Memorization of serial items by Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans 1

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Memorization of serial items by Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans 1"

Transcription

1 Japanese Psychological Research 1997, Volume 39, No. 3, Special Issue: Cognition and behavior of chimpanzees Memorization of serial items by Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans 1 NOBUAKI OHSHIBA 2 Faculty of Human Sciences, Osaka University, Yamadaoka, Suita 565, Japan Abstract: The memorization of serial items was investigated with three Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and six humans. The subjects were trained to touch two to four different-sized circles in an order determined by the experimenter. Two types of list were presented: monotonic and nonmonotonic. The subjects performed better with monotonic than with nonmonotonic lists, both in terms of the percentage of correct trials and reaction times. The subjects search strategies were classified as either a serial search or a collective search. The chimpanzee more often used a collective search than a serial search, compared with the Japanese monkeys. This tendency was also prevalent with the human subjects. Therefore, a collective search might be an evolutionarily new cognitive function. A serial search model based on evaluation and motion is proposed, and this could well explain the subjects reaction times. Key words: list learning, simultaneous chain, monotonicity, serial search, species differences. Simultaneous chaining, which was developed by Terrace (1983), is a commonly used experimental paradigm for investigating the seriallearning capacity of animals and humans. For example, Terrace (1987, 1991) and Terrace and Chen (1991a, 1991b) used simultaneous chaining to determine that pigeons may form chunks. In a study by D Amato and Colombo (1988), five tufted capuchins learned either a four-item list (A B C D) or a five-item list (A B C D E). After the training was complete, the subjects were presented two- or three-item subset lists extracted from the learned lists. The subjects tended to select the items in these subset lists in the order of A B C D or A B C D E. An analysis of their reaction times revealed that the later the position in the original list that the initial item of the subset list occupied, the longer it took to respond to that item, and that as the number of items increased that were between the initial item and the second item in the subset lists, the reaction time to that second item also increased. D Amato and Colombo (1989) then found that the subjects performance remained stable when one or two items were replaced with wild card(s), suggesting that the subjects did not select items based on any association between adjacent items, but did form representations as to what items occupied the positions in the lists. 1 This research was supported by the Cooperation Research Program of the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, from 1994 to 1995, and was conducted at the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University. 2 I am grateful to Professor Tetsuro Matsuzawa, Associate Professor Kazuo Fujita, and other members of the faculty and staff of the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, for their generous help. I thank Professors Naosuke Itoigawa and Tetsuhiro Minami of the Faculty of Human Sciences, Osaka University, for their constructive instruction. Thanks are also due to anonymous referees Japanese Psychological Association. Published by Blackwell Publishers Ltd, 108 Cowley Road, Oxford OX4 1JF, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA.

2 Memorization of serial items by primates 237 Still photographs as well as geometric forms have been used as stimuli. In a study by Swartz, Chen, and Terrace (1991), two rhesus monkeys learned eight 4-item lists consisting of pictures of birds, flowers, frogs, and so on. The reaction times to the subset lists implied that the subjects formed linear representations of the lists. Tomonaga, Matsuzawa, and Itakura (1993) used a modified version of a simultaneous chain, where the selected items disappeared, to have a chimpanzee learn a list consisting of numerals. An analysis of the reaction times revealed a symbolic distance effect: the greater the difference between the two numerals presented, the shorter the reaction time the subject needed to judge which was smaller than the other. The simultaneous chain paradigm has also been applied to human subjects. In a study by Terrace and McGonigle (1994), 5- and 7-yearold children learned three types of simultaneous chain. One was an arbitrary list consisting of five colored rectangles (white red green yellow pink). The remaining two lists consisted of five different-sized rectangles (labeled, from small to large, 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5); one was a monotonic list ( ), and the other was a nonmonotonic list ( ). Both the 5- and 7-year-old children showed poorer performance for the nonmonotonic list than for either the arbitrary list or a monotonic list. The 7-year-old children made significantly fewer errors with the monotonic list than with the arbitrary list. However, there was no significant difference in the performance of the 5-year-old children between the number of errors for the monotonic list and that for the arbitrary list. Based on these findings, the authors concluded that humans need to be 7 or more years old to use a selection strategy exploiting a rule, which in this case was the monotonicity among the sizes of the stimulus items. As stated above, there have been various studies using different species (pigeons, monkeys, chimpanzees, humans, etc.) and different types of lists (e.g., monotonic and nonmonotonic). However, an integrated investigation focusing on these factors has not yet been performed. Therefore, three comparisons were made in the present study. The first involved the difference in performance between the original simultaneous chain invented by Terrace (1983) and the modified version used by Tomonaga et al. (1993). In the original version of the simultaneous chain, the selected stimuli remain throughout a trial. In the version of Tomonaga et al., the selected stimuli are removed, or disappear. The present study examined how this difference in procedure affected subjects performance. The second comparison involved the difference in performance between a monotonic list and a nonmonotonic list. Hulse (1978) found for rats and Terrace et al. (1994) found for 7-year-old human children that performance with monotonic lists is better than that with nonmonotonic lists. The present study investigated whether or not this finding also applies to Japanese monkeys and chimpanzees. The third comparison involved inter-species differences in list-learning capacity among Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans. Although there have been separate studies of different species, there is no previous integrated study directly comparing the listlearning capacities of several primate species. Clarifying the differences among these closely related species might contribute to our understanding of cognitive evolution. Method Subjects Three Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata), Atsushi, Kyoko, and Yusaku, a female chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes), Pan, and six humans (Homo sapiens) served as subjects. All the monkey subjects were laboratoryborn, and had had no experience of learning sequences experimentally. They were housed in individual cages with ad-libitum access to water. Daily food (biscuits and sweet potatoes) was delivered after each experimental session. The daily rations were sufficient to maintain the subjects at more than 90% of their freefeeding body weights. Atsushi and Yusaku were 4-year-old males at the start of the experiment, and Kyoko was a 6-year-old female.

