How fast is the barberpole? Interactions of motion and depth in the perception of velocity. Fauzia Mosca & Nicola Bruno

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "How fast is the barberpole? Interactions of motion and depth in the perception of velocity. Fauzia Mosca & Nicola Bruno"

Transcription

1 How fast is the barberpole? Interactions of motion and depth in the perception of velocity Fauzia Mosca & Nicola Bruno Università di Trieste, Trieste, Italy Submitted to: Perception & Psychophysics, August 2000 Revision: March 2001 Running Head: Barberpole Speed Correspondence: Nicola Bruno Dipartimento di Psicologia Università di Trieste via S. Anastasio, TRIESTE, Italy

2 How fast is the barberpole? 2 Abstract We developed and tested a new method to study motion perception and integration as a function of binocular disparity. Observers viewed contours in one of two disparity conditions (in front of a rectangular frame, intrinsic contour terminators, or behind, extrinsic terminators) within a vertically or horizontally oriented rectangular aperture ("barberpole"). Before the onset of motion, observers adjusted the disparity of a small probe to a preset depth. Next, the first stimulus was moved for 204 ms. Finally, a second stimulus was presented for 204 ms. Observers had to decide which was faster. Discrimination data were used to compute matching speeds as a function of presentation conditions. Speed matches confirmed that motion integration is influenced by depth according to a simple coplanarity rule. However, integration was not influenced by aperture orientation as claimed in other studies. Differences with previous studies that used direction judgments are interpreted as due to our better control of fixation, convergence, and pursuit.

3 How fast is the barberpole? 3 Introduction When a stimulus varies along a single spatial dimension, its motion is inherently ambiguous. The component of motion parallel to stimulus orientation does not change the spatiotemporal structure of the generated optic array. Thus, only the component perpendicular to stimulus orientation (Vp) can be detected and a class of different motions cannot be distinguished (P. Stumpf, 1911/1996; Wallach, 1935/1996). Because local kinematic ambiguity is often demonstrated by looking at a contour through a circular aperture (see Figure 1a), this fundamental feature of 1D motion signals is often termed the «aperture» problem (Hildreth, 1984; 1987). There is substantial consensus that the aperture problem arises in human vision because of the existence of motion-sensitive cortical units that are selective for stimulus orientation (Movshon, Adelson, Gizzi & Newsome, 1985). To recover the true motion of objects in the environment, the human visual system must be able to integrate local signals in ways that effectively reduce local ambiguities. Several investigators have argued that solutions to the aperture problem may involve velocity computations, either by intersecting constraints in velocity space (Adelson & Movshon, 1982) or, more likely, by computing a spatially-weighted vector average of local signals (Castet, Lorenceau, Shiffrar & Bonnet, 1993; Ferrera & Wilson, 1990; Mingolla, Todd, & Norman, 1992; Rubin & Hochstein, 1993). --Figure 1--

4 How fast is the barberpole? 4 Critical to a spatially-weighted integrative mechanism is the idea that ambigous motion signals can be integrated («motion linking») with unambiguous signals from surface corners or edge terminators. For instance, when a grating or a contour is moved in a rectangular window, motion is perceived predominantly in the direction of the longer side of the window. This is generally referred to as the «barberpole» effect (Wallach, 1935/1996, see Figure 1b). Traditionally, the barberpole effect is explained by noting that the terminators of the moving line do move along the preferred perceived direction. A weighted-integration process that assigns greater weight to the unambiguous terminator component (Vt) would therefore predict motion along the longer side of the rectangular window. Consistent with the notion of long-range interactions between Vp and Vt signals, several studies have noted effects from remote motion signals on the motion of an edge seen through an aperture. For instance, Ben-Av & Shiffrar (1995) investigated the spatial integration of moving edges and corners seen through separate apertures. The study found that the unambiguous signal provided by the corners could propagate to the ambigous moving edges if the edges were properly aligned in space. In a related study, Mussap & Grotenhuis (1996) assessed the effect of external prolongations as a function of orientation and phase differences relative to an edge that moved within a rectangular aperture. In general, they found that the larger the difference (in phase or orientation) between the external prolongations and the inner edge, the smaller the capture effect of the external

5 How fast is the barberpole? 5 prolongations. Finally, Kim & Wilson (1997) investigated the direction of motion in a center circular aperture as a function of the properties of a surrounding annular aperture. They found directional shifts as a function of surround direction, size of the gap between disk and annulus, contrast, speed, and extent of the surround area. However, dependencies of disk motion on surround motion did not occur if the two moving sinewaves differed in spatial frequency. Taken in consort, these findings suggest that the processing of motion within one aperture can be influenced by more remote motions presented either in the surround of the test aperture or in other apertures. Although the factors governing motion linking in these complex conditions are still not clear, they may involve similarities in relatively low level stimulus properties, such as orientation, alignment, contrast, and spatial frequency. Besides contextual constraints on the cooperation/competion between local motion signals, other investigators have argued that the spatial integration of motion is also affected by higher-level constraints arising from surface occlusions and depth ordering. For instance, if the barberpole contour is placed at a farther depth plane than the rectangular window via binocular disparity, the barberpole effect is abolished and motion is again perceived in the direction of Vp (Shimojo, Silverman & Nakayama, 1989). In support of the importance of occlusion constraints, figural factors controlling depth ordering in occlusion configurations have also been shown to yield effects comparable to binocular disparity in causing partial abolition of the barberpole effect (Bressan, Ganis, &

6 How fast is the barberpole? 6 Vallortigara 1993; Gerbino & Bruno, 1997; Ramachandran, 1990; Tommasi & Vallortigara, 1999; Trueswell & Hayhoe, 1993; Vallortigara & Bressan, 1991). In the model of Shimojo, Silverman & Nakayama (1989), local disparity information acts as a sort of gate mechanism, allowing Vt signals to spread to the rest of the moving contour when disparity is consistent with an object (intrinsic) terminator, but not when Vt signals are classified as belonging to spurious terminators due to occlusion (extrinsic). Later on, we characterize this hypothetical mechanism in terms of a capture/suppression model: intrinsic terminators predominate over perpendicular signals, causing the barberpole effect, whereas extrinsic terminators are suppressed, abolishing the barberpole effect. However, it is presently not clear whether such mechanism is rigid, such that Vt signals are always fully suppressed with extrinsic terminators, or modulated by other factors. In fact, the data reported by Shimojo et al suggest that this modulation from additional factors is possible. In their paradigm, three expert observers performed a forcedchoice task between vertical and perpendicular directions with intrinsic or extrinsic terminators. Shimojo et al reported that the proportion of "vertical" choices was essentially 1 with intrinsic terminators, whereas it reduced to about 0.4 with extrinsic terminators. This seems to imply that extrinsic terminators were not always excluded from the integration process. However, other interpretations are possible. In their reanalysis of depth-motion interactions in the barberpole effect, Castet, Charton & Dufour (1999) examined näive observers but kept track of individual direction

7 How fast is the barberpole? 7 judgements. They found that the distributions of such judgments exhibited a marked tendency towards bimodality with extrinsic terminators, centering around Vt and Vp; whereas they exhibited a single central tendency at Vt with intrinsic terminators. On the basis of these findings, they argued that extrinsic terminators fail to completely suppress Vt components, essentially producing directional bistability in these conditions. On the basis of this finding, they argued that the reponses shift towards perpendicula directions with extrinsic terminators may be due to averaging two kinds of responses. In their proposed explanation, therefore, the terminator effect may be characterized in terms of a capture/bistability model. Intrinsic terminators predominate over perpendicular components, causing the barbepole effect; whereas extrinsic terminators actively compete with perpendicular components, causing bistability. Averaging over different trials then causes final estimates of directions to shift towards the perpendicular components. Although the finding of Castet et al is interesting, the question remains open as to what may cause the apparent bistability under extirnsic terminators. Given relatively long presentations, it is possible that directional judgements were contaminated by where observer fixated or by pursuit of certain features over time. For instance, intrinsic terminators may tend to attract an observer's foveation even if observers were required to fixate the center of the display. This in turn might enhance Vt signals relative to comparable extrinsic terminator conditions. In this paper, we sought to investigate the integration process while controlling for fixation and pursuit. To this aim, we did two things. First, we performed a preliminary experiment using a between-observer design to

8 How fast is the barberpole? 8 collect forced-choice directional judgments from naïve observers seeing extrinsic- or intrinsic-terminator barberpoles. Our own observations in this preliminary experiment readily demonstrated that the effect is not abolished by extrinsic terminators for all observers. To test the hypothesis that the effect of extrinsic terminators may have been contaminated by the lack of control of fixation and pursuit, we then performed three additional experiments. In these experiments, we developed and tested a different method for assessing motion integration in stereoscopic barberpoles. The core idea of the method lies in the use of a speed discrimination paradigm rather than collecting directional judgments. Use of this method allowed us to study motion integration with brief presentations (that effectively minimize pursuit). We also added an initial depth adjustment task to each trial in order to force observers to the appropriate convergence and to control fixation during the presentation. Our speed discrimination results, as well as their variability, provide support for suppression of extrinsic terminator component in these conditions and no evidence for bistability. In addition, they also suggest that intrinsic terminator components are averaged with perpendicular components to yield a final velocity in these conditions. Experiment 1 We showed a standard barberpole display having either intrinsic (rectangular frame behind the monitor plane) or extrinsic (frame in front of the monitor plane) bar terminators, and collected direction judgments from independent groups of naïve observers. The aim of

9 How fast is the barberpole? 9 this experiment was to evaluate the variability of such judgments under relatively unconstrained conditions of observation. Methods Equipment. The experiment was performed on a Silicon Graphics - Indigo workstation equipped with a stereo-ready SGI monitor (1280 x 1024 pixel resolution, 256 simultaneously displayable grey-levels, approximately 2 log units luminance range). Stereoscopic viewing was achieved by means of a Crystal Eyes field-sequential system. In this system, an emitter broadcasts an infrared signal to switch liquid crystal lenses on and off in exact synchronization with the monitor (60 Hz per eye rate). The lenses are mounted on glasses worn when observing the displays. The swap interval between screen buffers was set to two screen refreshes, allowing us to control stimulus speeds up to 0.5 pixels/60 Hz frame. Observers. Fifty members of the University of Trieste community participated. All observers were fully naïve to our purpose and had never served in a psychophysical experiment before. All were tested for normal stereoscopic vision by means of a randomdot stereogram before serving. Twenty-five were shown a stereoscopic barberpole with intrinsic terminators. The other twenty-five were shown the barberpole with extrinsic terminators.

