increase = the instantaneous rate of change in population size (per individual). If r > 0, then increasing

r = intrinsic rate of natural increase = the instantaneous rate of change in population size (per individual). If r > 0, then increasing Zoology 357 - Evolutionary Ecology - First Exam 1. (6 points) List three types of natural selection and show how the population changes over time (graph the initial phenotype frequency distribution and

More information

Role of sensitisation to testosterone in the early development of aggression in the blackheaded

Role of sensitisation to testosterone in the early development of aggression in the blackheaded University of Groningen Role of sensitisation to testosterone in the early development of aggression in the blackheaded gull Ros, Albert Frank Huáscar IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. Bat Predation and Sexual Advertisement in a Neotropical Anuran Author(s): Michael J. Ryan, Merlin D. Tuttle, A. Stanley Rand Source: The American Naturalist, Vol. 119, No. 1 (Jan., 1982), pp. 136-139 Published

More information

12/3/2012. I. Benefits of mate choice. A. Direct benefits. Examples of Female Choice. Mechanisms of Sexual Selection. A.

12/3/2012. I. Benefits of mate choice. A. Direct benefits. Examples of Female Choice. Mechanisms of Sexual Selection. A. Examples of Female Choice Mechanisms of Sexual Selection A. Mate competition B. Mate choice green frogs common terns mottled sculpins smooth newts elephant seals three-spined sticklebacks house finches

More information

GROUP DECISION MAKING IN RISKY ENVIRONMENT ANALYSIS OF GENDER BIAS

GROUP DECISION MAKING IN RISKY ENVIRONMENT ANALYSIS OF GENDER BIAS GROUP DECISION MAKING IN RISKY ENVIRONMENT ANALYSIS OF GENDER BIAS Andrea Vasiľková, Matúš Kubák, Vladimír Gazda, Marek Gróf Abstract Article presents an experimental study of gender bias in group decisions.

More information

University of Groningen

University of Groningen University of Groningen Effect of social defeat in a territorial bird (Parus major) selected for different coping styles Carere, C; Welink, D; Drent, Piet J.; Koolhaas, Jaap M.; Groothuis, Ton Published

More information

How Organisms Evolve Chapters The Theory of Evolution. The Theory of Evolution. Evolution can be traced through the fossil record.

How Organisms Evolve Chapters The Theory of Evolution. The Theory of Evolution. Evolution can be traced through the fossil record. How Organisms Evolve Chapters 14-15 The Theory of Evolution Evolution is the process of change in the inherited traits of a population of organisms from one generation to the next. The inherited traits

More information

PHOTOPERIODIC CONTROL OF THE TESTICULAR CYCLE IN THE EASTERN RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD, (AGELAIUS PHOENICEUS PHOENICEUS)

PHOTOPERIODIC CONTROL OF THE TESTICULAR CYCLE IN THE EASTERN RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD, (AGELAIUS PHOENICEUS PHOENICEUS) University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Bird Control Seminars Proceedings Wildlife Damage Management, Internet Center for September 1970 PHOTOPERIODIC CONTROL OF

More information

Positive Carotenoid Balance Correlates with Greater Reproductive Performance in a Wild Bird

Positive Carotenoid Balance Correlates with Greater Reproductive Performance in a Wild Bird Positive Carotenoid Balance Correlates with Greater Reproductive Performance in a Wild Bird Rebecca J. Safran 1 *, Kevin J. McGraw 2, Matthew R. Wilkins 1, Joanna K. Hubbard 1, Julie Marling 1 1 Department

More information

USE AND MISUSE OF MIXED MODEL ANALYSIS VARIANCE IN ECOLOGICAL STUDIES1

USE AND MISUSE OF MIXED MODEL ANALYSIS VARIANCE IN ECOLOGICAL STUDIES1 Ecology, 75(3), 1994, pp. 717-722 c) 1994 by the Ecological Society of America USE AND MISUSE OF MIXED MODEL ANALYSIS VARIANCE IN ECOLOGICAL STUDIES1 OF CYNTHIA C. BENNINGTON Department of Biology, West

More information

The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction

The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction I. Introduction to Behavioral Ecology Behavioral ecologists test hypotheses about the adaptive function of a behavior Behavioral Ecology Behavioral ecologists

More information

University of Groningen. BNP and NT-proBNP in heart failure Hogenhuis, Jochem

University of Groningen. BNP and NT-proBNP in heart failure Hogenhuis, Jochem University of Groningen BNP and NT-proBNP in heart failure Hogenhuis, Jochem IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check

More information

Sex ratio adjustment in relation to paternal attractiveness in a

Sex ratio adjustment in relation to paternal attractiveness in a Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 11723-11728, October 1996 Evolution Sex ratio adjustment in relation to paternal attractiveness in a wild bird population (sexual selection/sex allocation/ficedula

More information

Self-Defense and Female-Perpetrated Violence. Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford

Self-Defense and Female-Perpetrated Violence. Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford Self-Defense and Female-Perpetrated Violence Rachel M. James and Todd K. Shackelford Synonyms: retaliation, self-defense, physical aggression, male sexual jealousy, intimate partner violence Definition:

More information

University of Groningen. Fracture of the distal radius Oskam, Jacob

University of Groningen. Fracture of the distal radius Oskam, Jacob University of Groningen Fracture of the distal radius Oskam, Jacob IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

Lecture 6 Behavior ethology Communication

Lecture 6 Behavior ethology Communication Lecture 6 Behavior ethology Communication Rich history of behavioral studies of birds Birds display a wide range of innate and learned behavior patterns Overview - Behavior Chapter 8 (Gill) Vocalizations

More information