3 238 N. Ohshiba The chimpanzee subject, Pan, had had extensive experience with experiments on perceptual and cognitive abilities before the present experiment (e.g., Kojima, 1989; Tanaka, 1995, 1996). She lived in an open enclosure with other chimpanzees. Daily food (biscuits, apples, bananas, sweet potatoes, etc.) was delivered at noon and in the evening. No restrictions on food or water were imposed. She was 11 years old at the start of the experiment. The monkeys and the chimpanzee were handled strictly following the guidelines of the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University. All the human subjects were graduate students at the Primate Research Institute of Kyoto University. Five were males and one was a female. They ranged in age from 24 to 29 years. They were divided into two groups of three, and labeled A1, A2, A3, B1, B2, and B3. The author was not one of the subjects. Apparatus For the monkeys. The chamber in which the monkeys worked measured cm. The front panel housed a 14-inch color monitor ( pixels) on which the stimuli appeared. The monitor supported a touch-screen system. Small pieces of an apple or sweet potato served as reinforcers, and were delivered to a food cup located on the left of the panel. The experimental room was illuminated by a white bulb. Extraneous sounds were masked by white noise. Displaying of stimuli, delivery of food rewards, illumination, and recording of data were all controlled by a personal computer (NEC PC9801RA). For the chimpanzee and the humans. The room in which the chimpanzee and the humans worked measured cm. The front panel housed a 20-inch color monitor ( pixels) on which stimuli appeared. The monitor again had a touch-screen system. Small pieces of an apple served as reinforcers and were delivered to a food tray beneath the monitor. The experimental room was illuminated by fluorescent lights. Displaying of stimuli, delivery of food rewards, and recording of data were all controlled by a personal computer (NEC PC9801 Xn). Stimuli The same stimuli were used for all species. The stimuli were four different-sized circles labeled 1, 2, 3, and 4, and were 18, 36, 54, and 72 pixels in diameter, respectively. The colors of the circles varied according to the subjects and the tasks. For the disappearing-type simultaneous chain, the monotonic list was , and the nonmonotonic list was For the remaining-type simultaneous chain, the monotonic list was , and the nonmonotonic list was For half the human subjects, the order of the items was reversed. The lists were designated A B C D, respectively. Procedure General. Almost identical procedures were used for all the species. The disappearing-type simultaneous chain was investigated first, and the remaining-type simultaneous chain was examined next. For either type of simultaneous chain, the subjects were to respond to the stimuli in the order requested by the experimenter. There were two orders for each subject and each task. These two orders were cued by the color of the stimulus item. If the stimuli were all one color (say, red), then the subjects were required to select the items in the order or (monotonic lists). If the stimuli were another color (say, cyan), then the subjects were required to follow the order or (nonmonotonic lists). When a trial began, a start key (a black rectangle) appeared at the center of the monitor. If the subject touched the start key, it disappeared and two to four same-colored and different-sized stimuli appeared. There were four positions on the screen (upper right, upper left, lower right, and lower left) at which the stimuli could appear, and the configurations of the stimuli were randomized and counterbalanced across trials. The subjects had to select the stimuli by touching them in the order determined by the experimenter. If the subject successfully selected all the stimuli, an electric chime was sounded and a food reward was provided. If the subject made an error by selecting a wrong item or failed to respond to

4 Memorization of serial items by primates 239 any of the items after 10 s, then all the stimuli disappeared, the room light was turned off for 5 s (blackout, for monkey subjects only), and the same trial was repeated (correction). Intertrial intervals (ITI) were 5 s. Each session consisted of 192 trials: 96 trials with the monotonic list and 96 trials with the nonmonotonic list. Training. Training was conducted with the monkeys and the chimpanzee, but not with the humans. The subjects were first trained with a two-item list, A B. After the subjects performance reached the criterion level of 90% correct trials, item C was added to the tail of A B, and the subjects were trained with a three-item list, A B C. After the subjects performance reached the criterion level of 85% correct trials, item D was added to the tail of A B C. The criterion for the four-item list, A B C D, was 80% correct trials. Even if the performance for one list reached the criterion, training was continued until the performance for the other list satisfied the criterion. Hence, the subjects had the same amount of experience with the monotonic and nonmonotonic lists. Kyoko started with a four-item list for the disappearing-type simultaneous chain. Reaction times to subset lists. All the possible two-, three-, and four-item subset lists were presented in each session. Each subset list was tested 120 times. The procedures were the same as in the training. Reaction times were measured in units of 10 ms. For human subjects, only the four-item lists were presented. Paired t-tests and two-factor ANOVA were used for statistical analyses. A p value less than 0.05 was considered significant. Results Training General. Acquisition functions for the monkeys and the chimpanzee are shown in Figure 1. It took more sessions to acquire the remainingtype simultaneous chain than the disappearingtype chain. Paired t-tests revealed that the percentage of correct trials for the monotonic lists was significantly better than that for the nonmonotonic lists for all the subjects for the Figure 1a. For caption see next page.