10 How fast is the barberpole? 10 Displays. Displays consisted of a gray rectangular frame (6.5 cd m -2, 2.5 degs horizontally x 7.5 degs vertically) surrounded by a larger circular surround (diameter 20 degs). The inside part of the rectangle and the outer surround were white (20 cd m -2 ). Inside the rectangular frame, we presented a single line (2.5 cd m -2 ) oriented at 45 deg. Outside the larger circular surround, screen luminance was effectively zero yielding a deep black surround. The disparity of the rectangular frame relative to the line, either crossed or uncrossed, was always 10 min of visual angle. The line was translated vertically with a Vt speed equal to 4.5 pixels/frame (Vp component equal 3.18 pixels/frame). The starting and ending positions for the motion of the line were calculated so that the terminators always remained on the longer sides of the aperture. Line motion always begun from the top position and cycled up and down until the observer entered a response. Procedure. Observers sat at a 57 cm distance from the monitor. They viewed the displays while wearing stereo goggles (on top of prescriptions if needed) for as long as they wished and simply performed a forced choice between vertical and perpendicular motion directions. They were required to fixate the center of the display but no attempt was made to enforce this requirement or check that they complied with it. Results Proportions of "vertical" judgments in extrinsic and intrinsic conditions are presented in Figure 2. In the intrinsic terminator condition, observers unanimously reported

11 How fast is the barberpole? 11 vertical motion. In the extrinsic terminator condition, only 40% of the observers reported vertical motion (standard error 10%). -- figure 2-- Discussion Our preliminary result is basically consistent with that reported by Shimojo and collaborators using expert observers, and confirms that extrinsic terminators do not necessarily produce suppression of Vt signals in all trials. To test whether apparent failures to completely suppress extrinsic terminators can be ascribed to incomplete control over fixation and pursuit, we performed a series of additional experiments using a speed matching paradigm. Overview of Experiments 2-4 In an attempt to control for unwanted influences of fixation, eye movements, and spatial attention, we have developed a speed discrimination paradigm to study motion perception within stereoscopic apertures while carefully controlling for exposure time and fixation. The core idea lies in the joint use of two tasks. An initial task, performed on the stationary barperpole pattern, involves fixating a stereoscopically presented target square and adjusting its disparity to position it in depth at the same distance as the surrounding

12 How fast is the barberpole? 12 rectangular aperture. This task ensures that the observer is fixating the center of the display and that convergence is appropriate for the desired stereoscopic effect. Upon completion of the first task, observers performed a discrimination task on the same display. This second task required comparing the speed of the contour moving within the rectangular aperture with the speed of a second stimulus, presented immediately afterwards. The presentation of both stimuli is of the order of 200 ms, so as to effectively minimize the possibility of ocular pursuit (see Hallet, 1986) and the potential temporal integration of successive fixations. Using the above discrimination procedure, we obtained estimates of the matching speed of contours within rectangular apertures. This matching speed, in turn, provides indirect information about the relative importance of the Vt and Vp components in the integration process. The relationship between these two components is illustrated, for a contour oriented at 45 degrees (such as those investigated here), in Figure 3. Note that Vt = Vp 2 in these conditions. Hence, if Vt signals for extrinsic terminators are excluded from the integration, we expect speed matches to correspond to the law match = Vp standard speed (a line with slope = 1). If Vt signals for intrinsic terminators are instead included in the integration and fully capture the motion of the contour, we expect speed matches to correspond to the law match = Vp standard speed 2. This pattern of results would thus yield two diverging lines with an angular difference of about 10 degrees. --Figure 3--

13 How fast is the barberpole? 13 General Methods Equipment. The equipment was the same as in experiment 1. Procedure. The structure of the basic experimental trial is summarized in Figure 4. Before performing speed discriminations, observers were presented with a static barberpole display having the rectangular frame either in front of the line (intrinsic terminator condition) or behind the line (extrinsic terminator condition). In the center of the rectangle was a small green square which was stereoscopically placed at same disparity as the line. Before initiating each trial, observers were requested to adjust the disparity of this square probe until its position in depth was the same as that of the currently presented frame. As soon as they had done this, observers started the animation by pressing the space bar on the computer console. Speed matches were derived from comparative judgements of the standard and test stimuli, each standard - test pair being presented in succession with a blank interval equal to 1 screen refresh (approx. 17 ms ) in between. The standard stimulus always preceded the test stimulus and they both lasted 12 screen refreshes (about 204 ms). Observers were instructed to press one console button if they believed that the standard was faster, and another button if they believed that the test was. The standard displays were always the same throughout experiments 2-4, as detailed in the next subsection, except for their orientation which was vertical in experiments 2 and 3 and horizontal in experiment 4. The nature and number of test stimuli above and below the speed of the standard stimulus

14 How fast is the barberpole? 14 depended on the psychophysical procedure implemented in any given experiment, as reported in the relevant subsections. --Figure 4-- Standard Stimuli. In experiments 2-4, barberpole standard stimuli consisted of a gray rectangular frame identical to that used in experiment 1. This frame was oriented vertically in the second and third experiments, whereas it was horizontal in the fourth experiment. The inside part of the rectangle and the outer surround were white (20 cd m -2 ). Inside the rectangular frame, we presented a single line ( luminance approximately 2.5 cd m -2 and orientation 45 deg). Outside the larger circular surround, screen luminance was effectively zero yielding a deep black surround. The disparity of the rectangular frame relative to the line, either crossed or uncrossed, was always 10 min of visual angle. In all experiments, the Vp components of the speed of the standard line were 2.12, 3.18, and 4.24 pixels/frame, corresponding to Vt motion components equal to 3, 4.5, and 6 pixels/frame. The starting and ending positions for the motion of the standard line were calculated so that the terminators always remained on the longer sides of the aperture (in experiments 2-3, the vertical ones; in experiment 4, the horizontal ones). This also insured that the line did not change length during its motion. Test Stimuli. Test stimuli consisted of single lines 1 having the same length and orientation as the lines presented in the standards, and moving either in the Vp direction or in the Vt

15 How fast is the barberpole? 15 direction. This single line was presented within the larger circular surround. Given that the line was exactly the same length and orientation as the lines presented in the standards, it was much smaller than the circular surround and essentially appeared as a line moving over a homogeneous field. Data plotting. For each experiment, 95% confidence intervals around speed matches were plotted individually for each observer as a function of the Vp component of the speed of the standard. If perceived speeds are entirely determined by this component, they should lie on a line having slope equal to 1 and zero intercept. If instead they are entirely determined by the Vt motion component, they should lie on a line having slope equal to 2 or about Because in many cases the plotted error bars were very small, companion graphs of standard deviations were also prepared for direct comparison of variability in the crossed and uncrossed disparity conditions. Analysis. The data from all the experiments were analyzed in three steps. As a first step, we examined speed matches by subjecting them to a series of 2 (disparity) x 3 (Vp component of the standard) repeated measures general linear model analyses. The effect of the binocular disparity on the speed of the standard line predicts that the data should lie on two lines having different slopes but both intersecting the axes at zero. The linear model that tests this pattern implies a significant effect of the covariate and a significant interaction between the standard Vp component and disparity, but no difference between the adjusted disparity means as the intercepts of the two lines are both at zero. Because matches in

16 How fast is the barberpole? 16 different experiments were collected using different types of test stimuli and with different psychophysical procedures, these analyses were performed separately for each experiment. As a second step, we estimated the variability of the matches in the different experiments. In experiments 2 (method of constant stimuli), the standard deviations of these matches were estimated by probit analysis. In experiments 3 and 4 (staircase method), standard deviations were estimated from the variability of reversals, by computing the SD of each set of reversals and by then averaging them across the five sessions. As a third step, finally, we computed the differences between slopes in the two experimental conditions in all experiments and evaluated the results against an expected difference of 10 degrees. Experiment 2 Participants to this experiment compared intrinsic or extrinsic standards with test stimuli that moved in the direction predicted by the hypothesized effect of terminator type. Thus, observers compared intrinsic standards with lines that moved in the Vt direction, but they compared extrinsic standards with line that moved in the Vp direction. To estimate matching speeds from discrimination performance, this experiment used the method of constant stimuli. Methods Observers. Three observers participated. One of them (F.M.) was the first author. The

17 How fast is the barberpole? 17 other two (I. C. and S. L.) were unpaid members of the University of Trieste community and were naïve to the purpose of the study. All observers had normal or corrected-tonormal acuity and normal stereoscopic vision. Test Stimuli and Procedure. Test stimuli moved either in the Vp direction (extrinsic condition) or in the Vt direction (intrinsic condition). Speed matches were collected using the method of constant stimuli. For each level of the standard speed, 13 test speeds were presented. For a Vp standard speed of 3 pixel/frame the range was between 0.5 and 6.5 pixel/frame. For a Vp standard speed of 4.5 pixel/frame the range was between 2 and 8 pixel/frame. Finally, for a Vp standard speed of 6 pixel/frame the range was between 3.5 and 9.5 pixel/frame. In each experimental block, each test stimulus was presented twice, yielding a total of (13 speed levels x two terminator types x two presentations) 52 trials per block. Each observers served in 25 blocks. Therefore, each psychometric function used to estimate a matching speed was based on 650 trials in this experiment. Analysis. Matching speeds (points of subjective equality) were computed for each level of the standard speed in intrisic and extrinsic standards by subjecting the proportion of «test faster» responses to probit analysis. The variability associated to the estimates of matching speeds was evaluated from the standard deviations of the distributions underlying the comparative responses. Results

18 How fast is the barberpole? 18 Figure 5 presents a typical pair of psychometric functions of discrimination judgements in the extrinsic and intrinsic terminator conditions. Note that the sigmoid from the intrinsic condition appears slightly displaced to right relative to the sigmoid for the extrinsic condition, as one would expect if discriminative judgments were affected by terminator type. Matching speeds (points of subjective equality) were estimated using probit analysis. Thus, standard Vp values corresponding to 50% "test faster" judgments were taken as estimates of matching speeds for each standard speeds. The inverses of the slope of the probit-transformed proportions were taken as estimates of standard deviation of the matching speeds. Matching speeds and associated standard deviations are presented individually in Figure 6. A clear difference is apparent as a function of terminator condition. Confidence intervals around individual regression fits of observers F. M. and I. C. demonstrated that their matches were consistent with the Vp- and the Vt-component expectations in the extrinsic and intrinsic conditions, respectively (see Table 2). Fits of the data from observer S. L. yielded higher slopes than the expectations. Even for this observer, however, the difference between the slopes was essentially the same as the expected one (11 deg difference in the arctangent of the observed slopes, against 10 deg expected). Statistical analysis of this pattern confirmed that the matches were linear functions of the standard speed with different slopes but equal intercepts. Matching speeds varied

19 How fast is the barberpole? 19 linearly with the Vp component speed of the standard, F (1, 2) = 24.7, p < 0.04, the interaction between Vp component and disparity was significant, F (1, 2) = 109.5, p < 0.009, whereas the main effect of disparity was not, F (1, 2) = 0.3, p > Finally, matching speeds were different in different observers, F (2, 6) =11.1, p < 0.01, as was the interaction between Vp component and observer, F (2, 6) = 11.2, p < These results suggest that a barberpole line having intrinsic terminators move at the same speed as a single line having equal length, orientation, and Vt and Vp components. Conversely, a barberpole line having extrinsic terminators moves at the same speed as a single line having equal length, orientation, and Vp component only. Thus, the present results may be taken as evidence for suppression of the terminator components under extrinsic conditions. In addition, we note that there was little difference in variability between conditions (lower part of the figure). We will comment on this latter result in the general discussion section. --Figure 5 e 6-- Experiment 3 The results of the previous experiment may be confounded by a test stimulus effect. In experiment 2, test stimulus directions were matched to the expected direction of the standard barberpole stimulus, assuming a suppression effect due to terminator

20 How fast is the barberpole? 20 classification. Thus, intrinsic terminator standards were matched with tests moving in the Vt direction, whereas extrinsic terminator standards were matched with tests moving in the Vp direction. Given this matching, we cannot rule out that the observed results were at least in part due to an unknown effect of test motion direction rather than terminator classification. To test this possibility, we performed a third experiment using both a "matched" and a "crossed" set of test motion directions. Thus, both intrinsic and extrinsic terminator standards were compared with tests moving in the Vp and Vt directions. To test all four discrimination conditions within observers without excessively long sessions, an interleaved staircase procedure was adopted instead of the method of constant stimuli. Methods Observers. Three observers participated. One of them (F.M.) was the first author. The other two (F.S. and R.D.) were unpaid members of the University of Trieste community and were naïve to the purpose of the study. All observers had normal or corrected-tonormal acuity and normal stereoscopic vision. Procedure and Design. Matches were estimated using an interleaved staircase procedure (Cornsweet, 1962). Because randomizing the ascending and descending staircases yielded markedly more variable responses in a pilot run of the experiment, strict alternation was implemented with a 1 up -1 down stepping rule to control the speed of the test stimulus. Step size was set at 1 pixel/frame. Initial values were always 3 steps above or below the

21 How fast is the barberpole? 21 Vp standard speed for the descending and ascending staircases, respectively. The experimental design consisted in 3 standard speeds x 2 test directions x 2 terminator conditions. Therefore, 12 pairs of interleaved staircases were presented to each observer in a randomized order. In each presentation, the procedure was halted after 7 reversals in each direction. Matching speeds were computed by averaging reversals in both directions. Matching variability was estimated by computing the standard deviations of the 14 reversals. Each observer contributed five experimental sessions, each yielding six matching speeds for the the same standard speeds used in experiment 2. Test Stimuli and Analysis. They were the same as those of experiment 2, except for the pairings of standard and test stimuli. In experiment 2, intrinsic standards were only paired with tests moving vertically whereas extrinsic standards were only paired with tests moving perpendicularly. In the present experiment, terminator conditions and test stimulus directions were manipulated factorially, yielding two types of speed discriminations: matched (intrinsic vertical vs. extrinsic-perpendicular) or crossed (intrinsic-perpendicular vs. extrinsic-vertical) with respect to the expected directions of the standard barberpole. This manipulation allowed us to measure the effects of terminator type and test type independently. Results Matching speeds are presented individually in figure 7 as a function of the Vp

22 How fast is the barberpole? 22 component speed of the standard, as well as terminator type (top) and test type (bottom). These plots confirm that matching speeds were influenced systematically by the classification of terminators into intrinsic or extrinsic, based on the provided disparity. However, they also confirm that matches varied in a systematic fashion according to the direction of the test stimulus. A general linear model analysis of these main effects demonstrated that both the effect of terminator type, F (1, 2) = 19.7, p < , and the effect of test type F (1,2) = 43.3, p < , were statistically reliable when controlling for standard speed, F (1, 2) = 104.3, p < To better illustrate the nature of these two effects, we replotted the results after dividing the data into matched- or crossed-set discriminations. Matching speeds as well as match standard deviations in the intrinsic and extrinsic terminator conditions are presented in the two parts of Figure 8. Figure 8a presents matching speeds from the matched set. Thus, the data presented in Figure 8a are an exact replication of those presented in Experiment 2. Figure 8b presents matching speeds from the crossed set. As can be seen from comparing the graphs in Figure 8 with those in Figure 7, matching speeds tended to be systematically higher both in the intrinsic relative to the extrinsic condition and with vertical relative to perpendicular tests. In the matched set, these two tendencies combined, yielding results similar to those observed in experiment 2. In the crossed set, they worked against each other, essentially removing the difference due to terminator type. This interpretation was confirmed by subjecting the matching speeds to a 3 (Vp component standard speed) x 2 (2 terminator type) x 2 (set type, matched or crossed) general linear

23 How fast is the barberpole? 23 model analysis. This analysis yielded a significant three way interaction (standard speed x terminator x set), F(1, 14) = 12.3, p < , and significant main effects of standard speed, F(1, 2) = 31,2, p < 0.03 as well as terminator type, F(1, 2) = 81.6, p < The main effect of set type failed to reach significance, F(1, 2) < 1, as did all other interactions, all F's < Figures 7 and 8-- The nature of the observed three way interaction is further illustrated in table 3, which summarizes individual regression slopes as a function of terminator and set type. In the matched-set data, these slopes were similar to those of experiment 2 (see table 2), with marked differences in slope as a function of terminator type. In the crossed set data, the slopes were essentially indistinguishable. Finally, and again as in the previous experiment, we note that no obvious increase in the variability of matches could be observed as a function of terminator type in either matched or crossed conditions (bottom graphs of Figure 8). Discussion of experiments 2-3 The pattern of results observed in experiments 2 and 3 support the hypothesis that speed discrimination is affected by terminator type, as predicted from a model of motion