5 240 N. Ohshiba Figure 1b. (a) Percentage of correct trials as a function of the number of sessions for disappearingtype simultaneous chains in training. Kyoko was not trained with either two- or three-item lists. (b) Percentage of correct trials as a function of the number of sessions for remaining-type simultaneous chains in training. M = monotonic; NM = nonmonotonic.

6 Memorization of serial items by primates 241 Table 1. Significance of difference in the percentage of correct trials between monotonic lists and for nonmonotonic lists during training, by the paired t-test Subjects Type of task Atsushi Kyoko Yusaku Pan Disappearing t(14) = t(12) = t(16) = t(17) = p.01 p.01 p.01 ns Remaining t(11) =.498 t(57) = t(54) = t(29) = ns ns p.05 p.01 disappearing-type simultaneous chain, but only for Yusaku for the remaining-type chain (Table 1). Disappearing-type simultaneous chain. When the lists were increased in length, performance with the nonmonotonic lists deteriorated, whereas that with the monotonic lists did not. These results imply that the subjects regarded the monotonicity of the list items as a rule (in this case, from smaller to larger). Hence, the subjects could be expected to make a correct response regardless of the length of the monotonic lists. Remaining-type simultaneous chain. The difference in performance between the monotonic lists and the nonmonotonic lists was not as remarkable as it was for the disappearingtype simultaneous chain. However, the performance with the four-item monotonic list was better than that with its nonmonotonic counterpart. Reaction times to subset lists General. Figures 2 and 3 present the reaction times to each item of all the possible subset lists. Two-factor analyses of variance type of list (monotonic/nonmonotonic) item were conducted for each subject and each task (Table 2). The reaction times with the nonmonotonic lists were significantly longer than those with the monotonic lists. This implies that the nonmonotonic lists were more difficult to perform than the monotonic lists. The interaction between type of list and item was not tested since the factor item branched under the factor type of list. Disappearing-type simultaneous chain. Let us examine the performance with the two-item subset lists for the disappearing-type simultaneous chain (Figure 2). The reaction times to the second or final items were almost uniform. In these trials, the stimulus items disappeared as soon as the subjects touched them. When the final item was to be selected, no other item was available. Hence, the subjects did not have to consider which item to select. Naturally, this was also true in the three- and four-item subset lists. We found that the later the position of an item in the original list that the initial item of the subset list occupied, the longer was the reaction time to that item. For example, in the case of the first item in the two-item subset lists, the reaction times when B was the first item were longer than when A was the first item, and the reaction times when C was the first item were even longer. This was presumably because the subjects searched for the items in the order A B C. These results suggest that the subjects performed serial searches. The subjects first tried to locate item A; if they could locate it, then they selected it, otherwise they tried to locate item B, and so on. This assumption explains the prolonged reaction time to item C in the subset list A C D and that to item B in the subset list B C D. D Amato and Colombo (1988) found that the reaction times to the first items in the subset lists increased as the items occupied later positions in the original list, and that the reaction times increased substantially as the number of missing items which separated the current item from the previous item increased. Remaining-type simultaneous chain. Individual differences were found with this type of

7 242 N. Ohshiba Figure 2a. Reaction times to the subset lists of disappearing-type simultaneous chains: (a) the monkeys data.

8 Memorization of serial items by primates 243 Figure 2b. The chimpanzee and human data. M = monotonic; NM = nonmonotonic. simultaneous chain (Figure 3). Atsushi responded uniquely, whereas the other animals showed similar tendencies. Atsushi s performance was similar to that for the disappearingtype simultaneous chain. This implies that he was also performing a serial search for the remaining-type simultaneous chain. For Kyoko, Yusaku, and Pan, the reaction times for the first items of the lists were much longer than those for the other items. This suggests that they touched the stimulus items after they had identified all the target items. This strategy was referred to as a collective search. Serial search model. As stated above, the reaction times suggested that the subjects performed serial searches with the disappearingtype simultaneous chain. The reaction times could be composed of two independent processes: evaluation and motion (Sternberg, 1967, 1969a, 1969b). Evaluation is the process whereby the subject decides whether the item concerned is the current target. The duration of evaluation processes depends on the difficulty of the stimuli to which the subjects should respond. If the stimuli are easy to evaluate, then it should

9 244 N. Ohshiba Figure 3a. Reaction times to the subset lists of remaining-type simultaneous chains. (a) The monkeys data.

10 Memorization of serial items by primates 245 Figure 3b. The chimpanzee and human data. M = monotonic; NM = nonmonotonic. Table 2. Results of two-factor analysis of variance (type of list item) of the reaction time to the subset lists Subjects Type of task Factors df Atsushi Kyoko Yusaku Pan Disappearing Types of lists (1,6664) Items (54,6664) Remaining Types of lists (1,6664) Items (54,6664) The values of F are displayed. The values of p were all less than The interactions between type of task and item were not tested since the factor item branched under the factor of type of task.