24 How fast is the barberpole? 24 integration that takes depth information into account. However, the results of experiment 3 also revealed an unexpected effect of the direction of the test stimulus. At first blush, this effect may seem evidence that the direction of the test stimulus can alter the perception of the standard speed. Although the results of our experiment 1, as well as previous similar findings, confirm that the effect of disparity on motion integration is not an all-or-none phenomenon and is probably influenced by additional factors such as fixation and eye movements, we doubt that an explanation of the present results in terms of a direct bias from the test direction would be convincing. Recall that our test stimuli were always presented after, not before or together with, the standard barberpoles. If test motion direction could bias the perception of a previously presented standard, this would entail a sort of backwards priming effect. To our knowledge, no such effect has ever been reported in the literature. As an alternative, we believe that a simple explanation for the observed difference between the matched- and the crossed-set data can be found in a simple modification of the motion integration model that motivated our experiments. Recall the motion integration model that was used to derive the predictions for the present experiments (Shimojo, Silverman & Nakayama, 1989; see also th overview of experiments 2-4). In this model, the speed of the standard stimulus is entirely determined by the Vt component when the terminators are classified as intrinsic, whereas it is entirely determined by the Vp component when the terminators are classified as extrinsic. We call this a capture/suppression model of motion integration. With intrinsic terminators, the Vt components from the line terminators are fully included in the integration, and therefore

25 How fast is the barberpole? 25 capture the motion of the line entirely. With extrinsic terminators, the Vt components are fully suppressed and therefore the line moves only in the Vp direction. Within the capture/suppression model, both the matched and the crossed-set matching speeds vary as a function of terminator type, according to the following laws: match = Vt (intrinsic terminators) or match = Vp (extrinsic terminators). In the matched set discriminations, this amounts to saying that the intrinsic barberpole appears to have the same speed as a test line moving vertically with speed equal to Vt, whereas the extrinsic barberpole appears to have the same speed as a test line moving perpendicularly with speed equal to Vp. In the crossed set discriminations, this amounts to saying that the intrinisic barberpole appears to have the same speed as a test line moving perpendicularly with speed equal to Vt, whereas the intrinsic barberpole appears to have the same speed as a test line moving vertically with speed equal to Vp. Clearly, the pattern of results observed in experiment 3 is not consistent with such a capture/suppression model. A simple modification of this model, however, can predict the observed pattern without invoking a backwards bias from the test motion direction. A number of studies have demonstrated that the local integration of motion signals resembles a form of vector averaging (Castet, Lorenceau, Shiffrar & Bonnet, 1993; Ferrera & Wilson, 1990; Mingolla, Todd & Norman, 1992; Rubin & Hochstein, 1993). It is therefore plausible to hypothesize that terminator components, when included in the integration process, do not fully capture the motion of the test line. Instead, they may compete with the perpendicular components, causing the line to move with speed and directions that compromise between the two

26 How fast is the barberpole? 26 components. We may call this an averaging/suppression model. In this model, when terminators are classified as extrinsic or when a test line moves in the Vp direction, the speed of the line corresponds to Vp. Conversely, when terminators are classified as intrinsic or when a test line moves in the Vt direction, the speed of the line corresponds to a weighted average of Vt and Vp. It is easy to see that such an averaging/suppression model makes the same predictions as the capture/suppression model for the matched-set discriminations, whereas it predicts essentially no effect of terminator type in the crossedset discriminations. Consider a simple numerical example. Suppose that Vp = 1 pixel/s and therefore Vt = 1.4 pixel/s with the present orientations. In the matched set, when observers compare an extrinsic standard with a perpendicular test, they should produce a speed match whenever Vp (standard) = Vp (test); whereas when they compare and intrinsic test with a vertical test, they should produce a speed match whenever average (Vt standard, Vp standard) = average (Vt test,vp test). But the latter condition is true whenever Vt (standard) = Vt (test) = 1.4 pixel/s and Vp (standard) = Vp (test) = 1 pixel/s. Hence, the predicted pattern of results is exactly the same as that predicted by the capture/suppression model. Conversely, in the crossed set, when observers compare an extrinsic standard with a vertical test, they should produce a speed match whenever Vp (standard) = average (Vt test, Vp test). For this condition to be true, then the Vt(test) speed which we plotted in our graphs must be greater than 1 pixel/s. For instance, if the extrinsic standard moves perpendicularly with speed = 1 pixel/s, the average (Vt test, Vp test) equals 1 when Vt(test) = 1.2 and Vp(test) = 0.8. By

27 How fast is the barberpole? 27 the same logic, when observers compare an intrinsic standard with a perpendicular test, they should produce a speed match whenever average (Vt standard, Vp standard) = Vp (test). For this condition to be true, then the Vp(test) speed which we plotted in our graphs must be smaller than 1.4 pixel/s. For instance, if the intrinsic standard moves (in a direction intermediate between vertical and perpendicular) with speed equal to average (Vt standard, Vp standard) = 1.2 pixel/s, this speed will match a Vp(test) = 1.2. Hence, in the crossed set data we should expect approximately the same matching speeds for both intrinsic and extrinsic terminator conditions. We conclude that the results of experiments 2 and 3 fully support an effect of terminator type on motion integration. In addition, we suggest that the nature of this effect can be captured by an averaging/suppression model in these conditions. Extrinsic terminators are suppressed, whereas intrinsic terminators compete with perpendicular components. Experiment 4 To further test the hypothesis that speed matches are influenced by perceived depth and therefore by the classification of contour terminators as intrinsic or extrinsic, we performed a final experiment. Experiment 4 was a replication of the matched-set conditions of experiment 3, except for the orientation of the rectangular frame. In experiment 3, as in the previous experiments, the rectangular frame was vertical. In

28 How fast is the barberpole? 28 experiment 4, we used a horizontal frame. This manipulation is interesting, because the use of a horizontal frame removes from the display one potential source of binocular information about depth order: the lack of binocular correspondence between small areas that are visibles by one eye, but not by the other ("Da Vinci" stereopsis, sometimes also called "half occlusions"). In a previous paper, Castet, Dufour & Bonnet (1999) reported that the barberpole effect was no longer abolished by extrinsic terminators when the barberpole configuration was horizontal rather than vertical (see also Anderson, 1999). This finding has been interpreted as evidence that retinal disparity is not sufficient, in itself, to cause the exclusion of Vt components from motion integration process. This is so because, when a horizontal frame is displaced binocularly relative to a line that abuts the horizontal contours of the frame, all parts of this line remain fully visible to each eye and therefore no "Da Vinci" stereopsis is present in the display. Conversely, when a vertical frame is subjected to an equivalent displacement relative to a line that abuts the vertical contours of the frame, parts of this line become visible to the left eye but remain invisible to the right eye, and other parts become visible to the right eye but remain invisible to the left eye. However, our informal observations of the barberpole displays tested in experiments 1-3 after rotating the rectangular frame to a horizontal orientation indicated that the abolition of the barberpole effect can be obtained even in these conditions, provided that one maintains fixation in the center of the frame during the motion. To

29 How fast is the barberpole? 29 corroborate this observation with more objective evidence, we measured speed matches as we did for vertical frames in the previous experiments. Methods Observers. Three observers participated. One of them (F.M.) was the first author. The other two (M.M. and C.F.) were unpaid members of the University of Trieste community and were naïve to the purpose of the study. All observers had normal or corrected-tonormal acuity and normal stereoscopic vision. Standard Stimuli. Standard stimuli were the same as those of the matched set used in experiment 3, except for the orientation of the rectangular frame and therefore the direction of the line presented within this frame. Given the orientation of the frame, the standard line moved with a Vt component in the horizontal direction. The Vp component of the standard line was exactly the same as that of all previous experiments. Test Stimuli. Test stimuli were same as those of the matched set used in experiment 3, except for the direction of their motion. Given that the standard stimuli were presented in horizontal frame in this experiment, the Vt component of the test stimuli was now a horizontal translation instead of vertical. The Vp component of the test stimuli was exactly the same as that of all previous experiments.

30 How fast is the barberpole? 30 Procedure and Analysis. They were the same as those of experiment 3. Results Mean observer matching speeds and standard deviations in the intrinsic and extrinsic conditions are presented individually in Figure 9. Speed matches exhibited a clear difference as a function of terminator type. Visual inspection of the suggested that these were quite close to the theoretical expectations based on Vt and Vp for the first two observers (F.M. and M.M.), whereas they were considerably higher than those expectations for the third. Confidence intervals around individual regression slopes confirmed this visual impression (see Table 4). Even for observer C.F., however, the difference in angular coefficients was quite close to the expected one (7 deg difference between the arctangents of the observed slopes, against 10 deg expected). Statistical analysis of this pattern confirmed that the matching speeds were linear functions of the standard speed with different slopes but equal intercepts. As in experiments 2 and 3, matches varied linearly with the Vp component speed of the standard, F (1, 2) = 23.1, p < The interaction between Vp component and disparity was significant, F (1, 2) = 21.2, p < 0.05, and the main effect of disparity was not, F (1, 2) = 4.8, p > 0.1. Also consistent with the previous experiments, there were individual differences in mean matches, F (2, 6) = 0.4, p > 0.6, as well as individual slopes as tested by the two-way interaction, F (2, 6) = 27.7, p <

31 How fast is the barberpole? 31 --Figure 9-- Discussion Given that the rotating the frame to a horizontal orientation removed "Da Vinci" stereopsis from these displays, the present results confirm that retinal disparity is sufficient, in itself, to produce a change in speed matches as a function of terminator type. Second, the effect of binocular disparity is present even in horizontal barberpoles. This finding is not consistent with reports by Castet, Dufour & Bonnet (1999) and Anderson (1999). These authors collected direction judgments and found that the barberpole effect was not abolished with extrinsic terminators and horizontal orientations of the rectangular frame. They interpeted this result as evidence that the crucial factor in determining the abolition is «Da Vinci» stereopsis, not retinal disparity. In our data, however, we found a strong effect of binocular disparity even in horizontal barberpoles, where «Da Vinci» stereopsis was not present. General Discussion We have developed and tested a speed matching paradigm aimed at investigating the effect of binocular disparity for depth on motion integration. Our results are in good general agreement with previous reports of such effects on barberpole displays (Shimojo,

32 How fast is the barberpole? 32 Silverman & Nakayama, 1989). This agreement validates the method and provides further support for the hypothesis that binocular disparity supports a form of perceptual classification of edge terminators, which in turn affects the relative importance of Vp and Vt components in the integration of motion components. Although our results are partly a replication of previous findings with a different methodology, they provide important evidence supporting the idea that directional judgements of relatively long-lasting complex displays may confound effects due to terminator classification with effects that have to do with where an observers is fixating, or with where and how the eyes move. That eye movements are an additional source of information for motion perception has been argued by several investigators (Beutter, Lorenceau & Stone, 1996; Beutter & Stone, 1997; Buetter & Stone, 1998; Wallach, 1982). Given that the systems involved in pursuit eye movements and in the perception of motion direction are localized both in MT and MST cortical areas, it is extremely plausible that these two functions are related. In particular, Buetter & Stone (1998) have showed similar perceptual and oculomotor biases with elongated apertures, suggesting that pursuit and perception share a critical motion processing stage. The importance of carefully controlling for fixation and eye movements along with figure/ground segregation is also demonstrated by the elegant study of Vallortigara & Bressan (1991). Vallortigara & Bressan investigated barberpole patterns presented with the addition of external prolongations, such as those in Figure 1c. They found that when the

33 How fast is the barberpole? 33 frame was narrower than the lines, then the Vp component of the external edges tended to capture the motion of the edge inside the rectangular window, abolishing the barberpole effect. This outcome is consistent with an effect of figure/ground segregation based on relative widths, promoting a classification of the terminators as extrinsic (see also Trueswell & Hayhoe, 1993). However, when frame and lines had equal widths, the prevalent direction was vertical, as in the standard effect, but only when observers fixated the central part of the display. When they distributed their attention on the whole configuration, the barberpole effect was again abolished. The experiments presented here controlled for fixation and eye movements in two ways. First, we asked observers to perform an initial depth adjustment task to force them to fixate at a target square, positioned in the center of the barberpole stimulus. Second, we measured speed matches instead of directional judgments, which allowed us to employ stimulus durations that were sufficiently brief to minimize the possibility of pursuit eye movements, or of conscious attempts to estimate the direction of motion. In addition, we used only movements such that both line terminators were always along the longer sides of the rectangle. This feature of our displays avoided the need to consider potential integrations of terminator components in different directions. Using this paradigm, we found evidence for an averaging/suppression model of motion integration (experiments 2-3) and for a role of binocular disparity in controlling the suppression effect (experiment 4). These findings are not consistent with other reports

34 How fast is the barberpole? 34 suggesting directional bistability with extrinsic terminators and a crucial role of "Da Vinci" stereopsis, rather than binocular disparity, in modulating the terminator effect (see Anderson, 1999; Castet, Charton, & Dufour, 1999). Rather, they suggest that motion integration obeys a simple coplanarity rule: components that are placed on the same depth plane by disparity information are integrated (via averaging); components that are placed on a different plane from that of a target object are excluded from the computation of that object's speed (via suppression). These findings also differ from reports that claimed bistability of barberpole displays having extrinsic terminators (Castet, Charton, & Dufour, 1999). In all our speed matching experiments, estimates of match variability were about the same with extrinsic and extrinsic terminators. Although many questions are still open about the process of motion integration in complex displays, we stress that further studies will have to pay greater attention to the potential effect of fixation and eye movements, especially when relatively long stimulus durations are employed.

35 How fast is the barberpole? 35 Author Note The present results were reported in part at the 1998 Meeting of the Association for Research on Vision and Ophtalmology (Ft. Lauderdale, FL, May ) and at the 1999 European Conference on Visual Perception (Trieste, Italy, August 1999). Supported by the University of Trieste and by MURST grant n to the second author.

36 How fast is the barberpole? 36 Footnotes 1. In a pilot experiment, speed matches were collected for intrinsic or extrinsic standards containing a set of several lines that were compared with test stimuli consisting of a circular aperture containing again several lines. Although all observers yielded slightly higher matches in the intrinsic condition relative to the extrinsic one, the difference between the two conditions was small and the statistical analysis failed to detect the expected difference in velocity matches as a function of terminator condition. Given that using several lines in the displays may produce spatiotemporal summation of motion signals and therefore floor effects, we decided to use single lines in all experiments.

37 How fast is the barberpole? 37 Figure Captions Figure 1. (a) The «aperture» problem: When a homogeneus contour moves inside a circular aperture its direction is ambiguous. (b) The «barberpole» effect: when a homogeneus line is moved inside an aperture, it tends to move in the direction of the aperture s longer side. (c) Effects of surrounding contours, fixation, and figure/ground segregation on the barberpole effect. In the left configuration, when attending to the inside of the rectangle one perceives vertical motion; conversely, when attending to the whole configuration one perceives perpendicular motion. In the right configuration, the rectangular frames is perceived behind the bars and only perpendicular motion is seen (from Vallortigara & Bressan, 1991). Figure 2. First experiment. Proportion of vertical judgments in extrinsic and intrinsic terminators. Figure 3. Perpendicular (Vp) and terminator (Vt) motion components for a single line translating within a rectangular aperture; their relation for a contour oriented at 45 degrees relative to the longer side of the aperture. Figure 4. Example of typical psychometric function in extrinsic (open diamonds) and in intrinsic (fill square) terminators. As is possible to observe that the intrinsic sigmoid is in

38 How fast is the barberpole? 38 advantage position with respect extrinsic sigmoid. Figure 5. Schematics of experimental trials. Each trial began with a static presentation of the standard plus a small green square on the same depth plane as the surrounding circle. Observers manipulated the disparity of this square probe until it appeared at the same depth plane of the rectangular frame. Next, they pressed the console spacebar to initiate the matching trial. The square disappeared, and the presentation of the standard motion began (204 ms). Finally, the test bar motion was presented (204 ms). The response was provided by pressing the left- (standard faster) or right (test faster) mouse buttons. Figure 6. Second experiment. Top: matching speeds (points of subjective equality) as a function of the Vp component in the standard bars (filled squares: crossed disparity rectangular frame in front of the bars; open circles: uncrossed disparity rectangular frame behind the bars; dashes: theoretical Vp (matching speeds = Vp standard speed) and Vt (matching speeds = Vp standard speed 2) matches. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Bottom: standard deviations of the judgments. Figure 7. Thirth experiment. Top: matching speeds (points of subjective equality) as a function of the Vp component in the standard bars (filled squares: crossed disparity rectangular frame in front of the bars; open circles: uncrossed disparity rectangular frame behind the bars) matches for terminators type. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Bottom: matching speeds (points of subjective equality) as a function of the Vp

39 How fast is the barberpole? 39 component in the standard bars (filled squares: vertical test moving; open circles: perpendicular test moving) matches for test motion type. Figure 8a. Third experiment matched-set. Top: matching speeds (points of subjective equality) as a function of the Vp component in the standard bars (filled squares: crossed disparity rectangular frame in front of the bars; open circles: uncrossed disparity rectangular frame behind the bars; dashes: theoretical Vp (matching speeds = Vp standard speed) and Vt (matching speeds = Vp standard speed 2) matches. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Bottom: standard deviations of the judgments. Figure 8b. Third experiment crossed-set. Figure 9. Fourth experiment. Top: matching speeds (points of subjective equality) as a function of the Vp component in the standard bars (filled squares: crossed disparity rectangular frame in front of the bars; open circles: uncrossed disparity rectangular frame behind the bars; dashes: theoretical Vp (matching speeds = Vp standard speed) and Vt (matching speeds = Vp standard speed 2) matches. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Bottom: standard deviations of the judgments. Tab 1. First experiment: individual slopes ± 2SE. Tab 2. Second experiment: individual slopes ± 2 SE.

40 How fast is the barberpole? 40 Tab 3. Third experiment: individual slopes ± 2 SE. Table 4. Fourth experiment: individual slopes ± 2 SE.