11 246 N. Ohshiba take a short time to evaluate them, and vice versa. Motion literally refers to moving the hand to touch the target. Motion processes mainly reflect physical factors such as muscle movement. Therefore, the time necessary for a single motion process should be uniform for a given subject. Considering both evaluation and motion processes, the author has proposed a serial search model. This model assumes that a subject makes zero or more evaluations before it makes one motion. The subject s reaction times can be estimated based on the time necessary for a single evaluation, α, and the time necessary for a single motion, β. Let us consider a three-item list A C D as an example to explain how to calculate the expected reaction times. When the trial begins, there are three stimulus items displayed on the monitor, labeled I1, I2, and I3. The first target item is A. The subject evaluates whether or not I1 is equivalent to A. If I1 is A, then the subject will touch it. In this case, the subject makes one evaluation and one motion. If I1 is not A, then the subject evaluates whether or not I2 is A. If I2 is A, then the subject will touch it. In this case, the subject makes two evaluations and one motion. If I2 is also not A, then the subject evaluates whether or not I3 is A. If neither I1 nor I2 is A, then I3 should be A, and the subject will touch I3. In this case, the subject makes three evaluations and one motion. While it is certain that I1, I2, or I3 is A, exactly which item is A is probabilistic. Since the probabilities that I1, I2, or I3 is A are all 1 3, the expected reaction time to select A in A C D is: 1 3 {(α + β) + (2α + β) + (3α + β)} = 2.0α + β When the subject selects A, it disappears. Now there are two stimulus items displayed on the monitor, labeled J1 and J2. The target item is now B. The subject then evaluates whether either J1 or J2 is B, but in this example neither item is B. Therefore, the subject shifts the target item from B to C. The subject evaluates whether or not J1 is C. If J1 is C, then the subject will touch it. In this case, the subject makes three evaluations and one motion. If J1 is not C, then the subject evaluates whether or not J2 is C. If J1 is not C, then J2 should be C, and the subject will touch J2. In this case, the subject makes four evaluations and one motion. While it is certain that either J1 or J2 is C, exactly which item is C is probabilistic. Since the probabilities that J1 or J2 is C are both 1 2, the expected reaction time to select C in A C D is 1 2 {(3α + β) + (4α + β)} = 3.5α + β When the subject selects C, it disappears. Now there is only one stimulus item displayed on the monitor, and the subject has only to touch it. In this case, the subject makes no evaluation and one motion. The expected reaction time to select D in A C D is β. The expected reaction times for any items in any lists can be calculated by the same algorithm, as summarized in Table 3. The coefficients of α vary, whereas the coefficients of β are constantly 1. Therefore, the reaction times can be regarded as a linear function of the number of evaluations. Conformity of the serial search model to experimental results. Linear regression analyses were performed between the number of evaluations expected from the serial search model and the actual reaction time (Figure 4). The values of the coefficients and the equations of the regression lines are summarized in Table 4. For the monkeys, the agreement between the results of the serial search model and the actual reaction times was higher for the disappearing-type simultaneous chain than for the remaining-type chain. For the chimpanzee, this conformity was not as high as that for the monkeys for either type of simultaneous chain. The slope of the regression line reflects how much the reaction times would increase if the number of evaluations was increased by one, which is the time necessary for a single evaluation, α. The intercept of the regression line denotes the reaction time when the subject makes no evaluation, that is, the time necessary for a single motion, β. Table 4 shows that the values of the slopes varied remarkably while

12 Memorization of serial items by primates 247 Table 3. Reaction times expected from the serial search model Item position in the list Lists First Second Third Fourth A B 1.5α + β β A C 1.5α + β β A D 1.5α + β β B C 3.5α + β β B D 3.5α + β β C D 5.5α + β β A B C 2.0α + β 1.5α + β β A B D 2.0α + β 1.5α + β β A C D 2.0α + β 3.5α + β β B C D 5.0α + β 1.5α + β β A B C D 2.5α + β 2.0α + β 1.5α + β β α = time necessary for an evaluation; β = time necessary for a motion. those of the intercepts did not. This supports the assumption that the time necessary for a single evaluation may vary while the time necessary for a single motion should be constant. Discussion Comparison of disappearing- and remaining-type simultaneous chains The disappearing-type simultaneous chain is logically easier to perform than the remainingtype chain. The percentage of correct trials expected when a subject randomly selects the stimulus items is 16.7%, 8.3%, 4.2%, and 0.9% for three-item lists of the disappearing-type simultaneous chain, three-item lists of the remaining type, four-item lists of the disappearing type, and four-item lists of the remaining type, respectively. Hence, the better performance observed for the disappearing-type simultaneous chain than for the remaining-type chain was expected by chance. Comparison of monotonic and nonmonotonic lists The better performance with the monotonic lists than with the nonmonotonic lists suggests that the monotonic lists were easier to acquire than the nonmonotonic lists. Also, shorter reaction times with the monotonic lists suggests that they were easier to memorize than the nonmonotonic lists. Monotonicity among the list items may have promoted acquisition and improved the performance. Hulse (1978), using rats, also found better performance for monotonic lists than for nonmonotonic lists. With monotonic lists, the subject can resort to possible rules (e.g., from smaller to larger). This rule learning might make it easier to acquire monotonic lists. In contrast, nonmonotonic lists do not offer such rules. Therefore, the subject must learn which item to select before/after the other items. Comparison of Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans A major difference among the subject species was found regarding the reaction times for the disappearing-type simultaneous chain. Two types of search strategies were inferred: a serial search and a collective search. A serial search is a strategy whereby the subject decides which item to select at each stage. A collective search, on the other hand, is a strategy whereby the subject identifies all the target items before selecting them. The types of search used by the subjects are summarized in Table 5. The monkeys used a serial search for both the

13 248 N. Ohshiba Figure 4a. Linear correlation between the number of evaluations expected from the serial search model and the actual reaction time for (a) disappearingtype simultaneous chains.