41 How fast is the barberpole? 41 References Adelson, E. H. & Movshon, J. A. (1982) Phenomenal coherence of moving visual pattterns. Nature, 300, Anderson, B. L. (1999) Stereoscopic occlusion and the aperture problem for motion: a new solution. Vision Research, 39, Ben-Av, M. B. & Shiffrar, M. (1995) Disambiguating velocity estimates across image space. Vision Research, 35, Beutter, B. R., Lorenceau, J. & Stone, L. S. (1996) Visual coherence affects smooth pursuit. Perception Supplement, 25, 5. Buetter, B. R. & Stone, L. S. (1997) Pursuit and direction perception are driven by similar and largely veridical object-motion signals. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science Supplement, 38, 693. Buetter, B. R. & Stone, L. S. (1998) Human motion perception and smooth eye movements show similar direction biases for elongated apertures. Vision Research, 38, 9, Bressan, P., Ganis, G. & Vallortigara, G. (1993) The role of depth stratification in

42 How fast is the barberpole? 42 the solution of the aperture problem. Perception, 22, Castet, E., Charton, V. & Dufour, A. (1999) The extrinsic/intrisic classification of 2D motion signals with barber-pole stimuli. Vision Research, 39, Castet, E., Lorenceau, J., Shiffrar, M. & Bonnet, C. (1993) Perceived speed of moving lines depends on orientation, lenght, speed and luminance. Vision Research, 14, Cornsweet, T. N. (1962). The staircase-method in psychophysics. American Journal ofpsychology, 75, Ferrera, V. P. & Wilson, H. R. (1990) Perceived direction of moving twodimentional patterns. Vision Research, 30, Gerbino, W & Bruno, N. (1997) Paradoxical rest. Perception,26, Hallet, E. (1986). Eye movements. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, and J. P. Thomas (Eds), Handbook of Perception and Human Performance, 10: New York: Wiley. Press. Hildreth, E. C. (1984) The measurement of visual motion. Cambridge, MA: MIT

43 How fast is the barberpole? 43 Hildreth, E. C. (1987) The analysis of visual motion: from computational theory to neural mechanisms. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 10, Kim, J., & Wilson, H. (1997) Motion integration over space: interaction of center and surround motion. Vision Research, 37, Liden, L. & Pack, C. The role of terminators and occlusio cues in motion integration and segmentation: a neural network model. Vision Research, 39, Mingolla, E., Todd, J. T., and Norman, J. F. (1992). The perception of globally coherent motion. Vision Research, 32, Movshon, J. A., Adelson, E. H., Gizzi, M. S. & Newsome, W. T. (1985) The analysis of moving visual patterns. In C. Chagas, R. Gattass & C. Gross (Eds.), Pattern recognition mechanisms, Vatican City: Vatican Press. Mussap, A. J. & Grotenhuis, K. T. (1996) The influence of aperture surface an the barber-pole illusion. Perception, 25, Ramachandran, V. S. (1990) Visual perception in people and machines. In A. Blake and T. Troschanko (Eds) AI and the eye. Wiley,

44 How fast is the barberpole? 44 Rubin, N. & Hochstein, S. (1993) Isolating the effect of one-dimensional motion signals on the perceived direction of moving two-dimensional objects. Vision Research, 33, Shimojo, S., Silverman, G. H., and Nakayama, K. (1989) Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion. Vision Research, 29, Stone, L. S., Buetter, B. R. & Lorenceau, J. (submitted) On the visual input driving human smooth pursuit eye movements. Stumpf, P. (1911). Über die Abhängigkeit der visuellen Bewegungsempfindung und ihres negativen Nachbildes von den Reizvorgängen auf der Netzhaut (Vorläufige Mitteilung). Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 59, Engl. Trans. by D.Todorovic, On the dependence of the visual sensation of motion and its negative aftereffect on the stimulation processes on the retina. Perception, 25, (1996). Trueswell, J. C. and Hayhoe, M. M. (1993) Apertures only constrain motion of figures not ground. Investigative Ophtalmology & Visual Science, S34, Tommasi, L. & Vallortigara, G. (1999) Figure ground segregation modulates perceived direction of ambiguous moving gratings and plaids. Vision Research, 39, 777-

45 How fast is the barberpole? Vallortigara, G. & Bressan, P. (1991) Occlusion and the perception of coherent motion.vision Research, 31, Wallach, H. (1935) Über visuell wahrgenommene Bewegungsrichtung. Psychologische Forscheung, 20, Engl. Transl. by S. Wuerger, R. Shapley, & N. Rubin, On the visually perceived direction of motion. Perception, 25, (1996). Wallach, H. (1982). Eye movements and motion perception. In A. H. Wertheim, V.A. Wagenaar, H.W. Leibowitz (Eds)Tutorials on motion perception, New York: Plenum Press, 1-18.

46 Figure 1. (a) How fast is the barberpole? 46 (c) (b)

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity S. B. Stevenson *, L. K. Cormack Vision Research 40, 2881-2884. (2000) * University of Houston College of Optometry, Houston TX 77204

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit Vision Research 45 (2005) 1877 1884 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit Mark Edwards *, John A. Greenwood School of Psychology, Australian National

More information

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth 1574 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/ Vol. 25, No. 7/ July 2008 Shioiri et al. Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth Satoshi Shioiri, 1, * Tomohiko

More information

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Current Biology, in press Supplemental Information: Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Grigorios Sotiropoulos, Aaron R. Seitz, and Peggy Seriès Supplemental Data Detailed

More information

Systematic perceptual distortion of 3D slant by disconjugate eye movements

Systematic perceptual distortion of 3D slant by disconjugate eye movements Vision Research 46 (2006) 2328 2335 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Systematic perceptual distortion of 3D slant by disconjugate eye movements Hyung-Chul O. Li * Department of Industrial Psychology, Kwangwoon

More information

Amodal representation depends on the object seen before partial occlusion

Amodal representation depends on the object seen before partial occlusion Vision Research 39 (1999) 283 292 Amodal representation depends on the object seen before partial occlusion Julian S. Joseph *, Ken Nakayama Vision Sciences Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Har ard

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system

Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system Vision Research 39 (1999) 4333 4340 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system Mark Edwards *, Clifton M. Schor School of Optometry, Uni ersity of California, Berkeley,

More information

Amodal Completion in Visual Search Preemption or Context Effects?

Amodal Completion in Visual Search Preemption or Context Effects? PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Amodal Completion in Visual Search Preemption or Context Effects? Robert Rauschenberger, 1 Mary A. Peterson, 1 Fauzia Mosca, 2,3 and Nicola Bruno 3 1 University of

More information

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli Vision Research 39 (1999) 3849 3854 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli A. Johnston a, *, C.P.

More information

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings Perception & Psychophysics 1980,27 (6),574-578 The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings ALISON BOWLING and WILLIAM LOVEGROVE University oftasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia 700/

More information

BINOCULAR DEPTH PERCEPTION IN SMALL-ANGLE

BINOCULAR DEPTH PERCEPTION IN SMALL-ANGLE Brit. J. Ophthal. (1959) 43, 662. BINOCULAR DEPTH PERCEPTION IN SMALL-ANGLE STRABISMUS* BY E. J. NAYLOR AND A. STANWORTH Department of Ophthalmology, University of Manchester MEASUREMENTS of the binocular

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK?

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK? CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK? Bobby Nguyen 1, Yan Zhuo 2 & Rui Ni 1 1 Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA 2 Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy

More information

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth Journal of Vision (2010) 10(4):1, 1 27 http://journalofvision.org/10/4/1/ 1 Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth Anna Seydell David C. Knill Julia Trommershäuser Department

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

7 Grip aperture and target shape

7 Grip aperture and target shape 7 Grip aperture and target shape Based on: Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. The influence of target object shape on maximum grip aperture in human grasping movements. Exp Brain Res, In revision 103 Introduction

More information

2012 Course : The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Science

2012 Course : The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Science 2012 Course : The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Science Stanislas Dehaene Chair in Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture No. 4 Constraints combination and selection of a

More information

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Fundamentals of Psychophysics Fundamentals of Psychophysics John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3045! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Visual neuroscience physiology stimulus How do we see the world? neuroimaging

More information

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Paul E. Hemeren (paul.hemeren@his.se) University of Skövde, School of Humanities and Informatics

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk

Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk Supplementary Materials fmri response (!% BOLD) ).5 CD versus STS 1 wedge

More information

What is mid level vision? Mid Level Vision. What is mid level vision? Lightness perception as revealed by lightness illusions

What is mid level vision? Mid Level Vision. What is mid level vision? Lightness perception as revealed by lightness illusions What is mid level vision? Mid Level Vision March 18, 2004 Josh McDermott Perception involves inferring the structure of the world from measurements of energy generated by the world (in vision, this is

More information

Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot

Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot Rajani Raman* and Sandip Sarkar Saha Institute of Nuclear Physics, HBNI, 1/AF, Bidhannagar,

More information

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH Laurence D. Smith University of Maine ldsmith@maine.maine.edu Ronald M. Pickett Marjan Trutschl Institute for Visualization and Perception

More information

Seeing motion in depth using inter-ocular velocity differences

Seeing motion in depth using inter-ocular velocity differences Vision Research 45 (2005) 2786 2798 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Seeing motion in depth using inter-ocular velocity differences Julian Martin Fernandez *, Bart Farell Institute for Sensory Research,

More information

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 916-920 Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion JOHN PREDEBON University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia Two experiments

More information

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E. In D. Algom, D. Zakay, E. Chajut, S. Shaki, Y. Mama, & V. Shakuf (Eds.). (2011). Fechner Day 2011: Proceedings of the 27 th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 89-94). Raanana,

More information

The Dynamics of Bi-stable Alternation in Ambiguous Motion

The Dynamics of Bi-stable Alternation in Ambiguous Motion Vision Research, in press, 2003 The Dynamics of Bi-stable Alternation in Ambiguous Motion Displays: a Fresh Look at Plaids Jean-Michel Hupé and Nava Rubin Center for Neural Science New York University

More information

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON Journal of Experimental Psychology 1971, Vol. 89, No. 1, 147-151 JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION DOMINIC W. MASSARO» University of Wisconsin AND NORMAN H. ANDERSON University of California,

More information

Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions. Arthur Shapiro and Dejan Todorovic, Editors. Chapter xx. Illusory Color Spread from Apparent Motion

Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions. Arthur Shapiro and Dejan Todorovic, Editors. Chapter xx. Illusory Color Spread from Apparent Motion Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions Arthur Shapiro and Dejan Todorovic, Editors Chapter xx Illusory Color Spread from Apparent Motion Carol M. Cicerone 1, Professor Emeritus, and Donald D. Hoffman, Professor

More information

Opposite Influence of Perceptual Memory on Initial and Prolonged Perception of Sensory Ambiguity

Opposite Influence of Perceptual Memory on Initial and Prolonged Perception of Sensory Ambiguity Opposite Influence of Perceptual Memory on Initial and Prolonged Perception of Sensory Ambiguity Maartje Cathelijne de Jong 1 *, Tomas Knapen 2, Raymond van Ee 1,3,4,5 1 Physics of Man, Helmholtz Institute,

More information

Lateral interactions in visual perception of temporal signals: cortical and subcortical components

Lateral interactions in visual perception of temporal signals: cortical and subcortical components PSYCHOLOGY NEUROSCIENCE Psychology & Neuroscience, 2011, 4, 1, 57-65 DOI: 10.3922/j.psns.2011.1.007 Lateral interactions in visual perception of temporal signals: cortical and subcortical components Claudio

More information

The oblique plaid effect

The oblique plaid effect 06/25/2003, Vision Research (under review) The oblique plaid effect Jean-Michel Hupé *, Nava Rubin Center for Neural Science, New York University, 4 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003, USA Abstract Plaids

More information

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Materials Supplementary Materials Supplementary Figure S1: Data of all 106 subjects in Experiment 1, with each rectangle corresponding to one subject. Data from each of the two identical sub-sessions are shown separately.

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES

THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES IN THE VISUAL CORTICAL HIERARCHY JOHAN WAGEMANS LABORATORY OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF LEUVEN, BELGIUM DIPARTIMENTO DI PSICOLOGIA, UNIVERSITÀ DI MILANO-BICOCCA,

More information

Consciousness The final frontier!

Consciousness The final frontier! Consciousness The final frontier! How to Define it??? awareness perception - automatic and controlled memory - implicit and explicit ability to tell us about experiencing it attention. And the bottleneck

More information

Visual Search: A Novel Psychophysics for Preattentive Vision

Visual Search: A Novel Psychophysics for Preattentive Vision Visual Search: A Novel Psychophysics for Preattentive Vision Elizabeth Williams under the direction of Dr. Jeremy M. Wolfe and Ms. Serena Butcher Brigham and Women s Hospital Research Science Institute

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Calculating volume scotomas for patients with central scotomas

Calculating volume scotomas for patients with central scotomas APPENDIX Calculating volume scotomas for patients with central scotomas This appendix provides formulas to derive the shape and extent of volume scotomas for some simple examples of bilateral central field

More information

Learning Bayesian priors for depth perception

Learning Bayesian priors for depth perception Learning Bayesian priors for depth perception David C. Knill Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, 14627 How the visual system learns the statistical regularities (e.g. symmetry)

More information

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get IAT 355 Perception 1 Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get Why we need to understand perception The ability of viewers to interpret visual (graphical) encodings of information and

More information

Local Disparity Not Perceived Depth Is Signaled by Binocular Neurons in Cortical Area V1 of the Macaque

Local Disparity Not Perceived Depth Is Signaled by Binocular Neurons in Cortical Area V1 of the Macaque The Journal of Neuroscience, June 15, 2000, 20(12):4758 4767 Local Disparity Not Perceived Depth Is Signaled by Binocular Neurons in Cortical Area V1 of the Macaque Bruce G. Cumming and Andrew J. Parker

More information

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE Åke Hellström Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, S-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden Email: hellst@psychology.su.se Abstract Participants compared successive

More information

Outline: Vergence Eye Movements: Classification I. Describe with 3 degrees of freedom- Horiz, Vert, torsion II. Quantifying units- deg, PD, MA III.

Outline: Vergence Eye Movements: Classification I. Describe with 3 degrees of freedom- Horiz, Vert, torsion II. Quantifying units- deg, PD, MA III. Outline: Vergence Eye Movements: Classification I. Describe with 3 degrees of freedom- Horiz, Vert, torsion II. Quantifying units- deg, PD, MA III. Measurement of Vergence:- Objective & Subjective phoria

More information

Visual search for a circular region perceived as a figure versus as a hole: Evidence of the importance of part structure

Visual search for a circular region perceived as a figure versus as a hole: Evidence of the importance of part structure Journal Perception & Psychophysics 2006,?? 68 (?), (5),???-??? 776-791 Visual search for a circular region perceived as a figure versus as a hole: Evidence of the importance of part structure MARCO BERTAMINI

More information

Integration of one- and two-dimensional motion signals in infants: Evidence from the barber-pole illusion

Integration of one- and two-dimensional motion signals in infants: Evidence from the barber-pole illusion Vision Research 46 (2006) 3360 3372 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Integration of one- and two-dimensional motion signals in infants: Evidence from the barber-pole illusion Karen R. Dobkins a, *, Lindsay

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Effects of Attention on MT and MST Neuronal Activity During Pursuit Initiation

Effects of Attention on MT and MST Neuronal Activity During Pursuit Initiation Effects of Attention on MT and MST Neuronal Activity During Pursuit Initiation GREGG H. RECANZONE 1 AND ROBERT H. WURTZ 2 1 Center for Neuroscience and Section of Neurobiology, Physiology and Behavior,

More information

Short-term temporal recruitment in structure from motion

Short-term temporal recruitment in structure from motion Vision Research 42 (2002) 1213 1223 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Short-term temporal recruitment in structure from motion Corrado Caudek a, *, Fulvio Domini b, Massimiliano Di Luca b a Department of

More information

Gathering and Repetition of the Elements in an Image Affect the Perception of Order and Disorder

Gathering and Repetition of the Elements in an Image Affect the Perception of Order and Disorder International Journal of Affective Engineering Vol.13 No.3 pp.167-173 (2014) ORIGINAL ARTICLE Gathering and Repetition of the Elements in an Image Affect the Perception of Order and Disorder Yusuke MATSUDA

More information

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* Perception & Psychophysics 1973. Vol. 13. So. 2.356-360 Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* ANGEL VASSILEV+ Institu te of Physiology. Bulgarian Academy of Sciences.

More information

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination Timothy N. Rubin (trubin@uci.edu) Michael D. Lee (mdlee@uci.edu) Charles F. Chubb (cchubb@uci.edu) Department of Cognitive

More information

Motion parallel to line orientation: Disambiguation of motion percepts

Motion parallel to line orientation: Disambiguation of motion percepts ion, 1999, volume 28, pages 1243 ^ 1255 DOI:.68/p298 Motion parallel to line orientation: Disambiguation of motion percepts Gregory Francis, Hyungjun Kim Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University,

More information

An Exponential Pyramid-Based Model of Contour Classification in Figure-Ground Segregation

An Exponential Pyramid-Based Model of Contour Classification in Figure-Ground Segregation An Exponential Pyramid-Based Model of Contour Classification in Figure-Ground Segregation MICHAEL R. SCHEESSELE Indiana University at South Bend and ZYGMUNT PIZLO Purdue University In a retinal image,

More information

Principals of Object Perception

Principals of Object Perception Principals of Object Perception Elizabeth S. Spelke COGNITIVE SCIENCE 14, 29-56 (1990) Cornell University Summary Infants perceive object by analyzing tree-dimensional surface arrangements and motions.

More information

The effect of contrast intensity and polarity in the achromatic watercolor effect

The effect of contrast intensity and polarity in the achromatic watercolor effect Journal of Vision (2011) 11(3):18, 1 8 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/3/18 1 The effect of contrast intensity and polarity in the achromatic watercolor effect Bo Cao Arash Yazdanbakhsh Ennio

More information

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks*

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks* The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks* ARNOLD D. WELLt University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 Multidimensional stimuli, which could vary on one, two, or all three

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items

Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items Journal of Vision (2003) 3, 6-21 http://journalofvision.org/3/1/2/ 6 Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items Jun Saiki PRESTO, JST, Kawaguchi, Japan; and Graduate School

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 J'OURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2, 446-452 (1963) Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 A visual configuration, or pattern, has qualities over and above those which can be specified

More information

Structure mapping in spatial reasoning

Structure mapping in spatial reasoning Cognitive Development 17 (2002) 1157 1183 Structure mapping in spatial reasoning Merideth Gattis Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, Germany Received 1 June 2001; received in revised

More information

Perceptual grouping in space and time: Evidence from the Ternus display

Perceptual grouping in space and time: Evidence from the Ternus display Perception & Psychophysics 1997, 59 (1), 87-99 Perceptual grouping in space and time: Evidence from the Ternus display PETER KRAMER and STEVEN YANTIS Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland We report

More information

The global slowdown effect: Why does perceptual grouping reduce perceived speed?