14 Memorization of serial items by primates 249 Figure 4b. Remaining-type simultaneous chains. M = monotonic; NM = non-monotonic.

15 Table 4. Results of linear regression analyses between the number of evaluations expected from the serial search model and the actual reaction time Subjects Type of task Type of list Atsushi Kyoko Yusaku Pan Disappearing Monotonic ( ) r 2 = 0.948y = 180x r 2 = 0.813y = 224x r 2 = 0.703y = 108x r 2 = 0.422y = 69x Nonmonotonic ( ) r 2 = 0.915y = 334x r 2 = 0.877y = 346x r 2 = 0.770y = 206x r 2 = 0.601y = 173x Remaining Monotonic ( ) r 2 = 0.916y = 196x r 2 = 0.641y = 132x r 2 = 0.512y = 51x r 2 = 0.376y = 60x Nonmonotonic ( ) r 2 = 0.888y = 281x r 2 = 0.822y = 228x r 2 = 0.762y = 137x r 2 = 0.532y = 179x N. Ohshiba Upper = coefficient of determination; lower = equation of the regression line.

16 Memorization of serial items by primates 251 Table 5. Summary of types of searches Species Type of task Type of list Japanese monkey Chimpanzee Human Disappearing Monotonic Serial Collective Collective Nonmonotonic Serial Serial Collective Remaining Monotonic Collective Collective Collective Nonmonotonic Collective Collective Collective monotonic and nonmonotonic lists. The chimpanzee also used a serial search for nonmonotonic lists, but used a collective search for monotonic lists. The humans used a collective search for both the monotonic and nonmonotonic lists. These results indicate that species with higher cognitive abilities tended to use a collective search more often than a serial search. Hence, a collective search is presumed to be an evolutionarily new cognitive function. With the remaining-type simultaneous chain, all the subjects used a collective search for both the monotonic and nonmonotonic lists. Serial search model This paper has also described a serial search model based on two processes: evaluation and motion. According to this model, reaction times can be regarded as linear functions of the number of evaluations. Linear regression analyses of the number of evaluations and the actual reaction time confirmed the feasibility of this model, especially for disappearing-type simultaneous chains for the monkeys and the chimpanzee. The greatest merit of the serial search model is that it is possible to represent the difficulty of a list in terms of the time for a single evaluation. If the time required for a single evaluation is long, then the list is difficult, and vice versa. Therefore, the serial search model may be helpful for investigating how difficult lists are compared with one another. References D Amato, M. R., & Colombo, M. (1988). Representation of serial order in monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, D Amato, M. R., & Colombo, M. (1989). Serial learning with wild card items by monkeys (Cebus apella): Implications for knowledge of ordinal positions. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103, Hulse, S. H. (1978). Cognitive structure and serial pattern learning by animals. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kojima, S. (1989). Vocal-auditory functions in the chimpanzee: Vowel perception. International Journal of Primatology, 10, Sternberg, S. (1967). Two operations in character recognition: Some evidence from reaction-time measurements. Perception and Psychophysics, 2, Sternberg, S. (1969a). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders method. Acta Psychologica, 30, Sternberg, S. (1969b). Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57, Swartz, K. B., Chen, S., & Terrace, H. S. (1991). Serial learning by rhesus monkeys: I. Acquisition and retention of multiple four-item lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, Tanaka, M. (1995). Object sorting in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Classification based on physical identity, complementarity, and familiarity. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109,

17 252 N. Ohshiba Tanaka, M. (1996). Information integration about object-object relationships in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, Terrace, H. S. (1983). Simultaneous chaining: The problem it poses for traditional chaining theory. In M. L. Commons, R. J. Hernstein, & A. R. Wagner (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior, Vol. 4 (pp ). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger. Terrace, H. S. (1987). Chunking by a pigeon in a serial learning task. Nature, 325, Terrace, H. S. (1991). Chunking during serial learning by a pigeon: I. Basic evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, Terrace, H. S., & Chen, S. (1991a). Chunking during serial learning by a pigeon: II. Integrity of a chunk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, Terrace, H. S., & Chen, S. (1991b). Chunking during serial learning by a pigeon: III. Some necessary conditions for the formation of a chunk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, Terrace, H. S., & McGonigle, B. (1994). Memory and representation of serial order by children, monkeys, and pigeons. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, Tomonaga M., Matsuzawa T., & Itakura S. (1993). Teaching ordinals to a cardinal-trained chimpanzee (in Japanese with English summary). Primate Research, 9, (Received Oct. 17, 1996; accepted March 15, 1997)

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

Serial Learning by Rhesus Monkeys: I. Acquisition and Retention of Multiple Four-Item Lists

Serial Learning by Rhesus Monkeys: I. Acquisition and Retention of Multiple Four-Item Lists Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 4, 396-410 Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/$ 3.00 Serial Learning by Rhesus

More information

ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI TOMONAGA

ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI TOMONAGA JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1999, 72, 57 79 NUMBER 1(JULY) ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI

More information

EMERGENCE OF SYMMETRY IN A VISUAL CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION BY CHIMPANZEES (PAN TROGLODYTES) '

EMERGENCE OF SYMMETRY IN A VISUAL CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION BY CHIMPANZEES (PAN TROGLODYTES) ' Psychological Reports, 1991, 68, 51-60. O Psychological Reports 1991 EMERGENCE OF SYMMETRY IN A VISUAL CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION BY CHIMPANZEES (PAN TROGLODYTES) ' MASAKI TOMONAGA ' Osaka University TE'TSURO