The global slowdown effect: Why does perceptual grouping reduce perceived speed? Atten Percept Psychophys (2014) 76:780 792 DOI 10.3758/s13414-013-0607-x The global slowdown effect: Why does perceptual grouping reduce perceived speed? Peter Jes Kohler & Gideon Paul Caplovitz & Peter

More information

Department of Computer Science, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK;

Department of Computer Science, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK; vision Article Ocularity Feature Contrast Attracts Attention Exogenously Li Zhaoping Department of Computer Science, University College London, London WCE 6BT, UK; z.li@ucl.ac.uk Received: 7 December 27;

More information

General and specific perceptual learning in radial speed discrimination

General and specific perceptual learning in radial speed discrimination Journal of Vision (2011) 11(4):7, 1 11 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/4/7 1 General and specific perceptual learning in radial speed discrimination Xuan Huang Hongjing Lu Yifeng Zhou Zili Liu

More information

Sensory Adaptation within a Bayesian Framework for Perception

Sensory Adaptation within a Bayesian Framework for Perception presented at: NIPS-05, Vancouver BC Canada, Dec 2005. published in: Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems eds. Y. Weiss, B. Schölkopf, and J. Platt volume 18, pages 1291-1298, May 2006 MIT

More information

Segregation from direction differences in dynamic random-dot stimuli

Segregation from direction differences in dynamic random-dot stimuli Vision Research 43(2003) 171 180 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Segregation from direction differences in dynamic random-dot stimuli Scott N.J. Watamaniuk *, Jeff Flinn, R. Eric Stohr Department of Psychology,

More information

Dynamical vs. judgmental comparison: hysteresis effects in motion perception

Dynamical vs. judgmental comparison: hysteresis effects in motion perception Spatial Vision, Vol. 18, No. 3, pp. 317 335 (2005) VSP 2005. Also available online - www.vsppub.com Dynamical vs. judgmental comparison: hysteresis effects in motion perception HOWARD S. HOCK, LORI BUKOWSKI,

More information

Perceptual learning on orientation and direction discrimination

Perceptual learning on orientation and direction discrimination Vision Research 39 (1999) 3692 3701 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Perceptual learning on orientation and direction discrimination Nestor Matthews a, Zili Liu b, Bard J. Geesaman c, Ning Qian a, * a Columbia

More information

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory Alejandro Lleras, 1 Ronald A. Rensink, 2 and James T. Enns

More information

Chromatic and luminance signals in visual memory

Chromatic and luminance signals in visual memory W. Sachtler and Q. Zaidi Vol. 9, No. 6/June 1992/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 877 Chromatic and luminance signals in visual memory W. Sachtler and Qasim Zaidi epartment of Psychology, Columbia University, New York,

More information

Aging and the Detection of Collision Events in Fog

Aging and the Detection of Collision Events in Fog University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2009 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 23rd, 12:00 AM Aging and the Detection of Collision Events in Fog Zheng Bian University of California,

More information

There s more behind it: Perceived depth order biases perceived numerosity/density

There s more behind it: Perceived depth order biases perceived numerosity/density Journal of Vision (2012) 12(12):9, 1 16 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/12/12/9 1 There s more behind it: Perceived depth order biases perceived numerosity/density Alexander C. Schutz Abteilung

More information

The Visual Perception of Three-Dimensional Length

The Visual Perception of Three-Dimensional Length Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Human Perception and Performance 0096-1523/96/$3.00 1996, Vol. 22, No. l, 173-186 The Visual Perception

More information

Latency differences and the flash-lag effect

Latency differences and the flash-lag effect Vision Research 43 (2003) 1829 1835 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Latency differences and the flash-lag effect Derek H. Arnold a,b, *, Szonya Durant a,c, Alan Johnston a,b,c a Department of Psychology,

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Seeing Sound: Changing Visual Perception Through Cross-Modal Interaction. Tracey D. Berger. New York University. Department of Psychology

Seeing Sound: Changing Visual Perception Through Cross-Modal Interaction. Tracey D. Berger. New York University. Department of Psychology Cross-Modal Effects on Perception 1 Seeing Sound: Changing Visual Perception Through Cross-Modal Interaction. Tracey D. Berger New York University Department of Psychology Faculty Sponsor : Denis Pelli

More information

Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression

Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression Supplementary Material Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression 1, 2, * Bahador Bahrami Petra Vetter 1, 3 Eva Spolaore 1, 4 Silvia Pagano 1, 4 Brian Butterworth 1,3 Geraint Rees 1,

More information

Representational Momentum Beyond Internalized Physics

Representational Momentum Beyond Internalized Physics CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Representational Momentum Beyond Internalized Physics Embodied Mechanisms of Anticipation Cause Errors in Visual Short-Term Memory Dirk Kerzel University of

More information

Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system

Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system Vision Research 38 (1998) 3057 3068 Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system Clifton M. Schor *, Mark Edwards, David R. Pope School of Optometry, Uni ersity of California,

More information

Spatial or Temporal 2AFC May Give Different Results Depending on Context

Spatial or Temporal 2AFC May Give Different Results Depending on Context Spatial or Temporal 2AFC May Give Different Results Depending on Context E. Peli 1, M.A. García-Pérez 2, R.G. Giorgi 1, R.L. Woods 1 1 Schepens Eye Research Institute, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA

More information

Image-Based Grouping during Binocular Rivalry Is Dictated by Eye-Of-Origin

Image-Based Grouping during Binocular Rivalry Is Dictated by Eye-Of-Origin Image-Based Grouping during Binocular Rivalry Is Dictated by Eye-Of-Origin Sjoerd M. Stuit 1 *, Chris L. E. Paffen 1, Maarten J. van der Smagt 1, Frans A. J. Verstraten 1,2 1 Universiteit Utrecht, Neuroscience

More information

Spatial versus temporal grouping in a modified Ternus display

Spatial versus temporal grouping in a modified Ternus display Vision Research 47 (2007) 2353 2366 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Spatial versus temporal grouping in a modified Ternus display Julian M. Wallace *, Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel Department of Experimental

More information

The perceived structural shape of thin (wire-like) objects is different from that of silhouettes

The perceived structural shape of thin (wire-like) objects is different from that of silhouettes Perception, 2006, volume 35, pages 1679 ^ 1692 DOI:10.1068/p5534 The perceived structural shape of thin (wire-like) objects is different from that of silhouettes Marco Bertamini, Tracy Farrant School of

More information

Attention capacity and task difficulty in visual search

Attention capacity and task difficulty in visual search Cognition 94 (2005) B101 B111 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Attention capacity and task difficulty in visual search Liqiang Huang, Harold Pashler* Department of Psychology 0109, University

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES?

PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES? PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS AND ERGONOMICS SOCIETY 8th ANNUAL MEETING 88 PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES? J. E. Thropp,

More information

The dependence of motion repulsion and rivalry on the distance between moving elements

The dependence of motion repulsion and rivalry on the distance between moving elements Vision Research 40 (2000) 2025 2036 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres The dependence of motion repulsion and rivalry on the distance between moving elements Nestor Matthews a, Bard J. Geesaman b, Ning Qian

More information

Mechanisms of generalization perceptual learning

Mechanisms of generalization perceptual learning Mechanisms of generalization perceptual learning In Zili Lin Rutgers University, Newark Daphna Weinshall Hebrew University, Israel Abstract The learning of many visual perceptual tasks has been shown to

More information

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1996, 3 (2), 222 226 Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli ROBERT M. NOSOFSKY Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana and THOMAS J. PALMERI Vanderbilt University,

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

Evidence of Stereoscopic Surface Disambiguation in the Responses of V1 Neurons

Evidence of Stereoscopic Surface Disambiguation in the Responses of V1 Neurons Cerebral Cortex, 1, 1 1 doi:.93/cercor/bhw4 Original Article 7 1 2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Evidence of Stereoscopic Surface Disambiguation in the Responses of V1 Neurons Jason M. Samonds 1,4, Christopher W. Tyler

More information

Detection of the sign of expansion as a function of field size and eccentricity

Detection of the sign of expansion as a function of field size and eccentricity Perception & Psychophysics 1996, 58 (3), 401-408 Detection of the sign of expansion as a function of field size and eccentricity SUSAN F. TE PAS, ASTRID M. L. KAPPERS, and JAN J. KOENDERINK Universiteit

More information

A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND. CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica

A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND. CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND CONTINGENT AFTER-EFFECT CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica A model is advanced to explain the McCollough effect and the contingent motion after-effect.

More information

Stream/bounce perception and the effect of depth cues in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)

Stream/bounce perception and the effect of depth cues in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:1532 1545 DOI 10.3758/s13414-011-0126-6 Stream/bounce perception and the effect of depth cues in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Toyomi Matsuno & Masaki Tomonaga Published

More information

Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence. Edwin Blake

Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence. Edwin Blake Presence and Perception: theoretical links & empirical evidence Edwin Blake edwin@cs.uct.ac.za This Talk 2 Perception Bottom-up Top-down Integration Presence Bottom-up Top-down BIPs Presence arises from

More information