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Learning and Motivation

Learning and Motivation Learning and Motivation 41 (2010) 273 286 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Learning and Motivation journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m What is learned when concept learning fails? A

More information

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 322 328 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.322

More information

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented 1 Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample A Thesis Presented by Kimberly M. Walter The Department of Counseling and Applied

More information

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? Keyed. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 55B (1), 1 25 Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? J.H. Wearden, A. Parry, and L. Stamp University of

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

Visual Search for Human Gaze Direction by a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)

Visual Search for Human Gaze Direction by a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Visual Search for Human Gaze Direction by a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Masaki Tomonaga*, Tomoko Imura Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Aichi, Japan Abstract Background: Humans detect

More information

Serial List Linking by Macaque Monkeys (Macaca mulatta): List Property Limitations

Serial List Linking by Macaque Monkeys (Macaca mulatta): List Property Limitations Journal of Comparative Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 121, No. 3, 250 259 0735-7036/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.121.3.250 Serial List Linking by Macaque

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Elizabeth M. Brannon*, Courtney J. Wusthoff #, C.R. Gallistel^, and John Gibbon @ * Center for Cognitive Neuroscience

More information

innate mechanism of proportionality adaptation stage activation or recognition stage innate biological metrics acquired social metrics

innate mechanism of proportionality adaptation stage activation or recognition stage innate biological metrics acquired social metrics 1 PROCESSES OF THE CORRELATION OF SPACE (LENGTHS) AND TIME (DURATIONS) IN HUMAN PERCEPTION Lev I Soyfer To study the processes and mechanisms of the correlation between space and time, particularly between

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Comp Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 October 12.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Comp Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 October 12. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Comp Psychol. 2007 November ; 121(4): 363 371. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.121.4.363. A Comparative Analysis of Serial Ordering in Ring-Tailed

More information

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS The Psychological Record, 2000, 50, 429-442 LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS LUIS A. PEREZ-GONZALEZ, JOSEPH E. SPRADLIN, and KATHRYN J. SAUNDERS University of Oviedo, Spain

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Summer 2005 Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli William

More information

The Role of Reference Points in Ordinal Numerical Comparisons by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta)

The Role of Reference Points in Ordinal Numerical Comparisons by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 2, 120 134 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.2.120

More information

Representation of the Numerosities 1-9 by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatto)

Representation of the Numerosities 1-9 by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatto) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Betuvkx Processes 2000, Vol. 26, No. 1,31-49 Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/QQ/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0097-7403.26.1.31

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1

fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1 BEHAVIORAL AND NEURAL BIOLOGY 36, 298-303 (1982) BRIEF REPORT Food-Aversion Conditioning in Japanese Monkeys (Macaca fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1 TETSURO MATSUZAWA Primate Research Institute,

More information

Learning Utility for Behavior Acquisition and Intention Inference of Other Agent

Learning Utility for Behavior Acquisition and Intention Inference of Other Agent Learning Utility for Behavior Acquisition and Intention Inference of Other Agent Yasutake Takahashi, Teruyasu Kawamata, and Minoru Asada* Dept. of Adaptive Machine Systems, Graduate School of Engineering,

More information

Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) select Arabic numerals or visual quantities corresponding to a number of sequentially completed maze trials

Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) select Arabic numerals or visual quantities corresponding to a number of sequentially completed maze trials Learning & Behavior 7, 35 (1), 53-5 Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) select Arabic numerals or visual quantities corresponding to a number of sequentially completed maze trials EMILY H. HARRIS, DAVID A.

More information

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 1 The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning Anila D Mello Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 2 Abstract Learning and changing

More information

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques C O N F I D E N T I A L Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques DATE This study was conducted under the terms of a Materials Transfer and Services Agreement between NeuroDetective International and

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

How other kinds of animals think about the world we share is one of the most interesting and

How other kinds of animals think about the world we share is one of the most interesting and Same/Different Concept Formation in Pigeons Robert G. Cook How other kinds of animals think about the world we share is one of the most interesting and difficult of scientific questions to answer. One

More information

Psychology Research Process

Psychology Research Process Psychology Research Process Logical Processes Induction Observation/Association/Using Correlation Trying to assess, through observation of a large group/sample, what is associated with what? Examples:

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare. Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos

Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare. Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos Supplemental Experimental Procedures Subjects Seven adult capuchin monkeys were tested.

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity Perception & Psychophysics 1980,28 (3), 249-253 Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity STEPHEN HANDEL and JAMES W. RHODES University oftennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee 37916 Subjects

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shikakeology: Designing Triggers for Behavior Change: Papers from the 2013 AAAI Spring Symposium The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University;

More information

Abstract-Concept Learning Carryover Effects From the Initial Training Set in Pigeons (Columba livia)

Abstract-Concept Learning Carryover Effects From the Initial Training Set in Pigeons (Columba livia) Journal of Comparative Psychology 29 American Psychological Association 29, Vol. 123, No. 1, 79 89 735-36/9/$12. DOI: 1.137/a13126 Abstract-Concept Learning Carryover Effects From the Initial Training

More information

Cognitive dissonance in children: Justification of effort or contrast?

Cognitive dissonance in children: Justification of effort or contrast? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (3), 673-677 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.3.673 Cognitive dissonance in children: Justification of effort or contrast? JÉRÔME ALESSANDRI AND JEAN-CLAUDE DARCHEVILLE University

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization Acoust. Sci. & Tech. 24, 2 (23) PAPER The role of low components in median plane localization Masayuki Morimoto 1;, Motoki Yairi 1, Kazuhiro Iida 2 and Motokuni Itoh 1 1 Environmental Acoustics Laboratory,

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed Through Partial Versus Total Reversals of Many-to-One Conditional and Simple Discriminations

Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed Through Partial Versus Total Reversals of Many-to-One Conditional and Simple Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psycholo Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 2, 194-201 Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/43.00 Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed

More information

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08 Auditory Perception - Detection versus Discrimination - Localization versus Discrimination - - Electrophysiological Measurements Psychophysical Measurements Three Approaches to Researching Audition physiology

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z

Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 10, 522-527 (1971) Memory Scanning for Words Versus Categories z JAMES F. JUOLA AND R. C. ATKINSON Stanford University, Stanford, California 94305 Two groups

More information

Rats Show Adaptive Choice in a Metacognitive Task With High Uncertainty

Rats Show Adaptive Choice in a Metacognitive Task With High Uncertainty Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2017 American Psychological Association 2017, Vol. 43, No. 1, 109 118 2329-8456/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000130 Rats Show

More information

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08 Auditory Perception - Detection versus Discrimination - Localization versus Discrimination - Electrophysiological Measurements - Psychophysical Measurements 1 Three Approaches to Researching Audition physiology

More information

Olfactory Matching-To-Sample In Rats Using a Novel Apparatus. Rhiannon D. Thomas

Olfactory Matching-To-Sample In Rats Using a Novel Apparatus. Rhiannon D. Thomas Olfactory Matching-To-Sample In Rats Using a Novel Apparatus Rhiannon D. Thomas A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

More information

Pattern recognition in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): The role of the spatial organisation of stimulus parts

Pattern recognition in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): The role of the spatial organisation of stimulus parts Behavioural Brain Research 181 (2007) 96 109 Research report Pattern recognition in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): The role of the spatial organisation of stimulus parts Carlo De Lillo a,, Giovanna

More information

Masaki Ishizuka, Tetsumi Moriyama. Tokiwa University, Mito, Japan

Masaki Ishizuka, Tetsumi Moriyama. Tokiwa University, Mito, Japan Psychology Research, September 2018, Vol. 8, No. 9, 411-434 doi:10.17265/2159-5542/2018.09.001 D DAVID PUBLISHING Symmetry Formation by Undergraduates in Successive Matching: A Re-evaluation of Urcuioli

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons

Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons Supplementary Information for: Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons Steven W. Kennerley, Timothy E. J. Behrens & Jonathan D. Wallis Content list: Supplementary

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

Partial reinforcement delayed extinction effect after regularly alternating reward training

Partial reinforcement delayed extinction effect after regularly alternating reward training Animal Learning & Behavior 1986, 14 (3), 293-300 Partial reinforcement delayed extinction effect after regularly alternating reward training MASATO ISHIDA Osaka University of Education, Osaka, Japan When

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

We illustrate some of these key building blocks of evolutionary cognitive growth based on relational competence in the sections below:

We illustrate some of these key building blocks of evolutionary cognitive growth based on relational competence in the sections below: To appear in The Encyclopedia of Applied Animal Behaviour and Welfare edited by D.Mills CABI Publishing. Entry: Insight and relational mechanisms Brendan McGonigle and Margaret Chalmers Relational mechanisms

More information

Multidimensional Auditory Stimulus Control in a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)

Multidimensional Auditory Stimulus Control in a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) PRIMATES, 31(4): 545-553, October 1990 545 Multidimensional Auditory Stimulus Control in a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) MASAKI TOMONAGA* Osaka University ABSTRACT. An adolescent female chimpanzee (Pan

More information

Testing the translational-symmetry hypothesis of abstract-concept learning in pigeons

Testing the translational-symmetry hypothesis of abstract-concept learning in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2010, 38 (1), 35-41 doi:10.3758/lb.38.1.35 Testing the translational-symmetry hypothesis of abstract-concept learning in pigeons JEFFREY S. KATZ Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama BRADLEY

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

Testing pigeon memory in a change detection task

Testing pigeon memory in a change detection task Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21, 17 (2), 243-249 doi:1.3758/pbr.17.2.243 Testing pigeon memory in a change detection task ANTHONY A. WRIGHT University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston, Texas JEFFREY

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons

Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons Entire Set of Printable Figures For Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons Cook In one experiment, Cerella (1980) trained pigeons to discriminate intact drawings of Charlie Brown from normal drawings

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University; Graduate School of Economics, Osaka University syamane@kindai.ac.jp Abstract We examined

More information

Parallel scanning ofauditory and visual information

Parallel scanning ofauditory and visual information Memory & Cognition 1975, Vol. 3 (4),416-420 Parallel scanning ofauditory and visual information DAVID BURROWS and BARRY A. SOLOMON State University ofnew York, College at Brockport, Brockport, New York

More information

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action PSY 402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action Categorization and Discrimination Animals respond to stimuli in ways that suggest they form categories. Pigeons

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Business Statistics Probability

Business Statistics Probability Business Statistics The following was provided by Dr. Suzanne Delaney, and is a comprehensive review of Business Statistics. The workshop instructor will provide relevant examples during the Skills Assessment

More information

Title: Stimulus Pairing Training in. Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Kosuke Takahashi a. Jun ichi Yamamoto b

Title: Stimulus Pairing Training in. Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Kosuke Takahashi a. Jun ichi Yamamoto b Running head: STIMULUS PAIRING TRAINING Title: in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Kosuke Takahashi a E-Mail:kosuke77529@hotmail.com Jun ichi Yamamoto b E-Mail:yamamotj@flet.keio.ac.jp and Fumiyuki

More information

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Hidehito Honda (hito@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Kimihiko Yamagishi (kimihiko@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Graduate School of Decision Science

More information

Transfer of a Serial Representation between Two Distinct Tasks by Rhesus Macaques

Transfer of a Serial Representation between Two Distinct Tasks by Rhesus Macaques Transfer of a Serial Representation between Two Distinct Tasks by Rhesus Macaques Greg Jensen*, Drew Altschul, Erin Danly, Herbert Terrace* Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, New

More information

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Infant and Child Development Inf. Child Dev. 10: 39 45 (2001) DOI: 10.1002/icd.245 Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Olivier Pascalis a, *, Elisabeth

More information

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses?

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses? Mental Imagery What is Imagery? What is nature of the images? Exact copy of original images? Represented in terms of meaning? If so, then how does the subjective sensation of an image arise? What is the

More information

Optimal exploration strategies in haptic search

Optimal exploration strategies in haptic search Optimal exploration strategies in haptic search Vonne van Polanen, Wouter M. Bergmann Tiest, Noortje Creemers, Merel J. Verbeek, and Astrid M. L. Kappers MOVE Research Institute, Faculty of Human Movement

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

RESULTS Human vs Macaques

RESULTS Human vs Macaques 14 RESULTS Human vs Macaques As mentioned before, this experiment aimed to test the ability of monkeys in categorizing monkeys and humans into two different conceptual classes. The two classes were presented

More information

Annotation and Retrieval System Using Confabulation Model for ImageCLEF2011 Photo Annotation

Annotation and Retrieval System Using Confabulation Model for ImageCLEF2011 Photo Annotation Annotation and Retrieval System Using Confabulation Model for ImageCLEF2011 Photo Annotation Ryo Izawa, Naoki Motohashi, and Tomohiro Takagi Department of Computer Science Meiji University 1-1-1 Higashimita,

More information

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E. In D. Algom, D. Zakay, E. Chajut, S. Shaki, Y. Mama, & V. Shakuf (Eds.). (2011). Fechner Day 2011: Proceedings of the 27 th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 89-94). Raanana,

More information

Is Cognitive Science Special? In what way is it special? Cognitive science is a delicate mixture of the obvious and the incredible

Is Cognitive Science Special? In what way is it special? Cognitive science is a delicate mixture of the obvious and the incredible Sept 3, 2013 Is Cognitive Science Special? In what way is it special? Zenon Pylyshyn, Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science Cognitive science is a delicate mixture of the obvious and the incredible What

More information

Discrimination Reversal Learning in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella)

Discrimination Reversal Learning in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) The Psychological Record, 2008, 58, 3 14 Discrimination Reversal Learning in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) Michael J. Beran, Emily D. Klein, Theodore A. Evans, Betty Chan, Timothy M. Flemming, Emily

More information

Pushing the Right Buttons: Design Characteristics of Touch Screen Buttons

Pushing the Right Buttons: Design Characteristics of Touch Screen Buttons 1 of 6 10/3/2009 9:40 PM October 2009, Vol. 11 Issue 2 Volume 11 Issue 2 Past Issues A-Z List Usability News is a free web newsletter that is produced by the Software Usability Research Laboratory (SURL)

More information

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, 152 162 2329-8456/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000053 Memory Systems

More information

Measuring the User Experience

Measuring the User Experience Measuring the User Experience Collecting, Analyzing, and Presenting Usability Metrics Chapter 2 Background Tom Tullis and Bill Albert Morgan Kaufmann, 2008 ISBN 978-0123735584 Introduction Purpose Provide

More information

Mental operations on number symbols by-children*

Mental operations on number symbols by-children* Memory & Cognition 1974, Vol. 2,No. 3, 591-595 Mental operations on number symbols by-children* SUSAN HOFFMAN University offlorida, Gainesville, Florida 32601 TOM TRABASSO Princeton University, Princeton,

More information

Simple Linear Regression the model, estimation and testing

Simple Linear Regression the model, estimation and testing Simple Linear Regression the model, estimation and testing Lecture No. 05 Example 1 A production manager has compared the dexterity test scores of five assembly-line employees with their hourly productivity.

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons by Thomas A. Daniel A thesis proposal submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Auburn University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree

More information

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 B87 Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized Wim Gevers*,

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Colour Naming and Classification in a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)

Colour Naming and Classification in a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Tetsuro Matsuzawa Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Aichi 484, Japan Received 22 November 1984 and accepted 3 January 1985 Keywords: chimpanzee, colour naming, colour classification,

More information

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee The Psychological Record, 1997,47,145-166 THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pigeons served

More information

Dogs Can Discriminate Emotional Expressions of Human Faces

Dogs Can Discriminate Emotional Expressions of Human Faces Current Biology Supplemental Information Dogs Can Discriminate Emotional Expressions of Human Faces Corsin A. Müller, Kira Schmitt, Anjuli L.A. Barber, and Ludwig Huber probe trials (percent correct) 100

More information